ArticlePDF Available

Relationship between academic pRocRastinaton and self-contRol: the mediational Role of self-esteem

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The present study examined the mediational effect of self-esteem on the relationship between self-control and academic procrastination. The sample consisted of 426 emerging adults (university students; 218 female, 208 male; M age = 21.3 years old, SD = 1.5) who completed reliable and valid academic procrastination, self-control, and self-esteem measures. Results revealed that academic procrastination was negatively predicted by self-esteem, and self-control. Moreover, findings suggested that self-esteem mediated the association of academic procrastination with self-control. Results are discussed in terms of prevention and intervention implications.
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Relationship between academic
pRocRastinaton and self-contRol: the
mediational Role of self-esteem

Bahcesehir University, Department of Psychological Counseling and Guidance

Australian College of Applied Psychology

DePaul University
    
      
        
    M     SD    
       

   
    



Procrastination    

 

  
    
     
    
       
     
     
      
    
      
    
    
   

      

    
     
     
  
 
  
academic
procrastination   

 

    
      
     
2 / College Student Journal
to academic procrastination in a university
demonstrating that academic procrastination
is closely related to lower levels of self-con-
trol (Digdon & Howell, 2008), self-efficacy
(Klassen et al., 2008) and academic perfor-
mance with a concurrent increase in anxiety
levels (Chabaud, Ferrand, & Maury, 2010).
Researchers have focused on various
aspects of academic procrastination includ-
ing cognitive and behavioral components
(Kachgal, Hansen, & Kevin, 2001; Schow-
uenburg et al., 2004). Studies based on the
cognitive perspective investigated why stu-
dents make the conscious decision to delay al-
though it has negative consequences (Ferrari,
2010; Karas & Spada, 2009). This line of re-
search investigated the relationship between
self-esteem and academic procrastination
(e.g., Tice, 1991). Self-esteem is used as a per-
sonality (Brown & Marshall, 2006) variable
representing the way people generally feel
about themselves. One of the most popular
theories concerning the etiology of procrasti-
nation conceptualizes chronic procrastination
as a method of self-protection for fragile
self-esteem (Burka & Yuen, 1983; Ferrari &
Tibbett, 2017). Theory suggests performance
reflects ability, which is also a reflection of
self-worth. This assertion would then lead to
a link between performance, ability and self-
worth; hence, failure at a task may become an
indicator for lack of ability and low self-worth
in the absence of procrastination or self-hand-
icapping. Therefore, in an academic setting,
students may develop a fear of failure, fueling
their academic procrastination behaviours
because of the strong emphasis placed on
success when defining self-worth. As such,
academic procrastination may serve a protec-
tive function reinforcing the behaviour: since
performance has been impaired by time con-
straints, performance does not equal ability
and therefore does not equal self worth. In this
way, academic procrastination serves as an
ego defensive mechanism; therefore, it may
be used frequently as a protective device by
those with fragile self-esteem (Burka & Yuen,
1983). In this respect, numerous studies found
a significant inverse relationship between
procrastination and self-esteem (e.g., Ferrari,
1994; Uzun Ozer, Demir, & Harrington, 2012
such that low self-esteem and weak self-effi-
cacy are related to frequent academic-related
delays (Beck, Koons, & Milgrim, 2000; Fer-
rari, 1994; Sirois, 2004) whereby, feelings of
worthlessness cause task avoidance that may
result in failure (Ferrari, 2010). In this vein,
the findings of the previous research demon-
strate that student self-esteem may be a strong
predictor of academic-related task delays and
may serve a mediating role between academic
procrastination and other variables.
Some researchers investigating be-
havioural aspects of academic procrastina-
tion (Ferrari & Tice, 2000; Beck et al., 2000;
Van Eerde, 2003) argued that self-control
is one of the strongest behavioral predic-
tors of all forms of procrastination. Broadly
speaking, self-control refers to the capacity
to modify and adapt the self in order to op-
timize the fit between self and the external
environment (e.g., Rothbaum, Weisz, &
Snyder, 1982). Specifically, Vohs and Bau-
meister (2004) identified four key domains
of self-control which include the effective
control of thoughts, emotions, impulses, and
performance. Therefore, self-control involves
the ability to restrain oneself from engaging
in behaviors that are known to have negative
implications (e.g. smoking, purchasing be-
havior, breaking laws or procrastinating; e.g.,
Faber & Vohs, 2004). Ferrari (2010) suggests
procrastination is a ‘self-control failure of
performance’ (p. 391) in which procrastina-
tors fail to regulate their performance in situa-
tions of stress and high cognitive loads. Thus,
the process of self-control involves making
a comparison of one’s behavioral state to
an ideal or standard of behavior (Duval &
Wicklund, 1972). Concordantly, researchers
A Mediational Model of Academic Procrastination / 3
(e.g., Klassen, et al., 2008; Howell & Watson,
2007) believe procrastinators have problems
regulating their behaviors and are engaging
in undercontrol. Relatedly, a metanalysis
(Ferrari, 2010) investigating cause and effect,
determined chronic everyday procrastination
was strongly predicted by trait characteristics
such as self-control (i.e. distractibility, organi-
zation, and achievement) and impulsiveness.
Thus, a strong body of evidence supports the
strong inverse relationship between self-con-
trol and forms of procrastination, suggesting
lower levels of self-control are related to
higher levels of academic procrastination
(e.g., Uzun Ozer, et. al., 2014).
These facts demonstrate the need for
close examination of academic procrastina-
tion among emerging adults (i.e., university
students) because of its frequency and debil-
itating impact. Yet, detailed models explain-
ing the contributing dynamics of academic
procrastination are limited; thus, numerous
questions remain with regards to root causes
and operating mechanisms. In this study we
explored causal correlates, putting forth a pre-
liminary model investigating the relationship
between academic procrastination, self-es-
teem and self-control in greater depth.
Taken together, low self-esteem exists in
the presence of low self-control may serve
as a powerful mediator between maladaptive
behaviours that increase the frequency of
academic procrastination among emerging
adults in university settings. Thus, the current
study built upon existing literature by pro-
viding a better understanding of the link be-
tween self-esteem, self-control and academic
procrastination.
Considering previous research (see Fer-
rari, 2010; Ferrari & Emmons, 1995; Ferrari
& Tibbett, 2017), it was hypothesized that
self-control would be negatively correlated
with academic procrastination. Similarly,
self-esteem was hypothesized to be negatively
associated with academic procrastination and
positively related to self-control, suggesting
that students with lower levels of self-esteem
will have lower levels of self-control.
Method
Participants
Data were collected from undergraduate
students enrolled in various departments at a
major-state funded, urban university in Tur-
key. For the sample selection, proportional
sampling created five subgroups from five
faculties selected for the sample in the same
proportion, as they exist in the general pop-
ulation. The sample of 426 undergraduate
students included 218 female and 208 male
students. The participants consisted of 107
first year students, 78 sophomore, 160 junior,
and 81 senior students. The mean age of our
emerging adult participants was 21.3 years
old (SD = 1.5) with an age range between 18
and 25.
Data Collection Instruments
In the present study, variables were as-
sessed utilizing self-report reliable and valid
questionnaires, namely.
Tuckman procrastination scale (TPS;
Tuckman, 1991) assesses college students’
academic procrastination tendencies. It is 16
16 items reliable (α = 0.89; Tuckman, 2007)
and valid (r = -.47 with the General Self-Ef-
ficacy Scale) and demonstrated high internal
consistency (0.88), retest stability (0.82).
In the Turkish adaptation study, Uzun Özer,
Sackes, and Tuckman (2013) found the scale
to be reliable and valid (α = 0.90; r = 22 with
self-efficacy and r = 23 with self-esteem).
Self Control Schedule (SCS; Rosenbaum,
1980) assessed students’ tendencies to exert
self-control methods to resolve behavioral
problems. The scale consists of 36 items.
In the Rosenbaum’s study, an alpha coeffi-
cient was computed on six different samples
ranging from 0.78 to 0.86, indicating a high
4 / College Student Journal
internal consistency among items. The SCS’s
evidence for validity was provided by correla-
tions with Croskey’s Measure of Communi-
cation Apprehension (r = -.37; Rosenbaum,
1980). In a Turkish adaptation study, Siva
(1991) found the scale to be reliable and valid
= 0.79; test-retest r = 0.80; r = -22 with
locus of control).
Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES;
Rosenberg, 1965) assessed participants’ glob-
al self-esteem or self-worth. It is a 10 items
scale demonstrating high internal consistency
= .80). Evidence for convergent validity
was provided by positive correlations with the
Copersmith Self-Esteem Inventory (r = .60)
and the Health Self Image Questionnaire (r
= .83). In a Turkish adaptation studies, Celik
(2004) reported the scale to be reliable and
valid (α = 0.84; r = -22 with locus of control).
Procedures
Demographic questionnaires including an
explanation of the study and a set of scales
were administered to the participants during
regular classroom hours. After obtaining per-
missions from the Human Research Ethical
Committee and the instructor of each class,
volunteer students were asked to respond to
the scales.
Results
To test our hypothesized model, the
mediational role of self-esteem on the rela-
tionship between self-control and procras-
tination, a confirmatory factor analysis was
first conducted to ensure the model was an
acceptable fit to the data. Then the structural
model was tested to assess the hypothesized
relationship. The Maximum Likelihood
method in AMOS 25 (Byrne, 2016) was
utilized to examine the measurement and
structural model. Four indecies were used to
assess the goodness of fit of the models on
the basis of the traditional crriteria for good
fit: the comparative fit index, the incremental
fit index, the standardized root mean square
residual, and the root mean square error of
approximation (Hu & Bentler, 1999).
Latent Variables
To test the model, indicators of the la-
tent construct were observed. The observed
indicators of the latent variables of procras-
tination, self-control and self-esteem were
parceled following the recommendation of
Russell, Kahn, Spoth, and Althmaier (1998).
Four parcels were created for each scale items
seperately. Item parcelling was chosen for the
variables in order to reduce the number of
parameters that would result from using indi-
vidual items thereby improving the estimation
of the effect (Russell et al., 1998). Bandalos
(2008) and Nasser and Wisenbaker (2003)
also asserted that parcel scores are more like-
ly to be distributed normally than those of sin-
gle items. Thus, ‘the resulting reduction in the
complexity of measurement models should
lead to more realistic models’ (Nasser and
Wisenbaker, 2003; p.730). The correlations
among the study variables were small to mod-
erate in magnitude (r = -.20 and r = -.48). As
expected, self-esteem and self-control were
negatively associated with procrastination.
Measurement Model
A test of the measurement model resulted
in a good fit to the data, scaled X2 (50, N =
426) = 134.67, p < 0.001, CFI = .96, IFI = .96,
RMSEA = .63. All of the variables were sig-
nificantly loaded on their perspectives latent
variables and appear to have been adequately
measured by their perspective indicators.
Structural Model
The structural model used to test the
hypothesis (see Figure 1) showed an excel-
lent fit to the data, scaled X2 (50, N = 426)
= 102.98, p < 0.001, CFI = .97, IFI = .97,
RMSEA = .50. Self-control and self-esteem
predicted procrastination. Seven percent of
A Mediational Model of Academic Procrastination / 5
the variance in self-esteem and 46% of the
variance in procrastination was accounted
for in the model. Thus, the hypothesized
model was the best fitting model and used in
the subsequent boostrapping and invariance
testing analysis.
Boostraping
Significant levels of indirect effects for the
meditational model were tested by following
the boostrapt procedure recommended by
Shrout and Bolger (2002). Analysis showed
that indirect effects were normally distributed.
The indirect effects specified in the hypothe-
ses were estimated via bootstrapping (set at
1000), and bias corrected bootstrap (BC) and
95% confidence intervals were requested.
The bootstrap values provided significant me-
diated paths from self-control through self-es-
teem to procrastination (β = .31, p < .01).
Discussion
The present study investigated the media-
tional affects of self esteem in the relationship
between self-control and academic procrasti-
nation. Results showed both self-esteem and
self control negatively predicted academic
procrastination, indicating lower levels
of self-esteem and self-control predicted
higher levels of academic procrastination.
Self-control was also a significant predictor
of self-esteem suggesting an indirect effect
on academic procrastination via self-esteem.
Results demonstrated that the hypothesized
relationship was well supported using the cur-
rent data. The findings, both statistically and
theoretically, supported the important effect
of self-esteem in the relationship between
self-control and academic procrastination
among emerging adults.
Figure 1. Standardized parameter estimates of the structural model. Ps are the parcels
for latent variables. Numbers next to arrows indicate the relationships between the latent
and measured variables.
6 / College Student Journal
The present findings suggest individuals’
cognitions about themselves lead to their
behavioral control, which in turn directly pre-
dicted procrastination. Specifically, findings
revealed that students’ low levels of self-es-
teem led to decrease self-control, which re-
sulted in an increase in procrastination. The
results were consistent with the previous find-
ings demonstrating a relationship between
self-esteem and self-control (Steel, Brothen,
& Wambach, 2001; Tice & Bratslavsky,
2000). The findings also supported the theory
that contingencies of self-worth may serve
a self-regulatory function. Accordingly, in-
creased self-worth in an academic setting may
serve to enhance self-control, which in turn
decreases academic procrastination (Roth-
man, Baldwin, & Hertel., 2004).
Another important finding of the present
study may be the behavioral control features
of self-control. As suggested earlier (e.g.,
Fitzsimons & Bargh, 2004), self-esteem
mechanisms may provide students with con-
trolling thoughts, feelings and behavior. In
line with self-control theory, it has been seen
as willpower or effortful control in cogni-
tive-affective processing systems, which is a
dynamic of delay of gratification (Mischel &
Ayduk, 2004).
Several implications may be drawn from
the findings of the present study for counsel-
ors and educators. The present study explored
the relationships between behavioral factors
such as self-control and its ability to mediate
personal cognitive factors among a represen-
tative sample of university students enrolled
in various departments at a major state funded
university. Therefore, the present study has
the potential to generate meaningful infor-
mation for understanding student procrasti-
nation associated with factors contributing
to cognitive and behavioral patterns. Hence,
the results of the present study may provide
valuable cues for both university counselors
and staff to develop new programs that may
reduce the negative effects of procrastination.
From an applied standpoint, the current
findings suggest interventions targeting both
the cognitive and behavioral components of
procrastination may be effective. In support,
interventions focused on enhancing self-con-
trol by creating routines, timetables and
predetermined activities have demonstrated
efficacy (Ferrari, 2010).
From a cognitive behavioral perspective,
cognitive restructuring, combined with be-
havioural experiments, has demonstrated
effictiveness and has been associated with
concurrent increases in self-esteem (Van
Eerde, 2003). In conclusion, taking an idio-
graphic approach, combining behavioral and
cognitive components and exploring alterna-
tive methods of delivery, may be a promising
avenue for future research, as they are theo-
retically supported and adventageous from
a practical standpoint, due to scalability and
cost effectiveness.
A limitation of the current study was the
use of a homogenous, university sample.
Further research with larger and more de-
mographically diverse populations would
strengthen the findings of the current study.
Therefore, it is suggested to conduct future
studies with samples from different universi-
ties and in different regions.
A Mediational Model of Academic Procrastination / 7
References
Bandalos, D. L. (2002). Is parcelling really necessary?
A comparison of results from item parcelling
and categorical variable methodology. Structural
Equation Modeling, 15, 211-240. Doi: 10.1207/
S15328007SEM0901_5
Beck, B. L., Koons, S. R., & Milgrim, D. L. (2000).
Correlates and consequences of behavioral procras-
tination: The effects of academic procrastination,
self-consciousness, self-esteem and self-handicap-
ping. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality,
15(5), 3-13.
Brown, J. D., & Marshall, M. A. (2006). The three faces
of self-esteem. In M. Kernis (Ed.), Self-esteem: Is-
sues and answers (pp. 4-9). New York: Psychology
Press.
Burka, J. B., & Yuen, L. M. (1983). Procrastination: Why
do you do it, what to do about it. CA: Addison- Wes-
ley Publishing Company.
Byrne, B. M. (2016). Structural equation modeling with
AMOS : Basic concepts, applications, and program-
ming (3rd edition). NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Chabaud, P., Ferrand, C., & Maury, J. (2010). Individual
diffrerences in undergraduate student athletes: The
roles of perfectionism and trait anxiety perception of
procrastination behavior. Social Behavior & Person-
ality: An International Journal, 38(8), 1041-1056.
Digdon, N. L., & Howell, A. J. (2008). College students
who have an eveningness preference report lower
self-control and greater procrastination. Chronobi-
ology International: The Journal of Biological &
Medical Rhythm Research, 25(6), 1029-1046.
Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objec-
tive self-awareness. NY: Academic Press.
Ellis, A., & Knaus, W. (1977). Overcoming procrastina-
tion. NY: Signet.
Faber, R. J., & Vohs, K. D. (2004). To buy or not to buy?
Self control and self-control failure in purshase be-
havior. In R. F. Baumesister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.),
Handbook of self-control: Research, theory and ap-
plications. NY: The Guilford Press.
Ferrari, J. R. (1994). Dysfunctional procrastination and
Its relationship with self-esteem, interpersonal de-
pendency, and self-defeating behaviors. Personality
and Individual Differences, 17(5), 673-679.
Ferrari, J. R., & Emmons, R. A. (1995). Methods of
procrastination and their relation to self-control and
self-reinforcement: An exploratory study. Journal of
Social Behavior and Personality, 10(1), 135.
Ferrari, J.R. (2010). Still Procrastinating? The No Re-
grets Guide to Getting It Done. New York: John
Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Ferrari, J. R., Johnson, J. L., & McCown, W. G. (Eds.).
(1995). Procrastination and task avoidance: Theory
Research, and Treatment. NY: Plenum Press.
Ferrari, J., & Pychyl, T. (2008). Procrastination: Current
Issues and New Directions, 15(5).
Ferrari, J. R., & Tice, D. M. (2000). Procrastination as a
self-handicap for men and women: A task-avoidance
strategy in a laboratory setting. Journal of Research
in Personality, 34(1), 73-83.
Ferrari, J. R., & Tibbett, T. P. (2017). Procrastination. In
V. Zeigler-Hill & T.K. Shackelford (Eds). Encyclo-
pedia of Personality and Individual Differences, pp.
1–8. New York: Springer Meteor Press.
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). Automatic
self-control. In R. F. Baumesister & K. D. Vohs
(Eds.), Handbook of self-control: Research, theory
and applications. NY: The Guilford Press.
Howell, A. J., & Watson, D. C. (2007). Procrastination:
Associations with achievement goal orientation and
learning strategies. Personality and Individual Dif-
ferences, 43(1), 167-178.
Howell, A. J., Watson, D. C., Powell, R. A., & Buro, K.
(2006). Academic procrastination: The pattern and
correlates of behavioural postponement. Personality
and Individual Differences, 40(8), 1519-1530.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit in-
dexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional
criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation
Modeling, 6, 1-55.
Kachgal, M. M., Hansen, L. S., & Kevin, N. J. (2001).
Academic procrastination prevention/intervention
strategies and recommendations. Journal of Devel-
opmental Education, 25(1), 14-20.
Karas, D., & Spada, M. M. (2009). Brief cognitive-be-
havioural coaching for procrastination: a case series.
Coaching: An International Journal of Theory, Re-
search & Practice, 2(1), 44-53.
Klassen, R. M., Krawchuk, L. L., & Rajani, S. (2008).
Academic procrastination of undergraduates: Low
self-efficacy to self-regulate predicts higher levels of
procrastination. Contemporary Educational Psychol-
ogy, 33(4), 915-931.
Mischel, W., & Ayduk, O. (2004). Willpower in a cog-
nitive-affective processing system: The dynamics
of delay gratification. In R. F. Baumesister & K. D.
Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-control: Research,
theory and applications. NY: The Guilford Press.
Nasser, F., & Wisenbaker, J. (2006). A Monte Carlo study
investigating the impact of item parcelling on mea-
sures of fit in confirmatory factor analysis. Educa-
tional and Psychological Measurement, 63, 729-757.
Doi: 101177/0013164403258228.
Roseberg, M. (1965). Society and adolescence self-im-
age. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University.
8 / College Student Journal
Rosenbaum, M. (1980). A schedule for assessing
self-control behaviors: Preliminary findings. Behav-
ior Therapy, 11(1), 109-121.
Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J. R., & Snyder, S. S. (1982).
Changing the world and changing the self: A
two-process model of perceived control. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 42(1), 5.
Rothman, A. J., Baldwin, A., & Hertel, A. (2004).
Self-control and behavior change: Disentangling
behavioral initiation and behavioral maintenance.
In K. Vohs & R. Baumeister (Eds.), The handbook
of self-control (pp. 130-148). New York: Guilford
Press.
Russell, D. W., Kahn, J. H., Spoth, R., & Altmaier, E.
M. (1998). Analyzing data from experimental stud-
ies: A latent variable structural equation modeling
approach. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45,
18–29.
Schowuenburg, H. C., Lay, C. H., Pychyl, T. A., & Fer-
rari, J. R. (2004). Counseling the procrastinator in
academic settings. Washington, DC: American Psy-
chological Association.
Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in ex-
perimental and non- experimental studies: New
procedures and recommendations. Psychological
Methods, 7, 422–445.
Sirois, F. M. (2004). Procrastination and counterfactual
thinking: Avoiding what might have been. British
Journal of Social Psychology 43, 269-286.
Siva, A. (1991). İnfertilede stresle başetme, öğrenilmiş
güçlülük ve depresyonun incelenmesi [Examining
stress coping, learned resourcefullness, and depres-
sion in infertility]. Unpublished Doctoral Disserta-
tion, Hacettepe University, Ankara.
Steel, P., Brothen, T., & Wambach, C. (2001). Procrasti-
nation and personality, performance, and mood. Per-
sonality and Individual Differences, 30(1), 95-106.
Tan, J., Ma, Z., & Li, X. (2015). Global self-esteem
mediates the effect of general self-efficacy on Chi-
nese undergraduates’ general procrastination. Social
Behavior and Personality: An international Journal,
43(8), 1265-1271.
Tice, D. M., & Bratslavsky, E. (2000). Giving in to feel
good: The place of emotion control in the context of
general self-control. Psychological Inquiry, 11(3),
149-159.
Tuckman, B. W. (1991). The development and concurrent
validity of the procrastination scale. Educational &
Psychological Measurement, 51(2), 473.
Tuckman, B. W. (2007). The effect of motivational
scaffolding on procrastinators’ distance learning
outcomes. Computers & Education, 49(2), 414-422.
Uzun Özer, B., Demir, A., & Harrington, N. (2012).
Psychometric properties of Frustration Discomfort
Scale in a Turkish Sample. Psychological Reports:
Measures & Statistics, 111(1), 117-128.
Uzun Özer, B., O’Callaghan, J., Bokszczanin, A., Eder-
er, E., & Essau, C. A. (2014). Dynamic interplay
of depression, perfectionism, and self-control on
procrastination. British Journal of Guidance and
Counselling, 42(3), 309-319.
Uzun Özer, B., Saçkes, M., & Tuckman, W. B. (2013).
Psychometric Properties of the Tuckman Procrasti-
nation Scale in a Turkish Sample. Psychological Re-
ports: Measurement and Statistics, 113(3), 874-884.
Van Eerde, W. (2003). Procrastination: Self-regulation in
initiating aversive goals. Applied Psychology, 49(3),
372-389. Doi: 10.1111/1464-0597.00021
Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2004). Understanding
self-control: An introduction. Handbook of self-con-
trol: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 1–9).
... Sagar (2021) found that self-control is a significant predictor of social media addiction in university students. In a study on university students, Uzun et al. (2020) concluded that self-control predicts academic procrastination. Another study focusing on 21-year-olds engaged in online gaming found that self-control predicts addiction (Kim et al., 2008). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to examine the effects of self-control and future time perception on social media addiction and academic procrastination and determine whether social media addiction affects academic achievement through academic procrastination. A total of 559 university students participated. The participants’ most commonly used social media platforms were WhatsApp and Instagram. Path analysis technique was used to analyze the theoretical model. In the theoretical model created to test the 13 hypotheses in the research, future time perception (value, distance, commitment, and speed) and self-control are exogenous variables, social media addiction and academic procrastination are moderating variables, and academic achievement is endogenous variable. According to the research findings, social media addiction is not a significant predictor variable for either academic achievement or academic procrastination. Self-control and speed are significant predictors of both social media addiction and academic procrastination. Commitment predicts social media addiction, and value predicts academic procrastination. On the contrary, value is not a significant predictor of social media addiction, and commitment is not a significant predictor of academic procrastination. Distance is not a significant predictor of social media addiction or academic procrastination.
... Researchers generally agree that one of the significant factors contributing to academic procrastination is a failure in self-control (Uzun et al., 2020). Individuals' levels of self-control can negatively predict the frequency of their procrastination behaviors (Przpiorka et al., 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigates the relationship between academic procrastination, self-control, time management, and academic self-efficacy among vocational college students. Grounded in self-control theory, the study utilized convenience sampling to conduct an online survey of 994 students from vocational colleges in Henan Province, China. Descriptive analysis, correlation analysis, and hierarchical regression analysis were performed on the data using SPSS. The data revealed that self-control has a negative and significant impact on academic procrastination; time management plays a partial mediating role between self-control and academic procrastination; academic self-efficacy can play a partial mediating role between self-control and academic procrastination; time management and academic self-efficacy play a chain mediating role between self-control and academic procrastination.
... These students tend to justify this conduct by the poor time planning and lack of interest generated by academic tasks and activities (Klassen et al., 2008). Procrastination behaviour in university students may be exacerbated by different factors such as perceived competence (Haghbin et al., 2012), fear of failure (Zhang et al., 2018), lack of self-control (Uzun et al., 2020), depression (Kinik & Odaci, 2020), low self-esteem (Duru & Balkis, 2014) or anxiety (Carden et al., 2004). In addition, procrastination hinders goal achievement and is associated with feelings of guilt, shame, and stress processes (Myrick, 2015), as well as poorer academic performance (Kim & Seo, 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Academic procrastination is a behaviour that delays the completion of tasks, time management or other academic activities. However, it is possible to develop interventions that eliminate or minimise this procrastinating behaviour. This study presents a systematic review of the literature following the PRISMA method guidelines, with the aim of identifying interventions for the reduction or elimination of procrastinating behaviours in university students. A search was conducted for records published in peer-reviewed scientific journals between 2010 and 2022. Three electronic databases were consulted. A total of ten studies that met the inclusion criteria were analysed. This review shows the main features of the included studies, with special emphasis on the changes perceived depending on the intervention modes applied: face-to-face, online and hybrid. The results revealed that face-to-face interventions showed high levels of changes perceived, as participants presented improved time management, task planning, awareness and reduced procrastination behaviour. However, although the hybrid and online interventions showed some changes in terms of the competencies identified, they did not report changes perceived in terms of changes in procrastination behaviour.
... This inclination toward postponement is often accompanied by subjective distress and is not merely a matter of low responsibility and time management but represents a genuine issue of self-regulation on cognitive, emotional, and behavioral levels (Klingsieck, 2013). The propensity to procrastinate can be exacerbated by factors such as perceived self-competence (Haghbin et al., 2012), low self-control (Uzun et al., 2020), fear of failure (Zhang et al., 2018), depression (Kınık and Odacı, 2020), low self-esteem (Hajloo, 2014), or anxiety (Spada et al., 2006), among others. (O'Connor and Paunonen, 2007;Vedel, 2014;Lamas, 2015;Sorić et al., 2017;Stajkovic et al., 2018;Hidalgo-Fuentes et al., 2021), consistently found that the factor of conscientiousness (known as tenacity in the HFM) is frequently associated with academic performance and serves as a powerful, significant predictor thereof. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction The High Five Model (HFM) categorizes five positive human characteristics-erudition, peace, joviality, honesty, and tenacity-utilizing an inductive psycholexic approach. This study examines the predictive power of HFM on academic performance among university students, hypothesizing that it surpasses conventional predictors such as academic motivation, exam anxiety, and academic procrastination. Methods A non-experimental cross-sectional correlational design was implemented using a non-probabilistic convenience sample of 1,007 Ecuadorian university students (403 females). Self-reported measures of the “high factors,” academic motivation, exam anxiety, and academic procrastination were collected. Linear regression analysis was utilized to evaluate the predictive capacity of the HFM on academic performance. Results The analysis revealed that the high factors of the HFM significantly predict academic performance, demonstrating a stronger predictive ability than traditional psychological predictors. Discussion The findings suggest that incorporating the HFM into academic settings could enhance understanding and prediction of student performance. This could potentially inform targeted interventions that leverage these high factors, thereby fostering better academic outcomes. Further research could explore the integration of the HFM with other educational strategies and its applicability across diverse educational contexts.
... This inclination toward postponement is often accompanied by subjective distress and is not merely a matter of low responsibility and time management but represents a genuine issue of self-regulation on cognitive, emotional, and behavioral levels (Klingsieck, 2013). The propensity to procrastinate can be exacerbated by factors such as perceived self-competence (Haghbin et al., 2012), low self-control (Uzun et al., 2020), fear of failure (Zhang et al., 2018), depression (Kınık and Odacı, 2020), low self-esteem (Hajloo, 2014), or anxiety (Spada et al., 2006), among others. (O'Connor and Paunonen, 2007;Vedel, 2014;Lamas, 2015;Sorić et al., 2017;Stajkovic et al., 2018;Hidalgo-Fuentes et al., 2021), consistently found that the factor of conscientiousness (known as tenacity in the HFM) is frequently associated with academic performance and serves as a powerful, significant predictor thereof. ...
Article
Full-text available
The High Five Model (HFM) categorizes five positive human characteristics-erudition, peace, joviality, honesty, and tenacity-utilizing an inductive psycholexic approach. This study examines the predictive power of HFM on academic performance among university students, hypothesizing that it surpasses conventional predictors such as academic motivation, exam anxiety, and academic procrastination.
Article
Dalam dunia pendidikan, penundaan akademik masih menjadi masalah yang lazim terjadi, terutama di kalangan siswa sekolah menengah. Mengatasi hal ini, penelitian ini menyelidiki korelasi antara penundaan akademis dan kontrol diri. Dengan menggunakan pendekatan korelasional kuantitatif, data dikumpulkan dari 327 siswa di Sekolah Menengah Kejuruan Negeri 1 Krian, yang dipilih melalui tabel Krejcie-Morgan dan pengambilan sampel secara tidak sengaja. Dengan menggunakan instrumen psikologis yang sudah ada, skala kontrol diri dan pengukuran penundaan akademis digunakan, yang menunjukkan reliabilitas masing-masing sebesar 0,823 dan 0,935. Temuan mengungkapkan hubungan negatif yang signifikan antara kontrol diri dan penundaan akademis (r= -.189, sig= .001), yang menunjukkan bahwa tingkat kontrol diri yang lebih tinggi dikaitkan dengan penurunan penundaan akademis. Penelitian ini menggarisbawahi pentingnya mengembangkan kemampuan kontrol diri untuk mengurangi kecenderungan penundaan akademik di kalangan siswa sekolah menengah, sehingga memberikan wawasan yang berharga bagi para praktisi pendidikan dan pembuat kebijakan.
Article
Bu araştırma üniversite öğrencilerinin akademik ertelemelerinin, ebeveyne bağlanmalarının ve lise döneminde kariyer danışmanlığı almalarının, kariyer uyumlarını ve kariyer iyimserliklerini anlamlı bir biçimde yordayıp yordamadığını incelemeyi amaçlayan betimsel tarama araştırmasıdır. Araştırmanın katılımcıları, 399 üniversite öğrencisidir. Araştırmada gerekli verileri elde etmek için “Kariyer Uyumu ve İyimserliği Ölçeği”, “Akademik Erteleme Ölçeği”, “Ebeveyn ve Arkadaşa Bağlanma Ölçeği” ile “Kişisel Bilgi Formu” kullanılmıştır. Verilerin analizinde, Doğrusal Regresyon Analizi yapılmıştır. Araştırmada, anlamlılık düzeyi .05 olarak alınmıştır.Araştırmanın sonuçlarına göre, üniversite öğrencilerinin akademik ertelemelerinin, ebeveyne bağlanmalarının ve lise döneminde kariyer danışmanlığı almalarının, kariyer uyumlarını ve kariyer iyimserliklerini anlamlı bir düzeyde yordadığı bulunmuştur. Araştırmanın bulguları, ilgili alanyazın ışığında tartışılmış ve öneriler sunulmuştur.
Article
Full-text available
There is ample theoretical and empirical support for the idea that both conscious and unconscious volitional processes, predecisional and postdecisional aspects, and the aspect of having or not having self-access play a role in procrastination episodes. The unconscious volitional processes, predecisional aspects, and the aspect of self-access are yet rather under-researched. If they are considered, however, procrastination can be understood on an even deeper level and treated more sustainably. Moreover, considering these aspects opens up a humanistic perspective on procrastination episodes, thereby, putting procrastination and procrastination interventions in the bigger picture of self-actualization, self-alienation, and self-growth.
Article
Student academic procrastination has been established as a strong predictor of several unfavorable consequences such as poor academic performance, low academic self-efficacy, and negative emotional experience. Increasing knowledge on what variables affect academic procrastination can assist nurturers and educators in minimizing student academic procrastination in the primary school setting. Based on the temporal decision model, this study examined the link between self-control and academic procrastination via the mediational role of task aversiveness and outcome value. The model was tested using structural equation modeling and cross-sectional data from 3028 primary school students (50.8% boys) in grades 3–6. The results showed a positive association between the impulse system and academic procrastination, and a negative association between the control system and academic procrastination among primary school students. Moreover, both task aversiveness and outcome value were found to mediate the link between self-control and academic procrastination among primary school students. These findings suggest that encouraging and guiding students to think more about the positive outcome and less about the negative engagement of tasks may be an effective strategy to reduce their academic procrastination.
Article
Full-text available
Claims that attributions and their related behaviors may reflect a type of perceived control that is generally overlooked. People attempt to gain control by bringing the environment into line with their wishes (primary control) and by bringing themselves into line with environmental forces (secondary control). Four manifestations of secondary control are considered: (a) Attributions to severely limited ability can serve to enhance predictive control and protect against disappointment; (b) attributions to chance can reflect illusory control, since people often construe chance as a personal characteristic akin to an ability ("luck"); (c) attributions to powerful others permit vicarious control when the individual identifies with these others; and (d) the preceding attributions may foster interpretive control, in which the individual seeks to understand and derive meaning from otherwise uncontrollable events in order to accept them. (5½ p ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed at investigating the intercorrelation among affective, cognitive and behavioural components on procrastination. A total of 402 undergraduate students (115 males, 286 females, 1 not indicated) participated in the study. Students completed the General Procrastination Scale, Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale, Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale and Self-regulation Inventory. A constructed hypothesised model was tested by utilising path analysis. Overall, the analysis indicated that the model sufficiently explained the data. The findings showed the multicorrelation among the variables, ranging from 14 to 58. Squared multiple correlation coefficients (R2) of procrastination revealed that endogenous and mediator variables explained 33% of the total variance in procrastination. The results demonstrated the importance of affect, cognition and behaviour on problematic delay referred to as procrastination.
Article
We investigated the mediating role of global self-esteem in the relationship between general self-efficacy and general procrastination among a sample of 304 Chinese undergraduate students. An online survey method was employed for data collection and willing participants completed an online survey consisting of the General Self-Efficacy Scale, the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale, and the Aitken Procrastination Inventory. The results showed that procrastination was negatively related to self-efficacy and self-esteem, and that self-efficacy was positively correlated with self-esteem. Mediation analysis revealed that self-esteem completely mediated the effect of self-efficacy on procrastination. Thus, we suggest that general self-efficacy decreases general procrastination because high self-efficacy fosters high self-esteem. As a result, it is necessary for preventive therapy in the context of procrastination to be focused on the enhancement of self-efficacy in order to cultivate a sense of self-worth in Chinese undergraduate student procrastinators.