ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Based on the continuity hypothesis of dreaming with waking life, we have studied the effects of isolation on the dreams of Italian persons, during the time of the COVID-19 pandemic. The present study included a sample of 796 subjects (73.2% women; ages 18–79 years, M = 30.3, SD = 12.8). Participants were asked to complete a dream questionnaire, as well as to report their most recent dream, by responding to specific questions related to the content of their dream (e.g., realism/bizarreness, positive vs. negative emotions, emotional intensity and tone). Results indicate the following: Female participants were higher recallers than men and reported higher emotional intensity and a predominantly negative emotional tone of their dreams, as well as higher negative emotions and sensory impressions in their most recent dreams; 159 dreams (20%) included explicit COVID-19 references; participants knowing people affected by or who have died of COVID-19 report higher emotional intensity and sensory impressions in their most recent dreams; and the most recent dreams have been set in external locations and have presented strong negative emotions, especially with respect to dangerous, violent, and frustrating situations. In sum, the findings of this study indicate that the method of quali-quantitative dream content analysis is a very informative approach for studying the effects of significant contextual and catastrophic events, such as COVID-19 pandemic, on people’s inner lives.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Dreaming in the Time of COVID-19: A Quali-
Quantitative Italian Study
Ilaria Iorio, Massimiliano Sommantico, and Santa Parrello
University of Naples Federico II
Based on the continuity hypothesis of dreaming with waking life, we have
studied the effects of isolation on the dreams of Italian persons, during the time of the
COVID-19 pandemic. The present study included a sample of 796 subjects (73.2%
women; ages 18 –79 years, M 30.3, SD 12.8). Participants were asked to complete
a dream questionnaire, as well as to report their most recent dream, by responding to
specific questions related to the content of their dream (e.g., realism/bizarreness,
positive vs. negative emotions, emotional intensity and tone). Results indicate the
following: Female participants were higher recallers than men and reported higher
emotional intensity and a predominantly negative emotional tone of their dreams, as
well as higher negative emotions and sensory impressions in their most recent dreams;
159 dreams (20%) included explicit COVID-19 references; participants knowing
people affected by or who have died of COVID-19 report higher emotional intensity
and sensory impressions in their most recent dreams; and the most recent dreams
have been set in external locations and have presented strong negative emotions,
especially with respect to dangerous, violent, and frustrating situations. In sum, the
findings of this study indicate that the method of quali-quantitative dream content
analysis is a very informative approach for studying the effects of significant
contextual and catastrophic events, such as COVID-19 pandemic, on people’s inner
lives.
Keywords: dreams, COVID-19 pandemic, isolation, negative emotions, traumatic events
In January 2020, the Chinese Center for Disease Control and Prevention
reported that a new coronavirus (SARS-CoV-2) was identified as the causative
agent of the respiratory disease later called COVID-19. In March 2020, the World
Health Organization declared the spread of COVID-19 no longer an epidemic
XIlaria Iorio, XMassimiliano Sommantico, and XSanta Parrello, Department of Humanities,
University of Naples Federico II.
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. The study complied with the American
Psychological Association ethical standards in the treatment of human research participants and conformed
to the provisions of the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments. Informed consent was obtained
from all individual participants included in the study. All authors contributed in the same way to conceive the
study and its design, to draft the manuscript, and to interpret the data. All authors read and approved the
final manuscript.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Massimiliano Sommantico,
Department of Humanities, University of Naples Federico II, via Porta di Massa 1, 80133 Naples, Italy.
E-mail: sommanti@unina.it
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
199
Dreaming
© 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 30, No. 3, 199–215
ISSN: 1053-0797 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/drm0000142
confined to certain geographical areas, but a pandemic spread all over the planet.
Since then, the data regarding contagions (and deaths) in the world have been
steadily increasing, affecting 208 countries or regions. Italy is one of the European
countries most affected by the pandemic, with 218,268 confirmed cases and 30,395
deaths (the data at the time of writing).
COVID-19 contagion occurs from person to person with ease, and even people
with few symptoms or without symptoms can be contagious. For this reason, the
main defense against contagion, in the absence of vaccines, is social distancing. To
face the pandemic, the Italian government extended restrictive measures initially
adopted only for the regions identified as “red zones” (localities that sustained
community transmission) throughout the whole country. These measures, in
addition to very strict quarantine measures for infected people and for those who
have come into contact with them, include, among others: (a) the prohibition of all
forms of gathering people in public places; (b) the suspension of all sporting
activities in public or private locations; (c) the suspension of teaching activities of
any order and degree, with online teaching preferred; (d) the closure of museums;
(e) the suspension of commercial retail activities, except for the sale of food and
basic necessities; (f) the suspension of all catering activities (including bars, pubs,
restaurants, ice cream parlors, pastry shops); (g) the suspension of activities relating
to personal services (including hairdressers, barbers, beauticians); and (h) a
maximum use of smart-working methods. At the end of March, the Italian
government decided to suspend the majority of industrial and commercial produc-
tion and to prohibit all persons from leaving home, except for proven work needs,
emergency, or for health reasons. In these cases, a self-certification attesting the
reasons for moving from home was required, and numerous checks were carried out
by the police to verify its truthfulness. These measures were still in force during the
present study.
Such a drastic form of social isolation has never been seen historically. All
citizens, of all ages and social conditions, were asked to drastically change their
daily lives, interrupting relationships and activities, and staying at home. Only those
who carry out essential work remained exposed to danger, especially doctors and
nurses, who have in some cases died. A general climate of concern, therefore,
quickly sprang up, linked both to the fear of the disease and to the fear of the
economic consequences of inactivity. It is, therefore, not excessive to define this
period as traumatic, during which mental health services recorded both a
worsening of the symptoms of various people with past problems and a
generalized spread of anxiety and depression (Wang, Zhou, & Zong, 2020;
Zandifar & Badrfam, 2020).
Indeed, recent studies (Brooks et al., 2020;Pappa et al., 2020) have highlighted
that the quarantine–isolation related to the COVID-19 pandemic is associated with
increased psychological distress (especially in terms of symptoms of posttraumatic
stress disorder), anxiety, and depression. Furthermore, several studies (Altena et
al., 2020;Cellini, Canale, Mioni, & Costa, 2020;Huang & Zhao, 2020;Sher, 2020)
have especially focused on sleep disorders or poor sleep quality related to the
consequences of quarantine–isolation, such as, between others, changes in sleep
timing, the smart working, as well as the lack of rest and regular physical exercise.
In particular, Xiao, Zhang, Kong, Li, and Yang (2020) have demonstrated a positive
association between perceived reduction of the social capital (a sum of social trust,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
200 IORIO, SOMMANTICO, AND PARRELLO
belonging, and participation) during the quarantine–isolation, higher stress, higher
anxiety, and poorer sleep quality.
Based on the continuity hypothesis of dreaming, which states that dream
content reflects waking life (Domhoff, 1996;Schredl & Piel, 2006), it is possible to
think that the subjective impact of catastrophic life events such as the COVID-19
pandemic could be reflected in the dream content of people subject to isolation
measures. Indeed, findings in the literature of dream studies, as related to war
conflicts, to catastrophic and/or traumatic events (Barrett, 2001;Hartmann &
Basile, 2003;Rosen, Reynolds, Yeager, Houck, & Hurwitz, 1991;Schredl & Piel,
2006), as well as to waking concerns or threats (Bradshaw, Lafrenière, Amini,
Lortie-Lussier, & De Koninck, 2016;Davidson & Lynch, 2012;Hartmann, 2011;
Mathes & Schredl, 2016), show that these events have a strong impact on dreams
due to their emotional salience, thus influencing dream content.
In a similar way, according to classical psychoanalytic thinking (Adams-Silvan
& Silvan, 1990;Freud, 1900,1915–1917;Sommantico, 2016,2018;Sommantico, De
Rosa, & Parrello, 2017;Velotti & Zavattini, 2019), dreams can be interpreted, not
only as an attempt at the fulfillment of repressed wishes but also as a way to
retrospectively master and transform a waking life traumatic event. And so,
following this line of thought, it seems that significant events in waking life can be
associated with specific experiences of dreaming, especially with dreamt emotions
and threats.
The Present Study: Aims and Hypotheses
The main aim of the present study is to analyze the content of dreams in the
time of COVID-19. In accordance with the continuity hypothesis of dreaming,
with respect to catastrophic and traumatic events (Barrett, 2001;Hartmann &
Basile, 2003;Rosen et al., 1991;Schredl & Piel, 2006), we hypothesized that the
dreams of people living with the isolation measures related to the COVID-19
pandemic are mainly realistic and are characterized by strong negative emo-
tional intensity, a strong emotional tone, and the presence of sensory impres-
sions.
Furthermore, we were interested in understanding if, and how, the danger of
contagion and the quarantine–isolation measures were represented in dreams. We
then hypothesize that most recent dreams are set in locations external to the places
where the quarantine–isolation is lived out. Above all, negative emotions, such as
anxiety and fear, will emerge, related to dangerous situations.
Finally, based on previous dream research (Nielsen et al., 2000;Schredl,
2002;Schredl & Reinhard, 2008;Settineri, Frisone, Alibrandi, & Merlo, 2019),
we hypothesized that women recall their dreams more often, and they also
evaluate their dreams as more emotionally intense, than men do. In particular,
studies and meta-analyses (Schredl, 2010b;Schredl & Lahl, 2010) not only
indicated that women tend to recall their dreams more often than men but also
tried to identify what factors might explain this difference, such as frequency of
nocturnal awakenings, interest in dreams, neuroticism, and sex role orientation.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
DREAMING IN THE TIME OF COVID-19 201
Method
Research Instruments
Sociodemographic Questionnaire. Respondents provided sociodemographic
data (e.g., age, gender, region of residence, level of education, and profession) via
a basic Sociodemographic Questionnaire. Respondents were also asked to report
information about their quarantine (e.g., number of people, dimensions of their
house, availability of a private room), and whether they know people affected by or
who have died of COVID-19.
Dream Questionnaire. Dream frequency recall was measured on a 7-point
scale (0 never;1less than once a month;2about once a month;3about
two to three times a month;4about once a week;5several times a week;6
almost every morning;Schredl, 2002,2010a;Settineri et al., 2019). Following the
indications of Schredl (2002,2010a), participants were also asked to self-rate their
dreams’ realism (on a 3-point scale: 0 nonrealistic dreams;1dreams are
sometimes realistic;2dreams are often realistic), the creative aspect of their
dreams (on a 5-point scale, ranging from 1 no creative aspects to 5 many
creative aspects), the emotional intensity of their dreams (on a 5-point scale, ranging
from 1 not intense to 5 very intense), and the overall emotional tone of their
dreams (on a 3-point scale: 1predominantly negative;0balanced,1
predominantly positive).
Regarding the instructions for reporting “The Most Recent Dream,” we
followed the indications set out by Hall and Van de Castle (1966), later modified by
Domhoff (1996,2003). We asked participants to indicate (a) whether the dream
occurred last night, last week, or last month; (b) the date the dream occurred; and
(c) what time of the day the dream was recalled. Participants were also asked to (a)
describe the dream exactly and as fully as they could remember it; (b) describe the
setting of the dream, whether it was familiar or not; (c) describe the people and
their sex, age, and relationship to the dreamer; (d) describe any animals or objects
that appeared in the dream; (e) describe their feelings during the dream and
whether they were pleasant or unpleasant; and (f) tell exactly what happened
during the dream to themselves and the other characters. Participants were also
asked to self-rate their dream (Domhoff, 1996;Schredl, 2002,2010a) regarding (a)
the intensity of the positive emotions of the dream (on a 4-point scale, ranging from
0no emotions to 3 intense emotions); (b) the intensity of the negative emotions
of the dream (on a 4-point scale, ranging from 0 no emotions to 3 intense
emotions); (c) realism/bizarreness of the dream (on a 4-point scale, ranging from
0realistic dream to3bizarre dream); and (d) the presence of sensory
impressions in the dream (on a 4-point scale, ranging from 0 no sensory
impressions to 3 strong sensory impressions).
Dream Content Analysis
As stated by Schredl (2010a), the crucial aim of content analysis is to quantify
“particular aspects of the verbal material [. . .] for differentiating between findings
which are due to chance and findings which might reflect ‘real’ differences in the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
202 IORIO, SOMMANTICO, AND PARRELLO
population” (p. 65). Despite the fact that dream content analysis has several
shortcomings (such as the loss of information and the impossibility to capture the
uniqueness of people’s dreams), one of the most important issue is related to the
use of the validity of dream content scales, as well as the presence of independent
judges analyzing dream emotions (Hoffman, 2013;Schredl, 2002;Schredl, 2010a;
Schredl & Doll, 1998).
For the present quantitative analyses, the rating scales used in the study were
the same as those used for The Most Recent Dream. This was done to compare
participants’ self-ratings and judges’ ratings of the following: (a) the intensity of
the positive emotions; (b) the intensity of the negative emotions; (c) realism/
bizarreness; and (d) the presence of sensory impressions. All scales used the same
format as the self-rating scales in the Dream Questionnaire. The interrater
reliability coefficients (Spearman rank correlations) for these scales were high and
ranged from .69 (realism/bizarreness) to .81 (negative emotions).
For the qualitative analyses (Richards, 2015), the Grounded Theory Model
(Glaser & Strauss, 1967) was used to explore the presence of common aspects
in participants’ dreams, analyzed by three independent researchers. Grounded
theory “is an inductive, theory discovery methodology that allows the researcher to
develop a theoretical account of the general features of a topic while simultane-
ously grounding the account in empirical observations or data” (Martin & Turner,
1986, p. 141). Beyond the researcher’s need to come up with preliminary hypoth-
eses, which provides greater freedom to explore the research area and which allows
for issues to emerge, the grounded theory approach offers several other benefits.
These include (a) rigorous insight into areas that are relatively unknown by the
researchers; (b) capacity to interpret complex phenomena; (c) accommodation of
social issues; (d) appropriateness for socially constructed experiences; and (e) no
constraints of a priori knowledge.
The content analyses were oriented around specific questions. The search for
recurring emotional valence and symbolic elements was linked to (a) elements of
the traumatic situation people are experiencing; (b) aspects related to the condition
of quarantine–isolation; and (c) aspects related to “escape” from the quaranti-
ne–isolation situation.
Data Analyses
Survey data were then entered into SPSS 23.0 (IBM Corp, 2015) for the
quantitative analyses and QRS NVivo 11 (Bazeley & Richards, 2000;Richards,
1999) for the quali-quantitative analyses. Data were checked/verified by project
staff for accuracy, to maintain the researcher’s irreplaceable meaning-making
process. Ordinal logistic regression models were used for ordinal outcome variables
(i.e., dream recall frequency, etc.; pvalue .05).
Procedure and Participants
Participants were recruited in Italy via the Internet from April 2020 to May
2020, according to the following criteria: more than 18 years of age and compliant
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
DREAMING IN THE TIME OF COVID-19 203
with the quarantine measures. All data were collected through self-report ques-
tionnaires, written or translated into the Italian language, using an Internet-based
survey (Hewson, Vogel, & Laurent, 2016). Participation in the study was voluntary,
anonymous, and unpaid. All participants included in the study gave consent to
participate on the first page of the survey. The informed consent included detailed
information about the aims and procedures of the study, confidentiality, and the
anonymity of the responses. After signing the informed consent, participants first
completed the Sociodemographic Questionnaire. Participants then completed the
Dream Questionnaire and reported The Most Recent Dream. Means across each
participant’s reported dreams were calculated for word count. Dream reports were
arranged and scored on the dream rating scales by three independent judges.
The study complied with the American Psychological Association ethical
standards in the treatment of human research participants and conformed to the
provisions of the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments. Further-
more, the study was approved by the Ethical Committee of Psychological Research
of the Department of Humanities of the University of Naples Federico II (protocol
no. 14/2020).
There were 890 respondents, but only 796 (73.2% women; ages 18–79 years,
M30.3, SD 12.8) reported their most recent dream. Half of the participants
were students (50.1%). Participants living in the center or in the suburbs of a big
city were 48.8%. The majority of participants (40.2%) were in a relationship but
lived alone (32.8% were single, 23.9% married or cohabiting, 2.5% were divorced,
and 0.6% were widows). Participants living with family during the quarantine
period were 82.3% (10.3% in couple, 5.3% alone, and 2.1% with friends). The
mean number of people cohabiting during the quarantine period was 3.5 (SD
1.3). The majority of participants (51.5%) live in a house that is between 80 and 140
m
2
, with their own room available (75.4%), and many had animals living with them
(38.4% had dogs and/or cats). The sample had a high level of education, with 52.4%
of the participants having completed secondary school and 41.4% having completed
a university degree or a postuniversity degree. Participants who knew someone
infected by COVID-19 were 29.5%, and participants who knew someone who died
of COVID-19 were 8.4%.
Results
Dream Recall Frequency and Content Measures
Regarding the dream recall frequency, the sample was divided, according to
the recommendations of Schredl (2002), into three categories: (a) low recallers
never to about once a month, 17.3%; (b) medium recallers about two to three
times a month to about once a week, 34.9%; and (c) high recallers several times
a week to almost every morning, 47.7%. Regarding realism/bizarreness, partici-
pants reported dreams, on average, sometimes realistic (M1.2; SD 0.7).
Regarding the creative aspect, participants reported medium creative content (M
2.9; SD 1.2) of their dreams. Regarding the emotional intensity, participants
reported a high emotional intensity (M3.9; SD 1.0) of their dreams. Finally,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
204 IORIO, SOMMANTICO, AND PARRELLO
regarding the emotional tone, participants reported a mainly balanced emotional
tone in their dreams (M⫽⫺0.2; SD 0.6; see Table 1).
The dependent ordinal variables (dream recall frequency, realism/bizarreness,
creative aspects, emotional intensity, and emotional tone) were compared with
gender as an independent variable, with age and level of education controlled.
Results are shown in Table 2.
In line with previous studies, significant causal relationships were found with
dream recall frequency, emotional intensity, and emotional tone, thus indicating
that female participants are higher recallers than men, as well as report higher
emotional intensity and a predominantly negative emotional tone of their dreams.
No significant causal relationships were found for realism/bizarreness and creative
aspects.
Most Recent Dream Quali-Quantitative Analysis
In reporting the Most Recent Dream, the majority of participants (55.9%)
indicated that the dream occurred during the last week (25.6% last night and 18.5%
last month) and that they mainly recalled the dream during the morning (83.9%).
The mean dream length was 134.0 words (SD 136.9).
Regarding the intensity of emotions, participants reported medium positive
emotions (M1.5; SD 1.1) and high negative emotions (M1.9; SD 1.1).
Regarding realism/bizarreness, participants reported medium realism (M1.6;
SD 0.9). Finally, regarding the presence of sensory impressions, participants
reported a high presence of sensory impressions (M1.8; SD 1.0; see Table 3).
Zero-order correlations between participants’ self-ratings and judges’ mean
ratings are shown in Table 4. Results showed medium to large significant positive
associations, with rvalues (p.01) ranging from .52 to .66.
The dependent ordinal variables (positive emotions, negative emotions,
realism/bizarreness, and sensory impressions) were compared with gender, the
COVID-19 variables (knowing a COVID-19 case or death), and the time when the
most recent dream occurred (last night, last week, last month) as independent
variables, with age and level of education controlled. Results are shown in Table 5.
Regarding the effect of gender, and in line with previous studies, significant
causal relationships were found with positive emotions, negative emotions, and
Table 1
Dream Recall Frequency and Content
Dream variables
Females Males Total sample
N583 (% or M,SD)N213 (% or M,SD)N796 (% or M,SD)
Low recallers 14.1 26.3 17.3
Medium recallers 35.2 34.3 34.9
High recallers 50.8 39.4 47.7
Realism/bizarreness 1.2, 0.7 1.1, 0.7 1.2, 0.7
Creative aspects 2.9, 1.2 2.9, 1.2 2.9, 1.2
Emotional intensity 3.9, 0.9 3.6, 1.0 3.9, 1.0
Emotional tone 0.2, 0.6 0.1, 0.6 0.2, 0.6
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
DREAMING IN THE TIME OF COVID-19 205
sensory impressions, thus indicating that female participants report higher emo-
tional intensity and sensory impressions in their most recent dreams. Regarding the
effect of knowing a COVID-19 case or death, significant causal relationships were
found with positive emotions, negative emotions, and sensory impressions, thus
indicating that participants knowing people affected by or who have died of
COVID-19 report higher emotional intensity and sensory impressions in their most
recent dreams. Finally, regarding the effect of the time when the most recent dream
occurred, significant causal relationships were found with positive emotions, thus
indicating that participants whose most recent dreams occurred last week reported
higher positive emotions. No significant causal relationships were found for
realism/bizarreness.
With the support of NVivo, three independent judges classified 5,738 refer-
ences, in a textual corpus consisting of 796 dreams (1,06,727 words). Each reference
was placed in one or more nodes: (a) main nodes, or starting macrocategories,
which corresponded to the structural elements of the dream sought by the judges
(settings, characters, actions, etc.); (b) subnodes that emerged from the exploration
of the text (see Tables 6 and 7).
Node 1. Settings. The dream settings are internal (home,prison,madhouse,
hospital, etc.) in 319 references (37%), but mostly external (543 references, 63%):
streets,squares,beaches,swimming pools,shopping centers,fairs,bars, and so forth.
Both the houses, which are often crowded and with many windows, and the
external places are sometimes familiar, sometimes “strange.” Here are some
examples of excerpts from dreams: “It was a kind of stilt house, beautiful super
Table 2
Regression Analyses for Dream Variables
Dream variables
Effect of gender
␤␹
2
p
Dream recall frequency .045 36.57 .000
*
Realism/bizarreness .003 2.41 .300
Creative aspects .007 5.63 .060
Emotional intensity .027 22.05 .000
*
Emotional tone .015 12.39 .002
*
Note. ␤⫽standardized estimates.
*
p.05.
Table 3
Most Recent Dreams’ Content
Dream variables
Females Males Total sample
N583 (% or M,SD)N213 (% or M,SD)N796 (% or M,SD)
Dream length 138.4, 134.5 122.1, 142.7 134.0, 136.9
Last night 26.4 23.5 25.6
Last week 56.9 53.1 55.9
Last month 16.6 23.5 18.5
Positive emotions 1.4, 1.1 1.7, 0.9 1.5, 1.1
Negative emotions 2.0, 1.1 1.7, 1.1 1.9, 1.1
Realism/bizarreness 1.5, 1.0 1.6, 1.0 1.6, 0.9
Sensory impressions 1.8, 1.0 1.7, 1.0 1.8, 1.0
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
206 IORIO, SOMMANTICO, AND PARRELLO
elegant and full of windows”; “I was locked up in a madhouse and there was a
woman chasing me with the intent to kill me”; “I was with someone at a fair, but
I don’t remember who he is, however I get lost among the stalls scrupulously
looking at every object and there were a lot of people and I struggled to breathe
and I felt oppressed”; “I was at the bar in the city where we happen to go with one
of my closest friends and my brother”; “We went to the sea, to the swimming pool
and in the evening, we went out to the town.”
Node 2. Characters. Apart from the dreamer,family members,friends, and
colleagues who belong to the daily life of the present appear in the dreams (713
references, 52.9%). So do many people from the past, that is, people with whom we
have not had relationships for a long time (244 references, 16.1%): ex-boyfriends,
old school friends, and loved ones who died. Finally, unknown and crowd appear in
391 references (29%). Here are some examples: “My girlfriend and I arrive in a
tourist village surrounded by nature: during the trip our bags were lost”; “I was with
my colleagues in the workplace, very busy but calm”; “I was at my house, I don’t
remember if the current one or not, my parents quarreled, and I had to decide who
was right”; “I was speaking at a village festival with a friend of mine with whom I
have lost relationships lately and they talked about our estrangement. Then I also
saw an ex-boyfriend of mine in the dream, but I don’t remember what we said”; “I
dreamt of being at my house with my grandmother, who died about 3 years ago. She
asked me to turn on the stove because she was cold, and I tried”; “There was the
eruption of Vesuvius. Many people ran towards a kind of bunker. Instead, my
family proceeded to the volcano.”
Table 4
Zero-Order Correlations Between Participants’ Self-Ratings and Judges’ Ratings (N 796)
Rating scales Positive
emotions J Negative
emotions J Realism/
bizarreness J Sensory
impressions J
Positive emotions P .56
*
Negative emotions P .66
*
Realism/bizarreness P .54
*
Sensory impressions P .52
*
Note.Pparticipants; J judges.
*
p.01.
Table 5
Regression Analyses for Most Recent Dream Variables
Dream variables
Effect of
gender
Effect of
knowing a
COVID-19 case
Effect of
knowing a
COVID-19 death
Time when
the dream
occurred
␤␹
2
p␤␹
2
p␤␹
2
p␤␹
2
p
Positive emotions .019 15.28 .000
*
.012 9.88 .007
*
.013 9.81 .007
*
.018 14.42 .002
*
Negative emotions .026 20.96 .000
*
.013 10.40 .006
*
.011 9.07 .011
*
.008 6.61 .086
Realism/Bizarreness .003 2.07 .355 .003 2.12 .347 .003 2.59 .274 .004 2.96 .398
Sensory impressions .009 7.36 .025
*
.008 6.41 .040
*
.009 7.46 .024
*
.010 7.81 .051
Note. ␤⫽standardized estimates.
*
p.05.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
DREAMING IN THE TIME OF COVID-19 207
Node 3. Animals. Only in a small number of references (113) do animals
appear. In 70.8% of the cases, they are pets (80 references), dogs and cats; in 29.2%
of cases, they are other kind of animals (33 references). Sometimes they are
dangerous, and sometimes they are themselves in need of help. Here are some
examples: “A dog enters and bite me. I’m frightened and I try to run away even if
in itself the dog was not aggressive at all, in fact it seemed very quiet”; “We are out
of the hospital, my mother walks away, my cat is with me, dying. I hold her in my
arms”; “Suddenly a huge rat comes in and tries to attack me, I close the front door”;
“A cockroach with a single antenna walks on the wall, scares me and I seek help
from the people present”; “There is a rabbit in a cage, and I am worried and want
to free it.”
Node 4. Objects. In 783 references, objects appear: (a) generic (549 refer-
ences, 70.1%), that is, household objects, furnishings, and so forth; (b) movement
Table 6
Nodes and Subnodes of Most Recent Dreams’ Categorical Analysis (1; N 796)
Nodes Subnodes
1. Settings Internal External
862 ref. 319 ref. 543 ref.
(37%) (63%)
2. Characters Present Past Unknown/crowd
1,348 ref. 713 ref. 244 ref. 391 ref.
(52.9%) (18.1%) (29%)
3. Animals Pets Others
113 ref. 80 ref. 33 ref.
(70.8%) (29.2%)
4. Objects Generic Movement related Technological Weapons
783 ref. 549 ref. 146 ref. 60 ref. 28 ref.
(70.1%) (18.6%) (7.7%) (3.6%)
Note. ref. references. There are 5,738 analyzed references.
Table 7
Nodes and Subnodes of Most Recent Dreams’ Categorical Analysis (2; N 796)
Nodes Subnodes
5. Emotions Positive Negative
994 ref. 242 ref. 752 ref.
(24.4%) (75.6%)
Anxiety/fear Frustration Generic
410 ref. 211 ref. 131 ref.
(54.5%) (28.1%) (17.4%)
6. Actions Canonical Exceptional
1,491 ref. 799 ref. 692 ref.
(53.6%) (46.4%)
Sharing Body Movement Help/care Danger Violence/
transgression Incomplete Death
276 ref. 215 ref. 268 ref. 40 ref. 343 ref. 146 ref. 164 ref. 39 ref.
(34.5%) (27%) (33.5%) (5%) (49.6%) (21.1%) (23.7%) (5.6%)
7. COVID-19 Disease Quarantine Checks
147 ref. 43 ref. 77 ref. 27 ref.
(29.2%) (52.4%) (18.4%)
Note. ref. references. There are 5,738 analyzed references.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
208 IORIO, SOMMANTICO, AND PARRELLO
related (146 references, 18.6%), that is, stairs and means of locomotion such as cars,
motorbikes,planes, and so forth; (c) technological (60 references, 7.7%), that is,
computers,mobiles, and so forth; and (d) weapons (28 references, 3.6%), pistols,
knives,axes, and so forth. Here are some examples: “I had to take a plane to go
from Madrid to Santiago de Chile. As always, I was very afraid of flying, but also
of getting infected”; “I was on my old scooter on the ring road, even if the
surrounding landscape was very different and more desolate than the real one, with
my cousin and her friend”; “I was on a boat. I was hiking with my old high school
classmates”; “I was trying to escape from someone who wanted to kill me. He was
a person familiar to me, but I can’t remember him: He had a pistol with a silencer.”
Node 5. Emotions. Terms that explicitly refer to emotions and feelings
appear in 994 references. Positive emotions are found only in 242 references
(24.4%): serenity,joy,pleasure, and so forth. Negative emotions appear in 752
references (75.6%) and are divided into three areas: (a) anxiety/fear (410 refer-
ences, 54.5%): anxiety,anguish,fear,panic,terror, and so forth; (b) frustration (211
references, 28.1%): sense of helplessness,sense of frustration,anger,sense of guilt,
and so forth; and (c) generic negative emotions (131 references, 17.4%): negative
feelings,sadness,annoyance,bewilderment, and so forth. Here are some examples:
“I’m going to get married. Our wedding was scheduled for late May, but we had to
postpone it. I am happy”; “I dream of making love with my girlfriend for weeks, a
dream obviously with very positive and pleasant emotions”; “I was anxious because
my daughter had to hand over homework and she was failing to stay on schedule”;
“I dreamt of being in a very crowded cinema. I was starting to cover my mouth and
nose with a scarf, and I remember well the panic sensation that suddenly assailed
me, the terror of being able to become infected pervaded me”; “I remembered that
I had not brought self-certification with me and I was panicking.”
Node 6. Actions. The categorized actions number 1,491 and have been
divided into (a) canonical (799 references, 53.6%), that is, actions that are part of
the routine of daily life and (b) exceptional (692 references, 45.4%), that is,
unusual, strange.
The canonical actions are divided as follows: (a) sharing actions (276 refer-
ences, 34.5%), which refer to being together (cooking,speaking,walking,fighting,
etc.); (b) body related (215 references, 27%): sometimes in the sense of affective
intimacy (hugging,kissing,having sex, etc.), other times related to bodily percep-
tions or expressions (crying,screaming,feeling pain, etc.); (c) movement related
(268 references, 33.5%), like going out,running away,traveling, and so forth; and
(d) help/care (40 references, 5%): how to help,cure, and protect. Here are some
examples: “I went to a disco on the beach with my friends, while they took a drink
all around me it became my graduation party”; “We go to his house, I embrace him
like a 16-year-old girl, I squeeze him to hurt him almost and I wanted his kisses as
much as ever”; “I dreamt of going to the supermarket with my cousin and a friend
of mine.”
Exceptional actions have been divided into (a) dangerous actions (343
references, 49.6%), in which the dreamer or other characters are threatened by
various dangers and uncertain situations, but also wars,earthquakes,eruptions, and
so forth; (b) violence/transgression (146 references, 21.1%), in which the dreamer
witnesses or suffers robberies,assaults,betrayals, and so forth; (c) incomplete
actions (164 references, 23.7%), that is, actions that the dreamer cannot conclude
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
DREAMING IN THE TIME OF COVID-19 209
or keep under control (not controlling the car,a bike losing balance,inability in
taking an exam,cannot ask for help, etc.); and (d) death (39 references, 5.6%); in
a small percentage of references, the death of the dreamer or other characters is
told. Here are some examples: “I was in a bunker and there was war outside. I was
terrified of the sound of exploding bombs”; “I was subjected to a robbery by a
pickpocket to whom I offered my belongings”; “We were celebrating something but
after a few moments we heard shots from outside. We catapulted onto the street
and witnessed a shooting. After a few moments one of my friends was hit by a
bullet”; “We have to go back in and I’m going to get back the car that is parked next
to the sidewalk. The car is no longer there, so I try to contact the police with the
mobile, but I can’t dial the numbers”; “I feel like I’m in a maze, I can’t find the way
out. Suddenly I find myself stuck in corridors that look like those of a luxury hotel”;
“A colleague of mine came to my office and commits suicide by jumping off the
balcony. I start to be afraid of being blamed for murder and I don’t know why.”
Node 7. COVID-19. Only in 147 references do dreamers make explicit
reference to COVID-19: It is cited as a virus causing disease, contagion,infection,
and various symptoms (43 references, 52.4%), but above all as the cause of
quarantine (77 references, 52.4%), and, finally, as a cause of the checks to which
people are subjected (27 references, 18.4%), which impose the use of self-
certifications, the meeting with the police, and the risk of fines. Here are some
examples: “I dreamt that I contracted the virus and therefore had to go to a clinic
to be isolated. I was fine and I wasn’t attached to respirators or anything, the only
emotion I remember having was a sense of amazement”; “I was leaving the house
for the first time during the quarantine with my friends, but was not yet allowed to
leave”; “We were in the car in a long queue and I find that the queue was due to
the police checks that allowed only the people who had a self-certification to pass:
At that point I was taken by anxiety because I have no documents with me.”
Discussion
Beyond collecting data from the Dream Questionnaire, this study examined
the Most Recent Dreams reported by adult Italians. All dreams were coded by
three independent judges to provide a more reliable and complete picture of the
extent to which specific themes can be observed in dreams during the time of
COVID-19.
Consistent with previous research (Nielsen et al., 2000;Schredl, 2002,2010b;
Schredl & Reinhard, 2008;Settineri et al., 2019), women reported recalling dreams
more often than men. Furthermore, women reported significantly higher emotional
intensity and a predominantly negative emotional tone in their dreams. Finally,
women more frequently reported a frustrating situation with higher negative
emotions in their most recent dreams.
Participants who knew people affected by or who have died of COVID-19
reported higher emotional intensity and sensory impressions in their most recent
dreams. In light of the transformative function of dreams (Adams-Silvan & Silvan,
1990;Sommantico, 2018), we can hypothesize that knowing people affected by or
who have died of COVID-19 is a real emotionally significant experience, which
could further stimulate the need for a dream job that would manifest itself in a
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
210 IORIO, SOMMANTICO, AND PARRELLO
greater emotional intensity of dreams, as well as in a greater presence of sensory
impressions. Furthermore, these data are in line with both the continuity hypothesis
of dreaming with waking life (Barrett, 2001;Hartmann & Basile, 2003;Rosen et al.,
1991;Schredl & Piel, 2006) and the classical psychoanalytic thinking, according to
which significant events in waking life can be associated with specific experiences of
dreaming, especially with dreamt emotions and threats.
According to previous investigations (Schredl, 2002;Schredl & Doll, 1998),
dreams’ emotions (especially positive ones) are underestimated by judges when
compared with dreamers’ self-ratings. This may be due to the fact that the judges
took into account the presence of words that explicitly name emotions. Despite this
difference, the correlation coefficients between judges’ ratings and dreamers’
self-ratings remain statistically significant.
Regarding the settings of the most recent dreams, they were mainly external to
the places where the quarantine–isolation is lived (e.g., streets, beaches, bars, etc.).
On the contrary, objects (e.g., household objects, furniture, computers, mobiles,
etc.) and animals (pets) are those the people use and encounter while inside the
quarantine–isolation place. We can interpret these findings by hypothesizing that
people subject to strict quarantine–isolation measures project the desire to escape
from forced domestic life in their dreams. Particularly interesting is the recurrence
in dreams of characters with whom the dreamer had not had relationships for a long
time (ex-partners, ex-friends, dead people, etc.), who now return to ask for contact
and with whom meeting does not appear uncanny, but natural. We can perhaps
hypothesize that this is a way of representing the lack of social contact, which the
quarantine–isolation enforces as “reversible,” or as an expression of nostalgia for
the lost past.
Regarding the emotions elicited in most recent dreams, it is interesting to note
that positive emotions (e.g., serenity, pleasantness, happiness, etc.) were signifi-
cantly less intense than negative emotions (e.g., anguish, fear, panic, terror, etc.).
Furthermore, these negative emotions were mainly related to dangerous or violent
situations (in which the dreamer is involved as a spectator or as an actor), as well
as to frustrating situations, in which the dreamer fails or is prevented from
completing something. It should also be noted that there are some situations of
danger, violence, death, and the impossibility to act in which the emotion
experienced is not explicitly mentioned, as in a sort of singular “alexithymia,”
typical of traumatic processes (Di Giacinto et al., 2015;Taylor & Bagby, 2013).
Furthermore, the significant number of canonical actions that refer to relationality
and to the body also suggests how central this issue is in the condition of forced
quarantine–isolation.
Finally, regarding the themes that emerged from the qualitative analyses, we
highlighted a partial superposition with previous studies that investigated typical
themes in most recent dreams, in terms of the rank order of the themes (Mathes,
Schredl, & Göritz, 2014;Nielsen et al., 2003;Yu, 2015). Indeed, in our sample,
between the most prevalent typical dream themes, there were “being chased,
pursued, arguments,” “trying something again and again,” “sexual experiences,”
“school, teachers, studying,” and “swimming or vacationing.” Despite this similar-
ity, there was a difference in the prevalence of other themes. Indeed, in our sample,
the other most prevalent, typical themes were “being physically attacked,” “being
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
DREAMING IN THE TIME OF COVID-19 211
blamed or punished,” “being persecuted,” “spouse or lover having extramarital
relations or being unfaithful,” and “shooting or remote attacks.”
Taken together, these results can be interpreted in light of the continuity
hypothesis of dreaming (Barrett, 2001;Hartmann & Basile, 2003;Rosen et al., 1991;
Schredl & Piel, 2006), indicating that the dreams of people living under the isolation
measures related to the COVID-19 pandemic are mainly realistic and are charac-
terized by strong negative emotional intensity, strong emotional tone, the presence
of sensory impressions, and the presence of dangerous and frustrating situations.
Despite this, we cannot exclude the existence of a possible bias highlighted by
Malinowski (2016), affirming that “it may be that there is a memory bias toward
recalling dreams that are particularly troubling, since they reflect what is troubling
also in waking life, which the participants may have been ruminating upon” (p.
119).
Strengths and Limitations
To our knowledge, this is the first completed Italian study on dreaming in the
time of COVID-19. The first strength of the present study, following the indication
of Schredl (2010a), is the use of both valid and reliable self-rating and judged dream
content scales. Furthermore, specifically regarding the used rating scales, the
interrater reliability coefficients are high, as well as the confidence levels and the
effect sizes (Hoffman, 2013).
The first general limitation is related to sampling strategy. Indeed, judgmental
sampling, as well as convenience sampling, such as snowball sampling, implies
specific possible biases: for example, volunteers’ bias (related to the special
characteristics of individuals who voluntarily participate in a study; e.g., Hoffman,
2013). Furthermore, our sample was not balanced with respect to gender. Future
research could try to work with more gender-balanced samples. A final limitation
relates to the participants’ wide age range. Indeed, it is possible that, as highlighted
by different studies (Schredl, Berres, Klingauf, Schellhaas, & Göritz, 2014;Settineri
et al., 2019), some constructs, such as dream recall frequency, vary across different
life phases. Future follow-up research could analyze this issue. In sum, previous
limitations and the cross-sectional study design also limit the conclusions that can
be drawn, especially their generalizability.
Conclusions
The present study was conducted during the worst phase of the COVID-19
pandemic in Italy, when the mass media communicated every day the increasing
numbers of the sick and dead, especially among the elderly, and continually
reminded audiences of the obligation to practice social distancing, as summarized
in the expression “I stay home.”
The large number of people who responded in a short time to our request to
participate in the study (890 in 14 days) perhaps speaks to the increase in recall of
dream activity at a time when there is a push to represent something new and
unknown, or even a need to narrate and confront it.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
212 IORIO, SOMMANTICO, AND PARRELLO
In accordance with the continuity hypothesis of dreaming and with psycho-
analytic thinking, the results show how catastrophic events are reflected in dream
life both directly, through realistic dreams and explicit references to COVID-19,
and indirectly and symbolically. In particular, the element of the pandemic that the
unconscious seems to have used the most is isolation in the home, represented in
dreams as an unusual and disturbing experience, especially through denial, escape
to external places, and close encounters with others, including those with whom
people have not had contact for a long time. The transgression of the quaranti-
ne–isolation, desired and feared, is thus the basis of dreams marked by danger and
the threat not so much of the other, but of relationships: One dreams, in fact, of
being attacked and attacking, being betrayed and betraying, being accused and
accusing, witnessing death and dying. But the most specific feature of the traumatic
experience is the sense of powerlessness: that is, the feeling that you do not have the
individual resources to face reality. However, dreamwork is just one way to start
elaborating this collective catastrophic experience. Narrating it and making it
available to the scientific community is a further step.
In sum, this study highlights that an integrative experimental and clinical
perspective, including qualitative and quantitative analyses, as well as different
theoretical approaches, such as the continuity hypothesis and the psychoanalytic
framework, could contribute to a more comprehensive analysis of dreaming.
References
Adams-Silvan, A., & Silvan, M. (1990). “A dream is the fulfillment of a wish”: Traumatic dream,
repetition compulsion, and the pleasure principle. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 71,
513–522.
Altena, E., Baglioni, C., Espie, C. A., Ellis, J., Gavriloff, D., Holzinger, B.,...Riemann, D. (2020).
Dealing with sleep problems during home confinement due to the COVID-19 outbreak: Practical
recommendations from a task force of the European CBT-I Academy. Journal of Sleep Research,
29, e13052. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jsr.13052
Barrett, D. (2001). Trauma and dreams. London, UK: Harvard University Press.
Bazeley, P., & Richards, L. (2000). The NVivo qualitative project book. London, UK: Sage. http://dx
.doi.org/10.4135/9780857020079
Bradshaw, S., Lafrenière, A., Amini, R., Lortie-Lussier, M., & De Koninck, J. (2016). Threats in dreams,
emotions and the severity of threatening experiences in waking. International Journal of Dream
Research, 9, 102–109. http://dx.doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2016.2.27214
Brooks, S. K., Webster, R. K., Smith, L. E., Woodland, L., Wessely, S., Greenberg, N., & Rubin, G. J.
(2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: Rapid review of the evidence.
The Lancet, 395, 912–920. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8
Cellini, N., Canale, N., Mioni, G., & Costa, S. (2020). Changes in sleep pattern, sense of time and digital
media use during COVID-19 lockdown in Italy. Journal of Sleep Research, 15, e13074. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1111/jsr.13074
Davidson, J., & Lynch, S. (2012). Thematic, literal, and associative dream imagery following a
high-impact event. Dreaming, 22, 58–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026273
Di Giacinto, A., Lai, C., Cieri, F., Cinosi, E., Massaro, G., Angelini, V.,...diGiannantonio, M. (2015).
Difficulty describing feelings and post-traumatic symptoms after a collective trauma in survivors of
L’Aquila earthquake. Journal of Mental Health, 24, 150–154. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/09638237
.2015.1019055
Domhoff, G. W. (1996). Finding meanings in dreams. A quantitative approach. New York, NY: Springer.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-0298-6
Domhoff, G. W. (2003). The scientific study of dreams: Neural networks, cognitive development, and
content analysis. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
10463-000
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
DREAMING IN THE TIME OF COVID-19 213
Freud, S. (1900). The interpretation of dreams. In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition of the
complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vols. 4 and 5, pp. 1–628). London, UK: Hogarth
Press.
Freud, S. (1915–1917). New introductory lessons on psycho-analysis. In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.), The
standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 15, pp. 83–242).
London, UK: Hogarth Press.
Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative
research. Chicago, IL: Aldine.
Hall, C. S., & Van de Castle, R. L. (1966). The content analysis of dreams. New York, NY:
Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Hartmann, E. (2011). The nature and functions of dreaming. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Hartmann, E., & Basile, R. (2003). Dream imagery becomes more intense after 9/11/01. Dreaming, 13,
61–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1023398924124
Hewson, C., Vogel, C., & Laurent, D. (Eds.). (2016). Internet research methods (2nd ed.). London, UK:
Sage. http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781473920804
Hoffman, C. (2013). Research articles in dreaming: A review of the first 20 years. Dreaming, 23, 216–231.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032905
Huang, Y., & Zhao, N. (2020). Generalized anxiety disorder, depressive symptoms and sleep quality
during COVID-19 epidemic in China: A web-based cross-sectional survey. Psychiatry Research,
288, 112954. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112954
IBM Corp. (2015). IBM SPSS statistics for windows, Version 23.0 [Computer software]. Armonk, NY:
Author.
Malinowski, J. E. (2016). Themes in participants’ understandings of meaning in their most recent dream:
Worries, relationships, and symbolism. International Journal of Dream Research, 9, 115–123.
http://dx.doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2016.2.29405
Martin, P. Y., & Turner, B. A. (1986). Grounded theory and organizational research. Journal of Applied
Behavioral Science, 22, 141–157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002188638602200207
Mathes, J., & Schredl, M. (2016). Threats in dreams: Are they related to waking-life? International
Journal of Dream Research, 9, 58–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2016.1.27499
Mathes, J., Schredl, M., & Göritz, A. S. (2014). Frequency of typical dream themes in most recent
dreams: An online study. Dreaming, 24, 57–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0035857
Nielsen, T. A., Laberge, L., Paquet, J., Tremblay, R. E., Vitaro, F., & Montplaisir, J. (2000).
Development of disturbing dreams during adolescence and their relation to anxiety symptoms.
Sleep, 23, 727–736. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/sleep/23.6.1
Nielsen, T. A., Zadra, A. L., Simard, V., Saucier, S., Stenstrom, P., Smith, C., & Kuiken, D. (2003). The
typical dreams of Canadian university students. Dreaming, 13, 211–235. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/
B:DREM.0000003144.40929.0b
Pappa, S., Ntella, V., Giannakas, T., Giannakoulis, V. G., Papoutsi, E., & Katsaounou, P. (2020).
Prevalence of depression, anxiety, and insomnia among healthcare workers during the COVID-19
pandemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 88, 901–907.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2020.05.026
Richards, L. (1999). Using NVivo in qualitative research. London, UK: Sage.
Richards, L. (2015). Handling qualitative data: A practical guide. London, UK: Sage.
Rosen, J., Reynolds, C. F., III, Yeager, A. L., Houck, P. R., & Hurwitz, L. F. (1991). Sleep disturbances
in survivors of the Nazi Holocaust. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 62–66. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1176/ajp.148.1.62
Schredl, M. (2002). Questionnaire and diaries as research instruments in dream research: Methodologi-
cal issues. Dreaming, 12, 17–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1013890421674
Schredl, M. (2010a). Dream content analysis: Basic principles. International Journal of Dream Research,
3, 65–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2010.1.474
Schredl, M. (2010b). Explaining the gender difference in dream recall frequency. Dreaming, 20, 96–106.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019392
Schredl, M., Berres, S., Klingauf, A., Schellhaas, S., & Göritz, A. S. (2014). The Mannheim Dream
Questionnaire (MADRE): Retest reliability, age and gender effects. International Journal of Dream
Research, 7, 141–147. http://dx.doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2014.2.16675
Schredl, M., & Doll, E. (1998). Emotions in diary dreams. Consciousness and Cognition, 7, 634–646.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1998.0356
Schredl, M., & Lahl, O. (2010). Gender, sex role orientation, and dream recall frequency. Dreaming, 20,
19–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018578
Schredl, M., & Piel, E. (2006). War-related dream themes in Germany from 1956 to 2000. Political
Psychology, 27, 299–307. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2006.00008.x
Schredl, M., & Reinhard, I. (2008). Gender differences in dream recall: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sleep
Research, 17, 125–131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00626.x
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
214 IORIO, SOMMANTICO, AND PARRELLO
Settineri, S., Frisone, F., Alibrandi, A., & Merlo, E. M. (2019). Italian adaptation of the Mannheim
Dream Questionnaire (MADRE): Age, gender and dream recall effects. International Journal of
Dream Research, 12, 119–129. http://dx.doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2019.1.59328
Sher, L. (2020). COVID-19, anxiety, sleep disturbances and suicide. Sleep Medicine, 70, 124. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2020.04.019
Sommantico, M. (2016). A couple’s unconscious communication: Dreams. British Journal of Psycho-
therapy, 32, 456–474. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjp.12251
Sommantico, M. (2018). La fonction transformatrice du rêve [The transformative function of the dream].
Revue française de psychanalyse, 82, 1475–1480. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/rfp.825.1475
Sommantico, M., De Rosa, B., & Parrello, S. (2017). Counselling university students: A psychoanalytic
approach of the single case report. Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 5, 1. http://dx.doi
.org/10.6092/2282-1619/2017.5.1354
Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2013). Psychoanalysis and empirical research: The example of
alexithymia. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 61, 99–133. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1177/0003065112474066
Velotti, P., & Zavattini, G. C. (2019). È ancora attuale l’uso del sogno nella pratica clinica? [Is the use
of dream still actual in clinical practice?] Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 3, 433–462.
Wang, C., Zhou, J., & Zong, C. (2020). Two cases report of epidemic stress disorder to novel coronavirus
pneumonia. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 51, 102070. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102070
Xiao, H., Zhang, Y., Kong, D., Li, S., & Yang, N. (2020). Social capital and sleep quality in individuals
who self-isolated for 14 days during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) outbreak in January
2020 in China. Medical Science Monitor, 26, e923921. http://dx.doi.org/10.12659/MSM.923921
Yu, C. K.-C. (2015). One hundred typical themes in most recent dreams, diary dreams, and dreams
spontaneously recollected from last night. Dreaming, 25, 206–219. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0039225
Zandifar, A., & Badrfam, R. (2020). Iranian mental health during the COVID-19 epidemic. Asian
Journal of Psychiatry, 51, 101990. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ajp.2020.101990
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
DREAMING IN THE TIME OF COVID-19 215
... One aspect that seems to be unanimous in most of the previous research is that there is a continuity between awake experiences and dreaming, and using this continuity hypothesis, researchers explained the presence of dream images directly related to COVID-19 (Civitarese, 2021;Iorio et al., 2020;Monaco et al., 2022;Mota et al., 2020;Parrello et al., 2021;Pesonen et al., 2020;Schredl & Bulkeley, 2020). According to Monaco et al. (2022), (…) participants reported typical COVID-19 symptoms (e.g., cough and fever) in their dreams, suggesting that oneiric life reflected people's real-world experiences. ...
... Although some researchers have found dream content with literal references to COVID-19 (Gorgoni et al., 2022;MacKay & DeCicco, 2020;Parrello et al., 2021;Schredl & Bulkeley, 2020), other researchers reported quite the opposite where there was no difference in dream content, before and during the pandemic (Pesonen et al., 2020). However, most of the research reported predominant themes, not directly related to COVID-19, but which can be seen to represent some type of threat, negative emotion, or an indication of mental suffering (Di Renzo & Tagliacozzi, 2021;Gorgoni et al., 2022;Iorio et al., 2020;MacKay & DeCicco, 2020). ...
... Contrary to previous research (Civitarese, 2021;Iorio et al., 2020;Monaco et al., 2022;Mota et al., 2020;Parrello et al., 2021;Pesonen et al., 2020;Schredl & Bulkeley, 2020), we found that the majority of the dream images make no direct reference to COVID-19. Only a few dreams explicitly referred to the pandemic. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article presents the final results from research considering dream images during the first COVID-19 lockdown in Mexico. The sample consisted of nine subjects, with an age range of 18–38 years of age, living in México during the lockdown of the first wave of COVID-19 (beginning in March 2020). This was a multiple case study, with a qualitative approach. Eleven subjects participated voluntarily in an average of two to three in-depth clinical interviews lasting 1 hr. During the interview, participants were asked to share their most recent dream. They then proceeded to find meaning in the dream imagery through an analytical dialogue in conversation with the interviewer. After gathering all the data, the authors proceeded to use the narrative method and grounded theory to process the findings. Results showed a predominance of images charged with negative emotions, especially fear. Additionally, participants expressed a perception of vulnerability associated with a lack of control and unclear boundaries. Finally, participants presented, through their dream images, an unstable, dangerous, and misleading worldview. Few dreams literally referred to the pandemic, and only two participants repeatedly mentioned being affected by COVID-19.
... According to the study that used the Hall/Van de Castle Coding System (HVDC) to investigate the coronavirus pandemic dreams in Canada, several virus-related words appeared in the dreams, suggesting that waking concerns and anxieties regarding COVID-19 were reflected in the dream imagery (MacKay & DeCicco, 2020). Moreover, Iorio et al. (2020) reported that people projected into their dreams the desire to be free from enforced isolation. Combining these invasive influences on everyday life with the continuity hypothesis, it was expected that the COVID-19 outbreak caused a significant rise in sleep disturbance and nightmarish dreaming. ...
... Of the descriptions of the recalled dreams, the rate of the pandemic dream was 3.9% and 3.0% during and after the lockdown, respectively. This was lower than the 20% and 8.1% rates reported during the lockdowns in Italy (Iorio et al., 2020) and the United States, respectively (Schredl & Bulkeley, 2020). COVID-19 quickly caused a state of collapse to the local health system, leading to the lockdown of cities in more than 30 countries worldwide (Adhikari et al., 2020). ...
... However, it was 14.3% during the lockdown in the Hubei province, and 8.2% for workers during and after the lockdown, showing relatively high values. This is similar to the data of Italy and the United States from the previous literature (Iorio et al., 2020;Schredl & Bulkeley, 2020). The Hubei province had taken the highest quarantine measures, and it is plausible that they were the most affected by the outbreak. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated the “coronavirus pandemic dream” phenomenon in Chinese including residing in Wuhan city based on the continuity hypothesis, which states that dream contents reflect waking life. We analyzed the effects of lockdown due to coronavirus disease-2019 (COVID-19) on dream reported from Chinese university students and workers residing in inside and outside of Hubei province including Wuhan city, where the infection was initially detected in the world. The questionnaire on dreams, consisting of descriptions of the most striking dreams and classification of dream characteristics with reference to the Hall/Van de Castle Coding System, was administered to 943 participants via online survey over two periods. The recall rate of the COVID-19-related dreams among the participants of the Hubei province was high at 10.3%, while that in other provinces was 3.2%. Due to the different degrees of impact and quarantine policies, the Chinese in various regions have been affected differently by COVID-19. In the characteristic analysis of the dream contents, the biggest difference between the participants in Hubei and in other provinces was the emergence of words such as abandon and end during the lockdown, which are common in nightmarish pandemic dreams in many cultures. People in areas where the epidemic is severe or lockdown measures are strict may have more serious psychological problems, and needs more attention to psychological problems. In conclusion, analyzing the dream content could help identify specific people who may be the most at risk. It could allow researchers to direct appropriate help to specific populations in the pandemic.
... In this study, pandemic-related dreams were associated with stress. An Italian study (Iorio et al., 2020) found that 20% of the recorded dreams during the pandemic included explicit COVID-19 references. Based on the qualitative assessment of the sample's most recent dreams, the authors found a partial superposition with previous studies that investigated typical dream themes (e.g., "being chased" or "trying something again and again"), but also a difference in the prevalence of other themes, mainly centering dangerous or frustrating situations (Iorio et al., 2020). ...
... An Italian study (Iorio et al., 2020) found that 20% of the recorded dreams during the pandemic included explicit COVID-19 references. Based on the qualitative assessment of the sample's most recent dreams, the authors found a partial superposition with previous studies that investigated typical dream themes (e.g., "being chased" or "trying something again and again"), but also a difference in the prevalence of other themes, mainly centering dangerous or frustrating situations (Iorio et al., 2020). This uniqueness of dream themes during the pandemic was also reported by Giovanardi et al. (2022). ...
... In summary, dream content analysis during the pandemic has provided empirical backing to psychoanalytic perspectives on dreaming (Giovanardi et al., 2022;Iorio et al., 2020), as well as to the ERT (Mota et al., 2020), TST (Giovanardi et al., 2022;Mota et al., 2020;Pesonen et al., 2020), and SST (Mota et al., 2020;Schredl & Bulkeley, 2020). If increased dream recall seems to be associated with changes in sleep patterns, namely an increase in nocturnal awakenings (Simões et al., 2023), dream content appears to be particularly connected to waking-life experiences and concerns (Revonsuo, 2000). ...
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic has affected the mental health of people around the world, altering sleep patterns and dream experiences. Since dream content is associated with waking-life experiences, one would expect pandemic-related concerns to pervade dreams. Building upon previous research efforts, this study aims to contribute to the ongoing debate on dream function and to address potential variability in dream content according to different pandemic experiences. The survey protocol included questions on sociodemographic information, pandemic-related experiences, impact on personal social networks, perceived negative impact on several areas of life, sleep patterns, and dream experiences/reports. A total of 1,020 participants responded to the online survey questionnaire, and 177 COVID-19-related dreams were reported. A thematic content analysis was performed to codify participants’ dream reports. Chi-square tests addressed possible relations between dream content and pandemic-related experiences. Participants who dreamed about the COVID-19 pandemic also tended to report decreased sleep duration, increased frequency of nocturnal awakenings, and increased frequency of nightmare recall. The most frequent dream themes were sickness, death, inefficiency, and work-related concern. Being a health professional was associated with inefficiency and work-related concern themes. Inefficiency dreams were also associated with the death of a significant person. There was a strong association between the pandemic impact on employment and the being chased theme. Data supports the notion that dreams reflect waking-life anxieties and concerns. Therefore, dream content analysis could assist in identifying vulnerable/high-risk groups during collective crises and offer valuable insight into individuals’ psychological state in psychotherapeutic interventions.
... Increases in the emotional intensity of dreams, the negative tone of dreams, and the frequency of nightmares during the pandemic have also been reported (Barrett, 2020;Gorgoni et al., 2021;Iorio, Sommantico, & Parrello, 2020;Solomonova et al., 2021). Theoretically, dreams often include aspects of salient waking experiences and thus might exist to help people develop successful strategies to cope with threats and to help regulate emotions (Domhoff, 1996;Revonsuo, 2000;Scarpelli et al., 2019;Schredl & Hofmann, 2003). ...
... Theoretically, dreams often include aspects of salient waking experiences and thus might exist to help people develop successful strategies to cope with threats and to help regulate emotions (Domhoff, 1996;Revonsuo, 2000;Scarpelli et al., 2019;Schredl & Hofmann, 2003). Supporting the idea that increased intensity, nightmares, and negative emotions during dreaming were brought on by the pandemic, studies of dream reports indicate an increase in themes such as death and other threatening events, fear of overcrowding/failure of social distancing, and even apocalypse, during the pandemic (Barrett, 2020;Cong et al., 2022;Iorio et al., 2020;Mota et al., 2020;Pesonen et al., 2020;Wang et al., 2021). Additionally, these pandemic-related changes in dreams are typically greater in females than in males and are larger in younger adults compared with older adults . ...
... Increases in dream recall frequency, nightmare frequency, dream intensity, and negative tone of dreams shortly after the pandemic onset compared with prior to the pandemic have been previously reported (Barrett, 2020;Fränkl et al., 2021;Gorgoni et al., 2021;Iorio et al., 2020;Scarpelli, Alfonsi, Mangiaruga, et al., 2021;Schredl & Bulkeley, 2020;Solomonova et al., 2021). These increases have been attributed to changes in sleep patterns because of lockdowns (Schredl & Bulkeley, 2020) and have also been interpreted with respect to theories of dreaming suggesting that dreams help individuals deal with threatening aspects of waking life and in regulating emotions (Domhoff, 1996;Revonsuo, 2000;Scarpelli et al., 2019;Schredl & Hofmann, 2003). ...
Article
Full-text available
Reports of changes in dreaming behaviors emerged immediately after the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic. However, it is unclear if these changes persisted long term, after initial lockdowns were lifted and people became accustomed to pandemic life. The current study measured dreaming behaviors known to have changed immediately after the pandemic onset and other behaviors not previously examined, over 1 year after the pandemic began in 591 university students from the United States and Germany. Retrospective reports of all dreaming behaviors before the pandemic began were also obtained, as were measures of current sleep quality, perceived change in sleep quality due to the pandemic, and attitude toward dreams. Perceived nightmare and dream recall frequency, dream intensity, and negative tone increased over 1 year after the pandemic began, indicating that these previously known changes were long-lasting. Perceived increases in lucid dreaming, dream sharing, and how often dreams affect daytime mood extend knowledge of how pandemic life influenced dreaming. Whereas having a positive attitude toward dreams predicted perceived increases in aspects of dreaming that may have been beneficial in coping with pandemic-related challenges, perceived sleep quality decline predicted increases in dream recall frequency and measures related to the emotional aspects of dreams, suggesting that individuals with the greatest increases in these measures are at high risk for sustained mental health issues stemming from the pandemic and that dream-based interventions could help.
... Nesse contexto, variadas investigações estabeleceram a dimensão onírica, objeto este pouco explorado pela Sociologia, enquanto temática de estudo (Barrett, 2020a;2020b;Iorio et al., 2020;MacKay & DeCicco, 2020;Schredl & Bulkeley, 2020;Guerrero-Gomez et al., 2021;Solomonova et al., 2021;Margherita & Caffieri, 2022). ...
... Diante disso, diversos estudos, de diferentes momentos da pandemia, permitem identificar as múltiplas facetas desta "necropolítica" (Caponi et al., 2021, p. 97) (Duvignaud et al., 1979;Bastide, 2002;Lahire, 2020Lahire, , 2021 (Barrett, 2020b;Iorio et al., 2020;Schredl & Bulkeley, 2020 No plano empírico deste trabalho foi possível observar, de modo geral, uma certa semelhança entre os resultados encontrados pelas pesquisas sobre a temática e os verificados no contexto da pandemia brasileira. É possível que a evidência mais interessante apresentada por esta investigação seja a relação significativa entre a atribuição de sentimentos negativos aos sonhos e pessoas de menor renda, ainda mais quando essas são do gênero feminino e/ou mais jovens. ...
Article
Full-text available
Este artigo procura, fundamentalmente, cumprir dois objetivos articulados: a) apresentar uma tipologia acerca dos estudos sociológicos dos sonhos, uma área relativamente escanteada no interior da sociologia, apesar de contar com variada abordagem desde os seus primórdios; b) e, em seguida, posicionar nossa própria pesquisa frente a tal classificação a partir da coleta de dados empíricos, sua descrição estatística e interpretação, de modo a demonstrar certas relações entre a dimensão subjetiva da vida, particularmente a onírica, e um conjunto de atravessamentos sociais, como pertencimento socioeconômico, gênero e raça durante a pandemia da Covid-19, especialmente no Brasil Meridional.
... Aligned with previous studies (Iorio et al., 2020;Mackay & DeCicco, 2020;Scarpelli, Alfonsi, Gorgoni, et al., 2021), we suggest that dream and nightmare investigation and analysis can be a valuable asset in the management of the psychological consequences of collective crises. Spellberg (2020) notes that while many cultures worldwide have developed elaborate customs for sharing dreams, in modern Western societies, these practices are largely confined to a few specific domains, notably psychoanalytic therapy. ...
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic and the ongoing Russo-Ukrainian War have profoundly affected individuals worldwide, eliciting heightened levels of stress, anxiety, and fear. This study investigates the impact of these crises on sleep patterns and dream experiences within Portugal’s general adult population. Online surveys administered during both crises examined changes in sleep patterns, dream recall frequency, and characteristics of crisis-related dreams. The study involved 1,020 participants during the COVID-19 pandemic and 703 participants during the Russo-Ukrainian War. Results reveal that while the pandemic significantly disrupted various life domains, including mental health, the war primarily impacted financial stability. Sleep disturbances unfolded during both crises, but pandemic-induced disruptions were more pronounced. Crisis-related dreams, characterized by fear and distress, were also more prevalent during the pandemic. Variations in crisis-related dream emotional and sensory content were identified. Anxiety was mainly associated with pandemic dreams, whereas sadness and anger characterized war-related dreams. These findings underscore the psychological ramifications of crises on sleep and dreams, emphasizing the importance of addressing mental health issues during tumultuous epochs. The study also suggests that monitoring sleep patterns and analyzing dream content can provide valuable insights into individuals’ emotional processing of collective crises.
... As a result, the everyday life of Italian citizens was radically transformed (Amerio et al., 2021) with the suspension of all the industrial and commercial activities that were not considered essential (Horowitz, 2020) and major limitations to individual movements. The whole population had to remain at home, whereas both infected people and people who were suspected of being infected were forced to quarantine themselves (Iorio et al., 2020). ...
Article
Possible side effects of using web job boards in the e-recruitment context, such as candidates dropping out from the hiring process, may emerge if these tools are not transparent about data usage, collection, and processing. In response, we developed a novel web job board designed to enhance transparency, simulating a job-matching recommender system. A qualitative study with 20 Italian participants, combining direct observation of the job board use with the Thinking Aloud protocol and interviews, examines participants’ privacy behaviours in terms of data disclosure and seclusion. Findings indicate a general willingness among participants to share personal data, except for information related to their identity. We found that both the design of the job board and the meanings ascribed by participants to data shaped their privacy behaviours. Features enhancing user understanding of data usage and control of privacy settings were positively received, underscoring the importance of design in fostering thoughtful engagement with job board technologies. We contribute to research on privacy behaviours in the context of job search and we draw suggestions from the study findings on how to design platforms that support data protection and allow safe and purposeful disclosure of personal data, sustaining job seekers throughout the recruitment process.
... Influenced by these traditionally derived cultural beliefs, if the bereaved was intimate with the deceased but unable to attend the funeral or provide a decent funeral, he or she would perceive the death as not good and feel angry at oneself and blame oneself, which leads to more frequent dreaming about the deceased and the loss (Zheng et al., 2016). During the COVID-19 pandemic, individuals knowing people affected by or who have died of COVID-19 reported higher emotional intensity and sensory impressions in their most recent dreams (Iorio et al., 2020). Moreover, some bereaved people become upset upon awakening even after dreams they enjoyed during the dream (Black et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Network analyses showed that the posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptom structures may vary across traumatic events and types of survivors. However, the structure of PTSD symptoms in COVID-19 bereavement remains unknown. This study aimed to depict the structure of the PTSD symptom network and illuminate how the quality of the predeath relationship between the bereaved and the deceased links to the PTSD symptom network. Method: Using self-reported data from 408 COVID-19 bereaved adults (225 male and 183 female) collected within 9-month postbereavement, we constructed two graphical lasso networks of PTSD symptoms. Results: Symptoms with the highest centrality were exaggerated startle, negative emotional states, and reckless/self-destructive behavior, with exaggerated startle having relatively low predictability. The strongest connections were found between exaggerated starkness and detachment, and between psychological cue response and thoughts of avoidance. Moreover, closeness to and conflict with the deceased were linked to the PTSD symptom network through nightmares and physiological cue reactivity, respectively. Conclusions: This study extends our understanding of what PTSD is for people who are bereaved due to the COVID-19 pandemic by unveiling the structure of the PTSD symptom network. It also helps to distinguish the symptom-level links between quality of relationship with deceased and PTSD among the bereaved.
... As a result, the everyday life of Italian citizens was radically transformed (Amerio et al., 2021) with the suspension of all the industrial and commercial activities that were not considered essential (Horowitz, 2020) and major limitations to individual movements. The whole population had to remain at home, whereas both infected people and people who were suspected of being infected were forced to quarantine themselves (Iorio et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
During most dreams, the dreamer does not realize that they are in a dream. In contrast, lucid dreaming allows to become aware of the current state of mind, often accompanied by considerable control over the ongoing dream episode. Lucid dreams can happen spontaneously or be induced through diverse behavioural, cognitive or technological strategies. Such induction techniques have spurred research into the potential therapeutic aspects of lucid dreams. In this review, we gather evidence on the link between lucid dreams and conditions like nightmare disorder, depression, anxiety, psychosis, and dissociative states, and explore the possible neurobiological basis of these associations. Furthermore, we delve into contemplative sleep practices that train lucid states during sleep, such as Dream/Sleep Yoga and Yoga Nidrâ. The potential drawbacks of lucid dreaming interventions are outlined, accompanied by an examination of the impacts of lucid dreams on individuals without clinical conditions. By shedding light on these intricate relationships, the review contributes to a deeper understanding of the therapeutic possibilities and implications of lucid dreaming.
Book
Full-text available
This book presents a new neurocognitive model of dreams that draws from empirical research to explain the process of dreaming and the nature of dream content. Until now, dream studies have been limited in their usefulness, but recent advances in neuroscience, dream content analysis, cognitive linguistics, statistics, and computer software have made it possible to revitalize this area of research with the use of scientific methods. G. W. Domhoff's model helps explain the neural and cognitive bases for dreaming. He discusses how dreams express conceptions and concerns and how they are consistent over years and decades. He also shows that there may be limits to understanding the meaning of dreams, as there are many aspects of dream content that cannot be related to waking cognition or personal concerns. In addition, the book includes a detailed explanation of the methods needed to test the new model as well as a case study of a comprehensive dream journal. A new system of content analysis that can be used for highly sophisticated studies is discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Italy is one of the major COVID‐19 hotspots. To reduce the spread of the infections and the pressure on Italian healthcare systems, since March 10, 2020, Italy has been under a total lockdown, forcing people into home confinement. Here we present data from 1,310 people living in the Italian territory (Mage = 23.91 ± 3.60 years, 880 females, 501 workers, 809 university students), who completed an online survey from March 24 to March 28, 2020. In the survey, we asked participants to think about their use of digital media before going to bed, their sleep pattern and their subjective experience of time in the previous week (March 17–23, which was the second week of the lockdown) and up to the first week of February (February 3–10, before any restriction in any Italian area). During the lockdown, people increased the usage of digital media near bedtime, but this change did not affect sleep habits. Nevertheless, during home confinement, sleep timing markedly changed, with people going to bed and waking up later, and spending more time in bed, but, paradoxically, also reporting a lower sleep quality. The increase in sleep difficulties was stronger for people with a higher level of depression, anxiety and stress symptomatology, and associated with the feeling of elongation of time. Considering that the lockdown is likely to continue for weeks, research data are urgently needed to support decision making, to build public awareness and to provide timely and supportive psychosocial interventions.
Article
Full-text available
In the current global home confinement situation due to the COVID-19 outbreak, most individuals are exposed to an unprecedented stressful situation of unknown duration. This may not only increase daytime stress, anxiety and depression levels, but also disrupt sleep. Importantly, because of the fundamental role that sleep plays in emotion regulation, sleep disturbance can have direct consequences upon next day emotional functioning. In this paper, we summarize what is known about the stress-sleep link and confinement as well as effective insomnia treatment. We discuss those effects of the current home confinement situation that can disrupt sleep but also those that could benefit sleep quality. We suggest adaptions of cognitive behavioural therapy elements that are feasible to implement for those facing changed work schedules and requirements, those with health anxiety and those handling childcare and home-schooling, whilst also recognizing the general limitations imposed on physical exercise and social interaction. Managing sleep problems as best as possible during home confinement can limit stress and possibly prevent disruptions of social relationships.
Article
Full-text available
Background From the end of December 2019, coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) began to spread in central China. Social capital is a measure of social trust, belonging, and participation. This study aimed to investigate the effects of social capital on sleep quality and the mechanisms involved in people who self-isolated at home for 14 days in January 2020 during the COVID-19 epidemic in central China. Methods Individuals (n=170) who self-isolated at home for 14 days in central China, completed self-reported questionnaires on the third day of isolation. Individual social capital was assessed using the Personal Social Capital Scale 16 (PSCI-16) questionnaire. Anxiety was assessed using the Self-Rating Anxiety Scale (SAS) questionnaire, stress was assessed using the Stanford Acute Stress Reaction (SASR) questionnaire, and sleep was assessed using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) questionnaire. Path analysis was performed to evaluate the relationships between a dependent variable (social capital) and two or more independent variables, using Pearson’s correlation analysis and structural equation modeling (SEM). Results Low levels of social capital were associated with increased levels of anxiety and stress, but increased levels of social capital were positively associated with increased quality of sleep. Anxiety was associated with stress and reduced sleep quality, and the combination of anxiety and stress reduced the positive effects of social capital on sleep quality. Conclusion During a period of individual self-isolation during the COVID-19 virus epidemic in central China, increased social capital improved sleep quality by reducing anxiety and stress.
Article
Background COVID-19 pandemic has the potential to significantly affect the mental health of healthcare workers (HCWs), who stand in the frontline of this crisis. It is, therefore, an immediate priority to monitor rates of mood, sleep and other mental health issues in order to understand mediating factors and inform tailored interventions. The aim of this review is to synthesize and analyze existing evidence on the prevalence of depression, anxiety and insomnia among HCWs during the Covid-19 outbreak. Methods A systematic search of literature databases was conducted up to April 17th, 2020. Two reviewers independently assessed full-text articles according to predefined criteria. Risk of bias for each individual study was assessed and data pooled using random-effects meta-analyses to estimate the prevalence of specific mental health problems. The review protocol is registered in PROSPERO and is available online. Findings Thirteen studies were included in the analysis with a combined total of 33,062 participants. Anxiety was assessed in 12 studies, with a pooled prevalence of 23·2% and depression in 10 studies, with a prevalence rate of 22·8%. A subgroup analysis revealed gender and occupational differences with female HCPs and nurses exhibiting higher rates of affective symptoms compared to male and medical staff respectively. Finally, insomnia prevalence was estimated at 38·9% across 5 studies. Interpretation Early evidence suggests that a considerable proportion of HCWs experience mood and sleep disturbances during this outbreak, stressing the need to establish ways to mitigate mental health risks and adjust interventions under pandemic conditions.
Article
China has been severely affected by Coronavirus Disease 2019(COVID-19) since December, 2019. We aimed to assess the mental health burden of Chinese public during the outbreak, and to explore the potential influence factors. Using a web-based cross-sectional survey, we collected data from 7,236 self-selected volunteers assessed with demographic information, COVID-19 related knowledge, generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), depressive symptoms, and sleep quality. The overall prevalence of GAD, depressive symptoms, and sleep quality of the public were 35.1%, 20.1%, and 18.2%, respectively. Young people reported a significantly higher prevalence of GAD and depressive symptoms than older people. Compared with other occupational group, healthcare workers were more likely to have poor sleep quality. Multivariate logistic regression showed that age (< 35 years) and time spent focusing on the COVID-19 (≥ 3 hours per day) were associated with GAD, and healthcare workers were at high risk for poor sleep quality. Our study identified a major mental health burden of the public during the COVID-19 outbreak. Young people, people spending too much time thinking about the outbreak, and healthcare workers were at high risk of mental illness. Continuous surveillance of the psychological consequences for outbreaks should become routine as part of preparedness efforts worldwide.