ArticlePDF Available

Behavioral Immune Trade-Offs: Interpersonal Value Relaxes Social Pathogen Avoidance

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Behavioral-immune-system research has illuminated how people detect and avoid signs of infectious disease. But how do we regulate exposure to pathogens that produce no symptoms in their hosts? This research tested the proposition that estimates of interpersonal value are used for this task. The results of three studies (N = 1,694), each conducted using U.S. samples, are consistent with this proposition: People are less averse to engaging in infection-risky acts not only with friends relative to foes but also with honest and agreeable strangers relative to dishonest and disagreeable ones. Further, a continuous measure of how much a person values a target covaries with comfort with infection-risky acts with that target, even within relationship categories. Findings indicate that social prophylactic motivations arise not only from cues to infectiousness but also from interpersonal value. Consequently, pathogen transmission within social networks might be exacerbated by relaxed contamination aversions with highly valued social partners.
Content may be subject to copyright.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620960011
Psychological Science
2020, Vol. 31(10) 1211 –1221
© The Author(s) 2020
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/0956797620960011
www.psychologicalscience.org/PS
ASSOCIATION FOR
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
Research Article
Everyday life involves navigating a minefield of infec-
tious microbes that aim to exploit our bodies for their
own gain. We deftly avoid most of these pathogens, as
if we have some awareness of where they lie, despite
their invisibility to the naked eye. Such avoidance is
often affectively motivated. For example, the scents of
bodily wastes—reliable sources of pathogens through-
out our evolutionary history—elicit disgust, which moti-
vates contact avoidance (Tybur, Lieberman, Kurzban,
& DeScioli, 2013). Similar avoidance occurs socially;
people shun those unlucky enough to display many
infectious disease symptoms, including the pustules
caused by smallpox, the asymmetric swellings caused
by mumps, and the fluid-filled lesions caused by yaws
(Oaten, Stevenson, & Case, 2011). Understanding
responses to these and other cues to pathogens has
formed the bedrock of behavioral-immune-system
research (Ackerman, Hill, & Murray, 2018; Murray &
Schaller, 2016; Neuberg, Kenrick, & Schaller, 2011).
Yet a behavioral immune system that motivates
avoidance of only individuals covered in rashes, pox,
or swellings would leave us exposed to myriad patho-
gens transmitted by individuals showing no signs of
illness. Consider the consequences of contact with the
early 20th century cook Mary Mallon (“Typhoid Mary”),
who transmitted sometimes-lethal typhoid infections to
dozens of people despite showing no symptoms of illness
herself. Similar asymptomatic transmission is common
960011PSSXXX10.1177/0956797620960011Tybur et al.Interpersonal Value and Pathogen Avoidance
research-article2020
Corresponding Author:
Joshua M. Tybur, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Department of
Experimental and Applied Psychology, Van der Boechorststraat 7,
Room MF-C570, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands
E-mail: j.m.tybur@vu.nl
Behavioral Immune Trade-Offs:
Interpersonal Value Relaxes
Social Pathogen Avoidance
Joshua M. Tybur1,2 , Debra Lieberman3, Lei Fan1,2 ,
Tom R. Kupfer1, and Reinout E. de Vries1,2
1Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam; 2Institute of Brain and
Behavior Amsterdam; and 3Department of Psychology, University of Miami
Abstract
Behavioral-immune-system research has illuminated how people detect and avoid signs of infectious disease. But how
do we regulate exposure to pathogens that produce no symptoms in their hosts? This research tested the proposition
that estimates of interpersonal value are used for this task. The results of three studies (N = 1,694), each conducted
using U.S. samples, are consistent with this proposition: People are less averse to engaging in infection-risky acts not
only with friends relative to foes but also with honest and agreeable strangers relative to dishonest and disagreeable
ones. Further, a continuous measure of how much a person values a target covaries with comfort with infection-risky
acts with that target, even within relationship categories. Findings indicate that social prophylactic motivations arise not
only from cues to infectiousness but also from interpersonal value. Consequently, pathogen transmission within social
networks might be exacerbated by relaxed contamination aversions with highly valued social partners.
Keywords
infectious disease, behavioral immune system, disgust, evolutionary psychology, welfare trade-offs, open data, open
materials, preregistered
Received 3/24/20; Revision accepted 8/24/20
1212 Tybur et al.
across infectious agents. For example, volunteer infec-
tion studies indicate that 90% of participants dosed with
influenza shed viral particles, but only 70% show symp-
toms (Carrat etal., 2008). Among people who do even-
tually become ill, viral shedding begins before symptoms
appear and peaks before illness does. Asymptomatic
transmission is typical of many sexually transmitted
infections (Farley, Cohen, & Elkins, 2003), and it appears
to underlie much of the spread of the SARS-CoV-2 virus
that causes COVID-19 (Li etal., 2020). Further, a person
can transmit pathogens without being infected, simply
by touching a pathogen-contaminated surface. Ulti-
mately, every person can transmit pathogens, and
apparent health tells little about many common infec-
tion threats.
How do people navigate a social world in which
infectious agents are ubiquitous yet often undetectable,
even to a behavioral immune system that seems tailored
to detecting and neutralizing pathogens?
Trade-Offs: The Costs and Benefits
ofMitigating Exposure to Pathogens
As highlighted in the behavioral-immune-system litera-
ture, investments in pathogen avoidance often impose
costs on other fitness-promoting behaviors (e.g., Oaten,
Stevenson, & Case, 2009; Schaller, 2015; Tybur & Lieberman,
2016). Consider the most severe and most relaxed pathogen-
avoidance strategies possible. On one extreme, we could
experience motivations to avoid all direct and indirect
contact with all people. While minimizing exposure to
pathogens, such motivations would largely eliminate
food sharing, sexual behavior, cooperation on joint
tasks, and aid to kin and romantic partners (e.g., Case,
Repacholi, & Stevenson, 2006; Fleischman, Hamilton,
Fessler, & Meston, 2015). On the other extreme, we
could experience no motivations to avoid direct and
indirect contact—we could feel comfortable touching
or licking any person or any object touched by another
person. This approach, while eliminating the social
costs of contact avoidance, would leave us severely
vulnerable to infection. A well-designed behavioral
immune system should instead balance the costs of
pathogen exposure against those of social avoidance in
a target-specific manner. Guided by the considerations
described above, researchers have uncovered evidence
that mandrills groom parasitized maternal kin but avoid
grooming other parasitized conspecifics (Poirotte &
Charpentier, 2020) and that human mothers report less
disgust toward their own baby’s diapers than other
babies’ diapers (Case etal., 2006).
More broadly, disgust “source effects” are consistent
with the idea that similar trade-offs operate outside the
kinship domain; for example, some studies have found
that people imagine the bodily fluids or wastes from a
friend to be less aversive than those from a stranger
(Curtis, Aunger, & Rabie, 2004; Peng, Chang, & Zhou,
2013; Rozin, Nemeroff, Wane, & Sherrod, 1989; Stevenson
& Repacholi, 2005). Rather than reflecting lower path-
ogen avoidance toward more valued conspecifics,
though, these findings have been interpreted as sug-
gesting that familiarity is treated as information regard-
ing infection threat, just as pustules and lesions are. In
the current study, we tested the alternative account
described above: that willingness to engage in infection-
risky behaviors tracks interpersonal value, even in the
absence of illness symptoms.
Interpersonal Value Between
andWithin Categories
Interpersonal value does not map neatly onto catego-
ries labeled with terms such as family, friend, and foe.
The category “kin” alone reflects multiple relationship
types (e.g., parent, offspring, sibling, half-sibling), and
relationships are differentially valued within such cat-
egories (e.g., siblings; Sznycer, De Smet, Billingsley, &
Lieberman, 2016). Strangers also vary in interpersonal
affordances: Some are more likely to become valuable
exchange partners, and others are more likely to inflict
social costs. Hence, if social pathogen avoidance tracks
Statement of Relevance
People deftly navigate around pathogens, including
those hiding in bodily wastes, spoiled foods, and
individuals with infectious disease symptoms, even
without consciously considering the consequences
of infection. They do so because natural selection
has shaped our sensory and motivational systems
as a kind of behavioral immune system. However,
many pathogen threats, including those posed by
asymptomatic influenza and COVID-19 carriers,
show no signs of infectiousness. The current work
uncovers new information regarding how people
navigate these types of infection threats. Results
from three studies indicate that people feel strongly
motivated to avoid infection-risky behaviors with
unsavory strangers and disliked acquaintances, but
they are more comfortable taking identical risks
with individuals whose welfare they value. These
findings may help explain epidemiological patterns
such as family-group clustering: Infections spread
not only because of proximity but also because
of greater comfort with exposure to the unseen
pathogens transmitted by people we value.
Interpersonal Value and Pathogen Avoidance 1213
perceptions of interpersonal value, then people should
be more comfortable with infection-risky behaviors not
only with individuals from less valued categories but
also with more interpersonally valued targets within
categories.
Although interpersonal value is strongly influenced
by kinship, it is also shaped by, among other things,
mutual valuation, as occurs in friendships, and inclina-
tions to engage in reciprocity, as occurs in exchange
partners (Tooby & Cosmides, 1996). These disparate
sources of benefits are putatively integrated into a
welfare-trade-off ratio (WTR)—an individual’s willing-
ness to trade off his or her welfare for that of another
(Delton & Robertson, 2016; Kirkpatrick, Delton,
Robertson, & de Wit, 2015; Smith, Pedersen, Forster,
McCullough, & Lieberman, 2017; Tooby, Cosmides, Sell,
Lieberman, & Sznycer, 2008). We used tasks that mea-
sure willingness to trade-off one’s own welfare for that
of another to investigate whether comfort with poten-
tially infectious contact tracks interpersonal value.
Overview of the Present Studies
Across three studies, we tested the hypothesis that moti-
vations to avoid infection-risky behaviors relate to
target-specific interpersonal value. In Studies 1 and 2,
participants reported their comfort with infection-risky
acts with a target they know personally (either a roman-
tic partner, a friend, an acquaintance, or a disliked
other), and they completed a target-specific WTR task.
In Study 3, participants reported their comfort with
these same infection-risky acts with a stranger, who
was described as either high or low on honesty-humility
and agreeableness, two personality traits that should
inform expected interpersonal value. Participants from
each study were U.S. residents recruited using Amazon’s
Mechanical Turk. Samples drawn from this pool are
similar to nationally representative samples in many
ways, though they tend to be a bit younger, less reli-
gious, and less politically conservative (Levay, Freese,
& Druckman, 2016). Preregistrations, data, and R analy-
sis scripts for all three studies are available on OSF
(https://osf.io/4agk8/).
Study 1
Method
Study 1 examined whether people are less avoidant of
potentially infectious contact with individuals from
more valuable relationship categories and whether
interpersonal value predicts pathogen avoidance within
categories.
Participants. We preregistered a target of 500 partici-
pants. We did not use an a priori effect-size estimate,
though this sample afforded 80% power to detect a small
effect size (r) of .12. Five hundred four individuals
(55.16% male; age: M = 35.88 years, SD = 10.2) partici-
pated in exchange for $1.50. All respondents provided
informed consent.
Procedure. After reporting demographic information
(e.g., sex, age, relationship status, income), each partici-
pant was randomly assigned to think of either (a) their
romantic partner, (b) their closest friend, (c) an acquain-
tance, or (d) someone they know personally but dislike.
Participants who had previously reported being in a
romantic relationship had a 40% chance of being assigned
to the romantic-partner condition and a 20% chance of
being assigned to each of the other three conditions; sin-
gle participants had a 33% chance of being assigned to
the three non-romantic-partner conditions. Participants
were first asked to write the target’s initials, which
appeared in the remaining questions about the target.
They were then asked to write a few sentences describ-
ing the target’s physical appearance, to report how long
they have known the target, and to report the target’s age
and sex.
To measure motivations to avoid pathogen exposure,
we generated 10 items inspired by the germ-aversion
subscale from the Perceived Vulnerability to Disease
scale (Duncan, Schaller, & Park, 2009). Example items
included “Using [target]’s deodorant stick on yourself,
“Wearing a hat that [target] has worn many times,” and
“Touching a handkerchief that [target] used to blow his
or her nose.” Participants rated each item on a scale
from 3 (very uncomfortable) to 3 (very comfortable),
with the midpoint labeled 0 (neutral). A principal-axis
factor analysis suggested that these items varied along
a single dimension (all factor loadings were above 0.74;
α = .96). Mean contact comfort was 0.06 (SD = 1.97).
Lower scores were interpreted as corresponding with
greater motivations to avoid exposure to the pathogens
potentially transmitted by the target.
To assess interpersonal value, we used a WTR task
(Delton & Robertson, 2016; Kirkpatrick etal., 2015;
Smith etal., 2017). In this task, participants are asked
to select one of two options, the first of which involves
the participant receiving money, and the second of
which involves the target receiving money. Each target-
benefiting decision is characterized by a different wel-
fare trade-off—that is, a different ratio of benefits
received by the target relative to what could have been
received by the participant. For example, for one of the
items, participants decided whether they would rather
receive $17 with the target receiving nothing or receive
1214 Tybur et al.
nothing with the target receiving $37. Choosing the
beneficial option for the target would imply a WTR
toward that target of at least 0.45 (i.e., 17/37). Partici-
pants completed the same 60 items described by
Kirkpatrick et al. (2015), which include six anchor
points (fixed values received by the target), each of
which has 10 values that the participant would receive.
Switch points—the ratio at which participants begin
choosing the benefit for the target—were calculated for
each anchor and averaged (α = .99). Further details are
provided in the Supplemental Material available online.
We also asked participants to rate the target’s honesty-
humility (for an overview, see de Vries, Tybur, Pollet,
& van Vugt, 2016) using the 10 honesty-humility items
from the HEXACO-60 (Ashton & Lee, 2009; α = .88).
Individuals higher in honesty-humility report less will-
ingness to exploit others (Van Gelder & de Vries, 2012),
and they behave more prosocially in tasks with financial
consequences (e.g., returning more money in trust
games, offering more money in dictator games;
Thielmann, Spadaro, & Balliet, 2020). In sum, partners
higher in honesty-humility are more likely to confer
benefits in social relationships and hence should be
more valued as relationship partners.
Finally, we also measured the extent to which par-
ticipants felt generally motivated to avoid pathogen
cues using the seven-item pathogen domain of the
Three Domain Disgust Scale (Tybur, Lieberman, &
Griskevicius, 2009), which asks participants to rate
seven items (e.g., “Stepping in dog poop”) on a scale
from 0 (not at all disgusting) to 6 (extremely disgusting;
α = .83).
Data exclusion. We excluded participants with more
than two switch points within any of the six WTR anchors
(n = 35), three participants whose descriptions of their
partners were nonsensical or demonstrated poor English,
and two participants who selected a gender option indi-
cating that they were neither a man nor a woman. These
latter participants were excluded so that sex differences
could be examined. Results reported below are based on
the remaining 464 participants. All outcomes of null-
hypothesis significance testing (i.e., p < .05) remained
when no exclusions were made.
Results
Participants were more comfortable with potentially
infectious contact with targets whose welfare they val-
ued, r = .68, 95% confidence interval (CI) = [.63, .73],
p < .001 (see Fig. 2), and with targets rated as higher
on honesty-humility, r = .47, 95% CI = [.40, .54], p <
.001. A number of other variables also related to contact
comfort, including sensitivity to pathogen disgust, r =
.22, 95% CI = [.30, .13], p < .001, and target sex, with
participants reporting greater comfort with infectious
contact with women than with men, r = .17, 95% CI =
[.08, .26], p < .001. Notably, the main effect of target sex
was qualified by an interaction with participant sex1
(details are provided in the Supplemental Material).
Critically, contact comfort also varied across relation-
ship type (romantic partner, close friend, acquaintance,
enemy), F(3, 460) = 213.52, p < .001, η2 = .58, 90%
CI = [.54, .62] (see Fig. 1), as did WTR and honesty-
humility (for target-category differences in WTR and
honesty-humility and a full correlation matrix, see the
Supplemental Material).
We next conducted hierarchical regression analyses
to test whether WTR value relates to contact comfort
independently of relationship type. In a first step
(adjusted R2 = .08; see Fig. 2), contact comfort was
regressed on variables unrelated to WTR, including par-
ticipant sex and income, target sex, and pathogen-
disgust sensitivity. Adding WTR, b = 2.36, p < .001,
rp2 = .33, 90% CI = [.27, .39], and target honesty-humility,
b = 0.19, p = .03, rp2 = .03, 90% CI = [.005, .06], to the
model accounted for an additional 42.82% of the vari-
ance in contact comfort. But were these effects of WTR
entirely accounted for by the category of partner that
participants were asked to imagine? No. Although the
third step incorporating three orthogonally coded vari-
ables representing the four relationship categories
accounted for an additional 13.43% of variance in con-
tact comfort, WTR continued to account for unique
variance, b = 0.85, p < .001, rp2 = .05, 90% CI = [.01, .08],
though target honesty-humility did not, b = 0.05, p =
.49, rp2 < .001, 90% CI = [.01, .01].
Study 2
Method
Study 2 closely mirrored Study 1, with four exceptions.
First, given that WTR, rather than target-rated honesty-
humility, uniquely related to contact comfort, we did
not assess target honesty-humility. We instead assessed
participants’ prosocial personality traits, which might
jointly relate to WTR and pathogen avoidance (Kirkpatrick
etal., 2015; Kupfer & Tybur, 2017). Second, given asym-
metries in target sex across the four categories used in
Study 1 (4%, 70%, 76%, and 74% same-sex for romantic
partner, closest friend, acquaintance, and disliked other,
respectively), we randomly assigned each participant
to picture either a male or a female target. To accom-
modate this change, we eliminated the romantic-partner
condition.
Interpersonal Value and Pathogen Avoidance 1215
Participants. We preregistered a recruitment target of
430 individuals, which we anticipated would be reduced
to approximately 387 after exclusions. This sample size
was targeted to facilitate exploratory analyses involving
participant sex and target sex (see the Supplemental
Material), and it provided more than 99% power to detect
the relation between WTR and contact comfort observed
in Study 1. We recruited only participants not enrolled in
Study 1. Four hundred thirty individuals (56.28% male;
age: M = 36.29 years, SD = 10.99) participated in exchange
for $2.00. All respondents provided informed consent.
Procedures. Procedures were identical to those in
Study 1, with a few notable exceptions. First, each partici-
pant was randomly assigned to picture either a man or a
woman from one of the three categories (i.e., closest
male friend, closest female friend, male acquaintance,
female acquaintance, male disliked other, or female dis-
liked other). Second, they provided self-reports of agree-
ableness (α = .84) and honesty-humility (α = .81; rather than
target ratings of honesty-humility) from the HEXACO-60
(Ashton & Lee, 2009). Third, given high consistency across
the six anchor points used in Study 1, they completed a
30-item WTR measure rather than the 60-item version (α =
.97). They also completed a handful of additional items,
which were not included in our preregistered analysis plan
(see the Supplemental Material).
Data exclusion. We excluded 19 participants with
more than two switch points within any of the three WTR
anchors, seven participants whose descriptions of their
partners were nonsensical or demonstrated poor English,
and one participant who described their gender identity
as neither male nor female. The results reported below
are based on the remaining 403 participants. All out-
comes of null-hypothesis significance testing (i.e., p <
.05) remained when no exclusions were made.
Results
As in Study 1, participants were more comfortable with
potentially infectious contact with more interpersonally
valued targets, r = .61, 95% CI = [.54, .67], p < .001 (see
Fig. 2). Contact comfort also related to pathogen-disgust
sensitivity, r = .24, 95% CI = [.33, .15], p < .001. And,
as expected, it also varied across relationship type
(close friend, acquaintance, enemy), F(2, 397) = 116.7,
p < .001, η2 = .37, 90% CI = [.31, .42] (see Fig. 1). A full
list of correlations is provided in the Supplemental
Material.
We next ran our preregistered analyses, in which we
first entered participant characteristics (sex, age, self-
reports of honesty-humility, agreeableness—as opposed
to target reports used in Study 1—and pathogen-disgust
sensitivity), then target characteristics (sex, age, WTR)
Study 1
3
2
1
0
1
2
3
Contact Comfort
Study 2 Study 3
Romantic
Partner
Friend Acquaintance Enemy Friend Acquaintance Enemy High-Value
Stranger
Low-Value
Stranger
Fig. 1. Mean comfort with infection-risky contact with each target type, separately for Studies 1 through 3. In each violin plot, the horizontal
line indicates the median, the white box indicates the interquartile range of the data, the shaded area indicates the density of the data, and
the whiskers extend 1.5 times the interquartile range. Outliers are indicated by dots.
1216 Tybur et al.
Target
Study 1
Romantic Partner
Friend
Acquaintance
Enemy
Study 3
High-Value Stranger
Low-Value Stranger
3
2
1
0
1
2
3
3
2
1
0
1
2
3
0.45 0.05 0.55 1.05 1.55
WTR
0.45 0.05 0.55 1.05 1.55
WTR
Study 1
Contact Comfort
Contact Comfort
3
2
1
0
1
2
3
Contact Comfort
Study 2
Study 3
0.45 0.05 0.55 1.05 1.55
WTR
Friend
Acquaintance
Enemy
Study 2
Fig. 2. Relations between welfare-trade-off ratio (WTR) and comfort with infection-risky contact, separately for each target type in
Studies 1 through 3. In each scatterplot, the solid line indicates the best-fitting relationship between WTR and contact comfort across
categories, and the dashed lines indicate the best-fitting relationship within each target category. Shaded areas around the regression
lines indicate 95% confidence intervals.
in a second step, then relationship category in a third
step. Participant characteristics accounted for approx-
imately 6.16% of the variance in contact comfort, and
target characteristics apart from relationship category
accounted for an additional 36.82% of the variance.
In that second step, WTR was most strongly related
to contact comfort, b = 2.06, p < .001, rp2 = .36, 90%
CI = [.29, .42]. Adding relationship category accounted
for an additional 7.77% of the variance in contact
comfort, but, as in Study 1, the unique effect of WTR
remained, b = 1.13, p < .001, rp2 = .09, 90% CI = [.05,
.14].
Interpersonal Value and Pathogen Avoidance 1217
Study 3
Method
Study 3 was designed to address limitations of Studies
1 and 2: Each participant pictured a different target,
and unmeasured third variables could have confounded
pathogen-avoidance motivations and interpersonal
value. For example, given that people with many symp-
toms of illness are socially devalued (Oaten et al.,
2011), less interpersonally valued partners might actu-
ally be more infectious, and the relation between WTR
and pathogen-avoidance motivations could have
reflected stronger avoidance of more infectious indi-
viduals. To address this limitation, we accounted for
the identity of the target in Study 3. Participants saw a
picture of a stranger and read a description containing
information about that person’s value as an exchange
partner. Given that individuals higher in honesty-
humility and agreeableness behave more prosocially in
social dilemmas (e.g., dictator games, trust games, ulti-
matum games; Thielmann, Spadaro, & Balliet, 2020),
and such games serve as abstractions of valuable
behavior in exchange relationships (e.g., willingness to
share resources, trust, forgiveness; Murnighan & Wang,
2016), we designed the descriptions to communicate
either high honesty-humility and agreeableness or low
honesty-humility and agreeableness. Similar types of
information about strangers have been shown to influ-
ence the magnitude of WTRs in the expected direction
(e.g., Smith etal., 2017). We predicted that motivations
to avoid pathogens would be higher for targets low in
honesty-humility and agreeableness, because these tar-
gets offer little benefits that might offset potential costs
of infection, and that target-specific WTR would again
relate to willingness to expose oneself to unseen patho-
gens. Notably, all targets were strangers to participants,
and any differences in contact comfort across high ver-
sus low agreeableness and honesty-humility targets
cannot be explained by putatively greater infection
threats posted by strangers relative to friends (e.g., Peng
etal., 2013).
Participants. We preregistered a recruitment target of
870 individuals, which we anticipated would be reduced
to approximately 800 after exclusions. This sample size
afforded 80% power assuming a small effect of the manip-
ulation (d = 0.20), with a target intercept variance compo-
nent of .15 and a target slope variance of .02 (Westfall
etal., 2014). Nine hundred five individuals (49.94% male;
age: M = 38.22 years, SD = 11.64) participated in exchange
for $1.60. All respondents provided informed consent.
Procedures. Each participant was randomly assigned to
see and read about a target that was described as either
high or low in honesty-humility and agreeableness. We
employed a stimulus-sampling approach to target appear-
ance, which allows for inferences across populations of
stimuli as well as populations of participants (Westfall
etal., 2014). Each participant was also randomly assigned
to see one of 40 different faces (20 male, 20 female)
selected from the Chicago Face Database (Ma, Correll, &
Wittenbrink, 2015). Given arguments that physical attrac-
tiveness is treated as indicative of infection risk (Park, van
Leeuwen, & Stephen, 2012), we aimed to sample from a
range of attractiveness levels. We identified the man rated
most attractive in the normed data set (a rating of 5 on a
1- to 7-point scale based on data reported by Ma et al.)
and selected him and 19 other men, each with an attrac-
tiveness rating 0.15 scale units below the previous face.
We then selected female targets that matched the male
targets on attractiveness ratings.
In both conditions, participants read a description
of the target. This description was based on items from
each of the four facets of honesty-humility (sincerity,
fairness, greed avoidance, and modesty) and each of
the four facets of agreeableness (forgiveness, gentle-
ness, flexibility, patience). Low-value targets were
described on the low end of each facet, and high-value
targets were described on the high end of each facet
(for complete descriptions, see the Supplemental Mate-
rial). After seeing the target and reading the description,
participants completed the same contact-comfort items
used in Studies 1 and 2 (α = .91) and the same 30-item,
three-anchor WTR measure used in Study 2 (α = .97).
They also rated the target’s honesty and kindness (on
11-point scales) as well as other characteristics (for a
complete list, see the Supplemental Material). As
intended, relative to targets high in prosocial personal-
ity traits, targets low in prosocial personality traits were
rated less honest (M = 9.53 vs. M = 2.39, p < .001) and
less kind (M = 9.33 vs. M = 2.93, p < .001). Further
details are provided in the Supplemental Material.
Data exclusion. We excluded 40 participants whose
des criptions of the target were nonsensical or demonstrated
poor English, eight participants who described their gender
identity as neither male nor female, and 30 participants
with more than two switch points within any of the three
WTR anchors. Results reported below are based on the
remaining 827 participants. All outcomes of null-hypothesis
significance testing (i.e., p < .05) remained when no
exclusions were made.
Results
Using a random-effects model, we regressed contact
comfort on target condition (high value vs. low value)
and initially modeled random intercepts for stimuli and
1218 Tybur et al.
random slopes for the effect of interpersonal value
across stimuli. We removed random slopes that pre-
vented model convergence. People were more comfort-
able with exposure to pathogens when the targets were
described as high in prosocial personality traits (M =
0.93, 95% CI = [1.04, 0.81]) than when they were
described as low in prosocial personality traits (M =
1.8, 95% CI = [1.89, 1.70], F(1, 822) = 126.1, p < .001.
When WTR was added as a predictor, both WTR and
the manipulation were related to comfort with exposure
to pathogens (both ps < .001; see Fig. 2). The effect of
the manipulation and WTR remained when we further
controlled for participant pathogen-disgust sensitivity,
participant sex, and target sex, ps < .001. Full details
are provided in the Supplemental Material.
Discussion
Results from each of three studies revealed that motiva-
tions to avoid infection-risky contact varied markedly
across targets with no clear symptoms of illness. Much
of this variation was accounted for by targets’ interper-
sonal value to perceivers. Participants were less averse
to infection-risky contact with targets from categories
that are, on average, more highly valued (e.g., close
friends vs. disliked others), and they were less averse
to infection-risky contact with agreeable and honest
strangers than with disagreeable and dishonest ones.
Further, even within target categories, comfort with
infection-risky contact related to a continuous measure
of interpersonal value—for example, people who valued
their closest friend more were also less averse to infec-
tion-risky contact with that friend. We discuss how these
findings can inform both the burgeoning behavioral-
immune-system literature and our understanding of
how infectious disease spreads.
Implications for understanding the
behavioral immune system
The behavioral-immune-system literature largely focuses
on understanding how people detect and respond to
features that putatively provide information regarding
infectiousness, such as pustules and swellings (Ackerman
etal., 2018; Murray & Schaller, 2016; Neuberg etal.,
2011; Oaten etal., 2011). The current study is a step
forward in understanding pathogen avoidance even in
the absence of such cues, and it raises critical issues
for future research.
First, growing evidence suggests that the behavioral
immune system does not output the same pathogen-
avoidance motivations across all contexts. It is instead
flexible, weighing strands of information to determine
the fitness value of contacting another person or item
(Neuberg etal., 2011; Tybur & Lieberman, 2016). The
current findings demonstrate that interpersonal value
is one such strand. Other findings suggest that patho-
gen avoidance is relaxed in situations that require some
exposure to pathogens, such as sexual interactions
(e.g., Fleischman etal., 2015) and childrearing (e.g.,
Case etal., 2006). Future work can test whether relaxed
pathogen avoidance toward offspring and mates results
only from their high interpersonal value or whether
sexual value and genetic relatedness, which inform
interpersonal value (but are not redundant with it),
additionally shape pathogen avoidance (cf. Tooby etal.,
2008).
Second, researchers have speculated that people are
more disgusted by infection-risky contact with strangers
relative to friends because social familiarity is treated
as a cue to infectiousness, just as rashes and sores are
(Curtis etal., 2004; Peng etal., 2013; Stevenson & Repacholi,
2005). Similarly, the behavioral-immune-system litera-
ture is replete with proposals that prejudices toward
members of various groups partially stem from people
treating morphological features (e.g., in the cases of the
physically disabled, obese, and elderly) or foreign eco-
logical origin (e.g., in the case of immigrants) as cues
to infectiousness (for a summary, see Murray & Schaller,
2016). The current results suggest an alternative—or, at
least, supplementary—approach to understanding how
the behavioral immune system contributes to social
biases: Prejudices toward the aforementioned groups
might result from perceptions of interpersonal value
rather than perceptions of infectiousness. Recent stud-
ies has reevaluated claims that anti-immigrant preju-
dices partially result from perceptions that foreign
ecological origin is indicative of infectiousness (e.g.,
Karinen, Molho, Kupfer, & Tybur, 2019; van Leeuwen
& Petersen, 2018); future work could similarly clarify
whether the behavioral immune system outputs preju-
dices toward the obese, elderly, and physical disabled
because they are perceived as infectious or because
they are perceived as not offering the interpersonal
benefits that offset the infection risks posed by any
social interaction.
Third, if infection-risky contact is embraced with
interpersonally valued others and avoided with inter-
personally devalued ones, then contact rituals (e.g.,
hugs, handshakes) might be used to signal, regulate,
and maintain interpersonal valuation. Refusals to
engage in such rituals with a specific target might be
interpreted as suggesting that the target is not valued
enough to risk infection, is perceived as having some
symptom of contagious illness, or both. These consider-
ations might contribute to our understanding of the cul-
tural evolution and maintenance of greeting rituals. They
also highlight an important limit on the generalizability
Interpersonal Value and Pathogen Avoidance 1219
of these data, which were collected in the United States.
Recent findings suggest that at least some contamina-
tion aversion exists across human populations (Api-
cella, Rozin, Busch, Watson-Jones, & Legare, 2018).
Universality does not imply an absence of variation,
though. Indeed, some evidence suggests that potentially
infectious ritualized contact is less prevalent in areas
with more infectious disease (Murray, Fessler, Kerry,
White, & Marin, 2017). Any signal value of contact and
contact avoidance might similarly vary across regions
as a function of ecological parasite stress, as might the
degree to which interpersonal value influences motiva-
tions to embrace or avoid infection-risky contact. Even
within a single nation, the relation between interper-
sonal value and contact comfort might vary as a func-
tion of transient infection threats, such as those posed
by COVID-19.
Implications for the spread
ofinfectious disease
Multiple factors constrain the effectiveness of pharma-
ceutical interventions in combating pandemics. Hence,
outcomes of our battles against microbes will hinge on
the effectiveness of nonpharmaceutical interventions
(Ferguson etal., 2020). In addition to better hygiene
(e.g., handwashing), such interventions might focus on
stemming contagion within social networks. Indeed,
research during the 2009 H1N1 influenza pandemic
indicated that infectious disease spreads within social
networks much faster than it spreads across the broader
population (Christakis & Fowler, 2010). Closer physical
proximity and more frequent social interactions doubt-
lessly contribute to such spread. The current findings
reveal another factor that likely exacerbates within-
network contagion: the relaxation of pathogen avoid-
ance toward interpersonally valued targets. This
observation might help inform approaches to dampen-
ing disease transmission during outbreaks. Whereas
people need little encouragement to avoid infectious-
risky behaviors with most people, they largely feel com-
fortable engaging in identical behaviors with targets
that they especially value interpersonally. Improving
our understanding of this and other features of the
behavioral immune system can enable a better defense
in our war against infectious disease.
Transparency
Action Editor: Eddie Harmon-Jones
Editor: Patricia J. Bauer
Author Contributions
J. M. Tybur developed the study concept, coordinated data
collection, and drafted the manuscript. J. M. Tybur designed
Study 1; J. M. Tybur and T. R. Kupfer designed Study 2; and
J. M. Tybur, T. R. Kupfer, D. Lieberman, and R.E. de Vries
designed Study 3. L. Fan analyzed the data under the super-
vision of J. M. Tybur. All authors provided critical revisions
on the initial draft of the manuscript. All authors approved
the final version of the manuscript for submission.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
A percentage of profit received from commercial sale of
the HEXACO personality inventory is used by the Vrije
Universiteit Amsterdam to support research by the last
author. The authors declared that there were no other
potential conflicts of interest with respect to the authorship
or the publication of this article.
Funding
This research was supported by Horizon 2020 European
Research Council Grant StG-2015 680002-HBIS.
Open Practices
All data, analysis scripts, and materials have been made
publicly available via OSF and can be accessed at https://
osf.io/4agk8/. The design and analysis plans for all three
studies were preregistered at https://osf.io/4agk8/. The
complete Open Practices Disclosure for this article can be
found at http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/suppl/10.11
77/0956797620960011. This article has received the badges
for Open Data, Open Materials, and Preregistration. More
information about the Open Practices badges can be found
at http://www.psychologicalscience.org/publications/
badges.
ORCID iDs
Joshua M. Tybur https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0462-6508
Lei Fan https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3531-9199
Supplemental Material
Additional supporting information can be found at http://
journals.sagepub.com/doi/suppl/10.1177/0956797620960011
Note
1. In each study, the main effect of target sex on contact comfort
was qualified by an interaction with participant sex. Further
details are provided in the Supplemental Material.
References
Ackerman, J. M., Hill, S. E., & Murray, D. R. (2018). The
behavioral immune system: Current concerns and future
directions. Social and Personality Psychology Compass,
12, Article e12371. doi:10.1111/spc3.12371
Apicella, C. L., Rozin, P., Busch, J. T., Watson-Jones, R. E., &
Legare, C. H. (2018). Evidence from hunter-gatherer and
subsistence agricultural populations for the universality of
contagion sensitivity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39,
355–363. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.03.003
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2009). The HEXACO–60: A
short measure of the major dimensions of personality.
1220 Tybur et al.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 91, 340–345.
doi:10.1080/00223890902935878
Carrat, F., Vergu, E., Ferguson, N. M., Lemaitre, M., Cauchemez,
S., Leach, S., & Valleron, A.-J. (2008). Time lines of infec-
tion and disease in human influenza: A review of volun-
teer challenge studies. American Journal of Epidemiology,
167, 775–785. doi:10.1093/aje/kwm375
Case, T. I., Repacholi, B. M., & Stevenson, R. J. (2006). My
baby doesn’t smell as bad as yours: The plasticity of
disgust. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 357–365.
doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.03.003
Christakis, N. A., & Fowler, J. H. (2010). Social network sensors
for early detection of contagious outbreaks. PLOS ONE,
5(9), Article e12948. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0012948
Curtis, V., Aunger, R., & Rabie, T. (2004). Evidence that disgust
evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proceedings of the
Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences, 271(Suppl. 4),
S131–S133. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2003.0144
Delton, A. W., & Robertson, T. E. (2016). How the mind makes
welfare tradeoffs: Evolution, computation, and emotion.
Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 12–16. doi:10.1016/j
.copsyc.2015.06.006
de Vries, R. E., Tybur, J. M., Pollet, T. V., & van Vugt, M. (2016).
Evolution, situational affordances, and the HEXACO
model of personality. Evolution and Human Behavior,
37, 407–421. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.04.001
Duncan, L. A., Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2009). Perceived
vulnerability to disease: Development and validation of a
15-item self-report instrument. Personality and Individual
Differences, 47, 541–546. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.001
Farley, T. A., Cohen, D. A., & Elkins, W. (2003). Asymptomatic
sexually transmitted diseases: The case for screening.
Preventive Medicine, 36, 502–509. doi:10.1016/S0091-
7435(02)00058-0
Ferguson, N. M., Laydon, D., Nedjati-Gilani, G., Imai, N.,
Ainslie, K., . . . Ghani, A. C. (2020). Impact of non-
pharmaceutical interventions (NPIs) to reduce COVID19
mortality and healthcare demand. Retrieved from https://
doi.org/10.25561/77482
Fleischman, D. S., Hamilton, L. D., Fessler, D. M., & Meston,
C. M. (2015). Disgust versus lust: Exploring the interac-
tions of disgust and fear with sexual arousal in women.
PLOS ONE, 10(6), Article e0118151. doi:10.1371/journal
.pone.0118151
Karinen, A. K., Molho, C., Kupfer, T. R., & Tybur, J. M. (2019).
Disgust sensitivity and opposition to immigration: Does
contact avoidance or resistance to foreign norms explain
the relationship? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
84, Article 103817. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103817
Kirkpatrick, M., Delton, A. W., Robertson, T. E., & de Wit,
H. (2015). Prosocial effects of MDMA: A measure of
generosity. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 29, 661–668.
doi:10.1177/0269881115573806
Kupfer, T. R., & Tybur, J. M. (2017). Pathogen disgust and
interpersonal personality. Personality and Individual
Differences, 116, 379–384. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2017.05.024
Levay, K. E., Freese, J., & Druckman, J. N. (2016). The demo-
graphic and political composition of Mechanical Turk
samples. SAGE Open, 6(1). doi:10.1177/2158244016636433
Li, R., Pei, S., Chen, B., Song, Y., Zhang, T., Yang, W., &
Shaman, J. (2020). Substantial undocumented infection
facilitates the rapid dissemination of novel coronavirus
(SARS-CoV2). Science, 368, 489–493. doi:10.1126/science.
abb3221
Ma, D. S., Correll, J., & Wittenbrink, B. (2015). The Chicago
Face Database: A free stimulus set of faces and norm-
ing data. Behavior Research Methods, 47, 1122–1135.
doi:10.3758/s13428-014-0532-5
Murnighan, J. K., & Wang, L. (2016). The social world as
an experimental game. Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, 136, 80–94. doi:10.1016/j
.obhdp.2016.02.003
Murray, D. R., Fessler, D. M., Kerry, N., White, C., & Marin,
M. (2017). The kiss of death: Three tests of the rela-
tionship between disease threat and ritualized physi-
cal contact within traditional cultures. Evolution and
Human Behavior, 38, 63–70. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbe
hav.2016.06.008
Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2016). The behavioral immune
system: Implications for social cognition, social interac-
tion, and social influence. In J. M. Olson & M. P. Zanna
(Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol.
53, pp. 75–129). doi:10.1016/bs.aesp.2015.09.002
Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., & Schaller, M. (2011). Human
threat management systems: Self-protection and disease
avoidance. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35,
1042–1051. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.011
Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as
a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin,
135, 303–321. doi:10.1037/a0014823
Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2011). Disease avoid-
ance as a functional basis for stigmatization. Proceedings
of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 366,
3433–3452. doi:10.1098/rstb.2011.0095
Park, J. H., van Leeuwen, F., & Stephen, I. D. (2012).
Homeliness is in the disgust sensitivity of the beholder:
Relatively unattractive faces appear especially unattractive
to individuals higher in pathogen disgust. Evolution and
Human Behavior, 33, 569–577. doi:10.1016/j.evolhum
behav.2012.02.005
Peng, M., Chang, L., & Zhou, R. (2013). Physiological and
behavioral responses to strangers compared to friends as
a source of disgust. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34,
94–98. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.10.002
Poirotte, C., & Charpentier, M. J. (2020). Unconditional care
from close maternal kin in the face of parasites. Biology
Letters, 16(2), 20190869. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2019.0869
Rozin, P., Nemeroff, C., Wane, M., & Sherrod, A. (1989).
Operation of the sympathetic magical law of contagion
in interpersonal attitudes among Americans. Bulletin
of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 367–370. doi:10.3758/
BF03334630
Schaller, M. (2015). The behavioral immune system. In D. M.
Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (2nd
ed., pp. 206–224). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
Smith, A., Pedersen, E. J., Forster, D. E., McCullough,
M.E., & Lieberman, D. (2017). Cooperation: The roles
of interpersonal value and gratitude. Evolution and
Interpersonal Value and Pathogen Avoidance 1221
Human Behavior, 38, 695–703. doi:10.1016/j.evolhum
behav.2017.08.003
Stevenson, R. J., & Repacholi, B. M. (2005). Does the source
of an interpersonal odour affect disgust? A disease risk
model and its alternatives. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 35, 375–401. doi:10.1002/ejsp.263
Sznycer, D., De Smet, D., Billingsley, J., & Lieberman, D.
(2016). Coresidence duration and cues of maternal invest-
ment regulate sibling altruism across cultures. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 111, 159–177.
doi:10.1037/pspi0000057
Thielmann, I., Spadaro, G., & Balliet, D. (2020). Personality
and prosocial behavior: A theoretical framework and
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146, 30–90.
doi:10.1037/bul0000217
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1996). Friendship and the banker’s
paradox: Other pathways to the evolution of adaptations
for altruism. In W. G. Runciman, J. Maynard Smith, &
R. I. M. Dunbar (Eds.), Evolution of social behavior
patterns in primates and man (pp. 119–143). Oxford,
England: Oxford University Press.
Tooby, J., Cosmides, L., Sell, A., Lieberman, D., & Sznycer,
D. (2008). Internal regulatory variables and the design
of human motivation: A computational and evolutionary
approach. In A. J. Elliot (Ed.), Handbook of approach
and avoidance motivation (pp. 251–271). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Tybur, J. M., & Lieberman, D. (2016). Human pathogen avoid-
ance adaptations. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 6–11.
doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.06.005
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., & Griskevicius, V. (2009).
Microbes, mating, and morality: Individual differences in
three functional domains of disgust. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 97, 103–122. doi:10.1037/a0015474
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R., & DeScioli, P. (2013).
Disgust: Evolved function and structure. Psychological
Review, 120, 65–84. doi:10.1037/a0030778
Van Gelder, J. L., & de Vries, R. E. (2012). Traits and states:
Integrating personality and affect into a model of criminal
decision making. Criminology, 50, 637–671. doi:10.1111/
j.1745-9125.2012.00276.x
van Leeuwen, F., & Petersen, M. B. (2018). The behavioral
immune system is designed to avoid infected individu-
als, not outgroups. Evolution & Human Behavior, 39,
226–234. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.12.003
Westfall, J., Kenny, D. A., & Judd, C. M. (2014). Statistical
power and optimal design in experiments in which sam-
ples of participants respond to samples of stimuli. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 2020–2045.
doi:10.1037/xge0000014
... Murray et al., 2011). Another hypothesis suggests that pathogen-based xenophobia might be especially pronounced for immigrants who offer fewer benefits as social partners (Tybur et al., 2020;Van Leeuwen et al., 2023). To test these secondary hypotheses, we described an immigrant that varied on his nationality, assimilation to the local culture, ability to provide benefits to locals and willingness to provide benefits to locals before asking participants to report their attitudes towards immigrants like that target. ...
... Faulkner et al., 2004;Karinen et al., 2019;Ji et al., 2019). In concert with findings that motivations to avoid interpersonal contact are much more strongly influenced by a target's symptoms of infection and their relationship closeness rather than their ethnic or national group membership Tybur et al., 2020;van Leeuwen & Peterson, 2018; also see Bressan, 2021Bressan, , 2023, the current findings suggest that the social effects of the behavioural immune system might be more limited than previously proposed (see also Makhanova et al., 2022). ...
... Second, pathogen-avoidance psychology could act as a cultural attractor for the development of ethnocentric beliefs (Fessler & Machery, 2012), potentially as a byproduct of the lower social affordances of outgroup members relative to ingroup members (cf. Tybur et al., 2020). These beliefs could be advantageous for groups even if such advantages are unrelated to avoiding infection (Fessler et al., 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Multiple proposals suggest that xenophobia increases when infectious disease threats are salient. The current longitudinal study tested this hypothesis by examining whether and how anti-immigrant sentiments varied in the Netherlands across four time points during the COVID-19 pandemic (May 2020, February 2021, October 2021 and June 2022 through Flycatcher.eu). The results revealed that (1) anti-immigrant sentiments were no higher in early assessments, when COVID-19 hospitalizations and deaths were high, than in later assessments, when COVID-19 hospitalizations were low, and (2) within-person changes in explicit disease concerns and disgust sensitivity did not relate to anti-immigrant sentiments, although stable individual differences in disgust sensitivity did. These findings suggest that anecdotal accounts of increased xenophobia during the pandemic did not generalize to the population sampled from here. They also suggest that not all increases in ecological pathogen threats and disease salience increase xenophobia.
... When the behavioral immune system is activated (e.g., by perceived risk of infection or feelings of disgust) people perceive lower shared fate with others [5] and become less willing to cooperate [64,65] or even interact with potentially infectious others, especially outgroup members [66,67]. These findings are in line with the idea that people are more willing to engage in behaviors that put them at risk of infection with people they value than with those they do not value as highly [68]. ...
... Helping during pandemics is different from helping during other crises because helping in person can increase the risk of infection. Hence, our measure of willingness to help by letting others move into your home for a week might not have captured cooperation alone but also a tradeoff between the motivation to cooperate and people's willingness to incur the risk of infection [68,106,107]. Similarly, our measure of perceived shared fate with neighbors might not have captured only perceptions of positive interdependence but also perceptions of negative interdependence. ...
Article
Full-text available
Did the COVID-19 pandemic bring people together or push them apart? While infectious diseases tend to push people apart, crises can also bring people together through positive interdependence. We studied this question by asking an international sample (N = 1,006) about their inclinations to cooperate, perceptions of interdependence (i.e., shared fate), and perceived risk as well as local prevalence of COVID-19 infection across 14 time points from March to August, 2020. While perceived interdependence with others tended to increase during this time period, inclinations to cooperate decreased over time. At the within-person level, higher local prevalence of COVID-19 attenuated increases in perceived interdependence with others, and was associated with lower inclinations to cooperate. At the between-person level, people with high perceived interdependence with others reported more stable, or increasing, inclinations to cooperate over time than people with low perceived interdependence. Establishing a high sense of perceived interdependence with others may thus allow people to maintain cooperation during crises, even in the face of challenging circumstances such as those posed by a highly transmissible virus.
... One reason is that disgust needs to be downregulated or suppressed in certain instances even when there is an objective pathogen threat; for example, when caring for one's offspring (e.g., Al-Shawaf et al., 2016;Case et al., 2006). Similarly, disgust and behavioral avoidance may be downregulated toward people high in social value to us, including those high in social status or who are our friends (e.g., Al-Shawaf et al., 2019;Tybur et al., 2020). Second, some objective pathogen threats do not contain cues observable to the human eye (e.g., Norton et al., 2021), and in such cases we would not expect disgust to systematically track threat level. ...
Article
Full-text available
Disgust is a protective emotion that coordinates a suite of cognitive and behavioral changes to reduce people’s likelihood of infection. Existing research demonstrates that people react with disgust to pathogenic stimuli compared to non-pathogenic stimuli, but there has been a paucity of research examining whether people are equipped with a more finely graded disgust response: do people discriminate between pathogen threats of different magnitudes and react with more disgust toward pathogen threats of a greater magnitude? We derived multiple novel predictions from the Threat-Dependent Disgust hypothesis and tested them across three experiments involving participants from the United States and India (total n = 1,333). In Study 1 (n = 428), we tested the prediction that people would be more disgusted by a pathogen threat touching their hand relative to their foot, given that touching a pathogen with the hand is more likely to result in the pathogen entering the body envelope (e.g., through the mouth). In Study 2 (n = 453), we tested the prediction that people would be more disgusted by a pathogen threat touching another person’s hand relative to another person’s foot; people touch others more with their hands than with their feet, so a conspecific with a contaminated hand poses a greater disease threat than one with a pathogen on their foot. In Study 3 (n = 452), we tested the prediction that people would be more disgusted by skin wounds caused by pathogenic infections than by surgical incisions; although both wounds pose the risk of exposure to another’s bodily fluids, wounds caused by pathogenic infections pose a greater disease threat. Results across all three experiments suggest that disgust is sensitive to the magnitude of pathogen threat in a more finely tuned manner than previously demonstrated. Discussion focuses on interpretation of this gradient-like sensitivity of disgust, incorporating these findings into the broader disgust literature, and future directions.
... Additionally, apparent health status is not a valid measure of the threat of infectious diseases such as AIDS. Pathogen avoidance motives do not just lead to individuals labeling specific out-group members as dangerous but to the broader population (even in-group members such as parents and neighbors) (Tybur et al. 2020). This has led to the awareness that dependence on external resources and others can increase the risk of infection with dangerous infectious diseases (Sacco et al. 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigates the impact of perceived infectious disease threats on consumer preferences for user‐designed products. Drawing on both secondary and experimental data, we demonstrate that infectious disease threats enhance consumer preferences for user‐designed products (Studies 1 and 2), with the desire for self‐reliance mediating this process (Studies 3 and 4). Additionally, consumer mindset moderates this effect (Study 5). The impact of infectious disease threats on user‐designed product preference is evident only in consumers with a growth mindset and not in those with a fixed mindset. This study reveals consumer interest in self‐reliance and user‐designed products in the face of pathogen threats, which can help organizations adopt effective marketing practices to respond to challenges during pathogen epidemics.
... Disgust sensitivity is positively correlated with food neophobia, suggesting that neophobia may in fact be an expression of this emotion, which in turn may reduce WTP for foods produced using these methods (Björklund and Hursti, 2004;Tybur et al., 2020). Moreover, neophobia is not limited to the food itself, but also to the technologies used to produce it (Song et al., 2022;Cox and Evans, 2008). ...
... Thus, in our study, we predicted that it can significantly moderate the frequency of active oral sex. Specifically, the use of oral sex as a mate retention behavior may be more prevalent among men with lower vulnerability to disease, as they prioritize maintaining committed relationships over concerns about pathogen avoidance and health protection (Tybur et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
From the evolutionary perspective, maintaining a committed relationship is beneficial for reproductive success but involves risks such as losing a partner or infidelity. People typically prefer partners with similar mate value (MV) to avoid rejection. However, when a mate value discrepancy (MVD) arises, the partner with lower MV might employ mate retention strategies to maintain the relationship. This study investigated whether men with lower MV compared to their female partners used cunnilingus more often and whether this effect was mediated by their motivation to satisfy the partner. Additionally, it tested the moderating role of men’s perceived vulnerability to disease (PVD), predicting that men less concerned about disease would show a stronger link between MVD and cunnilingus frequency, given the health risks associated with oral sex. Data from 540 men in committed heterosexual relationships confirmed that a higher MVD—where the man's MV was lower than his partner's—led to more frequent cunnilingus, and this relationship was mediated by a greater motivation to sexually satisfy the partner. However, the moderating role of PVD was not confirmed. We explore the evolutionary perspective that men may perform oral sex on their partners as a mate retention strategy. This behavior potentially serves as a benefit-provisioning mechanism, compensating for discrepancies in mate value.
... Decisions to engage in threat-mitigating behaviors are shaped by many interacting endogenous and exogenous factors, including individuals' real and perceived vulnerability to different threats (e.g., Fessler et al., 2005;Sparks et al., 2018); informational and cultural environments that structure how various threats and threat responses are viewed (e.g., Gelfand et al., 2021); the goodness-of-fit between a particular threat response and an individual's preexisting epistemological schemas (Fessler & Machery, 2012;Lévi-Strauss, 1963); and assessments of the costs and benefits of various possible mitigations (Tybur et al., 2020). As illustrated by the consequences of widespread variation in how people have responded to recent global threats, it is vital to understand how threat-mitigating CONTACT Theodore Samore theo.samore@gmail.com ...
... Neither germ aversion nor perceived infectability moderated a relationship between MVD and the frequency of engaging in oral sex on a female partner mediated by participant's motivation to sexually satisfy a female partner (model 7 PROCESS;Hayes & Rockwood, 2017). stronger effects among men with lower PVD scores, who face fewer trade-offs between health protection and relationship maintenance (Tybur et al., 2020). ...
Poster
Full-text available
Heterosexual men's motivation to sexually satisfy a committed female partner through active oral sex can function as a strategy for mate retention and compensation for MVD among those with a lower MV compared to their partners. Specifically: The higher the MVD in favor of the female partner, the greater the frequency of men's engagement in active oral sex. The higher the MVD in favor of the female partner, the higher the men's motivation for sexual satisfaction of the female partner. MVD in favor of the partner predicts the frequency of men's engagement in active oral sex, and this relationship is mediated by men's motivation for sexual satisfaction of the partner. The relationship between MVD and the frequency of men's engagement in active oral sex is not moderated by PVD (neither germ aversion nor perceived infectability). Motivation to sexually satisfy a female partner as a mediator of a relationship between MVD and the frequency of engaging in active oral sex to a female partner (model 4 PROCESS; Hayes & Rockwood, 2017). The model is significant: F(1, 538) = 9.76, p = .002, R2 = 0.02 Frequency of oral sex on a female partner Motivation to sexually satisfy a female partner
... stronger effects among men with lower PVD scores, who face fewer trade-offs between health protection and relationship maintenance (Tybur et al., 2020). ...
Poster
Full-text available
Maintaining a romantic relationship has been and continues to be a significant adaptive challenge throughout human evolutionary history (Conroy-Beam et al., 2015; 2016). This challenge can be addressed through various strategies, such as selecting a partner with a similar mate value (MV) or monitoring potential differences in mate value (referred to as mate value discrepancy, MVD) between romantic partners (Buss and Shackelford, 1997). One particularly important factor that prompts the implementation of partner-retention strategies is the presence of disparities in mate value. In cases of MVD between romantic partners, both individuals in the relationship employ various compensation strategies (Goetz and Meyer, 2018). For example, when one partner has a lower MV than the other, these strategies aim to retain the mate, while in the case of the partner with a higher MV, they may lead to infidelity (Goetz and Meyer, 2018; Conroy-Beam et al., 2016). The aim of our project is to investigate whether the motivation to sexually satisfy a long-term romantic partner through active oral sex can function as a strategy for mate retention and compensation for MVD among heterosexual men with a lower MV compared to their long-term partner. Our planned correlational study intends to determine whether differences in MV in favor of the partner predict the frequency of engaging in active oral sex among heterosexual men and whether this relationship can be explained by the motivation to sexually satisfy the long-term romantic partner. The results obtained from this study will enhance our understanding of the strategies employed by heterosexual men in long-term relationships characterized by MVD on both sides. Furthermore, drawing upon the premises of the Behavioral Immune System theory (Ackerman et al., 2018), which pertains to a set of psychological mechanisms and behaviors that assist individuals in avoiding contact with infectious pathogens while considering the principles of situational and individual flexibility – recognizing that not everyone benefits equally from avoiding pathogens at all times (Curtis et al., 2011; Murray and Schaller, 2016), we hypothesize that the frequency of engaging in active oral sex (which carries the risk of exposure to health-threatening pathogens) predicted by MVD in favor of the partner among heterosexual men will be moderated by their subjective perceived vulnerability to disease (PVD) (Duncan et al., 2009). More precisely, we anticipate that this relationship will be more pronounced among men with lower PVD scores, as these men do not have to make a trade-off between protecting their own health by avoiding pathogens and benefiting from maintaining a long-term romantic relationship (Tybur et al., 2020).
Preprint
Full-text available
The global impact of COVID-19 has been profound, and the public health threat it represents is the most serious seen in a respiratory virus since the 1918 H1N1 influenza pandemic. Here we present the results of epidemiological modelling which has informed policymaking in the UK and other countries in recent weeks. In the absence of a COVID-19 vaccine, we assess the potential role of a number of public health measures-so-called non-pharmaceutical interventions (NPIs)-aimed at reducing contact rates in the population and thereby reducing transmission of the virus. In the results presented here, we apply a previously published microsimulation model to two countries: the UK (Great Britain specifically) and the US. We conclude that the effectiveness of any one intervention in isolation is likely to be limited, requiring multiple interventions to be combined to have a substantial impact on transmission. Two fundamental strategies are possible: (a) mitigation, which focuses on slowing but not necessarily stopping epidemic spread-reducing peak healthcare demand while protecting those most at risk of severe disease from infection, and (b) suppression, which aims to reverse epidemic growth, reducing case numbers to low levels and maintaining that situation indefinitely. Each policy has major challenges. We find that that optimal mitigation policies (combining home isolation of suspect cases, home quarantine of those living in the same household as suspect cases, and social distancing of the elderly and others at most risk of severe disease) might reduce peak healthcare demand by 2/3 and deaths by half. However, the resulting mitigated epidemic would still likely result in hundreds of thousands of deaths and health systems (most notably intensive care units) being overwhelmed many times over. For countries able to achieve it, this leaves suppression as the preferred policy option. We show that in the UK and US context, suppression will minimally require a combination of social distancing of the entire population, home isolation of cases and household quarantine of their family members. This may need to be supplemented by school and university closures, though it should be recognised that such closures may have negative impacts on health systems due to increased absenteeism. The major challenge of suppression is that this type of intensive intervention package-or something equivalently effective at reducing transmission-will need to be maintained until a vaccine becomes available (potentially 18 months or more)-given that we predict that transmission will quickly rebound if interventions are relaxed. We show that intermittent social distancing-triggered by trends in disease surveillance-may allow interventions to be relaxed temporarily in relative short time windows, but measures will need to be reintroduced if or when case numbers rebound. Last, while experience in China and now South Korea show that suppression is possible in the short term, it remains to be seen whether it is possible long-term, and whether the social and economic costs of the interventions adopted thus far can be reduced.
Article
Full-text available
Undetected cases The virus causing coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) has now become pandemic. How has it managed to spread from China to all around the world within 3 to 4 months? Li et al. used multiple sources to infer the proportion of early infections that went undetected and their contribution to virus spread. The researchers combined data from Tencent, one of the world's largest social media and technology companies, with a networked dynamic metapopulation model and Bayesian inference to analyze early spread within China. They estimate that ∼86% of cases were undocumented before travel restrictions were put in place. Before travel restriction and personal isolation were implemented, the transmission rate of undocumented infections was a little more than half that of the known cases. However, because of their greater numbers, undocumented infections were the source for ∼80% of the documented cases. Immediately after travel restrictions were imposed, ∼65% of cases were documented. These findings help to explain the lightning-fast spread of this virus around the world. Science , this issue p. 489
Article
Full-text available
Several species mitigate relationships according to their conspecifics' parasite status. Yet, this defence strategy comes with the costs of depriving individuals from valuable social bonds. Animals therefore face a trade-off between the costs of pathogen exposure and the benefits of social relationships. According to the models of social evolution, social bonds are highly kin-biased. However, whether kinship mitigates social avoidance of contagious individuals has never been tested so far. Here, we build on previous research to demonstrate that mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx) modulate social avoidance of contagious individuals according to kinship: individuals do not avoid grooming their close maternal kin when contagious (parasitized with oro-faecally transmitted protozoa), although they do for more distant or non-kin. While individuals' parasite status has seldom been considered as a trait impacting social relationships in animals, this study goes a step beyond by showing that kinship balances the effect of health status on social behaviour in a non-human primate.
Article
Full-text available
Decades of research document individual differences in prosocial behavior using controlled experiments that model social interactions in situations of interdependence. However, theoretical and empirical integration of the vast literature on the predictive validity of personality traits to account for these individual differences is missing. Here, we present a theoretical framework that identifies four broad situational affordances across interdependent situations (i.e., exploitation, reciprocity, temporal conflict, and dependence under uncertainty) and more specific sub-affordances within certain types of interdependent situations (e.g., possibility to increase equality in outcomes) that can determine when, which, and how personality traits should be expressed in prosocial behavior. To test this framework, we meta-analyzed 770 studies reporting on 3,523 effects of 8 broad and 43 narrow personality traits on prosocial behavior in interdependent situations modelled in six commonly studied economic games (Dictator Game, Ultimatum Game, Trust Game, Prisoner’s Dilemma, Public Goods Game, and Commons Dilemma). Overall, meta-analytic correlations ranged between -.18 ≤ r ≤ .26 and most traits yielding a significant relation to prosocial behavior had conceptual links to the affordances provided in interdependent situations, most prominently the possibility for exploitation. Moreover, for several traits, correlations within games followed the predicted pattern derived from a theoretical analysis of affordances. On the level of traits, we found that narrow and broad traits alike can account for prosocial behavior, informing the bandwidth-fidelity problem. In sum, the meta-analysis provides a theoretical foundation that can guide future research on prosocial behavior and advance our understanding of individual differences in human prosociality.
Article
Full-text available
The phenomenon of magical contagion – the unobserved passage of properties between entities that come into physical contact – was described by anthropologists over a century ago, yet questions remain about its origin, function, and universality. Contagion sensitivity, along with the emotion of disgust, has been proposed to be part of a biologically-evolved system designed to reduce exposure to pathogens by increasing the avoidance of “contaminated” objects. Yet this phenomenon has not been studied using systematic psychological comparison outside of industrialized populations. Here we document contagion sensitivity in two culturally, geographically, and economically distinct populations with little exposure to Western biomedicine and formal education: the Hadza hunter-gatherers of Tanzania and Tannese subsistence-agriculturalists of Vanuatu. In both populations, a majority of individuals rejected familiar and palatable foods when contaminating items touched the food but were subsequently removed. The Tannese children in our study showed a similar response, consistent with previous research with Western children. Our data support the proposal that contagion sensitivity is universal in human populations.
Article
Full-text available
The behavioral immune system is a motivational system that helps minimize infection risk by changing cognition, affect, and behavior in ways that promote pathogen avoidance. In the current paper, we review foundational concepts of the behavioral immune system and provide a brief summary of recent social psychological research on this topic. Next, we highlight current conceptual and empirical limitations of this work and delineate important questions that have the potential to drive major advances in the field. These questions include predicting the ontological development of the behavioral immune system, specifying the relationship between this system and the physiological immune system, and distinguishing conditions that elicit direct effects of situational pathogen threats versus effects that occur only in interaction with dispositional disease concerns. This discussion highlights significant challenges and underexplored topics to be addressed by the next generation of behavioral immune system research.
Article
Full-text available
What is the adapted structure of the behavioral immune system? By definition, the behavioral immune system motivates pathogen avoidance. One prominent suggestion is that the behavioral immune system also contains an additional component that generates motivation to avoid individuals from unfamiliar outgroups. The evolvability of this component has recently been questioned, however, and it has been noted that all supportive evidence stems from WEIRD samples. In this paper, we conducted between-subject experiments in large samples of adult residents of the USA (N = 1615) and India (N = 1969). In the experiment, we measured comfort with physical contact with a depicted individual. The individual was either from an ethnic ingroup or outgroup and either showed a pathogen cue or not. Results were inconsistent with the view that the behavioral immune system motivates the avoidance of individuals from unfamiliar outgroups. Instead, the results strongly supported that the system simply motivates the avoidance of any infected individual regardless of their group membership.
Article
Past research suggests that pathogen-avoidance motives (e.g., disgust sensitivity) relate to greater opposition to immigration. Two accounts have been proposed to explain this relationship, one of which emphasizes proximally avoiding outgroups, and the other of which emphasizes adherence to traditional norms. According to the former, immigrants are perceived as being more infectious because they carry novel pathogens due to their foreign ecological origins. According to the latter, immigrants' foreign norms are perceived as posing a pathogen threat. This study aimed to disentangle these accounts. Participants (N = 975) were randomly assigned to read a description of an immigrant who had high or low contact with locals and high or low assimilation to local norms. The effect of disgust sensitivity on sentiments toward the immigrant (and immigrants like him) was compared across conditions. Results supported the traditional norms account: disgust sensitivity related to anti-immigrant sentiments when the immigrant was described as not assimilating to local norms, but not when he was described as assimilating. Contrary to the outgroup avoidance account, the relationship between disgust sensitivity and anti-immigrant sentiments did not vary across the high-contact and low-contact conditions. Results suggest that resistance to foreign norms, rather than avoidance of novel pathogens, better explains the relationship between pathogen avoidance and outgroup prejudice.
Article
Here we examine the roles of interpersonal valuation and gratitude in the formation of cooperative relationships. Building on prior work, we draw on the concept of a welfare tradeoff ratio (WTR), an internally computed index of the extent to which one person values another person's welfare relative to his or her own. We test several predictions regarding the effects of benefit delivery on changes in WTR, gratitude, and subsequent cooperation. We show that benefit delivery by a stranger: (i) raises the beneficiary's valuation of the stranger's welfare, (ii) predicts downstream cooperative behavior by the beneficiary toward the stranger, and (iii) is coincident with beneficiaries' expressions of gratitude. We find evidence that cooperation and gratitude, while both sparked via benefit delivery and both underpinned by estimates of welfare valuation, are nevertheless produced in parallel via different paths. Specifically, the updated magnitude-not the initial magnitude or degree of change-of a beneficiary's WTR toward a stranger predicts the beneficiary's downstream cooperative behavior. By contrast, the extent to which the beneficiary's WTR positively changes-and not the initial or updated WTR magnitude-predicts gratitude production, a feature proposed to reinforce the benefactor's actions and foreshadow future cooperative intent on the part of the beneficiary. Taken together, our findings point to the possibility that cooperative behavior might operate via internal estimates of welfare valuation, and that gratitude signals benefit reception and the intent to engage in a cooperative relationship.
Article
The behavioral immune system includes motivational systems for avoiding contact with pathogens, including those transmitted by other people. Motivations to avoid others may depend not only on the perceived risk of infection but also on perceived benefits of social interaction. Based on this idea, we hypothesise that more agreeable people may experience less disgust towards other people, but not necessarily less disgust towards other pathogen sources. Using two existing samples, we tested whether agreeableness and other personality factors related more to disgust towards human than non-human pathogen cues. As predicted, agreeableness negatively correlated with disgust towards human pathogen cues but not with disgust towards non-human pathogen cues. In contrast, extraversion had no relation with disgust towards human or non-human pathogen cues. These findings suggest that disgust felt towards others may reflect a trade-off between pathogen avoidance and the preferred quality of interpersonal relationships, but not the preferred quantity of relationships. Implications of these findings for understanding the relation between disgust and interpersonal attitudes and behaviours are discussed.