Content uploaded by Gonzalo Mucientes
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Gonzalo Mucientes on Mar 24, 2021
Content may be subject to copyright.
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Journal : Large 12601 Article No : 2Pages : 3MS Code : 2Dispatch : 22-3-2021
Vol.:(0123456789)
1 3
Ocean Science Journal
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12601-021-00002-6
NOTE
Unusual Association ofSkipjack Tunas Katsuwonus pelamis
andaLongline Vessel
GonzaloMucientes1,2,3· NairVilas-Arrondo4
Received: 24 July 2020 / Revised: 1 September 2020 / Accepted: 1 September 2020
© Korea Institute of Ocean Science & Technology (KIOST) and the Korean Society of Oceanography (KSO) and Springer Nature B.V. 2021
Abstract
Skipjack tuna were observed for the first time swimming in front of the bow of a fishing vessel during a tagging campaign in
the North Atlantic. Video recordings show how tuna adapted their speed and direction to the vessel, using the pectoral fins
to move from top to bottom and back and forth across the bow of the ship. We discuss the potential drivers of this behaviour.
Keywords Tuna association· Behaviour· Katsuwonus pelamis· North Atlantic· Fishing vessel· Interaction
1 Introduction
The skipjack tuna, Katsuwonus pelamis (Linnaeus 1758)
is a medium-sized tuna species with a wide distribution in
the Atlantic Ocean. It is a short-lived species that matures
at ~ 2 years and 45–48cm (Hazin etal. 2001). It feeds on
fishes, crustaceans, cephalopods and molluscs, but, canni-
balism has also been reported (Hart 1973). Large schools of
skipjack tuna have been observed in the open ocean. Tuna
schools confer important advantages to the individuals such
as predator avoidance, cooperative hunting or resting (Godin
1997). In fact, many species of tunas aggregate under float-
ing objects and fishers take advantage of this behaviour by
deploying fishing aggregation devices (FADs) that attract
fish (Dagorn etal. 2007). Schools of skipjack tuna have
also been observed in surface waters with dolphins, birds,
sharks, whales, while performing characteristic behaviours
like jumping, feeding or foaming (Hall 1992) but also as
free-swimming schools not associated with either dolphins
or floating objects. The objective of the present work is
to report and describe unusual behaviour of skipjack tuna
observed in relation to a fishing vessel and discuss similar
behaviours across several species.
2 Materials andMethods
During a tagging survey for pelagic sharks conducted in
September 2011 off the Azores islands, North Atlantic
(location 35°N and 38°W), unusual swimming behaviour
in association with a fishing vessel was observed from the
boat deck. The observations were recorded with a compact-
camera Sony Cyber-Shot G and GoPro Hero 3 (in time lapse
mode) attached to a pole, in good weather conditions and sea
surface temperature of 26.5°C.
3 Results andDiscussion
At least 17 skipjack tuna were observed and recorded swim-
ming close to the bow during fishing operations and vessel
displacements. Fish followed the boat movements adapt-
ing to changes in the vessel speed and direction. The skip-
jack tuna remained within 5–20m in front of the vessel at
the surface during hours when the vessel was travelling at
6–8 miles/hour (video footage, see Electronic Supplemen-
tary Material, A1). Fishes moved deeper when the vessel
stopped, disappearing from the camera focus and appar-
ently swimming slowly under the boat. Then, biologists
introduced a pole with a small camera recording the school
swimming at ~ 5–10m depth under the bow of the vessel
Online ISSN 2005-7172
Print ISSN 1738-5261
* Gonzalo Mucientes
gmucientes@iim.csic.es
1 Institute ofMarine Research, Spanish National Research
Council (IIM-CSIC), 36208Vigo, Spain
2 Research Centre inBiodiversity andGenetic
Resources(CIBIO/InBio), University ofPorto,
4485-661Vairão, Portugal
3 Ecoloxía Azul - Blue Ecology, 36208Vigo, Spain
4 Spanish Institute ofOceanography, Oceanographic Center
ofVigo, 36390Vigo, Spain
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
A1
A2
A3
A4
A5
A6
A7
A8
A9
A10
Author Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Journal : Large 12601 Article No : 2Pages : 3MS Code : 2Dispatch : 22-3-2021
G. Mucientes and N. Vilas-Arrondo
1 3
(Fig.1a, b). The school of skipjack tunas was observed to
swim using the pectoral fins to adapt their speed to that of
the boat, as well as to move from left to right and back and
forward in front of the bow (video footage, https:// youtu. be/
6gk3f srqRos). The same behaviour was observed for several
consecutive days between 12 and 15 September—in all cases
with sea surface being calm. On one occasion, skipjack tunas
were observed to approach the catch of the longline (blue
shark, Prionace glauca) as it was being hauled (Fig.1c).
Even if it is relatively common for sport fishers to see
groups of tunas that swim near the boats in the open sea,
as far as we know, this is the first report of tunas swimming
in front of the bow of the vessel, and the first time it was
recorded. A similar behavioural association during brief
moments was previously reported in yellowfin tuna, Thunnus
albacares (Brill etal. 1999; Dagorn and Fréon 2001). One
possible explanation for our observation is that the skipjack
tuna considered the vessels a floating object. According to
the “meeting point” hypothesis (Dagorn and Fréon 1999),
there might be a social driver for the observed fish asso-
ciation with floating objects (drifting logs, anchored FADs,
vessels) as it may increase their chances of encountering co-
specifics and the opportunity for social interactions (Castro
etal. 2002; Robert etal. 2014). An alternative explanation
is that skipjack tuna used the vessel to move or migrate to
other places, as it has been observed in tunas associated
with dolphins. Specifically, yellowfin tuna use dolphins to
move from one floating object to the next (Scott etal. 2012).
The behaviour described in this work is apparently similar
to dolphins jumping in front of vessels, a common activ-
ity of small cetaceans (Luseau 2006). Given that tuna can
use dolphins to move or migrate, tuna might have imitated
dolphin behaviours.
Likewise, the observed phenomenon consisting in the
approach of the skipjack tuna to the catch of the vessel
(Fig.1c) could be compared to the behaviour of the pilot
fish, Naucrates ductor, an oceanic species with a semi-obli-
gate commensal relationship with sharks, rays, bill fish and
turtles (Vassilopopoulou etal. 2005) Finally, the reported
behaviour of skipjack tuna might be due to alterations in
anthropogenic sound. Indeed, boat noise can create a physi-
ological alteration in the bluefin tuna, Thunnus thynnus,
resulting in apparent avoidance behaviour that consists of
increased vertical movements in the water column that reach
the sea surface (Sará etal. 2007). Skipjack tuna could have
displayed similar avoidance diving behaviour at times during
our observations.
In conclusion, although we cannot arrive at definite con-
clusions about the ultimate drivers of the observed behaviour
of skipjack tuna swimming in front of the boat bow, our
observation expands the range of behaviours known about
this species.
Acknowledgements The crew of Ameal fishing vessel for their support
in the open ocean and to David Villegas-Ríos for providing insights to
the manuscript. GM was supported by a grant number ICETA 2017-49,
under the project I&D MarInfo–Integrated Platform for Marine Data
Acquisition and Analysis, NORTE-01-0145-FEDER-000031.
References
Brill RW, Block BA, Boggs CH, Bigelow KA, Fruend EV, Marcinek DJ
(1999) Horizontal movements and depth distribution of large adult
yellowfin tuna (Thunnus albacares) near the Hawaiian Islands,
recorded using ultrasonic telemetry: Implications for the physi-
ological ecology of pelagic fishes. Mar Biol 133:395–408
Castro JJ, Santiago JA, Santana-Ortega AT (2002) A general theory on
fish aggregation to floating objects: an alternative to the meeting
point hypothesis. Rev Fish Biol Fish 11:255–277. https:// doi. org/
10. 1023/A: 10203 02414 472
Fig. 1 A moment showing tuna close to the vessel while the vessel
was stopped. a skipjack tuna swimming under the vessel; b skipjack
tuna waiting under the stopped boat and ignoring the blue shark; c
instant where two tuna are moving towards a captured blue shark
AQ1
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
Author Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Journal : Large 12601 Article No : 2Pages : 3MS Code : 2Dispatch : 22-3-2021
Association of Tunas and Vessels
1 3
Dagorn L, Fréon P (1999) Tropical tuna associated with floating
objects: a simulation study of the meeting point hypothesis. Can
J Fish Aquat Sci 56:984–993
Dagorn L, Josse E, Bach P (2001) Association of yellowfin tuna (Thun-
nus albacares) with tracking vessels during ultrasonic telemetry
experiments. Fish Bull 99:40–48
Dagorn L, Holland KN, Itano DG (2007) Behavior of yellowfin (Thun-
nus albacares) and bigeye (T. obesus) tuna in a network of fish
aggregating devices (FADs). Mar Biol 151:595–606. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1007/ s00227- 006- 0511-1
Godin JJ (1997) Behavioural ecology of teleost fishes. Oxford Univer-
sity Press, Oxford, p 400
Hall MA (1992) The association of tunas with floating objects and
dolphins in the Eastern Pacific Ocean. Part VII. Some hypoth-
eses on the mechanisms governing the association of tunas with
floating objects and dolphins. In: Proceedings of the International
Workshop on the Ecology and Fisheries for Tunas associated
with Floating Objects, 11–13 Feb, Inter-American Tropical Tuna
Commission
Hart JL (1973) Pacific fishes of Canada. Fisheries Research Board of
Canada, 740 p
Hazin FHV, Hazin HG, Zagaglia CR, Travassos P, Júnior MFG (2001)
Analices des captures de lapêche à la senne réalisées par le “B.P.
Xixili” dans l`Ocean Atlantique Équatorial. Collect Vol Sci Pap
ICCAT 52:488–498
Lusseau D (2006) The short-term behavioral reactions of bottlenose
dolphins to interactions with boats in Doubtful Sound, New
Zealand. Mar Mammal Sci 22:802–818. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/j.
1748- 7692. 2006. 00052.x
Robert M, Dagorn L, Deneubourg JL (2014) The aggregation of tuna
around floating objects: what could be the underlying social mech-
anisms? J Theor Biol 359:161–170. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jtbi.
2014. 06. 010
Sarà G, Dean J, D’Amato D, Buscaino G, Oliveri A, Genovese S, Ferro
S, Buffa G, Lo Martire M, Mazzola S (2007) Effect of boat noise
on the behaviour of bluefin tuna Thunnus thynnus in the Mediter-
ranean Sea. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 331:243–253. https:// doi. org/ 10.
3354/ meps3 31243
Scott MD, Chivers SJ, Olson RJ, Fiedler PC, Holland K (2012) Pelagic
predator associations: tuna and dolphins in the eastern tropical
Pacific Ocean. Mar Ecol-Prog Ser 458:283–302. https:// doi. org/
10. 3354/ meps0 9740
Vassilopopoulou V, Siapatis A, Christides G, Bekas P (2005) The biol-
ogy and ecology of juvenile pilotfish (Naucrates ductor) associ-
ated with Fish Aggregating Devices (FADs) in eastern Mediterra-
nean waters. Mediterr Mar Sci 5:61–70. https:// doi. org/ 10. 12681/
mms. 211
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
Author Proof
Journal : Large 12601 Article No : 2Pages : 1MS Code : 2Dispatch : 22-3-2021
Journal: 12601
Article: 2
Author Query Form
Please ensure you fill out your response to the queries raised below and return this form along
with your corrections
Dear Author
During the process of typesetting your article, the following queries have arisen. Please check your typeset proof
carefully against the queries listed below and mark the necessary changes either directly on the proof/online grid or in the
‘Author’s response’ area provided below
Query Details Required Author’s Response
AQ1 AUTHOR: Vassilopouou etal. 2004 has been changed to Vassilopopoulou etal. 2005
so that this citation matches the Reference List. Please confirm that this is correct.
Author Proof