ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

It is known that the extrastriate cortex is activated by visual symmetry. This activation generates an ERP component called the Sustained Posterior Negativity (SPN). SPN amplitude increases (i.e., becomes more negative) with repeated presentations. We exploited this SPN priming effect to test whether the extrastriate symmetry response is gated by element luminance polarity. On each trial, participants observed three stimuli (patterns of dots) in rapid succession (500 ms. with 200 ms. gaps). The patterns were either symmetrical or random. The dot elements were either black or white on a grey background. The triplet sequences either showed repeated luminance (black > black > black, or white > white > white) or changing luminance (black > white > black, or white > black > white). As predicted, SPN priming was comparable in repeated and changing luminance conditions. Therefore, symmetry with black elements is not processed independently from symmetry with white elements. Source waveform analysis confirmed that this priming happened within the extrastriate symmetry network. We conclude that the network pools information across luminance polarity channels.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
964
|
Eur. J. Neurosci.. 2021;53:964–973.
wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/ejn
1
|
INTRODUCTION
Visual symmetry has been studied extensively (Bertamini,
Silvanto, Norcia, Makin, & Wagemans,2018; Cattaneo,2017;
Treder,2010). Symmetry is an important cue for object rec-
ognition and figure-ground segmentation (Driver, Baylis,
& Rafal, 1992; Koffka, 1935). Here we consider reflec-
tional (mirror) symmetry only, although it not the only type
(Mach,1886). In reflectional symmetry, there is a correlation
between element position on either side of the axis (Barlow
& Reeves,1979). Therefore models of symmetry perception
consider how local element position signals are integrated to
Received: 5 June 2020
|
Revised: 28 August 2020
|
Accepted: 29 August 2020
DOI: 10.1111/ejn.14966
RESEARCH REPORT
Electrophysiological priming effects confirm that the extrastriate
symmetry network is not gated by luminance polarity
Alexis D. J.Makin1
|
AndreaPiovesan2
|
JohnTyson-Carr1
|
GiuliaRampone1
|
YiovannaDerpsch1
|
MarcoBertamini1
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original
work is properly cited.
© 2020 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Abbreviations: ANOVA, analysis of variance; CI, confidence interval; CLARA, classical LORETA recursively applied; ECD, equivalent current dipole;
EEG, electroencephalography; EOG, electrooculogram; ERP, event related potential; fMRI, functional Magnetic Resonance imaging; ICA, independent
components analysis; LCD, liquid crystal display; LOC, lateral occipital complex; LORETA, low resolution electromagnetic tomographic analysis; PCA,
principal component analysis; PNG, portable network graphics; SPN, sustained posterior negativity; TMS, transcranial magnetic stimulation.
1Department of Psychological Sciences,
University of Liverpool, Liverpool, United
Kingdom
2Department of Psychology, Edge Hill
University, Ormskirk, United Kingdom
Correspondence
Alexis D. J. Makin, Department of
Psychological Sciences, Eleanor Rathbone
Building, University of Liverpool,
Liverpool, Merseyside L69 7ZA, United
Kingdom.
Email: Alexis.makin@liverpool.ac.uk
Funding information
Economic and Social Research Council,
Grant/Award Number: ES/S014691/1
Abstract
It is known that the extrastriate cortex is activated by visual symmetry. This activa-
tion generates an ERP component called the Sustained Posterior Negativity (SPN).
SPN amplitude increases (i.e., becomes more negative) with repeated presentations.
We exploited this SPN priming effect to test whether the extrastriate symmetry re-
sponse is gated by element luminance polarity. On each trial, participants observed
three stimuli (patterns of dots) in rapid succession (500ms. with 200ms. gaps). The
patterns were either symmetrical or random. The dot elements were either black or
white on a grey background. The triplet sequences either showed repeated lumi-
nance (black>black>black, or white>white>white) or changing luminance
(black>white>black, or white>black>white). As predicted, SPN priming was
comparable in repeated and changing luminance conditions. Therefore, symmetry
with black elements is not processed independently from symmetry with white ele-
ments. Source waveform analysis confirmed that this priming happened within the
extrastriate symmetry network. We conclude that the network pools information
across luminance polarity channels.
KEYWORDS
luminance polarity, reflection, regularity, repetition enhancement, sustained posterior negativity
|
965
MAKIN et Al.
form a global gestalt (van der Helm & Leeuwenberg,1996;
Wagemans, Van Gool, Swinnen, & Vanhorebeek,1993).
Early vision can be conceptualized as a retinotopic array
of spatial frequency and orientation tuned filters (Campbell
& Robson,1968). Building on this foundation, filter models
presume that the first stage in symmetry perception involves
low-pass filtering of the image. This extracts midpoint-co-
linear blobs that are aligned orthogonally to any axes of
reflection. Symmetry discrimination can then proceed by
estimating blob alignment (Dakin & Hess, 1997; Dakin &
Watt,1994; Osorio, 1996). Dakin and Hess (1998) suggest
that symmetry is extracted in this way from a narrow inte-
gration window elongated about the axis. Julesz and Chang
(1979) found that participants can ignore noise masks as long
as they differ in spatial frequency from the underlying sym-
metrical pattern. Likewise, Rainville and Kingdom (2000)
found that participants can ignore noise masks with different
orientations. These considerations suggest specific symmetry
representations are linked to specific spatial frequency filters.
Therefore, different symmetry representations may not over-
lap and perceptually interfere with each other.
Reflected elements can vary in their low-level visual prop-
erties, such luminance or colour. To accommodate these vari-
ations, filter models sometimes incorporate a full or half wave
rectification stage at the front end (e.g. Dakin & Hess,1997).
Among other things, such rectification eliminates the dif-
ference between black and white elements on a grey back-
ground. Global integration can then utilize contrast signals
rather than luminance signals. Early half-wave rectification is
also incorporated into some influential shape detection mod-
els (Poirier & Wilson, 2010; Wilson & Wilkinson, 2002).
These considerations suggest that there should be complete
overlap between symmetry representations with different lu-
minance polarity (even if there is no overlap between symme-
try representations with different spatial frequencies).
Furthermore, Glass pattern (Glass, 1969) aftereffects are
known to transfer perfectly between black and white exem-
plars (Clifford & Weston, 2005). This again suggests that
some types of global structure are coded independently of
element luminance polarity. This could be true of reflectional
symmetry as well.
Without referring to filter models directly, Tyler and
Hardage (1996) also addressed these topics. They distin-
guished first-order (luminance) mechanisms, which only find
symmetrical correspondences between elements of the same
luminance polarity (and operate over short distances), and
second-order (contrast) mechanisms, which can find sym-
metrical correspondences between elements of opposite lumi-
nance polarity (and can operate over long distances spanning
the whole visual field). Tyler and Hardage (1996) measured
symmetry sensitivity while varying density and eccentricity.
If second-order mechanisms dominate, then symmetry sen-
sitivity should improve at low densities, and this was indeed
the case. Tyler and Hardage (1996) also compared matched
luminance polarity conditions (black regions paired with
black, and white paired with white) and opposite luminance
polarity conditions (white paired with black, and black paired
with white). Symmetry detection could tolerate opposite lu-
minance polarity when density was low. This again empha-
sises the role of second-order mechanisms. In light of these
findings, Tyler and Hardage (1996) concluded that long-
range, second-order mechanisms predominate in symmetry
detection. This account again suggests reflectional symmetry
is coded independently of element luminance polarity.
Opposite luminance polarity symmetry is often termed
anti-symmetry. Symmetry and anti-symmetry are sometimes
perceptually equivalent, as found by Tyler and Hardage (1996).
However, there are many cases where anti-symmetry is per-
ceptually weaker. For instance, anti-symmetry discrimination
declines at high element density, when multiple greyscale lev-
els are used, and when elements are presented in the periphery
(Mancini, Sally, & Gurnsey,2005; Wenderoth,1996). It seems
that global symmetry detection mechanisms are sensitive to lu-
minance polarity (mis)matching across the axis. Apparently,
early filter-rectification and/or second order mechanisms do
not always render black and white elements informationally
identical and thereby abolish all anti-symmetry costs.
Following these themes, Gheorghiu, Kingdom, Remkes, Li,
and Rainville (2016) tested whether symmetry perception is se-
lective for low-level properties: For example, are symmetrical
arrangements of black dots coded by one neural mechanism,
and symmetrical arrangements of white dots coded by another
neural mechanism? In other words, we can ask whether sym-
metry perception mechanisms are gated by luminance polar-
ity. Wright, Mitchell, Dering, and Gheorghiu (2018) presented
evidence against selectivity. They claimed that “symmetry de-
tection mechanisms pool both luminance-polarities into one
channel, and thus, extrastriate visual areas sensitive to sym-
metry are not gated by luminance polarity” (page 487). This
non-selectivity hypothesis is partially anticipated by the work
mentioned above, such as the second-order predominance ac-
count of Tyler and Hardage (1996), and filter models with an
early rectification stage (Dakin & Hess,1997). For simplicity,
we use the term non-selectivity hypothesis to refer to this fam-
ily of related ideas.
The current project tested the non-selectivity hypothe-
sis. We measured an established ERP called the Sustained
Posterior Negativity (SPN): After 200 ms, amplitude is
lower at posterior electrodes when participants view sym-
metrical compared to asymmetrical stimuli. This pos-
terior negativity was first identified by Norcia, Candy,
Vildavski, and Tyler (2002), and has been replicated many
times (Jacobsen & Höfel,2003; Makin, Rampone, Morris,
& Bertamini, 2020; Makin, Wilton, Pecchinenda, &
Bertamini,2012; Makin etal.,2016). Source localization
shows that the SPN is generated in the extrastriate cortex.
966
|
MAKIN et Al.
This is consistent with fMRI work, which has identified
symmetry activations in a network of extrastriate regions,
including V4 and the shape-sensitive Lateral Occipital
Complex (LOC). This extrastriate symmetry response
was first found in an fMRI study by Tyler et al. (2005),
and then replicated by others (Keefe etal.,2018; Kohler,
Clarke, Yakovleva, Liu, & Norcia,2016; Sasaki, Vanduffel,
Knutsen, Tyler, & Tootell, 2005; Van Meel, Baeck,
Gillebert, Wagemans, & Op de Beeck,2019). SPN local-
ization is also consistent with TMS research, which has
found that disruption of the LOC impairs symmetry de-
tection (Bona, Cattaneo, & Silvanto,2015; Bona, Herbert,
Toneatto, Silvanto, & Cattaneo,2014).
Recently, we found that SPN amplitude increases (that is,
becomes more negative) with repeated presentation of sym-
metrical patterns (Bertamini, Rampone, Oulton, Tatlidil, &
Makin, 2019). We term this repetition enhancement effect
SPN priming. Following an established research strategy (e.g.
Kim, Biederman, Lescroart, & Hayworth, 2009; Kourtzi &
Kanwisher,2001), our recent work has exploited SPN priming
to assess the independence of different symmetry representa-
tions (Makin, Tyson-Carr, Rampone, Derpsch, & Bertamini,
2020). Experiment 1 of Makin, Tyson-Carr, et al. (2020) found
SPN priming with repeated presentation of different exemplars,
but not repeated presentation of identical exemplars (Figure1).
Other experiments in Makin, Tyson-Carr, et al. (2020) demon-
strated that SPN priming survives changes in regularity type,
but not changes in retinal location, or unpredictable changes in
axis orientation. In the current work, we tested whether SPN
priming would survive changes in element luminance polarity,
as predicted by the non-selectivity account.
We used different exemplars (Figure2), known to pro-
duce SPN priming (red wave in Figure1). Following Makin,
Tyson-Carr, et al. (2020), we used a secondary task that
was unrelated to symmetry: Our participants discriminated
between normal sequences with three patterns, and oddball
sequences, with a blank in the middle (Figure2b).
In the Repeated luminance condition, polarity was held
constant across the three presentations in a trial (e.g.
black>black>black or white>white>white). Conversely,
in the Changing luminance condition, polarity alternated
(e.g. black>white>black or white>black>white). The
non-selectivity account predicts that SPN priming should be
equivalent in both Repeated and Changing luminance con-
ditions. These predictions were pre-registered (https://aspre
dicted.org/2rh7e.pdf).1
2
|
METHOD
2.1
|
Participants
Twenty-two participants were involved (mean age 20, range
18–50, 2 males, 1 left-handed). The pre-registered aim of
testing 24 participants was abandoned prematurely due to the
COVID19 pandemic. The experiment had local ethics com-
mittee approval and was conducted in accordance with the
Declaration of Helsinki (2008).
2.2
|
Apparatus
Participants sat 57 cm from a 29 × 51 cm LCD monitor,
updating at 60Hz. A chin rest was used for head stabiliza-
tion. EEG data was recorded continuously at 512 Hz from
64 scalp electrodes (BioSemi Active-2 system, Amsterdam,
Netherlands). Horizontal and Vertical EOG external chan-
nels were used to monitor excessive blinking and eye move-
ments. The experiment was programmed in Python using
open source PsychoPy libraries (Peirce,2007).
1We also ran an Identical exemplars experiment on a different group of 22
participants. Here all three presentations were the same in terms of element
position. This experiment was also pre-registered. We predicted that there
would be no SPN priming, either in the Repeated or Changing luminance
conditions. However, the results were inconclusive. There was no SPN
priming when analysing the electrode cluster chosen a priori. However,
there was evidence of SPN priming in both Repeated and Changing
luminance conditions when a larger posterior cluster was used. There was
also SPN priming in the source waveforms. These results reduce our
confidence in the claim that SPN priming is eliminated when exemplars are
identical (i.e. the result shown in Figure1). However, they do not
contradict in our claim that SPN priming is independent of luminance
polarity, which is the main result reported in this paper.
FIGURE 1 Selective SPN priming effect when novel exemplars are shown. Data from Makin, Tyson-Carr, et al. (2020). Participants viewed a
sequence of three reflectional symmetries (left). The sequence could involve different reflections (red outline) or identical reflections (blue outline).
The SPN was the difference between reflection and random waves (middle). SPN amplitude increased (i.e. became more negative) with repeated
presentations of different reflections (red) but not with repeated presentations of the same reflection (blue). This is shown in the bar chart on the
right (error bars=95% CI)
|
967
MAKIN et Al.
2.3
|
Stimuli
Patterns were comprised of 160 non-overlapping black or
white Gabor elements on a circular grey background. The
circular disk was 8° in diameter. The regions where the ele-
ments could fall was 7.14° diameter. The visible dot at the
centre of each circular Gabor was approximately 0.25 de-
grees diameter. In PsychoPy RGB coordinates (which vary
from −1 to 1), the background was dark grey [−0.25, −0.25,
−0.25], the circular region mid-grey [0, 0, 0], the black ele-
ments were black [−1, −1, −1] and the white elements were
white [1, 1, 1]. Stimuli were generated offline and saved as
PNG files. The reflection patterns had horizontal and verti-
cal axes. We used 2-fold reflection in this study to increase
signal strength (SPN amplitude almost doubles as we go up
from 1 to 2 folds, e.g. Makin etal.,2016). All experiments
used the same set of images, but these were shuffled so they
played a different role in for each participant.
2.4
|
Procedure
The timeline of a single trial was based on Makin, Rampone,
Morris, et al. (2020). The 1,500ms fixation baseline was fol-
lowed by three 500ms patterns with 200ms gaps. In 80% of
trials, all three presentations in the sequence were reflection
or random patterns (Figure 2b top row). In the remaining
20% there was blank oddball between two random patterns
(Figure2b bottom row). The participant's task was to dis-
criminate normal from oddball trials (by pressing buttons
labelled “All patterns” or “Blank in the middle”). The re-
sponse mapping switched between trials, so sometimes the
left (A) key was used to report ‘Blank in middle’ and some-
times the right (L) key was used to report “Blank in middle.”
Each participant completed 600 trials in total. 480 were
normal trials, 120 were oddball trials. Only the normal trials
were analysed. Of the 480 trials, there were 60 in each of the 8
triplet types shown in Figure2a. There were thus 120 trials in
FIGURE 2 (a) Triplet sequences
used in this experiment. Colour coding
matches the ERP results in Figures 4 and
5. These are just examples, in the real
experiment, every trial involved different
patterns. (b) Example trial structure. The
participant's task was to discriminate
common “all pattern” trials, (top row, 80%)
from infrequent “blank in the middle” trials
(bottom row, 20%)
968
|
MAKIN et Al.
the 4 crucial conditions [(Reflection, Random) X (Repeated
luminance, Changing luminance)]. The experiment was bro-
ken into 30 blocks of 20 trials. Within each block, conditions
were presented in a randomized order. A single practice block
was presented before the experiment began.
2.5
|
ERP analysis (sensor level)
Pre-processing conventions were chosen a-priori and pre-
registered. EEG data from 64 channels was analysed of-
fline using EEGLAB 13.3.4b toolbox in Matlab (Delorme &
Makeig,2004). The data were re-referenced to the scalp aver-
age, low pass filtered at 25Hz, downsampled to 128Hz and
segmented into −0.5 to+2.1 s epochs with a −200ms pre-
stimulus baseline. Eye blinks and other large artefacts were
removed using Independent Components Analysis (Jung
etal.,2000). An average of 10.05 (min 4, max 16) compo-
nents were removed. Trials where amplitude exceeded±100
microvolts were removed from all analysis (11%–12%).
Oddball trials were not included in the ERP analysis. We did
not remove ERP trials if participants entered an incorrect re-
sponse because these infrequent mistakes probably happen
during response entry and excluding error trials would make
only a negligible difference to ERP waveforms.
The SPN was computed as the difference between reflec-
tion and random waves at posterior electrode cluster [PO7,
O1, O2 and PO8]. SPN in the Repeated luminance condition
was defined as Repeated luminance reflection – Repeated
luminance random (averaging over black > black > black
and white>white>white sequences). SPN in the Changing
luminance condition was defined as Changing luminance
reflection – Changing luminance random (averaging over
black>white>black and white>black>white sequences).
Three windows were chosen a-priori for statistical analysis
of SPN priming: First window=250–600ms, Second win-
dow=950–1300ms, Third window=1650–2000ms.
SPN was analysed with repeated measures ANOVAs. There
were 2 within subject factors [Sequence position (first, second
third) X 2 Sequence type (Repeated luminance, Changing lumi-
nance)]. The assumption of sphericity was met (Mauchly's test
p>.159) and none of SPNs in this analysis violated the assump-
tion of normality according to Shapiro Wilk test (p>.366).
2.6
|
Source waveform analysis (source level)
Source waveform analysis (implemented in BESA v. 7.0,
MEGIS GmbH, Munich, Germany) was used to investigate the
spatiotemporal dynamics of the SPN priming effect. Accurate
localisation of cortical sources requires data with a large signal-
to-noise ratio. Hence, the average difference wave (symme-
try—random) was computed across the repeated and changing
luminance conditions, thus producing the average waveform
representing symmetry-specific activity.
The construction of a source dipole model requires that
equivalent current dipoles (ECDs) are fitted to describe the
3-dimensional source currents from cortical regions contrib-
uting maximally to the observed data. To identify the num-
ber of contributing sources, principle component analysis
(PCA) was used. In accordance with previous fMRI literature
identifying the extrastriate cortex as being the origin of the
SPN response (Keefe etal.,2018; Kohler etal.,2016; Sasaki
etal.,2005; Tyler etal.,2005; Van Meel etal., 2019), two
ECDs were fitted bilaterally within the extrastriate cortices.
Classical LORETA analysis recursively applied (CLARA)
was used as an independent source localisation method to
confirm and adjust the locations of the ECDs. A source di-
pole model including bilateral ECDs within the extrastriate
cortices explained 91.9% of the variance in the observed
data. Since the PCA identified no other significantly con-
tributing sources, no further ECDs were fitted. Finally, the
orientation of the ECDs had to be determined. Due to differ-
ences in gyral anatomy between subjects, ECD orientation
was determined on an individual subject basis but with the
constraint of fixed ECD location between subjects. A 4-shell
ellipsoid head volume conductor model was employed using
the following conductivities (S/m = Siemens per meter):
Brain=0.33S/m; Scalp = 0.33 S/m; Bone =0.0042S/m,
Cerebrospinal Fluid = 1 S/m. Source waveforms for each
experiment and condition were exported and analysed using
repeated-measures ANOVAs.
3
|
RESULTS
3.1
|
ERP analysis (sensor level)
Grand average ERP waves are shown in Figure3a. SPN dif-
ference waves are shown in Figure3b. SPN amplitude in the
three intervals are shown in Figure3c.
All six SPNs in Figure 3c constitute significant brain
responses to symmetry (amplitude < 0, one sample t tests,
p < .001). As expected, SPN amplitude increased over the
three presentations in both Repeated and Changing lumi-
nance conditions. Repeated measures ANOVA found a main
effect of Sequence position (F(2,42) = 3.624, p = .035,
ηp
2 = 0.147, linear contrast, F(1,21) = 4.722, p = .041,
ηp
2=0.184), but no effect of Sequence type (F<1), and no
interaction (F<1).
Additional analysis found no difference between SPNs
generated by black and white reflections (collapsing over
Sequence position and Sequence type, −1.89 vs. −1.97 mi-
crovolts, t (21)=0.427, p=.674). This replicates previous
work, where SPN amplitude was independent of luminance,
contrast and colour, at least when magnitudes are matched in
|
969
MAKIN et Al.
reflected locations (Martinovic, Jennings, Makin, Bertamini,
& Angelescu,2018; Wright etal.,2018).
3.2
|
Individual participant level analysis,
effect size and power
The left column in Figure3 shows ERP data in a standard
format. The right column in Figure3 shows the same ERP
data, but with a richer visualization of between participant
variation, as recommended by Rousselet, Foxe, and Bolam
(2016). Note individual subject waves behind grand averages
in Figure3a (and the necessarily extended vertical scale to ac-
commodate), 95% CI around difference waves in Figure3b,
and violin plots which represent distribution of individual-
participant SPNs in Figure3c.
Depending on condition, between 19/22 and 21/ 22 par-
ticipants had an SPN (reflection < random). This is sig-
nificantly more than the 11/22=0.5 expected by chance
(p=.001, binomial test). Just 16/22 participants demon-
strated an SPN priming effect (defined by a negative se-
quence position slope). This was only marginally greater
than 0.5 (p=.052, binomial test). This marginal non-para-
metric effect suggests that our experiment was statistically
underpowered. Furthermore, observed power for the sig-
nificant parametric main effect of Sequence position was
just 0.638, while the observed power of the linear contrast
was just 0.545. However, SPN priming was replicated by
Makin, Tyson-Carr, et al. (2020) in all the expected con-
ditions (plus some unexpected conditions) across five ex-
periments with 48 participants in each. Here effect size
ranged from 0.110 to 0.290. We are thus confident that
FIGURE 3 ERP results. Left and right
columns show the same data; however,
the right column has enriched visual
representation of between-participant
variability. (a) Left: Grand average ERP
waves from posterior electrode cluster [PO7,
O1, O2 and PO8]. Right: individual waves
are presented behind the grand averages,
and scale is expanded to accommodate
this variance. (b) Left: SPN as a difference
wave. The three intervals when the pattern
was presented are marked. Right: 95% CI
waves are presented behind the difference
waves (dashed lines). (c) Left: SPN in first,
second and third intervals (Error bars=95%
CI based on procedure of Morey,2008).
Right: a violin plot which represents density
of individual participant data points. Black
horizontal dashed lines indicate repeated
and changing condition averages. SPN
priming manifests as the widest bulge of the
violin moving from above to below the line.
Note that amplitude increases by around
0.5 microvolts in both repeated luminance
(red) and changing luminance (green)
conditions
970
|
MAKIN et Al.
SPN priming is a real effect, although future research that
exploits SPN priming should obtain larger samples than
our 22.
3.3
|
Source waveform analysis (source level)
As discussed in Makin, Tyson-Carr, et al. (2020), the SPN
priming effect is ambiguous when analysed at the sen-
sor level. Does the increase in amplitude reflect increase
in activation at extrastriate sources (as we assume) or later
activation of additional dipoles elsewhere in the cortex?
As in Makin, Tyson-Carr, et al. (2020), we ran additional
source-level analysis to confirm that the observed SPN
priming effect does indeed reflect increasing amplitude of
the extrastriate symmetry response itself. This analysis also
allowed us to assess hemispheric differences. This is impor-
tant, because previous work has reported some right later-
alization of the extrastriate symmetry response (Bertamini &
Makin,2014; Bona etal.,2015; Verma, Van der Haegen, &
Brysbaert,2013; Wright, Makin, & Bertamini,2015).
A source dipole model was constructed comprised of two
bilateral ECDs within the extrastriate regions. This model
explained 91.9% of variance in the average difference wave
across the repeated and changing conditions. Both the left
ECD1 (Brodmann area 19; Talairach – x=−27.7, y=−61.9,
z=9.9) and the right ECD2 (Brodmann area 19; Talairach
– x=31.2, y=−61.8, z=−7.2) were located within the fu-
siform gyrus (see Figure4a). The source waveforms for each
ECD are illustrated in Figure4b. It can be seen that the SPN
priming effect is bilateral, and comparable in Repeated and
Changing luminance conditions.
A three-way repeated measures ANOVA [Sequence type
(Repeated luminance; Changing luminance) X Sequence
position (First; Second; Third) X Hemisphere (Left ECD1;
Right ECD2)] was carried out on the source waveforms.
There was a main effect of Sequence position (F(1.371,
28.790)=5.455, p=.018, ƞp
2=0.206). Although the right
hemisphere response was numerically stronger, there were no
other effects or interactions (p≥.096).
4
|
DISCUSSION
One branch of previous work has discovered SPN priming:
SPN amplitude increases with repeated presentation of novel
symmetrical exemplars (Figure1). Another branch of previ-
ous work supports the non-selectivity hypothesis, and sug-
gests that the extrastriate symmetry network pools element
position information across low-level channels (Gheorghiu
etal.,2016; Wright etal.,2018). We combined both branches
of previous work and found new support for the non-selectiv-
ity hypothesis with SPN priming.
SPN priming was comparable when luminance polarity
was repeated or changed in the triplet sequences. This sug-
gests that black and white symmetries are not coded by inde-
pendent neural systems.2 Furthermore, source waveform
analysis confirmed the SPN priming results at the source
level and found no additional cortical sources. Therefore,
SPN enhancement was due to increased activation within the
extrastriate symmetry network. This is consistent with simi-
lar analysis in Makin, Tyson-Carr, et al. (2020).
TMS work has found that the disruption of LOC reduces
symmetry repetition effects as measured behaviourally
(Cattaneo, Mattavelli, Papagno, Herbert, & Silvanto,2011).
This also suggests repetition effects are mediated within
the extrastriate symmetry network, in line with our source
level analysis. However, the study by Cattaneo etal.(2011)
raises some caveats here that require further discussion.
In Cattaneo et al. (2011), participants were slower to dis-
criminate target symmetry when it was congruent with
the adaptor (e.g. Vertical > Vertical) than when it was
2Our experiment did not distinguish between a) element luminance and b)
element luminance polarity (i.e., the direction of luminance difference from
the grey background). Here white dots were a luminance increment
compared to the grey background (positive polarity), and black dots were a
luminance decrement compared to the grey background (negative polarity).
It would be possible to devise an experiment like ours, but where both dot
luminance levels constituted an increment [e.g. a black background with
dark grey dots (increment 1) or light grey dots (increment 2)]. Likewise, it
would be possible to devise an experiment like ours where both dot
luminance levels constituted a decrement [e.g. a white background with
light grey dots (decrement 1) or dark grey dots (decrement 2)]. We are
confident that SPN priming would be transfer across large changes in
element luminance even when these changes do not constitute a polarity
reversal. However, this has not been empirically established by the current
experiment.
FIGURE 4 Source model. (a) Final
source dipole model comprising two
extrastriate sources. (b) Source waveforms
for each ECD and mean amplitude within
the defined intervals. Error bars = ± 1SD.
(c) Mean scalp map for each sequence type
and latency interval
|
971
MAKIN et Al.
incongruent (Vertical > Horizontal). It is not clear why
Cattaneo etal.(2011) found this when our results would pre-
dict the opposite (a congruence advantage). It could be that
Cattaneo et al.’s participants had to inhibit the task-irrelevant
adaptor, and this was more difficult when it matched the
target orientation. In our study, the repeated trials were pre-
sented passively, so the symmetry response could accumulate
without such cognitive complications.
It has been shown that SPN amplitude is similar for lumi-
nance defined (achromatic) and colour defined (isoluminant)
stimuli when contrast greatly exceeds threshold (Martinovic
etal.,2018). This suggests that the symmetry network is in-
different to luminance and colour, as the non-selectivity hy-
pothesis claims. However, the ERP similarity demonstrated
by Martinovic etal.(2018) is only an approximate indicator
of neural similarity. Transfer of repetition effects, as demon-
strated in the current study, is more convincing evidence for
the non-selectivity hypothesis.
The non-selectivity hypothesis follows from other work.
For instance, Tyler and Hardage (1996) found that sec-
ond-order, polarity insensitive mechanisms are predominant
during some symmetry discrimination tasks. Furthermore,
filter models of symmetry perception can incorporate early
half or full wave rectification (Dakin & Hess,1997; Wilson
& Wilkinson, 2002). This work also suggests that regular-
ity coding should transcend element luminance polarity and
predicts priming should transfer across changes in luminance
polarity.
The current results are also in line with those of Clifford
and Weston (2005), who found that Glass pattern aftereffects
survive changes in luminance polarity. There are many sim-
ilarities between neural coding of reflectional symmetry and
Glass patterns (Rampone & Makin, 2020), so it is perhaps
unsurprising that both are indifferent to element luminance
polarity.
While the extrastriate symmetry network is not lumi-
nance polarity selective, we stress that it is sometimes sen-
sitive to luminance polarity (mis)matching across the axis.
This is revealed by experiments on anti-symmetry. As men-
tioned, some psychophysical work has found that anti-sym-
metry discrimination thresholds are elevated, especially
when element density is high (Gheorghiu et al., 2016;
Mancini etal., 2005; Wenderoth,1996). The SPN is gen-
erated by anti-symmetry, but amplitude is reduced (Makin,
Rampone, & Bertamini,2020; Makin etal.,2016; Wright
et al., 2018). Therefore, any future models of symmetry
perception must account for both sensitivity to luminance
polarity mismatching, AND the fact that extrastriate sym-
metry response is not gated by luminance polarity. The
future theoretical challenge is to accommodate both these
robust empirical findings.
Finally, we note that the visual effects of illumination and
shading are typically unstable in real environments. When
looking at a real object, the wavelengths reflected from its
surfaces change quickly, while the spatial relationships be-
tween its parts change slowly. It is perhaps adaptive for vi-
sual object recognition mechanisms to be tuned to spatial
relationships between parts, and ignore relatively superficial
variability in luminance, contrast and colour.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This project was part funded by an ESRC grant (ES/
S014691/1). We would like to thank University of Liverpool
3rd year students Claudia Barchieri and Sophie McCullough
who helped with data collection.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Alexis Makin designed the experiment, analysed the re-
sults and wrote the manuscript. John Tyson-Carr collected
some data and conducted the source localization analysis.
Yiovanna Derpsch and Andrea Piovesan helped with data
collection. Giulia Rampone assisted with EEG analysis and
interpretation of results. Marco Bertamini programmed the
stimuli, helped with interpretation of results and writing of
the manuscript. All authors contributed to the final report.
PEER REVIEW
The peer review history for this article is available at https://
publo ns.com/publo n/10.1111/ejn.14966
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The experiments, stimuli and raw pre-processed EEG data are
available on Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/2yjus/).
We are happy for other researchers to use this material.
ORCID
Alexis D. J. Makin https://orcid.
org/0000-0002-4490-7400
REFERENCES
Barlow, H. B., & Reeves, B. C. (1979). Versatility and absolute ef-
ficiency of detecting mirror symmetry in random dot displays.
Vision Research, 19(7), 783–793. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-
6989(79)90154 -8
Bertamini, M., & Makin, A. D. J. (2014). Brain activity in response
to visual symmetry. Symmetry, 6, 975–996. https://doi.org/10.3390/
sym60 40975
Bertamini, M., Rampone, G., Oulton, J., Tatlidil, S., & Makin, A. D.
J. (2019). Sustained response to symmetry in extrastriate areas
after stimulus offset: An EEG study. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 1–11.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s4159 8-019-40580 -z
Bertamini, M., Silvanto, J., Norcia, A. M., Makin, A. D. J., &
Wagemans, J. (2018). The neural basis of visual symmetry and
its role in mid- and high-level visual processing. Annals of the
972
|
MAKIN et Al.
New York Academy of Sciences, 1426(1), 111–126. https://doi.
org/10.1111/nyas.13667
Bona, S., Cattaneo, Z., & Silvanto, J. (2015). The causal role of the
occipital face area (OFA) and lateral occipital (LO) cortex in sym-
metry perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35(2), 731–738.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUR OSCI.3733-14.2015
Bona, S., Herbert, A., Toneatto, C., Silvanto, J., & Cattaneo, Z. (2014).
The causal role of the lateral occipital complex in visual mirror sym-
metry detection and grouping: An fMRI-guided TMS study. Cortex,
51, 46–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.11.004
Campbell, F. W., & Robson, J. G. (1968). Application of fourier analy-
sis to the visibility of gratings. The Journal of Physiology, 197(3),
551–566. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphys iol.1968.sp008574
Cattaneo, Z. (2017). The neural basis of mirror symmetry detection: A
review. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 29(3), 259–268. https://
doi.org/10.1080/20445 911.2016.1271804
Cattaneo, Z., Mattavelli, G., Papagno, C., Herbert, A., & Silvanto,
J. (2011). The role of the human extrastriate visual cortex in
mirror symmetry discrimination: A TMS-adaptation study.
Brain and Cognition, 77(1), 120–127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
bandc.2011.04.007
Clifford, C. W. G., & Weston, E. (2005). Aftereffect of adaptation to
Glass patterns. Vision Research, 45(11), 1355–1363. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.visres.2004.12.016
Dakin, S. C., & Herbert, A. M. (1998). The spatial region of integration
for visual symmetry detection. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 265(1397), 659–664. https://
doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1998.0344
Dakin, S. C., & Hess, R. F. (1997). The spatial mechanisms mediating
symmetry perception. Vision Research, 37(20), 2915–2930. https://
doi.org/10.1016/s0042 -6989(97)00031 -x
Dakin, S. C., & Watt, R. J. (1994). Detection of bilateral symme-
try using spatial filters. Spatial Vision, 8(4), 393–413. https://doi.
org/10.1163/15685 6894x 00071
Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source tool-
box for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including indepen-
dent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1),
9–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneum eth.2003.10.009
Driver, J., Baylis, G. C., & Rafal, R. D. (1992). Preserved figure-ground
segregation and symmetry perception in visual neglect. Nature,
360(6399), 73–75. https://doi.org/10.1038/360073a0
Gheorghiu, E., Kingdom, F. A. A., Remkes, A., Li, H.-C.-O., &
Rainville, S. (2016). The role of color and attention-to-color in mir-
ror-symmetry perception. Scientific Reports, 6, 29287. https://doi.
org/10.1038/srep2 9287
Glass, L. (1969). Moire effect from random dots. Nature, 223, 578–580.
Jacobsen, T., & Höfel, L. (2003). Descriptive and evaluative judg-
ment processes: Behavioral and electrophysiological indices
of processing symmetry and aesthetics. Cognitive Affective &
Behavioral Neuroscience, 3(4), 289–299. https://doi.org/10.3758/
CABN.3.4.289
Julesz, B., & Chang, J.-J. (1979). Symmetry perception and spa-
tial-frequency channels. Perception, 8(6), 711–718. https://doi.
org/10.1068/p080711
Jung, T. P., Makeig, S., Humphries, C., Lee, T. W., McKeown, M. J.,
Iragui, V., & Sejnowski, T. J. (2000). Removing electroencepha-
lographic artifacts by blind source separation. Psychophysiology,
37(2), 163–178. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3720163
Keefe, B. D., Gouws, A. D., Sheldon, A. A., Vernon, R. J. W., Lawrence,
S. J. D., McKeefry, D. J., … Morland, A. B. (2018). Emergence of
symmetry selectivity in the visual areas of the human brain: fMRI
responses to symmetry presented in both frontoparallel and slanted
planes. Human Brain Mapping, 39(10), 3813–3826. https://doi.
org/10.1002/hbm.24211
Kim, J. G., Biederman, I., Lescroart, M. D., & Hayworth, K. J. (2009).
Adaptation to objects in the lateral occipital complex (LOC): Shape
or semantics? Vision Research, 49(18), 2297–2305. https://doi.
org/10.1016/J.VISRES.2009.06.020
Koffka, P. J. (1935). Priciples of Gestalt Psychology. New York:
Harcourt, Brace & Company.
Kohler, P., Clarke, A., Yakovleva, A., Liu, Y., & Norcia, A. M. (2016).
Representation of maximally regular textures in human visual
cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36(3), 714–729. https://doi.
org/10.1523/JNEUR OSCI.2962-15.2016
Kourtzi, Z., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). Representation of perceived object
shape by the human lateral occipital complex. Science, 293(5534),
1506–1509. https://doi.org/10.1126/scien ce.1061133
Mach, E. (1886). The analysis of sensations and the relation of the
phsysical to the psychical. New York: Dover.
Makin, A. D. J., Rampone, G., & Bertamini, M. (2020). Symmetric
patterns with different luminance polarity (anti-symmetry) generate
an automatic response in extrastriate cortex. European Journal of
Neuroscience, 51(3), 922–936. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14579
Makin, A. D. J., Rampone, G., Morris, A., & Bertamini, M. (2020).
The formation of symmetrical gestalts is task independent, but
can be enhanced by active regularity discrimination. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 32(2), 353–366. https://doi.org/10.1162/
jocn_a_01485
Makin, A. D. J., Tyson-Carr, J., Rampone, G., Derpsch, Y., & Bertamini,
M. (2020). PsyArXiv Preprints | A new ERP repetition paradigm to
assess independence of regularity representations in the extrastriate
cortex. Retrieved May 28, 2020, from https://psyar xiv.com/a8n6b/.
Makin, A. D. J., Wilton, M. M., Pecchinenda, A., & Bertamini, M.
(2012). Symmetry perception and affective responses: A combined
EEG/EMG study. Neuropsychologia, 50(14), 3250–3261. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro psych ologia.2012.10.003
Makin, A. D. J., Wright, D., Rampone, G., Palumbo, L., Guest, M.,
Sheehan, R., … Bertamini, M. (2016). An electrophysiological
index of perceptual goodness. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 4416–4434.
https://doi.org/10.1093/cerco r/bhw255
Mancini, S., Sally, S. L., & Gurnsey, R. (2005). Detection of symmetry
and anti-symmetry. Vision Research, 45(16), 2145–2160. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2005.02.004
Martinovic, J., Jennings, B. J., Makin, A. D. J., Bertamini, M., &
Angelescu, I. (2018). Symmetry perception for patterns defined by
color and luminance. Journal of Vision, 18(8), 1–24. https://doi.
org/10.1167/18.8.4
Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data:
A correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutorials in Quantitative
Methods for Psychology, 4(2), 61–64. https://doi.org/10.20982/
tqmp.04.2.p061
Norcia, A. M., Candy, T. R., Pettet, M. W., Vildavski, V. Y., & Tyler, C.
W. (2002). Temporal dynamics of the human response to symmetry.
Journal of Vision, 2(2), 132–139. https://doi.org/10.1167/2.2.1
Osorio, D. (1996). Symmetry detection by categorization of spatial phase,
a model. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
263(1366), 105–110. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1996.0017
Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy - Psychophysics software in Python.
Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1–2), 8–13. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jneum eth.2006.11.017
|
973
MAKIN et Al.
Poirier, F., & Wilson, H. R. (2010). A biologically plausible model of
human shape symmetry perception. Journal of Vision, 10(1), https://
doi.org/10.1167/10.1.9
Rainville, S. J. M., & Kingdom, F. A. A. (2000). The functional role
of oriented spatial filters in the perception of mirror symmetry —
psychophysics and modeling. Vision Research, 40(19), 2621–2644.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042 -6989(00)00110 -3
Rampone, G., & Makin, A. D. J. (2020). Electrophysiological responses
to regularity show specificity to global form: The case of Glass pat-
terns. European Journal of Neuroscience, 52(3), 3032–3046. https://
doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14709
Rousselet, G. A., Foxe, J. J., & Bolam, J. P. (2016). A few simple
steps to improve the description of group results in neuroscience.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 44(9), 2647–2651. https://doi.
org/10.1111/ejn.13400
Sasaki, Y., Vanduffel, W., Knutsen, T., Tyler, C. W., & Tootell, R.
(2005). Symmetry activates extrastriate visual cortex in human
and nonhuman primates. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(8), 3159–3163.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.05003 19102
Treder, M. S. (2010). Behind the looking glass: A review on human sym-
metry perception. Symmetry, 2, 1510–1543. https://doi.org/10.3390/
sym20 31510
Tyler, C. W., Baseler, H. A., Kontsevich, L. L., Likova, L. T., Wade, A.
R., & Wandell, B. A. (2005). Predominantly extra-retinotopic cor-
tical response to pattern symmetry. NeuroImage, 24(2), 306–314.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro image.2004.09.018
Tyler, C. W., & Hardage, L. (1996). Mirror symmetry detection:
Predominance of second-order pattern processing throughout the
visual field. In C. W. Tyler (Ed.), Human Symmetry Perception
and Its Computational Analysis (pp. 157–171). Utrecht: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
van der Helm, P. A., & Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1996). Goodness of vi-
sual regularities: A nontransformational approach. Psychological
Review, 103(3), 429–456. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.
103. 3.429
Van Meel, C., Baeck, A., Gillebert, C. R., Wagemans, J., & Op de
Beeck, H. P. (2019). The representation of symmetry in multi-voxel
response patterns and functional connectivity throughout the ventral
visual stream. NeuroImage, 191, 216–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuro image.2019.02.030
Verma, A., Van der Haegen, L., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). Symmetry
detection in typically and atypically speech lateralized individuals:
A visual half-field study. Neuropsychologia, 51(13), 2611–2619.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro psych ologia.2013.09.005
Wagemans, J., Van Gool, L., Swinnen, V., & Vanhorebeek, J. (1993).
Higher-order structure in regularity detection. Vision Research,
33(8), 1067–1088. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(93)90241 -n
Wenderoth, P. (1996). The effects of the contrast polarity of dot-pair
partners on the detection of bilateral symmetry. Perception, 25(7),
757–771. https://doi.org/10.1068/p250757
Wilson, H. R., & Wilkinson, F. (2002). Symmetry perception: A novel
approach for biological shapes. Vision Research, 42(5), 589–597.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042 -6989(01)00299 -1
Wright, D., Makin, A. D. J., & Bertamini, M. (2015). Right-
lateralized alpha desynchronization during regularity discrimi-
nation: Hemispheric specialization or directed spatial attention?
Psychophysiology, 52, 638–647. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12399
Wright, D., Mitchell, C., Dering, B. R., & Gheorghiu, E. (2018).
Luminance-polarity distribution across the symmetry axis affects
the electrophysiological response to symmetry. NeuroImage, 173,
484–497. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEURO IMAGE.2018.02.008
How to cite this article: Makin ADJ, Piovesan A,
Tyson-Carr J, Rampone G, Derpsch Y, Bertamini M.
Electrophysiological priming effects confirm that the
extrastriate symmetry network is not gated by luminance
polarity. Eur. J. Neurosci.2021;53:964–973. https://doi.
org/10.1111/ejn.14966
... Reflection and Glass generated very similar SPN waves. (B) SPN priming effect in the work by Makin et al. [24]. Repeated presentation of different reflectional symmetries leads to increase in SPN amplitude (SPN priming, red). ...
... However, SPN priming did transfer from a vertical to a horizontal reflection as well as between reflectional and rotational symmetry. In another study, we found that SPN priming transferred between black and white exemplars [24]. SPN priming is consistent with behavioural results, where the repeated presentation of different exemplars can enhance symmetry detection [45]. ...
... In the current experiment, we present trials with three regularities in sequence ( Figure 2). As in the work conducted by Makin, Piovesan, Tyson-Carr, Rampone, Derpsch, and Bertamini [24], participants completed an oddball detection task, where they noted infrequent trials where the second pattern was blank ( Figure 3). We confidently predicted that we would see SPN priming on repeated regularity sequences ( Glass), given the putative differences between these regularities. ...
Article
Full-text available
The extrastriate visual cortex is activated by visual regularity and generates an ERP known as the sustained posterior negativity (SPN). Spatial filter models offer a biologically plausible account of regularity detection based on the spectral properties of an image. These models are specific to reflection and therefore imply that reflectional symmetry and Glass patterns are coded by different neural populations. We utilised the SPN priming effect to probe representational overlap between reflection and Glass patterns. For each trial, participants were presented with a rapid succession of three patterns. In the Repeated condition, three reflections or three Glass patterns were presented. In the Changing condition, patterns alternated between reflection and Glass patterns. An increase in SPN amplitude (priming) was observed in both the Repeated and Changing conditions. Results indicate a greater representational overlap in the brain between reflection and Glass patterns than predicted by spatial filter models.
... To avoid confusion, we should mention a phenomenon called SPN priming (i.e. increase in SPN amplitude with presentation of symmetrical images in succession [86][87][88] ). This form of past-history effect reflects interdependency of responses to fully-visible image symmetries. ...
... The repetition of identical information along the central axis may have caused an enhancement (priming) effect on the ERP response in t2. Recent research has shown that when three symmetrical images are presented in succession, SPN amplitude increases 43,86,87 . This effect has been termed SPN priming 87 , and suggests that amplitude of the extra-striate symmetry response is partially determined by the immediately preceding stimulus. ...
Article
Full-text available
Extrastriate visual areas are strongly activated by image symmetry. Less is known about symmetry representation at object-level rather than image-level. Here we investigated electrophysiological responses to symmetry, generated by amodal completion of partially-occluded polygon shapes. We used a similar paradigm in four experiments (N = 112). A fully-visible abstract shape (either symmetric or asymmetric) was presented for 250 ms (t0). A large rectangle covered it entirely for 250 ms (t1) and then moved to one side to reveal one half of the shape hidden behind (t2, 1000 ms). Note that at t2 no symmetry could be extracted from retinal image information. In half of the trials the shape was the same as previously presented, in the other trials it was replaced by a novel shape. Participants matched shapes similarity (Exp. 1 and Exp. 2), or their colour (Exp. 3) or the orientation of a triangle superimposed to the shapes (Exp. 4). The fully-visible shapes (t0–t1) elicited automatic symmetry-specific ERP responses in all experiments. Importantly, there was an exposure -dependent symmetry-response to the occluded shapes that were recognised as previously seen (t2). Exp. 2 and Exp.4 confirmed this second ERP (t2) did not reflect a reinforcement of a residual carry-over response from t0. We conclude that the extrastriate symmetry-network can achieve amodal representation of symmetry from occluded objects that have been previously experienced as wholes.
... In other recently published work, we found SPN priming transferred between black and white reflections. This shows the regularity code is independent of luminance polarity [36] as anticipated by previous work using other methods [37][38][39]. It seems that different regularities are coded by overlapping neural populations, but only when they are presented at the same retinal location. ...
... As mentioned, Makin et al. [36] used SPN priming technique to demonstrate that the regularity code is luminance independent. Future work could use the SPN priming effect to answer other questions about symmetry coding. ...
Article
Full-text available
An Event Related Potential (ERP) component called the Sustained Posterior Negativity (SPN) is generated by regular visual patterns (e.g. vertical reflectional symmetry, horizontal reflectional symmetry or rotational symmetry). Behavioural studies suggest symmetry becomes increasingly salient when the exemplars update rapidly. In line with this, Experiment 1 (N = 48) found that SPN amplitude increased when three different reflectional symmetry patterns were presented sequentially. We call this effect ‘SPN priming’. We then exploited SPN priming to investigate independence of different symmetry representations. SPN priming did not survive changes in retinal location (Experiment 2, N = 48) or non-orthogonal changes in axis orientation (Experiment 3, N = 48). However, SPN priming transferred between vertical and horizontal axis orientations (Experiment 4, N = 48) and between reflectional and rotational symmetry (Experiment 5, N = 48). SPN priming is interesting in itself, and a useful new method for identifying functional boundaries of the symmetry response. We conclude that visual regularities at different retinal locations are coded independently. However, there is some overlap between different regularities presented at the same retinal location.
... This suggests that symmetry perception mechanisms operate at a level that is insensitive to such differences, in higher stages of visual processing. This is consistent with other work showing that the symmetry response is indifferent to the visual channels that carry element position information: for instance, the symmetry response does not distinguish between black and white elements (Makin et al., 2021). ...
... However, it does transfer between horizontal and vertical reflection, and between vertical reflection and 90-degree rotation [159]. Furthermore, SPN priming also transfers from black to white exemplars [184], and between symmetry and Glass patterns [185]. In these papers, we noted that the transfer of SPN priming is more liberal than the filter models allow. ...
Article
Full-text available
This review of symmetry perception has six parts. Psychophysical studies have investigated symmetry perception for over 100 years (part 1). Neuroscientific studies on symmetry perception have accumulated in the last 20 years. Functional MRI and EEG experiments have conclusively shown that regular visual arrangements, such as reflectional symmetry, Glass patterns, and the 17 wallpaper groups all activate the extrastriate visual cortex. This activation generates an event-related potential (ERP) called sustained posterior negativity (SPN). SPN amplitude scales with the degree of regularity in the display, and the SPN is generated whether participants attend to symmetry or not (part 2). It is likely that some forms of symmetry are detected automatically, unconsciously, and pre-attentively (part 3). It might be that the brain is hardwired to detect reflectional symmetry (part 4), and this could contribute to its aesthetic appeal (part 5). Visual symmetry and fractal geometry are prominent in hallucinations induced by the psychedelic drug N,N-dimethyltryptamine (DMT), and visual flicker (part 6). Integrating what we know about symmetry processing with features of induced hallucinations is a new frontier in neuroscience. We propose that the extrastriate cortex can generate aesthetically fascinating symmetrical representations spontaneously, in the absence of external symmetrical stimuli.
... This suggests that symmetry perception mechanisms operate at a level that is insensitive to such differences, in higher stages of visual processing. This is consistent with other work showing that the symmetry response is indifferent to the visual channels that carry element position information: for instance, the symmetry response does not distinguish between black and white elements (Makin et al., 2021). ...
Article
Human perception of symmetry is associated with activation in an extended network of extrastriate visual areas. This activation generates an ERP called the Sustained Posterior Negativity (SPN). In most studies so far, the stimuli have been defined by luminance. We tested whether the SPN is present when stimuli are defined by stereoscopic disparity using random dot stereograms (RDS). In Experiment 1, we compared the SPN signal for contours specified by binocular disparity and contours specified by monocular cues. The SPN was equivalent, suggesting that the type of contour does not alter the SPN signal. In Experiment 2 we exploited the unique property of RDS to provide unambiguous figure-ground arrangements. Psychophysical work has shown that symmetry is more easily detected when it is a property of a single object (i.e., within a figure), compared to a property of a gap between two objects (i.e., the ground). Therefore, the target regions in this experiment could either be foreground or background. The SPN onset was delayed when the symmetry was in a ground region. This may be because object formation interferes with the processing of shape information in the ground region.
... The repetition of identical information along the central axis may have caused an enhancement (priming) effect on the ERP response in t2. Recent research has shown that repeated presentations of symmetrical patterns increase SPN amplitude 43,86,87 ; this effect has been termed SPN priming 86 . This response enhancement, however, is greater when the second exemplar is different from the rst one 43 . ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Extrastriate visual areas are strongly activated by image symmetry. Less is known about symmetry representation at object-, rather than image-, level. Here we investigated electrophysiological responses to symmetry, generated by amodal completion of partially-occluded polygon shapes. We used a similar paradigm in four experiments (N=112). A fully-visible abstract shape (either symmetric or asymmetric) was presented for 250ms (t0). A large rectangle covered it entirely for 250ms (t1) and then moved to one side to reveal half of the shape hidden behind (t2, 1000ms). Note that at t2 no symmetry could be inferred from retinal image information. In half of the trials the shape was the same as previously presented, in the other trials it was replaced by a novel shape. Participants matched shapes similarity (Exp. 1 and Exp. 2), or their colour (Exp. 3) or the orientation of a triangle superimposed to the shapes (Exp. 4). The fully-visible shapes (t0-t1) elicited automatic symmetry-specific ERP responses in all experiments. Importantly, there was an exposure -dependent symmetry-response to the occluded shapes that were recognised as previously seen (t2). Exp. 2 and Exp.4 confirmed this second ERP (t2) did not reflect a reinforcement of a residual carry-over response from t0. We conclude that the extrastriate symmetry-network can achieve amodal representation of symmetry from occluded objects that have been previously experienced as wholes.
Article
Full-text available
Until recently, the transformational approach provided the only available formal analysis of visual regularities like repetition and mirror symmetry. This theoretical study presents a new analysis, based on the recently developed concept of holographic regularity. This concept applies to the intrinsic character of regularity and specifies the unique formal status of perceptually relevant regularities. The crucial point is that the two analyses imply the same structure for repetition but a different structure for mirror symmetry. Transformationally, mirror symmetry is an all-or-nothing property, whereas holographically, it is a graded property. This difference pervades the understanding of both perfect regularities and perturbed regularities. Whereas the transformational approach explains hardly any goodness phenomenon, the holographic approach explains a wide variety of goodness phenomena in a coherent way that is ecologically plausible as well.
Article
Full-text available
The holographic weight of evidence model (van der Helm & Leeuwenberg, J Math Psychol, 35, 1991, 151; van der Helm & Leeuwenberg, Psychol Rev, 103, 1996, 429) estimates that the perceptual goodness of moiré structures (Glass patterns), irrespective of their global form, is comparable to that of reflection symmetry. However, both behavioural and neuroscience evidences suggest that certain Glass forms (i.e. circular and radial structures) are perceptually more salient than others (i.e. translation structures) and may recruit different perceptual mechanisms. In this study, we tested whether brain responses for circular, radial and translation Glass patterns are comparable to the response for onefold bilateral reflection symmetry. We recorded an event‐related potential (ERP), called the sustained posterior negativity (SPN), which has been shown to index perceptual goodness of a range of regularities. We found that circular and radial Glass patterns generated a comparable SPN amplitude to onefold reflection symmetry (in line with the prediction of the holographic model), starting approx. 180 ms after stimulus onset. Conversely, the SPN response to translation Glass patterns had a longer latency (approx. 400 ms). These results show that Glass patterns are a special case of visual regularity, and perceptual goodness may not be fully explained by the holographic identities that constitute it. Specialised processing mechanisms might exist in the regularity‐sensitive extrastriate areas, which are tuned to global form configurations.
Article
Full-text available
The brain can organize elements into perceptually meaningful gestalts. Visual symmetry is a useful tool to study gestalt formation, and we know that there are symmetry-sensitive regions in the extrastriate cortex. However, it is unclear whether symmetrical gestalt formation happens automatically, whatever the participant's current task is. Does the visual brain always organize and interpret the retinal image when possible, or only when necessary? To test this, we recorded an ERP called the sustained posterior negativity (SPN). SPN amplitude increases with the proportion of symmetry in symmetry + noise displays. We compared the SPN across five tasks with different cognitive and perceptual demands. Contrary to our predictions, the SPN was the same across four of the five tasks but selectively enhanced during active regularity discrimination. Furthermore, during regularity discrimination, the SPN was present on hit trials and false alarm trials but absent on miss and correct rejection trials. We conclude that gestalt formation is automatic and task-independent, although it occasionally fails on miss trials. However, it can be enhanced by attention to visual regularity.
Article
Full-text available
People can quickly detect bilateral reflection in an image. This is true when elements of the same luminance are matched on either side of the axis (symmetry) and when they have opposite luminance polarity (anti‐symmetry). Using electroencephalography, we measured the well‐established sustained posterior negativity (SPN) response to symmetry and anti‐symmetry. In one task, participants judged the presence or absence of regularity (Regularity Discrimination Task). In another, they judged the presence or absence of rare colored oddball trials (Colored Oddball Task). Previous work has concluded that anti‐symmetry is only detected indirectly, through serial visual search of element locations. This selective attention account predicts that the anti‐symmetry SPN should be abolished in the Colored Oddball Task because there is no need to search for anti‐symmetry. However, this prediction was not confirmed: The symmetry and anti‐symmetry SPN waves were not modulated by task. We conclude that at least some forms of anti‐symmetry can be extracted from the image automatically, in much the same way as symmetry. This is an important consideration for models of symmetry perception, which must be flexible enough to accommodate opposite luminance polarity, while also accounting for the fact anti‐symmetry is often perceptually weaker than symmetry.
Article
Full-text available
Electrophysiological (EEG) studies of human perception have found that amplitude at posterior electrodes is more negative for symmetrical patterns compared to asymmetrical patterns. This negativity lasts for hundreds of milliseconds and it has been called sustained posterior negativity (SPN). Symmetry activates a network of visual areas, including the lateral occipital complex (LOC). The SPN is a response to presence of symmetry in the image. Given the sustained nature of this activation, in this study we tested the persistence of the SPN after stimulus offset. Two shapes were presented (for 0.5 s each) with a 1 s blank interval in between. We observed a sustained response after stimulus offset, irrespective of whether the task required processing of shape information. This supports the idea that the response to symmetry is generated by information in the image, independently of task, and that it is sustained over approximately one second post stimulus onset.
Article
Full-text available
Perception of visual symmetry is fast and efficient and relies on both early low-level and late mid- and high-level neural mechanisms. To test for potential influences of early low-level mechanisms on symmetry perception, we used isoluminant, achromatic, and combined (color + luminance) patterns in a psychophysical and an event-related-potential (ERP) experiment. In the psychophysical experiment, pattern contrast was fixed at individual symmetry-discrimination threshold. Participants then judged whether a pattern was symmetric or random. Stimuli at isoluminance were associated with a large bias toward symmetry, achromatic stimuli introduced the opposite bias, and stimuli containing a balance of both color and luminance were perceived without bias. These findings are in line with distinct contrast sensitivity functions for color and luminance, with color providing low-frequency information useful for symmetry detection and luminance providing high-frequency information useful for detection of detail. The subsequent ERP experiment was run at high contrasts to assess processing of symmetry in suprathreshold conditions. Sustained posterior negativity, a symmetry-sensitive ERP component, was observed in all conditions and showed the expected dependence on symmetry. However, interactions between symmetry and contrast type were not observed. In conclusion, while our findings at threshold support models that propose an important contribution of low-level mechanisms to symmetry perception, at suprathreshold these low-level contributions do not persist. Therefore, under everyday viewing conditions, symmetry perception engages a relatively broad cortical network that is not constrained by low-level inputs.
Preprint
An ERP component called the Sustained Posterior Negativity (SPN) indexes the brain response to regularity in visual patterns (e.g. vertical reflectional symmetry, horizontal reflectional symmetry or rotational symmetry). However it is unclear if different regularities are coded by independent or overlapping neural populations. Previous work has successfully exploited repetition effects to assess representational independence, and we adapted this approach to investigate visual regularity. Experiment 1 (N = 48) found that SPN amplitude increased when three reflectional symmetry patterns were presented sequentially. This SPN priming effect did not survive changes in retinal location (Experiment 2, N = 48) or non-orthogonal changes in axis orientation (Experiment 3, N= 48). However, SPN priming transferred between vertical and horizontal axis orientation (Experiment 4, N= 48) and between reflectional and rotational symmetry (Experiment 5, N= 48). We conclude that visual regularities at different retinal locations are coded independently. However, there is some overlap between different regularities presented at the same retinal location.
Article
Several computational models explain how symmetry might be detected and represented in the human brain. However, while there is an abundance of psychophysical studies on symmetry detection and several neural studies showing where and when symmetry is detected in the brain, important questions remain about how this detection happens and how symmetric patterns are represented. We studied the representation of (vertical) symmetry in regions of the ventral visual stream, using multi-voxel pattern analyses (MVPA) and functional connectivity analyses. Our results suggest that neural representations gradually change throughout the ventral visual stream, from very similar part-based representations for symmetrical and asymmetrical stimuli in V1 and V2, over increasingly different representations for symmetrical and asymmetrical stimuli which are nevertheless still part-based in both V3 and V4, to a more holistic representation for symmetrical compared to asymmetrical stimuli in high-level LOC. This change in representations is accompanied by increased communication between left and right retinotopic areas, evidenced by higher interhemispheric functional connectivity during symmetry perception in areas V2 and V4.