Content uploaded by Kristen Bottema-Beutel
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Kristen Bottema-Beutel on Sep 05, 2020
Content may be subject to copyright.
Avoiding Ableist Language:
Suggestions for Autism Researchers
Kristen Bottema-Beutel, PhD,
1
Steven K. Kapp, PhD,
2
Jessica Nina Lester, PhD,
3
Noah J. Sasson, PhD,
4
and Brittany N. Hand, PhD, OTR/L
5
Abstract
In this commentary, we describe how language used to communicate about autism within much of autism research
can reflect and perpetuate ableist ideologies (i.e., beliefs and practices that discriminate against people with
disabilities), whether or not researchers intend to have such effects. Drawing largely from autistic scholarship on
this subject, along with research and theory from disability studies and discourse analysis, we define ableism and its
realization in linguistic practices, provide a historical overview of ableist language used to describe autism, and
review calls from autistic researchers and laypeople to adopt alternative ways of speaking and writing. Finally, we
provide several specific avenues to aid autism researchers in reflecting on and adjusting their language choices.
Keywords: autism, ableism, language, ableist discourse, neurodiversity
Lay Summary
Why is this topic important?
In the past, autism research has mostly been conducted by nonautistic people, and researchers have described
autism as something bad that should be fixed. Describing autism in this way has negative effects on how society
views and treats autistic people and may even negatively affect how autistic people view themselves. Despite
recent positive changes in how researchers write and speak about autism, ‘‘ableist’’ language is still used.
Ableist language refers to language that assumes disabled people are inferior to nondisabled people.
What is the purpose of this article?
We wrote this article to describe how ableism influences the way autism is often described in research. We also
give autism researchers strategies for avoiding ableist language in their future work.
What is the perspective of the authors?
We believe that ableism is a ‘‘system of discrimination,’’ which means that it influences how people talk about
and perceive autism whether or not they are aware of it, and regardless of whether or not they actually believe
that autistic people are inferior to nonautistic people. We also believe that language choices are part of what
perpetuates this system. Because of this, researchers need to take special care to determine whether their
language choices reflect ableism and take steps to use language that is not ableist.
What is already known about this topic?
Autistic adults (including researchers and nonresearchers) have been writing and speaking about ableist lan-
guage for several decades, but nonautistic autism researchers may not be aware of this work. We have compiled
this material and summarized it for autism researchers.
1
Lynch School of Education and Human Development, Boston College, Chestnut Hill, Massachusetts, USA.
2
Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, United Kingdom.
3
School of Education, Indiana University at Bloomington, Bloomington, Indiana, USA.
4
School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, University of Texas at Dallas, Richardson, Texas, USA.
5
School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences at The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio, USA.
ªKristen Bottema-Beutel et al., 2020; Published by Mary Ann Liebert, Inc. This Open Access article is distributed under the terms of
the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits any non-
commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and the source are cited.
AUTISM IN ADULTHOOD
Volume 00, Number 00, 2020
Mary Ann Liebert, Inc.
DOI: 10.1089/aut.2020.0014
1
What do the authors recommend?
We recommend that researchers understand what ableism is, reflect on the language they use in their written and
spoken work, and use nonableist language alternatives to describe autism and autistic people. For example, many
autistic people find terms such as ‘‘special interests’’ and ‘‘special needs’’ patronizing; these terms could be
replaced with ‘‘focused interests’’ and descriptions of autistic people’s specific needs. Medicalized/deficit lan-
guage such as ‘‘at risk for autism’’ should be replaced by more neutral terms such as ‘‘increased likelihood of
autism.’’ Finally, ways of speaking about autism that are not restricted to particular terms but still contribute to
marginalization, such as discussion about the ‘‘economic burden of autism,’’ should be replaced with discourses
that center the impacts of social arrangements on autistic people.
How will these recommendations help autistic people now or in the future?
Language is a powerful means for shaping how people view autism. If researchers take steps to avoid ableist
language, researchers, service providers, and society at large may become more accepting and accommodating
of autistic people.
Introduction
The purpose of this commentary is to define, describe,
and offer alternatives to ableist language used in autism
research. According to the Center for Disability Rights, ableism
‘‘is comprised of beliefs and practices that devalue and dis-
criminate against people with physical, intellectual, or psy-
chiatric disabilities and often rests on the assumption that
disabled people need to be ‘fixed’ in one form or the other.’’
1
The effects of ableism on autistic people include, but are not
limited to, underemployment, mental health conditions, and
victimization.
2–6
The motivation for this article stems from
ongoing discussions between autism researchers and the au-
tistic community,
7–9
with noteworthy contributions from indi-
viduals who belong to both groups.
10–15
We prioritize the
perspectives of autistic people because they have first-hand
expertise about autism and have demonstrated exceptional
scientific expertise.
16
Autistic adults have led advocacy against
ableist language, with broad applicability across age groups.
The autistic community advocates for autism research that is
accessible, inclusive of autistic participation and perspectives,
reflective of the priorities of the autistic community, of high
quality, and written in such a way that it does not contribute to
the stigmatization of autistic people.
10,12,17–19
Although there
has been progress on these fronts,
20
some of the language used
to describe autism and autistic people within published research
continues to increase marginalization.
15,21–24
In an effort to improve researchers’ language practices, we
discuss these issues in five sections. First, we discuss the rel-
evance of ableism to autism research. Second, we discuss the
ramifications of ableist language choices and give a historical
overview of how such language persists in autism research.
Third, we review empirical research on the language prefer-
ences of the autistic community. Fourth, we discuss recent
language debates, focusing on objections made by autism re-
searchers to some nonableist language options. Finally, we
provide practical strategies for avoiding ableist language and
provide suggested alternatives in Table 1, which will be dis-
cussed in detail in the Suggestions for Researchers section.
We bring expertise in special education, psychology, and
occupational therapy and hope to express a range of concerns
across these disciplines. The second author is also an autistic
researcher, with expertise in neurodiversity and autism ad-
vocacy. While we do not claim to be comprehensive in our
discussion of these issues, we can provide insight on a range
of language practices that occur across disciplines, which
may render our discussion useful for those in a variety of
research traditions.
What Does Ableism Have to Do
with Autism Research?
Ableism is perpetuated by culturally shared norms and
values, as well as ways of speaking and writing about dis-
ability and disabled people. These social processes culmi-
nate in, and originate from, societal expectations about the
abilities required for granting individuals full social rights,
agency, and even personhood.
25
Ableism intersects with
other systems of oppression, including racism, sexism, ho-
mophobia, and transphobia.
26,27
This means that ableism is
compounded by experiences such as racism. In addition,
disabled people of color are more likely to experience the
effects of ableism than white disabled people.
28
Understanding the concept of ableism, and how it manifests
in language choices, is critical for researchers who focus on
marginalized groups such as the autistic community. Since
autism was first identified by researchers, deficit discourses
have pervaded descriptions of autistic research participants.
29
In fact, some interpretations of research findings have tacitly
or explicitly questioned the humanity of autistic people.
30–32
Autistic rhetorician Melanie Yergeau offers an example from
theory of mind research, which showed differences between
autistic and nonautistic groups on false belief task perfor-
mance. This finding was used as evidence that autistic people
lack an essential element that ‘‘makes us human.’’
14
This type
of ableist discourse can have far-reaching negative impacts on
disability policy, education, therapeutic practices, and social
attitudes about autistic people.
25,33
When, for instance, re-
search findings are presented as evidence that autistic people are
‘‘emotionless,’’
34
they may influence public perceptions about
autistic people that are incorrect and negatively impact their
ability to form relationships and participate in society. In this
way, language choices can perpetuate stigma, increase mar-
ginalization, and contribute to negative internalized self-beliefs
within autistic people.
35–37
In many parts of the world, ableism
is a default system of discrimination that can be reinforced
through language and other symbolic modalities.
38,39
Re-
searchers who wish to counteract this system must therefore
2 BOTTEMA-BEUTEL ET AL.
adopt an anti-ableist stance and be intentional about their lan-
guage choices. We believe that such a commitment will result
in less discriminatory and more accurate discussions of re-
search findings.
The Impact and History of Language
Used to Talk About Autism
Why does the language we use to talk about
autism matter?
Language is not simply descriptive but is also performa-
tive.
40
That is, language use is constructive of social life;
through language we make a case, take a particular stance,
and produce identities.
41
What people say or write produces
specific versions of the world, one’s self, and others,
42
and
language conveys, shapes, and perpetuates ideologies.
43
Language choices are also reflective of power structures and
mirror dominant narratives and ideologies about social phe-
nomena. From this critical perspective, ideologies are con-
ceptualized in such a way that includes consideration of the
role of power in the ‘‘positions, attitudes, beliefs, perspec-
tives, etc. of social groups’’
42
(p. 9). Thus, ideologies evi-
denced in everyday and institutional discourse are assumed to
both establish and maintain power relationships.
42
Disrupting
dominant discourses about autism, primarily controlled by
those in positions of power, is therefore necessary to change
Table 1. Potentially Ableist Terms and Discourse That Commonly Appear
in Autism Research and Suggested Alternatives
Potentially ableist term/discourse Suggested alternatives
Patronizing language
Special interests
99
Areas of interest or areas of expertise, focused, intense, or
passionate interests
Special needs
98,100,101
Description of specific needs and disabilities
Challenging behavior/disruptive behavior/problem
behavior
7,37,102,103
Meltdown (when uncontrollable behavior), stimming (when
relevant), specific description of the behavior (e.g., self-
injurious or aggressive behavior)
Person-first language (to refer to autism)
8,17,65,72,104–107
Identity-first language; ‘‘on the autism spectrum’’
Medicalized/deficit-based language
High/low functioning; high/low severity or support
needs
9,17,84,85
Describe specific strengths and needs, and acknowledgment
that the level of support needs likely varies across
domains (e.g., requires substantial support to participate
in unstructured recreation activities, but minimal support
to complete academic work)
‘‘At risk’’ for ASD
73
Increased likelihood/chance of autism
Burden of/suffering from autism
108
Impact, effect
Co-morbid
109,110
Co-occurring
Autism symptoms
17
Specific autistic characteristics, features, or traits
Treatment Support, services, educational strategies (when applicable)
Healthy controls/normative sample
111,112
Nonautistic (if determined via screening), neurotypical
(if determined via extensive screening ruling out most
forms of neurodivergence), comparison group (with
description of relevant group characteristics)
Psychopathology
98
Neurodevelopmental conditions, neuropsychiatric
conditions, developmental disabilities, mental illnesses
(or specific mental health condition)
Ableist discourses: ways of discussing autism not relegated to the use of particular terms, that reflect and/or contribute to
dehumanization, oppression, or marginalization of autistic people
Discussions about economic impacts of autism that
situate costs in the existence of autistic people
themselves, or compare the costs to those of
potentially fatal diseases/conditions such as cancer or
stroke.
113
Discussions about economic impacts of autism that situate
costs in society’s systemic failure to accommodate
autistic people and that recognize the people most
affected by oppression due to this failure are autistic
people themselves (not ‘‘taxpayers’’)
Interpretations of all group differences between autistic
and nonautistic groups as evidence of autistic
deficits
20,22,29,114
Interpretations of group differences that consider the
possibility that autistic people may have relative strengths
over nonautistic people or that differences between
groups are value-neutral unless actively demonstrated
otherwise
Cure/recovery/‘‘optimal outcome’’ rhetoric.
115,116
Discussions focusing on quality-of-life outcomes that
prioritize what autistic people want for themselves
Prioritizing ‘‘passing’’ as nonautistic (e.g., some ‘‘social
skills’’ training) at the expense of mental health and
well-being.
35,117,118
Prioritizing mental health and well-being, which can include
embracing autistic identities
Autism as a puzzle.
119,120
Autism as part of neurodiversity
Autism as an epidemic.
121
Autism as increasingly recognized/diagnosed
AVOIDING ABLEIST LANGUAGE 3
conceptualizations about the nature of autism. Additionally,
all communication involves language choices, and there are
no ‘‘neutral’’ options independent of an ideological stance.
44
The fact that representations of research participants and
results are shaped by research biases, and may not accurately
reflect how participants perceive themselves, has long been
emphasized in qualitative methodological literature.
45–47
Furthermore, social science researchers produce various ver-
sions of reality, theory, and descriptions of people and places
when writing research reports.
48
As such, researchers’ lan-
guage choices shape how people and places come to be known.
Historical reflections on language about autism
Autism arose as a clinical category in the 1940s, but de-
scriptions of autism have varied across time and place and are
shaped by complex disciplinary histories and discourses.
49
The
works of Leo Kanner and Hans Asperger continue to influence
how autism is understood. While Kanner is typically credited
with classifying autism as a diagnostic category, Eugene Bleuler
first coined the term in 1911, using it to describe ‘‘psychotic
patients’’ tendency to withdraw into fantastical worlds.
50
Dur-
ing the 1920s and 1930s, the term ‘‘schizophrenic autism’’ was
used to describe children who appeared to separate from reality
and become affectively withdrawn.
Even recently, autism is often written about using medical
model frameworks, which construe all differences associated
with autism to be evidence of deficits, and advocates for curing
these deficits via intervention.
51
In keeping with the medical
model, autism has linguistically, culturally, and politically been
constructed in relation to a normal/abnormal binary.
52
Can-
guilhem
53
noted that a ‘‘normal or physiological state is no
longer simply a disposition which can be revealed and ex-
plained as fact, but a manifestation of an attachment to some
value’’ (p. 57). The medical model traditionally dichotomizes
people as ‘‘healthy’’ and ‘‘sick’’ or ‘‘non-disabled’’ and ‘‘dis-
abled,’’ without reference to the social systems that render these
categories meaningful, and with the assumption that disability
is inherently inferior to nondisability. Treatment is admin-
istered in hopes of transforming disabled individuals into
nondisabled individuals.
54
The medical model, therefore,
inherently relies on deficit construals of autism, even though
both Kanner
55
and Asperger
56
noted autistic strengths.
In the medical model, autism is ‘‘essentially a narrative
condition .diagnoses of autism are essentially storytelling
in character, narratives that seek to explain contrasts between
the normal and the abnormal, sameness and difference’’
57
(p. 201). Autism diagnosis, even when observational tests or
interviews are administered, largely relies on subjective in-
terpretations of behavior. Indeed, the medical model of dis-
ability centers on identifying autism by assessing behaviors
and interactions with others. As such, it constructs autism as a
within-person phenomenon, even though the condition is
diagnosed through social behavior. These conceptualizations
of autism locate the source of impairment in autistic people
who may have difficulty understanding nonautistic social
behavior but do not question why nonautistic people expe-
rience the same difficulty in understanding autistic social
behavior (this is termed ‘‘the double empathy problem’’).
11,13
In contrast, the social model distinguishes between impair-
ments, which are socially valued differences in functioning or
appearance, and disabilities, which are environmentally medi-
ated and emphasize the loss of opportunities to participate in
society.
58
Under this framework, autism is disabling in socie-
ties that do not make efforts to remove barriers to participation
that autistic people face.
59
For example, autism can be dis-
abling when an autistic person seeks employment but is not
accommodated for communication differences that impact
their ability to participate in an interview.
Current conceptualizations of the social model acknowl-
edge that social barriers do not explain all aspects of disability
and recognize individual contributions in the context of a
disabling society.
60
Proponents of the autistic-led neurodi-
versity movement conceptualize autism in such a way that
autism itself can be celebrated while still recognizing im-
pairments and support needs.
61
Neurodiversity scholars and
activists: (1) recognize that barriers imposed by nonautistic
society hinder the fulfillment of autistic people and assert that
it is a societal responsibility to remove these barriers
59
; and
(2) acknowledge the transaction between inherent weaknesses
of autism and the social environment, viewing autism as both
a difference and a disability.
11,61–63
Therefore, they support
an integrative model of disability, which values impairment as
a valid form of human diversity
12,64
and dovetails with the
nuanced views of autism among autistic adults.
16,65
Inspired by the disability rights movement, Sinclair served
as the primary founder of the neurodiversity movement and
its use of identity-first language.
66
Sinclair’s
67
essay ‘‘Why
I Dislike Person First Language’’ is a foundational text that
explains why many neurodiversity advocates prefer identity-
first language such as ‘‘autistic person.’’ This piece explains
autism as inseparable from and fundamental to an individual’s
experience of the world. Perhaps most controversially, Sinclair
critiques the need to emphasize personhood as paradoxically
dehumanizing (‘‘Saying person with autism suggests that au-
tism is something so bad that it isn’t even consistent with being
a person,’’ para. 3). While person-first constructions were
originally promoted by self-advocates with intellectual dis-
ability (ID) in the late 1960s and 1970s, becoming widespread
by the 1990s as the self-advocacy movement came of age,
68
many within the autistic community now reject them. Neuro-
diversity proponents may recognize that supporters of either
language preference may share the value of upholding autistic
people’s dignity and worth but disagree on rhetorical means for
doing so.
8
The positioning of autism as an entity that can be discovered
in one’s genetics, neurological systems, or biochemistry
49,51
‘‘implies a lack of reflexivity about how autism is constructed
through our representational practices in research, in therapy,
and in popular accounts’’
69
(p. 20). Medicalized representa-
tions of autism rarely include consideration of the experiences
and everyday practices of autistic people. Historically, disabled
people have rarely been allowed to ‘‘control the referent ‘dis-
ability’’’ and the ‘‘terminology that has been used to linguis-
tically represent the various human differences referred to as
‘disabilities’’’
70
(p. 122). This type of medical-model rhetoric
has material consequences for shaping research agendas. Pre-
sently, several funding initiatives promote prevention research
(with the goal of eradicating autistic people) and certain
strands of intervention research that attempt to teach autis-
tic people to pass as nonautistic. Both these avenues of re-
search ignore aspects of disability that are socially mediated,
which requires social and structural changes to how autistic
people are viewed, valued, and treated in lieu of efforts to
4 BOTTEMA-BEUTEL ET AL.
exclusively change autistic people. See Figure 1 for an in-
fographic contrasting the medical and social models.
Current Research on the Language Preferences
of the Autistic Community
Formal research characterizing language preferences of
autistic people is emerging.
71
This research highlights dis-
crepancies in the language used by health care professionals
and that which is preferred by autistic adults and other
members of the autism community (e.g., family members,
friends), particularly surrounding identify-first versus person-
first language.
17
Whereas autistic adults in the United King-
dom endorsed ‘‘autistic’’ and ‘‘autistic person’’ in greater
numbers than ‘‘person with autism,’’ professionals endorsed
‘‘person with autism’’ in greater numbers than ‘‘autistic’’ or
FIG. 1. Infographic con-
trasting medical and social
models of disability.
AVOIDING ABLEIST LANGUAGE 5
‘‘autistic person.’’ Parents were also much less likely to en-
dorse ‘‘autistic person’’ than were autistic participants.
Research on Australian samples has shown that autistic
people rated the terms ‘‘autistic,’’ ‘‘person on the spectrum,’’
and ‘‘autistic person’’ significantly higher than ‘‘person with
autism,’’ ‘‘person with ASD’’ (autism spectrum disorder), and
‘‘person with ASC’’ (autism spectrum condition). U.S.- and
U.K.-based research has shown that self-identification as au-
tistic and awareness of the neurodiversity movement are as-
sociated with stronger preferences for the term ‘‘autistic
person’’ over ‘‘person with autism.’’
65
Data suggest that ‘‘on
the autism spectrum’’ may be the least polarizing way to refer
to autism,
17,72
but even using this description is a political
decision.
Outside identifying language, there is some evidence to
suggest relative consensus among stakeholder groups about
other preferred language choices. For example, Kenny et al.
17
found few autistic adults, family members/friends, or health
care professionals endorse the use of functioning-level de-
scriptors such as ‘‘high-functioning’’ autism (approximately
20% endorse) and ‘‘low-functioning’’ autism (<10% endorse).
Additionally, most autistic adults and other stakeholders prefer
the use of diversity-focused language (e.g., neurodiversity) as
opposed to phrases such as disability, deficit, or disorder.
17
Furthermore, evidence suggests autism community members
predominantly prefer probabilistic language over danger-
oriented terms. For example, the terms ‘‘infants with high
autism likelihood’’ or ‘‘infants with higher chance of devel-
oping autism’’ are preferred over terms such as ‘‘at-risk’’ when
describing infants with autistic siblings.
68
It is worth noting that this work largely describes the
preferences of English-speaking research participants, a
disproportionate amount of whom are white. Additionally,
as most studies were surveys, it is unclear the extent to
which these preferences represent those of autistic people
with marked impairments in written communication and/or
intellectual functioning that may hinder their ability to
participate in survey research. Further work is needed to
understand language preferences of autistic people from
diverse racial, ethnic, linguistic, and cultural backgrounds,
and those with a range of communication and intellectual
abilities. Finally, quantitative survey approaches should be
supplemented with qualitative analyses of language-in-use,
to understand how language choices are implicated in ev-
eryday experience.
Ongoing Controversies Around Language Usage
Many autism researchers may be unaware of how lan-
guage forms can reflect ableist ideologies. Other research-
ers who are aware of the potentially ableist implications of
some terms and discourses, but continue to use them in their
work, may appeal to three commonly used arguments for
this choice: (1) a lack of complete consensus from the au-
tistic community on their language preferences, (2) con-
cerns about a lack of scientific accuracy conveyed by
nonableist language, and (3) misunderstandings about terms
that originated from autistic and other disabled advocates.
In this section, we address each of these arguments and
provide a rationale for why researchers should continue to
make efforts to audit their written and spoken work so as to
avoid ableist language.
Lack of consensus
Even when polling is available to identify group-level lan-
guage preferences, controversy remains as to the representa-
tiveness of samples of autistic people. Whether researchers
should replace person-first language with identity-first lan-
guage has received particular attention recently.
74
An argu-
ment primarily put forward by nonautistic researchers for why
autistic people’s preferences should not lead to changes in
their language practices is that there remain members of the
autistic community who prefer person-first constructions over
identity-first constructions,
69
or are unable to participate in
discussions about language preferences because of commu-
nication impairments.
We contend that this line of reasoning works to maintain the
status quo by allowing nonautistic researchers to avoid en-
gagement with the expressed preferences of many members of
the autistic community, and discount or minimize their argu-
ments around language choices. Complete consensus is un-
likely to be gained for a set of terms for any marginalized
community. Yet, it remains important to avoid using language
with known stigmatizing effects (unless referring to a specific
individual who has indicated their language preferences), such
as many usages of person-first language.
71
Stakeholders (in-
cluding parents) may eventually move toward consensus on
preference for identity-first language, because it is positively
correlated with the growing awareness of the neurodiversity
movement that also increases acceptance of and positive
emotions toward autism.
65
While it is the case that non-
speaking autistic people with profound communication im-
pairment cannot have their views directly incorporated into the
neurodiversity movement, nonspeaking autistic people do
participate, as do those who formerly did not have an effective
means to communicate, but now do.
75,76
Additionally, speak-
ing individuals in this movement consider the unique needs of
communication-impaired autistic people in their advocacy
surrounding language choices, such as support for augmenta-
tive and alternative communication.
61–63
Likewise, many
caregivers of communication-impaired autistic people align
themselves with neurodiversity proponents when advocating
for their children.
77,78
We recommend that researchers consider the majority
preferences for particular language (which could involve
polling their research participants as part of standard data
collection methods), the specific arguments made by autistic
community members when articulating their preferences, and
existing recommendations by academic and professional or-
ganizations to respect the majority language preferences of
the group being referred to.
79,80
We recommend that journals
not require person-first language, and researchers still disin-
clined to adopt identity-first language may opt for the rela-
tively neutral term ‘‘on the autism spectrum.’’ This may be
especially important for autistic people with ID given the
language disagreements between the self-advocacy and neu-
rodiversity movements.
Concerns about accuracy
A second reason researchers may choose not to adopt non-
ableist language is because they believe that it inaccurately or
imprecisely represents their research findings. However, as
several examples illustrate, the opposite is often true. Ableist
language, and the implicit assumptions that underlie it, clouds
6 BOTTEMA-BEUTEL ET AL.
research findings in ways that are not helpful to researchers or
autistic communities.
31
In this section, we review several ex-
amples of this phenomenon.
Autistic people without ID are sometimes referred to as
‘‘high-functioning,’’ with the assumption that these individ-
uals will function better than autistic individuals with ID
(often referred to as ‘‘low functioning’’). Autistic people
have argued against the use of these labels both because they
are stigmatizing, and because they inaccurately reflect their
experience. An autistic individual’s intellectual and adaptive
functioning can vary significantly across domains (i.e., so-
called ‘‘spiky’’ cognitive profiles); for example, hyperlexia
can co-occur with dyscalculia.
76,81
This suggests that ‘‘in-
tellectual ability’’ is not uniformly distributed for any given
autistic person, and blanket-level functioning labels may
mask this reality. Functioning can also vary across time and
context and may depend more on the adequacy of supports
provided than on the presence or absence of ID.
9
For exam-
ple, autistic people without ID more often experience a drop-
off in services following high school, whereas those with ID
are more likely to transition into a supported context (e.g.,
supported employment, organized daytime activities).
82,83
Recent empirical research supports these criticisms; mea-
sures of adaptive functioning do not correlate with measures
of intellectual ability in autistic children,
84
and the divergence
between these two domains tends to increase with higher age
and IQ scores.
85
Functioning labels are therefore not only
inaccurate, but they can also result in situations where autistic
people labeled ‘‘high functioning’’ are not provided with the
supports they need, while autistic people labeled ‘‘low func-
tioning’’ are underestimated in regard to their actual cap-
abilities.
9
We encourage researchers to replace terms such
as ‘‘high-’’ and ‘‘low-functioning’’ with descriptors of the
characteristics they intend to convey (e.g., ID).
Another instance of this phenomenon is when autistic
people are described as ‘‘severely’’ affected, without includ-
ing specific information about what contributes to this clas-
sification. Researchers may mean to convey ID, structural
language impairment, or substantial support needs. These
characteristics often, but not always, co-occur, and research-
ers should specify the characteristics they are indicating when
using the term.
85
Other similar examples of a lack of precision
in terminology include ‘‘challenging behavior’’ and ‘‘autistic
traits.’’ These terms have different meanings across studies,
making comparisons between research findings more diffi-
cult. ‘‘Challenging behavior,’’ for instance, is a value-laden
phrase that does not specify what the behavior is, indicate who
perceives the behavior as challenging and why, or consider
the possible adaptive value the behavior has for the person
employing it.
7
Likewise, ‘‘autistic traits’’ is sometimes used
by researchers as a catchall phrase for autistic characteristics
that extend into the general population when present at less
impactful degrees. However, autism consists of a constella-
tion of traits that vary in presence, intensity, and function
across individuals. As such, the term ‘‘autistic traits,’’ while
not problematic in itself, should not be used for distinguish-
able and uncorrelated characteristics. The Autism Quotient,
86
a measure purporting to quantify autistic traits, includes
subscales measuring distinct autistic traits that are weakly
associated, suggesting the overall score of ‘‘autistic traits’’
lacks coherent meaning.
87
The term ‘‘autistic traits’’ is also
rarely used to refer to autistic advantages or neutral charac-
teristics, and therefore may offer a biased view of what con-
stitutes ‘‘autistic traits.’’
A final example in this category is using ‘‘typically devel-
oping’’ or ‘‘neurotypical’’ to describe participants serving as a
reference group. Although these terms are meant to indicate
participants do not meet criteria for clinical diagnoses, this is
rarely assessed. Many researchers assess the comparison group
for autism and may employ some basic exclusionary criteria
for large disqualifiers such as substance abuse, but most do not
screen for relatively common clinical conditions such as
anxiety, ADHD, and depression. Given that many people meet
criteria for some clinical condition at some point during their
lives,
88
groups labeled ‘‘neurotypical’’ include individuals
with clinically relevant features that go unaccounted for. This
is not to suggest that researchers should always screen for the
presence or history of all clinical conditions—doing so is often
resource-prohibitive. Rather, we note that the term ‘‘neuroty-
pical’’ is often misleading and promotes the assumption that
any nonautistic person is ‘‘typical.’’ Researchers might instead
label their comparison group ‘‘non-autistic,’’ if they conducted
appropriate screeners. The term ‘‘comparison group’’ is pre-
ferred to ‘‘control group’’ because autism is not an experi-
mentally manipulated assigned condition, so full ‘‘control’’ of
all variables besides autism between groups is impossible.
Misunderstanding terms
A third controversy involves researchers using terms in
ways other than how they are used by the disability com-
munity that coined them. Neurodiversity is a term that has
been used and written about by several autistic scholars, but
may be new to some autism researchers. The neurodiversity
framework conceptualizes autism as a natural form of human
variation, inseparable from individuals’ identity, and not in
need of a cure or normalization.
61
Recently, calls for tem-
pering the claims put forward by neurodiversity proponents
have been made by nonautistic researchers.
89,90
However,
some of this pushback is couched in inaccurate representa-
tions of neurodiversity as both a concept and a movement.
Some may purport that neurodiversity focuses on strengths
without reference to disablement, which is inaccurate.
65,91
Calls for temperance of the neurodiversity framework cate-
gorize specific autistic features as either ‘‘disabilities’’ or
‘‘differences,’’ but this is a false dichotomy.
65
The extent to
which differences constitute impairments, which can in turn
be disabling, requires reference to the supports that are pro-
vided (or not) in particular environments, and the sociocul-
tural contexts in which particular abilities are valued (or not).
These misrepresentations of terms set the stage for arguments
that distort and over-simplify the neurodiversity framework.
This in turn reinforces the status quo and allows researchers
to sidestep reflecting on the concerns raised by autistic peo-
ple. Taking the time to understand terms autistic people use to
describe their perspectives and experiences should be stan-
dard procedure for any researchers focusing on autism.
Suggestions for Researchers
In this section, we provide practical guidance to help re-
searchers make language choices that reduce stigmatization,
misunderstanding, and exclusion of autistic people. While
our suggestions are primarily targeted to researchers, health
care providers and other direct support professionals may
AVOIDING ABLEIST LANGUAGE 7
also find them useful. We have divided our guidance into
three sections. First, we briefly discuss participatory models
of autism research, and the ways that they may improve
discourse around autism. Second, we provide a set of ques-
tions that researchers can ask themselves about their lan-
guage choices that may help them identify problematic
wording. Third, we have compiled a noncomprehensive list
of potentially ableist terms and discourses that regularly recur
in autism research (including some of our own prior work)
and provide suggested alternatives.
Participatory models of autism research
For many nonautistic autism researchers, language choi-
ces may be dictated by historical conventions and a desire to
be consistent with language in the academic journals in
which they publish. These conventions persist, despite au-
tistic preferences, in part because of a failure to integrate
representative numbers of autistic people and stakeholders
into executive research positions. To counter this, partici-
patory models of autism research have been developed.
A hallmark of these approaches is that autistic people are
included in the research process conducted by nonautistic
investigators, and editorial decisions made by nonautistic
publishers elevate autistic voices into roles with greater
power. (See Refs.
13,18,59,92,93
for descriptions and guidance
on this approach). This can help break down conventional
barriersandleadtoresearchthatbettermatchestheprefer-
ences and priorities of the autistic community. We advocate
for a shift in funding priorities (which traditionally favor
causation and cure research
94–96
), so that grant money is
available to compensate autistic people for their participa-
tion. For autistic nonresearchers who are interested, these
funds could be used to provide training on basic research
methodology that would further enable substantive contri-
butions to research design and analysis procedures. Non-
autistic researchers should also be trained on how to partner
with autistic people (including those without academic
backgrounds) through all phases of the research process.
Procedures for remote participation (e.g., video conference
or instant messaging) have been developed that will enable
researchers to cast a wide net in soliciting autistic partners
and communicate about research activities using accessible
modalities.
92,97
Additionally, hiring and promoting autistic researchers
into faculty positions can ensure that autistic people play
leading roles in autism research. Autistic autism researchers
have already made significant contributions to our under-
standing of autism and have provided much of the language
guidance we draw on in this article. In tandem with mean-
ingfully involving autistic people (researchers and non-
researchers alike) in the research process, participatory
research should be conducted that is rigorous and high
quality.
10
Researcher questions for self-reflection
on language choices
Below are seven questions that may help researchers de-
termine if they have adequately considered the impacts of
their language choices on autistic communities. If the answer
to question one is ‘‘no’’ or the answers to questions two
through seven are ‘‘yes,’’ researchers should consider alter-
native ways of speaking or writing (which we provide in
Table 1, explained below).
1. Would I use this language if I were in a conversation
with an autistic person?
2. Does my language suggest that autistic people are
inherently inferior to nonautistic people, or assert that
they lack something fundamental to being human?
3. Does my language suggest that autism is something to
be fixed, cured, controlled, or avoided?
4. Does my language unnecessarily medicalize autism
when describing educational supports?
5. Does my language suggest to lay people that the goal
of my research is behavioral control and normaliza-
tion, rather than granting as much autonomy and
agency to autistic people as reasonably possible?
6. Am I using particular words or phrases solely because
it is a tradition in my field, even though autistic people
have expressed that such language can be stigmatizing?
7. Does my language unnecessarily ‘‘other’’ autistic peo-
ple, by suggesting that characteristics of autism bear no
relationships to characteristics of nonautistic people?
Alternatives to commonly used terms
that are potentially ableist
Finally, Table 1 provides concrete examples of how re-
searchers might replace potentially ableist terms/discourses with
suggested nonableist alternatives. In generating this table, we
relied on the work of autistic scholars, researchers, and advo-
cates, as well as on research by nonautistic scholars that centers
autistic perspectives. We provide references to this work so that
researchers can understand the rationale behind these language
suggestions and determine if it applies to their work. We ac-
knowledge that what can be considered ‘‘ableist language’’
depends on the time, place, and manner in which it is used.
98
Therefore, not all instances of using the terms/discourses de-
scribed in the table are necessarily ableist. Likewise, not all
nonableist language suggestions we offer will be appropriate or
preferred for all of autistic people; in those cases, other terms
may be necessary. While the autistic writers whose language
suggestions appear in this table may not be representative of the
entire autistic community, their suggestions may nonetheless
have broad applicability. It is also likely that this table will need
revising as language usage will inevitably evolve. Still, while our
partial compilation will not serve as hard-and-fast rules, it offers
researchers an opportunity to interrogate their language choices.
Conclusion
In this commentary, we have defined and described ableism,
traced the historical trajectory of ableist language in autism
research, and provided arguments for why researcher attempts
to avoid ableist language will result in better outcomes for the
autistic community as well as improved communication in
research. Language choices are important, as they shape atti-
tudes about autism and people’s understanding of what it
means to be an autistic person. Now more than ever, re-
searchers are taking autistic perspectives into account in their
writing, and we applaud these changes. While the views pre-
sented in this Perspective are by definition partial, we hope this
commentary contributes to ongoing discussions about lan-
guage use and offers avenues for researchers to adapt their
8 BOTTEMA-BEUTEL ET AL.
language practices. Moving forward, journals that publish
autism-related research should encourage researchers to in-
terrogate and explain their language choices to ensurethat they
have considered autistic perspectives and the implications of
their choices for autistic people.
Acknowledgment
The authors would like to thank Sue Fletcher-Watson for
her commentary on an earlier version of this article.
Authorship Confirmation Statement
K.B.-B. proposed the initial outline of the article and
oversaw editing of the final document. K.B.-B., S.K.K.,
J.N.L., N.J.S., and B.N.H. conceptualized the article, gath-
ered literature for the review, wrote sections of the article,
and contributed to editing the final document. All authors
have reviewed and approved the article before submission.
This article has been submitted solely to this journal and is
not published, in press, or submitted elsewhere.
Author Disclosure Statement
No competing financial interests exist.
Funding Information
The authors did not receive any funding in support of this
article.
References
1. Smith L. #Ableism. Center for Disability Rights. n.d.
http://cdrnys.org/blog/uncategorized/ableism (accessed
April 26, 2020).
2. Botha M, Frost DM. Extending the minority stress model
to understand mental health problems experienced by the
autistic population. Soci Mental Health. 2020;10(1):20–
34.
3. Bottema-Beutel K, Cuda J, Kim SY, Crowley S, Scanlon
D. High school experiences and support recommendations
of autistic youth. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;1–16.
4. Cage E, Di Monaco J, Newell V. Experiences of autism
acceptance and mental health in autistic adults. J Autism
Dev Disord 2018;48(2):473–484.
5. Johnson TD, Joshi A. Dark clouds or silver linings? A
stigma threat perspective on the implications of an au-
tism diagnosis for workplace well-being. JApplPsy-
chol. 2016;101(3):430.
6. Sarrett J. Interviews, disclosures, and misperceptions:
Autistic adults’ perspectives on employment related chal-
lenges. Disabil Stud Q. 2017;37(2). https://dsq-sds.org/
article/view/5524/4652 (accessed August 28, 2020).
7. Ballou EP [chavisory]. A checklist for identifying sources of
aggression. We Are Like Your Child. http://wearelikeyour
child.blogspot.com/2014/05/a-checklist-for-identifying-
sources-of.htm (accessed April 26, 2020).
8. Brown L. The significance of semantics: Person first
language: Why it matters. https://www.autistichoya.com/
2011/08/significance-of-semantics-person-first.html (ac-
cessed April 26, 2020).
9. Garnder F. The problem with functioning labels. http://
www.thinkingautismguide.com/2018/03/the-problems-
with-functioning-labels.html (accessed April 26, 2020).
10. Dawson M. What history tells us about autism research, re-
dux. http://autismcrisis.blogspot.com/2017/11/what-history-
tells-us-about-autism.html (accessed April 26, 2020).
11. Kapp SK. Empathizing with sensory and movement dif-
ferences: Moving toward sensitive understanding of au-
tism. Front Integr Neurosci. 2013;7:38.
12. Kapp SK. Interactions between theoretical models and
practical stakeholders: The basis for an integrative, col-
laborative approach to disabilities. In: Ashkenazy E, La-
timer M, eds. Empowering Leadership: A Systems Change
Guide for Autistic College Students and Those with Other
Disabilities. The Autistic Press; 2013;104–113.
13. Milton D. On the ontological status of autism: The ‘double
empathy problem’. Disabil Soc. 2012;27(6):883–887.
14. Yergeau M. Clinically significant disturbance: On theorists
who theorize theory of mind. Disabil Stud Q. 2013;33(4).
https://dsq-sds.org/article/view/3876/3405 (accessed August
28, 2020).
15. Yergeau M. Authoring Autism: On Rhetoric and Neurologi-
cal Queerness. Durham, NC: Duke University Press; 2018.
16.Gillespie-LynchK,KappSK,BrooksPJ,PickensJ,
Schwartzman B. Whose expertise is it? Evidence for autistic
adults as critical autism experts. Front Psychol. 2017;8:438.
17. Kenny L, Hattersley C, Molins B, Buckley C, Povey C,
Pellicano E. Which terms should be used to describe
autism? Perspectives from the UK autism community.
Autism. 2016;20(4):442–462.
18. Nicolaidis C, Raymaker D, Kapp SK, et al. The AASPIRE
practice-based guidelines for the inclusion of autistic
adults in research as co-researchers and study participants.
Autism. 2019;23(8):2007–2019.
19. Pellicano E, Dinsmore A, Charman T. What should au-
tism research focus upon? Community views and prior-
ities from the United Kingdom. Autism. 2014;18(7):756–
770.
20. Fletcher-Watson S, Happe
´F. Autism: A New Introduction
to Psychological Theory and Current Debate. London,
UK: Routledge; 2019.
21. Byrne P. Stigma of mental illness and ways of diminishing
it. Adv Psychiatr Treat. 2000;6(1):65–72.
22. Gernsbacher M. Stigma from psychological science:
Group differences, not deficits-Introduction to stigma
special section. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2010;5(6):687.
23. McGuire A. Life without autism: A cultural logic of
violence. In Runswich-Cole K, Mallet R, Timmi S, eds.
Re-thinking Autism: Diagnosis, Identity and Equality.
London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers; 2016;93–109.
24. Murray S. Representing Autism: Culture, Narrative, Fas-
cination. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2008.
25. Billawala A, Wolbring G. Analyzing the discourse sur-
rounding Autism in the New York Times using an ableism
lens. Disabil Stud Q. 2014;34(1). https://dsq-sds.org/
article/view/3348 (accessed August 28, 2020).
26. Gillborn D. Intersectionality, critical race theory, and the
primacy of racism: Race, class, gender, and disability in
education. Qual Inq. 2015;21(3):277–287.
27. Lewis L. Longmore lecture: Context, clarity and grounding.
https://www.talilalewis.com/blog (accessed April 26, 2020).
28. Blanchett WJ, Klingner JK, Harry B. The intersection of
race, culture, language, and disability: Implications for
urban education. Urban Educ. 2009;44(4):389–409.
29. Gernsbacher MA, Dawson M, Mottron L. Autism: Com-
mon, heritable, but not harmful. Behav Brain Sci. 2006;
29(4):413–414.
AVOIDING ABLEIST LANGUAGE 9
30. Gernsbacher MA. On not being human. APS Observer.
2007;20(2):5.
31. Jaswal VK, Akhtar N. Being versus appearing socially
uninterested: Challenging assumptions about social moti-
vation in autism. Behav Brain Sci. 2019;42:e82.
32. Kapp SK. How social deficit models exacerbate the
medical model: Autism as case in point. Autism Policy
Pract. 2019;2(1):3–28.
33. Woods R. Exploring how the social model of disability
can be re-invigorated for autism: In response to Jonathan
Levitt. Disabil Soc. 2017;32(7):1090–1095.
34. Mundy P. Lessons learned from autism: An information
processing model of joint attention and social cognition. In:
Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology: Meeting the
Challenge of Translational Research in Child Psychology,
vol. 35. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons; 2009;59–113.
35. Bottema-Beutel K, Park H, Kim SY. Commentary on
social skills training curricula for individuals with ASD:
Social interaction, authenticity, and stigma. J Autism Dev
Disord. 2018;48(3):953–964.
36. Crane L, Adams F, Harper G, Welch J, Pellicano E. ‘Some-
thing needs to change’: Mental health experiences of young
autistic adults in England. Autism. 2019;23(2):477–493.
37. Kim SY, Bottema-Beutel K. Negotiation of individual and
collective identities in the online discourse of autistic
adults. Autism Adulthood. 2019;1(1):69–78.
38. Cherney JL. The rhetoric of ableism. Disabil Stud Q.
2011;31(3).
39. Wolbring G. The politics of ableism. Development. 2008;
51(2):252–258.
40. Sacks H. Lectures on Conversation. Oxford: Blackwell;
1992.
41. Potter J. Discourse analysis and discursive psychology. In:
Cooper H, ed. APA Handbook of Research Methods in
Psychology: Quantitative, Qualitative, Neuropsychologi-
cal, and Biological, vol. 2. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association; 2012;111–130.
42. Potter J, Wetherell M. Discourse and Social Psychology.
London: Sage; 1987.
43. Fairclough N. Critical Discourse Analysis: The Critical Study
of Language, 2nd ed. New York, NY: Routledge; 2013.
44. Wittgenstein L. Philosophical Investigations, 2nd ed.
(G. E. M. Anscombe, Trans.). Oxford: Blackwell; 1958.
45. Kuntz AM. Representing representation. Int J Qual Stud
Educ. 2010;23(4):423–433.
46. Noblit GW, Flores SY, Murillo EG, eds. Postcritical Eth-
nography: Reinscribing Critique. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton
Press; 2004.
47. Rallis SF. ‘That is NOT what’s happening at Horizon!’:
Ethics and misrepresenting knowledge in text. Int J Qual
Stud Educ. 2010;23(4):435–448.
48. Edwards D. Discourse and Cognition. London: Sage; 1997.
49. Nadesan MH. Constructing autism: A brief genealogy. In:
Osteen M, eds. Autism and Representation. New York:
Routledge; 2008;78–95.
50. Alexander FG, Selesnick ST. The History of Psychiatry:
An Evaluation of Psychiatric Thought and Practice from
Prehistoric Times to the Present. New York: Harper &
Row; 1966.
51. Broderick AA, Ne’eman A. Autism as metaphor: Narra-
tive and counter narrative. Int J Inclusive Educ. 2008;
12(5–6):459–476.
52. Ashby CE. The trouble with normal: The struggle for
meaningful access for middle school students with de-
velopmental disability lab. Disabil Soc. 2010;23(3):345–
358.
53. Canguilhem G. The Normal and the Pathological. Brooklyn,
NY: Zone Books; 1989.
54. Brickman P, Rabinowitz VC, Karuza J, Coates D, Cohn E,
Kidder L. Models of helping and coping. Am Psychol.
1982;37(4):368–384.
55. Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Ner-
vous Child. 1943;2(3):217–250.
56. Asperger H. Die ‘‘Autistischen Psychopathen’’ im Kin-
desalter. Eur Arch Psychiatr Clin Neurosci. 1944;117(1):
76–136.
57. Duffy J, Dorner R. The pathos of ‘‘mindblindness’’:
Autism, science, and sadness in ‘‘Theory of Mind’’ nar-
ratives. J Literary Cult Disabil Stud. 2011;5(2):201–215.
58. Oliver M. Defining impairment and disability: Issues at
stake. In: Barnes C, Mercer G, eds. Exploring the Divide.
Leeds, UK: The Disability Press; 1996;29–54.
59. Chown N, Robinson J, Beardon L, et al. Improving re-
search about us, with us: A draft framework for inclusive
autism research. Disabil Soc. 2017;32(5):720–734.
60. Oliver M. The social model of disability: Thirty years on.
Disabil Soc. 2017;28(7):1024–1026.
61. Kapp SK, ed. Autistic Community and the Neurodiversity
Movement: Stories from the Frontline. Singapore: Pal-
grave MacMillan; 2020.
62. Bailin A. Misconceptions about neurodiversity. https://
blogs.scientificamerican.com/observations/clearing-up-some-
misconceptions-about-neurodiversity (accessed June 6,
2019).
63. Ballou EP. What the neurodiversity movement does- and
doesn’t offer. http://www.thinkingautismguide.com/2018/
02/what-neurodiversity-movement-doesand.html (ac-
cessed February 6, 2018).
64. Swain J, French S. Towards an affirmation model of dis-
ability. Disabil Soc. 2000;15(4):569–582.
65. Kapp SK, Gillespie-Lynch K, Sherman LE, Hutman T.
Deficit, difference, or both? Autism and neurodiversity.
Dev Psychol. 2013;49(1):59–71.
66. Waltz Mitzi. Autism: A Social and Medical History. Ba-
singstoke: Palgrave Macmillan; 2013.
67. Sinclair J. Why I dislike person first language. Autonomy
2013;1(2). http://www.larry-arnold.net/Autonomy/index.php/
autonomy/article/view/OP1/html_1 (accessed August 28,
2020).
68. Wehmeyer M, Bersani H, Gagne R. Riding the third
wave: Self-determination and self-advocacy in the 21st
century. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabil. 2000;15(2):
106–115.
69. Nadesan MH. Constructing autism: A brief genealogy. In:
Osteen M, ed. Autism and Representation. New York:
Routledge; 2008;78–95.
70. Mutua K, Smith RM. Disrupting normalcy and the prac-
tical concerns of classroom teachers. In: Danforth S, Ga-
bel SL, eds. Vital Questions Facing Disability Studies in
Education. New York, NY: Peter Lang; 2006;121–132.
71. Shakes P, Cashin A. Identifying language for people on
the autism spectrum: A scoping review. Issues Mental
Health Nurs. 2019;40(4):317–325.
72. Bury SM, Jellett R, Spoor JR, Hedley D. ‘‘It defines who I
am’’ or ‘‘It’s something I have’’: What language do [au-
tistic] Australian adults [on the autism spectrum] prefer?
J Autism Dev Disord. 2020; [Epub ahead of print]; DOI:
10.1007/s10803-020-04425-3.
10 BOTTEMA-BEUTEL ET AL.
73. Fletcher-Watson S, Apicella F, Auyeung B, et al. Atti-
tudes of the autism community to early autism research.
Autism. 2017;21(1):61–74.
74. Furfaro H. Tensions ride high despite reshuffle at autism
science meeting. https://www.spectrumnews.org/news/
tensions-ride-high-despite-reshuffle-autism-science-meeting
(accessed April 29, 2019).
75. Savarese ET. What we have to tell you: A roundtable with
self-advocates from AutCom. Disabil Stud Q. 2009;30(1).
https://dsq-sds.org/article/view/1073/1239 (accessed August
28, 2020).
76. Sequenzia A, Grace EJ, eds. Typed Words, Loud Voices.
Fort Worth, TX: Autonomous Press; 2015.
77. Cevik K. #AutisticWhileBlack: Diezel Braxton and be-
coming indistinguishable from one’s peers. http://theautism
wars.blogspot.com/2018/07/autisticwhileblack-diezel-braxton-
and_24.html (accessed July 24, 2018).
78. Greenburg C, Des Roches Rosa S. Two winding parent paths
to neurodiversity advocacy. In: Kapp SK, ed. Autistic Com-
munity and the Neurodiversity Movement: Stories from the
Frontline. Singapore: Palgrave MacMillan; 2020;155–166.
79. American Psychological Association. Publication Man-
ual, 6th ed. Washington, DC: Author; 2009.
80. National Center on Disability and Journalism. Disability
Language Style Guide. http://ncdj.org/style-guide 2018
(accessed August 28, 2020).
81. Jones CR, Happe
´F, Golden H, et al. Reading and arith-
metic in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders:
Peaks and dips in attainment. Neuropsychology. 2009;
23(6):718–728.
82. Shattuck PT, Wagner M, Narendorf S, Sterzing P, Hensley
M. Post-high school service use among young adults with
an autism spectrum disorder. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med.
2011;165(2):141–146.
83. Taylor JL, Seltzer MM. Employment and post-secondary
educational activities for young adults with autism spec-
trum disorders during the transition to adulthood. J Autism
Dev Disord. 2011;41(5):566–574.
84. Alvares GA, Bebbington K, Cleary D, et al. The misnomer
of ‘high functioning autism’: Intelligence is an imprecise
predictor of functional abilities at diagnosis. Autism. 2020;
24:221–232.
85. Kapp S, Ne’eman A. ASD in DSM-5: What the research
shows and recommendations for change. 2012. https://
autisticadvocacy.org/wp-content/uploads/2012/06/ASAN_
DSM-5_2_final.pdf (accessed August 28, 2020).
86. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J,
Clubley E. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence
from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males
and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev
Disord. 2001;31(1):5–17.
87. English MCW, Gignac GE, Visser TAW, Whitehouse
AJO, Maybery MA. A comprehensive psychometric
analysis of autism-spectrum quotient factor models using
two large samples: Model recommendations and the in-
fluence of divergent traits on total-scale scores. Autism
Res. 2020;13:45–60.
88. Merikangas KR, He JP, Burstein M, et al. Lifetime prev-
alence of mental disorders in US adolescents: Results from
the National Comorbidity Survey Replication-Adolescent
Supplement (NCS-A). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psy-
chiatry. 2010;49(10):980–989.
89. Baron-Cohen S. The Concept of Neurodiversity Is Dividing
the Autism Community. Scientific American Blog Network.
2019. https://blogs.scientificamerican.com/observations/the-
concept-of-neurodiversity-is-dividing-the-autism-community
Last accessed April 30, 2020.
90. Jaarsma P, Welin S. Autism as a natural human variation:
Reflections on the claims of the neurodiversity movement.
Health Care Anal. 2012;20(1):20–30.
91. den Houting J. Neurodiversity: An insider’s perspective.
Autism. 2019;23(2):271–273.
92. Nicolaidis C. What can physicians learn from the neuro-
diversity movement?. AMA J Ethics. 2012;14(6):503–510.
93. Fletcher-Watson S, Adams J, Brook K, et al. Making the
future together: Shaping autism research through mean-
ingful participation. Autism. 2019;23(4):943–953.
94. Pellicano E, Dinsmore A, Charman T. What should autism
research focus upon? Community views and priorities
from the United Kingdom. Autism. 2014;18(7):756–770.
95. Singh J, Illes J, Lazzeroni L, Hallmayer J. Trends in US
autism research funding. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;
39(5):788–795.
96. Den Houting J, Pellicano E. A portfolio analysis of autism
research funding in Australia, 2008–2017. J Autism Dev
Disord. 2019;49(11):4400–4408.
97. Gillespie-Lynch K, Kapp S, Shane-Simpson C, Smith D,
Hutman T. Intersections between the autism spectrum and
the internet: Perceived benefits and preferred functions of
computer-mediated communication. Intellect Dev Disabil.
2014;52(6):456–469.
98. Brown L. Ableism/Language. Autistichoya.com. 2018.
https://www.autistichoya.com/p/ableist-words-and-terms-
to-avoid.html (accessed April 30, 2020).
99. Cynthia K. What’s so special about a special interest?.
Musings of an Aspie. 2012. https://musingsofanaspie.com/
2012/11/07/whats-so-special-about-a-special-interest (ac-
cessed April 30, 2020).
100. Gernsbacher M, Raimond A, Balinghasay M, Boston J.
‘‘Special needs’’ is an ineffective euphemism. Cogn Res
Princ Implic. 2016;1(1):29.
101. Kailes J. Language Is More Than A Trivial Concern! 10Th
Edition. Center for Disability and Health Policy. 2010.
https://www.resourcesforintegratedcare.com/sites/default/
files/Language%20Is%20More%20Than%20A%20Trivial
%20Concern.pdf (accessed April 30, 2020).
102. Kapp S, Steward R, Crane L, et al. ‘People should be
allowed to do what they like’: Autistic adults’ views and
experiences of stimming. Autism. 2019;23(7):1782–1792.
103. Schaber A. Ask an autistic #15 - what are autistic melt-
downs?. YouTube. 2014. https://www.youtube.com/watch?
v=FhUDyarzqXE&t=1s (accessed April 30, 2020).
104. Boue S. The sound of stigma: A journey across the rope
bridge. #autismacceptance. The Other Side. 2017. https://
soniaboue.wordpress.com/?s=rope+bridge (accessed April
30, 2020).
105. Gernsbacher M. Editorial Perspective: The use of person-
first language in scholarly writing may accentuate stigma.
J Child Psychol Psychiatr. 2017;58(7):859–861.
106. Lynch C. Person-first language: What it is, and when not
to use it. Neuroclastic. 2019. https://theaspergian.com/
2019/04/19/person-first (accessed April 30, 2020).
107. Robison J. Talking about autism—thoughts for research-
ers. Autism Res. 2019;12(7):1004–1006.
108. Jones S. Disability day of mourning: We are not bur-
dens—Rooted in rights. Rooted in Rights. 2017. https://
rootedinrights.org/disability-day-of-mourning-we-are-not-
burdens (accessed April 30, 2020).
AVOIDING ABLEIST LANGUAGE 11
109. Kaplan B, Dewey D, Crawford S, Wilson B. The term
comorbidity is of questionable value in reference to
developmental disorders. JLearnDisabil. 2001;34(6):
555–565.
110. Griffin E, Pollak D. Student experiences of neurodiversity
in higher education: Insights from the BRAINHE project.
Dyslexia. 2009;15(1):23–41.
111. Henderson H, Ono K, McMahon C, Schwartz C, Usher L,
Mundy P. The costs and benefits of self-monitoring for
higher functioning children and adolescents with autism.
J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(2):548–559.
112. Walker N. Neurodiversity: Some basic terms and defini-
tions. Neurocosmopoloitanism. 2014. http://neurocosmo
politanism.com/neurodiversity-some-basic-terms-definitions
(accessed April 30, 2020).
113. Pitney J. ‘‘Lifetime Social Cost.’’ Autism Politics and
Policy. 2020. http://www.autismpolicyblog.com/2020/02/
lifetime-social-cost.html (accessed April 30, 2020).
114. Oolong. Autism when it’s not a disability. 2019. https://
medium.com/@Oolong/autism-when-its-not-a-disability-
a3dcafa732a (accessed April 30, 2020).
115. Autistic Self Advocacy Network (ASAN). ASAN State-
ment on Fein Study on Autism and ‘‘Recovery.’’ ASAN.
2013. https://autisticadvocacy.org/2013/01/asan-statement-
on-fein-study-on-autism-and-recovery (accessed April 30,
2020).
116. Kapp S. A critical response to ‘‘The kids who beat autism.’’
Thinking Person’s Guide to Autism. 2014. http://www
.thinkingautismguide.com/2014/08/a-critical-response-to-
kids-who-beat.html (accessed April 30, 2020).
117. Hull L, Petrides K, Allison C, et al. ‘‘Putting on My Best
Normal’’: Social camouflaging in adults with autism
spectrum conditions. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;47(8):
2519–2534.
118. Rose K. Masking: I am not okay. The Autistic Advocate.
2018. https://theautisticadvocate.com/2018/07/masking-i-
am-not-ok.html (accessed April 30, 2020).
119. Gernsbacher M, Raimond A, Stevenson J, Boston J, Harp
B. Do puzzle pieces and autism puzzle piece logos evoke
negative associations?. Autism. 2018;22(2):118–125.
120. Pellicano L, Mandy W, Bo
¨lte S, Stahmer A, Lounds
Taylor J, Mandell D. A new era for autism research, and
for our journal. Autism. 2018;22(2):82–83.
121. Gernsbacher M, Dawson M, Hill Goldsmith H. Three
reasons not to believe in an autism epidemic. Curr Dir
Psychol Sci. 2005;14(2):55–58.
Address correspondence to:
Kristen Bottema-Beutel, PhD
Lynch School of Education and Human Development
Boston College
140 Commonwealth Avenue
Chestnut Hill, MA 02467
USA
Email: kristen.bottema-beutel@bc.edu
12 BOTTEMA-BEUTEL ET AL.