ArticlePublisher preview available

The Homogeneity and Heterogeneity of Moral Functioning in Preschool

Wiley
Child Development
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

The current study examined relations between distinct aspects of moral functioning, and their cognitive and emotional correlates, in preschool age children. Participants were 171 typically developing 3‐ to 6‐year‐olds. Each child completed several tasks, including (a) moral tasks assessing both performance of various moral actions and evaluations of moral scenarios presented both verbally and nonverbally; and (b) non‐moral tasks assessing general cognitive skill, executive functioning, theory‐of‐mind, and emotion recognition. Shyness and empathic concern were assessed from video acquired during participation. Results demonstrated positive associations among distinct moral actions, as well as among distinct moral evaluation tasks, but few associations between tasks assessing moral actions and moral evaluation. Empathic concern and inhibitory control each emerged as important predictors of preschoolers’ moral functioning.
The Homogeneity and Heterogeneity of Moral Functioning in Preschool
Enda Tan , Amori Y. Mikami , Anastasiya Luzhanska, and J. Kiley Hamlin
University of British Columbia
The current study examined relations between distinct aspects of moral functioning, and their cognitive and
emotional correlates, in preschool age children. Participants were 171 typically developing 3- to 6-year-olds.
Each child completed several tasks, including (a) moral tasks assessing both performance of various moral
actions and evaluations of moral scenarios presented both verbally and nonverbally; and (b) non-moral tasks
assessing general cognitive skill, executive functioning, theory-of-mind, and emotion recognition. Shyness and
empathic concern were assessed from video acquired during participation. Results demonstrated positive
associations among distinct moral actions, as well as among distinct moral evaluation tasks, but few associa-
tions between tasks assessing moral actions and moral evaluation. Empathic concern and inhibitory control
each emerged as important predictors of preschoolersmoral functioning.
From an early age, children demonstrate a wide
range of skills which allow them to navigate the
complex social and moral world. As third parties,
children evaluate others based on their moral
actions, viewing intentional moral transgressions as
wrong (Killen, Mulvey, Richardson, Jampol, &
Woodward, 2011), judging prosocial individuals to
be nice, and allocating punishment to antisocial
individuals (Van de Vondervoort & Hamlin, 2017).
As rst parties, children engage in their own proso-
cial behaviors, helping others achieve instrumental
goals (Svetlova, Nichols, & Brownell, 2010), com-
forting distressed others (Zahn-Waxler, Radke-Yar-
row, Wagner, & Chapman, 1992), and sharing
resources with others (Warneken, Lohse, Melis, &
Tomasello, 2011). The diversity of these morally rel-
evant skills (including moral evaluations and moral
behaviors) begs two important questions: Should
we view childrens moral functioning as a unied
construct? And if so, what are the mechanisms that
unify these seemingly distinct mental and behav-
ioral processes? The current study aimed to address
these questions by assessing the consistency
between different aspects of moral functioning, and
their cognitive and emotional correlates, in pre-
school-age children.
Relations Between Different Forms of Moral Behaviors
Relations Between Types of Prosocial Behaviors
Are there consistencies in childrens production
of different types of moral behaviors? Some scholars
argue that different forms of prosocial behaviors are
unrelated, primarily because they are based on dis-
tinct mechanisms. For instance, Duneld (2014)
argues that helping, comforting, and sharing are eli-
cited by different negative states (i.e., instrumental
need, emotional distress, and unmet material desire,
respectively), and that these negative states are rep-
resented by different neural mechanisms. In a simi-
lar vein, Paulus (2014) proposes that different
prosocial behaviors are driven by distinct motiva-
tions: While instrumental helping is motivated by a
desire to fulll othersgoals or to engage in social
interactions, comforting is driven by emotional con-
tagion or empathic concerns, and sharing is driven
by compliance to an explicit request, conformity to
social norms, and a desire to interact. These theories
suggest that, given the diverse mechanisms underly-
ing different forms of prosocial behaviors, prosocial
behaviors should not be viewed as a homogeneous
construct (Duneld, 2014; Paulus, 2014).
Empirically, the heterogeneity of prosocial
behaviors is supported by studies nding no
This work was supported in part by Social Sciences and
Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) Grant and
UBC Hampton Fund Research Grant to K. Hamlin. We thank
Shannon Bridson, Hannah de Vries, Vivian Wong, Rachelle Gra-
ham, Brianna Vissers, Thaddeus Grams, Adam Shillington, Kristi
Atkinson, and Giulia Santaca for their assistance. The data
reported in the paper are deposited in the Department of Psy-
chology at UBC. The authors have no conict of interest to
declare.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Enda Tan, Department of Psychology, University of British
Columbia, 2136 West Mall, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
V6T 1Z4. Electronic mail may be sent to enda.tan@psych.ubc.ca.
©2020 Society for Research in Child Development
All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2021/9203-0013
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13458
Child Development, May/June 2021, Volume 92, Number 3, Pages 959975
... Scholars from a dispositional view propose that reliable individual differences across prosocial behaviours are possible given that the mechanisms of prosocial behaviours are not entirely mutually exclusive (Eisenberg et al., 2015) and may be biologically rooted in the evolutionary history of the human species (Tan et al., 2021). ...
... Notably, by the third wave, we found these two groups as well, although by this age (34 months) most children were engaging in both instrumental and empathic helping. This latter finding of a group of helpers is similar to that of Tan et al. (2021) who found that among preschoolers (5-year-olds), instrumental helping and empathic helping were more strongly correlated with each other than with sharing (although notably we did not find this to be the case in the present study). These results generally support the notion that already in the second year, prosocial behaviours are showing some consolidation and cohesion at least for some children, and that as toddlers mature there is greater consolidation of prosocial behaviours, particularly helping. ...
... Variable-centred analyses (correlations) focusing on across task and across wave correlations found significant but low consistency across tasks and low stability for each prosocial behaviour over time. In addition, and consistent with several previous studies (e.g., Tan et al., 2021) that used a variable-centred approach, different types of prosocial behaviours were correlated with each other both cross-sectionally and longitudinally but at modest levels. ...
Article
Full-text available
Addressing the debate on whether there are reliable individual differences in toddler's prosociality, this study employed a person‐centred approach to investigate (1) profiles of instrumental helping, sharing and empathic helping at 22, 28 and 34 months, and (2) the predictive role of toddler's temperament (anger/frustration, impulsivity and inhibitory control) to these profiles. Variable‐centred analyses examined cross‐task and over time associations. Participants were 93 Dutch toddlers observed in standardised behavioural assessments at each wave. Parents rated their toddler's temperament at wave 1. Results revealed small but significant across‐task and over time associations of the prosocial behaviours and children were distinguished into three profile groups at 22 months (high prosocial, instrumental helper and low prosocial), three at 28 months (high, moderate and low) and two profiles at 34 months (high prosocial and instrumental and empathic helpers), with low to moderate membership stability across waves. For 34‐month‐olds, those in the high prosocial group were rated as being less impulsive compared to their peers in the helper group. These findings indicate reliable individual differences of prosociality exist at early ages with impulsivity as a potential precursor to these individual differences.
... Furthermore, shift in attention or flexibility, planning skills and working memory are defined as aspects of executive functions (Cowell et al., 2015;Hinnant et al., 2013). Tan et al. (2020) additionally conceptualize the ability to delay gratification as an aspect of executive functions (Mischel et al., 1972;Rodriguez et al., 1989). With regard to the respective definitions and aspects, overlaps with the temperament-related aspects of self-regulation were found. ...
... Further included studies suggest that prosocial behavior can mirror moral behavior and antisocial behavior can mirror immoral behavior (Asendorpf & Nunner-Winkler, 1992;Augustine & Stifter, 2015;Colasante et al., 2014;Stifter et al., 2009). For example, sharing or donating has been conceptualized as prosocial as well as moral behavior (Cowell et al., 2015(Cowell et al., , 2017Reis & Sampaio, 2019;Smith et al., 2013;Tan et al., 2020;Wildeboer et al., 2017). Reis and Sampaio (2019) conceptualize sharing as a behavioral component associated with moral reasoning, arguing that sharing shows ways in which children apply and judge norms of justice. ...
... Further studies used other conceptualizations such as moral motivation (Asendorpf & Nunner-Winkler, 1992) and moral abilities or competencies in a broader sense (Feldman, 2007;Garner, 2012;Tan et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
The importance of self-regulatory skills for the socio-emotional competencies of children is being researched and discussed extensively. However, in order to make a clear statement about the impact of self-regulation on children’s morality, a system- atic review of the literature is urgently needed. The aim of this systematic review and meta-analysis is to analyze associations between self-regulation and morality of preschool and elementary school children. In this context, distinctions among dif- ferent definitions and operationalizations of self-regulation and morality are considered. Search terms were entered in the bibliographic databases PsycINFO, Scopus and Web of Science. To meet the inclusion criterion, studies needed to report empirical associations between self-regulation and morality in children of preschool and elementary school age. Further- more, the studies should report primary data and be published in English in a peer-reviewed journal. Studies with secondary or summarized data, special populations or with certain designs were excluded. A total of 37 studies were included in the narrative synthesis. 35 of these studies were included in the meta-analysis. The narrative synthesis showed that different definitions and operationalizations were used for both self-regulation and morality. There also seems to be no consensus regarding the association between the constructs. Meta-analysis results revealed a small positive combined effect between self-regulation and morality, especially between temperament-related self-regulation and moral behavior and moral emo- tions. In order to gain a better understanding of the effects of self-regulation on morality, longitudinal research and further research addressing different forms of these constructs are essential.
... Patterns across primary studies that have investigated childhood EF using canonical performance-based measures are difficult to discern, given mixed findings and large variation in sample sizes and effect sizes. For example, one study found that EF measured in kindergarten is related to prosocial behaviors, even after controlling for sex, mother's education, and verbal and nonverbal IQ (Hubert et al., 2017); however, a more recent study found no such relation (Tan et al., 2021). Similarly, some studies suggest EF is negatively related to internalizing symptoms (Muñoz & Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous, 2011), whereas others have found the opposite (e.g., Troller-Renfree et al., 2019) or no relation (e.g., Henning et al., 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
Executive function (EF), the set of mental processes and skills involved in goal-oriented planning, organizing, and controlling behavior, is believed to support child development across many domains of life. However, although ample evidence suggests a relation between childhood EF and academic skills, it is less clear what its role is in domains beyond academics. We report a meta-analysis of relations between early childhood EF (assessed at 36–60 months of age) and social, health, and behavioral outcomes assessed concurrently and longitudinally (1,459 effect sizes, 158 studies, n = 144,642). No significant relations were found between EF and health outcomes. EF was concurrently associated with emotion understanding and regulation, prosocial skills, peer acceptance, and skill at lying. Both concurrently and longitudinally, EF was positively associated with peer acceptance, adaptive classroom behavior, and social competence, and negatively associated with internalizing and externalizing problems, and inattention and hyperactivity. For adolescent outcomes, only the relation between EF and internalizing could be meta-analyzed, and no significant relation was found between these variables. The magnitude of effect sizes varied, with absolute values ranging from r = .02 to .30. Moderator analyses indicated the relation between EF and behavioral outcomes varied by outcome, but there was no such finding for EF and social or health outcomes. EF was more strongly associated with teacher- versus parent-reported behavioral problems. There was little evidence that quality indicators affected effect sizes. There was also little evidence of publication bias. Overall, our results are consistent with the possibility of broad relevance of EF for child development; yet, future research should provide stronger causal tests of hypothesized relations between EF and outcomes to better understand the nature of EF and whether fostering it can promote healthy development.
... Im Weiteren werden die empirischen Originalarbeiten und -analysen beschrieben, die dem (Baker et al., 2021;Cowell et al., 2015Cowell et al., , 2017Hinnant et al., 2013;Tan, Mikami, Luzhanska & Hamlin, 2020;Wang, Hong, Pei, Wang & Su, 2021;Zelazo & Müller, 2011 (Eisenberg & Spinrad, 2004). Zudem wird in einer Studie zwischenmenschliche Emotionsregulation beschrieben, was die Regulierung der Emotionen anderer umfasst (Gummerum & López-Pérez, 2020 (Bandura, 2002;Guerra, Nucci & Huesmann, 1994). ...
Thesis
Ziel dieser Dissertation ist die differenzierte Erforschung verschiedener Konstrukte der Selbstregulation sowie die Untersuchung von Verhaltensproblemen im Kontext der Moralentwicklung und die Überprüfung eines Messinstruments zur Erfassung moralischer Konstrukte im Kindesalter. Zunächst wurde ein systematisches Review, einschließlich einer Meta-Analyse, durchgeführt. Die anschließenden empirischen Analysen basieren auf Quer- (Npilot = 194, NT1 = 348) und Längsschnittdaten (NT2 = 189) von Grundschulkindern und deren Sorgeberechtigten. Die moralische Entwicklung in der mittleren Kindheit ist komplex und wird von verschiedenen Faktoren beeinflusst. Diese Arbeit hebt die Rolle der Selbstregulation, insbesondere der Emotionsregulation, hervor und betont die Bedeutung einer differenzierten der Konzepte. Darüber hinaus verdeutlicht die Arbeit die Notwendigkeit eines validen Messinstruments zur Erfassung differenzierter moralischer Konstrukte. Sie zeigt auf, dass sowohl zu niedrige als auch zu hohe moralische Vorstellungen mit Verhaltensproblemen assoziiert sein können.
... Other studies have showed that higher level of ES may be linked to specific social behaviors, including a preference to pause to check and withdrawal from overstimulating situations (i.e., shyness; Aron et al., 2005). Prosocial behaviors require an intensive social involvement which can lead to higher difficulties to engage in prosocial behaviors especially in shy individuals (for example in children, Eisenberg et al., 2019;MacGowan & Schmidt, 2021;Tan et al., 2021). Thus, we can infer that higher levels of ES can also be linked to lower engagement in prosocial behaviors. ...
Article
Full-text available
Cooperation, as a mutual collaboration, is a defining feature of human social life. Individual characteristics can influence cooperation. Recent studies have shown a quadratic relationship between cardiac vagal tone (CVT), an index of self‐regulation, and prosocial behaviors. Individual differences in cooperation might also vary as a function of people's environmental sensitivity (ES), i.e., may be influenced by individual differences in the perception and processing of inner and external stimuli. We tested the direct and interactive effect of CVT and ES on cooperative behaviors in two independent samples. We measured heart rate and engagement in cooperative acts in 80 young adults (M = 23.61; SD = 1.60) who were also asked to self‐report on ES. We found an inverted‐U relationship between CVT and cooperative behaviors. ES, on the other hand, did not predict differences in cooperative behaviors but moderated the relationships between CVT and cooperation, although the magnitude of this result was small. Specifically, only among individuals with higher ES cooperative behaviors change as a function of CVT. Highly sensitive individuals with lower or higher CVT, were less cooperative than low sensitive ones. Subsequently, we replicated the same study design in a second sample of 88 undergraduate students (M = 23.69; SD = 4.91). Once again, we found evidence supporting the presence of an inverted‐U relationship between CVT and cooperative behaviors, and we also found that ES moderated the relationships between the quadratic CVT term and cooperation.
... However, previous research found a gap between children's normative stances and their actual sharing behavior (Blake, 2018;Smith et al., 2013;Tan et al., 2021), highlighting the complex relationship between moral decision-making and the act of sharing. Theories and empirical evidence emphasized the moral self-concept (MSC) as a potential moderator to bridge this gap (Blasi, 1983;. ...
... Patterns across primary studies that have investigated childhood EF using canonical performance-based measures are difficult to discern, given mixed findings and large variation in sample sizes and effect sizes. For example, one study found that EF measured in kindergarten is related to prosocial behaviors, even after controlling for sex, mother's education, and verbal and nonverbal IQ (Hubert et al., 2017); however, a more recent study found no such relation (Tan et al., 2021). Similarly, some studies suggest EF is negatively related to internalizing symptoms (Muñoz & Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous, 2011), whereas others have found the opposite (e.g., Troller-Renfree et al., 2019) or no relation (e.g., Henning et al., 2011). ...
Preprint
Preschool executive function (EF) is widely believed to support development across many domains of life. While it is established that EF predicts academic achievement, claims are also frequently made that it predicts social, health, and behavioral outcomes. These claims are often made on the basis of research assessing constructs that are related to but distinct from EF, such as self-control and self-regulation. The primary literature shows inconsistent patterns. We report a meta-analysis of relations between preschool EF in typically developing children and social, health, and behavioral outcomes measured concurrently, in middle childhood, and in adolescence (904 effect sizes, k = 125, n = 106,426). Preschool EF was related to several outcomes concurrently (prosociality, emotion regulation, lie understanding, lower body mass) and longitudinally (social competence, emotion understanding, peer acceptance, adaptive classroom behavior, lower externalizing behavior); however, effect sizes varied in magnitude and were smaller than those reported in a recent meta-analysis of self-regulation (Robson et al., 2020). Preschool EF was not related to internalizing behaviors or physical fitness. Meta-analysis of some longitudinal associations was precluded due to insufficient studies. Findings were not moderated by EF component (i.e., working memory, inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility). We did not find evidence that composite measures of EF were better predictors than individual indices. These results indicate that preschool EF, as commonly conceptualized and measured, may play a role in outcomes beyond academic achievement, but this role may be more limited than commonly thought. Interventions targeting preschool EF should be reevaluated in light of this evidence.
... Preschool EF skills like emotional regulation are also linked to children's social and academic success in kindergarten and beyond (Denham, 2006). Moreover, these EF skills have been linked to preschoolers' language skills (Fujik et al., 2002), vocabulary development (Bohlmann et al., 2015), theory of mind (Carlson & Moses, 2001;Devine & Hughes, 2014), and even their moral behavior (Tan et al., 2020). Taken together, EF plays an important role in children's academic and overall development. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Many of the skills children need to be successful in school are related to executive functioning (EF) skills. There is evidence that preschoolers’ EF skills, including their working memory, attention shifting, inhibition, and emotional regulation, help them to develop the academic and social-emotional skills required for success in school. Given how critically important these skills are, it is somewhat surprising that EF skills are not typically assessed in early education settings. Despite the absence of EF assessments in early childhood classrooms, there are a number of developmentally appropriate assessments that measure young children’s EF skills. This chapter focuses on the various methods used to assess EF skills in early childhood education settings. First, this chapter reviews the literature on EF skills in preschool-age children. Next, assessments that are appropriate for measuring EF skills in young children are described. Then, several evidence-based interventions that incorporate assessment of EF skills into the preschool program are discussed. The chapter concludes with recommendations for implementing EF assessments in early childhood education settings and future directions for research related to early assessment of EF skills.KeywordsExecutive functionEarly educationEarly interventionPreschoolAssessment
Article
Grossmann posits that heightened fearfulness in humans evolved to facilitate cooperative caregiving. We argue that three of his claims - that children express more fear than other apes, that they are uniquely responsive to fearful expressions, and that expression and perception of fear are linked with prosocial behaviors - are inconsistent with existing literature or require additional supporting evidence.
Article
Full-text available
The components of self-regulation were analyzed, extending the self-imposed delay of gratification paradigm to older children with social adjustment problems. Delay behavior was related to a network of conceptually relevant cognitive person variables, consisting of attention deployment strategies during delay, knowledge of delay rules, and intelligence. A positive relationship was demonstrated between concurrent indexes of intelligence, attention deployment, and actual delay time. Moreover, attention deployment, measured as an individual differences variable during the delay process, had a direct, positive effect on delay behavior. Specifically, as the duration of delay and the frustration of the situation increased, children who spent a higher proportion of the time distracting themselves from the tempting elements of the delay situation were able to delay longer. The effect of attention deployment on delay behavior was significant even when age, intelligence, and delay rule knowledge were controlled. Likewise, delay rule knowledge significantly predicted delay time, even when age, attention deployment, and intelligence were controlled.
Article
Full-text available
When young children recruit others to help a person in need, media reports often treat it as a remarkable event. Yet it is unclear how commonly children perform this type of pro-social behavior and what forms of social understanding, cognitive abilities, and motivational factors promote or discourage it. In this study, 48 three- to four-year-old children could choose between two actors to retrieve an out-of-reach object for a third person; during this event, one actor was physically unable to provide help. Nearly all of children’s responses appropriately incorporated the actors’ action capacities, indicating that rational prosocial reasoning—the cognitive basis for effective indirect helping—is common at this young age. However, only half of children actually directed an actor to help, suggesting that additional motivational factors constrained their prosocial actions. A behavioral measure of social inhibition and within-task scaffolding that increased children’s personal involvement were both strongly associated with children’s initiation of indirect helping behavior. These results highlight social inhibition and recognizing one’s own potential agency as key motivational challenges that children must overcome to recruit help for others.
Article
Full-text available
Many studies suggest that preschoolers initially privilege outcome over intention in their moral judgments. The present findings reveal that, in contrast, even younger preschoolers can privilege intentions when evaluating characters who successfully or unsuccessfully help or hinder a third party in achieving its goal. Following a live-action puppet show originally created for infant populations, children made a forced-choice social judgment (which puppet was liked) and two forced-choice moral judgments (which puppet was nicer, which puppet should be punished), and were asked to explain their punishment allocations. In two experiments (N = 195), 3- and 4-year-olds evaluated characters with distinct intentions to help or to hinder who were associated with either positive or negative outcomes. Both ages judged characters with more positive intentions as nicer, and allocated punishment to characters with more negative intentions; neither of these tendencies depended on the outcomes the characters were associated with. Three-year-olds’ responses were somewhat less consistent than were 4-year-olds’, in that 3-year-olds’ judgments were disrupted by ambiguous harmful intent. Notably, children’s social judgments were less consistent than their moral judgments. In a third and final experiment (N = 100), children evaluated characters with the same intention but who were associated with different outcomes. Children showed inconsistent responding across age and outcome valence, but only 4-year-olds evaluating two characters with positive intentions reliably responded based on outcome. When providing informative responses in all three studies, children most frequently explained their punishment allocations by appealing to the puppet’s (attempted) hindering action or failure to help. These findings raise questions as to what underlies different patterns of response across studies in the literature, and suggests that observing live interactions may facilitate young children’s intention-based moral judgments.
Article
From early in life, children help, comfort, and share with other people. Recent research has deepened scientific understanding of the development of prosociality—efforts to promote the welfare of others. In this article, we discuss two key insights about the emergence and early development of prosocial behavior, focusing on the development of helping. First, children’s motivations and capabilities for helping change in quality as well as quantity over the opening years of life. Specifically, helping begins in participatory activities without prosocial intent in the first year of life, becoming increasingly autonomous and motivated by prosocial intent over the second year. Second, helping emerges through bidirectional social interactions starting at birth: Caregivers and other individuals support the development of helping in a variety of ways, and young children play active roles that often influence caregiver behavior. The question now is not whether but how social interactions contribute to the development of prosocial behavior. Recent methodological and theoretical advances provide exciting avenues for future research on the social and emotional origins of human prosociality.
Article
Do children's values relate to their prosocial behavior? To answer this question, this study investigated N = 586 Australian children aged 6–12; the children reported their values and their prosocial behavior was assessed by peer nominations. As hypothesized, prosocial behavior was negatively correlated with self-enhancement values and positively correlated with self-transcendence and conservation values. In addition, age moderated the relations between values and prosocial behavior. For younger children, negative relations were found between openness-to-change values and prosocial behavior, but for older children, the relations were significantly positive. A mirror image appeared for the interaction of age and conservation values. The results have implications for values development among children and for moral development.
Article
The current study investigated associations between children's preferences and evaluations of moral and social-conventional transgressors in a novel puppet task and their links with explicit judgments in a standard interview. Children aged 2-3.25 years (M = 2.53 years, SD = 0.35) and 3.5-5 years (M = 4.38 years, SD = 0.52) watched two pairs of live puppet shows depicting actors committing a moral transgression and a conventional transgression and chose which transgressor they liked more, preferred more as a friend, thought was more wrong, and should get in more trouble; they also distributed resources to the transgressors. At both ages, children allocated fewer resources to moral transgressors than to conventional transgressors, but younger children's other responses did not exceed chance levels. In contrast, older children chose the moral transgressor as more wrong, more deserving of punishment, and less likeable. Preferences were associated with evaluations in the puppet task, particularly among older children. In contrast, all children differentiated between moral and conventional transgressions in their explicit judgments, with age differences found only in rule independence. More mature moral judgments, as assessed by latent difference scores reflecting moral-conventional distinctions, were associated with preferring to befriend the conventional transgressor and evaluating the moral transgressor as more wrong. Together, these results show age-related increases in children's moral understanding of-and stronger associations between-preferences and evaluations with age.
Article
A majority of current work indicates that the different types of prosocial behavior in young children (helping, sharing, comforting) are not related to each other. Here, I review recent studies that examined the relations between prosocial actions as well as the antecedents and correlates of the particular domains of prosociality. I argue that in addition to different social-cognitive demands also different motivations are involved in early prosocial action, and that prosociality is thus a concept that encompasses – at least early in development – heterogeneous behaviors and motivations.
Article
Two experiments explored preschoolers' social preferences and moral judgments of prosocial and antisocial others. In Experiment 1, 3- to 5-year-olds (N=74) observed helping and hindering scenarios previously used to explore sociomoral evaluation in preverbal infants. Whereas 3-year-olds in Experiment 1 did not reliably distinguish between the helper and hinderer when reporting social preferences or moral judgments, both 4- and 5-year-olds preferred the helper, judged the helper to be "nicer" than the hinderer, selectively allocated punishment to the hinderer, and were able to justify their punishment allocations. A simplified procedure and the addition of comprehension questions in Experiment 2 (N=24) improved 3-year-olds' performance, suggestive that their performance in Experiment 1 was likely due to processing or memory difficulties rather than an inability to engage in explicit social and moral evaluation. These studies reveal that young children readily interpret helping and hindering scenarios as socially and morally relevant.