PresentationPDF Available

The evolution of syntax: What do we know?

Authors:

Abstract

An explorative and narrative review of the literature on language evolution with an emphasis on the psycholinguistics and neurolinguistics of lexical semantics and syntax. Based on my thesis for an M. A. degree in Linguistics.
The evolution of
syntax: what do we
know?
PETAR GABRIĆ
GENERAL LINGUISTICS, DEPARTMENT OF ENGLISH AND LINGUISTICS, JOHANNES
GUTENBERG-UNIVERSITÄT MAINZ
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
The evolution of
syntax: what do we
know?
PETAR GABRIĆ
GENERAL LINGUISTICS, DEPARTMENT OF ENGLISH AND LINGUISTICS, JOHANNES
GUTENBERG-UNIVERSITÄT MAINZ
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
NOTHING
PROLOGUE: do we know anything
about language evolution?
For centuries, people have speculated over the origins of human language. [] The
irony is that the quest is a fruitless one. Each generation asks the same questions, and
reaches the same impasse the absence of any evidence relating to the matter,
given the vast, distant time-scale involved. We have no direct knowledge of the
origins and early development of language, nor is it easy to imagine how such
knowledge might ever be obtained. We can only speculate, arrive at our own
conclusions, and remain dissatisfied. Indeed, so dissatisfied was one group of 19th-
century scholars that they took drastic action: in 1866, the Linguistic Society of Paris
published an edict banning discussion of the topic at their meetings. But the
theorizing continues, and these days there is a resurgence of interest, as new
archaeological finds and modern techniques of analysis provide fresh hints of what
may once have been.(Crystal 1998)
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Contents
Human evolution
Genetics
Epistemology
Exaptation vs. modularity
Continuity vs. discontinuity
Exaptation and
continuity
Animacy and oncreteness/abstractness
Action semantics
Embodied lexical
semantics
Transitivity
Word order
Embodied syntax
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Human evolution
It would appear that the modern-human-origins debate is not for
the faint of heart.(Lindly & Clark 1990:251)
Human evolution is a highly controversial topic both within and
outside of the scientific realm.
Creationism
General public etc.
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Source:
ScienceAlert
EPISTEMOLOGY: Human evolution
Language evolution and
archaeology (1)
Language evolution is necessarily an interdisciplinary topic.
Linguistic studies and discussion are few and far between.
Much of the work so far has been done by palaeoanthropologists
and archaeologists. Their hypotheses remain, however, mostly
speculative.
E.g. the putative perforated Nassarius kraussianus shell beads from
South Africa (Henshillwood et al.2004;d’Errico et al.2005)
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
EPISTEMOLOGY: Human evolution
Language evolution and
archaeology (2)
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Source: Botha (2009: 95)
EPISTEMOLOGY: Human evolution
Language evolution and
archaeology (3)
What if all eighty remaining speakers of Banawá died out suddenly and
their bones were discovered only 100,000 years hence? [W]ould their
material culture leave any evidence that they were capable of
language and symbolic reasoning? Arguably it would leave even less
evidence of language than has been found for neanderthalensis or
erectus.Banawá art [] and their tools [] are biodegradable. So their
material culture would disappear without a trace in much less than the
800,000 to 1,500,000 years that have passed since the appearance of the
earliest cultures. []It is known that current populations of Amazonians
have fully developed human languages and rich cultures, so care must
be taken not to conclude prematurely that the absence of evidence
about language or culture in the prehistoric recrod indicates that ancient
human populations lacked these essential cognitive attributes.(Everett
2016: 89)
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
EPISTEMOLOGY: Human evolution
Language evolution and genetics
Axiomatically, language evolution began via mutations: „First, there
is mutation. [] Without mutation, there could be no evolution”
(McMahon & McMahon 2013: 8).
Genetics of language remains largely understudied.
Some topics in the genetics of language (evolution) include:
forkhead box protein P2 (FOXP2), Williams syndrome, specific
language impairment, ProtocadherinX/Y etc.
„[S]ince genes operate in vast interacting networks affected by
both each other and the environment, the concept of the gene for
…’ is at best oversimplified and at worst fundamentally flawed.
(McMahon & McMahon 2013:178)
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
EPISTEMOLOGY: Genetics
Chomsky’s „genetic model”
Noam Chomsky has become famous for proposing a mutation-
basedaccount of language evolution. However, his hypotheses
are, in short, biologically nonsensical.
„[T]he original mutation will typically have had only a small effect, and
that subsequent development of the trait in question will have to wait
for further relevant mutations to arise.(McMahon & McMahon 2013: 9)
EPISTEMOLOGY: Genetics
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Negation of genetics in language
evolution
Some linguists are reluctant to include genetics into language
evolution models, e.g. Everett (2016:7071):
„[M]utations for language are superfluous because language evolution
can be explained without them. []In fact, the idea of language as a
mutation simply offers no insights at all that help to understand the
evolution of language. That is to say that language evolution can be
explained without mutations, based instead on gradual, uniformitarianist
assumptions, rendering superfluous proposals of language-specific
genes or language-specific mutations.(p.7071)
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
EPISTEMOLOGY: Genetics
Language evolution and
(cumulative) culture
Today many stress the role of (cumulative) culture in language evolution, but
some also appear to misunderstand it.
Language gradually emerged from a culture, formed by people who communicated
one another via human brains. Language is the handmaiden of culture.(Everett 2016:
xvii)
The term culture is in need of a more precise definition.
We find that 39 different behaviour patterns, including tool usage, grooming and
courtship behaviours, are customary or habitual in some communities but are absent in
others where ecological explanations have been discounted. Among mammalian and
avian species, cultural variation has previously been identified only for single behaviour
patterns, such as the local dialects of song-birds. The extensive, multiple variations now
documented for chimpanzees are thus without parallel. Moreover, the combined
repertoire of these behaviour patterns in each chimpanzee community is itself highly
distinctive, a phenomenon characteristic of human cultures but previously unrecognised
in non-human species.(Whiten et al.1999:682)
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
EPISTEMOLOGY: Genetics
Language evolution as gene-
culture co-evolution
Language evolution is a
case of gene-culture co-
evolution, as in the case of
the appearance of milk
consumption.
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Source: Curry (2013)
EPISTEMOLOGY: Genetics
Exaptation in evolutionary biology
Exaptation = the process of the emergence of structures and/or
functions from pre-existing structures and/or functions (Gould & Vrba
1982)
Source: manfredrichter
(Pixabay)
Bird feathers
Heat
regulation,
display, flight
Anatomy
Mouth licking
in wolves and
domestic dogs
Begging for
food, signal for
submissiveness
Behavior
Source: ElfinFox (Pixabay)
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Exaptation vs. modularity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Exaptation in human evolution
Exaptationist evolution vs. modular evolution
Exaptation has played a crucial role in the evolution of parts of the
human muscular system.
Each and every muscle that has been long accepted to be “uniquely
human” and to provide “crucial singular functional adaptations” for our
bipedalism, tool use and/or vocal/facial communication, is actually
present as an intra-specific variant or even as normal phenotype in
bonobos and/or other apes.(Diogo 2018: 1)
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Exaptation vs. modularity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Domain-general systems, and
speech (1)
In the context of speech and language evolution we are talking
about the exaptation from pre-existing brain structures and functions.
Motor brain areas are involved in speech production via activation of
motor plans and muscle articulators, as well as movement
coordination.
Lateral and midsagittal
views of crucial speech
cortical areas (including
reading)
Source: Mildner (2015:36)
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Exaptation vs. modularity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Domain-general systems, and
speech (2)
Motor areas are also probably involved in speech perception (e.g.
the motor theory of speech perception).
Bishop (2001) reports correlations and considerable genetic overlap
between developmental dyspraxia and specific language
impairment, mostly in speech production quality.
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Exaptation vs. modularity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Domain-general systems, and
language (1)
Concerning language evolution, most likely candidates for
exaptation are:
1. sensorimotor processing (including visuospatial processing)
2. declarative and procedural memory
3. executive functioning and general working memory
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Exaptation vs. modularity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Domain-general systems, and
language (2)
De Beni et al. (2005): role of verbal and visuospatial working memory
in language reception
Listening of spatialand non-spatialtexts with concurrent cognitive
tasks
Van Beilen et al. (2004): correlations between semantic fluency and
executive functioning
Hamrick et al. (2018): role of procedural memory in grammar
acquisition and advanced grammar learning
The findings yielded large effect sizes and held consistently across
languages, language families, linguistic structures, and tasks(Hamrick
et al.2018:1487)
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Exaptation vs. modularity
Hauser et al. (2002) and the faculty
of language (1)
Hauser, Chomsky & Fitch (2002): faculty of language in the broad
sense (FLB) vs. faculty of language in the narrow sense (FLN)
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Exaptation vs. modularity
Source: Hauser et al. (2002)
Their assumptions are axiomatic.
Their assumptions are introspective.
Recursion is never defined (!).
It is not clear why recursionshould be
considered as the only uniquely human
component of the faculty of language.
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Hauser et al. (2002) and the faculty
of language (2)
[Hauser et al.] (2002)is an ambitious paper, written by a well-known
and interdisciplinary team of authors, and published in a very
prominent scientific journal.(McMahon & McMahon 2013:199)
Recursion, often undefined except in the loosest of senses, has
been seen by some as a symbolic last-ditch stand for a domain-
specific innatist view of human uniqueness. This is how Hauser et al.
(2002) have been interpreted, and indeed possibly how their paper
was intended.(Hurford 2012:390)
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Exaptation vs. modularity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Embodiment doesn’t necessarily
imply exaptation
Synchronic embodiment can suggest phylogenetic exaptation
(Occam’s razor).
The alternative, modular, hypothesis is problematic:
It would imply that there were two phases of language evolution
(language emergence and language embodiment).
It would imply that there was a significant brain reorganization in the
wake of various genetic mutations.
It doesn’t explain why some linguistic phenomena are more embodied
than others.
It is unclear how a modular language system would have functioned.
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Exaptation vs. modularity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Cartesian philosophy, discontinuity
and the modernhumanities
Much of the resistance to attributing concepts to animals comes from
philosophers and other scholars in the humanities. Until quite recently,
the detailed workings of the brain have been terra incognita, and
introspection by humans about their own mental activity was the main
source of theorizing about concepts. Thinkers for whom their own
mental activity is the foundation of all knowledge have some distance
to travel before they can be convinced that other people have minds,
let alone that animals have them, and let alone that the contents of
animal minds include entities like our own concepts. The Cartesian
tradition emanating from cogito ergo sum still has a strong grip. The
main objection to solipsism is that it flies in the face of common sense.
Neuroscientists and animal researchers have their own more
advanced common sense, and most start from a common assumption
of continuity between animal and human minds.(Hurford 2007: 9)
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Continuity vs. discontinuity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Humans are primates (1)
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Continuity vs. discontinuity
Source: Smithsonian Institution
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Humans are primates (2)
Studies testing linguistic laws outside language have provided
important insights into the organization of biological systems. For
example, patterns consistent with Zipf’s law of abbreviation (which
predicts a negative relationship between word length and frequency
of use) have been found in the vocal and non-vocal behaviour of a
range of animals, and patterns consistent with Menzerath’s law
(according to which longer sequences are made up of shorter
constituents) have been found in primate vocal sequences []. Both
laws have been linked to compressionthe information theoretic
principle of minimizing code length. (Heesen et al.2019: 1)
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Continuity vs. discontinuity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Bickerton’s „paradox of continuity
(1)
[L]anguage was first and foremost a system of representation. It
was therefore, like all other such systems, a mechanism that to a
large extent created its own outputrather than merely replicating,
in another mode, what was fed into it. Precisely because of this,
language was able to increase, by several orders of magnitude, not
just the things but the kinds of thing that creatures could
communicate about. No mere communicative mechanism could
ever have done this. Thus, if we are to seek for the ultimate origins of
language, we cannot hope to find those origins by looking at the
means by which other creatures communicate with one another.
(Bickerton 1990:75)
LANGUAGE IS UNIQUE.
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Continuity vs. discontinuity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Bickerton’s „paradox of continuity
(2)
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Continuity vs. discontinuity
Problems
with
Bickerton’s
paradox
of
continuity
Absence of evidence about the existence of a
particular phenomenon in a sample doesn’t imply
nonexistence of the phenomenon in the sample, let
alone nature (!).
Some aspects of animal communication systems can
be characterized as productive.
It is not clear why should qualitative/quantitative
differences between language and animal
communication systems be taken as arguments for
discontinuity.
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Ideas of discontinuity in linguistics
(1)
It would appear that the argumentation of discontinuity in linguistics
is following:
Language is phylogenetically independent on animal
communication systems because language is unique”, and
language is uniquebecause it appears sure to the naked linguist’s
eye that language has certain features which are lacking in animal
communication systems.
It is not clear how such argumentation is theoretically or empirically
valid from the viewpoint of the present models of evolutionary
biology.
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Continuity vs. discontinuity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Ideas of discontinuity in linguistics
(2)
[M]an bears in his bodily structure clear traces of his descent from some lower
form; it may be urged that, as man differs so greatly in his mental power from
all other animals, there must be some error in this conclusion. No doubt the
difference in this respect is enormous […] If no organic being excepting man
had possessed any mental power, or if his powers had been of a wholly
different nature from those of the lower animals, then we should never have
been able to convince ourselves that our high faculties had been gradually
developed. But it can be clearly shewn that there is no fundamental
difference of this kind. We must also admit that there is a much wider interval
in mental power between one of the lowest fishes, as a lamprey or lancelet,
and one of the higher apes, than between an ape and man; yet this immense
interval is filled up by numberless gradations. […] [T]here is no fundamental
difference between man and the higher mammals in their mental faculties.
(Darwin 2013: 2930)
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Continuity vs. discontinuity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
(„Lexical”) Semantics in vervet
communication (1)
Struhsaker (1967) and Seyfarth et al. (1980): semantic communication
in vervets (Chlorocebus pygerythrus)
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Continuity vs. discontinuity
Source:
Amandad
(Pixabay)
Alarm calls for five predators:
leopard (Panthera pardus),
martial eagle (Polemaetus
bellicosus), African rock
python (Python sebae),
babbons (Papio) and
unfamiliar humans
Vervet alarm calls are
semantic (denotative) and
symbolic (in Saussurean
sense)
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
(„Lexical”) Semantics in vervet
communication (2)
Vervet alarm calls appear to be at least partially learned (vs. innate).
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Continuity vs. discontinuity
Source: Seyfarth et al. (1980: 802)
(„Lexical”) Semantics in animal
communication (3)
There are other similar examples in other animals, and not only mammals:
Digweed et al. (2005) describe two such calls in white-faced capuchin
monkeys: one call for bird predators, and one more general call for a
range of snakes and mammals. Writing of the pale-winged trumpeter, an
Amazonian bird, Seddon et al. (2002, p. 1331) write: ‘On detection of
danger, trumpeters gave two acoustically different calls, one for aerial
predators, and another for terrestrial predators or conspecific intruders.
They also produced distinct calls on detection of large prey items such as
snakes. These (alarm and snake-finding) call types seemed to evoke
different responses by receivers and therefore appeared to be
functionally referent.Domestic chickens also have alarm calls
differentiated for aerial and ground predators (Karakashian et al.1988;
Evans et al.1993).(Hurford 2007:226)
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Continuity vs. discontinuity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Syntax in naturalanimal
communication (1)
„Birds sing in combinations of notes, but the individual notes don’t
mean anything. A very complex series of notes, such as nightingale’s,
only conveys a message of sexual attractiveness or a threat to rival
male birds. So birdsong has syntax, but no compositional semantics. It is
the same with complex whale songs. Despite this major difference from
human language, we can learn some good lessons from closer study
of birds’ and whales’ songs. They show a control of phrasal structure,
often quite complex. The songs also suggest that quantitative
constraints on the length and phrasal complexity of songs cannot be
naturally separated from their structure.(Hurford 2012: 1)
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Continuity vs. discontinuity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Syntax in naturalanimal
communication (2)
Suzuki et al. (2016) claim to have found semantic syntax in the
Japanese tits (Parus minor)
EXAPTATION AND CONTINUITY: Continuity vs. discontinuity
Source: Santa3 (Pixabay)
scan for danger
ABC
approach the caller
D
scan and approach
ABC-D
mostly no change in
behavior
D-ABC
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Concept modality vs. amodality
Semantic version of the embodiment vs. modularity dichotomy
[T]here is not a general consensus on the specific properties that
distinguish modal and amodal representations, and different authors
adopt different criteria, which leads to a cross classification of certain
representations depending on the chosen criterion.(Haimovici 2018:10)
EMBODIED LEXICAL SEMANTICS
Source: Pulvermüller (2013: 465)
Animacy and biological motion (1)
Animacy is a semantic as well as a morphosyntactic category.
E.g. in Serbo-Croatian”:Vidim miš-Ø.vs.Vidim miš-a. ‘I see a mouse
(Barić et al.2005:104)
Animacy is cognitively related to the detection of biological motion:
‘Biological motion’ is a label attached to a kind of motion typical of an
animal; it is distinct from trees waving in the wind, rocks tumbling down a
cliff, waves in the sea, or eddies in a stream. Recognizing biological
motion is not just a matter of certain sensors being excited. There has to
be a quite complex calculation of the temporal and spatial relations
among the moving parts. (Hurford 2007:41)
Animacy can be understood as potential biological motion (Hurford
2007:43).
EMBODIED LEXICAL SEMANTICS: Animacy and concreteness/abstractness
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Animacy and biological motion (2)
Detection of biological motion appears to be innate in humans as well as
other animals:
Here we report that newly hatched chicks, reared and hatched in darkness, at
their first exposure to point-light animation sequences, exhibit a spontaneous
preference to approach biological motion patterns. Intriguingly, this predisposition
is not specific for the motion of a hen, but extends to the pattern of motion of other
vertebrates, even to that of a potential predator such as a cat. The predisposition
seems to reflect the existence of a mechanism in the brain aimed at orienting the
young animal towards objects that move semi-rigidly (as vertebrate animals do),
thus facilitating learning, i.e., through imprinting, about their more specific features
of motion.(Vallortigara et al.2005:1312)
The biological motion studies take off from a paper by Johansson (1973), who
showed that human babies attend preferentially to mobile patterns generated by
lights attached to the joints of moving animals.(Hurford 2007:42)
EMBODIED LEXICAL SEMANTICS: Animacy and concreteness/abstractness
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Lexical animacy
In linguistics, meaning of animate words is believed to be founded
on sensory features as opposed to functional features which
characterize the meaning of inanimate words (e.g. Caramazza &
Shelton 1998).
EMBODIED LEXICAL SEMANTICS: Animacy and concreteness/abstractness
elephant
ears
tail
feet
trunk scissors
shear
blade
metal
cut
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
The animacy effect
Animacy is a prominent cognitive category that affects processing in
other cognitive domains, e.g. the so called animacy effect on memory
tasks.
Animate words and visual depictions of animate concepts are better
remembered relative to inanimate words and concepts (Bonin et al.
2014; Nairne et al.2013).
The animacy effect is also found in metamemory (Li et al.2016).
EMBODIED LEXICAL SEMANTICS: Animacy and concreteness/abstractness
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Lexical concreteness/abstractness
Concreteness and abstractness are not categorical, but gradual
measures (Peti-Stantić et al.2018).
Concrete words are recognized faster and are remembered better
compared to abstract words (Kroll & Merves 1986; Marschark &
Paivio 1977; James 1975).
Cognitive linguistics: abstract semantics arises from concrete
semantics via mechanisms of metaphor and image schemas (e.g.
Lakoff 1987; Taylor 2003).
EMBODIED LEXICAL SEMANTICS: Animacy and concreteness/abstractness
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Lexical concreteness/abstractness:
neuroscience (1)
Neuroscience of language is currently in development: [I]t remains
a [] challenge to integrate neuroscience with the social sciences”
(Stout & Hecht 2015).
Concrete words are processed bilaterally with a modest leftward
asymmetry”, while abstract words appear to be left-lateralized
(Binder et al.2005; Mildner 2015:199).
EMBODIED LEXICAL SEMANTICS: Animacy and concreteness/abstractness
Source: Pulvermüller (2013: 465)
Lexical concreteness/abstractness:
neuroscience (2)
Abstract words are also relatively embodied, namely in motor and
emotional brain regions.
Moseley et al. (2012): fMRI, passive listening of emotional (e.g.
dread,spite), face (e.g. gnaw,chew) and hand verbs (e.g. peel,
grasp)
Emotional verbs correlated with activation in the limbic and primary
motor cortices (incl. face and hand areas).
We conclude that, similar to their role in action word processing,
activation of frontocentral motor systems in the dorsal stream reflects
the semantic binding of sign and meaning of abstract words denoting
emotions and possibly other body internal states.(p.1634)
EMBODIED LEXICAL SEMANTICS: Animacy and concreteness/abstractness
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Lexical concreteness/abstractness:
neuroscience (3)
Dreyer & Pulvermüller (2018): fMRI, passive reading of mentalwords
such as thought,logic etc. compared to action words
They found activation in the face area of the primary motor cortex
associated with processing of mentalwords.
We conclude that a role of motor systems in semantic processing is not
restricted to concrete words but extends to at least some abstract mental
symbols previously thought to be entirely ‘disembodied’ and divorced
from semantically related sensorimotor processing.(p.52)
EMBODIED LEXICAL SEMANTICS: Animacy and concreteness/abstractness
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Action semantics: neuroscience (1)
Neurophysiological studies show somatotopic activation of words
related to face/mouth, hand/arm and foot actions.
Hauk et al. (2004): fMRI, passive reading of face (e.g. lick), hand
(e.g. pick) and foot action verbs (e.g. kick)
This rules out a unified “meaning center” in the human brain and
supports a dynamic view according to which words are processed by
distributed neuronal assemblies with cortical topographies that reflect
word semantics(p.301)
EMBODIED LEXICAL SEMANTICS: Animacy and concreteness/abstractness
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Action semantics: neuroscience (2)
Studies investigating the ACE effect (action-sentence compatibility
effect) support the notion of the embodiment of action semantics.
Mollo et al. (2016): EEG and MEG, motoric priming during lexical and
semantic decision with hand and foot action verbs (SOA: 500 ms)
Congruency effects were found in motor areas, but also the superior
temporal cortex.
The connection between motor and language functions is also found in
areas traditionally associated with language or semantic functions.
EMBODIED LEXICAL SEMANTICS: Animacy and concreteness/abstractness
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Action semantics: neuroscience (3)
Studies using transcranial magnetic stimulation also support the
embodiment theory of action semantics.
Pulvermüller et al. (2005): TMS of leftmotor areas during lexical
decision with hand and foot action verbs
Arm area TMS led to faster arm than leg word responses and the
reverse effect, faster lexical decisions on leg than arm words, was
present when TMS was applied to leg areas.(p.793)
Evidence of embodiment is also found in psychosemantics, e.g. the
BOI measure (bodyobject interaction), but also measures of
graspability, ease of gesticulation and the number of possible
actions (Heard et al.2018; Siakaluk et al.2008a,b).
EMBODIED LEXICAL SEMANTICS: Animacy and concreteness/abstractness
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Neural correlates of linguistic syntax
As semantic processing, syntactic processing is also associated with
distributed neural activation in the frontotemporoparietal areas.
The involvement of left-hemispheric middle and superior temporal,
inferior-posterior parietal, as well as inferior frontal brain regions in various
sentence comprehension tasks has been revealed by a host of
neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies []. Part of this
supportive brain network is also the left-hemispheric lateral premotor
cortex, sometimes extending more posteriorly into the primary motor
area and more anteriorly into the middle frontal gyrus []. The
involvement of the motor system in sentence processing is not only due
to phonological and articulatory mapping [] because it also provides
a grounding node for certain kinds of conceptual-semantic information.
(Ghio & Tettamanti 2016:647)
EMBODIED SYNTAX
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Neural correlates of linguistic syntax
As semantic processing, syntactic processing is also associated with
distributed neural activation in the frontotemporoparietal areas.
The involvement of left-hemispheric middle and superior temporal,
inferior-posterior parietal, as well as inferior frontal brain regions in various
sentence comprehension tasks has been revealed by a host of
neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies []. Part of this
supportive brain network is also the left-hemispheric lateral premotor
cortex, sometimes extending more posteriorly into the primary motor
area and more anteriorly into the middle frontal gyrus []. The
involvement of the motor system in sentence processing is not only due
to phonological and articulatory mapping [] because it also provides
a grounding node for certain kinds of conceptual-semantic information.
(Ghio & Tettamanti 2016:647)
EMBODIED SYNTAX
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Action production (and
understanding)
EMBODIED SYNTAX
Source: Stout (2010: 165)
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Transitivity in theoretical linguistics
(1)
Semantic vs. syntactic transitivity
Semantic transitivity is not a categorical, but a gradual phenomenon
Prototypical semantic transitivity: a volitional agent acts on a patient by
changing his state or position (thus, the typical agent is human)
Hitting events are not prototypically transitive events, because the
affected (or non-agentive) participant in a hitting event does not undergo
a change of state or position, and consequently is not a typical patient. As
regards eating events, the point is that the primary motivation of the
action performed by the active participant in an eating event is not to
change the state of the other participant or control its position, but rather
to satisfy a physiological need, and consequently, the active participant in
an eating event is not a typical agent.(Creissels 2016:18)
The typical patient is inanimate?
EMBODIED SYNTAX: Transitivity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Transitivity in theoretical linguistics
(2)
Core transitive verbs: verbs which code prototypical semantic events
(basic transitive coding)
Compare: He broke the window. vs.I feel love. vs.She is crossing the
street./She was sleeping all night.
Core transitive verbs are considered to be a linguistic universal and to
show „a high degree of formal homogeneity.
This suggests a cognitive prominence of this semantic class of verbs, and
justifies giving it a central status in a typology of argument coding and in
a typology of the interface between argument structure and
morphosyntax.(Creissels 2016:1920)
EMBODIED SYNTAX: Transitivity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Transitivity in theoretical linguistics
(3)
Wright (2001, 2002) differentiates between externally and internally
caused change of state verbs (e.g. break vs. bloom).
Internally caused change of state verbs relative to externally
caused change of state verbs
are significantly less coded in transitive frames,
they include significantly more often natural forces as agents,
and that they are rated as significantly less acceptable in transitive
frames.
EMBODIED SYNTAX: Transitivity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Neuroscience of syntactic
transitivity (1)
The methodology in these studies is a horror-story.
Tettamanti et al. (2005): fMRI, passive listening of sentences with face,
foot and hand-related actions („abstractsentences as controls)
Sentences were created by matching a transitive verb in the first-person
singular to a syntactically and semantically congruent object complement.
Each verb was paired with different objects each time. Subjects heard a
total of 40 sentences per experimental condition (mouth, hand, leg), plus
120 baseline (abstract) sentences.
Ghio & Tettamanti (2016: 649) claim that „sentence of the subjectverb
object typewere used.
Kemmerer (2012: 58), however, writes: „[P]articipants listened passively to
four types of Italian sentences that were syntactically equivalent but
described different kinds of situations: leg foot actions (e.g., Calcio il pallone
‘‘I kick the ball’’);arm⁄hand actions (e.g., Afferro il coltello ‘‘I grasp a knife’’);
mouth actions (e.g., Mordo la mela ‘‘I bite an apple’’); and psychological
states (e.g., Apprezzo la sincerita ‘‘I appreciate sincerity’’).
EMBODIED SYNTAX: Transitivity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Neuroscience of syntactic
transitivity (2)
Tettamanti et al. (2005): Methodology remains shrouded in mystery,
but it appears they compared transitive constructions which were
semantically transitive in the experimental set and intransitive in the
control set.
Compared to the control set, experimental sentences evoked greater
activation in the frontotemporoparietal network incl. the pars
opercularis (BA 44), premotoric cortex, inferior parietal lobule,
intraparietal sulcus and pMTG.
These data provide the first direct evidence that listening to sentences
that describe actions engages the visuomotor circuits which subserve
action execution and observation.(action semantics)
Syntactic transitivity doesn’t have a unified neural correlate.
Is the embodiment effect due to the entire transitive scenario or due to
the meaning of particular components (e.g. verbs)?
EMBODIED SYNTAX: Transitivity
Neuroscience of syntactic
transitivity (3)
Desai et al. (2010): fMRI, active listening of hand and arm-related
action sentences compared to abstractsentences (e.g. They
consider the risk.).
Experimental sentences produced greater activation in parts of the
parietal and temporal cortices.
Previous question remain unanswered.
Similar studies with similar results and similarly enigmatic interpretations
concerning the embodiment of semantic and syntactic transitivity
include Van Dam & Desai (2016), Aziz-Zadeh et al. (2006), Scorolli et al.
(2012), Progovac et al. (2018).
EMBODIED SYNTAX: Transitivity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Neuroscience of syntactic
transitivity (4)
Ferretti et al. (2001): transitive verbs prime typical agents (arrestingcop),
patients (arrestingcriminal) and instruments (stirredspoon), but not
locations (swamocean)
A short SOA (250 ms) was used, indicating an automatic neural connection.
Results suggest that we can really talk about the embodiment of semantic
transitivity in previous studies as it would be difficult to separate the effects of
verb meaning, and agents and patients (and instruments).
Glenberg & Kaschak (2002): ACE, hand actions and transfer sentences
differing in the direction of action/transfer
Imperative sentences, sentences denoting transfer of concrete objects,
sentences denoting „transfer of abstract entitites
ACE was found for all sentence types!
EMBODIED SYNTAX: Transitivity
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Basic word order
Basic word order is a controversial linguistic construct (Newmeyer
2003):
There are no clear criteria for determining basic word order across
languages.
Some languages appear to have no basic word order.
The universalist approach to the subject and object components is
potentiall problematic from a cross-linguistic perspective.
The relative frequency of the sentence type used for establishing the
basic word order is extremely low.
Nevertheless, Newmeyer (2003) concludes that the basic word
order approach is okay.
EMBODIED SYNTAX: Word order
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Basic word order from a cross-
linguistic perspective
EMBODIED SYNTAX: Word order
According to: Dryer (2011)
SOV and SVO are also
the dominant word orders
in sign languages (Napoli
& Sutton-Spence 2014).
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Dominance of SOV and SVO word
orders
Additional evidence that SOV and SVO word orders are overwhelmingly preferred comes
from several other sources. First, Kimmelman [2012] found that in a sample of 24 sign
languages, 21 (88%) have SOV and ⁄or SVO as the dominant sequencing pattern(s).
Second, within the last 70 years, Al-Sayyid Bedouin Sign Language (ABSL) has gradually
arisen in an isolated community with a high incidence of genetically based prelingual
deafness, and in the space of a single generation, it assumed a grammatical structure
characterized by SOV order (Sandler et al. 2005). Given that none of the neighboring
spoken or signed languages are SOV, this property of ABSL presumably developed
spontaneously. Third, Goldin-Meadow et al. (2008) asked speakers of three SVO languages
(English, Spanish, and Mandarin) and one SOV language (Turkish) to perform two non-
verbal tasks: first, describe events using manual gestures without speech; and second,
reconstruct events illustrated in pictures. The investigators found that in both tasks all of the
participants were strongly inclined to use the same sequencing strategy specifically,
agent-patient-action, which is analogous to the SOV pattern in spoken languages. Taken
together, these three sets of results support the view that SOV and SVO word orders
perhaps especially the former reflect the most cognitively natural ways of linearizing the
fundamental elements in a transitive clause. (Kemmerer 2012:52)
EMBODIED SYNTAX: Word order
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Agent/subject saliency (1)
Two principles appear to affect the word order proportions: subject
saliency, and verbobject juxtapositioning
According to Kemmerer (2012), subject saliency reflects how the
brain understands core transitive events in which the agent is the
head of a causal chain affecting the patient
Agent saliency is evidenced in empirical studies (Cohn & Paczynski
2013; Cohn et al.2017):
information about the agent compared to the patient facilitates
prediction of action in the future
agents are longer viewed in visual depictions than patients
visual depictions primed by agents are processed faster compared to
depictions primed by patients
EMBODIED SYNTAX: Word order
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Agent/subject saliency (2)
Nominative-accusative languages are cross-linguistically more frequent
compared to ergative-absolutive languages (Bickel et al.2015; Nichols
1993)
Bornkessel et al. (2004): ERP, reception of dependent object clauses in
which the syntactic and semantic roles are ambiguous until the end of
the sentence
dass Betram Surferinnen gratuliert hat.vs.dass Betram Surferinnen
gratuliert haben.
A combination of a biphasic negativity after 400 ms and late positivity in
the latter sentence type
Results suggest that the first argument is automatically processed as an
agent until further analysis shows otherwise
Similar results by Bickel et al. (2015) who investigated Hindi.
EMBODIED SYNTAX: Word order
Agent/subject saliency and Broca’s
area (1)
Kemmerer (2012) speculates that the two dominant word orders reflect the
ways in which Broca’s area processes actions in general.
Broca’s area is a very controversial topic and it appears that today nobody
really knows precisely what Broca’s area is, nor where it is.
Nevertheless, theorizing and research continue (as they should).
Broca’s area is highly multifunctional: motor skills in tool use (Hopkins et al.2017),
production of nonverbal motor behaviors (Hupfeld et al.2017), nonverbal
action understanding (Fazio et al.2009), imitation (Kühn et al.2013), music
(Elmer et al.2018), visuospatial perception (Bahlmann et al.), speech,
language, etc.
These findings have prompted a search for a common functional denominator, and
some of the major candidates currently being debated are cognitive control,
sequence processing, and hierarchical processing.(Kemmerer 2012:55)
EMBODIED SYNTAX: Word order
Agent/subject saliency and Broca’s
area (2)
In short, SOV and SVO would reflect the temporal structure of the
causal action chain which is coded in Broca’s area, and which is
enabled through a phylogenetically older system of sequential and
hierarchical organization of bodily movements and actions.
Kemmerer (2012:62) admits his theory is speculative: Although this
proposal is grounded in a wealth of empirical studies, it is admittedly
a bold and speculative attempt to bridge the gap between
linguistic typology and cognitive neuroscience. Whether it is on the
right track remains to be seen, but hopefully it will stimulate further
research on the interaction between the cross-linguistic
representation of action and the functional architecture of Broca’s
area.
EMBODIED SYNTAX: Word order
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Conclusions (1)
While I have given no direct evidence for exaptation in language evolution,
relative synchronic embodiment of particular linguistic functions suggests
that exaptation is a more plausible evolutionary scenario than modularity.
Animal studies suggest that at least some animals have symbolic
communication (in the Saussurean sense). Furthermore, at least some
animals display syntactic capacities within their communication systems.
These results stand opposed to the discontinuity theories of language
evolution.
There is strong evidence for the embodiment of particular lexical-semantic
phenomena. This includes concrete and action words, as well as abstract
words. Bilaterality of the processing of concrete meanings suggests that
functional laterelization was not a prerequisite for concrete words. Many
studies reported somatotopic association between action verbs and brain
activation.
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Conclusions (2)
Animacy appears to rely on robust mechanisms of biological motion
detection suggesting that it might have been an early semantic
phenomenon. Furthermore, the typical agents are animate or
specifically human. The typical patient appears to be inanimate.
Transitivity studies also show connections to the visuomotor/sensorimotor
brain system involved in action production and understanding.
Processing of prototypical semantic transitivity appears to be highly
embodied. Thus, transitive events might have been one of the first
experiences to have been linguistically lexicalized. Syntactic transitivity
could also be relatively embodied, however, further studies are
needed. It appears that syntactic transitivity phylogenetically arose
from semantic transitivity.
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Conclusions (3)
The principle of agent/subject saliency could suggest that
rudimentary syntactic components might have been an early
phenomenon in language evolution, i.e. if agents were lexicalized,
they were probably linearly placed first. This is supported by the
cross-linguistic dominance of SOV and SVO word orders, the
neurocognitive saliency of agents, the prevalence of Nom.-Acc.
relative to Erg.-Abs. languages and neurophysiological studies
showing that the first argument is automatically analysed as the
agent. Kemmerer (2012) suggested this reflects how Broca’s area, or
parts of the visuomotor system, process prototypical transitive events
in which the starting focus is on the agent.
Linguistisches Kolloquium 2019/20, Department of English and Linguistics, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, 18/11/2019
Aziz-Zadeh, I., Wilson, S. M., Rizzolatti, G., Iacoboni, M. (2006). Congruent embodied
representations for visually represented actions and linguistic phrases describing
actions. Current Biology, 16(18), 18181823. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.060
Bahlmann, J., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Rotte, M., Münte, T. F. (2007). An fMRI study of
canonical and noncanonical word order in German. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 940
949. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20318
Barić, E., Lončarić, M., Malić, D., Pavešić, S., Peti, M., Zečević, V., Znika, M. (2005).
Hrvatska gramatika. Zagreb: Školska knjiga.
Bickel, B., Witzlack-Makarevich, A., Choudhary, K. K., Schlesewsky, M., Bornkessel-
Schlesewsky, I. (2015). The neurophysiology of language processing shapes the
evolution of grammar: evidence from case marking. PLoS ONE, 10(8), e0132819.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132819
Bickerton, D. (1990). Language and Species. Chicago/London: The University of
Chicago Press.
Binder, J. R., Westbury, C. F., McKiernan, K. A., Possing, E. T., Medler, D. A. (2005).
Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract concepts. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(6), 113. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929054021102
Bishop, D. V. M. (2001). Motor immaturity and specific speech and language
impairment: evidence for a common genetic basis. American Journal of Medical
Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 114, 5663. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.1630
Bonin P, Gelin M, Bugaiska A (2014). Animates are better rememberd than inanimates:
further evidence from word and picture stimuli. Mem Cognit 42(3): 370382.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0368-8
Bornkessel, I., McElree, B., Schlesewsky, M., Friederici, A. D. (2004). Multi-dimensional
contributions to garden path strength: dissociating phrase structure from case marking.
Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 495522.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.06.011
Botha, R. (2009). Theoretical underpinnings of inferences about language evolution: the
syntax used at Blombos Cave. U: Botha, R., Knight, C. (ur.). The Cradle of Language.
New York: Oxford University Press, 93111.
Caramazza, A., Shelton, J. R. (1998). Domain-specific knowledge systems in the brain:
the animateinanimate distinction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(1), 134.
https://doi.org/10.1162/089892998563752
Cohn, N., Parczynski, M. (2013). Prediction, events, and the advantage of Agents: the
processing of semantic roles in visual narrative. Cognitive Psychology, 67, 7397.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.07.002
Cohn, N., Parczynski, M., Kutas, M. (2017). Not so secret agents: event-related
potentials to semantic roles in visual event comprehension. Brain and Cognition, 119,
19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2017.09.001
Creissels, D. (2016). Transitivity, valency and voice. European Summer School in
Linguistic Typology, Porquerolles.
Crystal D (1998). The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Curry, A. (2013). The milk revolution. Nature, 500, 2022.
https://doi.org/10.1038/500020a
Darwin, C. (2013). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Seks with an
Introduction by Janet Browne. Hertfordshire: Wordsworth Editions Limited.
d’Errico, F., Lawson, G., Vanhaeren, M., and van Niekerk, K. (2005). Nassarius
kraussianus shell beads from Blombos Cave: evidence for symbolic behaviour in the
Middle Stone Age. Journal of Human Evolution, 48(1), 324.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2004.09.002
De Beni, R., Pazzaglia, F., Gyselinck, V., Meneghetti, C. (2005). Visuospatial working
memory and mental representation of spatial descriptions. European Journal of
Cognitive Psychology, 17(1), 7795. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440340000529
Desai, R. H., Conant, L. L., Binder, J. R., Park, H., Seidenberg, M. S. (2010). Activation
of sensory-motor areas in sentence comprehension. Cerebral Cortex, 20(2), 468478.
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp115
Digweed, S. M., Fedigan, L. M., Rendall, D. (2005). Variable specificity in the anti-
predator vocalizations and behaviour of the white-faced capuchin, Cebus capucinus.
Behaviour, 142, 9971021. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853905774405344
Diogo, R. (2018). First detailed anatomical study of bonobos reveals intra-specific
variations and exposes just-so stories of human evolution, bipedalism, and tool use.
Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 6, 53. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2018.00053
Dreyer, F. R., Pulvermüller, F. (2018). Abstract semantics in the motor system? – An
event- related fMRI study on passive reading of semantic word categories carrying
abstract emotional and mental meaning. Cortex, 100, 5270.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.10.021
Dryer, M. S. (2011). Order of subject, object, and verb. U: Haspelmath, M., Dryer, M. S.,
Gil, D., Comrie, B. (ur.). The World Atlas of Language Structures. New York: Oxford
University Press, 330333.
Elmer, S., Albrecht, J., Valizadeh, S. A., François, C., Rodríguez-Fornells (2018). Theta
coherence asymmetry in the dorsal stream of musicians facilitates word learning.
Scientific Reports, 8, 4565. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-22942-1
Everett, D. L. (2016). How Language Began. The Story of Humanity’s Greatest
Invention. New York / London: Liveright Publishing Corporation.
Fazio, P., Cantagallo, A., Craighero, L., D’Ausilio, A., Roy, A. C., Pozzo, T., … Fadiga,
L. (2009). Encoding of human action in Broca’s area. Brain, 132, 1980–1988.
https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awp118
Ghio, M., Tettamanti, M. (2016). Grounding sentence processing in the sensory-motor
system. U: Hickok, G., Small, S. L. (ur.). Neurobiology of Language. Amsterdam [etc.]:
Academic Press, 647657. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407794-2.00052-3
Glenberg, A. M., Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 558565. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196313
Haimovici, S. (2018). The modalamodal distinction in the debate on conceptual format.
Philosophies, 3(2), 7. https://doi.org/10.3390/philosophies3020007
Hamrick, P., Lum, J. A. G., Ullman, M. T. (2018). Child first language and adult second
language are both tied to general-purpose learning systems. PNAS, 115(7), 14871492.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1713975115
Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action
words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41(2), 301307.
https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00838-9
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: what is it,
who ha sit, and how did it evolve?. Science, 298, 15691579.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.298.5598.1569
Heard, A., Madan, C. R., Protzner, A. B., Pexman, P. M. (2019). Getting a grip on
sensorimotor effects in lexical-semantic processing. Behavior Research Methods, 51(1),
113. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1072-1
Heesen R, Hobaiter C, Ferrer-i-Cancho R, Semple S (2019). Linguistic laws in
chimpanzee gestural communication. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological
Sciences 286: 20182900. http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.2900
Henshilwood, C., Vanhaeren, M., van Niekerk, K., and Jacobs, Z. (2004). Middle Stone
Age shell beads from South Africa. Science, 304, 404.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1095905
Hopkins, W. D., Meguerditchian, A., Coulon, O., Misiura, M., Pope, S., Mareno, M. C.,
Schapiro, S. J. (2017). Motor skill for tool-use is associated with asymmetries in Broca’s
area and the motor hand area of the precentral gyrus in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).
Behavioural Brain Research, 318, 7181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.10.048
Hupfeld, K. E., Ketcham, C. J., Schneider, H. D. (2017). Transcranial direct current
stimulation (tDCS) to Broca’s area: persisting effects on non-verbal motor behaviors.
Neurological Disorders and Therapeutics, 1(1), 15.
https://doi.org/10.15761/NDT.1000102
Hurford, J. R. (2007). Language in the Light of Evolution 1. The Origins of Meaning.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Hurford, J. R. (2012). Language in the Light of Evolution 2. The Origins of Grammar.
New York: Oxford University Press
James, C. T. (1975). The role of semantic information in lexical decisions. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1(2), 130136.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.1.2.130
Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its
analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201211.
https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03212378
Karakashian, S. J., Gyger, M., Marler, P. (1988). Audience effects on alarm calling in
chickens (Gallus gallus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 102(2), 129135.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.102.2.129
Kemmerer, D. (2012). The cross-linguistic prevalence of SOV and SVO word order
reflects the sequential and hierarchical representation of action in Broca’s area.
Language and Linguistics Compass, 6(1), 5066. https://doi.org/10.1002/lnc3.322
Kroll, J. F. (1986). Lexical access for concrete and abstract words. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12(1), 92107.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.12.1.92
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things. What Categories Reveal about
the Mind. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Li, P., Jia, X., Li, X., Li, W. (2016). The effect of animacy on metamemory. Memory and
Cognition, 44, 696705. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-016-0598-7
Lindly, J. M., Clark, G. A. (1990). Symbolism and modern human origins. Current
Anthropology, 31(3), 233261. https://doi.org/10.1086/203836
Marschark, M., Paivio, A. (1977). Integrative processing of concrete and abstract
sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16(2), 217231.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80048-0
McMahon, A., McMahon, R. Evolutionary Linguistics. New York: Cambridge University
Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511989391
Megan Fox thinks archaeologists are too narrow minded (2018, 16. svibnja).
ScienceAlert. https://www.sciencealert.com. [12/10/2019]
Mildner, V. (2015) [2008]. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Human Communication. New
York: Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203838105
Mollo, G., Pulvermüller, F., Hauk, O. (2016). Movement priming of EEG/MEG brain
responses for action-words characterizes the link between language and action. Cortex,
74, 262276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.10.021
Moseley, R., Carota, F., Hauk, O., Mohr, B., Pulvermüller, F. (2012). A role for the motor
system in binding abstract emotional meaning. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 16341647.
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr238
Nairne, J. S. (2010). Adaptive memory: evolutionary constraints on remembering. U:
Ross, B. H. (ur.). Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Volume 53. Burlington:
Academic Press, 132. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(10)53001-9
Napoli, D. J., Sutton-Spence, R. (2014). Order of the major constituents in sign
languages: implications for all language. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 375.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00376
Newmeyer, F. J. (2003). ‘Basic word order’ in formal and functional linguistics and the
typological status of ‘canonical’ sentence types. U: Willems, D., Defra,ncq B., Colleman,
T., Noël, D. (ur.). Contrastive Analysis in Language. Identifying Linguistic Unites of
Comparison. London: Palgrave Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1057/9780230524637_4
Nichols J (1993) Ergativity and linguistic geography. Australian Journal of Linguistics,
13, 3989. https://doi.org/10.1080/07268609308599489
Peti-Stantić, A., Anđel, M., Keresteš, G., Ljubešić, N., Stanojević, M.-M., Tonković, M.
(2018). Psiholingvističke mjere ispitivanja 3.000 riječi hrvatskoga jezika: konkretnost i
predočivost. Suvremena lingvistika, 44(85), 91–112.
https://doi.org/10.22210/suvlin.2018.085.05
Progovac, Lj., Rakhlin, N., Angell, W., Liddane, R., Tang, L., Ofen, N. (2018). Diversity
of grammars and their diverging evolutionary and processing paths: evidence from
functional MRI study of Serbian. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 278.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00278
Pulvermüller, F., Hauk, O., Nikulin, V. V., Ilmoniemi, R. J. (2005). Functional links
between motor and language systems. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 21,
793 797. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03900.x
Pulvermüller, F. (2013). How neurons make meaning: brain mechanisms for embodied
and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(9), 45870.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.004
Scorolli, C., Jacquet, P. O., Binkofski, F., Nicoletti, R., Tessari, A., Borghi, A. M. (2012).
Abstract and concrete phrases processing differentially modulates cortico-spinal
excitability. Brain Research, 1488, 6070. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2012.10.004
Seddon, N., Tobias, J. A., Alvarez, A. (2002). Vocal communication in the pale-winged
trumpeter (Psophia leucoptera): repertoire, context and functional reference. Behaviour,
139, 13311359. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853902321104190
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., Marler, P. (1980). Monkey responses to three different
alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science,
210, 801803. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7433999
Siakaluk, P. D., Pexman, P. M., Aguilera, L., Owen, W. J., Sears, C. R. (2008a).
Evidence for the activation of sensorimotor information during visual word recognition:
the body object interaction effect. Cognition, 106, 433443.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.011
Siakaluk, P. D., Pexman, P. M., Sears, C. R., Wilson, K., Locheed, K., Owen, W. J.
(2008b). The benefits of sensorimotor knowledge: bodyobject interaction facilitates
semantic processing. Cognitive Science, 32, 591605.
https://doi.org/10.1080/03640210802035399
Stout, D. (2010). “Possible relations between language and technology in human
evolution”. U: Nowell, A., Davidson, I. (ur.). Stone Tools and the Evolution of Human
Cognition. Colorado: University Press of Colorado, 15984.
Stout, D., Hecht, E. (2015). “Neuroarchaeology”. U: Bruner, E. (ur.). Human
Paleoneurology. Cham: Springer, 14575. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-08500-5_7
Struhsaker T. T. (1967). Auditory communication among vervet monkeys
(Cercopithecus aethiops). U: Altmann, S. A. (ur.). Social Communication Among
Primates. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 281324.
Suzuki, T. N., Wheatcroft, D., Griesser, M. (2016). Experimental evidence for
compositional syntax in bird calls. Nature Communications, 7, 10986.
https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms10986
Taylor, J. R. (2003). Cognitive Grammar. New York: Oxford University Press.
Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman, M. C., Gallese, V., Danna, M., Scifo, P., …
Perani, D. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor
circuits. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(2), 273281.
https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929053124965
Vallortigara, G., L. Regolin, and F. Marconato (2005). Visually inexperienced chicks
exhibit spontaneous preference for biological motion patterns. PLoS Biology, 3(7), e208.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030208
van Beilen, M., Pijnenborg, M., van Zomeren, E. H., van den Bosch, R. J., Withaar, F.
K., Bouma, A. (2004). What is measured by verbal fluency tests in schizophrenia?.
Schizophrenia Research, 69, 267276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.007
van Dam, W. O., Desai, R. H., (2016). The semantics of syntax: the grounding of
transitive and intransitive constructions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 28(5): 693
709. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00926
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., …
Boesch, C. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682685.
https://doi.org/10.1038/21415
Wright, S. K. (2001). Internally Caused and Externally Caused Change of State Verbs.
Unpublished PhD thesis: Northwestern University, Evanston, IL.
Wright, S. K. (2002). Transitivity and change of state verbs. U: Larson, J., Paster, M.
(ur.). Proceedings of the Twenty-Eighth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics
Society. General Session and Parasession on Field Linguistics. Berkeley: Berkeley
Linguistics Society, 339350. https://doi.org/10.3765/bls.v28i1.3849
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
The male domestic chicken (Gallus gallus) has been found to modulate the production of vocal signals in response to the presence or absence of a suitable audience. We investigated effects on alarm calling by presenting overhead predator models to cockerels in the presence of a variety of social companions. The production of aerial predator calls in response to hawk silhouettes varied with the presence or absence of a member of the same species. The kinds of audience investigated included the mate, unfamiliar females, other females and males with which subjects had had prior visual and auditory contact, and broody hens with and without young. Domestic chicks, unrelated to the subjects, were almost as effective an audience as conspecific adults. A member of another species, however, failed to potentiate alarm-call production. The subjects gave more alarm calls when they were in the presence of either a male or a female audience than when they were alone. By manipulating the visibility of overhead predator models to the subjects and to the audience, we showed that the subjects were not cued by alarm and escape behaviors of the audience. Comparisons with food calling indicate that, in deciding whether to emit a signal in response to the appropriate referent (e.g., food or predators), chickens respond to subtle differences in the nature of the audience with behaviors that vary from one communicative context to another.
Article
Full-text available
Studies testing linguistic laws outside language have provided important insights into the organization of biological systems. For example, patterns consistent with Zipf's law of abbreviation (which predicts a negative relationship between word length and frequency of use) have been found in the vocal and non-vocal behaviour of a range of animals, and patterns consistent with Menzerath's law (according to which longer sequences are made up of shorter constituents) have been found in primate vocal sequences, and in genes, proteins and genomes. Both laws have been linked to compression—the information theoretic principle of minimizing code length. Here, we present the first test of these laws in animal gestural communication. We initially did not find the negative relationship between gesture duration and frequency of use predicted by Zipf's law of abbreviation, but this relationship was seen in specific subsets of the repertoire. Furthermore, a pattern opposite to that predicted was seen in one subset of gestures—whole body signals. We found a negative correlation between number and mean duration of gestures in sequences, in line with Menzerath's law. These results provide the first evidence that compression underpins animal gestural communication, and highlight an important commonality between primate gesturing and language.
Article
Full-text available
This article presents the initial results of the project The Building Blocks of Croatian Mental Grammar: Constraints of Information Structure, within the scope of which the Croatian Psycholinguistic Database is being constructed. The database contains empirically established values for the categories of concreteness, imageability, subjective frequency and age of acquisition for 3000 lexemes (nouns, verbs and adjectives) of Croatian extracted from the hrLex lexicon whose frequency was extracted from the hrWaC corpus. The article starts with an overview of research dealing with concreteness and imageability, followed by a presentation of the methodology employed in the construction of the Croatian Psycholinguistic Database. The central section of the article presents the statistics concerning concreteness and imageability estimates for the sample taken in its entirety, as well as divided according to lexical class. The results show that both concreteness and imageability differ according to lexical class. These results are discussed from the perspective of constructional cognitive linguistic theories.
Article
Full-text available
Just-so stories are prominent in human evolution literature because of our tendency to create simple progressionist narratives about our “special” place in nature, despite the fact that these stories are almost exclusively based on hard tissue data. How can we be so certain about the evolution of human facial communication, bipedalism, tool use, or speech without detailed knowledge of the internal anatomy of for instance, one of the two extant species more closely related to us, the bonobos? Here I show how many of these stories now become obsolete, after such a comprehensive knowledge on the anatomy of bonobos and other primates is finally put together. Each and every muscle that has been long accepted to be “uniquely human” and to provide “crucial singular functional adaptations” for our bipedalism, tool use and/or vocal/facial communication, is actually present as an intra-specific variant or even as normal phenotype in bonobos and/or other apes.
Article
Full-text available
In this paper, I review the main criteria offered for distinguishing the modal and amodal approaches to conceptual format: the type of input to which the representations respond, the relation they bear to perceptual states, and the specific neural systems to which they belong. I evaluate different interpretations of them and argue that they all face difficulties. I further show that they lead to cross-classifications of certain types of representations, using approximate number representations as an example.
Article
Full-text available
We address the puzzle of “unity in diversity” in human languages by advocating the (minimal) common denominator for the diverse expressions of transitivity across human languages, consistent with the view that early in language evolution there was a modest beginning for syntax and that this beginning provided the foundation for the further elaboration of syntactic complexity. This study reports the results of a functional MRI experiment investigating differential patterns of brain activation during processing of sentences with minimal versus fuller syntactic structures. These structural layers have been postulated to represent different stages in the evolution of syntax, potentially engaging different brain networks. We focused on the Serbian “middles,” analyzed as lacking the transitivity (vP) layer, contrasted with matched transitives, containing the transitivity layer. Our main hypothesis was that transitives will produce more activation in the syntactic (Broca's–Basal Ganglia) brain network, in comparison to more rudimentary middles. The participants (n = 14) were healthy adults (Mean age = 33.36; SD = 12.23), native speakers of Serbo-Croatian. The task consisted of reading a series of sentences (middles and transitives; n = 64) presented in blocks of 8, while being engaged in a detection of repetition task. We found that the processing of transitives, compared to middles, was associated with an increase in activation in the basal ganglia bilaterally. Although we did not find an effect in Broca's area, transitives, compared to middles, evoked greater activation in the precentral gyrus (BA 6), proposed to be part of the “Broca's complex.” Our results add to the previous findings that Broca's area is not the sole center for syntactic processing, but rather is part of a larger circuit that involves subcortical structures. We discuss our results in the context of the recent findings concerning the gene-brain-language pathway involving mutations in FOXP2 that likely contributed to the enhancement of the frontal-striatal brain network, facilitating human capacity for complex syntax.
Article
Full-text available
Word learning constitutes a human faculty which is dependent upon two anatomically distinct processing streams projecting from posterior superior temporal (pST) and inferior parietal (IP) brain regions toward the prefrontal cortex (dorsal stream) and the temporal pole (ventral stream). The ventral stream is involved in mapping sensory and phonological information onto lexical-semantic representations, whereas the dorsal stream contributes to sound-to-motor mapping, articulation, complex sequencing in the verbal domain, and to how verbal information is encoded, stored, and rehearsed from memory. In the present source-based EEG study, we evaluated functional connectivity between the IP lobe and Broca’s area while musicians and non-musicians learned pseudowords presented in the form of concatenated auditory streams. Behavioral results demonstrated that musicians outperformed non-musicians, as reflected by a higher sensitivity index (d’). This behavioral superiority was paralleled by increased left-hemispheric theta coherence in the dorsal stream, whereas non-musicians showed stronger functional connectivity in the right hemisphere. Since no between-group differences were observed in a passive listening control condition nor during rest, results point to a task-specific intertwining between musical expertise, functional connectivity, and word learning.
Article
One of the strategies that researchers have used to investigate the role of sensorimotor information in lexical-semantic processing is to examine effects of words’ rated body-object interaction (BOI; the ease with which the human body can interact with a word’s referent). Processing tends to be facilitated for words with high BOI compared to words with low BOI, across a wide variety of tasks. Such effects have been referenced in debates over the nature of semantic representations, but their theoretical import has been limited by the fact that BOI is a fairly coarse measure of sensorimotor experience with words’ referents. In the present study we collected ratings for 621 words on seven semantic dimensions (graspability, ease of pantomime, number of actions, animacy, size, danger, and usefulness) in order to investigate which attributes are most strongly related to BOI ratings, and to lexical-semantic processing. BOI ratings were obtained from previous norming studies (Bennett, Burnett, Siakaluk, & Pexman, 2011; Tillotson, Siakaluk, & Pexman, 2008) and measures of lexical-semantic processing were obtained from previous behavioural megastudies involving the semantic categorization task (concrete/abstract decision; Pexman, Heard, Lloyd, & Yap, 2017) and the lexical decision task (Balota et al., 2007). Results showed that the motor dimension of graspability, ease of pantomime, and number of actions were all related to BOI and that these dimensions together explained more variance in semantic processing than did BOI ratings alone. These ratings will be useful for researchers who wish to study how different kinds of bodily interactions influence lexical-semantic processing and cognition.
Article
Significance It has long been debated whether the mechanisms that underlie language are dedicated to this uniquely human capacity or whether in fact they serve more general-purpose functions. Our study provides strong evidence that language—indeed both first and second language—is learned, in specific ways, by general-purpose neurocognitive mechanisms that preexist Homo sapiens . The results have broad implications. They elucidate both the ontogeny (development) and phylogeny (evolution) of language. Moreover, they suggest that our substantial knowledge of the general-purpose mechanisms, from both animal and human studies, may also apply to language. The study may thus lead to a research program that can generate a wide range of predictions about this critical domain.
Article
Previous research showed that modality-preferential sensorimotor areas are relevant for processing concrete words used to speak about actions. However, whether modality-preferential areas also play a role for abstract words is still under debate. Whereas recent fMRI studies suggest an involvement of motor cortex in processing the meaning of abstract emotion words as, for example, ‘love’, other non-emotional abstract words, in particular ‘mental words’, such as ‘thought’ or ‘logic’, are believed to engage ‘amodal’ semantic systems only. In the present event-related fMRI experiment, subjects passively read abstract emotional and mental nouns along with concrete action related words. Contrary to expectation, the results indicate a specific involvement of face motor areas in the processing of mental nouns, resembling that seen for face related action words. This result was confirmed when subject-specific regions of interest (ROIs) defined by motor localizers were used. We conclude that a role of motor systems in semantic processing is not restricted to concrete words but extends to at least some abstract mental symbols previously thought to be entirely ‘disembodied’ and divorced from semantically related sensorimotor processing. Implications for neurocognitive theories of semantics and clinical applications will be highlighted, paying specific attention to the role of brain activations as indexes of cognitive processes and their relationships to ‘causal’ studies addressing lesion and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects. Possible implications for clinical practice, in particular speech language therapy, are discussed in closing.