ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Purpose Narrative interventions are a class of language interventions that involve the use of telling or retelling stories. Narrative intervention can be an efficient and versatile means of promoting a large array of academically and socially important language targets that improve children's access to general education curriculum and enhance their peer relations. The purpose of this tutorial is to supply foundational information about the importance of narratives and to offer recommendations about how to maximize the potential of narrative interventions in school-based clinical practice. Method Drawing from decades of cognitive and linguistic research, a tutorial on narratives and narrative language is presented first. Ten principles that support the design and implementation of narrative interventions are described. Results Clinicians can use narrative intervention to teach story grammar, complex language, vocabulary, inferencing, and social pragmatics. Storytelling, as an active intervention ingredient, promotes the comprehension and production of complex language. Conclusion When narrative intervention is implemented following a set of principles drawn from research and extensive clinical experience, speech-language pathologists can efficiently and effectively teach a broad set of academically and socially meaningful skills to diverse students.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
LSHSS
Research Article
Narrative Intervention:
Principles to Practice
Trina D. Spencer
a
and Douglas B. Petersen
b
Purpose: Narrative interventions are a class of language
interventions that involve the use of telling or retelling stories.
Narrative intervention can be an efficient and versatile means
of promoting a large array of academically and socially
important language targets that improve childrens access
to general education curriculum and enhance their peer
relations. The purpose of this tutorial is to supply foundational
information about the importance of narratives and to offer
recommendations about how to maximize the potential of
narrative interventions in school-based clinical practice.
Method: Drawing from decades of cognitive and linguistic
research, a tutorial on narratives and narrative language is
presented first. Ten principles that support the design and
implementation of narrative interventions are described.
Results: Clinicians can use narrative intervention to teach
story grammar, complex language, vocabulary, inferencing,
and social pragmatics. Storytelling, as an active intervention
ingredient, promotes the comprehension and production
of complex language.
Conclusion: When narrative intervention is implemented
following a set of principles drawn from research and
extensive clinical experience, speech-language pathologists
can efficiently and effectively teach a broad set of academically
and socially meaningful skills to diverse students.
Narrative intervention is one of the most powerful
approaches to language intervention that school-
based speech-language pathologists (SLPs) can
use. We define narrative intervention as any language inter-
vention that involves children telling or retelling stories that
have specific language-related features purposefully tar-
geted by the interventionist (Petersen, 2011). In the last
10 years, there has been an increased focus on narrative
intervention in research. Consistently, effect sizes for improv-
ing various aspects of language in children with and without
language disorder have been large (e.g., S. L. Gillam et al.,
2014; Spencer et al., 2018). In addition to yielding powerful
effects on language, the research documents that narrative
intervention is also highly feasible and flexible. While our
purpose in this article is not to critically review the extant
literature, we provide empirical evidence, share clinical in-
sights, and outline how narrative intervention is versatile,
potent, and academically and socially relevant. We have or-
ganized the available empirical and practical guidance in a
manner that facilitates its transfer to clinical practice. As
we strive to help clinicians maximize their effective imple-
mentation of narrative intervention, we first present a brief
tutorial on narratives. Our understanding of narratives and
their characterizations emerge from a depth of clinical expe-
rience and empirical studies conducted over several decades.
The major points are summarized in this article to provide
the reader with an introduction to narrative language (see
Boudreau, 2008, and Johnston, 2008, for other summaries).
Following the tutorial, we offer 10 principles that can guide
the strategic use of narrative intervention in schools.
Introduction to Narratives
In the academic context, narratives are most often the
monologic telling or retelling of a real or imaginary event
(R. B. Gillam & Ukrainetz, 2006). Such narratives are goal
directed, where an agents efforts to resolve problems or
complications and the resulting consequences are described.
Canonical elements of stories include an initiating event
(usually a problem), an attempt to solve the problem, and a
consequence (Mandler, 1987; Stein & Glenn, 1979). Together,
these form a basic, minimally complete episode, and the in-
clusion of additional story grammar elements such as an
explicit plan to solve the problem and a resolution increases
a storys episodic sophistication. When attempts to solve a
problem are ineffective, additional episodes extend the story
a
Department of Child and Family Studies, University of South
Florida, Tampa
b
Department of Communication Disorders, Brigham Young
University, Provo, UT
Correspondence to Trina D. Spencer: trinaspencer@usf.edu
Editor-in-Chief: Holly L. Storkel
Editor: Katie Squires
Received February 6, 2020
Revision received April 8, 2020
Accepted June 16, 2020
https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_LSHSS-20-00015
Disclosure: The authors receive financial benefits from the sale of Story Champs,
which is one of the published narrative interventions mentioned in this article.
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools Vol. 51 10811096 October 2020 Copyright © 2020 The Authors
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
1081
until an eventual resolution is achieved, as is common in
novels.
Stories are not stories without causal and temporal
relations between events. It is the causal links between the
initiating event (or problem), attempt, and consequence
that reveal a characters goal-oriented behavior (Peterson
& McCabe, 1983). If story events are presented in an un-
expected order, comprehension of the causal connections
can be diminished. Nonetheless, specific sequences and
temporal and causal devices are culturally grounded. They
are taught through extensive exposure to a linguistic com-
munitys storytelling patterns, which, over years of exposure,
shapes the understanding and production of narratives in a
certain way.
There can be cultural differences in narrative structure
(T. B. Champion, 1998; Gee, 1989; S. L. Gillam, Fargo,
et al., 2012; Gorman et al., 2011; Mills et al., 2013; Tappe &
Hara, 2013; C. E. Westby, 1994). For example, internal re-
sponsestendtobeemphasizedtoagreaterextentinnarratives
produced by Latino, Spanish-speaking children (Castilla-Earls
et al., 2015). Native American stories often do not unfold in a
linear manner (Sharifian, 2002), with story grammar that does
not always follow Stein and Glenn (1979) conventions.
Worth and Adair (1972) noted that Native Americans often
focus considerably more on setting information than stories
found in academic settings, with a major focus on detail and
implicit information as opposed to explicitly stated temporal
and causal connections (Basso, 1990; C. Westby et al.,
2002). Japanese children also tend to have a more implicit
style of narration, often producing brief, minimalistic stories
(Minami, 2002) or stories with minimal goal structure
(Matsuyama, 1983). African American children have been
noted to use a topic associating approach to narration simi-
lar to that used by Native Americans (T. Champion et al.,
1995; Gee, 1989; Hyon & Sulzby, 1994) as well as a call
response interaction (Smitherman, 1977), which entails the
narrator producing a story with interspersed callsfrom
the listener.
Despite cultural variations, most stories still have pro-
tagonists seeking to solve challenges, and all children who
reside in the United States, including those from culturally,
ethnically, and linguistically diverse backgrounds, can bene-
fit from learning the story grammar and relational features
expected in U.S. schools. Narrative intervention can be ap-
plied to any approach to narrative structure (e.g., Applebee,
1978; Hudson & Shapiro, 1991; Labov & Waletzky, 1967),
yet what has been developed thus far has mostly followed
Stein and Glenns (1979) model of story grammar. For ex-
ample, each of the following assessments draw from Stein
and Glenns story grammar framework to some extent: the
Assessment of Story Comprehension (Spencer & Goldstein,
2019), the Edmonton Narrative Norms Initiative (Schneider
et al., 2005), the Monitoring Indicators of Scholarly Lan-
guage (S. L. Gillam et al., 2016), the Narrative Assessment
Protocol (Bowles et al., 2020), the Narrative Language
Mea sures subtest of the CU BED assessment (Petersen &
Spencer, 2012, 2016), the wordless picture books or other
sampling procedures used in conjunction with the Systematic
Analysis of Language Transcripts and the Narrative Scoring
Scheme (Heilmann et al., 2010; Miller et al., 2015), and
Westerveld and Gillons (2010) language sampling protocol.
Additionally, the following manualized intervention pro-
grams promote Stein and Glenns story grammar structure
for U.S. children: Lets Know! (Language and Reading
Research Consortium et al., 2017), Story Champs, (Spencer
& Petersen, 2016), Story Grammar Marker (Moreau &
Fidrych, 1994), and Supporting Knowledge in Language
and Literacy (SKILL; S. L. Gillam & Gillam, 2016). Although
it is common in the United States to teach the story grammar
framework, doing so does not imply that such an approach
to narration should be considered a replacement for the
students own culturally unique storytelling approach. Teach-
ing children to tell stories in a manner that aligns with cur-
riculum expectations is a matter of teaching code-switching,
not code-overwriting.
Interwoven throughout the narrative structure is the
language used to express it. Whereas story grammar is used
to organize the gross discourse structure of a narrative, syn-
tax, morphology, semantics, and other features of language
are used to make up the narrative microstructure (Colozzo
et al., 2011). When a narrative contains microstructural
features that reflect literate, academic language, it is of-
ten markedly more complex than dialogic conversation
(MacLachlan & Chapman, 1988). This literate language
expressed in the context of a story, which we refer to as nar-
rative language, often consists of multiple causal and temporal
adverbial, relative, and nominal subordinate clauses, elabo-
rated noun phrases, adjectives, adverbs, appositives, and dia-
logue (Benson, 2009; Greenhalgh & Strong, 2001; Peterson
& McCabe, 1983; Roth & Spekman, 1986). To tell about
an event (either orally or in written form) that the listener/
reader did not also experience, and for the story to be com-
prehensible, the storyteller frequently uses sophisticated,
complex, precise language to paint a detailed picture (Bruner,
1986; Johnston, 2008; C. E. Westby, 1985). Effective storytell-
ing requires complex grammar to depict specific images
and evoke purposeful emotions, whereas effective conversa-
tion benefits from suprasegmentalssuchasgesturesand
joint attention, facilitating comprehension despite the use
of imprecise sentences (Hudson & Shapiro, 1991). Narra-
tives also often contain many complex and infrequent vocab-
ulary words that facilitate comprehension (Petersen et al.,
2020). The spontaneous use of complex vocabulary or
domain-specific words during storytelling demonstrates se-
mantic complexity and is arguably a better indicator of
ones lexical aptitude (Hadley & Dickinson, 2018).
Narratives are commonly examined at macro and
micro levels, but that is not to suggest they are restricted
to story grammar or morphosyntactic analyses. In fact,
narratives showcase a large number of core skills that serve
as the foundation to a range of academic (C. E. Westby,
1985) and social abilities (Brinton & Fujiki, 2017). Narra-
tives bring several inconspicuous repertoires together to
form an integrated higher order composite that is clini-
cally convenient and useful (Boudreau, 2008). To be an
accomplished storyteller, a child must orchestrate many
1082 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools Vol. 51 10811096 October 2020
simultaneously converging processes including attention,
cognition, memory, inferencing, and theory of mind
(Curenton, 2011; Dodwell & Bavin, 2008; R. B. Gillam
& Johnston, 1992; Hudson & Shapiro, 1991; Johnston,
2008; Lahey, 1990; Nippold & Schwartz, 1996). Consider-
ing narratives are a pervasive and complex form of dis-
course, they have considerable clinical utility.
A notable literature documents the predictive nature
of early narrative language skills for later academic achieve-
ment (Bishop & Edmondson, 1987; Dickinson & McCabe,
2001; Fazio et al., 1996; Feagans & Appelbaum, 1986). Chil-
drens narrative abilities are related to listening comprehen-
sion (Bourg et al., 1997), receptive vocabulary (Heilmann
et al., 2010), and writing (Griffin et al., 2004; Kim et al.,
2015; Scott & Windsor, 2000). The relation between early
oral narrative skills and later reading comprehension is par-
ticularly strong (Catts et al., 2002; Dickinson & McCabe,
2001; Gilmore et al., 1999; Griffin et al., 2004). For instance,
C. E. Snow et al. (2007) reported that the ability to produce
a narrative in preschool predicts reading comprehension per-
formance in the fourth, seventh, and 10th grades. Likewise,
Suggate et al. (2018) demonstrated that narrative skill at
school entry predicted reading comprehension performance
10 years later. Moreover, evidence exists of causal relation-
ships between narrative-focused oral language interventions
and reading comprehension (Clarke et al., 2010; Language
and Reading Research Consortium et al., 2019) and writing
outcomes (Kirby et al., 2020; Petersen et al., 2019; Spencer
& Petersen, 2018).
The value of narratives reflected by the current body
of research is mirrored in prevailing academic standards.
For example, the Common Core State Standards, adopted
by the majority of U.S. states (National Governors Associ-
ation Center for Best Practices & Council of Chief State
School Officers, 2010), specifically details story grammar
and language complexity outcomes for all primary, middle,
and high school grade levels. Objectives in the Common
Core State Standards related to narration can be found in
the English Language Arts Standards, dispersed through-
out specific reading literature, speaking and listening, and
language standards.
Narratives are socially important too. Parents of chil-
dren with disabilities often express their desire for children to
tell them about their day or what happened at school (Pituch
et al., 2011). Teachers need information about events that
took place on the playground or over the weekend, and peers
want to be entertained. All of these are related to storytell-
ing, and, in general, children with language disabilities do
not excel at social communication (Colozzo et al., 2011;
Dodwell & Bavin, 2008; Fey et al., 2004; Kaderavek &
Sulzby, 2000). Children who are good storytellers are more
accepted by their peers (Hart et al., 2004; P. C. McCabe &
Marshall, 2006) and receive parent and teacher approval
(Bliss & McCabe, 2012). Additionally, increased exposure
to narrative conversations predicts childrens future ability
to solve social problems (Leyva et al., 2014).
Storytelling appears to be a pivotal social skill in
that children naturally extend what is learned in training
environments (e.g., classroom or clinic room with SLP)
to untrained social contexts with minimal effort. From a
clinical perspective, what could be better? Narrative inter-
vention uses an authentic, functional context, which entails
the formation of a genuine connection between adult and
child, where meaningful information is conveyed, and lan-
guage processing, pragmatics, and socialemotional learn-
ing are integrated (Brinton & Fujiki, 2019). The natural
environment (i.e., peer and adult attention) can maintain
childrens efforts to tell stories and, by doing so, provide
additional opportunities for practicing the newly acquired
language skills. Possible evidence of this generalization is
seen in an important study comparing a written text compre-
hension intervention, an oral narrative intervention, and
a combination intervention (Clarke et al., 2010). Researchers
found that students in the oral language intervention-only
condition, but not the other two conditions, continued to
improve their reading comprehension performance, even
after the intervention was withdrawn. Brinton and Fujiki
(2019) examined whether a narrative-based language inter-
vention promoted language processing, pragmatics, and social
and emotional learning for children with developmental lan-
guage disorder. Intervention sessions were focused on story-
telling and story enactment. Children were taught how to
write or dictate a brief journal entry focusing on new con-
cepts learned from the story. Teacher ratings showed im-
provement in prosocial behavior, and the school-based SLP
reported improvement in expressive narrative language and
syntax.
Improving oral narration can improve written narra-
tion. Spoken narratives play a fundamental role in the devel-
opment of written stories and are more commonly addressed
by SLPs. Because young children cannot write what they
cannot say, children with language disabilities are at a par-
ticular disadvantage with respect to academic writing tasks
(Koutsoftas & Gray, 2012; Pavelko et al., 2017). Fortunately,
recent research indicates that an explicit and intensive focus
on oral narrative language has a direct and meaningful impact
on the quality of written narratives children produce, even
for children with significant disabilities (Kirby et al., 2020;
Petersen et al., 2019; Spencer & Petersen, 2018). Researchers
suggest that this transfer from oral narration to written nar-
ration occurs because the story grammar and linguistic struc-
tures taught and practiced through oral storytelling evolve
into a cognitive schema that generalizes to alternative modali-
ties, including writing (Hayes & Flowers, 1980; Rumelhart,
1980; C. Snow, 1983; C. E. Westby, 1984). Based on sche-
mata theory (Anderson & Pearson, 1984; Mandler, 1987),
the quickest way to promote story writing is to promote
oral storytelling that takes advantage of the shared story
level and sentence level structures.
One of the greatest advantages of addressing oral
narration for the purpose of improving written narration
is that it eliminates the interference during intervention
from other potentially delayed or emerging skills related
to transcription (i.e., handwriting mechanics, letter forma-
tion, spelling, and letter sounds), while garnering childrens
cognitive focus for generating and organizing their thinking
Spencer & Petersen: Narrative Intervention 1083
and speaking (V. Berninger & Graham, 1998; McCutchen,
1996). Even when transcription and decoding skills are au-
tomatized, the quality of written narratives and reading com-
prehension is intimately dependent on the oral language
foundation the child brings to the task (V. W. Berninger &
Abbott, 2010; Catts et al., 2006; Fey et al., 2004). Hence, it
is recommended that oral narratives receive considerable
attention during therapy, especially for young students and
those with language-based disabilities.
Clinicians must be strategic and planful to be able to
facilitate the development of narrative schemas that will
readily transfer to academically and socially meaningful
repertoires. Ultimately, the flexible and pragmatic use of
acquired narrative content and form is the goal. Various
conditions (Hughes et al., 1997) in which storytelling oc-
curs form a continuum of narrative tasks (see Figure 1).
For example, stories can be told or retold. Generating
quality narratives can be more challenging than retelling
a story (R. B. Gillam & Johnston, 1992; Kaderavek &
Sulzby, 2000), yet there can be simple story generations and
very complex story retells. Because retells reflect both listen-
ing comprehension and story production skills (Boudreau,
2008; Petersen & Spencer, 2012; Wagner et al., 1999), indi-
viduals who have weak receptive language may find gener-
ating a story easier than retelling one (Weddle et al., 2016).
Telling about recently heard/read events is a common aca-
demic task and would be considered a retell of a model
story. Stories about personal experiences are most often
produced for social reasons such as gaining attention from
peers or approval from adults (McCabe, 2017). The first
telling of the experience would be considered a story gener-
ation, but subsequent repeated productions would be best
classified as retells. Given social attention is the natural
(and powerful) consequence of storytelling and it is easier
to talk about real events (A. McCabe et al., 2008), personal
stories are pervasive in childrens spontaneous communica-
tion (P. C. McCabe & Marshall, 2006; Preece, 1987; C.
Westby & Culatta, 2016). When a story is not true and has
never been told before (either orally or written), we would
characterize it as a fictional generation. Fictional stories
can be more challenging than personal event narratives
(Allen et al., 1994; A. McCabe et al., 2008), yet this is not
always the case. A less common type of narrative is a script,
which has been defined as a recount of typical events,
based on a collection of personal experiences rather than a
single event (Hayward et al., 2007; Hudson & Shapiro,
1991; Hughes et al., 1997). For instance, if a child tells
about what he usually does on Sundays or how his fam-
ily celebrates a specific holiday, he is generating a script
narrative. This type of discourse is highly related to ex-
pository discourse because it does not focus on a specific
event.
Often, story retelling is the go-to context for initial
teaching. Once children have acquired a proper story gram-
mar framework, it can be extended to the generation of per-
sonal stories and eventually to the generation of fictional
stories and writing. Retelling a short, familiar book or a
story that was specifically designed for intervention allows
for graduated, controlled, and targeted practice because the
clinician can select stories based on an individual childs
language skills.
Photos or illustrations that provide clues about story
content can be used to modify the demands of storytelling,
depending on the goal of narrative intervention and the
childs abilities. Although visual materials can scaffold re-
tells (e.g., Meyer, 1969) and fictional stories (S. L. Gillam
& Gillam, 2016), not all classroom academic tasks, to which
narrative skills should generalize, involve visuals (e.g., listen-
ing and reading comprehension), especially as the grades
increase. Likewise, pictures and icons are generally not
available in social contexts. Some research suggests that
young children and children with language impairment
retell more complete episodes and include more informa-
tion units when they listen to stories without pictures as op-
posed to orally and pictorially presented stories (Schneider,
1996; Schneider & Dubé, 2005). Photos and illustrations
can support storytelling, but they may result in a low esti-
mate of a childs abilities. Therefore, it is important to use
visuals strategically, prevent dependence on them, and pro-
mote the generalization of storytelling skills to more natural
and sophisticated contexts.
Principles to Practice
There is not one right way to implement narrative inter-
vention. Nonetheless, the extant research evidence and our
clinical experiences have revealed 10 principles of narrative
intervention that can help guide practice. Notwithstanding
the collection of principle-driven guidelines described below
and listed in Table 1, it is important to remember that chil-
drens Individualized Education Program goals and in-
structional objectives should drive the selection of specific
intervention parameters. Relevant assessment data should
inform the selection of individual goals and objectives, ensur-
ing a direct link from assessment to intervention. A chief
purpose of valid, reliable, and meaningful assessment is to
motivate efficient and effective intervention. Therefore, SLPs
are responsible for using purposeful assessments, converting
assessment data to goals, and making wise choices about
Figure 1. General continuum from easier to more difficult storytelling.
1084 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools Vol. 51 10811096 October 2020
whether to work on story grammar or complex language,
retells or generations, personal or fictional stories, and what
type and timing of scaffolds to use. There are a number of
commercially available narrative intervention programs (e.g.,
SKILL, Story Grammar Marker, Story Champs) that can
reduce the time and energy needed for preparation, but
clinicians are still responsible for making evidence-based
choices regarding the selection of programs and for using
them effectively with their clients.
Build Story Structure Before Vocabulary
and Complex Language
If a child cannot yet tell or retell a story using a
complete episode, that is where we recommend clinicians
start. To work efficiently on syntax, grammar, or academic
vocabulary (or anything else) during storytelling, it is help-
ful for children to be able to tell and retell basic stories.
Once a child can produce the basic structure, storytelling
becomes the context for teaching a wide variety of other
skills (Justice et al., 2008; Swanson et al., 2005). Ensuring
children develop a cognitive schema related to story gram-
mar early can help reduce frustration of children with ex-
tremely limited language skills, by gradually increasing the
cognitive and linguistic demands. Spencer et al. (2013) pro-
vide an example of how this might unfold. They examined
an individual narrative intervention with 3- and 4-year-old
children with developmental disabilities, focusing exclu-
sively on story grammar for 12 sessions. Once storytelling
established and each child could retell a minimally com-
plete episode, language complexity targets such as tempo-
ral and causal subordinating ties were systematically folded
into the sessions.
Interventions with older children may not need to begin
at the level of story grammar construction. Research indi-
cates that children with language disabilities tend to strug-
gle with both story grammar and grammar/syntax, but
their use of complex language is often affected to a greater
degree (Colozzo et al., 2011; Fey et al., 2004; R. B. Gillam
& Johnston, 1992). If that is the case, then the goal can be
to provide a child with extra, incidental practice telling
stories while specifically modeling, prompting, and encour-
aging the use of correct syntactical forms. The same princi-
ple applies to vocabulary. If definitions of words and the
use of more sophisticated words are the desired targets,
children will learn them more efficiently if their cognitive
energy is not exhausted on constructing the narrative schema.
Gardner and Spencer (2016) investigated explicit vocabulary
instruction embedded in a narrative intervention delivered
to small groups of at-risk first graders. They observed that,
as retells improved, children became better at learning the
target vocabulary words. They also became better at fig-
uring out the meaning of novel, unknown words from a
story context. S. L. Gillam et al. (2014) also embedded
vocabulary targets in their classroom narrative intervention.
Although, as a group, the first graders who received the
intervention learned the vocabulary specifically taught to
them, children who were at risk acquired fewer new word
meanings.
An important point to remember is that, although
teaching children to tell stories may be necessary, narrative
intervention does not need to stop there. Numerous other
therapeutic targets can be contextualized in storytelling once
story structure is established to a minimal degree (S. L. Gillam,
Gillam, & Reece, 2012). If stories are strategically and care-
fully designed for intervention, it may be easier to facilitate
instruction on the embedded targets (see Spencer & Petersen,
2016) than if clinicians are dependent on what is available in
published storybooks. Stories can be designed (or selected)
for instructional purposes that include the promotion of gram-
mar, syntax, cohesion, elaborated noun phrases, mental
state verbs, dialogue, inference, theory of mind, vocabulary,
and social problem solving, to name a few. The focus of
narrative intervention is on oral narratives at this moment,
but the path from strong oral narratives to written narratives
is unconstrained (Petersen et al., 2019; Spencer & Petersen,
2018). Other critical academic skills that clinicians can
address from a firm oral narrative foundation include lis-
tening comprehension (i.e., answering questions about
what one hears; e.g., Spencer et al., 2013), reading compre-
hension (i.e., answering questions about what one reads;
e.g., Clarke et al., 2010), and expository discourse (i.e.,
telling or retelling information; e.g., Lee, 2020; Petersen
et al., 2019).
Use Multiple Exemplars to Promote Metalinguistics
and Generalization
Multiple exemplar training is a systematic procedure
to promote the generalization of higher order concepts (Le-
France & Tarbox, 2020). For narrative intervention, this
means that clinicians should use several different stories (as
opposed to one or two) with the same story grammar ele-
ments to teach story grammar. If the goal of the interven-
tion was for a child to tell or retell a specific story, then
practicing the same story over and over would work fine.
However, the child may not learn how to tell stories gener-
ally and may not know much about story grammar as a
higher order concept that could be transferred to other
stories or contexts. To avoid promoting the memorization
of specific stories, several exemplars (i.e., different stories
with the same story grammar components) should be used
as models in consecutive intervention sessions. Rapid ac-
quisition of story grammar concepts can be accomplished
Table 1. Principle-driven narrative intervention guidelines.
1. Build story structure before vocabulary and complex language
2.Usemultipleexemplarstopromotemetalinguisticsand
generalization
3. Promote active participation
4. Contextualize, unpack, and reconstruct stories
5. Use visuals to make abstract concepts concrete
6. Deliver immediate corrective feedback
7. Use efficient and effective prompts
8. Differentiate, individualize, and extend
9. Arrange for generalization opportunities
10. Make it fun
Spencer & Petersen: Narrative Intervention 1085
if the same stories are not repeated because, in narrative
intervention, the multiple exemplars promote childrens ab-
straction of the pattern. Thus, by presenting children with
different exemplars of the concept across sessions (Layng,
2019), story grammar schema emerges. Children quickly
learn that story content is not the salient feature, and what
is important is the schematic pattern. Knowledge of this
schema and the ability to describe the pattern of stories (i.e.,
naming character, problem, feeling, action, and ending) is
metalinguistic. A childs successful generation or retelling
of a novel story resulting from narrative intervention is evi-
dence of generalization and metalinguistic development.
Although several narrative interventions have
employed storybooks (Adlof et al., 2014; Brinton & Fujiki,
2017; Catts et al., 2016; Petersen et al., 2010), some re-
searchers (including us) have strategically designed stories
for initial intensive and targeted narrative practice (Brown
et al., 2014; Favot et al., 2018; Hayward & Schneider, 2000;
Petersen et al., 2014; Spencer et al., 2020, Spencer, Petersen,
& Adams, 2015; Spencer & Slocum, 2010; Swanson et al.,
2005). Some advantages of using prepared stories (over story-
books) for narrative intervention include being able to control
what structures, vocabulary, and complex language children
are exposed to, easily teaching metalinguistics through multi-
ple exemplar training and rapid practice cycles within session
because stories are brief, and building narrative comprehen-
sion and production simultaneously. Most approaches to
narrative intervention eventually move away from a focus
on program-specific stories to the integration of authentic
children stories and curriculum materials.
Promote Active Participation
Decades of research indicate that active participation
leads to greater learning opportunities (Archer & Hughes,
2011; Ellis & Worthington, 1994; Engelmann & Carnine,
1982). Maintaining a brisk teaching pace increases chil-
drens opportunities to respond and reduces opportunities
for behavior problems (Forsyth & Archer, 1997). When
students are actively engaged and responding, they are learn-
ing (Greenwood et al., 1984; Pratton & Hales, 1986). Inter-
estingly, Knapp and Desrochers (2009) found that students
prefer interactive instruction in which they play a promi-
nent role.
Children need repeated opportunities to practice mean-
ingful language within an activity or session to ensure long-
term maintenance of the targeted skills (Ukrainetz, 2006).
Focused practice, supported by modeling and corrective
feedback, will enhance childrens ability to use the skills
spontaneously in nontraining environments. With respect
to narratives, this means that children must have ample
opportunities to tell or retell stories. Listening to stories
may improve comprehension of stories and have marginal
impacts on narrative expression, but to hasten childrens
ability to tell/retell stories, they must practice it. More prac-
tice is better.
To maintain high numbers of opportunities to respond
in the context of narrative intervention, the size of the inter-
vention group needs to be considered. If intervention is
delivered individually, it is easy to support the child to do
most of the talking. However, when other students are in the
group, time should be evenly distributed across the students
so that each student is responding frequently and everyone
has an opportunity to tell/retell at least one complete story,
beginning to end. It is common for children to have individ-
ual turns during small group narrative interventions, but it
is important to keep the groups small enough so that each
child receives numerous opportunities to practice storytell-
ing. Some small group narrative interventions encourage the
children who are listening to play games that demonstrate
they are actively listening and participating (Spencer &
Petersen, 2018; Spencer & Slocum, 2010; Story Grammar
Marker, 2020; Weddle et al., 2016). Although all the chil-
dren may not be talking at the same time, they are doing
something that requires their attention to story grammar,
which benefits comprehension and serves as scaffolding
for when it is their turn to tell the story. Through active
responding games, children also learn to be better listeners
and can even be encouraged to play the role of co-teacher.
If intervention will be delivered to large groups of
children, there are a number of other available strategies
that can support active engagement. In S. L. Gillam et al.s
(2014) classroom-based narrative intervention, students
used graphic organizers. When children retold stories indi-
vidually, a teacher walked around the room to check on
them. Choral responding (Heward et al., 1989), which means
that all children respond in unison, has been used in several
large group narrative intervention studies (Petersen et al.,
2019; Spencer, Petersen, & Adams, 2015; Spencer, Petersen,
Slocum, et al., 2015). This strategy is helpful when there are
too many students for the teacher or interventionist to give
each one individual support. In these studies, children cho-
rally named the parts of the story (i.e., character, problem,
feeling, action, ending) as well as the story content, one
sentence at a time (e.g., John was sad because his knee
hurt.). After children had told the story in parts with the
group, they were paired with a friend in a class-wide peer
tutoring arrangement to retell the model story individually
(Bowman-Perrott et al., 2013). Choral responding ensures
that all children in the class/group talk, not just the students
who are confident enough to raise their hands, and peer
tutoring provides an opportunity for each child to retell a
story while their peer is attentive and playing the role of
teacher.
Contextualize, Unpack, and Reconstruct Stories
When teaching storytelling, it is important to make
the activity meaningful and relevant to children (R. B.
Gillam & Ukrainetz, 2006). Storytelling is used for pur-
poseful communication with one or more listeners. This
intentional, communicative purpose should not be lost dur-
ing the intervention process. It may be tempting to eliminate
story content that children are not yet able to retell indepen-
dently. Although that may be a good reason to select stories
designed for narrative intervention over storybooks, there
are several reasons to retain all the main episodic elements
(e.g., problem, attempt, and consequence) of a story. First,
1086 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools Vol. 51 10811096 October 2020
comprehension of a narrative is facilitated through the rela-
tions between the story grammar elements (Curenton, 2011;
Schick & Melzi, 2010) and, when components are missing,
children cannot detect the causal and temporal relations
between them. Stories are much more than the sum of their
parts. Having children practice stating the character and
setting of a story without cohering it to the rest of the story
eliminates the purposeful, communicative nature of story-
telling and renders the activity a sentence repetition or pic-
ture description task. Second, children do not necessarily
develop story grammar in the sequence they are told and
often taught (e.g., character and setting first) because the
meaningful, related parts are in the middle. Although young
children can talk about characters and settings, storytelling
does not become storytelling until a child associates an
initiating event with an attempt and a consequence (cause
and effect). Development of storytelling can be thought
of occurring from the inside out, beginning with the feeling
that some past event triggered. As children develop, they
begin to add layers of information about the problem that
caused the feeling or what was done to resolve it, and even-
tually add the more distant contextual details such as the
setting and resolution. Third, when the goal of narrative
intervention is to teach the cognitive schema related to
narration, it is necessary for every story to adhere to that
pattern, otherwise, meta-skills will not be achieved. It is
difficult to promote the development of narrative schema
using multiple exemplars unless there are clear patterns
across those exemplars.
We recommend that stories used in intervention con-
tain, at a minimum, a basic, minimally complete episode
(problem, attempt, consequence) and that, before discuss-
ing the individual components in the intervention session,
the entire story is presented in its complete and complex
form first. This provides contextualizing information and
ensures children see it as a whole, thereby enhancing pur-
pose and motivation for the task (R. B. Gillam & Ukrainetz,
2006). Once the whole is modeled, the story can be broken
down into more manageable chunks, ensuring multiple
opportunities for children to practice each part. Yet, even
when each part is practiced individually, it is important
to contextualize the childs utterance and attach meaning
to it. For example, asking questions related to story gram-
mar (e.g., What was Johns problem in this story?or
Whatdidhedotofixhisproblem?) or having children
practice retelling the parts, one part at a time (e.g., John
was riding his bike down the street.or He asked his
mom for a bandage.), is an appropriate scaffolding tech-
nique, allowing the interventionists to provide several models
and feedback during guided practice, yet we also encour-
age children to place those parts back into either the entire
narrative or a larger section of the narrative. For example,
after prompting a child to include the characters feeling,
we often ask the child to go back one element in the story
(e.g., to the problem) and to retell both the problem and
the feeling so that there is cohesion and purpose in the task.
After a period of supported telling/retelling of the parts
within the same session, the story should be reconstructed
into its whole. Children should be encouraged (and sup-
ported) to tell/retell the story in its entirety and in order,
and to the extent possible, use cohesive devices to caus-
ally and temporally connect the episodic elements. Thus,
within a single intervention session, a story is presented as
a whole unit to contextualize it, then unpacked as parts for
guided practice, and reconstructed as a whole in indepen-
dentpractice(R.B.Gillam&Ukrainetz,2006).Inthis
manner, children are frequently reminded of the purpose
of each story grammar element and the importance of
their cohesion for generating a more meaningful whole.
This wholepartwhole framework driven by purposeful,
explicit instruction has been shown to be effective in a
number of narrative intervention studies (e.g., Brinton &
Fujiki, 2017, 2019; S. L. Gillam, Gillam, & Reece, 2012;
all Story Champs studies).
Use Visual Support to Make Abstract Concepts Concrete
Because storytelling requires the confluence of numer-
ous higher order processes, it is a demanding task for stu-
dents with language disabilities. We have already established
that the structure of stories is a concept (or schema) learned
through exposure to multiple exemplars. However, teaching
children about schemas can be a slow process without the
help of visual support (e.g., pictures, drawings, icons, graphic
organizers). Although clinicians should be cautious about
allowing children to always use visuals when telling or retell-
ing stories, they can be an extremely effective teaching tool,
especially when introducing a new story. Specifically, visual
supports can be used to make abstract concepts such as story
grammar and linguistic features (e.g., causal subordinate
conjunctions) more tangible and concrete for children.
The most popular visual support strategies include
using pictography, icons, symbols, gestures, or manipulatives
that represent the story grammar elements or linguistic tar-
gets. Icons, symbols, and manipulatives can be designed to
have correspondence to specific components (S. L. Gillam
et al., 2018; Spencer & Petersen, 2016; Story Grammar
Marker, 2020). In some studies, interventionists have
taught each component with its symbol or icon in isola-
tion and spent time explaining its purpose in stories (e.g.,
Brown et al., 2014; S. L. Gillam et al., 2014), but in our
research, we teach children the names of the story grammar
parts while modeling a new story. Through the repeated
visual presentation of the icons in a specific order and re-
peated aural association with the names of the parts, chil-
dren quickly acquire an understanding of the schema. Icons
have also been used to represent less common vocabulary
(i.e., Tier 2 words; Beck et al., 2013) and complex language
features such as causal and temporal ties (Gardner & Spencer,
2016; Petersen et al., 2019). Pictography, which involves the
use of simple line drawings to represent ideas and events in a
story, can support story retelling, story generation, and the
production of complex language (R. B. Gillam & Ukrainetz
2006; McFadden, 1998). We find pictography particularly
useful when children are asked to generate their own story.
By quickly drawing the content of the childs story, children
can use the visual representation to support subsequent
Spencer & Petersen: Narrative Intervention 1087
retellings (Petersen et al., 2014; Spencer et al., 2013). Graphic
organizers and concept maps are additional examples of vi-
suals that have been used to help children learn the pattern
of stories (Boulineau et al., 2004; S. L. Gillam et al., 2015).
When visuals are used during intervention, clinicians
should be strategic about fading them. Sometimes, pictures
or illustrations correspond to the specially designed stories,
which reduce the demands for reticent children. If using
storybooks, interventionists need to be cognizant that the
illustrations can provide more information about the story
than is needed. In some cases, relying on that information
is beneficial, but sometimes, too many visual supports can
interfere in the acquisition of the cognitive schema that al-
lows them to use the structures in a generalized manner.
Icons, symbols, pictography, and manipulatives serve as
reminders of the narrative structure but contain less infor-
mation about a specific story than detailed pictures and il-
lustrations do. Therefore, we recommend fading pictures
and illustrations before icons so that children can benefit
from a smooth and gradual transition from easy to difficult
tasks within a session. For example, in recent studies featur-
ing small group narrative intervention with Spanish-speaking
preschoolers learning English (Spencer et al., 2019, 2020),
illustrations and icons were displayed while the story was
modeled and when the first child retold the story indepen-
dently. When the second child retold the story independently,
only the icons were available, and when the third child re-
told the story, no visuals were available.
Deliver Immediate Corrective Feedback
Feedback is a powerful teaching tool (Hattie &
Timperley, 2007). If a child is saying things you want
them to say again or if they are using language you want
them to repeat, the best positive feedback includes active
listening (e.g., nodding head, eye contact) and recasting
(Cleave et al., 2015). If the child is not including specific
aspects of story structure or language that you desire, a
correction is needed. Effective corrective feedback has three
main characteristics, all of which apply to delivering feed-
back in narrative interventions. First, effective feedback
focuses on what the child should do and minimizes atten-
tion given to the incorrect response. Students need infor-
mation about their performance and what they can do to
improve it. Ultimately, a successful correction leads to the
child producing the correct response (Watkins & Slocum,
2004). A possible correction could simply be a model of
what the child should say (e.g., Say it like this. John was
sad.) with a subsequent opportunity for the child to try
it again.
Second, effective feedback is immediate (Watkins &
Slocum, 2004). Practiced errors are likely to continue. There-
fore, as soon as an error is made, a correction should follow.
For example, if a child is retelling a story and skips a critical
element (e.g., action), this would be considered an error. The
interventionist should stop the story immediately and make
an appropriate correction. It is important not to wait until
the end of the story to provide the correction because feed-
back that is delayed from the performance is less effective
than feedback that is delivered immediately and contingent
up student responses (Archer & Hughes, 2011). This immedi-
ate feedback can be delivered in a naturalistic way, where
the interventionist can express genuine interest or confusion
about missing information in the story.
Third, effective feedback is specific (Watkins & Slocum,
2004). It is tempting to give a reminder or hint at what the
child should have said or provide some general information
that a mistake was made (Oops. You forgot something.).
Unfortunately, less intrusive corrections such as these can
confuse or frustrate children, further depressing their moti-
vation to try challenging things. If the response was firmly
in a childs repertoire (in other words, it was easy), they
would have made it. It is best to assume that the response
is still too hard and to give that child-specific information
about what to do instead. This can be resolved by explicitly
telling the child what was missed (e.g., Oops. You forgot
to tell us his feeling. How did he feel?) or modeling what
the child should say and then giving the child the opportu-
nity to imitate it.
Telling or retelling story grammar elements in a con-
sistent sequence can hasten the acquisition of the schema
and prevent excluding critical information accidentally.
In a way, the story grammar components operate like the
links in a chain that function properly when arranged in a
certain order. For that reason, when a child skips a story
grammar element, it can be helpful to have them practice
the correct sequence again immediately following the cor-
rection. In Petersen et al. (2017), the interventionists made
immediate corrections and then asked the student to con-
tinue retelling the story starting with the component before
the one that was missed. This type of overcorrection proce-
dure honors the special cohesion between the elements and
helps students to depend on the ordinal unfolding of events
as they tell or retell stories.
Use Efficient and Effective Prompts
There is a large array of options when it comes to
prompting (Cengher et al., 2018). One method of prompt-
ing (and fading) was described above as it relates to the
use of visual materials. When the gradual withdrawal of
visual materials (e.g., illustrations and icons) is embedded
in the interventions procedural steps, prompting is con-
trolled by the curriculum. In other words, they are crucial
active ingredients that should not be altered by the inter-
ventionist. Aside from these built-in prompts, clinicians are
in control of the rest. It is the interventionists responsibil-
ity to select and use efficient and effective prompts.
Over the years, we have come to appreciate a simple
prompting technique that can reduce the amount of time
spent prompting while ensuring that the child is successful
in two easy steps. We call this procedure two-step prompting.
For any clinician-controlled response prompt, we recom-
mend first asking a question that includes specific informa-
tion about what the child missed or should have mentioned.
Although furthering prompts like Then what happened?or
What happened next?can be used, we do not consider these
response prompts because they do not provide information
1088 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools Vol. 51 10811096 October 2020
about what response is expected from the child. Furthering
prompts are typically used to encourage the child to keep
going or redirect them to the task, but assume they do not
need help forming the expected response. In two-step prompt-
ing, we suggest that clinicians use questions such as How
did John feel about his problem?or What did he do to fix
the problem?because they direct the childs attention to the
specific part of the story that needs to be told/retold. They
also enhance childrens ability to answer comprehension
questions about a story without explicitly teaching children
to answer wh-questions (Spencer et al., 2013). The second
part of two-step prompting is to model it for the child. If
the child is unable to tell/retell the expected target within a
few seconds after asking the question, the clinician should
quickly follow it up with a model sentence such as John
was sad because he cut his knee. Now, you say that.or He
asked his mom for a bandage. Your turn.
Two-step prompting has a number of advantages.
First, by using wh-questions as a prompting strategy, chil-
drens ability to answer wh-questions improves without spe-
cifically teaching it. In Spencer, Petersen, Slocum, and Allen
(2015), preschoolers who received large group narrative
intervention that addressed retelling using wh-questions
as prompts made substantial gains over the control group
on a story comprehension measure (d= 54). Second, the
two-step prompting procedure helps children be successful.
Children do not have to fail multiple times to get the amount
of help they need. It is important that children experience
success and receive approval and praise for their efforts,
which ultimately determines if they will try again. Third,
two-step prompting is efficient. It can take less than 10 s to
complete both steps if they are needed. This reduces the
amount of time a child experiences difficulty and reduces
the time other children are passively observing. Fourth, the
two-step prompting procedure is easy for the clinician to
remember. Multistep, systematic prompting can become
laborious for the interventionist to remember the steps, espe-
cially if those steps are individualized for different children.
Fifth, two-step prompting is generally applicable. This means
that it works for most children and most targets. It simplifies
how an interventionist differentiates within a group, depend-
ing on the needs of the children. For example, let us say
one child is still working on retelling a story with all the basic
story grammar elements and another child is working on
using complex vocabulary. For the first child, the interven-
tionist can ask a question about the story grammar element
missed, and for the second child, the interventionist can ask,
What is the special word that goes in this sentence?and
then model the sentence if needed (e.g., Weddle et al., 2016).
Finally, two-step prompting leads to independence while re-
ducing prompt dependency. By starting the prompting with
the question, which is often a natural part of storytelling be-
tween conversational partners, the clinician does not provide
more help than is needed. This allows the child an opportu-
nity to produce a mostly independent response when they
can do it.
While two-step prompting works with most children,
question prompts are not equally effective with all children.
In a recent narrative intervention study with children with
autism (Garcia et al., 2019), we discovered that participants
rarely responded to the first step of two-step prompting.
Presumably, they did not understand the questions. Model-
ing sentences and delaying the timing of models were more
effective than the story grammarfocused wh-questions. It
became apparent that, if a child has to be taught how to
respond to the intended prompts, then they cannot be used
effectively to promptbehavior. In our experience, two-
step prompting is a good procedure to start with, but clini-
cians may need to adapt their prompting procedures to align
with individual childrens strengths and weaknesses like we
had to do in the study with children with autism. Regardless
of adaptation of prompting, clinicians should continue to
follow the general recommendation of using the most effi-
cient and effective prompts possible. For children with
autism and other developmental disabilities, this may in-
volve providing longer wait times, limiting verbal prompts,
and strategically fading visual prompts (Favot et al., 2018;
Garcia et al., 2019; S. L. Gillam et al., 2015; Petersen et al.,
2014). If interventions are delivered individually, more
gradual least-to-most prompting (i.e., more than two steps)
is easier to manage and may be needed for some children
with autism to acquire storytelling skills (Whalon et al., 2019).
Differentiate, Individualize, and Extend
Small group narrative intervention is extremely versa-
tile and can facilitate differentiation and individualization
(Petersen et al., 2019; Spencer et al., 2018; Weddle et al.,
2016) because so many different targets can be addressed
within the context of a single story and in a single session.
If the story is purposefully selected, clinicians can focus in-
tervention for some children on basic targets such as using
the story grammar elements while encouraging other chil-
dren to practice advanced targets such as using more com-
plex sentences or complex vocabulary. It is even possible
to include children in the group who use augmentative
and alternative communication (Soto et al., 2007) or whose
targets involve receptive language. Likewise, the promotion
of perspective taking (Dodd et al., 2011) and inferencing
(Gardner & Spencer, 2016) can be targeted within a narra-
tive intervention. It is important to set the demands of the
intervention task (e.g., retelling, answering questions, manip-
ulating pictures) according to each childs language targets
(Westerveld & Gillon, 2010) and to use two-step prompting
flexibly. School-based SLPs are frequently pressured to do
more with less time. Small group narrative intervention is
one way to accomplish that.
Depending on the age and development of the chil-
dren, clinicians can extend their interventions to promote
grade-appropriate academic and social skills. This means
that intervention should not end when children can tell or
retell a story using relevant story structure. This is just the
starting place that serves as a solid foundation for teaching
other critical skills that children with language disabilities
often lack (e.g., personal and fictional generation, writing,
using complex vocabulary). Even the discourse and sen-
tence level structures of expository language can be more
Spencer & Petersen: Narrative Intervention 1089
easily addressed once the narrative language foundation
has been established (Petersen et al., 2019). If students be-
come bored during intervention sessions, this is a red flag
that the clinician has not moved them along to more chal-
lenging tasks.
Sometimes, clinicians can struggle with what to do next.
Fortunately, there are a number of published programs that
can be helpful in this regard. Story Champs (Spencer &
Petersen, 2016) includes 57 different lesson plans with eight
add-on lessons so that teachers and clinicians have a large
variety of targets to choose from, including simple to com-
plex oral and written narratives, oral and written exposition,
vocabulary, and complex language. The Story Grammar
Marker program has a number of variations that facilitate
social emotional learning, text complexity and close reading,
and writing (Moreau & Fidrych, 2008). SKILL has explicit
procedures that move children from producing simple story
grammar elements to more complex stories with academic
language (S. L. Gillam et al., 2018). If a clinician decides
not to use a published program, we recommend consulting
the general education curriculum and academic standards
used locally or visiting http://www.coreknowledge.org for
helpful materials and grade-level academic sequences.
Arrange for Generalization Opportunities
Narratives are a great choice for language intervention
because they have direct relevance to highly valued academic
and social skills. To maximize the effect of intervention, we
recommend intentionally planning for generalization oppor-
tunities. One of our favorite ways to ensure this happens
is to involve classroom teachers in the intervention. For in-
stance, SLPs can support teachersimplementation of nar-
rative intervention, in large or small group arrangements.
A class can be divided into multiple small groups, and the
teacher and SLP can each focus on a group to work with at
the same time. In one study examining a multitiered imple-
mentation of narrative intervention in preschools (Spencer
et al., 2018), teachers and teaching assistants were both in-
volved in delivering the intervention. Cimburek and Petersen
(2017) examined a similar multitiered implementation for
preschool and kindergarten students. General education
teachers delivered large group narrative intervention while
SLPs delivered small group narrative intervention to at-risk
students. By engaging teachers in narrative intervention,
they learn the complex language that they should be en-
couraging their students to use and are more likely to extend
practice opportunities to other academic domains (e.g., so-
cial studies, writing). If teachers choose not to participate
in the intervention directly, the SLP might try to provide in-
tervention in the classroom (e.g., S. L. Gillam et al., 2014;
Weddle et al., 2016) so that the teacher at least observes
and hears what is being taught.
Another option is for SLPs to create activities for
students to practice narrative-related targets in other con-
texts and consult with teachers about how to facilitate their
use. For example, a simple lesson extension can be written
out that directs the teacher to have children turn-n-talk
about what they did over the weekend. SLPs can also
supply teachers with graphic organizers or concept maps
displaying the icons or symbols from narrative interven-
tion, making it easier for children to see how the skills
taught in intervention can be used during classroom activi-
ties such as reading comprehension or writing. If special
stories are used for initial training, then storybook reading
is an excellent context for generalization, whether the
books are read by an SLP, a teacher, or a parent. Family
engagement activities can be sent home so that children
have support retelling stories to family members. In a re-
cent study, we supplied teachers with posters of each of
the vocabulary words targeted during intervention (Spencer
et al., 2020). The purpose was to help the teacher and all
the students be on the lookout for those words and their
synonyms, which increases word consciousness (McKeown
et al., 2017). Resourceful SLPs can use their creativity to
craft activities that promote the generalized and spontane-
ous use of narrative-based language targets. The important
thing is that generalization is not left to chance. Planning
for it will hasten the acquisition and maintenance of trained
skills, as well as promote their use in meaningful contexts
(Alessi, 1987; Fey, 1988).
Make It Fun!
Simply put, people talk because they get attention
from others when they do. Other benefits may be associ-
ated with specific requests (e.g., asking for a drink), but
as humans, we are primarily motivated by social conse-
quences. When engaged in narrative intervention, SLPs
should keep in mind that the natural consequence of telling
a story is attention and approval from a listener. As clini-
cians strategically use their attention to reinforce storytelling
and make the interaction enjoyable, children will be willing
to try challenging things. It is helpful to choose published
narrative intervention programs that include games, mate-
rials, or activities that appeal to children, but it is danger-
ous to rely too heavily on them. If children are reticent,
bored, or complain, then it is the SLPs responsibility to
facilitate engagement. Because children naturally want
to talk about themselves and their experiences, narratives
are the perfect context for practicing language without it
feeling like work.
We have a number of suggestions for how to make nar-
rative intervention fun. First, SLPs can increase the value of
their attention by being animated when listening. Use over-
exaggerated facial expressions and comments (e.g., No
way!) to show children you are listening carefully. Recasting
is an effective way to show approval while having the added
benefit of modeling complex language and expanding the
childs utterance (Cleave et al., 2015). Second, teach other
children to be active, engaged listeners and praise them
when they show respect for the storyteller. Peers can be ex-
plicitly taught how to paraphrase a peersstorybriefly.
SLPs can model paraphrasing and encourage the other
children to do it after listening to a story. Third, play games
and use competition but do not eliminate attention and ap-
proval. It is okay to use consequences other than your at-
tention (e.g., points, getting to color in a square on a chart,
1090 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools Vol. 51 10811096 October 2020
or moving a game piece), but although they may be needed
temporarily for training purposes, attention will be neces-
sary to maintain storytelling skills outside the intervention
context. Fourth, take advantage of peer mediation. Putting
children together in teams or pairs can increase motivation
for the storytelling tasks. Children often work to impress
their friends and may accept help from peers better than
from adults. Finally, engage children in role plays, using
props or puppets, or movement related to the stories. Re-
cent research suggests that combining movement with story-
telling enhances childrens language outcomes (Brinton &
Fujiki, 2017; Culatta et al., 2010; Duncan et al., 2019).
Conclusions
Our purpose was to provide an overview of the aca-
demic and social value of narratives and to offer principle-
driven guidance based on research and our own clinical
experience implementing narrative intervention. Narratives
are an extremely versatile conduit for learning a large num-
ber of academic and socially meaningful skills. Narrative
intervention can be used with children as young as 3 years
old, all the way through high school. Research has docu-
mented its effectiveness for typically developing children,
those who are at risk, children with language disabilities,
children with autism, and English language learners. While
there are a handful of published narrative intervention pro-
grams available for clinical use, narrative interventions
can be developed using materials that most SLPs have
readily available. When narrative intervention is implemented
following the principles of intervention outlined in this article,
SLPs can have confidence that their approach is supported
by research and extensive clinical experience. It is our hope
that every school-based SLP can harness the power of stories
in their clinical practice.
References
Adlof, S. M., McLeod, A. N., & Leftwich, B. (2014). Structured
narrative retell instruction for young children from low socio-
economic backgrounds: A preliminary study of feasibility.
Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 391. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.
2014.00391
Alessi, G. (1987). Generative strategies and teaching for generali-
zation. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 5, 1527. https://doi.
org/10.1007/BF03392816
Allen,M.S.,Kertoy,M.K.,Sherblom,J.C.,&Pettit,J.M.
(1994). Childrens narrative productions: A comparison of
personal event and fictional stories. Applied Psycholinguistics,
15(2), 149176. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400005300
Anderson, R. C., & Pearson, P. D. (1984). A schema-theoretic
view of basic processes in reading comprehension. Handbook
of Reading Research, 1, 255291.
Applebee, A. (1978). The childs concept of story: Ages two to sev-
enteen. The University of Chicago Press.
Archer, A. L., & Hughes, C. A. (2011). Explicit instruction: Effec-
tive and efficient teaching. Guilford.
Basso, K. H. (1990). Western Apache language and culture: Essays
in linguistic anthropology. University of Arizona Press.
Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & Kucan, L. (2013). Bringing words
to life: Robust vocabulary instruction. Guilford.
Benson, S. E. (2009). Understanding literate language: Developmen-
tal and clinical issues. Contemporary Issues in Communication
Science and Disorders, 36, 174178. https://doi.org/10.1044/
cicsd_36_F_174
Berninger, V., & Graham, S. (1998). Language by hand: A synthe-
sis of a decade of research on handwriting. Handwriting Re-
view, 12, 1125.
Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2010). Listening comprehen-
sion, oral expression, reading comprehension, and written ex-
pression: Related yet unique language systems in grades 1, 3,
5, and 7. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(3), 635651.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019319
Bishop, D. V., & Edmondson, A. (1987). Language-impaired 4-year-
olds: Distinguishing transient from persistent impairment. Journal
of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52(2), 156173. https://doi.
org/10.1044/jshd.5202.156
Bliss,L.S.,&McCabe,A.(2012). Personal narratives: Assess-
ment and intervention. SIG 1 Perspectives on Language Learn-
ing and Education, 19(4), 130138. https://doi.org/10.1044/
lle19.4.130
Boudreau, D. (2008). Narrative abilities: Advances in research and
implications for clinical practice. Topics in Language Disorders,
28(2), 99114. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.TLD.0000318932.
08807.da
Boulineau, T., Fore, C., III., Hagan-Burke, S., & Burke, M. D.
(2004). Use of story-mapping to increase the story-grammar
text comprehension of elementary students with learning dis-
abilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 27(2), 105121. https://
doi.org/10.2307/1593645
Bourg, T., Bauer, P., & van den Broek, P. (1997). Building the
bridges: The development of event comprehension and repre-
sentation. In P. W. van den Broek, P. J. Bauer, & T. Bourg
(Eds.), Developmental spans in event comprehension and repre-
sentation: Bridging fictional and actual events (pp. 385407).
Erlbaum.
Bowles, R. P., Justice, L. M., Khan, K. S., Piasta, S. B., Skibbe,
L. E., & Foster, T. D. (2020). Development of the Narrative
Assessment Protocol-2: A tool for examining young chil-
drens narrative skill. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services
in Schools, 59(2), 390404. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_LSHSS-
19-00038
Bowman-Perrott, L., Davis, H., Vannest, K., Williams, L., Greenwood,
C., & Parker, R. (2013). Academic benefits of peer tutoring: A
meta-analytic review of single-case research. School Psychol-
ogy Review, 42(1), 3955.
Brinton, B., & Fujiki, M. (2017). The power of stories: Facilitating
social communication in children with limited language abili-
ties. School Psychology International, 38(5), 523540. https://
doi.org/10.1177/0143034317713348
Brinton, B., & Fujiki, M. (2019). Facilitating social communication
in children with developmental language disorder: A bibliother-
apeutic approach. Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest
Groups, 4(3), 532537. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_PERS-SIG16-
2019-0005
Brown, J. A., Garzarek, J. E., & Donegan, K. L. (2014). Effects
of a narrative intervention on story retelling in at-risk young
children. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 34(3),
154164. https://doi.org/10.1177/0271121414536447
Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Harvard Univer-
sity Press.
Castilla-Earls, A., Petersen, D., Spencer, T., & Hammer, K. (2015).
Narrative development in monolingual Spanish-speaking
preschool children. Early Education and Development, 26(8),
11661186. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2015.1027623
Spencer & Petersen: Narrative Intervention 1091
Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., & Weismer, S. E. (2006). Language
deficits in poor comprehenders: A case for the simple view of
reading. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
49(2), 278293. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/023)
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2002). A
longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children
with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and
Hearing Research, 45(6), 11421157. https://doi.org/10.1044/
1092-4388(2002/093)
Catts, H. W., Nielsen, D. C., Bridges, M. S., & Liu, Y. S. (2016).
Early identification of reading comprehension difficulties.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 49(5), 451465. https://doi.
org/10.1177%2F0022219414556121
Cengher, M., Budd, A., Farrell, N., & Fienup, D. M. (2018). A review
of prompt-fading procedures: Implications for effective and effi-
cient skill acquisition. Journal of Developmental and Physical
Disabilities, 30(2), 155173. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-
017-9575-8
Champion, T. B. (1998). Tell me somethingood: A description
of narrative structures among African American children. Lin-
guistics and Education, 9(3), 251286. https://doi.org/10.1016/
S0898-5898(97)90002-4
Champion, T., Seymour, H., & Camarata, S. (1995). Narrative
discourse among African American children. Journal of Narra-
tive Language History, 5(4), 333352. https://doi.org/10.1075/
jnlh.5.4.03dis
Cimburek, H., & Petersen, D. B. (2017, November). Comparing
the results of oral narrative intervention to shared storybook
intervention. Research presentation at the American Speech-
Language-Hearing Annual Convention,, Boston, MA, United
States.
Clarke, P. J., Snowling, M. J., Truelove, E., & Hulme, C. (2010).
Ameliorating childrens reading-comprehension difficulties a
randomized controlled trial. Psychological Science, 21(8),
11061116. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0956797610375449
Cleave, P. L., Becker, S. D., Curran, M. K., Van Horne, A. J. O.,
& Fey, M. E. (2015). The efficacy of recasts in language inter-
vention: A systematic review and meta-analysis. American
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 24(2), 237255. https://
doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJSLP-14-0105
Colozzo, P., Gillam, R. B., Wood, M., Schnell, R. D., & Johnston,
J. R. (2011). Content and form in the narratives of children
with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language,
and Hearing Research, 54(6), 16091627. https://doi.org/10.1044/
1092-4388(2011/100247)
Culatta, B., Hall-Kenyon, K. M., & Black, S. (2010). Teaching ex-
pository comprehension skills in early childhood classrooms.
Topics in Language Disorders, 30(4), 323338. https://doi.org/
10.1097/TLD.0b013e3181ff5a65
Curenton, S. M. (2011). Understanding the landscapes of stories:
The association between preschoolersnarrative comprehension
and production skills and cognitive abilities. Early Child Devel-
opment and Care, 181(6), 791808. https://doi.org/10.1080/
03004430.2010.490946
Dickinson, D. K., & McCabe, A. (2001). Bringing it all together:
The multiple origins, skills, and environmental supports of
early literacy. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 16(4),
186202. https://doi.org/10.1111/0938-8982.00019
Dodd, J. L., Ocampo, A., & Kennedy, K. S. (2011). Perspective tak-
ing though narratives: An intervention for students with ASD.
Communication Disorders Quarterly, 33(1), 2233. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1525740110395014
Dodwell, K., & Bavin, E. L. (2008). Children with specific language
impairment: An investigation of their narratives and memory.
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,
43(2), 201218. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820701366147
Duncan, M., Cunningham, A., & Eyre, E. (2019). A combined
movement and story-telling intervention enhances motor com-
petence and language ability in pre-schoolers to a greater ex-
tent than movement or story-telling alone. European Physical
Education Review, 25(1), 221235. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1356336X17715772
Ellis, E. S., & Worthington, L. A. (1994). Executive summary of
research synthesis on effective teaching principles and the design
of quality tools for educators.NationalCentertoImprove
the Tools of Educators,, College of Education, University of
Oregon.
Engelmann, S., & Carnine, D. (1982). Theory of instruction: Prin-
ciples and applications. Irvington Publishers.
Favot, K., Carter, M., & Stephenson, J. (2018). The effects of an
oral narrative intervention on the fictional narrative retells of
children with ASD and severe language impairment: A pilot
study. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 30(5),
615637. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-018-9608-y
Fazio, B. B., Naremore, R. C., & Connell, P. J. (1996). Tracking
children from poverty at risk for specific language impairment:
A 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Research, 39(3), 611624. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3903.611
Feagans, L., & Appelbaum, M. I. (1986). Language subtypes and
their validation in learning disabled children. Journal of Educa-
tional Psychology, 78(5), 373481. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
0663.78.5.358
Fey, M. E. (1988). Generalization issues facing language interven-
tionists: An introduction. Language, Speech, and Hearing
Services in Schools, 19(3), 272281. https://doi.org/10.1044/
0161-1461.1903.272
Fey, M. E., Catts, H. W., Proctor-Williams, K., Tomblin, J. B., &
Zhang, X. (2004). Oral and written story composition skills of
children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Lan-
guage, and Hearing Research, 47(6), 13011318. https://doi.
org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/098)
Forsyth, D. R., & Archer, C. R. (1997). Technologically assisted
instruction and student mastery, motivation, and matricula-
tion. Teaching of Psychology, 24(3), 207212. https://doi.org/
10.1207/s15328023top2403_17
Garcia, A. R., Spencer, T. D., & Kirby, M. S. (2019). Effect of
narrative intervention on the listening and reading comprehension
of children with autism [Manuscript submitted for publication].
Gardner, C., & Spencer, T. D. (2016, November). Oral narrative
intervention improves inferential word learning. Paper presented
at the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association An-
nual Convention,, Philadelphia, PA, United States.
Gee, J. P. (1989). Two styles of narrative construction and their
linguistic and educational implications. Discourse Processes,
12(3), 287307. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638538909544732
Gillam, R. B., & Johnston, J. R. (1992). Spoken and written lan-
guage relationships in language/learning-impaired and normally
achieving school-age children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Re-
search, 35(6), 13031315. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3506.1303
Gillam, R. B., & Ukrainetz, T. A. (2006). Language intervention
through literature-based units. In T. A. Ukrainetz (Ed.),
Contextualized language intervention (pp. 5994). Thinking
Publications.
Gillam, S. L., Fargo, J., Petersen, D. B., & Clark, M. (2012). As-
sessment of structure dependent narrative features in modeled
contexts: African American and European American children.
English Linguistics Research, 1(1), 117. https://doi.org/10.5430/
elr.v1n1p1
1092 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools Vol. 51 10811096 October 2020
Gillam, S. L., & Gillam, R. B. (2016). Narrative discourse inter-
vention for school-aged children with language impairment:
Supporting Knowledge in Language and Literacy. Topics in
Language Disorders, 36(1), 2034. https://doi.org/10.1097/
TLD.0000000000000081
Gillam,S.L.,Gillam,R.B.,Fargo,J.D.,Olszewski,A.,&Segura,H.
(2016). Monitoring Indicators of Scholarly Language: A progress-
monitoring instrument for measuring narrative discourse skills.
Communication Disorders Quarterly, 38(2), 96106. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1525740116651442
Gillam, S. L., Gillam, R. B., & Laing, C. E. (2018). Supporting
Knowledge in Language and Literacy (3rd ed.). Utah State
University.
Gillam, S. L., Gillam, R. B., & Reece, K. (2012). Language out-
comes of contextualized and decontextualized language inter-
vention: Results of an early efficacy study. Language, Speech,
and Hearing Services in Schools, 43, 276291. https://doi.org/
10.1044/0161-1461(2011/11-0022)
Gillam, S. L., Hartzheim, D., Studenka, B., Simonsmeier, V., &
Gillam, R. (2015). Narrative intervention for children with au-
tism spectrum disorder (ASD). Journal of Speech, Language,
and Hearing Research, 58(3), 920933. https://doi.org/10.1044/
2015_JSLHR-L-14-0295
Gillam, S. L., Olszewski, A., Fargo, J., & Gillam, R. B. (2014).
Classroom-based narrative and vocabulary instruction: Results
of an early-stage, nonrandomized comparison study. Language,
Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 45(3), 204219. https://
doi.org/10.1044/2014_LSHSS-13-0008
Gilmore, S. E., Klecan-Aker, J. S., & Owen, W. L. (1999). The re-
lationship of storytelling ability to reading comprehension in
children with learning disability. Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology and Audiology, 23, 142151.
Gorman, B. K., Fiestas, C. E., Pena, E. D., & Clark, M. R. (2011).
Creative and stylistic devices employed by children during a
storybook narrative task: A crosscultural study. Language,
Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 42, 167181. https://
doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2010/10-0052)
Greenhalgh, K. S., & Strong, C. J. (2001). Literate language fea-
tures in spoken narratives of children with typical language
and children with language impairments. Language, Speech,
and Hearing Services in Schools, 32(2), 114125. https://doi.
org/10.1044/0161-1461(2001/010)
Greenwood, C. R., Delquadri, J. C., & Hall, R. V. (1984). Oppor-
tunities to respond and student academic achievement. In W. L.
Heward, T. E. Heron, D. S. Hill, & J. Trap-Porter (Eds.), Focus
on behavior analysis in education (pp. 5888). Merrill Publishing
Company.
Griffin, T. M., Hemphill, L., Camp, L., & Wolf, D. P. (2004). Oral
discourse in the preschool years and later literacy skills. First Lan-
guage, 24(2), 123147. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723704042369
Hadley, E. B., & Dickinson, D. K. (2018). Measuring young chil-
drens word knowledge: A conceptual review. Journal of Early
Childhood Literacy, 20(2), 231253. https://doi.org/10.1177%
2F1468798417753713
Hart, K. I., Fujiki, M., Brinton, B., & Hart, C. H. (2004). The re-
lationship between social behavior and severity of language
impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
47(3), 647662. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/050)
Hattie, J., & Timperley, H. (2007). The power of feedback. Re-
view of Educational Research, 77(1), 81112. https://doi.org/
10.3102/003465430298487
Hayes, J. R., & Flower, L. S. (1980). The dynamics of composing:
Making plans and juggling constraints. Cognitive Processes in
Writing, 3150.
Hayward, D. V., Gillam, R. B., & Lien, P. (2007). Retelling a
script-based story: Do children with and without language im-
pairments focus on script and story elements? American Jour-
nal of Speech-Language Pathology, 16(3), 235245. https://doi.
org/10.1044/1058-0360(2007/028)
Hayward, D. V., & Schneider, P. (2000). Effectiveness of teaching
story grammar knowledge to pre-school children with lan-
guage impairment: An exploratory study. Child Language
Teaching and Therapy, 16(3), 255284. https://doi.org/10.1177/
026565900001600303
Heilmann, J., Miller, J. F., Nockerts, A., & Dunaway, C. (2010).
Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme using narrative
retells in young school-age children. American Journal of
Speech-Language Pathology, 19(2), 154166. https://doi.org/
10.1044/1058-0360(2009/08-0024)
Heward, W. L., Courson, F. H., & Narayan, J. S. (1989). Using
choral responding to increase active student response. Teach-
ing Exceptional Children, 21(3), 7275. https://doi.org/10.1177/
004005998902100321
Hudson, J. A., & Shapiro, L. R. (1991). From knowing to telling:
The development of childrens scripts, stories, and personal
narratives. In A. McCabe & C. Peterson (Eds.), Developing
narrative structure (pp. 89136). Erlbaum.
Hughes, D. L., McGillivray, L., & Schmidek, M. (1997). Guide
to narrative language: Procedures for assessment.Thinking
Publications.
Hyon, S., & Sulzby, E. (1994). African American kindergartners
spoken narratives: Topic associating and topic centered styles.
Linguistics and Education, 6(2), 121152. https://doi.org/10.1016/
0898-5898(94)90009-4
Johnston, J. R. (2008). Narratives: Twenty-five years later. Topics
in Language Disorders, 28(2), 9398. https://doi.org/10.1097/
01.TLD.0000318931.08807.01
Justice, E. C., Swanson, L. A., & Buehler, V. (2008). Use of narra-
tive-based language intervention with children who have co-
chlear implants. Topics in Language Disorders, 28(2), 149161.
https://doi.org/10.1097/01.TLD.0000318935.54548.36
Kaderavek, J. N., & Sulzby, E. (2000). Narrative production by
children with and without specific language impairment: Oral
narratives and emergent readings. Journal of Speech, Language,
and Hearing Research, 43(1), 3449. https://doi.org/10.1044/
jslhr.4301.34
Kim, Y. S., Al Otaiba, S., & Wanzek, J. (2015). Kindergarten pre-
dictors of third grade writing. Learning and Individual Differ-
ences, 37, 2737. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.11.009
Kirby, M. S., Spencer, T. D. & Chen, Y. I. (2020). Fostering kin-
dergartnersnarrative writing through oral language instruction
[Manuscript submitted for publication].
Knapp, F. A., & Desrochers, M. N. (2009). An experimental evaluation
of the instructional effectiveness of a student response system: A
comparison with constructed overt responding. International Jour-
nal of Teaching and Learning in Higher Education, 21(1), 3646.
Koutsoftas, A. D., & Gray, S. (2012). Comparison of narrative
and expository writing in students with and without language-
learning disabilities. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services
in Schools, 43(4), 395409. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461
(2012/11-0018)
Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: Oral ver-
sions of the personal experience. In J. Helm, (Ed.), Essays
on the verbal and visual Arts: Proceedings of the 1996 Annual
Spring Meeting of the American Ethnological Society (pp. 1244).
University of Washington Press.
LaFrance, D. L., & Tarbox, J. (2020). The importance of multiple
exemplar instruction in the establishment of novel verbal behavior.
Spencer & Petersen: Narrative Intervention 1093
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(1), 1024. https://doi.
org/10.1002/jaba.611
Lahey, M. (1990). Who shall be called language disordered? Some
reflections and one perspective. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Disorders, 55(4), 612620. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5504.612
Language and Reading Research Consortium, Jiang, H., & Davis, D.
(2017). Lets Know! Proximal impacts on prekindergarten through
grade 3 studentscomprehension-related skills. The Elementary
School Journal, 118(2), 177206. https://doi.org/10.1086/694220
Language and Reading Research Consortium, Jiang, H., & Logan, J.
(2019). Improving reading comprehension in the primary grades:
Mediated effects of a language-focused classroom intervention.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62(8), 117.
https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-19-0015
Layng, T. J. (2019). Tutorial: Understanding concepts: Implica-
tions for behavior analysts and educators. Perspectives on
Behavior Science, 42(2), 345363. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s40614-018-00188-6
Lee, K. (2020). Effects of large group expository language inter-
vention [Unpublished masters thesis]. Brigham Young
University.
Leyva, D., Berrocal, M., & Nolivos, V. (2014). Spanish-speaking
parentchild emotional narratives and childrens social skills.
Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(1), 2242. https://
doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.725188
MacLachlan, B. G., & Chapman, R. S. (1988). Communication
breakdowns in normal and language learning-disabled childrens
conversation and narration. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Disorders, 53(1), 27. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5301.02
Mandler, J. M. (1987). On the psychological reality of story struc-
ture. Discourse Processes, 10(1), 129. https://doi.org/10.1080/
01638538709544656
Matsuyama, U. K. (1983). Can story grammar speak Japanese?
The Reading Teacher, 36(7), 666669.
McCabe, A. (2017). Childrens personal narratives reflect where they
come from, reveal who they are and predict where they are going.
In N. Kucirkova, C. E. Snow, V. Grøver, & C. Mcbride (Eds.),
The Routledge international handbook of early literacy education
(pp. 308324). Taylor & Francis Group.
McCabe, A., Bliss, L., Barra, G., & Bennett, M. (2008). Compari-
son of personal versus fictional narratives of children with
language impairment. American Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology, 17(2), 194206. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360
(2008/019)
McCabe, P. C., & Marshall, D. J. (2006). Measuring the social com-
petence of preschool children with specific language impairment:
Correspondence among informant ratings and behavioral ob-
servations. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 26(4),
234246. https://doi.org/10.1177/02711214060260040401
McCutchen, D. (1996). A capacity theory of writing: Working
memory in composition. Educational Psychology Review, 8(3),
299325. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01464076
McFadden,T.U.(1998). Immediate effects of pictographic represen-
tation on childrens narratives. Child Language Teaching and Ther-
apy, 14(1), 5167. https://doi.org/10.1191/026565998673602439
McKeown, M. G., Deane, P. D., & Lawless, R. R. (2017). Vocabu-
lary assessment to support instruction: Building rich word-learning
experiences.Guilford.
Meyer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? (a boy, a dog, and a frog).
Dial Press.
Miller, J., Andriacchi, K., & Nockerts, A. (2015). Assessing lan-
guage production using SALT software: A clinicians guide to
language sample analysis (2nd ed.). University of Wisconsin,,
Waisman Center, Language Analysis Laboratory.
Mills, M. T., Watkins, R. V., & Washington, J. A. (2013). Structural
and dialectal characteristics of fictional and personal narratives
of school-age African American children. Language, Speech,
and Hearing Services in Schools, 44(2), 211223. https://doi.org/
10.1044/0161-1461(2012/12-0021)
Minami, M. (2002). Culture-specific language styles, the develop-
ment of oral narrative and literacy. Multilingual Matters.
Moreau, M., & Fidrych, H. (1994). How to use the Story Grammar
Marker: A guide for improving speaking, reading and writing
within your existing program. MindWing Concepts, Inc.
Moreau, M., & Fidrych, H. (2008). The Story Grammar Marker
teachers manual. MindWing Concepts, Inc.
National Governors Association Center for Best Practices &
Council of Chief State School Officers. (2010). Common Core
State Standards Initiative. http://www.corestandards.org
Nippold, M. A., & Schwartz, I. E. (1996). Children with slow ex-
pressive language development: What is the forecast for school
achievement? American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
5(2), 2225. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360.0502.22
Pavelko, S. L., Lieberman, R. J., Schwartz, J., Hahs-Vaughn, D.,
& Nye, C. (2017). The development of writing skills in 4-year-
old children with and without specific language impairment.
Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 31(79), 682696. https://doi.
org/10.1080/02699206.2017.1310298
Petersen, D. B. (2011). A systematic review of narrative-based lan-
guage intervention with children who have language impairment.
Communication Disorders Quarterly, 32(4), 207220. https://doi.
org/10.1177/152574010935937
Petersen, D. B., Brown, C. L., Ukrainetz, T. A., Wise, C., Spencer,
T. D., & Zebre, J. (2014). Systematic individualized narrative
language intervention on the personal narratives of children
with autism. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,
45, 6766. https://doi.org/10.1044/2013_LSHSS-12-0099
Petersen, D. B., Chanthongthip, H., Ukrainetz, T. A., Spencer, T. D.,
& Steeve, R. W. (2017). Dynamic assessment of narratives: Effi-
cient, accurate identification of language impairment in bi-
lingual students. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Research, 60(4), 983998. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-
L-15-0426
Petersen, D. B., Gillam, S. L., Spencer, T., & Gillam, R. B. (2010).
The effects of literate narrative intervention on children with
neurologically based language impairments: An early stage
study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(4),
961981. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/09-0001)
Petersen, D. B., & Spencer, T. D. (2012). The Narrative Language
Measures: Tools for language screening, progress monitoring,
and intervention planning. SIG 1 Perspectives on Language
Learning and Education, 19(4), 119129. https://doi.org/10.1044/
lle19.4.119
Petersen, D. B., & Spencer, T. D. (2016). CUBED Assessment.Lan-
guage Dynamics Group,, LLC. http://www.languagedynamicsgroup.
com
Petersen, D. B., Staskowski, M., Brough, M. P., Spencer, T. D., &
Foster, M. E. (2019). The effects of a multi-tiered system of lan-
guage support on kindergarten oral and written language: A
large-scale clustered randomized controlled trial. Poster session
presented at the American Speech-Language-Hearing Associa-
tion Annual Convention,, Orlando, FL, United States.
Petersen, D. B., Tonn, P., Spencer, T. D., & Foster, M. E. (2020).
The classification accuracy of a dynamic assessment of infer-
ential word learning for bilingual English/Spanish-speaking
school-age children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services
in Schools, 51(1), 144164. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_LSHSS-
18-0129
1094 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools Vol. 51 10811096 October 2020
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Three ways of looking at a
childs narrative: A psycholinguistic analysis. Plenum.
Pituch, K. A., Green, V. A., Didden, R., Lang, R., OReilly, M. F.,
Lancioni, G. E., & Sigafoos, J. (2011). Parent reported treat-
ment priorities for children with autism spectrum disorders. Re-
search in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(1), 135143. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.03.003
Pratton, J., & Hales, L. W. (1986). The effects of active participa-
tion on student learning. The Journal of Educational Research,
79(4), 210215. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1986.10885679
Preece, A. (1987). The range of narrative forms conversationally
produced by young children. Journal of Child Language, 14(2),
353373. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900012976
Roth, F. P., & Spekman, N. J. (1986). Narrative discourse: Spon-
taneously generated stories of learning-disabled and normally
achieving students. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,
51(1), 823. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5101.08
Rumelhart, D. E. (1980). On evaluating story grammars. Cognitive
Science, 4(3), 313316. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0403_5
Schick, A., & Melzi, G. (2010). The development of childrens oral
narratives across contexts. Early Education and Development,
21(3), 293317. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409281003680578
Schneider, P. (1996). Effects of pictures versus orally presented
stories on story retellings by children with language impair-
ment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 5(1),
8696. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360.0501.86
Schneider, P., & Dubé, R. V. (2005). Story presentation effects on
childrens retell content. American Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology, 14(1), 5260. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360
(2005/007)
Schneider, P., Dubé, R. V., & Hayward, D. (2005). The Edmonton
Narrative Norms Instrument. University of Alberta Faculty of
Rehabilitation Medicine.
Scott, C. M., & Windsor, J. (2000). General language performance
measures in spoken and written narrative and expository dis-
course of school-age children with language learning disabilities.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(2),
324339. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4302.324
Sharifian, F. (2002). Chaos in Aboriginal English discourse. In
A. Kirkpatrick (Ed.), Englishes in Asia: Communication, iden-
tity, power, and education (pp. 125141). Language Australia.
Smitherman, G. F. (1977). Talkin and testifyin: The language of
Black America. Houghton, Mifflin & Co.
Snow, C. (1983). Literacy and language: Relationships during the
preschool years. Harvard Educational Review, 53(2), 165189.
https://doi.org/10.17763/haer.53.2.t6177w39817w2861
Snow, C. E., Porche, M. V., Tabors, P. O., & Harris, S. R. (2007).
Is literacy enough? Pathways to academic success for adoles-
cents. Brookes.
Soto, G., Yu, B., & Henneberry, S. (2007). Supporting the devel-
opment of narrative skills of an eight-year old child who uses
an augmentative and alternative communication device. Child
Language, Teaching, and Therapy, 23(1), 2745. https://doi.
org/10.1177%2F0265659007072145
Spencer, T. D., & Goldstein, H. (2019). Assessment of Story Com-
prehension (ASC). Brookes.
Spencer, T. D., Kajian, M., Petersen, D. B., & Bilyk, N. (2013).
Effects of an individualized narrative intervention on chil-
drens storytelling and comprehension skills. Journal of
Early Intervention, 35(3), 243269. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1053815114540002
Spencer, T. D., Moran, M. K., Thompson, M. S., Petersen, D. B.,
& Restrepo, M. A. (2020). Early efficacy of multi-tiered dual
language instruction: Promoting preschoolersSpanish and
English oral language. AERA Open, 6(1), 116. https://doi.org/
10.1177/2332858419897886
Spencer, T. D., & Petersen, D. B. (2016). Story champs. Language
Dynamics Group, LLC.
Spencer, T. D., & Petersen, D. B. (2018). Bridging oral and writ-
ten language: An oral narrative language intervention study
with writing outcomes. Language, Speech, and Hearing Ser-
vices in Schools, 49(3), 569581. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018
Spencer, T. D., Petersen, D. B., & Adams, J. L. (2015). Tier 2 lan-
guage intervention for diverse preschoolers: An early-stage
randomized control group study following an analysis of re-
sponse to intervention. American Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology, 24(4), 619636. https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJSLP-
14-0101
Spencer, T. D., Petersen, D. B., Restrepo, M. A., Thompson, M.,
& Gutierrez Arvizu, M. N. (2019). The effect of Spanish and
English narrative intervention on the language skills of young
dual language learners. Topics in Early Childhood Special Edu-
cation, 38(4), 204219. https://doi.org/0271121418779439
Spencer, T. D., Petersen, D. B., Slocum, T. A., & Allen, M. M.
(2015). Large group narrative intervention in Head Start pre-
schools: Implications for response to intervention. Journal of
Early Childhood Research, 13(2), 196217. https://doi.org/
10.1177%2F1476718X13515419
Spencer, T. D., & Slocum, T. A. (2010). The effect of a narrative
intervention on story retelling and personal story generation
skills of preschoolers with risk factors and narrative language
delays. Journal of Early Intervention, 32(3), 178199. https://
doi.org/10.1177%2F1053815110379124
Spencer, T. D., Weddle, S. A., Petersen, D. B., & Adams, J. A.
(2018). Multi-tiered narrative intervention for preschoolers: A
Head Start implementation study. NHSA Dialog, 20(1), 128.
Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. G. (1979). An analysis of story compre-
hension in elementary school children. In R. O. Freedle (Ed.),
New directions in discourse processing: Vol. 2. Advances in dis-
course processes (pp. 53120). Erlbaum.
Story Grammar Marker. (2020). [Homepage]. https://mindwing-
concepts.com/collections/story-grammar-marker
Suggate, S., Schaughency, E., McAnally, H., & Reese, E. (2018).
From infancy to adolescence: The longitudinal links between
vocabulary, early literacy skills, oral narrative, and reading
comprehension. Cognitive Development, 47, 8295. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.04.005
Swanson, L. A., Fey, M. E., Mills, C. E., & Hood, L. S. (2005).
Use of narrative-based language intervention with children
who have specific language impairment. American Journal of
Speech-Language Pathology, 14(2), 131143. https://doi.org/
10.1044/1058-0360(2005/014)
Tappe, H., & Hara, A. (2013). Language specific narrative text
structure elements in multilingual children. Stellenbosch
Papers in Linguistics Plus, 42, 297331. https://doi.org/10.5842/
42-0-160
Ukrainetz, T. A. (2006). Assessment and intervention within a con-
textualized skill framework. In T. A. Ukrainetz (Ed.), Contextu-
alized language intervention (pp. 758). Thinking Publications.
Wagner, C. R., Sahlén, B., & Nettelbladt, U. (1999). Whats the
story? Narration and comprehension in Swedish preschool
children with language impairment. Child Language Teach-
ing and Therapy, 15(2), 113137. https://doi.org/10.1177/
026565909901500202
Watkins, C. L., & Slocum, T. A. (2004). The components of direct
instruction. In N. E. Marchand-Martella, T. A. Slocum, & R. C.
Martella (Eds.), Introduction to direct instruction (pp. 2865).
Allyn & Bacon.
Spencer & Petersen: Narrative Intervention 1095
Weddle, S. A., Spencer, T. D., Kakian, M., & Petersen, D. B. (2016).
An examination of a multitiered system of language support for
culturally and linguistically diverse preschoolers: Implications
for early and accurate identification. School Psychology Review,
45(1), 109132. https://doi.org/10.17105/SPR45-1.109-132
Westby, C. E. (1984). Development of narrative language abilities.
In G. Wallach & K. Butler (Eds.), Language learning disabilities
in school-age children (pp. 103107). Williams & Wilkins.
Westby, C. E. (1985). Learning to talkTalking to learn: Oral
literate language differences. In C. Simon (Ed.), Communication
skills and classroom success: Therapy methodologies for language-
learning disabled students (pp. 181213). College-Hill Press.
Westby, C. E. (1994). The effects of culture on genre, structure,
and style of oral and written texts. In G. P. Wallach & K. G.
Butler (Eds.), Language learning disabilities in school-age chil-
dren and adolescents (pp. 180218). Macmillan.
Westby, C., & Culatta, B. (2016). Telling tales: Personal event
narratives and life stories. Language, Speech, and Hearing
Services in Schools, 47(4), 260282. https://doi.org/10.1044/
2016_LSHSS-15-0073
Westby, C., Moore, C., & Roman, R. (2002). Reinventing the
enemys language: Developing narratives in Native American
children. Linguistics and Education, 13(2), 235269. https://doi.
org/10.1016/S0898-5898(01)00063-8
Westerveld, M. F., & Gillon, G. T. (2010). Oral narrative context
effects on poor readersspoken language performance: Story
retelling, story generation, and personal narratives. Interna-
tional Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12(2), 132141.
https://doi.org/10.3109/17549500903414440
Whalon, K., Henning, B., Jackson, E., & Intepe-Tingir, S. (2019).
Effects of an adapted story grammar intervention on the lis-
tening comprehension of children with autism. Research in
Developmental Disabilities, 95, Article 103507. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.ridd.2019.103507
Worth, S., & Adair, J. (1972). Through Navajo eyes. Indiana Univer-
sity Press.
1096 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools Vol. 51 10811096 October 2020
... For instance, traditional fables and fairy tales, characterized by their simple language and clear moral lessons, are particularly effective for lower levels of English learners and younger learners, as stories under this category support basic vocabulary development and ethical reasoning (Wiysahnyuy & Valentine, 2023). With different functions, contemporary short stories that portray real-life scenarios and include colloquial expressions are well-suited for advanced learners, exposing them to authentic language use and complex syntactic structures (Spencer & Petersen, 2020). Moreover, incorporating culturally relevant stories that depict local contexts and students' experiences can bridge the gap between textbook language and everyday communication, thereby improving engagement and promoting deeper learning (Huang, 2019). ...
... This careful selection process, coupled with targeted instructional strategies, ensures that the materials are both accessible and engaging, thereby maximizing their educational impact. Additionally, short stories stimulate imagination and critical thinking, which not only improves language skills but also encourages learners to engage more deeply with the content (Spencer & Petersen, 2020). By incorporating interactive activities such as group discussions, retelling, or dramatization, students practice various language skills while expanding their vocabulary mastery in an authentic and enjoyable manner. ...
... According to Krashen's (1982) input hypothesis, language acquisition occurs most effectively when learners are exposed to input that is slightly above their current proficiency level. Short stories can be chosen or adapted to meet this criterion, ensuring that students are consistently challenged to learn new words without feeling overwhelmed (Spencer & Petersen, 2020). Furthermore, repeated exposure to vocabulary through reading reinforces word retention and deepens understanding (Rahmawati, 2020), making short stories an ideal vehicle for vocabulary instruction in the EFL classroom. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study explores the effectiveness of short stories as a teaching tool to enhance vocabulary mastery among senior high school students. The research involved 33 eleventh-grade social studies students from a public high school in Yogyakarta, Indonesia, using a one-group pretest-posttest experimental design. Over four class sessions, students engaged with short stories to improve their vocabulary. Vocabulary proficiency was assessed through tests administered before and after the intervention, with data analyzed using SPSS version 25. The pretest revealed a moderate vocabulary level, with an average score of 54.42. Post-intervention results showed significant improvement, with the average score increasing to 85.39. Statistical analysis yielded a t-value of 39.689, exceeding the critical t-value of 2.042, thus supporting the hypothesis (H1) that short story reading significantly enhances vocabulary mastery in EFL students.
... The purpose of this work was to identify existing studies on language interventions in children with hearing loss and to analyze their effectiveness. Given the difficulties experienced by people with hearing loss on language skills (Arfé, 2015;Geers et al., 2003;Wie et al., 2020) and their direct relevance on academic and social skills (Gilkerson et al., 2018;Hart & Risley, 1995;Schneider & Dubé, 2005;Spencer & Petersen, 2020), our goal was to describe the characteristics of these interventions and to synthesize their overall effectiveness in oral discourse production through meta-analysis. ...
... The common characteristics of the interventions examined in the meta-analysis suggested positive effects for oral discourse production outcomes in the macrostructure and microstructure. The results obtained regarding the significant improvement provided by the intervention programs are comparable to those found by other authors (Gilkerson et al., 2018;Hart & Risley, 1995;Schneider & Dubé, 2005;Spencer & Petersen, 2020). However, these results should be viewed with caution given the small sample size and the high heterogeneity existing between them. ...
... A positive effect was obtained regarding the use of images in language therapies. These results support the findings of Arjmandi et al. (2022) and Spencer and Petersen (2020), who argued that the use of visual elements and other forms of visual communication in intervention programs is beneficial to language and reading and writing development. However, Spencer and Petersen also pointed out that always using visual materials is not necessarily beneficial in all cases, since not all classroom academic tasks, including narrative skills, involve visual elements (e.g., listening, reading comprehension), especially as the course years advance. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Vocabulary, grammar, and discourse skills represent distinct dimensions of language ability in young children. Research suggests that individuals with hearing loss often have difficulties with language skills as compared to their hearing counterparts. The aim of this systematic review and meta-analysis is to analyze the effectiveness of linguistic interventions aimed at improving oral discourse in children with hearing loss. Method A systematic review was conducted according to the PRISMA 2020 statement in five databases. A total of 23 studies were included in the systematic review. From this sample unit, 12 studies were included in the meta-analysis since they had sufficient data for analysis from a meta-analytic approach. Two meta-analyses were performed, one for each dimension of oral discourse skills, differentiating between macrostructure and microstructure and calculating the effects of the intervention and potential moderating variables. Results The results suggest positive effects of the interventions with effect sizes of d = 1.01 (95% confidence interval [CI; 0.58, 1.45], p < .001) for macrostructure and d = 0.87 (95% CI [0.02, 0.60], p < .001) for microstructure. Moderator variable analyses showed that the number of participants was the only significant factor identified for the microstructure dimension. Conclusions Linguistic intervention programs improve the language of children with hearing loss. Most of these interventions include therapies that use visual supports and grammar instruction. However, these results should be interpreted with caution given the small number of studies and their high heterogeneity.
... With the exception of Spencer et al. (2020), only the words directly taught during intervention were assessed in the narrative intervention studies with vocabulary outcomes. When generalised vocabulary improvements were assessed , improvements were not detected. ...
... Furthermore, the enhancement of narrative skills in children aids in bridging their oral and written language capabilities. Consequently, narrative skills serve as a significant indicator of future language and literacy development (Spencer & Petersen, 2020;Vretudaki, 2022). Young children's narrative ability develops predominantly within two contexts/ environments: the familial one as well as the broader social environment (Kelly & Bailey, 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Understanding cross-cultural differences in narratives, especially among young children from diverse cultural backgrounds, can assist teachers to appreciate these normal narrative variations. This study examines the similarities and differences in micro-macro-structural elements of young children's narrative from various cultural backgrounds. Our annotations indicate similarities across children's narratives in the developmental profile of each age group and narratives' structure around the theme of the story (topic-centered stories) whereas differences were found in the use of macrostructural elements, such as character development and references to the relationships between the characters of the story around the central theme of interest. Concerns arise about the influence of the educational environment in shaping children's emergent narrative discourse.
... Understanding the meaning of a story is connected to cognitive skills that enable children to explore themes and clues within the text (Aukerman & Chambers Schuldt, 2016). Moreover, research shows that story comprehension is associated not only with linguistic and cognitive skills but also with social skills (Froiland & Davison, 2020;Spencer & Petersen, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Okul öncesi dönemde çocukların öykü anlama becerileri okuduğunu anlama becerileri gibi birçok beceri ile ilişkilidir. Bu sebeple okul öncesi dönemde çocukların öykü anlama becerilerinin değerlendirilmesi oldukça önemli olduğu söylenebilir. Bu çalışmada; 48-72 aylık okul öncesi dönem çocuklarının öykü anlama becerilerinin değerlendirilmesine yönelik bir rubrik geliştirilmesi ve çocukların öykü anlama becerilerinin yaş ve cinsiyet değişkenlerine göre incelenmesi amaçlanmaktadır. Öykü Anlama Rubriği İstanbul ilinde okul öncesi eğitim kurumlarında öğrenim görmekte olan toplam 340 okul öncesi çocuğuyla uygulanmıştır. Bu rubriğin güvenirlik katsayılarının belirlenmesi amacıyla paralel formlar yöntemi kullanılmıştır. Ölçeğin kapsam geçerliliğinin belirlenmesi amacıyla 12 uzmandan görüş alınmış ve görüşler Lawshe tekniği ile analiz edilmiştir. Erken Okuryazarlık Becerileri Değerlendirme Aracı’nın Alt Testi olan Öyküyü Anlama Alt Testi ve Türkçe Erken Dil Gelişim Testi Alıcı Dil Alt Testi A Formu rubriğin ölçüt geçerliliğinin hesaplanması amacıyla kullanılmıştır. Güvenirlik analizlerinde rubriğin güvenirliğinin hesaplanması amacıyla Cronbach alpha katsayısı belirlenmiştir ve iki formunda yüksek düzeyde iç tutarlılığa sahip olduğu belirlenmiştir. Her iki uygulayıcı için uyum katsayısı incelenmiş ve uygulamacılar arasında anlamlı ilişki olduğu görülmüştür. Araştırma sonucunda, 10 maddelik ve T ve M olmak üzere iki formu bulunan Öykü Anlama Rubriği’nin geçerli ve güvenilir sonuçlar sağladığı bulunmuştur. Bu sonuca ek olarak yaş değişkeninin çocukların öykü anlama puanlarında anlamlı bir fark oluşturduğu ancak cinsiyet değişkeninin anlamlı bir fark oluşturmadığı görülmüştür.
... Third, the Story Grammar Marker can make children like stories more and not get bored with telling stories. Spencer and Petersen (2020) stated that using various modalities will improve children's thinking processes and understanding of multiple concepts. Using a few modalities will make children memorize a story, not understand it. ...
Article
Full-text available
Children with learning disorders often experience challenges in auditory memory processing, which affects their literacy and cognitive skills. Story Grammar Markers (SGM) have been used as an intervention tool to support narrative structure comprehension and memory retention. This study aims to evaluate the effectiveness of SGM in enhancing auditory memory in children with learning disorders in Surakarta. A quasi-experimental research design was implemented using a one-group pretest-posttest method with 15 students aged 7–8 years from various elementary schools. The intervention consisted of five sessions using SGM-based storytelling exercises. The results showed a significant increase in auditory memory scores, with an average pretest score of 16.13 and an average post-test score of 21.40 (p < 0.001). This study confirms that Story Grammar Markers can be an effective educational tool for improving auditory memory in children with learning disorders. These findings suggest the need for further exploration of SGM implementation in inclusive classrooms.
... Narrative intervention is a language intervention method in which the interventionist deliberately includes targeted linguistic elements and features in a narrative, prompting the child to tell or retell the story (Spencer & Petersen, 2020). Narrative intervention is conducted using an approach that acquires and develops narrative structure based on the SG framework of Stein and Glenn (1979). ...
Article
Objectives: The primary objective of this study was to identify the effectiveness of narrative intervention in Korean (L1)-English (L2) bilingual children. To achieve this, the groups were classified based on their L1 and L2 vocabulary performance, providing a comprehensive understanding of the impact of narrative intervention on bilingual children.Methods: Nineteen Korean-English bilingual preschool children (aged 5-6) participated and were divided into two groups: 1) language delay bilingual (LD; N= 9) and 2) typically developing bilingual (TD; N= 10). Participants completed the story generation task pre- and post-intervention and underwent eight sessions of narrative intervention in their L1.Results: After eight sessions of narrative intervention in their L1, both the LD and TD groups showed statistically significant increases in their post-intervention story grammar (SG), number of different words (NDW), and mean length of utterance in words (MLUw) in L1. Additionally, both groups demonstrated statistically significant increases in their post-intervention SG and NDW in L2. A significant correlation was found between L1 MLUw and both L1/L2 expressive and receptive vocabulary. Furthermore, L1 expressive vocabulary skills were significantly correlated with their L2 post-intervention SG and MLUw.Conclusion: The findings of this study are significant, confirming that L1 narrative intervention not only significantly enhances L1 narrative skills (SG, NDW, and MLUw) but also L2 narrative skills (SG and NDW). Moreover, it identified positive correlations between L1 expressive vocabulary and L2 narrative performance post-intervention. These results underscore the crucial role of L1 intervention in supporting the development of L2 narrative skills, thereby highlighting the cross-linguistic association in Korean-English bilingual children.
Article
Full-text available
This article addresses two crucial questions: what are the language demands of cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT), and how can CBT be more accessible to people with language difficulties? These questions are examined through a case study of Developmental Language Disorder (DLD). With approximately 7% of children living with DLD, mental health practitioners have inevitably encountered someone with this condition. Many children with behavioural and emotional problems have unidentified language difficulties, increasing their risk of mental health problems. Language is the medium through which CBT is delivered, so exploring its language-based accessibility is of critical concern. This paper (i) describes a language framework, (ii) identifies the language skills required for CBT engagement, and (iii) outlines strategies to improve language-based accessibility. These considerations are conceptualised in the context of DLD, yet they apply to any neurodivergent population with language needs who may benefit from CBT.
Article
This meta‐analytic review explored the characteristics and effectiveness of combined language (e.g., vocabulary) and code (e.g., phonological awareness) interventions, including synergistic intervention effects for at‐risk preschoolers. Data from 29 randomized controlled trials, published before March 2023, reporting on 43 interventions, including 9333 children (4–6 years; 55% male, 45% African American, 30% Hispanic) were included in the meta‐analyses. Composite intervention effects were small: language ( g = 0.11) and code ( g = 0.23). Language and code outcomes were significantly related ( p = 0.032). Interventions equally targeting code and language subskills produced equivalent or greater code and language outcomes than those with an unequal emphasis. Implications for future combined intervention studies are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose The purpose of this randomized controlled trial was to examine the effects of a multitiered system of language support (MTSLS) on kindergarteners' narrative retelling, personal stories, writing, and expository language. Method Participants were 686 kindergarten students from four school districts in the United States. Twenty-eight classrooms were randomly assigned to treatment (n = 337 students) or control (n = 349 students) conditions. The treatment group received 14 weeks of oral narrative language instruction using Story Champs, a multitiered language program. Classroom teachers delivered large group (Tier 1) instruction for 15–20 min a day for 4 weeks. After this short-duration whole-class instruction, speech-language pathologists began small group Story Champs (Tier 2) intervention with a random sample of students who did not make adequate progress from the large group instruction (n = 49). These students received Tier 2 intervention for 20 min twice a week in addition to continued Tier 1 instruction. Results Results indicated that the students in the treatment group had significantly higher scores on all outcome measures compared to the students in the control group. Analyses of outcomes from the 49 students who received Tier 2 intervention compared to a matching sample of at-risk control students revealed that the treatment group had significantly higher scores on narrative retells, personal stories, and expository retells. When compared to matched average-performing and advanced-performing control peers, the students who received Tier 2 intervention had significantly higher narrative retell scores and no longer had significantly lower personal story, expository, or writing scores. Conclusion This effectiveness study demonstrated that MTSLS can lead to meaningful improvements in kindergarteners' oral and written language skills, even helping at-risk students catch up to high-achieving peers.
Poster
Full-text available
Learning to write is one of the essential literacy skills to academic success (Biancarosa & Snow, 2004; Graham & Perin, 2007). Early intervention to foster children’s early writing development can prevent a long-term adverse effect on students’ writing performance (Berninger, Nielsen, Abbott, Wijsman, Raskind, 2008). The writing standards indicated that Kindergarteners should be able to “narrate a single event or several loosely linked events, tell about the events in the order in which they occurred, and provide a reaction to what happened” (National Governors Association Center for Best Practices & Council of Chief State School Officers, 2010, p. 19). Although some studies examined the writing development of preschool or Kindergarten students, these studies heavily focus transcription skills, such as spelling and writing conventions (e.g., Campbell, Chen, Shenoy, Cunningham, 2018). Converging evidence gathered from meta-analyses suggested that only transcription interventions have been researched with first-grade students or younger (Graham, McKeown, Kiuhara, Harrir, 2012; Datchunk & Kubina, 2012; McMaster et al., 2017). Nevertheless, narrative writing is more than a transcribing process. A well-established narrative writing includes a setting, initiating event, action, climax, ending, and reaction (Stein & Glenn, 1979). Writers need more than transcription skills. They also need to have knowledge of narrative structure or story grammar to generate a quality narrative (Graham & Harris, 1989; Graves, Montague, & Wong, 1990; Stein & Glenn, 1979). Writing interventions that only focus on transcription skills are not sufficient for fostering beginning writers’ narrative writing skills. The goal of our study is to bridge this research gap by examining the effect of an oral narrative intervention on Kindergartners’ narrative writing. A multiple baseline design across groups was conducted to examine the relationship between the oral language intervention and narrative writing outcomes. Six kindergarten students were assigned in pairs to one of three groups. Each of the three groups experienced three conditions: baseline, intervention, and maintenance. The onset of the intervention phase was staggered for the three groups based on generally stable baseline performances of the research participants in the group. During the baseline condition, students did not receive oral narrative instruction. However, a writing sample was collected from each student on most days. On the days in which students were absent, a writing sample was not collected. Once the intervention condition began for a group, the students participated in six small group (4-5 students) oral narrative instructional sessions. Each session lasted 20-30 minutes and occurred three times a week for two weeks. During the intervention condition, students continued to produce writing samples. For students who were available, two writing samples produced 3-4 weeks following the last instructional session were used to document the extent to which writing improvements maintained.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Narrative skill, a child's ability to create a temporally sequenced account of an experience or event, is considered an important domain of children's language development. Narrative skill is strongly predictive of later language and literacy and is emphasized in curricula and educational standards. However, the need to transcribe a child's narrative and the lack of psychometrically justified scoring methods have precluded broad consideration of narrative skill among practitioners. We describe the development and validation of the Narrative Assessment Protocol-2 (NAP-2), an assessment of narrative skill for children ages 3–6 years, which uses event-based frequency scoring directly from a video recording of a child's narrative. Method The NAP-2 underwent a rigorous development process involving creation of four wordless picture books and associated scripts and identification of a broad item pool, including aspects of narrative microstructure and macrostructure. We collected two narratives from each of 470 children using the NAP-2 elicitation materials and scored each with the 60 items in the initial item pool. Results Cross-validated exploratory factor analyses indicated a single narrative skill factor. Rasch measurement analysis led to selection of 20 items that maintained high reliability while having good fit to the model and no evidence of differential item functioning across books and gender. Conclusions The NAP-2 offers a psychometrically sound and easy-to-use assessment of narrative skill for children ages 3–6 years. The NAP-2 is available freely online for use by speech-language pathologists, educational practitioners, and researchers. Supplemental Material https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.11800779
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this cluster randomized group study was to investigate the effect of multitiered, dual-language instruction on children’s oral language skills, including vocabulary, narrative retell, receptive and expressive language, and listening comprehension. The participants were 3- to 5-year-old children (n = 81) who were learning English and whose home language was Spanish. Across the school year, classroom teachers in the treatment group delivered large-group lessons in English to the whole class twice per week. For a Tier 2 intervention, the teachers delivered small-group lessons 4 days a week, alternating the language of intervention daily (first Spanish, then English). Group posttest differences were statistically significant, with moderate to large effect sizes favoring the treatment group on all the English proximal measures and on three of the four Spanish proximal measures. Treatment group advantages were observed on Spanish and English norm-referenced standardized measures of language (except vocabulary) and a distal measure of language comprehension.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Educators often use results from static norm-referenced vocabulary assessments to aid in the diagnosis of school-age children with a language disorder. However, research has indicated that many of these vocabulary assessments yield inaccurate, biased results, especially with culturally and linguistically diverse children. This study examined whether the use of a dynamic assessment of inferential word learning was more accurate at identifying bilingual (English/Spanish-speaking) children with a language disorder when compared to static measures of vocabulary. Method Thirty-one bilingual Spanish/English school-age children—21 children with typical language and 10 children with a language disorder—ages 5;9–9;7 (years;months) were administered traditional static vocabulary assessments and a dynamic assessment of inferential word learning that used a test–teach–test design. Results Discriminant analysis and logistic regression indicated that the combined posttest scores and modifiability ratings from the dynamic assessment generated 90%–100% sensitivity and 90.5%–95.2% specificity, which were superior to the static vocabulary tests. Conclusion These preliminary findings suggest that dynamic assessment of inferential word learning may be an effective method for accurately identifying diverse children with a language disorder.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose This article includes results from a multistate randomized controlled trial designed to investigate the impacts of a language-focused classroom intervention on primary grade students' proximal language skills and distal reading comprehension skills. Method The sample included 938 children from 160 classrooms in 4 geographic regions in the United States; each classroom was randomly assigned to 1 of 2 experimental conditions (2 variations of a language-focused intervention) or business-as-usual control. For this study, the 2 experimental conditions were collapsed, as they represented minor differences in the language-focused intervention. All children completed assessments at multiple time points during the academic year. Proximal measures (curriculum-aligned measures of vocabulary, comprehension monitoring, and understanding narrative and expository text) were administered throughout the school year. Distal measures of reading comprehension were administered at the beginning and the end of the school year. Results Multilevel multivariate regression was conducted with results showing that students receiving the language-focused intervention significantly outperformed those in the control group in comprehension monitoring and vocabulary, with effect sizes ranging from 0.55 to 1.98. A small effect in understanding text (narrative) was found in 3rd grade only. Multilevel path analyses were then conducted to examine if the intervention had a positive impact on reading comprehension through the influence of proximal language outcomes. In all 3 grades, instruction impacted reading comprehension via the mediation of vocabulary, with sizable effects (1.89–2.26); no other indirect pathways were significant. Conclusions This study provides evidence that a language-focused intervention can positively impact students' performance on language measures that are closely aligned with the intervention, with indirect, large effects on distal reading comprehension measures. Theoretically, this study provides causally interpretable support for the language bases of reading comprehension.
Article
This implementation study examined the efficacy and feasibility of a multi-tiered narrative intervention when Head Start teachers and teaching assistants delivered all of the interventions and progress monitoring probes. In six Head Start classrooms, 105 children were assigned to receive the intervention or to receive their typical instruction. Children who attended the three classrooms that implemented the multi-tiered intervention showed significantly better story retelling and language comprehension skills than the children who attended preschool in the control classrooms. Additional questions addressed the feasibility of a multi-tiered system in Head Start classrooms. Findings indicate that Head Start teachers and teaching assistants can deliver large group, small group, and individual lessons with adequate fidelity; they can administer brief progress monitoring probes with fidelity and score children’s retells reliably. Furthermore, as teachers and teaching assistants become more comfortable with the procedures, they report greater self-efficacy, and overall, greater feasibility of a multi-tiered model in their classrooms.
Article
Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) often develop the word reading skills necessary to read text but struggle with reading comprehension. Comprehension skills are often under addressed in early elementary settings leaving children with ASD at a disadvantage once they are expected to read for meaning. This study investigated the impact of an adapted story grammar intervention on the listening comprehension of five children with ASD in kindergarten through second grade. The intervention embedded evidence-based practices shown to support the learning of children with ASD during story grammar instruction. All participating children demonstrated increased correct responding to fact and inference questions following intervention. Data suggest that all participants required visuals to participate in and learn from the intervention. Social validity data indicate the classroom teacher perceived the intervention as helpful and feasible. Implications for instruction and future research are discussed.