ArticlePDF Available

Surrendering to social emptiness: Chronic social exclusion longitudinally predicts resignation in asylum seekers

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The current knowledge of the long‐term consequences of social exclusion mostly relies on theoretical assumptions. (Williams, 2009, Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol., 41, 275) hypothesized that chronic ostracism drives individuals into a stage of resignation (depression, alienation, unworthiness, helplessness). We focused on asylum seekers (N = 112) as a social group at risk of experiencing prolonged instances of exclusion. Applying a three‐wave longitudinal design with a three‐month interval between each wave, we sought to advance the knowledge of the temporal development of chronic social exclusion. Cross‐lagged panel analyses showed that social exclusion influenced the development of feelings of resignation in the long term, from baseline to six months and between three and six months. In the same time frame, the perception of social exclusion became stable and chronic. These findings provide empirical evidence that chronic exclusion predicts resignation and shed light on the temporal development of the detrimental impact that pervasive exclusion can have on people belonging to marginalized social groups.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Chronic social exclusion & the resignation stage
This is an original manuscript published by Wiley at the British Journal of Social Psychology on
the 4th of August 2020, available online at:
https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12410
Surrendering to Social Emptiness: Chronic social exclusion longitudinally predicts
resignation in asylum-seekers
Chronic social exclusion & the resignation stage
Marco Marinucci* & Paolo Riva
University of Milano-Bicocca
*Corresponding author information: Marco Marinucci, University of Milano-Bicocca, Department
of Psychology, Piazza Ateneo Nuovo, 1, 20126 Milano (Italy). e-mail:
m.marinucci@campus.unimib.it
Keywords:
Chronic social exclusion; resignation stage; asylum seekers; longitudinal study; cross-lagged panel
model.
Data availability statement:
The dataset supporting the findings and the analytic codes are publicly available at this anonymized link in
the OSF repository: https://osf.io/82z7u/?view_only=ff88e2e4c1134c0184bf0b0c98bfb519.
Acknowledgements:
This work was supported by a Grant from the Italian non-profit organization “Fondazione Roberto
Franceschi ONLUS” (grant number: 2017-NAZ-0176).
2
Abstract
The current knowledge of the long-term consequences of social exclusion mostly relies on theoretical
assumptions. Williams (2009) hypothesized that chronic ostracism drives individuals into a stage of
resignation (depression, alienation, unworthiness, helplessness). We focused on asylum-seekers
(N=112) as a social group at risk of experiencing prolonged instances of exclusion. Applying a three-
wave longitudinal design with a three-month interval between each wave, we sought to advance the
knowledge of the temporal development of chronic social exclusion. Cross-lagged panel analyses
showed that social exclusion influenced the development of feelings of resignation in the long term,
from baseline to six months and between three and six months. In the same time-frame, the perception
of social exclusion became stable and chronic. These findings provide empirical evidence that chronic
exclusion predicts resignation and shed light on the temporal development of the detrimental impact
that pervasive exclusion can have on people belonging to marginalized social groups.
3
Surrendering to Social Emptiness:
Chronic social exclusion longitudinally predicts resignation in asylum-seekers
Imagine that, on a hot day in August, while riding your bike along your usual route by the
seaside, you see a man lying on a mattress under a bridge and wrapped in a dirty blanket. After a few
days, you still see him there, so you decide to approach him and ask how he feels. You understand
that he can only speak Urdu, and you realize that he is a migrant who ended up somehow in that
remote countryside area. When you come back in December for the Winter break, you see that he is
still there; he has already spent four months in that remote area, isolated, unable to talk with anybody,
and continuously exposed to the indifference of the locals who rarely even pass by. This story, which
was witnessed by one of the authors (MM), speaks about a real-life persistent condition of social
exclusion, broadly defined as the “experience of being kept apart from others physically (e.g., social
isolation) or emotionally (e.g., being ignored or told that someone is not wanted)” (Riva & Eck, 2016,
p. ix).
Social exclusion has severe consequences for individuals’ health. Several studies showed that
social exclusion increases negative emotions, lowers self-esteem (Gerber & Wheeler, 2009), impairs
psychological well-being (Lieberman, 2013), is associated with infectious diseases, cardiovascular
and sleep problems (Aldridge et al., 2018; Cacioppo et al., 2002), and increases the risk of suicide
and mortality (Schinka et al., 2011; Rubin, 2017). The broad definition of social exclusion
encompasses a variety of adverse events in which people experience detachment and devaluation
from others. These events have been categorized into two categories of rejection- and ostracism-
based experiences (Riva & Eck, 2016). In episodes of rejection, individuals receive explicit negative
attention from others, suggesting that they are not wanted; when ostracized, people are ignored, and
they receive no attention from others (Wesselmann et al., 2016). Rejection can occur via
dehumanizing language (Haslam, 2006), stigmatization and discrimination (Richman et al., 2016),
4
and microaggressions (Sue et al., 2007). In contrast, ostracism can be detected when someone is
forgotten (King & Geise, 2011), when others avoid eye contact (Nezlek et al., 2012), when someone
is excluded from important information (Jones et al., 2009), when others speak in unknown foreign
languages (Dotan-Eliaz et al., 2009), and in cases where one is a target of the silent treatment
(Williams et al., 1998). Despite the many faces that social exclusion can take, rejection- and
ostracism-based instances of exclusion evoke similar negative emotions and threats in their victims
(Wesselman et al., 2019), highlighting the harmful consequences that social exclusion implies
regardless of whether it comes in extreme or subtle forms.
Approximately a decade ago, the publication of the Temporal Need-Threat Model of
Ostracism (Williams, 2009) pushed forward the conceptualization of the psychological consequences
of social exclusion.
The Temporal Need-Threat Model
Williams (2009) focused on the temporal development of individuals’ reactions to ostracism,
proposing a theoretical model that identified three sequential stages of responses. The first assumption
is that individuals are innately over-sensitive to detecting ostracism, given that it threatens humans’
survival, endangering group membership and access to group resources. Individuals quickly detect
being the target of ostracism, and in the first reflexive stage, they experience social pain and negative
emotions, signaling the severe threat they are exposed to. Specifically, ostracism threatens the need
to belong (Baumeister & Leary, 1995), the need to maintain positive self-esteem (Steele, 1988), the
need to perceive a sense of control over the environment (Burger, 1992), and the need to feel
recognized as meaningfully existing (Greenberg et al., 1986). After the painful reflexive experience,
individuals enter the reflective stage when they attempt to cope with the ostracism and recover the
satisfaction of their needs. People can react either with prosocial behaviors (e.g., social
compliance)evoking the urge to reconnect with others (Maner et al., 2007)or with antisocial
behaviors (e.g., aggression)prompting the desire to exert control over the environment. Ultimately,
if all of the coping strategies fail to end ostracism, individuals enter the resignation stage, where
5
ostracized individuals become resigned to their condition of persistent ostracism. The resources
necessary to fortify the threatened needs are depleted, belongingness evolves into alienation, self-
esteem into depression, control into learned helplessness, and the need for meaningful existence into
unworthiness (Williams, 2009).
Due to the ethical unfeasibility of inducing a persistent condition of social exclusion in a
controlled experimental procedure, the resignation stage has received the least empirical support
(Wesselmann & Williams, 2017).
Chronic social exclusion
The Temporal Need-Threat Model (Williams, 2009) is, therefore, the main theoretical
framework that allows the prediction of the temporal development of individuals’ responses to social
exclusion. As noted, Williams (2009) proposed the psychological withdrawal of the resignation stage
as the inescapable consequence of prolonged ostracism when people lose the expectation of and hope
for social acceptance and passively surrender to their marginalized condition. However, in focusing
on behavioral responses rather than psychological responses, another influential theoretical model
seems to align with this prediction. Indeed, Richman and Leary (2009) identified social withdrawal
as a unique behavior provoked by the perception of chronicity and the pervasiveness of rejection,
framing it as the only response that can prevent the experience of further pain deriving from
persistently occurring exclusion (Vangelisti et al., 2005). Resignation and withdrawal seem to be the
only possible outcomes of chronic social exclusion according to the most influential theoretical
models available. However, there are still two main issues concerning the study of chronic social
exclusion: the absence of a clear time reference and the ambiguity between the perception and
experience of social exclusion.
Lack of a clear time reference
The lack of a clear definition of the chronicity of social exclusion has contributed to hindering
the development of this literature. The abovementioned theoretical models do not provide a clear
temporal framework defining how long exclusion must last to display its implications. Indeed,
6
chronicity referring to social stressors has been framed without any temporal delimitation. Zhang and
colleagues (2017, p. 2) defined chronic social adversity as “repeatedly, continuously, or
accumulatively” occurring. Williams (2009, p. 302) considered “persistent” ostracism, and Richman
and Leary (2009, p. 370) framed the chronicity of social rejection as occurring “over a prolonged
period of time”. In contrast, disciplines from medical and clinical sciences more accurately defined
the temporal extension of chronic diseases. For example, chronic fatigue lasts for at least six months
(Wessely et al., 1996; Surawy et al., 1995), whereas chronic pain is defined as physical pain lasting
for at least three months (Merskey et al., 1979; Blyth et al., 2001).
Drawing on the pain overlap theory (Eisenberger & Lieberman, 2004; see also Riva et al.,
2014), Riva and colleagues (2017) applied the temporal framework of chronic physical pain to
chronic social pain, defining it as an experience of social exclusion lasting at least three months. The
authors presented quasi-experimental evidence showing that people reporting a three-month
experience of social exclusion presented higher resignation than control groups (Riva et al., 2017).
Beside it, to our knowledge, no study has explored the temporal onset of the chronicity of social
exclusion or the resignation stage.
Subjective vs. objective chronic social exclusion
Other issues concern the disentanglement between subjective and objective chronic social
exclusion and the overcoming of the inferential limits of cross-sectional methodologies. The
reciprocal intertwinement between negative evaluations about oneself and one’s social world and a
depressed mood, for instance, (e.g., Jankowski et al., 2018; Drevers & Raichle, 1998), do not allow
drawing inferences about the directionality of cross-sectional associations between self-reported
chronic exclusion and resignation. Thus, researchers need to apply methodologies pointing at causal
inference (e.g., longitudinal study) that can distinguish between the effect that perceived chronic
exclusion can have on the long-term resignation outcomes from the reciprocal influence that
resignation can play in generating long-term feelings of being rejected and ostracized. This is the
case, for example, of the longitudinal study by Martin and colleagues (2018) investigating the short-
7
and long-term effects of rejection from university sorority recruitment. The authors showed that the
negative implications for participants’ well-being caused by an acute episode of rejection could last
for three months.
Currently, we see only two feasible ways to deal with the issues mentioned above. The first
way implies the use of objective measures of chronic social exclusion, such as peer-reported indices
(e.g., Asher & Coie, 1990). Another way consists of sampling individuals belonging to marginalized
social groups that experience objective (definable a priori) real-life conditions of social exclusion
from society at large. Examples of these groups include homeless people (Hulme, 2000), prisoners
(Wesselmann et al., 2014), and immigrants (Marinucci & Riva, 2020).
Asylum seekers as a case study
Asylum-seekers can be considered a marginalized social group at risk of experiencing
persisting instances of social exclusion. As pointed out by Ager and Strang (2008), the social
exclusion of asylum-seeking immigrants begins at a foundational level, given the limited social, legal,
and citizenship rights they are given, which prevents them from fully participating in social and
cultural life (UN, 2016)for example, asylum-seekers in Italy have limited access to the educational
system (Decreto Legislativo n.140, 2005).
Asylum seekers also face several losses that imply the disruption of social connections and
relatedness with close connections (Bennett et al., 1997; Yakushko et al., 2008; Saldaña, 1992). They
can feel left out by people speaking a foreign language (Dotan-Eliaz et al., 2009; McDonald, 1995),
be excluded by community activity (Munton & Foster, 1990), and experience rejection alongside the
difficulty of building new social relationships (Ammons et al., 1982). Finally, due to their ethnic and
racial belonging, asylum seekers can face the daily hostility and discrimination of the majority,
conveyed via societal discrimination (e.g., hardly being given a job; Ager & Strang, 2008),
dehumanizing language (Esses et al., 2013), and microaggressions or direct aggression (Sue et al.,
2007; Fijalkowski, 1993).
8
For all of these reasons, in the current geopolitical situation in Europe, asylum-seekers can be
considered objectively marginalized from the broader society, and research considering the
perspective of asylum-seekers can offer opportunities to deepen the knowledge on the consequences
of chronic social exclusion and its psychological mechanisms.
The present study
The present study tested the causal assertion that the chronicity of perceived social exclusion
leads to a resignation stage (Williams, 2009). We focused on a sample of asylum-seekers as a social
group at risk of experiencing prolonged instances of ostracism and rejection (for similar reasoning,
see Aydin et al., 2010). Applying a three-wave longitudinal design with a three-month interval
between each wave, we conducted a series of cross-lagged panel models to 1) test the causal
prediction that perceived chronic social exclusion leads to resignation over time (controlling for the
potential reciprocal effect that resignation could exert on the later perception of social exclusion) and
2) study the time onset of the resignation stage
1
.
Method
Participants and procedure
We recruited 112 asylum-seeking male immigrants in three different hosting centers (CAS
Extraordinary Reception Centers) in Northern Italy. CAS were private corporations identified by the
regional Prefectures as suitable to host asylum-seekers waiting for their asylum claims to be
processed, thus supplying hosting capacity to the overwhelmed National Reception System. Indeed,
during 2017, CAS accounted for 78% of the national hosting capacity. The centers in which the data
collection took place mainly consisted of large facilities collectively hosting from 30 to 100 asylum-
seekers, even though CAS could also consist of networks of small apartments hosting 3-5 persons.
Officially, CAS were supposed to host immigrants temporarily, covering the six months expected for
1
The present study, longitudinally testing the chronic exclusion-resignation causal assumption,
importantly builds on our previous research preliminary investigating the exclusion-resignation link
at a cross-sectional level using the first wave of the present dataset (see Study 1, blind citation).
9
the courts to evaluate the asylum claims. However, due to the overwhelming immigration emergency,
the length of stay in CAS centers lasted up to two years (Villa, 2018). Within these centers,
immigrants could freely interact with each other, with the social workers leading the facilities, and
with the local community, even though they were subject to mobility restrictions (e.g., the obligation
to return to the center by night). The services provided by the CAS aimed at meeting asylum-seekers’
physical basic needs (e.g., food, shelter, and medical assistance), extending to more articulated
integration programs depending on the corporations’ resources (e.g., legal assistance, job placement,
and educational programs).
Following the approval of the University Ethical Committee, data were collected using a
paper-and-pencil self-reported questionnaire during scheduled group meetings within the hosting
centers. Based on the official languages of participants’ countries of origin, the questionnaires were
available in English, French, and Italian (bilingual native speakers reviewed the translations).
Moreover, cultural-linguistic mediators supported the data collection providing, when needed,
explanation of the questionnaire considering cultural differences and linguistic meanings. Participants
could choose to complete the questionnaire in their home country official language (i.e., English or
French), or in the language in which they mostly felt proficient (e.g., Italian for the long-term asylum-
seekers whose home country language was not available). Participants received a payment of 15
Euros at the end of data collection for their commitment to the research.
At the first wave of data collection, participants were 29.9 years old (SD=6.5) and they had
undergone 10.2 years of full-time education (SD=4.1) in their countries of origin. Most of the
participants (86.9%) came from English and French speaking Western-African Countries (in order:
Nigeria, Gambia, Ivory Coast, Guinea, Mali, Sierra Leone, Ghana, Senegal, Cameroon, Benin,
Liberia, and Burkina Faso), 9% from Pakistan and Bangladesh, and the remaining 4.1% from other
African countries (Egypt and Somalia). Participants had been staying in Italy for 15.1 months
(SD=8.2) and 87.5% had reached Italy crossing the Mediterranean Sea (5.4% by land border, 7.1%
other or unknown). The great majority of them were single (75,9%), 15.2% were married (10.7% of
10
them living apart from their wives), and 7.2% were widowed, divorced, or separated. Overall, 94.6%
of the participants had their asylum claim pending, whereas 5.4% (n=6) already held a status of
international protection. Each of the waves of data collection occurred approximately after a three-
months interval (lagWave1-2=100.66 days, SD=9.79, lagWave2-3=102.16 days, SD=6.75). In all, of the
112 participants at Wave 1, 89 (79.46%) participated at Wave 2, and 70 (62.5%) at Wave 3.
Measures
Predictor: Social Exclusion. The self-reported index of social exclusion was based on the
literature categorizing the various forms of daily life social exclusion into rejection- and ostracism-
based experiences (Riva & Eck, 2016; Wesselmann & Williams, 2017). The index focused on the
two main categories of social exclusion and was computed averaging two items assessing the daily
feelings of being rejected and ignored (“I feel rejected,” “I feel ignored”). The index was measured
on a 5-point Likert scale, with higher score indicating higher feelings of exclusion (range = 1-5).
Outcome: Resignation. As in previous studies (Riva et al., 2017; blind citation), the index of
resignation stage was computed averaging a series of items that tapped into alienation, depression,
unworthiness, and helplessness (see the Appendix for the complete list of items). Alienation was
measured with eight items from the Social Connectedness Scale (Lee & Robbins, 1995). To measure
depression, we included eleven items from the Symptom Checklist-90 (Revised) Depression
subscale (Derogatis & Unger, 2010). Feelings of unworthiness were measured with five items from
the Rosemberg Self-Esteem scale (Rosenberg, 1965). Lastly, helplessness was measured with four
items from the Beck Hopelessness Scale (Beck et al., 1974). Participants were instructed to answer
the items thinking about how they felt or thought in the previous three months on a 5-point response
scale (range=1-5). Table 1 summarizes the descriptive statistics and correlations between the
variables.
Sociodemographic information. Participants responded to sociodemographic questions
asking their gender, age, years of full-time education, nationality, religion, marital status, length of
their stay in Italy and how they reached Italy, and the status of their asylum claim.
11
Statistical Analyses
We conducted a series of cross-lagged panel models (CLPMs) within a SEM approach that
allowed us to compare the different models using RStudio (RStudio Team, 2016) and the package
“lavaan” (Rosseel, 2012). Missing data were handled using the full-information maximum likelihood
estimation that provides a more reliable estimation than traditional approaches (Schafer & Graham,
2002). No data were excluded from the analyses. Because of non-normality, we used robust “Huber-
White” standard error (Freedman, 2006). CLPM is a widely used statistical technique that can “lend
support to a causal claim” because of the temporal order of the considered variables. (Selig & Little,
2012, p. 271). Employing CLPMs, we tested the effect from previous perceived social exclusion to
future resignation, while considering the reverse path from the previous level of resignation to future
perceptions of chronic exclusion, the autoregressive paths, and covariation between exclusion and
resignation (Cole & Maxwell, 2003). The autoregressive paths and both the forward and reciprocal
causation cross-lagged paths were specified between Wave1 and Wave2/Wave3 and between Wave2
and Wave3 (Finkel, 1995; Reitz et al., 2016; Reitz et al., 2015). This full specification of the model
allowed us to comprehensively study the development of the relationship between exclusion and
resignation, focusing both on the short- and long-term cross-lagged effects (Hawkley et al., 2010; see
also Konradt & Eckardt, 2016). We started performing a) a baseline model specifying the
autoregressive and correlation paths, and we proceeded comparing the baseline model with larger
model additionally estimating b) the forward causation paths (i.e., perceived exclusion predicts future
resignation), c) the reverse causation paths (i.e., resignation predicts future perceived exclusion), and
d) the reciprocal causation paths. After the identification of the best fitting model, we proceeded to
test progressively nested models with equality constrains to identify the best fitting and most
parsimonious model. We assessed the model fit comparing the RMSEA (acceptable below 0.08), the
CFI (acceptable above 0.90), the SRMR (acceptable below 0.07), and the chi-square difference test
(Hu & Bentler, 1999; Marsh et al., 2005). Finally, to ensure an adequate sample size categorized as
medium by the Kline’s (2005) N ≥ 100 guideline –, we managed to present a final statistical model
12
respecting the rule-of-thumb setting the minimum ratio observations/parameters to 5 (Bentler &
Chou, 1987; Kline, 2011). Open data and analysis code can be found at this link
https://osf.io/82z7u/?view_only=ff88e2e4c1134c0184bf0b0c98bfb519.
Results
Table 2 shows the goodness of fit of the CLPMs tested and the model comparison. Overall,
we observed that the forward (b) and the reverse causation (c) models showed a better fit than the
baseline model (a), and the just-identified reciprocal causation model (d) had the best fit. These results
indicated that the perception of chronic social exclusion and resignation reciprocally influenced each
other over time. From model (d), we tested additional models progressively adding equality
constraints to identify the most parsimonious and best-fitting model. Results led to the identification
of model (i) that showed a good fit according to all the indicators. In model (i), non-significant paths
were constrained to zero, and the resignation autoregressive paths, as well as the exclusion-causation
paths, were constrained to be equal (Figure 1).
The final model highlighted a complex relationship between perceived chronic exclusion and
the resignation stage, whose reciprocal influence over time varied across the two time-lags. The two
constructs also differed in their stationarity. As highlighted in the model comparison (f vs. e), the
resignation stage was stable in both the time-lags.
Differently, social exclusion became stable in the long-run. Exclusion at six months (Wave 3)
was predicted by exclusion at the first measurement (Wave 1) and after three months (Wave 2),
whereas exclusion at the first measurement (Wave 1) did not influence the perception of exclusion at
three months (Wave 2).
These results suggest that the most appropriate time frame to consider chronic experiences of
social exclusion is six months. According to our data, six months seems to be the time interval
required to recurring episodes of ostracism and rejection to be perceived as chronic and stable over
time. Considering that exclusion at Wave 1 did not predict exclusion at Wave 2, a time-frame of three
13
months did not seem to be sufficiently long for individuals to perceive the occurring social exclusion
as stable hence chronic over time.
This pattern of results is similar to the cross-lagged paths highlighting the relation between
early exclusion on later resignation. Controlling for autoregressive paths, exclusion at the first
measurement (Wave 1) and after three months (Wave 2) predicted resignation at six months (Wave
3), whereas resignation at three months (Wave 2) was not influenced by previous exclusion (Wave
1).
These results can indicate that the development of the resignation stage originated from
persistent social exclusion occurs in a six-months interval. Accordingly, the non-significant path from
baseline exclusion to three-months resignation would indicate that an initial time-lag of three months
may be insufficient for the negative consequences of social exclusion to stably develop. Instead,
social exclusion would influence the development of the resignation stage in six months, when the
final resignation level (Wave 3) would depend on the exclusion perceived during the previous three
and six months.
Regarding the reverse causation path (from previous resignation to future perception of
exclusion), results showed that only the baseline levels of resignation influenced in the short-term (in
the three months between Wave 1 and Wave 2) the perception of social exclusion, whereas the
perception of social exclusion at six months was predicted neither by baseline nor by three-months
preceding resignation. These results can indicate that at three months baseline resignation can still
influence the perception of social exclusion; differently, when at six months the perception of social
exclusion has become chronic, it mainly derives from previous perceptions of exclusion whereas the
influence of early resignation becomes no more detectable.
To sum up, results indicated a reciprocal predictive influence between perceived social
exclusion and resignation, but, whereas the reverse effect of resignation on future perception of
exclusion occurred in the first three-month lag as a short-term cross-lagged effect , the impact of
14
exclusion on future resignation as well as the perception of chronicity of the exclusion developed in
six months as a long-term effect.
General Discussion
The present research investigated the long-term consequences of chronic social exclusion.
Whereas the existing literature identifies psychological resignation as the sole outcome of chronic
social exclusion (Williams, 2009), it has not provided insight concerning the time frame development
of the associations between chronic exclusion and resignation outcomes. With the present study, we
aimed to fill this gap by conducting a three-way longitudinal study testing whether persistent social
exclusion leads to resignation. Most importantly, we took into account the timeframe required for this
process to develop. We focused on a sample of asylum-seekers, considering the high chances of
individuals belonging to this social group experiencing chronic exclusion.
Our results supported Williams’ (2009) theoretical assumption that people experiencing
persistent forms of social exclusion enter the resignation stage, characterized by feelings of alienation,
depression, unworthiness, and helplessness. Moreover, we observed that this process requires a six-
month interval to develop, as do persistent episodes of social exclusion to be perceived as chronic
and stable over time. Given their longitudinal design and the specific population they draw on, these
findings inform the current knowledge about the long-term consequences of interpersonal exclusion
from different standpoints.
First, the present study provided the first longitudinal empirical causal evidence that chronic
exclusion leads to resignation. Indeed, previous studies have only spoken about the long-lasting sting
of acute rejection episodes (e.g., Martin et al., 2018). Although, as recommended by Selig and Little
(2012), arguments favoring causal relationships based on cross-lagged panel analyses should be
drawn with care, the present findings are strengthened and supported by their conjunction with a)
converging and independent theoretical predictions from the main psychosocial models on
interpersonal devaluationpointing at psychological and behavioral withdrawal as the direct
outcome of chronic exclusion (Williams, 2009; Richman & Leary, 2009)b) qualitative (Zadro,
15
2004), cross-sectional (Marinucci & Riva, 2020), and quasi-experimental (Riva et al., 2017)
preliminary evidence of the exclusion-resignation link, and c) converging results coming from
conceptually related scientific fields. Relatedly, Van Zalk and Smith (2019), surveying a group of
long-term homeless people, found that perceived ostracism was associated with Williams’ (2009)
need-threat. Additionally, the sociometric literature on school-aged children and adolescents showed
that chronic peer rejection longitudinally predicted depressive symptoms, social withdrawal and
anxiety in later stages of life (e.g., Prinstein & Aikins, 2004; Ollendick et al., 1992). Additionally,
the literature from discrimination highlighted the negative long-term implications for psychological
health and well-being that these instances of relational devaluation can have in different contexts
(Schmitt et al., 2014; Pascoe & Richman, 2009), such as gender-based workplace discrimination
(Pavalko et al., 2003), the stigma experiences of people with mental illness (Ilic et al., 2013), ethnic-
based discrimination in school (Benner & Kim, 2009), and personal everyday discrimination in an
urban context (Schulz et al., 2006). Moreover, in an experimental study, Goodwin and colleagues
(2010) found that attributing ostracism to racism impeded the recovery of basic needs. Thus, although
the present findings do not allow us to make a conclusive claim of causal evidence, their alignment
with theoretical models, preliminary evidence, and convergent results from related fields support the
argument in favor of a causal exclusion-resignation link.
Second, the present findings identified six months as the interval that persistent episodes of
social exclusion require to be perceived as chronic and to influence the onset of the resignation stage.
To our knowledge, this is the first study that has tried to investigate the temporal development of the
chronicity of social exclusion, setting the criterion defining how long instances of social exclusion
must last to be perceived as chronic and stable and to display their negative implications on
individuals’ psychological well-being as six months. However, these results must be considered with
care, and further studies are needed to confirm or challenge the timeframe we identified. Indeed, the
six-month criterion we proposed as the temporal development of the resignation stage and the
perception of chronicity of social exclusion may be specific for the life condition and experience of
16
asylum-seeking immigrants, and future research should test whether the causal exclusion-resignation
link and its temporal development can be replicated in the general population as well as in other
marginalized social groups. Among others, stateless people those who are not recognized as citizens
by any state can be a particularly vulnerable group whose inherent condition of social exclusion
could greatly aggravate the risk to enter the resignation stage. The stateless condition puts people
under the severe psychological threat of not having a legal identity, which adds to the heightened risk
of being victims of human rights violations (Riley et al., 2017; Lewa, 2009). This unsafe condition
has been shown to aggravate the psychological impact of daily stressors, like being rejected or
disrespected (Riley et al., 2017). Based on these premises, future research could focus on the
resignation stage as a negative health outcome potentially harming stateless people, also looking at
how being stateless could influence the onset of resignation. Future research should account for other
unconsidered factors that could reduce or extend the developmental time-frame of the resignation
stage. Indeed, individual and personality differences can modulate the onset of the resignation stage.
For example, existing post-traumatic symptomatology and other mental disorders commonly
observed among asylum-seekers and refugees (Fazel et al., 2005) could heighten the perception and
sensitivity to social exclusion (Jobst et al., 2015; Jankowski et al., 2018), potentially shortening the
developmental interval of the resignation stage in vulnerable asylum-seekers. Oppositely, factors
such as resilient personality traits, as well as adaptive coping strategies, protective socio-demographic
conditions, and supportive relations could delay the development of the resignation stage, and perhaps
even prevent it (Waldeck et al., 2015; Lavie-Ajayi & Slonim-Nevo, 2017).
Third, these findings provide information about the psychological consequences induced by
chronic social exclusion in asylum seekers, a group that is the target of marginalization and relational
devaluation phenomena (e.g., Ager & Strang, 2008). In doing so, we extended the investigation of
individuals’ reactions to social exclusion to a non-WEIRD population (“Western, educated,
industrialized, rich and democratic”; Henrich et al., 2010a, p. 29), and this allowed us not only to
further validate Williams’ (2009) hypothesized model on the WEIRD social groups the model drew
17
on but also to consider it as a heuristic theoretical map that describes the processes that all humans
undergo when facing chronic exclusion, pointing at early, preliminary evidence of a large-scale
generalization. Nevertheless, the present findings provided evidence of the exclusion-resignation
causal link only on a specific subgroup of marginalized population: male, young adult, asylum-
seeking immigrants mainly coming from Western-Africa. Although this socio-demographic
background seems in line with the average asylum-seekers entering Italy whose prototype is a single
man younger than 25 coming from Western-Africa by the Mediterranean Sea (Cittalia et al., 2019)
, future research focusing on people with different socio-demographic characteristics (e.g., women,
non-immigrants, different ages and home countries) is needed for a better investigation of the
exclusion-resignation link.
Moreover, the investigation of the consequences of chronic interpersonal exclusion in a
chronically marginalized social group allowed us to bring together the interpersonal and intergroup
levels of analysis. Although we focused on the individual-level psychological consequences derived
from an unmet need to belong and feel connected to others (i.e., social exclusion; Baumeister &
Leary, 2005; Leary, 2001), the fact that we tried to answer this research question in group-level
marginalized individuals acknowledged the fact that such interpersonal exclusions occur not occur in
a “social vacuum” (Schmitt & Branscombe, 2002, p. 191) but rather in a context ruled by intergroup
dynamics. Thus, the perception of social exclusion that asylum-seeking participants self-reported in
this study could become more meaningful and interpretable in light of the objective condition of
social exclusion and inability to fully participate in the social life that they experience daily due to
numerous factors, such as a lack of full and equal social rights and citizenship as well as cultural
barriers (Ager & Strang, 2008), relocation stressors (Bennett et al., 1997), and the subtle or explicit
devaluation of the majority group (Sue et al., 2007; Esses et al., 2013). If the intersection of the
interpersonal and intergroup focuses allows contextualizing asylum-seekers’ experience of social
exclusion, it also brings further complexity to the understanding of this societal issue. Our data
showed that, despite their marginalized societal condition, participants’ average perception of
18
interpersonal exclusion was fairly low (see table 1), which could be accounted for by two main
reasons. First, a series of intervening factors can play a role in buffering asylum-seekers’ perception
of social exclusion in their current daily life. For example, while they could be victims of racist
aggression whether in its extreme or subtle forms (i.e., microaggression; Sue et al., 2007) and
discrimination from the local communities (Hersch, 2011; Pereira et al., 2010), at the same time, they
can be also hosted in welcoming environments, encounter social workers that care about them and
seek to promote their well-being as well as their civil and social integration (Nash et al., 2006). While
struggling with post-migration stressors (e.g., James et al., 2019), immigrants can develop supporting
relations with locals counteracting their perceived exclusion and its negative consequences (e.g.,
Marinucci & Riva, 2020). Thus, interpersonal factors could buffer asylum-seekers’ against their
condition of societal marginalization, leading to a lowered perception of interpersonal exclusion.
Future research should pay attention to intervening factors at the interpersonal level that can interact
with marginalized populations’ societal exclusion, aggravating or softening their perceived social
exclusion. Second, it is possible that the low scores on perceived social exclusion are linked with the
measure of social exclusion we adopted. The self-reported measure of social exclusion that we
adopted could not be sensitive enough to account for the complexity of the experience of pervasive
social exclusion asylum-seekers face. As detailed in the introduction, asylum-seekers can experience
social exclusion for a wide range of reasons (e.g., the disruption of existing connections, linguistic
exclusion, societal discrimination), and a two-item self-reported index might fail to capture the
overall experience of immigrants’ exclusion. As a general consideration, the assessment of daily and
persistent social exclusion is to date still an open methodological issue. Indeed, to our knowledge,
the investigation of pervasive social exclusion in real-life ecological settings exclusively relies on
self-reported measures, and, if a validate scale exists for measuring pervasive ostracism in adolescents
and the general population (see Gilman et al., 2013), future research should develop an appropriate
measure of pervasive rejection and ostracism suitable for marginalized populations.
19
Highlighting the severe negative repercussions that social exclusion can have on asylum-
seekers, the present study calls on policymakers and social workers to implement interventions
counteracting the implications of asylum-seekers’ chronic exclusion. Programs such as “The
Friendship Bench” (Chibanda et al., 2015)—aimed at improving individuals’ well-being by
promoting community belongingness via counselling sessions delivered by lay health workers in
public benchescould be applied to identify those marginalized individuals most vulnerable to
persistent exclusion, preventing its negative consequences by fostering belongingness to local
communities.
The present research calls for future studies to deepen the knowledge of the long-term
implications of social exclusion and to overcome the limits of the present study. Researchers should
replicate the presented exclusion-resignation causal link in another sample of immigrants, in the
general population, or in a sample of other marginalized social groups, such as homeless people,
prisoners, or stateless people. Researchers can consider the development of more detailed self-report
or other reported measures of chronic social exclusion to increase accuracy in measuring this
construct. Moreover, future studies should investigate other potential negative outcomes of persistent
social exclusion, considering detrimental behavioral responses, such as behavioral withdrawal,
substance use, or aggressive behaviors. In doing so, future research could consider the usage of more
sophisticated methodologies that allow a detailed assessment of behavioral reactions to social
exclusion in concomitance with the occurrence of episodes of exclusion (e.g., the Ecological
Momentary Assessment). With this type of procedures, researchers could also be able to understand
whether prosocial, aggressive, or reclusive behaviors are more likely to occur after social exclusion
as coping strategies to fortify the threatened fundamental needs. Future studies could also try to
identify the individual, social, and situational variables that can moderate the time-frame of the
development of the resignation stage, focusing on both protective factors counteracting the
development of the resignation stage such as positive social connections with the native majority
(Marinucci & Riva, 2020) , and risk factors accelerating its onset. Lastly, future studies need to
20
acknowledge potential undetected cultural differences that may have affected the presented results
(Henrich et al., 2010b).
Conclusion
In this study focusing on a marginalized social group, we sought to advance the current
knowledge of the temporal development of chronic social exclusion. Considering a sample of asylum
seekers, we provided longitudinal evidence that the perception of the chronicity of social exclusion
and its evolution into the resignation stage develop in six months. These results, providing causal
evidence that chronic exclusion relegates people into a condition of helpless surrender to social
devaluation, call for future research aimed at further exploring the development of the repercussions
of chronic exclusion and how we can prevent them from happening.
21
References
Aldridge, R. W., Story, A., Hwang, S. W., Nordentoft, M., Luchenski, S. A., Hartwell, G., ...
& Hayward, A. C. (2018). Morbidity and mortality in homeless individuals, prisoners, sex workers,
and individuals with substance use disorders in high-income countries: a systematic review and meta-
analysis. The Lancet, 391(10117), 24-250. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31869-X.
Asher, S. R., & Coie, J. D. (1990). Peer rejection in childhood. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Aydin, N., Fischer, P., & Frey, D. (2010). Turning to God in the face of ostracism: Effects of
social exclusion on religiousness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(6), 742-753. doi:
10.1177/0146167210367491.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497529. doi:
10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497.
Beck, A. T., Weissman, A., Lester, D., & Trexler, L. (1974). The measurement of pessimism:
the hopelessness scale. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 42(6), 861. doi:
10.1037/h0037562.
Benner, A. D., & Kim, S. Y. (2009). Experiences of discrimination among Chinese American
adolescents and the consequences for socioemotional and academic development. Developmental
psychology, 45(6), 16821694. doi:10.1037/a0016119
Bennett, H., Boshoff, A., & Rigby, C. (1997). The relationship between tenure, stress and
coping strategies of South African immigrants to New Zealand. South African Journal of
Psychology, 27(3), 160-165. doi: 10.1177/008124639702700305.
Bentler, P. M., & Chou, C. P. (1987). Practical issues in structural modeling. Sociological
Methods & Research, 16(1), 78-117. doi: 10.1177/0049124187016001004.
22
Blyth, F. M., March, L. M., Brnabic, A. J., Jorm, L. R., Williamson, M., & Cousins, M. J.
(2001). Chronic pain in Australia: a prevalence study. Pain, 89(2-3), 127-134. doi: 10.1016/S0304-
3959(00)00355-9.
Burger, J. M. (1992). Desire for control: Personality, social and clinical perspectives. New
York: Plenum. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4757-9984-2.
Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Crawford, L. E., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, M. H., Kowalewski,
R. B., Malarkey, W. B., Van Cauter, E., & Berntson, G. G. (2002). Loneliness and health: Potential
mechanisms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64(3), 407-417. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200205000-00005
Chibanda, D., Bowers, T., Verhey, R., Rusakaniko, S., Abas, M., Weiss, H. A., & Araya, R.
(2015). The Friendship Bench programme: a cluster randomised controlled trial of a brief
psychological intervention for common mental disorders delivered by lay health workers in
Zimbabwe. International journal of mental health systems, 9(1), 21. doi: 10.1186/s13033-015-0013-
y.
Cittalia, Fondazione Anci, & Ministero dell’Interno (2019). Atlante SPRAR/SIPROIMI 2018.
Rapporto annuale SPRAR/SIPROIMI 2018. Sistema di protezione per titolari di protezione
internazionale e per minori stranieri non accompagnati. Available at https://www.sprar.it/wp-
content/uploads/2019/11/Atlante-Sprar-Siproimi-2018-leggero.pdf,
Cole, D. A, & Maxwell, S. E. (2003). Testing mediational models with longitudinal data:
Questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112,
558577. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.558.
Decreto Legislativo n.140 (30 Maggio 2005). "Attuazione della direttiva 2003/9/CE che
stabilisce norme minime relative all'accoglienza dei richiedenti asilo negli Stati membri".
http://www.parlamento.it/parlam/leggi/deleghe/05140dl.htm
Derogatis, L. R., & Unger, R. (2010). Symptom checklist‐90‐revised. The Corsini
Encyclopedia of Psychology, 1-2. doi: 10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0970.
23
Dotan-Eliaz, O., Sommer, K. L., & Rubin, Y. S. (2009). Multilingual groups: Effects of
linguistic ostracism on felt rejection and anger, coworker attraction, perceived team potency, and
creative performance. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 31(4), 363-375. doi:
10.1080/01973530903317177.
Drevets, W. C., & Raichle, M. E. (1998). Reciprocal suppression of regional cerebral blood
flow during emotional versus higher cognitive processes: Implications for interactions between
emotion and cognition. Cognition and emotion, 12(3), 353-385. doi: 10.1080/026999398379646.
Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts: a common neural alarm
system for physical and social pain. Trends in cognitive sciences, 8(7), 294-300. doi:
10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.010.
Esses, V. M., Medianu, S., & Lawson, A. S. (2013). Uncertainty, threat, and the role of the
media in promoting the dehumanization of immigrants and refugees. Journal of Social Issues, 69(3),
518-536. doi: 10.1111/josi.12027.
Fazel, M., Wheeler, J., & Danesh, J. (2005). Prevalence of serious mental disorder in 7000
refugees resettled in western countries: a systematic review. The Lancet, 365(9467), 1309-1314. doi:
10.1016/S0140-6736(05)61027-6.
Fijalkowski, J. (1993). Aggressive nationalism, immigration pressure and asylum policy
disputes in contemporary Germany. International Migration Review, 27(4), 850-869. doi:
10.1177/019791839302700406.
Finkel, S. E. (1995). Causal analysis with panel data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
https://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781412983594
Freedman, D. A. (2006). On the so-called “Huber sandwich estimator” and “robust standard
errors”. The American Statistician, 60(4), 299-302. https://doi.org/10.1198/000313006X152207.
Gerber, J., & Wheeler, L. (2009). On being rejected: A meta-analysis of experimental research
on rejection. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(5), 468-488. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-
6924.2009.01158.x.
24
Gilman, R., Carter-Sowell, A., DeWall, C. N., Adams, R. E., & Carboni, I. (2013). Validation
of the ostracism experience scale for adolescents. Psychological Assessment, 25(2), 319.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030913.
Goodwin, S. A., Williams, K. D., & Carter-Sowell, A. R. (2010). The psychological sting of
stigma: The costs of attributing ostracism to racism. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 46(4), 612-618. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.02.002.
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1986). The causes and consequences of the
need for self-esteem: A terror management theory. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private
self (pp. 189-212). New York: Springer-Verlag. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9564-5_10.
Haslam, N. (2006). Dehumanization: An integrative review. Personality and social
psychology review, 10(3), 252-264. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_4.
Hawkley, L. C., Thisted, R. A., Masi, C. M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness predicts
increased blood pressure: 5-year cross-lagged analyses in middle-aged and older adults. Psychology
and aging, 25(1), 132141. doi:10.1037/a0017805
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010a). Most people are not
WEIRD. Nature, 466(7302), 29-29. doi: 10.1038/466029a
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010b). The weirdest people in the
world?. Behavioral and brain sciences, 33(2-3), 61-83. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
Hersch, J. (2011). The persistence of skin color discrimination for immigrants. Social Science
Research, 40(5), 1337-1349. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2010.12.006
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure
analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 155.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
Hulme, T. (2000). Societies ostracized: The homeless. Unpublished honors thesis, Macquarie
University, Sydney
25
Ilic, M., Reinecke, J., Bohner, G., Röttgers, H. O., Beblo, T., Driessen, M., ... & Corrigan, P.
W. (2013). Belittled, avoided, ignored, denied: Assessing forms and consequences of stigma
experiences of people with mental illness. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 35(1), 31-40.
https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2012.746619.
James, P., Iyer, A., & Webb, T. L. (2019). The impact of post‐migration stressors on refugees’
emotional distress and health: A longitudinal analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 49(7),
1359-1367. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2589.
Jankowski, K. F., Batres, J., Scott, H., Smyda, G., Pfeifer, J. H., & Quevedo, K. (2018).
Feeling left out: depressed adolescents may atypically recruit emotional salience and regulation
networks during social exclusion. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 13(8), 863-876.
https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsy055.
Jobst, A., Sabass, L., Palagyi, A., Bauriedl-Schmidt, C., Mauer, M. C., Sarubin, N., ... &
Padberg, F. (2015). Effects of social exclusion on emotions and oxytocin and cortisol levels in
patients with chronic depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 60, 170-177.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.11.001.
Jones, E. E., Carter-Sowell, A. R., Kelly, J. R., & Williams, K. D. (2009). I'm out of the loop':
Ostracism through information exclusion. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 12(2), 157-174.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430208101054.
King, L. A., & Geise, A. C. (2011). Being forgotten: Implications for the experience of
meaning in life. The Journal of Social Psychology, 151(6), 696-709.
doi:10.1080/00224545.2010.522620
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (Second ed.).
New York: Guilford.
Kline, R. B. (2011). Methodology in the Social Sciences. Principles and practice of structural
equation modeling (3rd ed.). Guilford Press.
26
Konradt, U., & Eckardt, G. (2016). Short-term and long-term relationships between reflection
and performance in teams: evidence from a four-wave longitudinal study. European Journal of Work
and Organizational Psychology, 25(6), 804-818. https://doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2016.1160058.
Lavie-Ajayi, M., & Slonim-Nevo, V. (2017). A qualitative study of resilience among asylum
seekers from Darfur in Israel. Qualitative Social Work, 16(6), 825-841.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1473325016649256.
Leary, M. R. (Ed.). (2001). Interpersonal rejection. Oxford University Press.
Lee, R. M., & Robbins, S. B. (1995). Measuring belongingness: The social connectedness and
the social assurance scales. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 42(2), 232. doi: 10.1037/0022-
0167.42.2.232.
Lewa, C. (2009). North Arakan: an open prison for the Rohingya in Burma. Forced Migration
Review, (32), 11.
Lieberman, M. D. (2013). Social: Why our brains are wired to connect. Oxford University
Press: Oxford, UK.
Maner, J. K., DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., & Schaller, M. (2007). Does social exclusion
motivate interpersonal reconnection? Resolving the" porcupine problem.". Journal of personality and
social psychology, 92(1), 42. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.42.
Marinucci, M., & Riva, P. (2020). How intergroup social connections shape immigrants’
responses to social exclusion. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430219894620
Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., & Grayson, D. (2005). Goodness of fit in structural equation
models. In A. Maydeu-Olivares & J. J. McArdle (Eds.), Contemporary psychometrics: A festschrift
for Roderick P. Mcdonald (pp. 275340). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Martin, J. L., Smart Richman, L., & Leary, M. R. (2018). A lasting sting: Examining the short-
term and long-term effects of real-life group rejection. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations,
21(8), 11091124. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430217695443
27
McDonald, J. (1995). Entitled to Learn? A Report on Young Refugees’ Experiences of Access
and Provision in the UK Education System. London: World University Service.
Merskey, H., Albe-Fessard, D. G., Bonica, J. J., Carmen, A., Dubner, R., Kerr, F. W. L., ... &
Pagni, C. A. (1979). IASP sub-committee on taxonomy. Pain, 6(3), 249-252.
Nash, M., Wong, J., & Trlin, A. (2006). Civic and social integration: A new field of social
work practice with immigrants, refugees and asylum seekers. International Social Work, 49(3), 345-
363. https://doi.org/10.1177/0020872806063407.
Nezlek, J. B., Wesselmann, E. D., Wheeler, L., & Williams, K. D. (2012). Ostracism in
everyday life. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 16(2), 91.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028029.
Ollendick, T. H., Weist, M. D., Borden, M. C., & Greene, R. W. (1992). Sociometric status
and academic, behavioral, and psychological adjustment: a five-year longitudinal study. Journal of
consulting and clinical psychology, 60(1), 80. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.60.1.80.
Pascoe, E. A., & Richman, L. S. (2009). Perceived discrimination and health: a meta-analytic
review. Psychological bulletin, 135(4), 531. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016059.
Pavalko, E. K., Mossakowski, K. N., & Hamilton, V. J. (2003). Does perceived discrimination
affect health? Longitudinal relationships between work discrimination and women's physical and
emotional health. Journal of Health and social Behavior, 18-33. https://doi.org/10.2307/1519813.
Pereira, C., Vala, J., & Costa‐Lopes, R. (2010). From prejudice to discrimination: The
legitimizing role of perceived threat in discrimination against immigrants. European Journal of
Social Psychology, 40(7), 1231-1250. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.718.
Prinstein, M. J., & Aikins, J. W. (2004). Cognitive moderators of the longitudinal association
between peer rejection and adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal of abnormal child
psychology, 32(2), 147-158. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JACP.0000019767.55592.63.
28
Reitz, A. K., Asendorpf, J. B., & Motti-Stefanidi, F. (2015). When do immigrant adolescents
feel personally discriminated against? Longitudinal effects of peer preference. International Journal
of Behavioral Development, 39(3), 197-209. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025414567008.
Reitz, A. K., Motti-Stefanidi, F., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2016). Me, us, and them: Testing
sociometer theory in a socially diverse real-life context. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 110(6), 908. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000073
Richman, L. S., & Leary, M. R. (2009). Reactions to discrimination, stigmatization, ostracism,
and other forms of interpersonal rejection: A Multimotive Model. Psychological Review, 116(2),
365-383. doi: 10.1037/a0015250
Richman, L. S., Martin, J., & Guadagno, J. (2016). Stigma-based rejection and the detection
of signs of acceptance. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(1), 53-60. doi:
10.1177/1948550615598376.
Riley, A., Varner, A., Ventevogel, P., Taimur Hasan, M. M., & Welton-Mitchell, C. (2017).
Daily stressors, trauma exposure, and mental health among stateless Rohingya refugees in
Bangladesh. Transcultural Psychiatry, 54(3), 304-331. https://doi.org/10.1177/1363461517705571.
Riva, P., & Eck, J. (2016). The many faces of social exclusion. In P. Riva & J. Eck (Eds.),
Social exclusion: Psychological approaches to understanding and reducing its impact (pp. ixxv).
Cham, Switzerland: Springer International. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-33033-4.
Riva, P., Montali, L., Wirth, J. H., Curioni, S., & Williams, K. D. (2017). Chronic social
exclusion and evidence for the resignation stage: An empirical investigation. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 34(4), 541-564. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407516644348.
Riva, P., Wesselmann, E. D., Wirth, J. H., Carter-Sowell, A. R., & Williams, K. D. (2014).
When pain does not heal: The common antecedents and consequences of chronic social and physical
pain. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 36(4), 329-346.
https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2014.917975.
29
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press. doi: 10.1515/9781400876136.
Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling and more. Version
0.512 (BETA). Journal of statistical software, 48(2), 1-36. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02.
RStudio Team. (2016). RStudio (Version 1.1.453) [Computer software]. www.rstudio.com
Rubin, R. (2017). Loneliness might be a killer, but what’s the best way to protect against it?.
Jama, 318(19), 1853-1855. doi: 10.1001/jama.2017.14591
Saldaña, D. H. (1992). Coping with stress: A refugee's story. Women & Therapy, 13(1-2), 21-
34. doi: 10.1300/J015V13N01_04.
Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2001). A scaled difference chi-square test statistic for moment
structure analysis. Psychometrika, 66(4), 507-514. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02296192.
Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: our view of the state of the
art. Psychological methods, 7(2), 147. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.7.2.147.
Schinka, K. C., VanDulmen, M. H., Bossarte, R., & Swahn, M. (2012). Association between
loneliness and suicidality during middle childhood and adolescence: longitudinal effects and the role
of demographic characteristics. The Journal of psychology, 146(1-2), 105-118.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2011.584084.
Schmitt, M. T., & Branscombe, N. R. (2002). The meaning and consequences of perceived
discrimination in disadvantaged and privileged social groups. European review of social
psychology, 12(1), 167-199. https://doi.org/10.1080/14792772143000058.
Schmitt, M. T., Branscombe, N. R., Postmes, T., & Garcia, A. (2014). The consequences of
perceived discrimination for psychological well-being: A meta-analytic review. Psychological
bulletin, 140(4), 921. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035754.
Schulz, A. J., Gravlee, C. C., Williams, D. R., Israel, B. A., Mentz, G., & Rowe, Z. (2006).
Discrimination, symptoms of depression, and self-rated health among African American women in
30
Detroit: results from a longitudinal analysis. American journal of public health, 96(7), 1265-1270.
https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2005.064543.
Selig, J. P., & Little, T. D. (2012). Autoregressive and cross-lagged panel analysis for
longitudinal data. In B. Laursen, T. D. Little, & N. A. Card (Eds.), Handbook of developmental
research methods (p. 265278). The Guilford Press.
Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self.
In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. New York: Academic Press.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60229-4.
Sue, D. W., Capodilupo, C. M., Torino, G. C., Bucceri, J. M., Holder, A., Nadal, K. L., &
Esquilin, M. (2007). Racial microaggressions in everyday life: Implications for clinical practice.
American Psychologist, 62(4), 271-286. doi: 10.1037/0003-19 066X.62.4.271.
Surawy, C., Hackmann, A., Hawton, K., & Sharpe, M. (1995). Chronic fatigue syndrome: a
cognitive approach. Behaviour research and therapy, 33(5), 535-544. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-
7967(94)00077-W.
United Nations (2016). Leaving No One Behind: The Imperative of Inclusive Development.
United Nations available at: http://www.un.org/esa/socdev/rwss/2016/full-report.pdf
Vangelisti, A. L., Young, S. L., Carpenter-Theune, K., & Alexander, A. L. (2005). Why does
it hurt? The perceived causes of hurt feelings. Communication Research, 32, 443477.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650205277319.
Villa M (ed.) (2018). Le città globali e la sfida dell’integrazione. Pubblicazioni ISPI.
Available at https://www.ispionline.it/sites/default/files/pubblicazioni/cittaglobali_def_web_2.pdf
Waldeck, D. B. S., Tyndall, I., & Chmiel, N. (2015). Resilience to ostracism: A qualitative
inquiry. The Qualitative Report, 20(10), 1646-1670. Retrieved from
http://nsuworks.nova.edu/tqr/vol20/iss10/8.
31
Wesselmann, E. D., & Williams, K. D. (2017). Social life and social death: Inclusion,
ostracism, and rejection in groups. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 20(5), 693-706.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430217708861.
Wesselmann, E. D., Grzybowski, M. R., Steakley-Freeman, D. M., DeSouza, E. R., Nezlek,
J. B., & Williams, K. D. (2016). Social exclusion in everyday life. In P. Riva & J. Eck (Eds.), Social
exclusion: Psychological approaches to understanding and reducing its impact (pp. 3-23). Cham,
Switzerland: Springer International. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-33033-4_1.
Wesselmann, E. D., Michels, C., & Slaughter, A. (2019). Understanding common and diverse
forms of social exclusion. In S. Rudert, R. Greifeneder, & K. D. Williams (Eds.), Current directions
in ostracism, social exclusion, and rejection research (pp. 1-17). London: Routledge.
https://doi.org/10.4324/9781351255912-1.
Wesselmann, E. D., Williams, K. D., Ren, D., & Hales, A. H. (2014). Ostracism and
solitude. In R. J. Coplan & J. C. Bowker (Eds.), The handbook of solitude: Psychological
perspectives on social isolation, social withdrawal, and being alone. John Wiley & Sons.
https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118427378.ch13.
Wessely, S., Chalder, T., Hirsch, S., Wallace, P., Wright, D. (1996). Psychological symptoms,
somatic symptoms, and psychiatric disorder in chronic fatigue and chronic fatigue syndrome: a
prospective study in the primary care setting. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 1050-1059.
https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.153.8.1050.
Williams, K. D. (2009). Ostracism: A temporal need‐threat model. Advances in experimental
social psychology, 41, 275-314. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)00406-1.
Williams, K. D., Shore, W. J., & Grahe, J. E. (1998). The silent treatment: Perceptions of its
behaviors and associated feelings. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 1(2), 117-141.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430298012002.
32
Yakushko, O., Watson, M., & Thompson, S. (2008). Stress and coping in the lives of recent
immigrants and refugees: Considerations for counseling. International Journal for the Advancement
of Counselling, 30(3), 167. doi: 10.1007/s10447-008-9054-0.
Zadro, L. (2004). Ostracism: What the real world can teach us about silence. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation. University of New South Wales, Sydney.
Zhang, J., Ding, C., Tang, Y., Zhang, C., & Yang, D. (2017). A measure of perceived chronic
social adversity: development and validation. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 2168.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02168.
33
Table 1. Mean (standard deviation) and correlations of the study variables.
Variables
M (SD)
Reliability
1
2
3
4
5
Wave 1
1.
Exclusion
1.86 (1.27)
r = .75*
-
2.
Resignation
2.16 (0.55)
α = .82
.58*
-
Wave 2
3.
Exclusion
1.57 (0.85)
r = .57*
.08
.40*
-
4.
Resignation
1.66 (1.05)
α = .87
.11
.49*
.55*
-
Wave 3
5.
Exclusion
1.66 (1.05)
r = .73*
.21
.17
.49*
.39*
-
6.
Resignation
1.96 (0.52)
α = .88
.40*
.52*
.51*
.68*
.66*
Note. *p<.01. α = Cronbach’s alpha; r = Pearson’s r. Pearson’s correlation coefficients.
34
Table 2. Goodness of fit of the tested models and comparisons
χ2
df
(nfp)
CFI
RMSEA
RMSEA
90% CI
SRMR
Model
comparison
Δχ2
df)
a) baseline
model
25.62***
6
(21)
0.88
0.17
[0.11, 0.23]
0.16
-
-
b) forward
causation
16.37***
3
(24)
0.92
0.20
[0.12, 0.29]
0.12
a vs. b
9.72*
(3)
c) reverse
causation
10.79**
3
(24)
0.95
0.15
[0.07, 0.24]
0.05
a vs. c
14.85**
(3)
d) reciprocal
causation1
0
0
(27)
1.00
0
-
0
a vs. d
25.62***
(3)
b vs. d
16.37***
(3)
c vs. d
10.79**
(3)
e) reciprocal
causation2
7.88
5
(22)
0.98
0.07
[0.00, 0.15]
0.05
e vs. d
7.88
(5)
f) reciprocal
causation3
8.89
6
(21)
0.98
0.07
[0.00, 0.14]
0.04
f vs. e
0.67
(1)
g) reciprocal
causation4
14.19*
7
(20)
0.96
0.10
[0.02, 0.16]
0.08
g vs. f
5.34*
(1)
h) reciprocal
causation5
16.84*
8
(19)
0.95
0.10
[0.04, 0.16]
0.10
h vs. f
7.19*
(2)
i) reciprocal
causation6
8.58
7
(20)
0.99
0.05
[0.00, 0.12]
0.05
i vs. f
0.23
(1)
Note. *** p < .001; ** p < .01; * p < .05. Letters in bold indicate the better fitting models. 1 Just-identified model. 2
Non-significant paths constrained to zero. 3 Resignation autoregressive paths constrained to be equal. 4 Exclusion
autoregressive paths constrained to be equal. 5 Correlation (Wave1) and residuals covariance (Wave2/Wave3)
constrained to be equal. 6 Exclusion forwards causation paths constrained to be equal.
35
Figure 1. Standardized coefficients (standard errors) of the cross-lagged panel model (i) analyzing
the longitudinal relationship between perceived chronic exclusion and resignation stage
Note. *** p < .001; ** p < .01; * p < .05. Coefficients with identical superscripts indicate equality
constraints. Non-significant paths constrained to zero are not depicted.
... Інші теоретичні моделі пов'язували поведінку соціального відсторонення від тривалого відторгнення. Однак інші дослідження стверджують, що такі наслідки для психічного здоров'я, пов'язані з виключенням, виявлені переважно або в постійно маргіналізованих соціальних групах, таких як мігранти, або в осіб із остракізмом, який може тривати роками [3]. Більше того, дані літератури про самотність підкреслюють її значний зв'язок із психічним здоров'ям [1]. ...
... Доступна література підкреслює, що кількість та якість особистих соціальних зв'язків може впливати на психологічне здоров'я людей, які вимушено перебувають у стані відчуження [3]. Наприклад, одне з досліджень у в'язниці показало, що ув'язнені, які відвідують особисті групові зустрічі, демонструють значно кращий психічний стан, ніж ув'язнені, які не приєднуються до групових сесій [8]. ...
... Наприклад, одне з досліджень у в'язниці показало, що ув'язнені, які відвідують особисті групові зустрічі, демонструють значно кращий психічний стан, ніж ув'язнені, які не приєднуються до групових сесій [8]. Аналогічні дослідження стверджували, що регулярне безпосереднє спілкування з корінними людьми захищало психологічне здоров'я мігрантів від шкоди соціального відчуження [3]. Як стверджують публікації, окрім особистих стосунків, соціальна взаємодія в Інтернеті за допомогою інформаційно-комунікаційних технологій (наприклад, сайти соціальних мереж) може захистити від впливу на психічне здоров'я постійного відчуження та ізоляції. ...
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic has led to an unprecedented crisis in mental health worldwide. Most of the affected countries, including Ukraine, have implemented physical distancing. Despite the efforts of many scientists over the past few months, research into the negative psychological effects of social isolation is still ongoing, and many questions remain unanswered.Purpose. To examine the data of scientific research on the impact of quarantine and social isolation on the mental state of the population.Materials and methods. In our search of the available literature, we identified 129 articles on the impact of the pandemic on mental health published between May 2020 and May 2022, and this review analyzes thirty-two articles (in this time range). In addition, data from six fundamental studies on loneliness and social isolation were used.Results. The 2019 coronavirus disease pandemic has affected the mental health of people around the world. The scientific publications we reviewed suggest that this large-scale health crisis has led to an increase in the prevalence of symptoms of anxiety, depression, and PTSD. According to the World Health Organization, the prevalence of conditions such as depression and anxiety increased by more than 25% in the first year of the COVID-19 pandemic. Research also suggests that measures to limit the spread of the disease (including physical distancing, quarantine, and isolation) can increase feelings of loneliness and withdrawal symptoms.Conclusions. The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on mental health is unprecedented in its scale. Most notably, during the time period covered by this literature review, serious mental disorders were observed in the general population, with a particularly high risk among people with prior mental illness.
... Also, research has largely neglected the investigation of risk and protective factors facilitating or hindering entry into the resignation stage. Emerging findings supported the persistent exclusion-resignation link in the general population (Riva & Eck, 2016;Rudert et al., 2021;Zamperini et al., 2020) and in marginalized social groups like prisoners (Aureli et al., 2020) and immigrants (Marinucci, Mazzoni, et al., 2022;Marinucci & Riva, 2021a, 2021bMazzoni et al., 2020). ...
... Even more, these results extend the literature by providing quasiexperimental evidence compared to the non-homeless population that the pervasive condition of exclusion experienced by homeless people also occurs at the interpersonal level through episodes of ostracism and rejection. Results indicated that homeless people, similar to other marginalized social groups (e.g., prisoners, asylum-seekers, and refugees; Aureli et al., 2020;Marinucci & Riva, 2021b), are at risk of entering the resignation stage (Williams, 2009). Moreover, the indirect effects of inequality on resignation via group identification and exclusion emphasized that the perception of economic inequality and identification with one's stigmatized group might increase the perception of social exclusion and its health impact. ...
... Also, the mediation models were based on cross-sectional data that do not allow to rule out the possibility of alternative explanations based on a different order of the considered variables. Previous longitudinal studies assessed the directionality of the effects confirming that social exclusion can lead over time to resignation among marginalized groups (Marinucci & Riva, 2021b) and depression among the general population (Rudert et al., 2021). Nevertheless, the interpretation of the directionality of the current findings should be taken with care. ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite the relevance of social exclusion and economic inequality for homelessness, empirical studies investigating how these issues relate to homeless people's psychological well-being are scarce. We aimed to fill this gap by conducting two quasi-experimental studies on homeless and non-homeless groups. The first study (N = 200) showed that homeless (vs. non-homeless) people presented higher levels of resignation, characterized by depression, alienation, helplessness , and unworthiness (Williams, 2009). The second study (N = 183) replicated the findings from Study 1 and showed that perceived economic inequality could increase homeless people's resignation by emphasizing perceptions of social exclusion. Additional analyses found that identification with the stigmatized homeless group could mediate the relationship between perceived inequality and social exclusion, increasing the resignation. Overall, the results showed that chronic social exclusion of homeless people is associated with higher levels of resignation. Moreover, they showed the role of perceived economic inequality and homeless group stigmatized identification as group-specific mechanisms favouring social exclusion and ultimately worsening psychological well-being. K E Y W O R D S economic inequality, homeless people, resignation stage, social exclusion, social identity
... In line with earlier conceptualizations, individuals suffering from chronic exclusion may experience a state of constant feelings of exclusion (Riva, Wesselmann, et al., 2014; see also Aureli et al., 2020). Some groups are especially at risk of being chronically excluded; for example, immigrants, asylum-seekers, or prison inmates (e.g., Aureli et al., 2020;Janke et al., under review;Marinucci et al., 2022;Marinucci & Riva, 2021a, 2021b. This raises the question of how chronic exclusion is experienced: do individuals get used to the sting of exclusion, or do the adverse effects of being excluded worsen with each new exclusion experience? ...
... Our findings may have important implications for two types of targets: first, for targets of bullying, the perpetrators and the bullying context are likely stable (e.g., workplace bullying or bullying at school), and targets have to face rejection and exclusion all the time. Second, our findings may also have implications for research on chronically excluded groups such as immigrants, asylumseekers, or prison inmates (e.g., Aureli et al., 2020;Marinucci & Riva, 2021a, 2021b. These two types of targets may ultimately be most at risk for developing depression or even suicidal tendencies in response to chronic exclusion (e.g., Chen et al., 2020;Rudert, Janke, et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Unlike one-time lab manipulations of exclusion, in real life, many people experience exclusion, from others and from groups, over extended periods, raising the question of whether individuals could, over time, develop hypo- or hypersensitive responses to chronic exclusion. In Study 1, we subjected participants to repeated experiences of inclusion or exclusion (three Cyberball games, time lag of three days, N = 194; 659 observations). We find that repeatedly excluded individuals become hypersensitive to inclusion, but not to exclusion. Study 2 ( N = 183) tested whether individuals with chronic experiences of real-world exclusion show hypo- or hypersensitive responses to a novel episode of exclusion. In line with Study 1, exclusion hurt to the same extent regardless of baseline levels of chronic exclusion in daily life. However, chronically excluded individuals show more psychological distress in general. We discuss theoretical and practical implications for dealing with chronically excluded individuals and groups.
... Data analysis also shows that the subjective and objective effects of this State-controlled waiting on a person's body and mind (Grace et al. 2018;Phillimore and Cheung 2021) are further compounded by the material restrictions of being legally recognised as an 'asylum seeker' in the UK and receiving an application registration card (ARC). Whilst the process of being identified as such 'legalises' a person's status and presence in the UK, it also formalises their exclusion from work, travel and access to public funds or educational opportunities (Morrice et al. 2021;Coddington 2019;Marinucci and Riva 2021). Faahim, for example, explains that whilst waiting for a decision on his asylum claim, he wanted to 'learn English in college' and hoped to enrol at university if his claim was successful. ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper draws on empirical data generated in the ‘Everyday Bordering in the UK’ project, with a focus on the experiences of people seeking asylum and hoping to establish a safe life in the UK. Specifically, we show that during the process of claiming asylum, people’s experiences of waiting and displacement—practices inherent in UK immigration policies—work as time- and space-based dimensions of power that are imbued with colonial logic. Existing studies apply the lens of Foucault’s governmentality approach to politics regulating people seeking asylum. In particular, the international literature describes the policy of dispersal, and associated periods of waiting, as a dynamic of power used by governments to control and regulate behaviours. However, these time- and space-related experiences are often considered separately, focusing on the rationalities underpinning these politics. This paper, by contrast, develops Foucault’s theories by examining how these two characteristics interconnect in the lived realities of people waiting for an asylum decision in the UK to create racialised politics of power and privilege that reproduce the colonial origins of European migration governance. In doing so, we contribute by illustrating how practices within the UK asylum system can be embodied by people seeking asylum to create a subject that modifies behaviours in response to being positioned as ‘less deserving’ than UK citizens—the ‘colonised self’.
... Furthermore, both language and intergroup contact influence refugees' wellbeing and psychological integration. Refugees often lack these contacts and support networks (Marinucci & Riva, 2021;Pernice & Brook, 1996), which decreases their well-being (Holt-Lunstad et al., 2015). For example, in an Australian study on integration, refugees described weak language proficiency and communication abilities as the biggest strain on their well-being (Watkins et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Unlabelled: Learning the host society's language and finding a job are important steps for the societal integration of refugees. Especially language proficiency is a key barrier for the integration of low literates. Often language training and gaining work skills are separated during the integration procedure. We investigated a 1-year pilot program for refugees with low-literacy levels in the Netherlands, which combined language training (daily classes, work-related language) with work experience in sheltered employment (second-hand shop) to facilitate language learning and prepare this group better for the labour market. Building on Ager and Strang's conceptual integration framework (2008), we expected that this combined program should improve agency (communication strategies, preparedness for the labour market) via intergroup contact at work. We used a mixed-method approach to follow the development of participants (N=10) longitudinally (baseline, after 6 months, after 11 months). We gathered questionnaire data, interviewed teachers and students, and observed interactions in classes and at work. Overall, use of communication strategies increased. Analyzing individual cases (profiles) offered nuanced insights into for whom and why the program seems to have differential impact, especially in terms of labour market preparation. We discuss results and the importance of creating intergroup contact to facilitate integration in a new society. Supplementary information: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s12134-023-01028-6.
... Social relationships are an important source of life satisfaction and meaning in life (Lucas & Dyrenforth, 2006;Stavrova & Luhmann, 2016;Sun et al., 2020), frequent (vs. rare) social contact predicts higher psychological well-being (Ren et al., 2021a, b), better physical health and longevity (Stavrova & Ren, 2020), whereas social isolation is associated with psychological distress and mental illness (Marinucci & Riva, 2021). Besides having a direct positive effect on well-being, social contact may serve as a protective factor of individuals' well-being against negative stressors (Cohen & Wills, 1985). ...
Article
Full-text available
Unlabelled: People are often advised to engage in social contact to cope with the experience of loneliness and improve well-being. But are the moments of loneliness actually more bearable when spent in other people's company? In this research, we proposed and tested two conflicting theoretical accounts regarding the role of social contact: social contact is associated with a stronger (the amplifying account) or with a weaker (the buffering account) negative effect of loneliness on psychological well-being. Analyses of three datasets collected using ecological momentary assessments (Nindividuals = 3,035) revealed that the negative association between loneliness and well-being was stronger when participants were with others than alone, consistent with the amplifying account. Further, when participants experienced high levels of loneliness, being with others was associated with the same or with even a lower level of well-being than being alone. These findings suggest that simply spending time with others (vs. alone) is not associated with a reduced burden of loneliness and may even backfire. Supplementary information: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s10902-023-00661-3.
... Social exclusion of migrants raises impelling political, social, and psychological concerns in many Western countries (Jetten & Esses, 2018). Social exclusion, by threatening fundamental human needs (Williams, 2009), harms the mental health of migrants exposed to rejection and discrimination (Marinucci & Riva, 2020). Besides, the migration phenomenon mobilizes national groups, dividing society into supporters for migrants' inclusion and those who pursue their exclusion. ...
Article
This preregistered study examined whether positive and negative intergroup contact with migrants relates to collective action supporting and opposing migrants as well as to interpersonal exclusion toward them via the key processes identified in the Social Identity Model of Collective Action. Structural equation models conducted on cross-sectional data from 506 Italian and English participants showed that positive (negative) contact related to higher (lower) collective inclusion and lower (higher) collective and interpersonal exclusion of migrants via group anger, identification, and efficacy. Considering positive and negative contact, collective action pro and against migrants and interpersonal behaviors simultaneously, this study provided a nuanced picture of the antecedents and processes underlying the advantaged group members’ behaviors toward migrants.
... Due to the fact that the bureaucratic asylum-seeking process is extremely complex and often takes a very long time, the lives of refugees can be immensely destabilized as they wait to be granted official refugee status. This destabilization can manifest in many different ways, including living under precarious conditions, loneliness, helplessness, the lack of fulfilling and meaningful daily activities, grief, the loss of social structures, social identity, and/or family [22][23][24]. All of these factors can accumulate and reciprocally amplify each other over time, further complicating the psychological care of refugees and asylum-seekers. ...
Article
Full-text available
Refugees constitute a vulnerable group with an increased risk of developing trauma-related disorders. From a clinician’s integrative perspective, navigating the detrimental impact of the social, economic, structural, and political factors on the mental health of refugees is a daily experience. Therefore, a collective effort must be made to reduce health inequities. The authors developed a treatment concept which provides broader care structures within a scientific practitioner’s approach. The resulting “Trauma Network” addresses the structural challenges for refugees in Middle Hesse. Accompanying research provided a sound basis for further discussions with policy-makers to improve the situation for refugees in the short- and long-term.
Article
The COVID-19 pandemic, accompanied by stringent social restrictions, wrought profound changes across various facets of human existence. Unprecedented measures, such as compulsory quarantines, curfews, and restrictions on mobility and social interactions, were implemented to mitigate infection rates. This paper delves into the repercussions of isolation, with a specific focus on its impact on the elderly population—an exceptionally vulnerable demographic. The primary objective of this study is to discern the ramifications of pandemic-induced isolation on the mental and physical well-being of senior citizens. This contribution underscores the comparative analysis of three prior studies that have illuminated the nexus between pandemic-induced isolation and heightened levels of anxiety, depression, and loneliness. A notable strength of this research lies in its comprehensive dataset, derived from comparisons with extant scientific literature and the utilization of diverse scientific methodologies. The preceding investigations centered on the Austrian populace, juxtaposing the effects of loneliness among senior citizens before and during the pandemic. However, these studies were constrained by their inability to explore the enduring consequences of isolation and loneliness post-repeal of anti-pandemic measures, and their incapacity to ascertain its correlation with senior citizens' mortality, particularly those residing in solitary circumstances. This article represents a partial outcome of the VEGA 1/0595/21 project, which investigates public administration interventions during the COVID-19 era and their influence on the quality of life of selected community residents.
Chapter
Marginalization occurs when a whistleblower is physically moved to minor assignments, relocated to a remote or an inferior location, or detailed to nominal projects not commensurate with their job description. This move indicates to others the lesser value of the whistleblower, which then enables devaluing and promotes shunning and mobbing. The message sent to other employees is that it is condoned to treat the whistleblower as if they were insignificant or unimportant and sends a message to other employees to fear retaliation if they align with the whistleblower. The whistleblower becomes inconsequential, irrelevant, and powerless to confront the wrongdoing. Since they are physically isolated within the workplace, they suffer the consequences seen in other populations that are marginalized and ignored by the mainstream. As social pariahs and outcasts, they become the scapegoat for other problems within the organization. This type of banishment can be viewed as cruel and unusual punishment that results in alienation and lost connectedness to the individual who then is likely to experience workplace traumatic stress.
Article
Full-text available
K. D. Williams (2009) theorized that chronic social exclusion would inescapably lead to a detrimental stage of resignation, characterized by depression, alienation, unworthiness, and helplessness. However, few studies empirically addressed this assumption. Considering immigrants as a population at risk of persistent exclusion, we investigated how social connections with the native-born majority and other immigrant minorities moderate the exclusion–resignation link. In Study 1 ( N = 112 asylum seekers), participants mainly connected with other immigrants showed a significant association between chronic exclusion and resignation. Crucially, this link vanished for people with social connections mainly composed of native people. In Study 2, we replicated and extended these results running secondary analyses on a data set of 2,206 immigrants (CILS4EU). This work, suggesting that the exclusion–resignation link can be moderated by social factors, highlighted the relevance of immigrants’ connections with the native majority for counteracting the risk of segregation when tackling the social issue of immigrants’ everyday exclusion.
Article
Full-text available
Social ostracism among the homeless is a prevailing problem, yet few studies have focused on whether internalizing psychopathology moderates the links between feeling ostracized and perceiving threats to fundamental human needs. This study used a person-oriented approach to identify commonly occurring profiles of internalizing psychopathology characterized by symptoms of social anxiety, generalized anxiety, and depression (Low, Medium, and High Internalizers) among homeless participants residing in London, United Kingdom (N = 114; age range = 18–74; Mage = 46; 25% women). Data on perceived ostracism (feeling ignored and daily discrimination) and need-threat (belonging, self-esteem, meaningful existence, and feelings of control) was also collected. Controlling for the effects of age, living arrangement, gender, and time being homeless, feeling ignored was a significant predictor of need-threat, whereas daily discrimination was not. One significant interaction on the links between daily discrimination and need-threat emerged between Low and Medium Internalizers. For Medium Internalizers, high levels of daily discrimination were associated with high levels of need-threat. The effect was similar for High Internalizers and the opposite for Low Internalizers, though it was not significant within those groups. Taken together, these results indicate that differences in patterns of internalizing psychopathology should be taken into account when attempting to make homeless individuals feel more included in their surroundings.
Article
Full-text available
Depression is associated with negative attention and attribution biases and maladaptive emotion responsivity and regulation, which adversely impact self-evaluations and interpersonal relationships. Using fMRI, we investigated the neural substrates of these impairments. We compared neural activity recruited by 126 clinically depressed and healthy adolescents (ages 11-17) during social exclusion (Exclusion>Inclusion) using Cyberball. Results revealed significant interaction effects within left anterior insula/inferior frontal gyrus and left middle temporal gyrus. Insula hyperresponsivity was associated with peer exclusion for depressed adolescents but peer inclusion for healthy adolescents. In additional, healthy adolescents recruited greater lateral temporal activity during peer exclusion. Complementary effect size analyses within independent parcellations offered converging evidence, as well as highlighted medium-to-large effects within subgenual/ventral anterior cingulate cortex and lateral prefrontal, lateral temporal, and lateral parietal regions implicated in emotion regulation. Depressogenic neural patterns were associated with negative self-perceptions and negative information processing biases. These findings suggest a neural mechanism underlying cognitive biases in depression, as reflected by emotional hyperresponsivity and maladaptive regulation/reappraisal of negative social evaluative information. This study lends further support for salience and central executive network dysfunction underlying social threat processing, and in particular, highlights the anterior insula as a key region of disturbance in adolescent depression.
Article
Full-text available
The goal of this study was to develop a measure that assesses negative daily social encounters. Specifically, we examined the concept of perceived chronic social adversity and its assessment, the Perceived Chronic Social Adversity Questionnaire (PCSAQ). The PCSAQ focused on the subjective processing of daily social experiences. Psychometric properties were examined within two non-clinical samples (N = 331 and N = 390) and one clinical sample (N = 86). Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses supported a three-factor model of the PCSAQ, which corresponds to three types of daily social stressors. The final 28-item PCSAQ was shown to be internally consistent, and to have good construct validity in terms of factor structure and group differences. It was also shown to have good concurrent validity in terms of association with outcome variables (sense of control, happiness, and mood and anxiety symptoms). Perceived chronic social adversity was also shown to be correlated with PTSD severity. Taken together, these findings suggest that the PCSAQ is a reliable, valid, and useful measure that can be used to assess negative social and clinical aspects of personal experiences. This study is an important exploratory step in improving our understanding of the relationship between the cumulative effect of negative social encounters and psychological difficulty.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Inclusion health focuses on people in extremely poor health due to poverty, marginalisation, and multimorbidity. We aimed to review morbidity and mortality data on four overlapping populations who experience considerable social exclusion: homeless populations, individuals with substance use disorders, sex workers, and imprisoned individuals. Methods: For this systematic review and meta-analysis, we searched MEDLINE, Embase, and the Cochrane Library for studies published between Jan 1, 2005, and Oct 1, 2015. We included only systematic reviews, meta-analyses, interventional studies, and observational studies that had morbidity and mortality outcomes, were published in English, from high-income countries, and were done in populations with a history of homelessness, imprisonment, sex work, or substance use disorder (excluding cannabis and alcohol use). Studies with only perinatal outcomes and studies of individuals with a specific health condition or those recruited from intensive care or high dependency hospital units were excluded. We screened studies using systematic review software and extracted data from published reports. Primary outcomes were measures of morbidity (prevalence or incidence) and mortality (standardised mortality ratios [SMRs] and mortality rates). Summary estimates were calculated using a random effects model. Findings: Our search identified 7946 articles, of which 337 studies were included for analysis. All-cause standardised mortality ratios were significantly increased in 91 (99%) of 92 extracted datapoints and were 11·86 (95% CI 10·42-13·30; I(2)=94·1%) in female individuals and 7·88 (7·03-8·74; I(2)=99·1%) in men. Summary SMR estimates for the International Classification of Diseases disease categories with two or more included datapoints were highest for deaths due to injury, poisoning, and other external causes, in both men (7·89; 95% CI 6·40-9·37; I(2)=98·1%) and women (18·72; 13·73-23·71; I(2)=91·5%). Disease prevalence was consistently raised across the following categories: infections (eg, highest reported was 90% for hepatitis C, 67 [65%] of 103 individuals for hepatitis B, and 133 [51%] of 263 individuals for latent tuberculosis infection), mental health (eg, highest reported was 9 [4%] of 227 individuals for schizophrenia), cardiovascular conditions (eg, highest reported was 32 [13%] of 247 individuals for coronary heart disease), and respiratory conditions (eg, highest reported was 9 [26%] of 35 individuals for asthma). Interpretation: Our study shows that homeless populations, individuals with substance use disorders, sex workers, and imprisoned individuals experience extreme health inequities across a wide range of health conditions, with the relative effect of exclusion being greater in female individuals than male individuals. The high heterogeneity between studies should be explored further using improved data collection in population subgroups. The extreme health inequity identified demands intensive cross-sectoral policy and service action to prevent exclusion and improve health outcomes in individuals who are already marginalised. Funding: Wellcome Trust, National Institute for Health Research, NHS England, NHS Research Scotland Scottish Senior Clinical Fellowship, Medical Research Council, Chief Scientist Office, and the Central and North West London NHS Trust.
Article
Refugees often experience poor physical and mental health outcomes following resettlement. These outcomes have been linked to the conditions that are experienced by refugees in the post‐migration context, but little is known about the mechanisms by which these conditions influence health. We therefore conducted secondary analyses of the Survey of New Refugees, a large longitudinal study commissioned by the U.K. Home Office with data collected at four time points spanning 21 months. Refugees’ experience of emotional distress such as feeling stressed, worried, and depressed fully mediated the relationship between post‐migration stressors and longitudinal general health. There was no evidence that perceived social support influenced this relationship. These findings suggest that emotional distress contributes to poor health outcomes among refugees and thus that interventions might target emotional distress. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Article
This analysis examines the content of the contemporary German asylum disputes, if a causative relation between immigration pressure and nationalist aggressiveness against foreigners exists at all, the extent immigration pressures are at all existent, what sort of positive policies and political reorientation are required to address the problems of aggressive nationalism; how to address the increasing inmigration pressures and whether asylum rights and immigration regulations offer at all the appropriate instruments to address the problems.
Article
Every year, British researcher Christina Victor, PhD, spots advertisements for charitable programs designed to ensure that no senior spends the Christmas holiday alone.