ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Dual-learning theories of evaluations posit that evaluations can be automatically (i.e., efficiently, unconsciously, uncontrollably, and involuntarily) acquired. They also often assume evaluative learning processes that are impervious to verbal information. In this article, we explain that recent research challenges both assertions for three categories of measures: “explicit” evaluative measures, “implicit” evaluative measures, and physiological measures of fear. In doing so, we also question the widespread assumption that “implicit” (i.e., typically behavioral and physiological) versus “explicit” (i.e., self-reported) evaluative measures are indicative of the way evaluations are acquired. In the second part of the article, we discuss the practical implications of these recent findings.
Behavioral and Physiological Evidence Challenges the Automatic Acquisition of
Evaluations.
Olivier Corneille, UCLouvain.
Gaëtan Mertens, Tilburg University & Utrecht University.
Manuscript in press in Current Directions in Psychological Science
Olivier Corneille (Contact author). Psychological Sciences Research Institute, UCLouvain, 10
Place du Cardinal Mercier, 1348 Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium. Email:
olivier.corneille@uclouvain.be
2
Behavioral and Physiological Evidence Challenges the Automatic Acquisition of
Evaluations.
1. The Automatic Acquisition of Evaluations.
An influential view pervading psychological research (e.g., in social, consumer,
health, and clinical psychology) is that evaluations can be acquired through an
associative/affective learning mode that automatically registers mere stimuli co-occurrences
encountered in the environment. In attitude research, associative learning is thought to allow
for the unconscious, efficient, involuntary, and uncontrollable formation of evaluations and to
be insensitive to the relational meaning of stimuli co-occurrences (e.g., Gawronski &
Bodenhausen, 2014). Likewise, influential theories of fear learning posit that fear can be
automatically acquired (i.e., without consciousness, effort, or intention, and uncontrollably),
resulting in non-conscious and unqualified associative representations that automatically elicit
fear when activated (e.g., LeDoux & Pine, 2016; Öhman & Mineka, 2001).
This automatic learning view implies a high susceptibility of individuals to social and
environmental influences. For instance, people may fall prey to a negative political
advertisement if the information it contains creates, uncontrollably or unconsciously, a
negative evaluation of the targeted candidate. It also often assumes a difficulty to change
automatic evaluative responses based on mere verbal information. For instance, a fear of dogs
may be difficult to change using verbal instruction if the fear response reflects the automatic
activation of an inaccessible or unconscious mental association between a dog and having
been bitten.
These questions have been mostly investigated using conditioning procedures. In
attitude research, a neutral stimulus (the conditioned stimulus, or the CS; e.g., a neutral face)
is paired with a positive or negative stimulus (the unconditioned stimulus, or the US: e.g., a
pleasant or unpleasant sound or picture). An evaluative conditioning effect is said to occur
3
when the evaluation of the CS changes after the CS-US pairing. In fear-conditioning research,
typically aversive USs (e.g., an electric shock, a loud noise) are used, and subjective,
behavioral, and physiological responses related to fear and arousal (e.g., distress ratings,
avoidance responses, skin conductance responses) are measured (e.g., Lipp, 2006). Evaluative
and fear conditioning procedures involve a simple associative procedure (i.e., pairing CSs
with USs) that is considered ideally suited to the investigation of associative/affective
learning.
2. Testing the Automatic Learning of Evaluations Using “Explicit”, “Implicit” and
Physiological Measures.
The automatic learning of evaluations has been tested using “explicit”, “implicit”, and
physiological measures. Explicit measures are self-reported evaluations, such as good/bad
judgments or direct scale ratings about an attitude object. Implicit measures are less clearly
defined (see Corneille & Hütter, 2020), but generally rely on indirect behavioral tasks that
reduce participants’ control and deliberation during measurement. The Implicit Association
Test is considered the gold standard of “implicit” evaluative measures. In this task,
respondents may sort faster positive stimuli alongside category A (e.g., thin people) and
negative stimuli alongside category B (e.g., obese people). The extent to which they do so (or
the opposite) indicates their implicit preference for one category over the other. Measurement
outcomes of this sort are thought to reflect biased mental associations, commonly referred to
as “implicit biases” or “unconscious biases.” Physiological measures record participants’
physiological responses to stimuli. Skin conductance responses and potentiation of the startle-
reflex are examples of such measures, and they capture a mix of arousal and evaluation
(Lonsdorf et al., 2017). Whereas “explicit” and “implicit” evaluative measures are frequently
used in social psychological, social cognition, and consumer research, physiological measures
are more common in clinical and neuroscience research.
4
Dual-learning theories assume that less controllable and deliberate measures of
evaluations (i.e., “implicit” and physiological measures) provide a better window into
associative/affective learning (e.g., Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2014; Ledoux & Pine, 2016).
This is because these measures reduce the influence of deliberate and controlled post-learning
processes on task performance.
3. Growing Evidence Challenges the Automatic Acquisition of Evaluations on all
Categories of Measures.
Research on evaluative conditioning has largely failed to obtain conclusive evidence
for automatic evaluative learning (for a comprehensive review, see Corneille & Stahl, 2019).
No evaluative learning effect is observed in procedures preventing a conscious encoding of
the stimuli, such as when using short-timed, parafoveal, or visually suppressed presentations.
Evaluative learning is also resource demanding. It is fully disrupted when participants are
engaged in a concurrent task during the stimuli presentation. Finally, research indicates that
evaluative learning is sensitive to participants’ processing goals. For instance, evaluative
learning effects are largely weakened when participants are distracted from processing the
affective quality of the information.
Of importance too, evidence that may be considered supportive of automatic
evaluative learning is unrelated to “implicit” versus “explicit” measurement. The mere
exposure effect, often considered the hallmark of “preferences-that-need-no-inferences”, is
typically demonstrated on self-reported evaluations (e.g., “Which of these two stimuli do you
prefer?”) Likewise, preliminary evidence for uncontrollable evaluative learning (for instance,
participants presumably unable not to start liking a stimulus paired with positive information)
has been best observed on “explicit” liking measures (Hütter & Sweldens, 2018). Hence,
contrary to a widespread dual-process view, that an evaluation is observed on an “implicit
5
task” or on a self-reported measure is unrelated to how it was acquired in the first place
(Corneille & Hütter, 2020).
Besides the automaticity of evaluative learning, dual-learning models of evaluations
posit that associative knowledge captures raw associations between events, unqualified by the
relational meaning of these associations. For instance, people may harbor negative
associations about a pain-relieving medication because of its co-occurrence with pain (i.e., the
raw contingency), despite the medication relieving the pain (i.e., the relational implication of
the medication). Preliminary evidence supporting the possibility of such unqualified
representations or processes, however, has again been found on “explicit” (e.g., Kükken,
Hütter & Holland, 2019) measures, and these effects are highly sensitive to task structure
when measured with “implicit” tasks (e.g., Bading, Stahl, & Rothermund, 2020).
As it appears, differences in performance on “explicit” vs. “implicit” tasks cannot be
univocally interpreted in terms of dissociations in learning modes. Post-learning processes
may be responsible for these differences. For instance, a smaller amount of information is
likely retrieved from memory when completing a speeded (e.g., an Implicit Association Test)
than a non-speeded (e.g., an evaluative rating) task. Hence, divergences in outcomes on
“implicit” versus “explicit” tasks may reflect divergences in retrieval, despite originating in a
unitary learning process that is neither unconscious nor immune to verbal information. As a
matter of fact, “implicit” attitudes can be created in a snap based on mere verbal information
(e.g., De Houwer, 2009). And this is also true when it comes to changing evaluations - for
instance, merely telling participants that Gandhi prevented his wife from taking a medical
treatment that could have saved her life results in a quick reversal of scores on “implicit”
measures (Van Dessel, Ye, & De Houwer, 2018). This runs counter a prevalent view that sees
“implicit biases” as the result of unconsciously and slowly acquired associations.
6
So far, we have discussed behavioral measures. As explained above, physiological
measures may provide a more sensitive test for the existence of a distinct learning mode
grounded in more affective systems. Here too, however, recent research questions automatic
learning and supports the sensitivity of physiological indicators of evaluations to verbal
instruction (e.g., Mertens et al., 2018). For instance, a recent meta-analysis has concluded in a
weak fear acquisition effect, plausibly driven by publication bias and methodological
artifacts, when using subliminal stimuli (see Mertens & Engelhard, 2020). And, similar to
explicit and implicit evaluative measures, fear responses can also be modulated by verbal
instruction (Atlas, 2019; Mertens et al., 2018). For example, conditioned physiological
responses can be rapidly abolished when participants are told that no more USs will be
delivered (Luck & Lipp, 2016). It is telling that these effects have also been demonstrated
using the startle-reflex (Luck & Lipp, 2015; Mertens & De Houwer, 2016), a basic defensive
reflex evident within 20-120 ms (Blumenthal et al., 2005) that is considered an index of
amygdala activity (Hamm & Weike, 2005).
4. Implications for theory and practice.
Implications of these findings for psychological theories are straightforward. There is
less evidence for and more evidence against the automatic learning of evaluations than one
may think. There is also more evidence than is commonly assumed for the role of verbal
information in the acquisition and change of evaluations. Alternative single learning models
should, therefore, be considered a more parsimonious alternative to dual-learning models.
Examples of such models are propositional models (De Houwer, 2009), goal-directed models
(e.g., Boddez, Moors, Mertens, & De Houwer, 2020) or retrieval-based models (e.g., Stahl &
Aust, 2018). Perhaps even more important are the practical implications of these recent
findings, which we now briefly discuss in the social, consumer, and health and clinical
psychology domains.
7
Social psychology: An influential social-psychological view that found its way to the
general public, and that is currently inspiring large-scale social interventions, is that “mental
associations” automatically formed about social groups (coined “unconscious bias” or
“implicit bias”) influence people’s judgment and behavior. Drawing on automatic evaluative
learning may facilitate people’s acceptance of their prejudice. Unfortunately, this may also
normalize prejudice (Eberhardt & Banks, 2019) and undermine accountability for social
discrimination (Daumeyer, Onyeador, Brown, & Richeson, 2019). That is, if “mental
associations” about social groups build up unconsciously and uncontrollably, one may
consider that people are not so much responsible for holding them.
As discussed here, however, the evidence is scarce that evaluations may be acquired
automatically. Likewise, it is unclear whether “implicit” measures assess unconscious
evaluations (e.g., Gawronski, 2019). As provocative as this may seem, even assuming
“implicit” tasks do assess unconscious and automatically acquired associations, we do not
even know whether they have any behavioral impact at all. Because outcomes on “implicit”
measures can be neither coherently nor distinctly induced, their causal role remains
unestablished (for a recent discussion, see Corneille & Hütter, 2020).
Again, this conclusion runs against a popular view holding that “unconscious bias
infiltrates every arena of life” and “can be just as devastating as the harm done by explicit
racism, sexism or homophobia » (Eberhardt & Banks, 2019). Admittedly, “unconscious bias”
may be understood here in terms of automatically enacted behaviors as opposed to
automatically acquired and unconsciously held “mental associations” presumably underlying
these behaviors (e.g., Gawronski, Ledgerwood & Eastwick, in press). However, this
alternative understanding is uncommon, and it would be highly problematic if the
“unconscious bias” construct were applied both to the cause and to its consequences.
8
Similar conclusions apply to personality research that has theorized discrepancies in
so-called “implicit” and “explicit” self-esteem. If “implicit” measures of the self are not
diagnostic of distinct learning processes and representations (see also Schimmack, 2019), one
is left to wonder how discrepancies in these measures should be interpreted, as well as how
and why “implicit” self-esteem should be changed at all.
Problematic theorization on the “implicit” construct is widespread in social
psychological research and this can be consequential (for a comprehensive discussion, see
Corneille & Hütter, 2020). During the Covid-19 outbreak, the Society for Personality and
Social Psychology featured research inviting people to indulge in comfort food to feed
“primitive and implicit feelings of being cared for and loved” (SPSP, 2020). This surprising
recommendation (literally: eat high-caloric food to please your unconscious inner self) may
contribute to another epidemic: the obesity one.
Consumer psychology: Consumers may be upset about the use of subliminal
conditioning (such as when briefly pairing Al Gore with “Rats” in a political advertisement).
In all likelihood, however, subliminal conditioning is largely ineffective for changing
people’s evaluations (see Corneille & Stahl, 2019). Ironically, people generally feel at ease
with blatant social influence techniques (e.g., a presidential candidate featured against the
American flag) that are much more influential. This is not to say that automatic processes,
including unconscious ones, play no role in consumer or social behavior. They certainly do.
Yet, as we have already pointed out, many of these effects may occur at a post-learning stage.
Consistent with associative/affective learning views, companies have relied on
“implicit” measures to assess “unconscious” preferences in their consumers. The company
Pizza Hut, for instance, has used eye-tracking technology to assess unconscious pizza topping
preferences in consumers. There is, however, no indication that eye-tracking is relevant to the
study of “unconsciously” acquired or “unconsciously” held pizza topping preferences (not to
9
speak of whether eye-tracking provides a valid measure of preferences at all). And, even if it
was, there is no evidence either that this measure of preference would allow for tastier
experiences. It is largely assumed, when using “implicit” measures, that testers know more
about respondents than respondents know about themselves. This, however, is a
philosophically and empirically tricky assumption. And, if it is incorrect, it may have ethically
questionable consequences (e.g. feeding consumers with pizza topping they don’t like,
persuading individuals that they hold conflicting conscious and unconscious evaluations about
themselves and about others).
Health and clinical psychology: That fear responding is acquired and may be changed
on the basis of mere verbal instructions could be inspirational for new clinical interventions.
For instance, in therapy, patients can be asked to formulate their expectations (e.g., if I walk
in the park, a dog will attack me) and asked how these expectations may be tested (e.g., take a
walk in the park during a busy afternoon). This approach can help identify patients’
idiosyncratic expectations and allow for more targeted exposure therapy. Interventions of this
sort have been successfully developed in cognitive-behavioral therapies (Hofmann, 2008).
Surprisingly enough, research on these types of interventions remains surprisingly scarce in
fundamental research on fear conditioning (see Carpenter, Pinaire, & Hofmann, 2019). A
broader appreciation for the role of consciously accessible representations could lead to a
more widespread application of such cognitive interventions to challenge dysfunctional
beliefs about contingencies in the world.
5. Clarification and limitations.
We want to emphasize that we fully endorse the view that automatic processes
influence people’s judgments, and behaviors. Clearly, for instance, people may show efficient
behavioral and physiological responses when exposed to disliked stimuli. Likewise, it would
be unreasonable to assume that people are constantly aware of the determinants of their
10
perceptions, judgments and behaviors. Instead, we are pointing to a growing body of
empirical evidence challenging the view that these responses are automatically learned and
are insensitive to verbal information. We also question the view that some measures (e.g.,
“implicit” and physiological measures) would best indicate an associative/affective learning
mode or qualitatively different representations. Although our analysis was mainly based on
conditioning procedures (for the reasons we have explained), similar conclusions have been
reached in related paradigms, such as approach-avoidance and the mere exposure effect (for
an in-depth discussion, see Corneille & Stahl, 2019).
Our analysis did not include studies with brain imaging techniques and brain-lesioned
populations. With regard to studies on brain-lesions and process-specific impairments, some
studies have shown that region-specific lesions (e.g., amygdala damage) result in process-
specific impairments (e.g., successful acquisition of fear conditioning with skin conductance
responses, but no acquisition of declarative knowledge), while the reverse being true for
damage in another region (i.e., hippocampal damage and unsuccessful fear conditioning, but
the successful acquisition of declarative knowledge; Bechara et al., 1995).
Such findings may be seen as providing strong evidence for dual-learning theories of
evaluations. However, other researchers have pointed out that double dissociations of this sort
do not necessarily need to be interpreted as evidence for modularity and independent
processes (e.g., Plaut, 1995). Furthermore, conflicting findings have been reported in the
literature. For instance, Coppens et al. (2009) reported that patients with unilateral amygdala
damage can in fact acquire conditioned fear responses after acquiring explicit knowledge of
CS-US contingencies.
With regard to brain imaging studies, we believe that brain activation should not be
considered a direct measure of attitude or fear. Instead, brain imaging data reflect a neural
implementation level of analysis and can be logically consistent with different models of how
11
evaluations are acquired, retrieved, and expressed. Additionally, it can be noted that
associative mental representations are not necessarily neuro-anatomically, -chemically, or –
biologically more plausible than non-associative (e.g., propositional) representations (e.g.,
Langille & Gallistel, 2020). Given their different levels of analysis, however, psychological
and neurocognitive evidence can be mutually informative to approximate a more accurate
psychological and neurobiological model of how evaluations and fears are acquired and can
be changed (e.g., Amodio & Ratner, 2011).
6. Conclusion
A growing body of research challenges the existence of an associative/affective formation of
attitudes and fears when this learning mode is defined as automatic and impervious to verbal
information. As discussed in this article, the acquisition of evaluations and fear is much less
automatic than often assumed, and both can be modulated using verbal instruction, even when
using “implicit” and physiological measures. Acknowledging more broadly this recent
evidence is important for both theory and practice. The unconscious and resource-free
learning of attitudes and fears should be questioned. In addition, the potential of verbal
instructions for changing “deep-rooted” evaluations offers a promising avenue for social and
clinical interventions.
12
References:
Amodio, D. M., & Ratner, K. G. (2011). A memory systems model of implicit social
cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 143-148.
Atlas, L. Y. (2019). How instructions shape aversive learning: higher order knowledge,
reversal learning, and the role of the amygdala. Current Opinion in Behavioral
Sciences, 26, 121-129.
Bading, K., Stahl, C., & Rothermund, K. (2020). Why a standard IAT effect cannot provide
evidence for association formation: The role of similarity construction. Cognition and
Emotion, 34(1), 128-143. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2019.1604322.
Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Adolphs, R., Rockland, C., & Damasio, A. R. (1995).
Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala
and hippocampus in humans. Science, 269(5227), 1115-1118.
Blumenthal, T. D., Cuthbert, B. N., Filion, D. L., Hackley, S., Lipp, O. V., & Van Boxtel, A.
(2005). Committee report: Guidelines for human startle eyeblink electromyographic
studies. Psychophysiology, 42(1), 1-15.
Boddez, Y., Moors, A., Mertens, G., & De Houwer, J. (2020). Tackling fear: Beyond
associative memory activation as the only determinant of fear responding. Neuroscience
& Biobehavioral Reviews.
Carpenter, J. K., Pinaire, M., & Hofmann, S. G. (2019). from extinction learning to anxiety
treatment: Mind the gap. Brain sciences, 9(7), 164.
Coppens, E., Spruyt, A., Vandenbulcke, M., Van Paesschen, W., & Vansteenwegen, D.
(2009). Classically conditioned fear responses are preserved following unilateral
temporal lobectomy in humans when concurrent US-expectancy ratings are
used. Neuropsychologia, 47(12), 2496-2503.
Corneille, O., & Hütter, M. (2020). Implicit? What do you mean? A comprehensive review of
the implicitness construct in attitude research. Personality and Social Psychology Review.
Corneille, O., & Stahl, C. (2019). Associative attitude learning: A closer look at evidence and
how it relates to attitude models. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 23(2), 161-
189.
13
Daumeyer, N. M., Onyeador, I. N., Brown, X., & Richeson, J. A. (2019). Consequences of
attributing discrimination to implicit vs. explicit bias. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 84, 103812.
De Houwer, J. (2009). The propositional approach to associative learning as an alternative for
association formation models. Learning & Behavior, 37(1), 1-20.
Eberhardt, J.L, Banks, S. (2019; July 12). Implicit bias puts lives in jeopardy. Can mandatory
training reduce the risk? Los Angeles Times. Retrieved from https://www.latimes.com
Gawronski, B. (2019). Six lessons for a cogent science of implicit bias and its criticism.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14, 574-595.
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2014). The associative-propositional evaluation
model: Operating principles and operating conditions of evaluation. Dual-process
theories of the social mind, 188-203.
Gawronski, B., Ledgerwood, A., & Esatwick, P. (in press). Implicit bias and anti-
discrimination policy. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
Hamm, A. O., & Weike, A. I. (2005). The neuropsychology of fear learning and fear
regulation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 57(1), 5-14.
Hofmann, S. G. (2008). Cognitive processes during fear acquisition and extinction in animals
and humans: Implications for exposure therapy of anxiety disorders. Clinical Psychology
Review, 28(2), 199-210.
Hütter, M., & Sweldens, S. (2018). Dissociating controllable and uncontrollable effects of
affective stimuli on attitudes and consumption. Journal of Consumer Research, 45, 320-
349.
Kukken, N., Hütter, M., & Holland, R. W. (2019). Are there two independent evaluative
conditioning effects in relational paradigms? Dissociating the effects of CS-US pairings
and their meaning. Cognition and Emotion, 34, 1-20.
Langille, J., & Gallistel, C. (2020). Locating the engram: Should we look for plastic synapses
or information-storing molecules? Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 169.
LeDoux, J. E., & Pine, D. S. (2016). Using neuroscience to help understand fear and anxiety:
a two-system framework. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173, 1083-1093.
Lipp, O. V. (2006). Human Fear Learning: Contemporary Procedures and Measurement.
Lonsdorf, T. B., Menz, M. M., Andreatta, M., Fullana, M. A., Golkar, A., Haaker, J., ... &
Drexler, S. M. (2017). Don’t fear ‘fear conditioning’: Methodological considerations for
14
the design and analysis of studies on human fear acquisition, extinction, and return of
fear. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 77, 247-285.
Luck, C. C., & Lipp, O. V. (2015). A potential pathway to the relapse of fear? Conditioned
negative stimulus evaluation (but not physiological responding) resists instructed
extinction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 66, 18-31.
Luck, C. C., & Lipp, O. V. (2016). Instructed extinction in human fear conditioning: History,
recent developments, and future directions. Australian Journal of Psychology, 68(3), 209-
227.
Mertens, G., & De Houwer, J. (2016). Potentiation of the startle reflex is in line with
contingency reversal instructions rather than the conditioning history. Biological
Psychology, 113, 91-99.
Mertens, G., Boddez, Y., Sevenster, D., Engelhard, I. M., & De Houwer, J. (2018). A review
on the effects of verbal instructions in human fear conditioning: Empirical findings,
theoretical considerations, and future directions. Biological Psychology, 137, 49-64.
Mertens, G., & Engelhard, I. M. (2020). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the
evidence for unaware fear conditioning. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 108,
254-266.
Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved
module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108(3), 483.
Plaut, D. C. (1995). Double dissociation without modularity: Evidence from connectionist
neuropsychology. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17(2), 291-
321.
Schimmack, U. (2019). The implicit association test: a method in search of a construct.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1745691619863798.
Stahl, C., & Aust, F. (2018). Evaluative conditioning as memory-based judgment. Social
Psychological Bulletin, 13(2), 1-30.
SPSP (2020, March 25)."Preparing and eating [comfort] foods activates a primitive and
implicit feeling of being cared for and loved”. [Tweet]
https://twitter.com/SPSPnews/status/1242837564156936194?s=20
Van Dessel, P., Ye, Y., & De Houwer, J. (2018). Changing Deep-Rooted Implicit Evaluation
in the Blink of an Eye: Negative Verbal Information Shifts Automatic Liking of Gandhi.
Social Psychological and Personality Science, 10, 266-273.
15
Recommended readings:
Corneille, O., & Hütter, M. (2020). Implicit? What do you mean? A comprehensive review of
the implicitness construct in attitude research. Personality and Social Psychology Review.
Corneille, O., & Stahl, C. (2019). Associative attitude learning: A closer look at evidence and
how it relates to attitude models. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 23(2), 161-
189.
De Houwer, J. (2009). The propositional approach to associative learning as an alternative for
association formation models. Learning & Behavior, 37(1), 1-20.
Gawronski, B. (2019). Six lessons for a cogent science of implicit bias and its criticism.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14, 574-595.
Mertens, G., & Engelhard, I. M. (2020). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the
evidence for unaware fear conditioning. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 108,
254-266.
... Whereas evidence is generally lacking for unaware and efficient evaluative learning (see Corneille & Mertens, 2020; for reviews), recent evidence supports uncontrolled evaluative effects (Gawronski et al., 2014(Gawronski et al., , 2015Hütter & Sweldens, 2018;Waroquier et al., 2022). The present research offers new insights into the uncontrollability question by introducing a new procedure, as well as ambivalence measures, to overcome limitations of previous approaches. ...
... Automatic evaluative learning is assumed in dual-learning attitude models, such as the Associative-Propositional Evaluation Model (e.g., Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006. However, and despite significant efforts, evidence for it remains scarce (for a review, see ; see also Corneille & Mertens, 2020). As we noted in the introduction, the most compelling evidence comes from Evaluative Conditioning (EC) research on uncontrollability (Gawronski et al., 2014(Gawronski et al., , 2015Hütter & Sweldens, 2018;Waroquier et al., 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
The contribution of uncontrolled processes to evaluative learning has been examined in evaluative conditioning procedures by comparing evaluations of conditioned stimuli between tasks or within tasks but between learning instruction conditions. In the present research, we introduced a new procedure that keeps both tasks and instructions constant. In addition, we introduced ambivalence measures to address this uncontrollability question. The new procedure involves forming an impression of conditioned stimuli based on their pairing with one unconditioned stimulus while attending but discarding the influence of another unconditioned stimulus holding the same (congruent trials) vs. a different (incongruent trials) valence. When the to-be-used and to-be-discarded unconditioned stimuli share the same (vs. a different) valence, controlled and uncontrolled processes should support the same (vs. opposite) responses. We used this approach in two preregistered experiments (Ntotal = 467) using dichotomous evaluative classifications (Experiments 1 and 2), evaluative ratings, and two measures of attitudinal ambivalence: mouse trajectories and felt ambivalence (Experiment 2). While we failed to find evidence for uncontrolled processes in evaluative classification frequencies separately in Experiments 1 and 2, analyses of aggregated classification frequencies across Experiments 1 and 2 suggested a small contribution of uncontrolled processes. In addition, we found larger felt ambivalence for incongruent than congruent trials. Overall, the present findings are mixed but support the possibility of a contribution of uncontrolled processes to evaluative learning, even when control is applied to a focal stimulus and additional influences come from a to-be-disregarded stimulus.
... Whereas evidence is generally lacking for unaware and efficient evaluative learning (see Corneille & Mertens, 2020; for reviews), recent evidence supports uncontrolled evaluative effects (Gawronski et al., 2014(Gawronski et al., , 2015Hütter & Sweldens, 2018;Waroquier et al., 2022). The present research offers new insights into the uncontrollability question by introducing a new procedure, as well as ambivalence measures, to overcome limitations of previous approaches. ...
... Automatic evaluative learning is assumed in dual-learning attitude models, such as the Associative-Propositional Evaluation Model (e.g., Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006. However, and despite significant efforts, evidence for it remains scarce (for a review, see ; see also Corneille & Mertens, 2020). As we noted in the introduction, the most compelling evidence comes from Evaluative Conditioning (EC) research on uncontrollability (Gawronski et al., 2014(Gawronski et al., , 2015Hütter & Sweldens, 2018;Waroquier et al., 2022). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The contribution of uncontrolled processes to evaluative learning has been examined in evaluative conditioning procedures by comparing evaluations of conditioned stimuli between tasks or within tasks but between learning instruction conditions. In the present research, we introduced a new procedure that keeps both tasks and instructions constant. In addition, we introduced ambivalence measures to address this uncontrollability question. The new procedure involves forming an impression of conditioned stimuli based on their pairing with one unconditioned stimulus while attending but discarding the influence of another unconditioned stimulus holding the same (congruent trials) vs. a different (incongruent trials) valence. When the to-be-used and to-be-discarded unconditioned stimuli share the same (vs. a different) valence, controlled and uncontrolled processes should support the same (vs. opposite) responses. We used this approach in two preregistered experiments (Ntotal = 467) using dichotomous evaluative classifications (Experiments 1 and 2), evaluative ratings, and two measures of attitudinal ambivalence: mouse trajectories and felt ambivalence (Experiment 2). While we failed to find evidence for uncontrolled processes in evaluative classification frequencies separately in Experiments 1 and 2, analyses of aggregated classification frequencies across Experiments 1 and 2 suggested a small contribution of uncontrolled processes. In addition, we found larger felt ambivalence for incongruent than congruent trials. Overall, the present findings are mixed but support the possibility of a contribution of uncontrolled processes to evaluative learning, even when control is applied to a focal stimulus and additional influences come from a to-be-disregarded stimulus.
... In spite of their differences, both UX and QoE research are increasingly embracing the notion that experience data can be acquired through associative methods, which automatically capture occurrences of stimuli in the multimedia environment. These associative methods are designed to capture unconscious, efficient, involuntary, and uncontrollable bodily reactions, which, in turn, generate evaluations (Corneille & Mertens, 2020). This perspective provides an alternative to traditional evaluation methods that heavily rely on subjective information voluntarily provided by users through surveys, ratings, questionnaires, and similar means. ...
Article
The evaluation of human responses in multimedia experiences using physiological data has a well-established presence in the academic literature. However, this field is currently undergoing transformative changes, driven by the accessibility of diverse and cost-effective devices, innovative software analysis methods, and the emergence of novel application domains such as Virtual and Augmented Reality and mulsemedia. To address the imperative of contextualizing these evolving trends in a contemporary context, this paper presents a systematic review with the objective of delineating the array of physiological data utilized in assessing Quality of Experience (QoE) and User Experience (UX) in multimedia studies. It also examines the devices employed for data collection and the analytical techniques applied to interpret the acquired data. While our review exposes both constraints and promising discoveries in these domains, it also emphasizes the escalating significance and practicality of leveraging physiological data in user assessments, especially as the boundaries between the physical and digital domains continue to blur.
... In that sense, although our research has consistently shown evidence for learning effects in the absence of CA, it is still necessary to verify if our results are generalizable to other forms or paradigms of implicit processing. This could also shed new light on the applicability of implicit perspectives to real-life phenomena (Gawronski et al., 2020), not only relevant for clinical issues, but also for social phenomena (Corneille & Mertens, 2020). Future research should reexamine classical implicit learning paradigms using the BACT, and we propose the inclusion of reliability analyses and Bayes factors in any examination of implicit or subliminal processing. ...
Article
Full-text available
Evidence for implicit aversive learning effects has been criticized for its lack of experimental rigor and statistical reliability. Here we examine whether attentional emotional responses to aversive conditioned stimuli can occur in the absence of stimulus-outcome contingency awareness, and use a novel Bayesian tool to reliably perform a post hoc categorization of awareness. Across two experiments (n = 40 and 69) participants completed an aversive conditioning task. A novel Bayesian awareness categorization tool was applied to sensitively measure contingency awareness. Finally, attentional and subjective responses toward conditioned stimuli were measured. For participants unaware of contingencies, conditioned stimuli generated attentional avoidance, but only aware participants showed subjective learning effects. For both experiments, awareness scores for unaware participants did not regress above chance level on a subsequent awareness check, revealing a reliable determination of unawareness states. These findings provide evidence for the existence of aversive learning in the absence of contingency awareness, as demonstrated via conditioned attentional responses, and build an analytical framework that can be extrapolated to other implicit paradigms.
... The propositional theory accounts for most of the disposable data on the subject (Hofmann et al., 2010) and there is little doubt that high-level propositional mechanisms process CS-US links to form attitudes. However, one of the most hotly debated topics in the EC literature (Baeyens et al., 2009;Corneille & Mertens, 2020;Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2018;Hütter, 2022;Moran et al., 2016Moran et al., , 2023 is whether a second set of automatic processes (i.e. fast, effortless, uncontrollable, unintentional, unconscious 1 ) also forms attitudes in a parallel and implicit manner, as predicted by dual-process theories of attitude formation (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006;Morewedge & Kahneman, 2010;Rydell & McConnell, 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
Several authors assume that evaluative conditioning (EC) relies on high-level propositional thinking. In contrast, the dual-process perspective proposes two processing pathways, one associative and the other propositional, contributing to EC. Dual-process theorists argue that attitudinal ambiguity resulting from these two pathways' conflicting evaluations demonstrate the involvement of both automatic and controlled processes in EC. Previously, we suggested that amplitude variations of error-related negativity and error-positivity, two well-researched event-related potentials of performance monitoring, allow for the detection of attitudinal ambiguity at the neural level. The present study utilises self-reported evaluation, categorisation performance, and neural correlates of performance monitoring to explore associative-propositional ambiguity during social attitude formation. Our results show that compared to associative-propositional harmony, attitudinal ambiguity correlates with more neutral subjective evaluations, longer response times, increased error commission, and diminished error-related negativity amplitudes. While our findings align with dual-process models, we aim to offer a propositional interpretation. We discuss dual-process theories in the context of evolutionary psychology, suggesting that associative processes may only represent a small piece of the EC puzzle.
... Il convient malgré tout d'être vigilant quant aux conclusions à rendre à partir de ce type de mesure. En effet, la mesure des attitudes indirectes est un champ de recherche très investigué par la psychologie sociale dont le consensus sur leur interprétation et sur les modèles théoriques sous-jacents n'est pas acquis (Corneille & Hutter, 2020 ;Corneille & Mertens, 2020 ;Greenwald & Lai, 2019). L'objet de ce chapitre n'est pas de se positionner en faveur d'un type de mesure en particulier, mais de sensibiliser aux limites des outils que nous utilisons. ...
... For instance, studies have claimed successful subliminal conditioning for salivating response but not for evaluations (Passarelli et al., 2022). Although recent research suggests the involvement of convergent evaluative learning processes for behavioral (direct or indirect) and physiological measures (Corneille & Mertens, 2020), one should be open to the possibility that different learning or decision making processes are operating when specific physiological responses, or specific populations, are under consideration. Likewise, one may speculate that behavioral measures capitalizing on preference rather than absolute evaluative judgments (Amd & Passarelli, 2020) may be more sensitive to memory-independent processes (but see Heycke et al., 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
Attitude research has capitalized on evaluative conditioning procedures to gain insight into how evaluations are formed and may be changed. In evaluative conditioning, a conditioned stimulus (CS; e.g., an unfamiliar soda brand) is paired with an unconditioned stimulus (US) of affective value (e.g., a pleasant picture). Following this pairing, a change in CS liking may be observed (e.g., the soda brand is liked better). A question with far-reaching theoretical and practical implications is whether the change in CS liking is found when participants feel they do not remember the CS-US pairings at the time an evaluation is produced about the CS. Here, we introduce a new conditional judgment procedure - the two-button-sets (TBS) task - for probing evaluative conditioning effects without feelings of remembering about the valence of the US paired with the CS. In three experiments, the TBS is (1) is successfully validated; it is also used to (2) provide preliminary information on the feeling of remembering question, and (3) to examine an affect-consistent bias in memory judgments for CS-US pairings. Results do not support evaluative effects in the absence of feelings of remembering, and they oppose the view that affect-consistent bias is limited to memory uncertainty. We discuss these finding in light of previous evidence and of dual-learning models of attitudes. We also discuss limitations and research avenues related to the new procedure. Preprint available at https://psyarxiv.com/rtqnx/
... Furthermore, recent research has supported the sensitivity of indirect tasks, such as the Implicit Association Test, to relational information (Bading et al., 2020). More generally, the assumption that indirect and direct tasks are differently sensitive to propositional and associative processes has now been widely questioned (see, e.g., Corneille & Hütter, 2020;De Houwer, 2009;Gawronski, 2019; for an extension to physiological measures, see Corneille & Mertens, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
People occasionally encounter information whose structure bears divergent evaluative implications. For instance, when reading that a sunscreen protects against skin cancer, the relational meaning of the information (i.e., “protects against skin cancer”) has positive evaluative implications for the sunscreen, whereas the co-occurrence (of “sunscreen” with “skin cancer”) is negative. An important theoretical (and practical) issue is whether the co-occurrence information influences people’s evaluations beyond the relational meaning of the information. This question has been recently investigated using task comparison procedures (comparing evaluative outcomes on different tasks) and process dissociation procedures (estimating relational and cooccurrence parameters within a given task). In this article, we report four experiments that examined this question by reducing interpretational ambiguities inherent in the two preceding approaches. This was achieved by using self-reported and mouse-tracking measures of ambivalence. We reasoned that when co-occurrence and relational information have divergent (rather than convergent) evaluative implications, more ambivalence should be found. We tested this prediction in experiential (Experiments 1 to 3) and instructed (Experiment 4) procedures. Higher self-reported ambivalence was found in divergent than convergent conditions in all experiments. Ambivalence, as estimated with mouse-tracking measures, was higher in divergent than convergent conditions in the experiential experiments but not in the instructed experiment. Results are discussed with reference to single-process (propositional and episodic) and dual-process attitude learning models.
Article
The evaluative conditioning (EC) effect has been documented in many experiments: Participants typically prefer stimuli that co-occurred with positive stimuli over stimuli that co-occurred with negative stimuli. The present research attempted to test whether demand characteristics are a dominant cause of the EC effect. In three experiments, we informed participants of the research hypothesis, sometimes indicating an expectation of a contrast effect, rather than an assimilative effect. That manipulation hardly moderated the EC effect. The manipulation influenced participants’ beliefs regarding the research hypothesis, although participants generally believed that an assimilative effect is a more plausible research hypothesis than a contrast effect. Even participants who believed that the researchers expected a contrast effect or assumed that stimulus co-occurrence typically causes a contrast effect still showed an assimilative effect. The results suggest that although demand characteristics might influence the EC effect, the overall influence of that factor is minor.
Article
Evaluative Conditioning (EC) research investigates changes in the evaluation of a stimulus after co-occurrence with an affective stimulus. To explain the motivation behind this research, this review begins with an overview of the history of EC research, followed by a summary of the state of the art with respect to three key questions. First, how should EC procedures be used to influence evaluation? We provide a guide based on evidence concerning the functional properties of EC effects. Second, how does the EC effect occur? We discuss the possible mediating cognitive processes and their automaticity. Third, are EC effects ubiquitous outside the lab? We discuss the evidence for the external validity of EC research. We conclude that the most important open questions pertain to the relevance of EC to everyday life and to the level of control that characterizes the processes that mediate the EC effect after people notice the stimulus co-occurrence. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Psychology, Volume 74 is January 2023. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/page/journal/pubdates for revised estimates.
Article
Full-text available
The science behind implicit bias tests (e.g., Implicit Association Test) has become the target of increased criticism. However, policy-makers seeking to combat discrimination care about reducing bias in people's actual behaviors, not about changing a person's score on an implicit bias test. In line with this argument, we postulate that scientific controversies about implicit bias tests are irrelevant for anti-discrimination policy, which should instead focus on implicit bias in actual discriminatory behavior that occurs outside of awareness (in addition to instances of explicit bias). Two well-documented mechanisms can lead to implicit bias in actual discriminatory behavior: biased weighting and biased interpretation of information about members of particular social groups. The policy relevance of the two mechanisms is illustrated with their impact on hiring and promotion decisions, jury selection, and policing. Implications for education and bias intervention are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
This article provides a comprehensive review of divergent conceptualizations of the "implicit" construct that have emerged in attitude research over the past two decades. In doing so, our goal is to raise awareness of the harmful consequences of conceptual ambiguities associated with this terminology. We identify three main conceptualizations of the "implicitness" construct: The procedural conceptualization (implicit as indirect), the functional conceptualization (implicit as automatic), and the mental theory conceptualization (implicit as associative), as well as two hybrid conceptualizations (implicit as indirect and automatic, implicit as driven by affective gut reactions). We discuss critical limitations associated with each conceptualization and explain that confusion also arises from their coexistence. We recommend discontinuing the usage of the "implicit" terminology in attitude research and research inspired by it. We offer terminological alternatives aimed at increasing both the precision of theorization and the practical value of future research.
Article
Full-text available
Whether fear conditioning can take place without contingency awareness is a topic of continuing debate and conflicting findings have been reported in the literature. This systematic review provides a critical assessment of the available evidence. Specifically, a search was conducted to identify articles reporting fear conditioning studies in which the contingency between conditioned stimuli (CS) and the unconditioned stimulus (US) was masked, and in which CS-US contingency awareness was assessed. A systematic assessment of the methodological quality of the included studies (k = 41) indicated that most studies suffered from methodological limitations (i.e., poor masking procedures, poor awareness measures, researcher degrees of freedom, and trial-order effects), and that higher quality predicted lower odds of studies concluding in favor of contingency unaware fear conditioning. Furthermore, meta-analytic moderation analyses indicated no evidence for a specific set of conditions under which contingency unaware fear conditioning can be observed. Finally, funnel plot asymmetry and p-curve analysis indicated evidence for publication bias. We conclude that there is no convincing evidence for contingency unaware fear conditioning.
Article
Full-text available
Laboratory models of extinction learning in animals and humans have the potential to illuminate methods for improving clinical treatment of fear-based clinical disorders. However, such translational research often neglects important differences between threat responses in animals and fear learning in humans, particularly as it relates to the treatment of clinical disorders. Specifically, the conscious experience of fear and anxiety, along with the capacity to deliberately engage top-down cognitive processes to modulate that experience, involves distinct brain circuitry and is measured and manipulated using different methods than typically used in laboratory research. This paper will identify how translational research that investigates methods of enhancing extinction learning can more effectively model such elements of human fear learning, and how doing so will enhance the relevance of this research to the treatment of fear-based psychological disorders.
Article
Full-text available
Recent research into evaluative conditioning (EC) shows that information about the relationship between the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli can exert strong effects on the size and direction of the EC effect. Additionally, the co-occurrence of these stimuli seems to exert an orthogonal effect on evaluations. This finding has been interpreted as support for two independent types of EC effects. However, previous research devoted to this question relied on aggregated evaluative measures, allowing for alternative interpretations. In four experiments, we developed and validated a multinomial processing tree model that distinguishes effects of the pairings from effects of the meaning of the pairings. Our findings suggest that two independent EC effects contribute to overall evaluative change in a relational EC paradigm. The model that we developed offers a helpful method for future research in that it allows for an assessment of the effects of manipulations on processes rather than overall performance on an evaluative measure.
Article
Full-text available
Moran and Bar-Anan (Moran, T., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2013). The effect of object-valence relations on automatic evaluation. Cognition and Emotion, 27(4), 743–752) demonstrated that evaluations on a direct measure reflected information on both US valence and CS-US relations, whereas evaluations on an indirect measure (IAT) reflected only information on US valence. This dissociation between measures supposedly tapping into propositional and associative processes apparently supports dual process models of EC. In the present study, we present an alternative explanation of this pattern, based on an interpretation of IAT effects in terms of flexible similarity construction processes. According to this account, processing draws on those features that discriminate between target categories, and help to align targets with attributes in the compatible block. Across two experiments, we consistently found that IAT effects did not reflect rigid associations, but instead depended on whichever information could be used for similarity constructions between targets and attributes in different variants of the IAT. The findings are discussed with regard to theoretical models of EC as well as in reference to prominent accounts of IAT performance.
Article
For decades already, the human fear conditioning paradigm has been used to study and develop treatments for anxiety disorders. This research is guided by theoretical assumptions that, in some cases indirectly, stem from the tradition of association formation models (e.g., the Rescorla-Wagner model). We argue that one of these assumptions – fear responding as a monotonic function of the associative activation of aversive memory representations – restricts the types of treatment that the research community currently considers. We discuss the importance of this assumption in the context of research on extinction-enhancing and reconsolidation interference techniques. While acknowledging the merit of this research, we argue that unstrapping the straitjacket of this assumption can lead to exploring new directions for utilizing fear conditioning procedures in treatment research. We discuss two determinants of fear responding other than associative memory activation. First, fear responding might also depend on relational information. Second, a recent goal-directed emotion theory suggests that goals might be the primary determinant of the response pattern characterized as fear.
Article
Karl Lashley began the search for the engram nearly seventy years ago. In the time since, much has been learned but divisions remain. In the contemporary neurobiology of learning and memory, two profoundly different conceptions contend: the associative/connectionist (A/C) conception and the computational/representational (C/R) conception. Both theories ground themselves in the belief that the mind is emergent from the properties and processes of a material brain. Where these theories differ is in their description of what the neurobiological substrate of memory is and where it resides in the brain. The A/C theory of memory emphasizes the need to distinguish memory cognition from the memory engram and postulates that memory cognition is an emergent property of patterned neural activity routed through engram circuits. In this model, learning re-organizes synapse association strengths to guide future neural activity. Importantly, the version of the A/C theory advocated for here contends that synaptic change is not symbolic and, despite normally being necessary, is not sufficient for memory cognition. Instead, synaptic change provides the capacity and a blueprint for reinstating symbolic patterns of neural activity. Unlike the A/C theory, which posits that memory emerges at the circuit level, the C/R conception suggests that memory manifests at the level of intracellular molecular structures. In C/R theory, these intracellular structures are information-conveying and have properties compatible with the view that brain computation utilizes a read/write memory, functionally similar to that in a computer. New research has energized both sides and highlighted the need for new discussion. Both theories, the key questions each theory has yet to resolve and several potential paths forward are presented here.
Article
In 1998, Greenwald, McGhee, and Schwartz proposed that the Implicit Association Test (IAT) measures individual differences in implicit social cognition. This claim requires evidence of construct validity. I review the evidence and show that there is insufficient evidence for this claim. Most important, I show that few studies were able to test discriminant validity of the IAT as a measure of implicit constructs. I examine discriminant validity in several multimethod studies and find little or no evidence of discriminant validity. I also show that validity of the IAT as a measure of attitudes varies across constructs. Validity of the self-esteem IAT is low, but estimates vary across studies. About 20% of the variance in the race IAT reflects racial preferences. The highest validity is obtained for measuring political orientation with the IAT (64%). Most of this valid variance stems from a distinction between individuals with opposing attitudes, whereas reaction times contribute less than 10% of variance in the prediction of explicit attitude measures. In all domains, explicit measures are more valid than the IAT, but the IAT can be used as a measure of sensitive attitudes to reduce measurement error by using a multimethod measurement model.
Article
Implicit bias has garnered considerable public attention, with a number of behaviors (e.g., police shootings) attributed to it. Here, we present the results of 4 studies and an internal meta-analysis that examine how people reason about discrimination based on whether it was attributed to the implicit or explicit attitudes of the perpetrators. Participants' perceptions of perpetrator accountability, support for punishment, level of concern about the bias, and support for various efforts to reduce it (e.g., education) were assessed. Taken together, the results suggest that perpetrators of discrimination are held less accountable and often seen as less worthy of punishment when their behavior is attributed to implicit rather than to explicit bias. Moreover, at least under some circumstances, people express less concern about, and are less likely to support efforts to combat, implicit compared with explicit bias. Implications for efforts to communicate the science of implicit bias without undermining accountability for the discrimination it engenders are discussed.