Content uploaded by Brad H Story
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Brad H Story on Jul 24, 2020
Content may be subject to copyright.
Chapter 1
History of Speech Synthesis
Brad H. Story
Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences
University of Arizona
Tucson, AZ
Citation: An invited chapter for The Routledge Handbook of Phonetics, Chapter 1, pp.
9-32, W. Katz & P. Assmann, Eds., Routledge (2019).
Introduction
For the past two centuries or more, a variety of devices capable of generating artifi-
cial or synthetic speech have been developed and used to investigate phonetic phenom-
ena. The aim of this chapter is to provide a brief history of synthetic speech systems,
including mechanical, electrical, and digital types. The primary goal, however, is not
to reiterate the details of constructing specific synthesizers but rather to focus on the
motivations for developing various synthesis paradigms and illustrate how they have
facilitated research in phonetics.
The mechanical and electro-mechanical era
On the morning of December 20, 1845, a prominent American scientist attended
a private exhibition of what he would later refer to as a “wonderful invention.” The
scientist was Joseph Henry, an expert on electromagnetic induction and the first Sec-
retary of the Smithsonian Institution. The “wonderful invention” was a machine that
could talk, meticulously crafted by a disheveled 60-year-old tinkerer from Freiburg,
Germany named Joseph Faber. Their unlikely meeting in Philadelphia, Pennsylvania,
arranged by an acquaintance of Henry from the American Philosophical Society, might
1
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
have occurred more than a year earlier had Faber not destroyed a previous version of
his talking machine in a bout of depression and intoxication. Although he had spent
some 20 years perfecting the first device, Faber was able to reconstruct a second version
of equal quality in a year’s time (Patterson, 1845).
The layout of the talking machine, described in a letter from Henry to his col-
league H.M. Alexander, was like that of a small chamber organ whose keyboard was
connected via strings and levers to mechanical constructions of the speech organs. A
carved wooden face was fitted with a hinged jaw, and behind it was an ivory tongue that
was moveable enough to modulate the shape of the cavity in which it was housed. A
foot-operated bellows supplied air to a rubber glottis whose vibration provided the raw
sound that could be shaped into speech by pressing various sequences or combinations
of 16 keys available on a keyboard. Each key was marked with a symbol representing
an “elementary” sound that, through its linkage to the artificial organs, imposed time-
varying changes to the air cavity appropriate for generating apparently convincing
renditions of connected speech. Several years earlier Henry had been shown a talking
machine built by the English scientist Charles Wheatstone, but he noted that Faber’s
machine was far superior because instead of uttering just a few words, it was “capable of
speaking whole sentences composed of any words what ever” (Rothenberg et al., 1992,
p. 362).
In the same letter, Henry mused about the possibility of placing two or more of
Faber’s talking machines at various locations and connecting them via telegraph lines.
He thought that with “little contrivance” a spoken message could be coded as keystrokes
in one location which, through electromagnetic means, would set into action another
of the machines to “speak” the message to an audience at a distant location. Another
30 years would pass before Alexander Graham Bell demonstrated his invention of the
telephone, yet Henry had already conceived of the notion while witnessing Faber’s ma-
chine talk. Further, unlike Bell’s telephone, which transmitted an electrical analog of
the speech pressure wave, Henry’s description alluded to representing speech in com-
pressed form based on slowly varying movements of the operator’s hands, fingers, and
feet as they formed the keystroke sequences required to produce an utterance, a sig-
nal processing technique that would not be implemented into telephone transmission
systems for nearly another century.
It is remarkable that, at this moment in history, a talking machine had been con-
structed that was capable of transforming a type of phonetic representation into a sim-
ulation of speech production, resulting in an acoustic output heard clearly as intelligible
speech - and this same talking machine had inspired the idea of electrical transmission
2
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
of low- bandwidth speech. The moment is also ironic, however, considering that no
one seized either as an opportunity for scientific or technological advancement. Henry
understandably continued on with his own scientific pursuits, leaving his idea to one
short paragraph in an obscure letter to a colleague. In need of funds, Faber signed on
with the entertainment entrepreneur P.T. Barnum in 1846 to exhibit his talking ma-
chine for a several months run at the Egyptian Hall in London. In his autobiography,
Barnum (1886) noted that a repeat visitor to the exhibition was the Duke of Welling-
ton, who Faber eventually taught to “speak” both English and German phrases with the
machine (Barnum, 1886, p. 134). In the exhibitor’s autograph book, the Duke wrote
that Faber’s “Automaton Speaker” was an “extraordinary production of mechanical ge-
nius.” Other observers also noted the ingenuity in the design of the talking machine
(e.g., “The Speaking Automaton,” 1846; Athenaeum, 1846), but to Barnum’s puzzle-
ment it was not successful in drawing public interest or revenue. Faber and his machine
were eventually relegated to a traveling exhibit that toured the villages and towns of
the English countryside; it was supposedly here that Faber ended his life by suicide,
although there is no definitive account of the circumstances of his death (Altick, 1978).
In any case, Faber disappeared from the public record, although his talking machine
continued to make sideshow-like appearances in Europe and North America over the
next 30 years; it seems a relative (perhaps a niece or nephew) may have inherited the
machine and performed with it to generate income (“Talking Machine,” 1880; Altick,
1978).
Although the talking machine caught the serious attention of those who understood
the significance of such a device, the overall muted interest may have been related to
Faber’s lack of showmanship, the German accent that was present in the machine’s
speech regardless of the language spoken, and perhaps the fact that Faber never pub-
lished any written account of how the machine was designed or built - or maybe a
mechanical talking machine, however ingenious its construction, was, by 1846, sim-
ply considered passé. Decades earlier, others had already developed talking machines
that had impressed both scientists and the public. Most notable were Christian Gottlieb
Kratzenstein and Wolfgang von Kempelen, both of whom had independently devel-
oped mechanical speaking devices in the late 18th century.
Inspired by a competition sponsored by the Imperial Academy of Sciences at St.
Petersburg in 1780, Kratzenstein submitted a report that detailed the design of five or-
gan pipe-like resonators that, when excited with the vibration of a reed, produced the
vowels /a, e, i, o, u/ (Kratzenstein, 1781). Although their shape bore little resemblance
to human vocal tract configurations, and they could produce only sustained sounds,
3
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
the construction of these resonators won the prize and marked a shift toward scien-
tific investigation of human sound production. Kratzenstein, who at the time was a
Professor of Physics at the University of Copenhagen, had shared a long-term interest
in studying the physical nature of speaking with a former colleague at St. Petersburg,
Leonhard Euler, who likely proposed the competition. Well known for his contribu-
tions to mathematics, physics, and engineering, Euler wrote in 1761 that “all the skill
of man has not hitherto been capable of producing a piece of mechanism that could
imitate [speech]” (p. 78) and further noted that “The construction of a machine capable
of expressing sounds, with all the articulations, would no doubt be a very important
discovery” (Euler, 1761, p. 79). He envisioned such a device to be used in assistance of
those “whose voice is either too weak or disagreeable” (Euler, 1761, p. 79).
During the same time period, von Kempelen - a Hungarian engineer, industrialist,
and government official - used his spare time and mechanical skills to build a talking
machine far more advanced than the five vowel resonators demonstrated by Kratzen-
stein. The final version of his machine was to some degree a mechanical simulation of
human speech production. It included a bellows as a “respiratory” source of air pressure
and air flow, a wooden “wind” box that emulated the trachea, a reed system to gener-
ate the voice source, and a rubber funnel that served as the vocal tract. There was an
additional chamber used for nasal sounds, and other control levers that were needed for
particular consonants. Although it was housed in a large box, the machine itself was
small enough that it could have been easily held in the hands. Speech was produced by
depressing the bellows, which caused the “voice” reed to vibrate. The operator then
manipulated the rubber vocal tract into time-varying configurations that, along with
controlling other ports and levers, produced speech at the word level, but could not
generate full sentences due to the limitations of air supply and perhaps the complexity
of controlling the various parts of the machine with only two hands. The sound quality
was child-like, presumably due to the high fundamental frequency of the reed and the
relatively short rubber funnel serving as the vocal tract. In an historical analysis of von
Kempelen’s talking machine, Dudley and Tarnoczy (1950) note that this quality was
probably deliberate because a child’s voice was less likely to be criticized when demon-
strating the function of the machine. Kempelen may have been particularly sensitive
to criticism considering that he had earlier constructed and publicly demonstrated a
chess-playing automaton that was in fact a hoax (cf., Carroll, 1975). Many observers
initially assumed that his talking machine was merely a fake as well.
Kempelen’s lasting contribution to phonetics is his prodigious written account of
not only the design of his talking machine, but also the nature of speech and language
4
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
in general (von Kempelen, 1791). In “On the Mechanism of Human Speech” [English
translation], he describes the experiments that consumed more than 20 years and clearly
showed the significance of using models of speech production and sound generation to
study and analyze human speech. This work motivated much subsequent research on
speech production, and to this day still guides the construction of replicas of his talking
machine for pedagogical purposes (cf., Trouvain and Brackhane, 2011).
One person particularly inspired by von Kempelen’s work was, in fact, Joseph Faber.
According to a biographical sketch (Wurzbach, 1856), while recovering from a serious
illness in about 1815, Faber happened onto a copy of “On the Mechanism of Human
Speech” and became consumed with the idea of building a talking machine. Of course,
he built not a replica of von Kempelen’s machine, but one with a significantly advanced
system of controlling the mechanical simulation of speech production. As remarkable
as Faber’s machine seems to have been regarded by some observers, Faber was indeed
late to the party, so to speak, for the science of voice and speech had by the early
1800s already shifted into the realm of physical acoustics. Robert Willis, a professor
of mechanics at Cambridge University, was dismayed by both Kratzenstein’s and von
Kempelen’s reliance on trial-and-error methods in building their talking machines,
rather than acoustic theory. He took them to task, along with most others working
in phonetics at the time, in his 1829 essay titled “On the Vowel Sounds, and on Reed
Organ-Pipes.” The essay begins:
The generality of writers who have treated on the vowel sounds appear
never to have looked beyond the vocal organs for their origin. Apparently
assuming the actual forms of these organs to be essential to their production,
they have contented themselves with describing with minute precision the
relative positions of the tongue, palate and teeth peculiar to each vowel, or
with giving accurate measurements of the corresponding separation of the
lips, and of the tongue and uvula, considering vowels in fact more in the light
of physiological functions of the human body than as a branch of acoustics.
(Willis, 1829, p. 231)
Willis laid out a set of experiments in which he would investigate vowel produc-
tion by deliberately neglecting the organs of speech. He built reed-driven organ pipes
whose lengths could be increased or decreased with a telescopic mechanism, and then
determined that an entire series of vowels could be generated with changes in tube
length and reeds with different vibrational frequencies. Wheatstone (1837) later pointed
out that Willis had essentially devised an acoustic system that, by altering tube length,
5
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
and hence the frequencies of the tube resonances, allowed for selective enhancement of
harmonic components of the vibrating reed. Wheatstone further noted that multiple
resonances are exactly what is produced by the “cavity of the mouth,” and so the same
effect occurs during speech production but with a nonuniformly shaped tube.
Understanding speech as a pattern of spectral components became a major focus
of acousticians studying speech communication for much of the 19th century and the
very early part of the 20th century. As a result, developments of machines to produce
speech sounds were also largely based on some form of spectral addition, with little or
no reference to the human speech organs. For example, in 1859 the German scientist
Hermann Helmholtz devised an electromagnetic system for maintaining the vibration
of a set of eight or more tuning forks, each variably coupled to a resonating cham-
ber to control amplitude (Helmholtz, 1859, 1875). With careful choice of frequencies
and amplitude settings he demonstrated the artificial generation of five different vow-
els. Rudolph Koenig, a well-known acoustical instrument maker in 1800s, improved
on Helmholtz’s design and produced commercial versions that were sold to interested
clients (Pantalony, 2004). Koenig was also a key figure in emerging technology that
allowed for recording and visualization of sound waves. His invention of the phonoau-
tograph with Edouard-Léon Scott in 1859 transformed sound via a receiving cone, di-
aphragm, and stylus into a pressure waveform etched on smoked paper rotating about
a cylinder. A few years later he introduced an alternative instrument in which a flame
would flicker in response to a sound, and the movements of flame were captured on a
rotating mirror, again producing a visualization of the sound as a waveform (Koenig,
1873).
These approaches were precursors to a device called the “phonodeik” that would be
later developed at the Case School of Applied Science by Dayton Miller (1909) who
eventually used it to study waveforms of sounds produced by musical instruments and
human vowels. In a publication documenting several lectures given at the Lowell Insti-
tute in 1914, Miller (1916) describes both the analysis of sound based on photographic
representations of waveforms produced by the phonodeik, as well as intricate machines
that could generate complex waveforms by adding together sinusoidal components and
display the final product graphically so that it might be compared to those waveforms
captured with the phonodeik. Miller referred to this latter process as harmonic synthe-
sis, a term commonly used to refer to building complex waveforms from basic sinusoidal
elements. It is, however, the first instance of the word “synthesis” in the present chapter.
This was deliberate to remain true to the original references. Nowhere in the literature
on Kratzenstein, von Kempelen, Wheatstone, Faber, Willis, or Helmholtz does “syn-
6
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
thesis” or “speech synthesis” appear. Their devices were variously referred to as talking
machines, automatons, or simply systems that generated artificial speech. Miller’s use of
synthesis in relation to human vowels seems to have had the effect of labeling any future
system that produces artificial speech, regardless of the theory on which it is based, a
speech synthesizer.
Interestingly, the waveform synthesis described by Miller was not actually synthesis
of sound, but rather synthesis of graphical representations of waveforms. To produce
synthetic sounds, Miller utilized a bank of organ pipes, each of which, by design, pos-
sessed a different set of resonant frequencies. By controlling the amplitude of the sound
produced by each pipe, he could effectively produce a set of nearly pure tones that were
summed together as they radiated into free space. The composite waveform could then
be captured with the phonodeik device and compared to the graphical synthesis of the
same vowel. These were primarily vowel synthesizers, where production of each vowel
required a different collection of pipes. There was little ability to dynamically change
any aspect of the system except for interrupting the excitation of the pipes themselves;
Miller did suggest such an approach to forming some basic words.
At this point in time, about a decade and a half into the 20th century, the mechanical
and electro-mechanical era of speech synthesis was coming to a close. The elaborate
talking machines of von Kempelen and Faber that simulated human speech produc-
tion were distant memories, having been more recently replaced by studies of vow-
els using electro-mechanical devices that produced the spectral components of speech
waveforms. Although there was much debate and disagreement about many details on
the production of speech, primarily vowels, the ideas generated in this era were funda-
mental to the development of phonetics. It had become firmly established by now (but
not universally accepted) that the underlying acoustic principle of speech production
was that resonances formed by a given configuration of an air cavity enhanced or ac-
centuated the spectral components of a sound source (Rayleigh, 1878). The enhanced
portions of the spectrum eventually came to be known as “formants,” a term that seems
to have been first used by Ludimar Hermann in his studies of vowel production using
phonograph technology (Hermann, 1894, 1895). Thus, the stage had been set to usher
in the next era of speech synthesis.
The electrical and electronic era
A shift from using mechanical and electro-mechanical devices to generate artificial
speech to purely electrical systems had its beginnings in 1922. It was then that John Q.
Stewart, a young physicist from Princeton published an article in the journal Nature
7
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
titled “An Electrical Analogue of the Vocal Organs” (Stewart, 1922). After military
service in World War I, during which he was the chief instructor of “sound ranging”
at the Army Engineering School, Stewart had spent two years as research engineer in
the laboratories of the American Telephone and Telegraph Company and the West-
ern Electric Company (Princeton Library). His article was a report of research he had
completed during that time. In it he presents a diagram of a simple electrical circuit
containing an “interrupter” or buzzer and two resonant branches comprised of vari-
able resistors, capacitors, and inductors. Noting past research of Helmholtz, Miller, and
Scripture, Stewart commented that “it seems hitherto to have been overlooked that a
functional copy of the vocal organs can be devised . . . [with] audio-frequency os-
cillations in electrical circuits” (1922, p. 311). He demonstrated that a wide range of
artificial vowels could be generated by adjusting the circuit elements in the resonant
branches. Because of the ease and speed with which these adjustments could be made
(e.g., turning knobs, moving sliders, etc.), Stewart also reported success in generating
diphthongs by rapidly shifting the resonance frequencies from one vowel to another.
Although the title of the article suggests otherwise, the circuit was not really an electri-
cal analog of the vocal organs, but rather a means of emulating the acoustic resonances
they produced. The design was essentially the first electrical formant synthesizer; in-
terestingly, however, Stewart did not refer to his system as a synthesizer, but rather as
an electrical analog of the vocal system.
Stewart moved on to a long productive career at Princeton as an astrophysicist and
did not further develop his speech synthesizer. He did, however, leave an insightful
statement at the end of his article that foreshadowed the bane of developing artificial
speech systems for decades to come, and still holds today. He noted that:
The really difficult problem involved in the artificial production of speech
sounds is not the making of the device which shall produce sounds which, in
their fundamental physical basis, resemble those of speech, but in the manip-
ulation of the apparatus to imitate the manifold variations in tone which are
so important in securing naturalness.
(Stewart, 1922, p. 312)
Perhaps by “naturalness” it can be assumed he was referring to the goal of achieving
natural human sound quality as well as intelligibility. In any case, he was clearly aware
of the need to establish “rules” for constructing speech, and that simply building a device
with the appropriate physical characteristics would not in itself advance artificial speech
as a useful technology or tool for research.
8
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
A few years later, in 1928, a communications engineer named Homer Dudley - also
working at the Western Electric Company (later to become Bell Telephone Laborato-
ries) - envisioned a system that could be used to transmit speech across the transatlantic
telegraph cable (Schroeder, 1981). Because it was designed for telegraph signals, how-
ever, the cable had a limited bandwidth of only 100 Hz. In contrast, transmission of the
spectral content of speech requires a minimum bandwidth of about 3000 Hz, and so the
telegraph cable was clearly insufficient for carrying an electrical analog of the speech
waveform. The bandwidth limitation, however, motivated Dudley to view speech pro-
duction and radio transmission analogously. Just as the information content carried by
a radio signal is embedded in the relatively slow modulation of a carrier wave, phonetic
information produced by movements of the lips, tongue, jaw, and velum could be con-
sidered to similarly modulate the sound wave produced by the voice source. That is,
the speech articulators move at inaudible syllabic rates that are well below the 100 Hz
bandwidth of the telegraph cable, whereas the voice source or carrier makes the signal
audible but also creates the need for the much larger bandwidth. Understanding the
difficulties of tracking actual articulatory movements, Dudley instead designed a circuit
that could extract low frequency spectral information from an acoustic speech signal via
a bank of filters, transmit that information along the low-bandwidth cable, and use it to
modulate a locally supplied carrier signal on the receiving end to reconstruct the speech.
This was the first analysis-synthesis system in which some set of parameters determined
by analysis of the original signal could be sent to another location, or perhaps stored for
later retrieval, and used to synthesize a new version of the original speech. Dudley had
achieved almost exactly that which Joseph Henry had imagined in that letter he wrote
long ago about linking together several of Faber’s talking machines to communicate
across a long distance.
Dudley’s invention became known as the VOCODER, an acronym derived from
the two words VOice CODER (to avoid the repetition of capital letters and to reflect
its addition to our lexicon, “Vocoder” will be used in the remainder of the chapter).
The Vocoder was demonstrated publicly for the first time on September 11, 1936 at the
Harvard Tercentary Conference in Cambridge, Massachusetts (Dudley, 1936). During
an address given by F.B. Jewitt, President of Bell Telephone Laboratories, Dudley was
called on to demonstrate the Vocoder to the audience (Jewett, 1936) and showed its
capabilities for analysis and subsequent synthesis of speech and singing. Dudley could
also already see the potential of using the Vocoder for entertainment purposes (Dudley,
1939). He noted that once the low frequency spectral modulation envelopes had been
obtained from speech or song, any signal with sufficiently wide bandwidth could be
9
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
substituted as the carrier in the synthesis stage. For example, instrumental music or the
sound of a train locomotive could be modulated with the spectral-phonetic information
present in a sentence, producing a bizarre but entirely intelligible synthetic version of
the original speech utterance (Dudley, 1940). Ironically, due to the international po-
litical and military events of the late 1930s and early 1940s, the first major application
of the Vocoder was not to amuse audiences, but rather to provide secure, scrambled
speech signals between government and military officials during World War II, par-
ticularly the conversations of Winston Churchill in London and Franklin D. Roosevelt
in Washington, D.C.
One of the difficulties that prevented wide acceptance of Vocoder technology for
general telephone transmission was the problem of accurately extracting pitch (funda-
mental frequency) from an incoming speech signal (Schroeder, 1993). Transmitting
pitch variations along with the other modulation envelopes was essential for recon-
structing natural sounding speech. It was not, however, necessary for transmitting
intelligible speech, and hence could be acceptably used when the security of a conser-
vation was more important than the naturalness of the sound quality. Even so, both
Churchill and Roosevelt complained that the Vocoder made their speech sound silly
(Tompkins, 2010), certainly an undesirable quality for world leaders. Eventually the
pitch extraction problem was solved, other aspects were improved, and Vocoder tech-
nology became a viable means of processing and compressing speech for telephone
transmission.
With the capability of isolating various aspects of speech, Dudley also envisioned
the Vocoder as a tool for research in phonetics and speech science. In 1939, he and
colleagues wrote,
After one believes he has a good understanding of the physical nature of
speech, there comes the acid test of whether he understands the construction
of speech well enough to fashion it from suitably chosen elements.
(Dudley et al., 1939a, p. 740)
Perhaps Dudley realized, much as Stewart (1922) had warned, that building a device
to decompose a speech signal and reconstruct it synthetically was relatively “easy” in
comparison to understanding how the fundamental elements of speech, whatever form
they may take, can actually be generated sequentially by a physical representation of
the speech production system, and result in natural, intelligible speech. With this goal
in mind, he and colleagues modified the Vocoder such that the speech analysis stage
was replaced with manual controls consisting of a keyboard, wrist bar, and foot pedal
10
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
(Dudley, Riesz, and Watkins, 1939). The foot pedal controlled the pitch of a relaxation
oscillator that provided a periodic voice source to be used for the voiced components
of speech; a random noise source supplied the “electrical turbulence” needed for the
unvoiced speech sounds. Each of the ten primary keys controlled the amplitude of the
periodic or noise-like sources within a specific frequency band, which together spanned
a range from 0 to 7500 Hz. By depressing combinations of keys and modulating the
foot pedal, an operator of the device could learn to generate speech.
This new synthetic speaker was called the “VODER” (or “Voder”) a new acronym
that comprised the capitalized letters in “Voice Operation DEmonstratoR” (Dudley,
Riesz, and Watkins, 1939). In a publication of the Bell Laboratories Record (1939), the
machine’s original moniker was “Pedro the Voder,” where the first name was a nod to
Dom Pedro II, a former Emperor of Brazil who famously exclaimed “My God, it talks!”
after witnessing a demonstration of Bell’s invention of the telephone in Philadelphia in
1876. The Bell publication (“Pedro the Voder,” 1939) pointed out that the telephone
did not actually talk, but rather transmitted talk over distance. In contrast, the Voder
did talk and was demonstrated with some fanfare at the 1939 World’s Fair in New York
and at the Golden Gate Exposition in San Francisco the same year. It is interesting that
this publication also states “It is the first machine in the world to do this [i.e., talk]”
(Bell Labs Pubs, 1939, p. 170). If this was a reference to synthetic speech produced by
an electronic artificial talker, it is likely correct. But clearly Joseph Faber had achieved
the same goal by mechanical means almost a century earlier. In fact, the description
of the Voder on the same page as a “little old-fashioned organ with a small keyboard
and a pedal” could have easily been used to describe Faber’s machine. In many ways,
Dudley and colleagues at Bell Labs were cycling back through history with a new form
of technology that would now allow for insights into the construction of speech that
the machines of previous eras would not reveal to their makers.
One of the more interesting aspects of the Voder development, at least from the per-
spective of phonetics, was how people learned to speak with it. Stanley S.A. Watkins,
the third author on the Dudley, Riesz, and Watkins (1939) article describing the Voder
design, was charged with prescribing a training program for a group of people who
would become “operators.” He first studied the ways in which speech sounds were char-
acterized across the ten filter bands (or channels) of Voder. Although this was found
to be useful information regarding speech, it was simply too complex to be useful in
deriving a technique for talking with the Voder. Various other methods of training
were attempted, including templates to guide the fingers and various visual indicators,
but eventually it was determined that the most productive method was for the oper-
11
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
ator to search for a desired speech sound by “playing” with the controls as guided by
their ear. Twenty-four people, drawn from telephone operator pools, were trained
to operate the Voder for the exhibitions at both sites of the 1939 World’s Fair. Typi-
cally, about one year was required to develop the ability to produce intelligible speech
with it. In fact, Dudley et al., wrote “the first half [of the year of training was] spent
in acquiring the ability to form any and all sounds, the second half being devoted to
improving naturalness and intelligibility” (Dudley, Riesz, and Watkins, 1939, p. 763).
Once learned, the ability to “speak” with the Voder was apparently retained for years
afterward, even without continued practice. On the occasion of Homer Dudley’s re-
tirement in 1961, one of the original trained operators was invited back to Bell Labs for
an “encore performance” with a restored version of the talking machine. As recalled
by James Flanagan, a Bell Labs engineer and speech scientist, “She sat down and gave a
virtuoso performance on the Voder” (Pieraccini, 2012, p. 55).
In his article “The Carrier Nature of Speech,” Dudley (1940) made a compelling
analogy of the Voder structure to the human speech production system. But the Voder
was really a spectrum shaping synthesizer: The cutoff frequencies and bandwidths of
the ten filters associated with the keyboard were stationary, and so control was imposed
by allowing the key presses to modulate the signal amplitude within each filter band.
In effect, this provided the operator a means of continuously enhancing or suppress-
ing the ten discrete divisions of the spectrum in some selective pattern such that an
approximation of time-varying formants were generated. It can be noted that Faber’s
mechanical talking machine from a century earlier presented an operator with essen-
tially the same type of interface as the Voder (i.e., keyboard, foot pedal), but it was
the shape of cavities analogous to the human vocal tract that were controlled rather
than the speech spectrum itself. In either case, and like a human acquiring the ability
to speak, the operators of the devices learned and internalized a set of rules for gener-
ating speech by modulating a relatively high-frequency carrier signal (i.e., vocal fold
vibration, turbulence) with slowly varying, and otherwise inaudible, “message waves”
(Dudley, 1940). Although the ability of a human operator to acquire such rules is
highly desirable for performance-driven artificial speech, it would eventually become
a major goal for researchers in speech synthesis to explicate such rules in an attempt
to understand phonology and motor planning in speech production, as well as to de-
velop algorithms for transforming symbolic phonetic representations into speech via
synthetic methods.
The research at Bell Labs that contributed to the Vocoder and Voder occurred in
parallel with development of the “sound spectrograph” (Potter, 1945), a device that
12
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
could graphically represent the time-varying record of the spectrum of a sound rather
than the waveform. The output of the device, called a “spectrogram,” was arranged
such that time and frequency were on the x-axis and y-axis, respectively, and intensity
was coded by varying shades of gray. It could be set to display either the narrowband
harmonic structure of a sound, or the wideband formant patterns. Although develop-
ment of the spectrograph had been initiated by Ralph Potter and colleagues just prior
to the United States’ involvement in World War II, it was given “official rating as a war
project” (Potter, 1945, p. 463) because of its potential to facilitate military communica-
tions and message decoding. During the war, the spectrograph design was refined and
used extensively to study the temporo-spectral patterns of speech based on the “spectro-
grams” that it generated. It wasn’t until several months after the war ended, however,
that the existence of the spectrograph was disclosed to the public. On November 9,
1945, Potter published an article in Science titled “Visible Patterns of Sound” in which
he gave a brief description of the device and explained its potential application as a tool
for studying phonetics, philology, and music. He also suggested its use as an aid for per-
sons who are hearing impaired; the idea was that transforming speech from the auditory
to the visual domain would allow a trained user to “read” speech. Other publications re-
garding the spectrograph soon followed with more detailed descriptions concerning its
design (Koenig, Dunn, and Lacy, 1946; Koenig and Ruppel, 1948) and use (Kopp and
Green, 1946; Steinberg and French, 1946; Potter and Peterson, 1948; Potter, 1949).
Just as instrumentation that allowed researchers to see acoustic speech waveforms
had motivated earlier methods of synthesis (e.g., Miller, 1916), the spectrographic vi-
sualization of speech would rapidly inspire new ways of synthesizing speech, and new
reasons for doing so. Following World War II, Frank Cooper and Alvin Liberman,
researchers at Haskins Laboratories in New York City, had begun extensive analyses
of speech using a spectrograph based on the Bell Labs design. Their goals, which were
initially concerned with building a reading machine for the blind, had been diverted
to investigations of the acoustic structure of speech, and how they were perceived and
decoded by listeners. They realized quickly, however, that many of their questions
could not be answered simply by inspection of spectrograms. What was needed was
a means of modifying some aspect of the visual representation of speech provided by
the spectrogram, and transforming it back into sound so that it could be presented to a
listener as a stimulus. The responses to the stimuli would indicate whether or not the
spectral modification was perceptually relevant.
In 1951, Cooper, Liberman, and Borst reported on the design of a device that would
allow the user to literally draw a spectrographic representation of a speech utterance on
13
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
a film transparency and transform it into a sound wave via a system including a light
source, tone wheel, photocell, and amplifier. The tone wheel contained 50 circular
sound tracks that, when turned by a motor at 1800 rpm, would modulate light to gen-
erate harmonic frequencies from 120-6000 Hz, roughly covering the speech spectrum.
The photocell would receive only the portions of spectrum corresponding to the pattern
that had been drawn on the film, and convert them to an electrical signal which could
be amplified and played through a loudspeaker. The “drawn” spectrographic pattern
could be either a copy or modification of an actual spectrogram, and hence the device
came to be known as the “Pattern Playback.” It was used to generate stimuli for nu-
merous experiments on speech perception and contributed greatly to knowledge and
theoretical views on how speech is decoded (cf., Liberman, Delattre, and Cooper, 1952,
1954, 1957; Liberman et al., 1957; Harris et al., 1958; Liberman, Delattre, and Cooper,
1967). The Pattern Playback was the first speech synthesizer used for large-scale sys-
tematic experimentation concerning the structure of speech, and proved to be most
useful for investigations concerning isolated acoustic cues such as formant transitions at
the onset and offset of consonants (Delattre et al., 1952; Borst, 1956).
The usefulness of speech synthesizers as research tools was summarized in a review
article by Cooper (1961) in which he wrote:
The essential point here, as in all of science, is that we must simplify Na-
ture if we are to understand her. More than that: we must somehow choose
a particular set of simplifying assumptions from the many sets that are possi-
ble. The great virtue of speech synthesizers is that they can help us make this
choice.
(Cooper, 1961, p. 4)
The Pattern Playback served the purpose of “simplifying Nature” by making the
spectrotemporal characteristics of speech accessible and manipulable to the researcher.
When used in this manner, a speech synthesizer becomes an experimenter’s “versatile
informant” that allows for testing hypotheses about the significance of various spectral
features (Cooper, 1961, pp. 4-5). One of advantages of the Pattern Playback was that
virtually anything could be drawn (or painted) on the film transparency regardless of the
complexity or simplicity, and it could be heard. For example, all of the detail observed
for a speech utterance in a spectrogram could be reconstructed, or something as simple
as a sinusoid could be drawn as a straight line over time. The disadvantage was that
the only means of generating an utterance, regardless of the accuracy of the prescribed
rules, was for someone to actually draw it by hand.
14
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
The users of the Pattern Playback became quite good at drawing spectrographic
patterns that generated intelligible speech, even when they had not previously seen an
actual spectrogram of the utterance to be synthesized (Delattre et al., 1952; Liberman et
al., 1959). Much like the operators of the speaking machines that preceded them, they
had, through practice, acquired or internalized a set of rules for generating speech.
Delattre et al. (1952) did attempt to characterize some speech sounds with regard to
how they might be drawn spectrographically, but it was Frances Ingemann who for-
malized rules for generating utterances with the Pattern Playback (Ingemann, 1957;
Liberman et al., 1959). The rules were laid out according to place, manner, and voic-
ing, and could be presumably used by a novice to draw and generate a given utterance.
Although the process would have been extremely time consuming and tedious, Mat-
tingly (1974) notes that this was the first time that explicit rules for generating speech
with a synthesizer had been formally documented.
Other types of synthesizers were also developed during this period that facilitated
production of artificial speech based on acoustic characteristics observed in a spectro-
gram, but were based on different principles than the Pattern Playback. In 1953, Wal-
ter Lawrence, a researcher for the Signals Research and Development Establishment in
Christchurch, England, introduced a speech synthesizer whose design consisted of an
electrical circuit with a source function generator and three parallel resonant branches.
The frequency of each resonance could be controlled by the user, as could the frequency
and amplitude of the source function. Together, the source and resonant branches pro-
duced a waveform with a time-varying spectrum that could be compared to a spectro-
gram, or modified for purposes of determining the perceptual relevance of an acoustic
cue. Because the parameters of the circuit (i.e., resonance frequencies, source fun-
damental frequency, etc.) were under direct control, Lawrence’s synthesizer became
known as the “Parametric Artificial Talker” or “PAT” for short. PAT was used by Pe-
ter Ladefoged and David Broadbent to provide acoustic stimuli for their well-known
study of the effects of acoustic context on vowel perception (Ladefoged and Broadbent,
1957).
At about the same time, Gunnar Fant was also experimenting with resonant circuits
for speech synthesis at the Royal Institute of Technology (KTH) in Stockholm. In-
stead of placing electrical resonators in parallel as Lawrence did in building PAT, Fant
configured them in a series or “cascade” arrangement. Fant’s first cascade synthesizer,
called “OVE I,” an acronym based on the words “Orator Verbis Electris,” was primarily
a vowel synthesizer that had the unique feature of a mechanical stylus that could be
moved in a two-dimensional plane for control of the lowest two resonance frequencies.
15
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
A user could then generate speech (vowels and vowel transitions) by moving the sty-
lus in the vowel space defined by the first two-formant frequencies, a system that may
have had great value for teaching and learning the phonetics of vowels. It may have
had some entertainment value as well. Fant (2005) reminisced that one of the three
“opponents” at his doctoral dissertation defense in 1958 was, in fact, Walter Lawrence
who had brought his PAT synthesizer with him to Stockholm. At one point during
the defense proceedings Fant and Lawrence demonstrated a synthesizer dialogue be-
tween PAT and OVE I. Eventually, Fant developed a second version of the cascade-type
synthesizer called “OVE II” (Fant and Martony, 1962). The main enhancements were
additional subsystems to allow for production of nasals, stops, and fricatives, as well as a
conductive ink device for providing time-varying parameter values to the synthesizer.
The development of PAT and OVE set the stage for a category of artificial speech
that would eventually be referred to as formant synthesis, because they provided for
essentially direct control of the formants observed in a spectrogram. In a strict sense,
however, they are resonance synthesizers because the parameters control, among other
things, the frequencies of the electrical (or later, digital) resonators themselves. In most
cases, though, these frequencies are aligned with the center frequency of a formant, and
hence resonance frequency and formant frequency become synonymous. Although it
may seem like a minor technological detail, the question of whether such synthesiz-
ers should be designed with parallel or cascaded resonators would be debated for years
to come. A parallel system offers the user the largest amount control over the spec-
trum because both the resonator frequencies and amplitudes are set with parameters.
In contrast, in a cascade system the resonance frequencies are set by a user, while their
amplitudes are an effect of the superposition of multiple resonances, much as is the case
for the human vocal tract (Flanagan, 1957). Thus, the cascade approach could po-
tentially produce more natural sounding speech, but with somewhat of a sacrifice in
control. Eventually, Lawrence reconfigured PAT with a cascade arrangement of res-
onators, but after many years of experimentation with both parallel and cascade systems,
John Holmes of the Joint Speech Research Unit in the U.K. later made a strong case
for a parallel arrangement (Holmes, 1983). He noted that replication of natural speech
is considerably more accurate with user control of both formant frequencies and their
amplitudes.
Simultaneous with the development of formant synthesizers in the 1950s, was an-
other type of synthesis approach, also based on electrical circuits, but intended to serve
as a model of the shape of the vocal tract so that the relation of articulatory configura-
tion to sound production could be more effectively studied. The first of this type was
16
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
described in 1950 by H.K. Dunn, another Bell Labs engineer. Instead of building reso-
nant circuits to replicate formants, Dunn (1950) designed an electrical transmission line
in which consecutive (and coupled) “T-sections,” made up of capacitors, inductors, and
resistors, were used as analogs of the pharyngeal and oral air cavities within the vocal
tract. The values of the circuit elements within each T-section were directly related to
the cross-sectional area and length of the various cavities, and thus the user now had
parametric control of the vocal tract shape. Although this was an advance, the vocal
tract configurations that could be effectively simulated with Dunn’s circuit were fairly
crude representations of the human system.
Stevens, Kasowski, and Fant (1953), in their article “An Electrical Analog of the
Vocal Tract,” describe a variation on Dunn’s design using a similar transmission line
approach; however, they were able to represent the vocal tract shape as a concatenation
of 35 cylindrical sections, where each section was 0.5 cm in length. The purpose in pur-
suing a more detailed representation of the vocal tract shape was to be able to “study in
detail the mechanism of speech production and to investigate correlations between the
acoustic and articulatory aspects of speech” and noted that “a speech synthesizer would
be required to simulate more closely the actual dimensions of the vocal tract” (p. 735).
Fant also began work on his own version of a Line Electrical Analog (LEA) that he used
for studies of speech sounds. Both Stevens and House (1955) and Fant (1960) used these
very similar synthesizers to better understand vowel articulation by first developing a
three parameter model of the vocal tract shape in which the location and radius of the
primary vowel constriction were specified, along with the ratio of lip termination area
to lip tube length. Their synthesizers allowed for a systematic exploration of the para-
metric space and resulted in nomographic displays that demonstrated the importance
of the location (place) and cross-sectional area of the primary vocal tract constriction in
vowels. Collectively, this work significantly altered the view of vowel production.
A limitation of both the Stevens, Kasowski, and Fant (1953) and Fant (1960) line ana-
log synthesizers was that they could not generate time-varying speech sounds because
they accommodated only static vocal tract configurations; i.e., they couldn’t actually
talk. Using a more complex line analog circuit system and a bank of switches, George
Rosen, a doctoral student at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, devised a means
of transitioning from one vocal tract configuration to another (Rosen, 1958). This new
system, known as “DAVO” for “dynamic analog of the vocal tract,” could generate fairly
clear diphthongs and consonant-vowel (CV) syllables, but was not capable of sentence-
level speech.
It can be noted that the parametric models of the vocal tract shape developed by
17
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
Stevens and House (1955) and Fant (1960) were independent of the transmission line
analogs that were used to produce the actual synthetic speech. The limitation of only
static vowels, or CVs in the case of DAVO, was entirely due to the need for a complicated
electrical circuit to simulate the propagation of acoustic waves in the vocal tract. The
vocal tract models themselves could have easily been used to generate time-dependent
configurations over the time course of a phrase or sentence, but a system for producing
the corresponding synthetic speech waveform with such temporal variation simply did
not yet exist, nor did the knowledge of how to specify the time-dependence of the
vocal tract parameters.
Yet another type of speech synthesizer was also under development during the
1950s. With significant improvements in the state of audio recording technology,
particularly those related to storing speech waveforms on magnetic tape, it was now
possible to consider synthesis - perhaps in the purest sense of the word - based on splic-
ing together small segments of prerecorded natural speech. Harris (1953a) designed a
system in which segments of tape were isolated that contained many instances (allo-
phones) of each consonant and vowel. Then, with a recording drum, tape loop, and
timing and selector circuits (Harris, 1953b), synthetic speech could be generated by
piecing together a sequence of segments deemed to match well with regard to formant
frequencies and harmonics. The speech produced was found to be fairly intelligible
but quite unnatural, presumably because of the discontinuities created at the segment
boundaries.
Rather than focusing on vowel and consonant segments, Peterson, Wang, and Sivert-
sen (1958) experimented with alternative segmentation techniques and determined that
a more useful unit for synthesizing speech could be obtained from segments extending
in time from the steady-state location of one phoneme to the next. Referring to this
unit as a “dyad,” they suggested that it preserved the acoustic dynamics of the transi-
tions between phonemes, precisely the information lost when the segmentation unit
is the phoneme itself. The potential of this method to generate intelligible speech was
demonstrated by Wang and Peterson (1958) where they constructed a sentence from
more than 40 dyad segments extracted from previously recorded utterances. Much
care was required in selecting the segments, however, in order to maintain continuity
of pitch, intensity, tempo, and vocal quality. The range of phonetic characteristics that
can be generated in synthetic speech by concatenating segments is, of course, limited
by the segment inventory that is available. Sivertsen (1961) conducted an extensive
study of the size of inventory needed relative to the type of segmentation unit chosen.
She considered various segments with a wide range of temporal extent that included
18
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
phonemes, phoneme dyads, syllable nuclei, half syllables, syllables, syllable dyads, and
words, and found that, in general, “the size of the inventory increases with the length
of the segment” (Sivertsen, 1961, p. 57). That is, a few small units can be combined
in many ways to generate hundreds or thousands of increasingly larger units, but if
the starting point is a large temporal unit, an enormous number is needed because the
possibilities for recombining them are severely limited.
This approach to synthesis clearly has played a major role in technological appli-
cations such as modern text-to-speech systems utilizing unit selection techniques (cf.,
Moulines and Charpentier, 1990; Sagisaka et al., 1992; Hunt and Black, 1996), but
Sivertsen (1961) also made a strong case for the use of segment concatenation meth-
ods as a research tool. In particular, she noted that using stored segments of various
lengths can be used for evaluating some theories of linguistic structure, as well as for
investigating segmentation of speech signals in general. In fact, Sivertsen suggested
that essentially all speech synthesis methods could be categorized relative to how the
speech continuum is segmented. If the segmentation is conceived as “simultaneous
components” then speech can be synthesized by controlling various parametric repre-
sentations “independently and simultaneously.” These may be physiological parame-
ters such as vocal tract shape, location and degree of constriction, nasal coupling, and
laryngeal activity, or acoustical parameters such as formant frequencies, formant band-
widths, fundamental frequency, voice spectrum, and amplitude. If, instead, the speech
continuum in segmented in time, synthetic speech can be accomplished by sequencing
successive “building blocks,” which may be extracted from recorded natural speech or
even generated electronically.
The advent of digital computing in the early 1960s would dramatically change the
implementation of speech synthesizers, and the means by which they may be con-
trolled. The underlying principles of the various synthesis methods, however, are often
the same or least similar to those that motivated development of mechanical, electri-
cal, or electronic talking devices. Thus, delineation of synthesizer type relative to the
segmentation of the speech continuum is particularly useful for understanding the dif-
ferences and potential uses of the wide range of synthetic speech systems that had so far
been advanced at the time, and also for those yet to be developed.
The digital and computational era
As Stewart (1922) had suggested in the early days of electrical circuit-based syn-
thesis, building a device capable of producing sounds that resemble speech is far less
difficult than knowing how to impose the proper control on its parameters to make the
19
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
device actually talk. Although much progress had been made in development of vari-
ous types of systems, controlling electronic speech synthesizers by manipulating circuit
parameters, whether they were vocal tract or terminal analog types, was cumbersome
and tedious. This could now be mitigated to some degree, however, by engaging a
digital computer to control speech synthesizers that were, themselves, still realized as
electronic circuits. That is, commands typed on a keyboard could be transformed into
control voltages that imposed parameter changes in the synthesis circuitry. In effect,
this allowed the “computational load” for generating the speech waveform to remain
in the analog circuit, while transferring the control of the system to a user via a com-
puter interface. It would not be long, however, before the hardware synthesizers were
replaced with software realizations of the same circuit elements, offering far greater
flexibility and ease with which synthetic speech could be generated.
Digital control facilitated development of “speech synthesis by rule” in which an
orthographic representation of an utterance could be transformed into artificial speech.
Based on a set of “rules” embedded in a computer program, a series of symbols repre-
senting the phonetic elements of a word or phrase were converted to temporal variations
of the parameters of a specific type of synthesizer. For example, Holmes, Mattingly, and
Shearme (1964) described the rules and associated computer program that calculated
the time course of the parameters of a parallel resonance (formant) synthesizer. These
included, among other variables, frequencies and amplitudes of three resonances, and
fundamental frequency. A word such as “you” (/ju/) might be produced with a sim-
ple interpolation of the second formant frequency, F2, from a high value, say 2200
Hz, down to a much lower value, perhaps 400 Hz, while other parameters could be
held constant. The interpolated F2 would then be used to alter the settings of circuit
elements over a particular period of time, resulting in a speech waveform resembling
“you.”
A similar goal of producing “synthetic speech from an input consisting only of the
names of phonemes and a minimum of pitch and timing information” was pursued by
Kelly and Lochbaum (1962, p. 1), but in their system a digital lattice filter, entirely
realized as a computer algorithm, was used to calculate the effective propagation of
acoustic waves in an analog of the vocal tract configuration. Control of the system
required specification of 21 time-varying cross-sectional areas representing the vocal
tract shape along the axis extending from the glottis to lips, as well as nasal coupling,
fundamental frequency, aspiration, and affrication. Each phoneme was assigned a vocal
tract shape (i.e., 21 cross-sectional areas) read from lookup table; change in tract shape
was accomplished by linearly interpolating, over time, each cross-sectional area speci-
20
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
fied for one phoneme to those of the next phoneme. This design functioned essentially
as a digital version of a synthesizer like George Rosen’s DAVO; but because it was soft-
ware rather than hardware, it allowed for more precise specification of the vocal tract
shape and almost endless possibilities for experimentation with interpolation types and
associated rules.
Kelly and Lochbaum expressed disappointment in the sound quality of the speech
generated by their system, but attributed the problem to inadequate knowledge of the
cross-sectional areas that were used as the vocal tract shapes corresponding to phoneme
targets. Although based on Fant’s (1960) well-known collection of vocal tract data
obtained from X-ray images, it would not be until the 1990s when imaging meth-
ods would allow for three-dimensional reconstructions of vocal tract shapes produced
by human talkers (cf., Baer et al., 1991; Story, Titze, and Hoffman, 1996, 1998), and
hence, improve this aspect of analog vocal tract synthesis. The time-varying spatial
characteristics of a linearly interpolated vocal tract shape were, however, also potential
contributors to the undesirable quality of the synthesis. A more complex set of rules for
control of a vocal tract analog was described a few years later by Nakata and Mitsuoka
(1965), and resulted in intelligible speech with “fairly good naturalness.” Nonetheless,
they, along with others believed that significant improvements in vocal tract analog
synthesis required more detailed knowledge of realistic articulatory movement from
which better timing rules could be derived.
By the 1960s, X-ray cineradiography technology had developed to a point where
the spatial and temporal resolution were suitable for studying the articulatory move-
ments of speech in a sagittal projection image. Motion picture X-ray films collected
for various speech utterances could be analyzed frame by frame to track the changing
positions of articulators and the time-varying configuration of the vocal tract outline.
Just as the instrumentation that allowed scientists to see waveforms and spectrograms
had motivated earlier forms of synthetic speech, the ability to now see the movement
of the articulators motivated development of a new type of synthesis paradigm called
“articulatory synthesis.”
In 1967, Cecil Coker of Bell Laboratories demonstrated a synthesis system based on a
computational model of the speech articulators. Simplified positions of the tongue, jaw,
lips, velum, and larynx were represented in the midsagittal plane, where each could
be specified to move with a particular timing function. The result was a time-varying
configuration of the midsagittal vocal tract outline. To produce the speech waveform,
the distances across the vocal tract airspace from glottis to lips at each time sample first
needed to be converted to cross-sectional areas to form the area function (cf., Heinz and
21
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
Stevens, 1964). These were then used in a vocal tract analog model like that of Kelly and
Lochbaum (1962) to calculate wave propagation through the system. The resonance
frequencies could also be calculated directly from the time-varying area function and
used to control a formant synthesizer (Coker and Fujimura, 1966). Similar articulatory
models were developed by Lindblom and Sundberg (1971) and Mermelstein (1973),
but incorporated somewhat more complexity in the articulatory geometry. In any case,
the temporal characteristics of the synthesized articulatory movement could be com-
pared to, and refined with, data extracted from midsagittal cineradiography films (e.g.,
Truby, 1965). An articulatory synthesizer developed at Haskins Laboratories called
“ASY” (Rubin, Baer, and Mermelstein, 1981), extended the Mermelstein model, incor-
porating several additional sub-models and an approach based on key frame animation
for synthesizing utterances derived from control of the movement over time of the vo-
cal tract. This was one of the earliest articulatory synthesis tools used for large-scale
laboratory phonetic experiments (e.g., Abramson et al., 1981). It was later enhanced
to provide more accurate representations of the underlying vocal tract parameters and
flexibility in their control by a user (the Haskins Configurable Articulatory Synthesizer,
or CASY; see Rubin et al., 1996).
Articulatory synthesis held much promise because it was assumed that the rules re-
quired to generate speech would be closer to those used by a human talker than rules
developed for controlling acoustic parameters such as formant frequencies. While that
may ultimately be the case, such rules have been difficult to define in such a way that
natural sounding, intelligible speech is consistently generated (Klatt, 1987). Articula-
tory synthesizers have become important tools for research, however, because they can
serve as a model of speech production in which the acoustic consequences of parametric
variation of an articulator can be investigated. Using the ASY synthesizer (Rubin, Baer,
and Mermelstein, 1981) to produce speech output, Browman et al. (1984), Browman
and Goldstein (e.g., 1985, 1991), Saltzman (1986, 1991), and others at Haskins Labora-
tories embarked on research to understand articulatory control. Guiding this work was
the hypothesis that phonetic structure could be characterized explicitly as articulatory
movement patterns, or “gestures.” Their system allowed for specification of an utterance
as a temporal schedule of “tract variables,” such as location and degree of a constriction
formed by the tongue tip or tongue body, lip aperture, and protrusion, as well as states
of the velum and glottis. These were then transformed into a task-dynamic system that
accounted for the coordination and dynamic linkages among articulators required to
carry out a specified gesture. Over the years, techniques for estimating the time course
of gesture specification have continued to be enhanced. Recently, for example, Nam et
22
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
al. (2012) reported a method for estimating gestural “scores” from the acoustic signal
based on an iterative analysis-by-synthesis approach.
Some developers of articulatory synthesis systems focused their efforts on particular
subsystems such as the voice source. For example, Flanagan and Landgraf (1968) pro-
posed a simple mass-spring-damper model of the vocal folds that demonstrated, with
a computational model, the self-oscillating nature of the human vocal folds. Shortly
thereafter, a more complex two-mass version was described (Ishizaka and Flanagan,
1972; Ishizaka and Matsudaira, 1972) that clearly showed the importance of the vertical
phase difference (mucosal wave) in facilitating vocal fold oscillation. Additional degrees
of freedom were added to the anterior-posterior dimension of the vocal folds by Titze
(1973, 1974) with a 16-mass model. Although the eventual goal of subsystem mod-
eling would be integration into a full speech synthesis system (cf., Flanagan, Ishizaka,
and Shipley, 1975; Sondhi and Schroeter, 1987), much of their value is as a tool for un-
derstanding the characteristics of the subsystem itself. Maeda (1988, 1990), Dang and
Honda (2004), and Birkholz (2013) are all examples of more recent attempts to inte-
grate models of subsystems into an articulatory speech synthesizer, whereas Guenther
(cf., 1994) and Kr´
’oger et al. (2010) have augmented such synthesizers with auditory
feedback and learning algorithms. The main use of these systems has been to study
some aspect of speech production, but not necessarily the conversion of a symbolic rep-
resentation of an utterance into speech. It can also be noted that a natural extension of
articulatory synthesis is the inclusion of facial motion that coincides with speaking, and
has led to development audiovisual synthetic speech systems that can be used explore
multi- modal nature of both speech production and perception (cf., Yehia, Rubin, and
Vatikiotis-Bateson, 1998; Massaro, 1998; Vatikiotis-Bateson et al., 2000). This area will
be covered in the chapter entitled “New horizons in clinical phonetics.”
Other researchers focused on enhancing models of the vocal tract analogs without
adding the complexity of articulatory components. Strube (1982), Liljencrants (1985),
and Story (1995) all refined the digital lattice filter approach of Kelly and Lochbaum
(1962) to better account for the acoustic properties of time-varying vocal tract shapes
and various types of energy losses. Based on the relation of small perturbations of a
tubular configuration to changes in the acoustic resonance frequencies, Mrayati, Carré,
and Guérin (1988) proposed that speech could be produced by controlling the time-
varying cross-sectional area of eight distinct regions of the vocal tract. The idea was
that expansion or constriction of these particular regions would maximize the change in
resonance frequencies, thus providing an efficient means of controlling the vocal tract
to generate a predictable acoustic output. Some years later a set of rules was developed
23
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
by Hill, Manzara, and Schock (1995) that specified the transformation of text input into
region parameters and were used to build a vocal tract-based text-to-speech synthesizer.
In a similar vein, Story (2005, 2013) has described an “airway modulation model” of
speech production (called “TubeTalker”) in which an array of functions can be activated
over time to deform the vocal tract, nasal tract, and glottal airspaces to produce speech.
Although this model produces highly intelligible speech, its primary use is for studying
the relation of structure and movement to the acoustic characteristics produced, and
the perceptual response of listeners (cf., Story and Bunton, 2010).
In parallel with development of articulatory-type synthesizers was the enhancement
of resonance or formant-based synthesis systems. Along with numerous colleagues,
Dennis Klatt’s research on digital resonators as well as his studies on the acoustic char-
acteristics of nearly all aspects of speech, led to a comprehensive system of rule-based
formant synthesis. With various names such as “Klattalk,” “MITalk,” “DecTalk,” and
later “KLSYN88,” this type of text-to-speech system has become well known to the
public, particularly because of its collection of standard voices (cf., Klatt, 1982, 1987;
Klatt and Klatt, 1990). Perhaps best known today is “Perfect Paul,” the voice that was
synonymous with the late British physicist Stephen Hawking who used the synthesizer
as an augmentative speaking device. Formant synthesis can also be combined with ar-
ticulatory methods. “HLSyn,” developed by Hanson and Stevens (2002), is a system
designed to superimpose high level (HL) articulatory control on the Klatt formant syn-
thesizer. This approach simplified the control scheme by mapping 13 physiologically
based HL parameters to the 40-50 acoustic parameters that control the formant syn-
thesis. The advantage is that the articulatory parameters constrain the output so that
physiologically unrealistic combinations of the voice source and vocal tract filter cannot
occur. This type of synthesizer can serve as another tool for studying speech production
with regard to both research and educational purposes.
Throughout this chapter, it has been presumed that regardless of the reasons for
developing a particular type of synthesizer, at some level, the goal was to generate
high-quality, intelligible speech. Some synthesizers have been developed, however, for
the explicit purpose of degrading or modifying natural, recorded speech. Such synthe-
sizers are useful for investigating speech perception because they allow researchers to
systematically remove many of the acoustic characteristics present in the signal while
preserving only those portions hypothesized to be essential cues. Remez et al. (1981)
described a synthesis technique in which the first three formant frequencies tracked
over the duration of a sentence, were replaced by the summation of three tones whose
frequencies were swept upward and downward to match the temporal variation of the
24
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
formants. Although the quality of the synthesized sound was highly artificial (per-
haps otherworldly), listeners were able to identify the sentences as long as the tones
were played simultaneously, and not in isolation of one another, revealing the power
of speech cues that are embedded in the dynamic spectral patterns of the vocal tract
resonances. Shannon et al. (1995) showed that intelligible speech could alternatively
be synthesized by preserving temporal cues, while virtually eliminating spectral infor-
mation. Their approach was essentially the same as Dudley’s Vocoder (1939) in which
the speech signal was first filtered into a set of frequency bands, and time-varying am-
plitude envelopes were extracted from each band over the duration of the recorded
speech. The difference was that the number of bands ranged from only one to four,
and the amplitude envelopes were used modulate a noise signal rather than an estima-
tion of the voice source. Shannon et al., showed that listeners were adept at decoding
sentence-level speech with only three bands of modulated noise. Similarly designed
synthesizers (e.g., Loizou, Dorman, and Tu, 1999) have been used simulate the signal
processing algorithms in cochlear implant devices for purposes of investigating speech
perception abilities of listeners under these conditions. Yet another variation on this
type of synthesis was reported by Smith, Delgutte, and Oxenham (2002) who devel-
oped a technique to combine the spectral fine structure of one type of sound with the
temporal variation of another to generate “auditory chimeras.” These have been shown
to be useful for investigating aspects of auditory perception.
Many other types of speech synthesis methods have been developed in the digital
era whose primary purpose is to generate high quality speech for automated messaging
or be embodied in a digital assistant that converses with a user. These systems typically
make use of synthesis techniques that build speech signals from information available
in a database containing many hours of recordings of one or more voice professionals
who produced a wide range of spoken content and vocal qualities. The “unit selection”
technique, also referred to as “concatenative synthesis,” is essentially the digital realiza-
tion of the tape splicing method of Harris (1953b) and Peterson, Wang, and Sivertsen
(1958), but now involves a set of algorithms that efficiently search the database for small
sound segments, typically at the level of diphones, that can be stacked serially in time
to generate a spoken message. A different technique, called “parametric synthesis,”
relies on an extensive analysis of the spectral characteristics of speech recordings in a
database to establish parametric representations that can later be used to reconstruct a
segment of speech (Zen, Tokuda, and Black, 2009). Unit selection typically produces
more natural sounding speech but is limited by the quality and size of the original
database. Parametric synthesis allows for greater flexibility with regard to modifica-
25
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
tion of voice characteristics, speaking style, and emotional content, but generally is of
lower overall quality. Both techniques have been augmented with implementation of
deep learning algorithms that improve the efficiency and accuracy of constructing a
spoken utterance, as well as increasing the naturalness and intelligibility of the syn-
thetic speech (Zen, Senior, and Schuster, 2013; Capes et al., 2017). More recently,
a new approach called direct waveform modeling has been introduced that utilizes a
deep neural network (DNN) to generate new speech signals based on learned features
of recorded speech (cf., van den Oord et al., 2016; Arik et al., 2017). This method has
the potential to significantly enhance the quality and naturalness of synthetic speech
over current systems, even though it is currently computationally expensive. It can be
noted, however, that because unit selection, parametric, and direct waveform synthe-
sizers construct speech signals based on underlying principles that are not specifically
related to the ways in which a human forms speech, they are perhaps less useful as a
tool for testing hypotheses about speech production and perception than many of the
other techniques discussed in this chapter.
Summary
For centuries, past to present, humans have been motivated to build machines that
talk. Other than the novelty, what is the purpose of speech synthesis, and what can be
done with it? Certainly, technological applications have resulted from development of
these devices, many of them having a major impact on how humans communicate with
each other. Mattingly (1974) seems to have hit it just about right when he suggested that
the “traditional motivation for research in speech synthesis” has been simply the desire
to explain the mystery of how we humans successfully use our vocal tracts to produce
connected speech. In other words, the primary means of scientifically investigating
speech production has been based on building artificial talking systems and collecting
relevant data with which to refine them. Mattingly (1974) also points out that, re-
gardless of the underlying principles of the synthetic speech system built, the scientific
questions are almost always concerned with deriving the “rules” that govern produc-
tion of intelligible speech. Such rules may be elaborate and explicitly stated algorithms
for transforming a string of text into speech based on a particular type of synthesizer,
or more subtly implied as general movements of structures or acoustic characteristics.
In any case, achieving an understanding of the rules and all their variations, can be
regarded as synonymous with understanding many aspects of speech production and
perception. As in any area of science, the goal in studying speech has been to first de-
termine the important facts about the system. Artificial talkers and speech synthesizers
26
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
embody these “facts,” but they typically capture just the essential aspects of speech. As
a result, synthesis often presents itself as an aural caricature that can be perceived as an
unnatural, and sometimes amusing rendition of a desired utterance or speech sound.
It is particularly unique to phonetics and speech science that the models used as tools
to understand the scientific aspects of a complex system produce a signal intended to
be heard as if it were a human. As such, the quality of speech synthesis can be rather
harshly judged because the model on which it is based has not accounted for the myriad
of subtle variations and details that combine in natural human speech. Thus, we should
keep in mind that the degree to which we can produce convincing artificial speech is a
measure of the degree to which we understand human speech production.
Acknowledgments
The research was supported in part by NIH R01 DC011275 and NSF BCS-1145011.
References
Abramson, A. S., Nye, P. W., Henderson, J. B., and Marshall, C. W., 1981. Vowel
height and the perception of consonantal nasality, Journal of the Acoustical Society
of America, 70, pp. 329-339.
Altick, R. D., 1978. The shows of London, Cambridge, MA and London: Belknap
Press of Harvard University Press, pp. 355-356.
Arik, S. O., Chrzanowski, M., Coates, A., Diamos, G., Gibiansky, A., Kang, Y., Li,
X., Miller, J., Ng, A., Raiman, J., Sengupta, S., and Shoeybi, M., 2017. Deep voice:
Real-time neural text-to-speech. arXiv:1702.07825 [Accessed 5 March 2018].
Athenaeum, 1846, Our weekly gossip, British Periodicals, July 25, 978, p. 765.
Baer, T., Gore, J. C., Gracco, L. C., and Nye, P. W., 1991. Analysis of vocal tract
shape and dimensions using magnetic resonance imaging: Vowels, Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America, 90, pp. 799-828.
Baldwin Jewett, F., 1936. The social implications of scientific research in electrical
communication, The Scientific Monthly, 43, pp. 466-476.
Barnum, P. T., 1886. Life of P. T. Barnum written by himself, Buffalo, NY: The
Courier Company Printers, p. 134.
27
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
Birkholz, P., 2013. Modeling consonant-vowel coarticulation for articulatory
speech synthesis, PLoS One, 84, e60603, pp. 1-17. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060603.
Borst, J. M., 1956. The use of spectrograms for speech analysis and synthesis,
Journal of the Audio Engineering Society, 4, pp. 14-23.
Browman, C. P., and Goldstein, L. M., 1985. Dynamic modeling of phonetic
structure, Phonetic Linguistics, pp. 35-53.
Browman, C. P., and Goldstein, L. M., 1991. Tiers in articulatory phonology,
with some implications for casual speech, In J. Kingston and M. E. Beckman (Eds.),
Papers in laboratory phonology I, between the grammar and the physics of speech,
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 341-376.
Browman, C. P., Goldstein, L. M., Kelso, J. A. S., Rubin, P. E., and Saltzman, E.,
1984. Articulatory synthesis from underlying dynamics, Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America, 75, p. S22.
Capes, T., Coles, P., Conkie, A., Golipour, L., Hadjitarkhani, A., Hu, Q., Hud-
dleston, N., Hunt, M., Li, J., Neeracher, M., Prahallad, K, Raitio, T., Rasipuram,
R., Townsend, G., Williamson, B., Winarsky, D., Wu, X., and Zhang, H., 2017.
Siri On-device deep learning-guided unit selection text-to- speech system, Pro-
ceedings of Interspeech 2017, pp. 4011-4015.
Carroll, C. M., 1975. The great chess automaton, New York: Dover Publications.
Coker, C. H., 1967. Synthesis by rule from articulatory parameters, Proceedings
of the 1967 Conference on Speech Communication Processes, Cambridge, MA:
IEEE, A9, pp. 52-53.
Coker, C. H., and Fujimura, O., 1966. Model for specification of the vocal tract
area function, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 40(5), pp. 1271-1271.
Cooper, F. S., 1961. Speech synthesizers, Proceedings of the 4th International
Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS’61), pp. 3-13.
Cooper, F. S., Liberman, A. M., and Borst, J. M., 1951. The interconversion
of audible and visible patterns as a basis for research in the perception of speech,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 37(5), pp. 318-325.
Dang, J., and Honda, K., 2004. Construction and control of a physiological artic-
ulatory model, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 115, pp. 853-870.
28
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
Delattre, P., Cooper, F. S., Liberman, A. M., and Gerstman, L. J., 1952. Speech
synthesis as a research technique, Proceedings of the Vllth International Congress
of Linguists, London, pp. 543-561.
Dudley, H., 1936. Synthesizing speech, Bell Laboratories Record, 15, pp. 98-102.
Dudley, H., 1939. The vocoder, Bell Laboratories Record, 18(4), pp. 122-126.
Dudley, H., 1940. The carrier nature of speech, Bell System Technical Journal,
19(4), pp. 495-515.
Dudley, H., Riesz, R. R., and Watkins, S. S. A., 1939. A synthetic speaker, Journal
of the Franklin Institute, 227(6), pp. 739-764.
Dudley, H., and Tarnoczy, T. H., 1950. The speaking machine of Wolfgang von
Kempelen, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 22(2), pp. 151-166.
Dunn, H. K., 1950. The calculation of vowel resonances and an electrical vocal
tract, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 22(6), pp. 740-753.
Euler, L., 1761. The wonders of the human voice, Letters of Euler on different
subjects in natural philosophy addressed to German Princess, David Brewster, ed.,
New York: J.J. Harper (publisher in 1833), pp. 76-79.
Fant, G., 1960. Acoustic theory of speech production, The Hague: Mouton.
Fant, G., 2005. Historical notes, Speech Transmission Laboratory Quarterly Progress
and Status Report (KTH), 47(1), pp. 9-19.
Fant, G., and Martony, J., 1962. Speech synthesis instrumentation for paramet-
ric synthesis (OVE II), Speech Transmission Laboratory Quarterly Progress and
Status Report (KTH), 2, pp. 18-24.
Flanagan, J. L., 1957. Note on the design of “terminal-analog” speech synthesizers,
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 29(2), pp. 306-310.
Flanagan, J. L., Ishizaka, K., and Shipley, K. L., 1975. Synthesis of speech from a
dynamic model of the vocal cords and vocal tract, Bell System Technical Journal,
54(3), 485-506.
Flanagan, J. L., and Landgraf, L., 1968. Self-oscillating source for vocal-tract syn-
thesizers, IEEE Transactions on Audio and Electroacoustics, 16(1), pp. 57-64.
Guenther, F. H., 1994. A neural network model of speech acquisition and motor
equivalent speech production, Biological Cybernetics, 72(1), 43-53.
29
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
Hanson, H. M., and Stevens, K. N., 2002. A quasiarticulatory approach to control-
ling acoustic source parameters in a Klatt-type formant synthesizer using HLsyn,
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 112(3), pp. 1158-1182.
Harris, C. M., 1953a. A study of the building blocks in speech, Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America, 25(5), pp. 962-969.
Harris, C. M., 1953b. A speech synthesizer, Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America, 25(5), pp. 970-975.
Harris, K. S., Hoffman, H. S., Liberman, A. M., Delattre, P. C., and Cooper, F.
S., 1958. Effect of third formant transitions on the perception of the voiced stop
consonants, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 30(2), pp. 122-126.
Heinz, J. M., and Stevens, K. N., 1964. On the derivation of area functions and
acoustic spectra from cineradiographic films of speech, Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America, 36(5), pp. 1037-1038.
Helmholtz, H., 1859. Ueber die Klangfarbe der Vocale [On the timbre of vowels],
Annalen der Physik, 123, pp. 527-528.
Helmholtz, H., 1875. On the sensations of tone as a physiological basis for the
theory of music, London: Longmans, Green, and Co.
Hermann, L., 1894. Nachtrag zur Untersuchung der Vocalcurven, Pfl´
’ugers Archiv
European Journal of Physiology, 58, pp. 264-279.
Hermann, L., 1895. Weitere Untersuchungen´
’uber das Wesen der Vocale, Pfl´
’ugers
Archiv European Journal of Physiology, 61(4), pp. 169-204.
Hill, D., Manzara, L., and Schock, C., 1995. Real-time articulatory speech-synthesis-
by-rules, Proceedings of AVIOS, 95, pp. 27-44.
Holmes, J. N., 1983. Formant synthesizers: Cascade or parallel? Speech Commu-
nication, 2(4), pp. 251-273.
Holmes, J. N., Mattingly, I. G., and Shearme, J. N., 1964. Speech synthesis by
rule, Language and Speech, 7(3), pp. 127-143.
Hunt, A. J., and Black, A. W., 1996. Unit selection in a concatenative speech
synthesis system using a large speech database, Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Pro-
cessing, ICASSP-96, Conference Proceedings, IEEE, 1, pp. 373-376.
30
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
Ingemann, F., 1957. Speech synthesis by rule, Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America, 29(11), pp. 1255-1255.
Ishizaka, K., and Flanagan, J. L., 1972. Synthesis of voiced sounds from a two?
Mass model of the vocal cords, Bell System Technical Journal, 51(6), pp. 1233-
1268.
Ishizaka, K., and Matsudaira, M., 1972. Fluid mechanical considerations of vocal
cord vibration, Speech Communications Research Laboratory Monograph.
Kelly, J. L., and Lochbaum, C. C., 1962. Speech synthesis, Proceedings of the
Fourth International Congress of Acoustics, G42, 1-4.
Kempelen, W. R. von, 1791. Mechanismus der menschlichen Sprache nebst der
Beschreibung seiner sprechenden Maschine, Vienna: J. B. Degen.
Klatt, D. H., 1982. The Klattalk text-to-speech conversion system, Acoustics,
Speech, and Signal Processing, IEEE International Conference on ICASSP’82,
7, IEEE, pp. 1589-1592.
Klatt, D. H., 1987. Review of text-to-speech conversion for English, Journal of
the Acoustical Society of America, 82(3), pp. 737-793.
Klatt, D. H., and Klatt, L. C., 1990. Analysis, synthesis, and perception of voice
quality variations among female and male talkers, Journal of the Acoustical Society
of America, 87(2), pp. 820-857.
Koenig, R., 1873. I. On manometric flames, The London, Edinburgh, and Dublin
Philosophical Magazine and Journal of Science, 45(297), pp. 1-18.
Koenig, W., Dunn, H. K., and Lacy, L. Y., 1946. The sound spectrograph, Journal
of the Acoustical Society of America, 18(1), pp. 19-49.
Koenig, W., and Ruppel, A. E., 1948. Quantitative amplitude representation in
sound spectrograms, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 20(6), pp. 787-
795.
Kopp, G. A., and Green, H. C., 1946. Basic phonetic principles of visible speech,
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 18(1), pp. 244-245.
Kratzenstein, C. G., 1781. Tentamen Resolvendi Problema ab Academia Scien-
tiarum Imperiali Petroplitana ad annum 1780 Publice Problema, Petropoli: Typis
Academiae Scientiarium.
31
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
Kr´
’oger, B. J., Birkholz, P., Lowit, A., and Neuschaefer-Rube, C., 2010. Phone-
mic, sensory, and motor representations in an action-based neurocomputational
model of speech production, Speech motor control: New developments in basic
and applied research, Maassen, B. and Van Lieshout, P. eds., New York: Oxford
University Press, pp. 23-36.
Ladefoged, P., and Broadbent, D. E., 1957. Information conveyed by vowels,
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 29(1), pp. 98-104.
Lawrence, W., 1953. The synthesis of speech from signals which have a low in-
formation rate, Communication Theory, pp. 460-469.
Liberman, A. M., 1957. Some results of research on speech perception, Journal of
the Acoustical Society of America, 29, 117-123. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1908635
Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D. P., and Studdert-Kennedy, M.,
1967. Perception of the speech code, Psychological Review, 74, pp. 431-461.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0020279
Liberman, A. M., Delattre, P., and Cooper, F. S., 1952. The role of selected
stimulus-variables in the perception of the unvoiced stop consonants, The Amer-
ican Journal of Psychology, 65, pp. 497-516. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1418032
Liberman, A. M., Delattre, P., Cooper, F. S., and Gerstman, L., 1954. The role
of consonant-vowel transitions in the perception of the stop and nasal consonants,
Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 68, pp. 1-13. http:/dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0093673
Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S., Hoffman, H. S., and Griffith, B. C., 1957. The
discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries, Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 54, pp. 358-368. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0044417
Liberman, A. M., Ingemann, F., Lisker, L., Delattre, P., and Cooper, F. S., 1959.
Minimal rules for synthesizing speech, Journal of the Acoustical Society of Amer-
ica, 31(11), pp. 1490-1499.
Liljencrants, J., 1985. Speech synthesis with a reflection-type line analog. DS
Dissertation, Dept. of Speech Comm. and Music Acous., Royal Inst. of Tech.,
Stockholm, Sweden.
Lindblom, B. E., and Sundberg, J. E., 1971. Acoustical consequences of lip, tongue,
jaw, and larynx movement, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 50(4B),
1166-1179.
32
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
Loizou, P. C., Dorman, M., and Tu, Z., 1999. On the number of channels needed
to understand speech, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106(4), pp.
2097-2103.
Maeda, S., 1988. Improved articulatory model, Journal of the Acoustical Society
of America, 84, Sup. 1, S146.
Maeda, S., 1990. Compensatory articulation during speech: Evidence from the
analysis and synthesis of vocal tract shapes using an articulatory model, Speech
production and speech modeling, W.L. Hardcastle, W.L. and Marcha, A., eds.
Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers, pp. 131-149.
Massaro, D. W., 1998. Perceiving talking faces: From speech perception to a
behavioral principle, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Mattingly, I. G., 1974. Speech synthesis for phonetic and phonological models,
Current trends in linguistics, Sebeok, T.A., ed., The Hague: Mouton, pp. 2451-
2487.
Mermelstein, P., 1973. Articulatory model for the study of speech production,
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 53(4), pp. 1070-1082.
Miller, D. C., 1909. The phonodeik, Physical Review, 28, p. 151.
Miller, D. C., 1916. The Lowell lectures: The science of musical sounds, New
York: MacMillan and Co., pp. 215-262.
Moulines, E., and Charpentier, F., 1990. Pitch-synchronous waveform processing
techniques for text- to-speech synthesis using diphones, Speech Communication,
9, pp. 453-467.
Mrayati, M., Carré, R., and Guérin, B., 1988. Distinctive regions and modes: A
new theory of speech production, Speech Communication, 7, pp. 257-286.
Nakata, K., and Mitsuoka, T., 1965. Phonemic transformation and control aspects
of synthesis of connected speech, Journal of the Radio Research Laboratory, 12,
pp. 171-186.
Nam, H., Mitra, V., Tiede, M., Hasegawa-Johnson, M., Espy-Wilson, C., Saltz-
man, E., and Goldstein, L., 2012. A procedure for estimating gestural scores from
speech acoustics, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132(6), pp. 3980-
3989.
33
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
Pantalony, D., 2004. Seeing a voice: Rudolph Koenig’s instruments for studying
vowel sounds, American Journal of Psychology, pp. 425-442.
Patterson, R., 1845. Stated Meeting, December 19, Proceedings of the American
Philosophical Society, 4(34), p. 222.
Pedro the voder: A machine that talks, 1939, Bell Laboratories Record, 17(6), pp.
170-171. (no author listed).
Peterson, G.E., Wang, W. S. Y., andSivertsen, E., 1958. Segmentation techniques
in speech synthesis, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 30(8), pp. 739-
742.
Pieraccini, R., 2012. The voice in the machine, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Potter, R. K., 1945. Visible patterns of sound, Science, 102(2654), pp. 463-470.
Potter, R. K., 1949. Objectives for sound portrayal, Journal of the Acoustical So-
ciety of America, 21(1), pp. 1-5.
Potter, R. K., and Peterson, G. E., 1948. The representation of vowels and their
movements, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 20(4), pp. 528-535.
Rayleigh, J. W. S., 1878. The theory of sound, vol. II, New York: MacMillan and
Co., pp. 469-478. Remez, R. E., 2005.
Perceptual organization of speech, The Handbook of speech perception, Pisoni,
D. B. and Remez, R. E., eds., Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 28-50.
Remez, R. E., Rubin, P. E., Pisoni, D. B., and Carrell, T. D., 1981. Speech per-
ception without traditional speech cues, Science, 212, pp. 947-950.
Rosen, G., 1958. Dynamic analog speech synthesizer, Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America, 30(3), pp. 201-209.
Rothenberg, M., Dorman, K. W., Rumm, J. C., and Theerman, P. H., 1992. 1846
letter from Joseph Henry to Henry M. Alexander, The papers of Joseph Henry:
Volume 6, Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press, pp. 359-364.
Rubin, P., Baer, T., and Mermelstein, P., 1981. An articulatory synthesizer for
perceptual research, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 70, pp. 321-
328.
34
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
Rubin, P., Saltzman, E., Goldstein, L., McGowan, R., Tiede, M., and Browman,
C., 1996. CASY and extensions to the task-dynamic model, Proceedings of the
4th Speech Production Seminar, Grenoble, France, pp. 125-128.
Sagisaka, Y., Kaiki, N., Iwahashi, N., and Mimura, K., 1992. ATR m-TALK
speech synthesis system, Proceedings of the International Conference on Spo-
ken Language Processing, ISCA Archive, http://www.isca-speech.org/archive,
pp. 483-486.
Saltzman, E., 1986. Task dynamic coordination of the speech articulators: A pre-
liminary model, Experimental Brain Research Series, 15, pp. 129-144.
Saltzman, E., 1991. The task dynamic model in speech production, Speech motor
control and stuttering, Peters, H. F. M., Hulstijn, W., and Starkweather, W., eds.,
pp. 37-52. AuQ5
Schroeder, M. R., 1981. Homer W. Dudley: A tribute, Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America, 69(4), p. 1222.
Schroeder, M. R., 1993. A brief history of synthetic speech, Speech Communica-
tion, 13(1-2), pp. 231-237.
Scott, E-L., 1859. The phonoautograph, Cosmos, 14, p. 314.
Shannon, R. V., Zeng, F-G., Kamath, V., Wygonski, J., and Ekelid, M., 1995.
Speech recognition with primarily temporal cues, Science, 270, pp. 303-304.
Sivertsen, E., 1961. Segment inventories for speech synthesis, Language and Speech,
4(1), pp. 27-90.
Smith, Z. M., Delgutte, B., and Oxenham, A. J., 2002. Chimaeric sounds reveal
dichotomies in auditory perception, Nature, 416, pp. 87-90.
Sondhi, M., and Schroeter, J., 1987. A hybrid time-frequency domain articulatory
speech synthesizer, IEEE Transactions on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Process-
ing, 35(7), pp. 955-967.
The Speaking Automaton, 1846. LondonTimes, August12. p. 3. www.phonozoic.net/library.html
Steinberg, J. C., and French, N. R., 1946. The portrayal of visible speech, Journal
of the Acoustical Society of America, 18(1), pp. 4-18.
35
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
Stevens, K. N., and House, A. S., 1955. Development of a quantitative description
of vowel articulation, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 27(3), pp. 484-
493.
Stevens, K. N., Kasowski, S., and Fant, C. G. M., 1953. An electrical analog of the
vocal tract, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25(4), pp. 734-742.
Stewart, J. Q., 1922. An electrical analogue of the vocal organs, Nature, 110(2757),
pp. 311-312.
Story, B. H., 1995. Physiologically-based speech simulation using an enhanced
wave-reflection model of the vocal tract. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Iowa,
Ann Arbor, MI: University Microfilms International.
Story, B. H., 2005. A parametric model of the vocal tract area function for vowel
and consonant simulation, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 117(5),
pp. 3231-3254.
Story, B. H., 2013. Phrase-level speech simulation with an airway modulation
model of speech production, Computer Speech and Language, 27(4), pp. 989-
1010.
Story, B. H., and Bunton, K., 2010. Relation of vocal tract shape, formant transi-
tions, and stop consonant identification, Journal of Speech, Language, and Hear-
ing Research, 53, pp. 1514-1528.
Story, B. H., Titze, I. R., and Hoffman, E. A., 1996. Vocal tract area functions
from magnetic resonance imaging, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,
100(1), pp. 537-554.
Story, B. H., Titze, I. R., and Hoffman, E. A., 1998. Vocal tract area functions for
an adult female speaker based on volumetric imaging, Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America, 104(1), 471-487.
Strube, H., 1982. Time-varying wave digital filters and vocal-tract models, Acous-
tics, Speech, and Signal Processing, IEEE International Conference on ICASSP’82,
7, IEEE, pp. 923-926.
Talking Machine, 1880. London Times, February 12. www.phonozoic.net/library.html
Titze, I. R., 1973. The human vocal cords: A mathematical model, Phonetica,
28(3-4), pp. 129-170.
36
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
Titze, I. R., 1974. The human vocal cords: A mathematical model, Phonetica,
29(1-2), pp. 1-21.
Tompkins, D., 2010. How to wreck a nice beach, The Vocoder from World War
II to Hip-Hop, the machine speaks, Brooklyn, NY: Melville House.
Trouvain, J., and Brackhane, F., 2011. Wolfgang von Kempelen’s speaking ma-
chine as an instrument for demonstration and research, Proceedings of the 17th In-
ternational Conference of Phonetic Sciences, International Phonetic Association,
https://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/ icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2011/index.htm,
pp. 164-167.
Truby, H. M., 1965. Matching speech cineradiography with pseudospeech, Jour-
nal of the Acoustical Society of America, 37(6), p. 1187.
Van den Oord, A., Dieleman, S., Zen, H., Simonyan, K., Vinyals, O., Graves, A.,
Kalchbrenner, N., Senior, A., and Kavukcuoglu, K., 2016. Wavenet: A generative
model for raw audio. arXiv preprint arXiv:1609.03499. [Accessed 5 March 2018].
Vatikiotis-Bateson, E., Kuratate, T., Munhall, K. G., and Yehia, H. C., 2000. The
production and perception of a realistic talking face, Proceedings of LP’98: Item
order in language and speech, Fujimura, O., Joseph, B. D., and Palek, B., eds.,
Prague: Karolinum Press (Charles University, 2,pp. 439-460).
Wang, W. S. Y., and Peterson, G. E., 1958. Segment inventory for speech syn-
thesis, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 30(8), pp. 743-746.
Wheatstone, C., 1837. Reed organ-pipes, speaking machines, etc., The scientific
papers of Sir Charles Wheatstone (1879), London: Taylor and Francis, pp. 348-
367. Originally published in the London and Westminster Review, No. xi and
liv., October 1837.
Willis, R., 1829. On the vowel sounds, and on reed organ pipes, Transactions of
the Cambridge philosophical society, 3, pp. 231-268.
Wurzbach, C., Biographies of the Empire Austria: Containing life sketches of
memorable people, who lived from 1750 to 1850 in the imperial state and in its
crown lands, 60, pp. 1856-1891, Vienna: Austrian Academy of Sciences Press.
Yehia, H., Rubin, P., and Vatikiotis-Bateson, E., 1998. Quantitative association of
vocal-tract and facial behavior, Speech Communication, 26(1-2), pp. 23-43.
37
B. Story, final draft 12.15.18
Zen, H., Senior, A., and Schuster, M., 2013. Statistical parametric speech synthesis
using deep neural networks, Acoustics, Speech and Signal Processing (ICASSP),
IEEE, pp. 7962-7966.
Zen, H., Tokuda, K., and Black, A. W., 2009. Statistical parametric speech syn-
thesis, Speech Communication, 51(11), pp. 1039-1064.
38