ArticlePublisher preview available
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

A growing line of research suggests that creativity and unethicality are intrinsically related to one another. However, the idea has been challenged both by theoretical arguments and by heterogeneous empirical findings. In the present work, we review the literature to reconcile seemingly opposed theoretical views on the relationship between creativity and unethicality. We then conduct a meta-analysis to clear up confusion about heterogeneous empirical findings in the literature (k = 36, N = 6783). We find a weak positive correlation between the 2 constructs (r = .09, 95% confidence interval [.01, .17], t = 2.24, p < .05). Consistent with social desirability response bias theory (Randall & Fernandes, 1991), we find that the correlation is significant in studies that rely upon objective measures of unethicality—that is, behavioral measures or other-reports—but not in studies that rely upon self-reports of unethicality. Altogether, our work suggests that creativity and unethicality are positively related as predicted by theory, and that some studies have failed at finding it because they used self-reports to assess unethicality rather than objective measures. Theoretical, methodological, and practical implications are discussed.
Creativity and Unethicality: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
Martin Storme
IESEG School of Management and LEM-CNRS 9221
Pinar Celik
Solvay Brussels School of Economics and Management
Nils Myszkowski
Pace University
A growing line of research suggests that creativity and unethicality are intrinsically related to one
another. However, the idea has been challenged both by theoretical arguments and by heterogeneous
empirical findings. In the present work, we review the literature to reconcile seemingly opposed
theoretical views on the relationship between creativity and unethicality. We then conduct a meta-
analysis to clear up confusion about heterogeneous empirical findings in the literature (k36, N
6783). We find a weak positive correlation between the 2 constructs (r.09, 95% confidence interval
[.01, .17], t2.24, p.05). Consistent with social desirability response bias theory (Randall &
Fernandes, 1991), we find that the correlation is significant in studies that rely upon objective measures
of unethicality—that is, behavioral measures or other-reports— but not in studies that rely upon self-
reports of unethicality. Altogether, our work suggests that creativity and unethicality are positively
related as predicted by theory, and that some studies have failed at finding it because they used
self-reports to assess unethicality rather than objective measures. Theoretical, methodological, and
practical implications are discussed.
Keywords: creativity, unethicality, meta-analysis
Creativity is considered one of the most important skills for the
21st century (Trilling & Fadel, 2009) because it is the source of
many technological and social advances (Shen, Yuan, Yi, Liu, &
Zhan, 2019). However, there are also many examples of the dark
side of creativity (Shen et al., 2019), leading some researchers to
wonder whether creativity is intrinsically linked to unethicality
(Gino & Ariely, 2012;Gino & Wiltermuth, 2014). Indeed, re-
search suggests that being creative tends to give a strong sense of
entitlement, helps finding justifications, and facilitates a rule
breaking mindset, which all can lead, in turn, to unethical behav-
iors (Vincent & Goncalo, 2014;Gino & Ariely, 2012;Gino &
Wiltermuth, 2014).
Nevertheless, the idea that creativity and unethicality are intrin-
sically related has been challenged both theoretically— by research
arguing that creativity is positively related to prosocial tendencies
(Xu & Mehta, 2015;Grant & Berry, 2011)—and empirically— by
heterogeneous findings across studies that investigated the phe-
nomenon. Researchers are thus left with many interrogations. Are
there valid theoretical objections to the idea that creativity and
unethicality are positively related? What could explain empirical
studies yielding seemingly different conclusions? Eventually, such
interrogations lead researchers to the ultimate question: Is creativ-
ity really positively related to unethicality?
In the present work, we start by reviewing theoretical frame-
works arguing that creativity and unethicality are positively re-
lated. We then review the literature that argues that creativity is
positively related to prosocial tendencies and we present the eth-
ical dissonance theoretical framework (Barkan, Ayal, & Ariely,
2015;Barkan, Ayal, Gino, & Ariely, 2012) as a way to reconcile
the seemingly contradictory relationships between creativity,
prosocial tendencies, and unethicality. Finally, we provide a meta-
analytic estimate of the correlation between creativity and unethi-
cality, and we investigate the moderating role of methodological
factors to explain the heterogeneity in findings.
Defining Creativity and Unethicality
Creativity and ethicality are regularly considered crucial quali-
ties for the 21st century. For example, the National Research
Council (2013) has cited both creativity and ethical decision mak-
ing abilities in its list of the 21st century knowledge and skills that
should be taught to children as a priority issue. Creativity and
ethicality are also important outcomes in the business world.
XMartin Storme, People, Organizations, and Negotiation, IESEG
School of Management, and Individus, Organisations et Société, LEM-
CNRS 9221; Pinar Celik, Centre Emile Bernheim, Solvay Brussels School
of Economics and Management; Nils Myszkowski, Department of Psy-
chology, Pace University.
All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Martin
Storme, People, Organizations, and Negotiation, IESEG School of Man-
agement, 3 Rue de la Digue, Lille 59000, France. E-mail: martinstorme@
ymail.com
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts
© 2020 American Psychological Association
ISSN: 1931-3896 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca0000332
2021, Vol. 15, No. 4, 664–672
664
This article was published Online First June 29, 2020.
... This can increase the tendency to generate positive or negative creative outcomes. In addition, positive and negative creativity and unethicality involve breaking the rules and schema, as if they relied on the same cognitive processes [34]. In other words, people with high relativism could be considered more creative regardless of the end of the production, as both creativity and high relativism do not involve conformity but are more compatible with the rejection of rigid adherence to standard principles. ...
... Such a blend of components was also proposed by both the ICT [11] and AMORAL [10] models, which give shape to positive and negative creativity, respectively. Particularly, relativism to ethics was used as a mediating factor for two reasons: rst, individuals characterized by a tendency to establish relativism are triggered by their personal stance and view of the situation, compromising on self-established ideas and style of working [30,31]; second, they are more prone to think that the end justi es the means, look at the problem or situation from different views, break rules [34]. The positive and negative creative production was assessed by the cartoon caption task. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Clarifying the mechanisms underlying individual differences in creativity is essential to understand the complex nature of this phenomenon. The current research, drawing on the Investment Theory of Creativity and the AMORAL model, investigated the mediating role of ethics positions (idealism and relativism) in the association between openness to experience and both positive (i.e., funny and irony) and negative creativity (i.e., grotesque and macabre). Two-hundred participants were instructed to fill in two self-report questionnaires, aimed to measure personality (HEXACO-60) and ethics positions (EPQ-5). They also were requested to provide creative captions to five different cartoons taken by the New Yorker, as requested in the cartoon caption task, which were evaluated by two independent judges in terms of positive (funny and ironic) and negative (grotesque and macabre) creativity. Results indicated that relativism mediated only the association between openness to experience and negative creativity, suggesting that ethics positions are partially involved in the creative generation. By investigating the complex relationship between openness to experience and ethics positions, the current study sheds light on the underlying mechanisms that lead people to creative expression, offering fruitful insights into the key role of the ethical dimension in human creativity. Implications, limitations, and future research avenues are discussed.
... Prior research has shown that subjective well-being increases self-confidence in that individuals trust their skills and abilities to control their life across a variety of situations (Briñol et al., 2007). Such self-confidence can influence the perception of risk (Gaspar & Schweitzer, 2021), especially for creative individuals who have a strong sense of entitlement and are more willing to cross the line to reach their goals (Storme et al., 2021). As a result, it may impair moral awareness and divert attention away from ethical issues in decision-making due to a grandiose self-image, a disrespectful attitude toward others, and a misperception of one's place in the world (McManus, 2018;Petit & Bollaert, 2012). ...
... It is especially true for creative individuals who have a strong sense of entitlement. The feeling of being unconstrained by rules makes them more willing to cross the line to reach their goals (Storme et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Creativity and ethics are two topics that are extremely important to business managers and researchers. However, only limited research has examined their relationship and has shown contradictory results: the effect of creativity on unethical behavior proves to be sometimes positive, sometimes negative. In this vein, building on social cognitive theory, we examine whether this relationship depends on a macroeconomic factor – endemic corruption – as a boundary condition. We further consider the mechanism through which creativity can lead to unethical behavior and focus on the role of subjective well‐being. We collect data from 1463 individuals in two high‐corruption and emerging countries, Brazil and China, and two low‐corruption and developed countries, France and the United States. Based on a multi‐level (or hierarchical) analysis, our results show that, in high‐corruption countries, creativity is negatively related to unethical behaviors. Conversely, in low‐corruption countries, the relationship is positive and mediated by subjective well‐being.
... In a meta-analysis, Storme et al. (2021) proposed the theory of ethical dissonance to address contradictory findings on the relationship between creativity and ethical behavior. Ethical dissonance, according to Barkan et al. (2012), occurs when individuals act in ways that contradict their moral values, leading to negative emotions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Outlining the profile of individuals more receptive to novel behaviors or ideas within a social system is crucial for understanding cultural change. In the pursuit of understanding innovativeness as a personality trait, efforts have produced a short self-report scale that yields an innovativeness score composed of two factors: 'willingness to try new things' and 'creativity or originality.' This study examines how self-reported measures of life-history, de-mographics, risk-taking propensity and attention deficit and hyperactivity signs/symptoms are associated with self-perceived innovativeness. Our findings indicate that the characteristics defining an innovator profile-that is, those reporting higher levels of self-perceived innovativeness-include age, which positively predicts inno-vativeness among individuals aged 18 to 50, being female (sex assigned at birth), identifying as non-binary, having fewer siblings and children, and being in a committed relationship (rather than single). Moreover, higher propensity for self-reported social and recreational risk-taking, and a low propensity for ethical risk-taking, is associated to higher self-perceived innovativeness. Lastly, self-reported inattentiveness is negatively associated with self-perceived innovativeness.
... The central question at hand is the reconciliation of creativity, often aligned with an ethos of conviction [61,62], with responsibility (as illustrated in Fig. 1). The ethic of conviction focuses solely on not betraying a value or transgressing a norm (e.g., truth, kindness), demanding absolute purity of means and showing indifference to consequences. ...
Article
Full-text available
Education today is evolving to foster essential psychosocial skills like creativity, critical thinking, and responsibility, equipping students for 21st-century challenges. However, it is crucial to examine the motivations driving these changes and their alignment with broader societal goals beyond merely economic interests. This article explores the ethical dimensions of creativity, recognizing its potential for both positive innovation and harm. It emphasizes the neutrality of creativity and the importance of evaluating the intentions and consequences behind creative acts, not just their outcomes. We introduce and define the concept of ‘‘responsible creativity,’’ characterized by the ability to generate original ideas, behaviors, or objects while considering the moral and legal repercussions of one's actions, both in the present and future. This concept is anchored in the ethics of conviction and responsibility, promoting a fundamental commitment to avoid harm and enhance well-being. Our model of responsible creativity incorporates qualities such as benevolence, flexibility, positivity, reliability, accountability, reflexivity, and wisdom. We advocate for nurturing this type of creativity in educational settings through innovative approaches such as board and video games, philosophy workshops, wisdom education, and mindfulness meditation. These pedagogical methods aim to cultivate future leaders who are ethically aware and capable of using their creativity to positively impact society and the environment. By navigating the interplay of creativity and ethics, this article provides valuable insights and practical guidance for educators and policymakers striving to create a more ethically conscious and innovative educational landscape.
... Creativity and morality have shared a tenuous relationship, with research either finding contrasting results (e.g., Shen et al., 2019;Storme et al., 2021) or being ironically riddled with original and dishonest academic practices (Stern, 2023). Both constructs can be studied in myriad ways, and measurement differences can yield diverse results. ...
... However, studies in this area remain inconclusive regarding whether creativity is positively (Shen et al., 2019) or negatively (Storme et al., 2020) associated with morality and morally tinged behaviors like deception. The nature of this association depends on various factors, including but not limited to: how creativity is measured (self-report/behavioral task), how deception is measured (self-report/behavioral task), the goal of deception (prosocial or antisocial), the context in which tasks are performed (artificial/real-world), and individual differences in proclivities to be creative and deceptive. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Creativity is usually associated with originality and usefulness, while innovation can be considered as the monetization of creativity through enterprise. Recent research has explored the dark side of creativity, where original thinking is used to meet negative or malicious goals, with or without the intent to hurt others. Behaviors and traits related to deception and misrepresentation feature prominently as correlates of such dark creativity. Research has also found robust differences in the manner in which deception is perceived and conveyed across cultures as well as in the preferences for truth-telling. Further, culture shapes perceptions and attitudes not only toward creative endeavors and accomplishments, but also toward the means of attaining them. This chapter ties together these strands of research, presenting an anecdotal account of the use of innovative deception in diverse cultures across contexts. We discuss examples of financial fraud and other unlawful activities to illustrate cases of innovative deception. Drawing from research in creativity, innovation, and morality across cultures, an integrative description of innovative deception is presented. Existing and emerging consequences of the same are discussed (e.g., fake news as innovative deception in political propaganda).KeywordsCross-cultural researchDeceptionDishonestyInnovationMalevolent creativity
... Shen et al. (2019) concluded that creativity was positively associated with morality by reviewing studies. However, a meta-analysis conducted by Storme et al. (2021) indicated a weak positive correlation between creativity and unethicality. The essence of the controversy may be that they ignored exploring malevolent creativity separately. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study aims to investigate the relationship between Honesty-Humility and malevolent creativity, by testing the sequential mediation models of prosocial moral emotional traits (gratitude, guilt, sympathy, and empathy) and prosocial tendencies. Participants (N = 592) filled out scales measuring Honesty-Humility, Emotionality, Openness to experience, malevolent creativity, trait guilt, trait gratitude, trait empathy, trait sympathy, and prosocial tendencies. The variables of Age, gender, Emotionality, and Openness to experience were controlled for in sequential mediation analyses. The results indicated that Honesty-Humility was negatively associated with malevolent creativity. Multiple mediation analyses showed that trait gratitude, trait guilt, and trait sympathy fitted the model that prosocial moral emotional traits and prosocial tendencies sequentially mediate the negative association between Honesty-Humility and malevolent creativity. However, trait empathy failed in fitting the sequential mediation model. The findings suggest that individuals with high Honesty-Humility are inclined towards experiencing prosocial moral emotions and then have prosocial tendencies. Consequently, they may act with fewer malevolent creative tendencies.
... Despite varied studies in the area, the relationship between morality and creativity seems muddy. Two review papers, only a year apart, found contradictory results: one concluding that creativity and morality are positively related (Shen et al., 2019) and the other identifying a weak positive relationship between creativity and unethicality (Storme et al., 2020). However, the latter paper made a distinction between studies that used objective (behavioral, other reports) or subjective self-reports of dishonesty; as social desirability can contaminate results in the morality-creativity space, the overall positive relationship was driven by studies using objective measures of unethicality, such as observations of dishonest behavior. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Dark creativity comprises novel actions that lead to deliberate or unintentional harm. Although recent research has begun examining the construct more widely, there exist several gaps in scholarship. One such area is the influence, association, and role of affective factors in determining whether and to what extent an entity engages in dark creativity. After introducing concepts like negative and malevolent creativity, this chapter reviews the existing literature on the link between affective factors and creativity. Thereafter, the features of dark personality traits, like psychopathy, are examined with reference to affective considerations such as low empathy in producing original harm. Owing to the relationship between dark creativity and moral concerns, we also examine how moral emotions like guilt and shame (or lack thereof) may contribute to an understanding of such creativity. The chapter concludes with suggestions for future research and avenues for interdisciplinary studies.
Chapter
The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity and Emotions provides a state-of-the-art review of research on the role of emotions in creativity. This volume presents the insights and perspectives of sixty creativity scholars from thirteen countries who span multiple disciplines, including developmental, social, and personality psychology; industrial and organizational psychology; neuroscience; education; art therapy, and sociology. It discusses affective processes – emotion states, traits, and emotion abilities – in relation to the creative process, person, and product, as well as two major contexts for expression of creativity: school, and work. It is a go-to source for scholars who need to enhance their understanding of a specific topic relating to creativity and emotion, and it provides students and researchers with a comprehensive introduction to creativity and emotion broadly.
Article
Full-text available
Creativity self-assessments (CSAs) are frequently used in empirical research. However, they are often chosen for the sake of expediency. If used as outcome variables, there are legitimate concerns with their accuracy or validity. However, CSAs are most valuable when serving other purposes. In this review, I establish a categorization of CSAs based on their primary focus: Activities, Evaluation, Process, and Beliefs. I give a brief description and some examples of each type. Finally, I highlight specific instances of how CSAs can offer information and insights beyond performance-based measures.
Article
Full-text available
The Alternate Uses Task (AUT) is one of the most commonly utilized measures of divergent thinking within the creativity research literature. Some participants respond to the AUT by describing the possibility of using various common objects in a violent way (e.g., beat someone with a book), and this unsolicited inclination is termed malevolent creativity. In this study, participant responses on the AUT were systematically coded for malevolent uses, and the relations among malevolence and various individual differences including sex, ideational fluency, and originality, were examined. Results revealed that male participants generated significantly more malevolent responses to the AUT than did females, despite no significant sex differences on originality or fluency. In addition, participant originality significantly predicted malevolence, although ideational fluency did not. Taken together, these findings suggest that malevolent responses on the AUT do not arise simply when very many uses are generated (i.e., when fluency is high) but may represent a particular inclination, disproportionately displayed by males, toward utilizing divergent thinking in a potentially violent way.
Article
Full-text available
Drawing on theoretical views of creative deviance, the present study hypothesized that employees with a tendency to break rules are more creative at their jobs (Hypothesis 1). Furthermore, we expected that this is particularly the case when employees face high problem-solving demands (i.e., they are expected to be creative) but at the same time they face high organizational constraints (e.g., they are not given the resources they need in order to be creative; Hypothesis 2). To test these expectations, we conducted a cross-sectional survey study (Study 1) and a daily diary survey study (Study 2) among employees from different occupational groups. Study 1 provided evidence that employee rule-breaking is related to other-rated employee creativity. Study 2 replicated this finding with a day-level measure of self-rated employee creativity. Furthermore, multilevel regression analyses revealed that the link is stronger when day-level problem-solving demands and organizational constraints were both high. Our findings contribute to the literature by providing empirical support to hardly examined propositions, namely, that non-compliant behaviors (i.e., rule-breaking) and obstacles (i.e., organizational constraints) may, in fact, boost creativity. Finally, suggestions for practice are made, recognizing the challenges posed to management in regulating employee rule-breaking.
Article
Full-text available
Research has been inconsistent in its quest to discover whether dispositional creativity is associated with more or less unethical behavior. Drawing on social cognitive theory, we propose that moral disengagement and moral imagination are 2 parallel mechanisms that encourage or inhibit unethical behavior, and that which of these mediation processes occur depends on moral identity. Study 1, a 3-wave study of a food service organization, shows that employees high on both dispositional creativity and moral identity are less likely to be morally disengaged and behave less unethically. The results of Study 2 replicate Study 1’s findings in a scenario-based study of college students, and further show that individuals who are high on both dispositional creativity and moral identity are more likely to be morally imaginative and to behave less unethically. Theoretical and practical implications of our model are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Recent research has uncovered the dark side of creativity by finding that creative individuals are more likely to engage in unethical behavior. However, we argue that not all creative individuals make trouble. Using moral self-regulation theory as our overarching theoretical framework, we examine individuals’ moral identity as a boundary condition and moral disengagement as a mediating mechanism to explain when and how individual creativity is associated with workplace deviant behavior. We conducted two field studies using multi-source data to test our hypotheses. In Study 1, the results indicated that creativity positively predicted moral disengagement for those low in moral identity. In Study 2 with multi-wave data, we replicated the finding that moral identity moderated the effect of creativity on moral disengagement in Study 1 and further revealed that moral disengagement mediated the interactive effects of creativity and moral identity on workplace deviant behavior. The theoretical and practical implications of these findings and directions for future research are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
The relationship between creativity and morality is not only a key theme in creative psychology and moral psychology but also a central issue regarding individual differences in creativity. Based on our well-described definitions of morality and creativity, the current work systemically assesses three main viewpoints on this issue, namely that there is a positive correlation between the two constructs, that there is no correlation between the two constructs, and that there is a negative correlation between the two constructs, through reviewing studies scattered across multiple disciplines. We then draw an evidence-based conclusion that a positive association exists between creativity and morality by critically examining and integrating relevant empirical studies. We also discuss the potential implications of these findings for integrative training and for the fostering of creativity and morality in conjunction with several future research directions.
Article
Full-text available
Is dishonesty affected by one's creativity and environment? Recent studies have documented the effects of creativity and environment on dishonesty respectively. However, little attention has been given to the interaction effect of creativity and environment. Based on past findings, we hypothesized that creative people, compared to their non-creative counterparts, may tell more lies in an enriched (vs. scarce) environment. An experiment was conducted on a sample of 97 undergraduate students to examine the moderation effect of environment on the linkage of creativity and cheating. Participants completed a creativity task and a questionnaire on general knowledge about Malaysia. Two-way ANOVA analysis showed that creative participants, as well as those in the enriched environment, were more likely to tell lies than their counterparts. More importantly, the interaction effect of creativity and environment was statistically significant. Specifically, the creative people were more likely to cheat in the enriched environment than in the scarce environment. The results not only shed light on the link between creativity and dishonesty but also suggest a new direction for minimizing dishonesty behaviours.
Article
This study explores the performance of classical methods for detecting publication bias—namely, Egger’s regression test, Funnel Plot test, Begg’s Rank Correlation and Trim and Fill method—in meta-analysis of studies that report multiple effects. Publication bias, outcome reporting bias, and a combination of these were generated. Egger’s regression test and the Funnel Plot test were extended to three-level models, and possible cutoffs for the estimator of the Trim and Fill method were explored. Furthermore, we checked whether the combination of results of several methods yielded a better control of Type I error rates. Results show that no method works well across all conditions and that performance depends mainly on the population effect size value and the total variance.
Article
Recent research has been investigating positive and negative valences of creativity; that is, using the creative process to meet positive and/or negative goals set by respective stakeholders. Given the past association between creativity and deception, this study examined whether deception was differentially related to these valences of creativity. Participants (N = 169) completed a real-world divergent thinking task, and responded to measures of trait deception and ideational fluency. Responses were coded for originality, valence, and whether deception was used to address the situation. Results supported the overall relationship between trait deception and originality. In the high creative subsample (n = 42), trait deception predicted not just originality, but also both valences of creativity. However, in contrast to positive creativity, individuals were more likely to use deceptive and creative processes only to engage in negative creativity. Thus, deception as a process variable was associated with negative creativity. In the four Ps framework of creativity (Rhodes, 1961), results suggested that deception operated as a Person component in positive creativity, but as Person and Process facets in negative creativity. Implications for future research include identifying other cognitive processes along which the valences of creativity differ, and assessing such differences via electrophysiological means.