Content uploaded by Sabrina Thai
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Sabrina Thai on Aug 16, 2020
Content may be subject to copyright.
Running head: SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
1
When Every Day is a High School Reunion: Social Media Comparisons and Self-Esteem
Claire Midgley
University of Toronto
Sabrina Thai
Brock University
Penelope Lockwood, Chloe Kovacheff, and Elizabeth Page-Gould
University of Toronto
Authors’ note: Claire Midgley, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto,
Ontario, Canada; Sabrina Thai, Department of Psychology, Brock University, St Catharines,
Ontario, Canada; Penelope Lockwood, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto,
Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Chloe Kovacheff, Rotman School of Management, University of
Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Elizabeth Page-Gould, Department of Psychology,
University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
This research was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of
Canada (SSHRC) Postdoctoral Fellowship to Sabrina Thai and a SSHRC Insight Grant to
Penelope Lockwood. Portions of the present research were presented at the 18th Annual Meeting
of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, San Antonio, Texas, USA and the
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
2
Psychology of Media & Technology Preconference at the 20th Annual Meeting of the Society for
Personality and Social Psychology, Portland, Oregon, USA.
We thank Laksmiina Balasubramaniam and Ariana Youm for their assistance with data
collection. We also thank Joanne Wood, Brett Q. Ford, Geoff MacDonald, and Emily Impett for
their helpful comments and suggestions on an earlier version of this manuscript.
Data are available by emailing the authors. Study materials, syntax for analyses, and
supplementary materials are available on OSF: https://osf.io/kn49t/
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Claire Midgley,
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, 100 St. George Street, Toronto, Ontario,
Canada M5S 3G3. Email: claire.midgley@mail.utoronto.ca
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
3
Abstract
Although past research has shown that social comparisons made through social media contribute
to negative outcomes, little is known about the nature of these comparisons (domains, direction,
and extremity), variables that determine comparison outcomes (post valence, perceiver’s self-
esteem), and how these comparisons differ from those made in other contexts (e.g., text
messages, face-to-face interactions). In four studies (N=798), we provide the first
comprehensive analysis of how individuals make and respond to social comparisons on social
media, using comparisons made in real-time while browsing news feeds (Study 1), experimenter-
generated comparisons (Study 2), and comparisons made on social media vs. in other contexts
(Studies 3-4). More frequent and more extreme upward comparisons resulted in immediate
declines in self-evaluations as well as cumulative negative effects on individuals’ state self-
esteem, mood, and life satisfaction after a social media browsing session. Moreover, downward
and lateral comparisons occurred less frequently and did little to mitigate upward comparisons’
negative effects. Furthermore, low self-esteem individuals were particularly vulnerable to
making more frequent and more extreme upward comparisons on social media, which in turn
threatened their already-lower self-evaluations. Finally, social media comparisons resulted in
greater declines in self-evaluation than those made in other contexts. Together, these studies
provide the first insights into the cumulative impact of multiple comparisons, clarify the role of
self-esteem in online comparison processes, and demonstrate how the characteristics and impact
of comparisons on social media differ from those made in other contexts.
Keywords: social comparisons, self-esteem, social media, Facebook, Instagram
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
4
When Every Day is a High School Reunion: Social Media Comparisons and Self-Esteem
In just over a decade, social media use has skyrocketed. In 2005, only 5% of Americans
reported using one or more social media platforms; by 2019, this number had risen to 72% (Pew
Research Center, 2019). Furthermore, the majority of Facebook, Instagram, Snapchat, and
YouTube users visit these sites at least once per day, contributing to a global average of over two
hours per day spent on social media per person (Clement, 2020). Although social media can
enhance social connection (Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007; Liu, Ainsworth, & Baumeister,
2016) and provide opportunities for self-disclosure and perceived social support (Davis, 2012;
Ko & Kuo, 2009), the preponderance of research indicates that social media use is associated
with negative outcomes, such as envy, romantic jealousy, decreased self-esteem and subjective
well-being, increased loneliness and social isolation, and depression (Burke, Marlow, & Lento,
2010; Hwang, Cheong, & Feeley, 2009; Kalpidou, Costin, & Morris, 2011; Krasnova,
Wenninger, Widjaja, & Buxmann, 2013; Kross et al., 2013; Muise, Christofides, & Desmarais,
2009; Tandoc, Ferrucci, & Duffy, 2015; Verduyn et al., 2015; Valkenburg, Peter, & Schouten,
2006; Vogel, Rose, Roberts, & Eckles, 2014; Woods & Scott, 2016; for reviews, see Best,
Manktelow, & Taylor, 2014 and Verduyn, Ybarra, Résibois, Jonides, & Kross, 2017). Despite
these negative associations, however, social media use continues to grow (Clement, 2020; Pew
Research Center, 2019); thus, it is important to understand when and how social media will
result in negative outcomes, and for whom these negative outcomes will be most significant.
Social Media is Associated with Threatening Social Comparisons
A growing body of research suggests that social media exerts a negative impact on users
through social comparison processes: Individuals see that others on social media appear to be
experiencing more positive outcomes, and consequently feel worse about themselves. Indeed, a
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
5
number of studies point to associations between Facebook use, upward comparisons, and
negative outcomes. Heavy users, in contrast to infrequent users, are more likely to agree that
others are happier, have better lives, and are doing better (Chou & Edge, 2012; de Vries &
Kühne, 2015). Furthermore, making more upward Facebook comparisons has been associated
with negative self-perceptions of one’s own social competence and attractiveness, increased
depressive symptoms, and lower overall well-being (Appel, Crusius, & Gerlach, 2015; de Vries
& Kühne, 2015; Fardouly & Vartanian, 2015; Feinstein et al., 2013; Gerson, Plagnol, & Corr,
2016; Liu et al., 2017; Steers, Wickham, & Acitelli, 2014; Tandoc et al., 2015; Vogel et al.,
2014; Wang, Wang, Gaskin, & Hawk, 2017). These negative effects, moreover, seem especially
pronounced for low self-esteem individuals (Cramer, Song, & Drent, 2016; Jang, Park, & Song,
2016). This past research, however, does not provide clear evidence that social comparison is
responsible for negative social media outcomes (Appel, Gerlach, & Crusius, 2016). Because
these studies relied primarily on retrospective reports, it may be that individuals who are
experiencing negative outcomes in these domains are simply more likely to recall or report on
comparisons to superior others.
It is unclear, moreover, which characteristics of the social media context lead to
particularly negative social comparison outcomes. Because past studies showing the connection
between social media and social comparison have focused exclusively on social media contexts
(e.g. Appel et al., 2015), it remains unclear how these processes may differ from other, non-
social-media contexts. Presumably, the cognitive mechanisms underlying social comparison will
be similar regardless of whether one is exposed to a superior other on social media, another
online context, or in real life, with upward comparisons typically leading to threats to self-esteem
and diminished mood and life satisfaction (Gerber, Wheeler & Suls, 2018). One may feel worse
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
6
if one learns about a friend’s superior academic performance regardless of whether one hears
about the friend’s success through social media or face-to-face interaction. Why then, are social
media comparisons associated with especially negative outcomes?
In the present studies, we identify specific attributes of social media comparisons that are
especially damaging to the self. Through examining comparisons in real-time in lab studies, as
well as in studies using both experimental and experience-sampling designs, we make three key
contributions to the literature: First, we show that social media provides more opportunities for
individuals to make comparisons, in particular to superior others; individuals thus make more
frequent upward comparisons when using social media than in other contexts. Second, because
social media posts tend to be highly positive, individuals make comparisons that are more
extreme in their “upwardness” than in other contexts. This greater frequency and extremity of
upward comparisons results in a particularly negative impact of social media use on the self.
Third, we advance the literature on social comparison and self-esteem by showing that low self-
esteem individuals are especially likely to make more frequent and extreme upward
comparisons, which in turn leads to a more negative impact on their self-evaluations; we further
examine whether social media amplifies these negative outcomes relative to other contexts.
Social Media and Upward Comparison Frequency
Social media provides a continuous stream of information about other people’s
accomplishments. Past research suggests that social comparisons occur automatically (Chatard,
Bocage-Barthélémy, Selimbogović, & Guimond, 2017; Gilbert, Giesler & Morris, 1995;
Mussweiler, Rüter, & Epstude, 2004); when individuals encounter information about another
person, their own self-perceptions will be affected. The sheer number of posts in a news feed,
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
7
each offering a thumbnail sketch about another person, would thus seem likely to yield numerous
comparison opportunities.
Furthermore, to the extent that social media posts are positive, they are most likely to
yield upward comparisons, resulting in negative outcomes for the self (Gerber et al., 2018).
Indeed, evidence suggests that news feed content is predominantly about positive experiences.
Although people do not typically post false information about themselves online (Back et al.,
2010), they do engage in selectively positive self-presentation (Walther, 2007; Wilson, Gosling,
& Graham, 2012) and are more likely to post positive rather than negative content (e.g., Dorethy,
Fiebert, & Warren, 2014; Qiu, Lin, Leung, & Tov, 2012; Seidman, 2013). As a result,
individuals browsing their news feeds are more likely to see posts about friends’ exciting social
activities than dull days at the office, affording numerous opportunities for comparisons to
seemingly better-off others.
To date, research has not directly tested whether social media exerts a negative impact on
the self by eliciting more frequent upward comparisons than in other, non-social-media contexts.
A number of studies suggest that individuals who are especially prone to making comparisons
experience negative outcomes when using social media (Alfasi, 2019; de Vries, Möller,
Wieringa, Eigenraam, & Hamelink, 2018; Hanna et al., 2017; Stapleton, Luiz, & Chatwin, 2017;
Vogel, Rose, Okdie, Eckles, & Franz, 2015; Wang et al., 2017). These studies, however, did not
explicitly measure comparison frequency, instead assessing whether an orientation to make
comparisons more generally (Gibbons & Buunk, 1999) or comparison tendency on a particular
platform (e.g., “Today, when I was on Facebook, I felt less confident about what I have achieved
compared to other people”; Steers et al, 2014) might be related to social media use outcomes.
Such global reports may not accurately reflect the degree to which individuals were engaging in
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
8
actual comparison activities (Gerber et al., 2018) and may instead reflect individuals’ lay
theories regarding social comparisons on social media. That is, individuals’ perceptions of how
often comparisons are occurring may not match how often they actually make comparisons on
social media (Cramer et al., 2016).
Moreover, although recent research suggests that individuals with a greater orientation
toward making social comparisons experience worse outcomes after using social media (Lee,
2014; Vogel et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2017), research conducted prior to the advent of social
media suggests that these individuals experience similarly negative outcomes, such as increased
depressive symptoms and social anxiety, in non-social-media contexts (Gibbons & Buunk,
1999). Consequently, it is unclear whether frequent social media comparisons are associated
with more negative outcomes, or whether individuals with a propensity to compare themselves
experience more negative outcomes in any context. One experience-sampling study that did
measure comparison frequency demonstrated that physical appearance comparisons made on
social media were actually less frequent than comparisons resulting from in-person encounters
(Fardouly, Pinkus, & Vartanian, 2017). Given this study’s focus on physical appearance,
however, it is unclear how social media comparison frequency predicts comparison outcomes
more broadly. In the present research, we examined whether upward comparisons would be
more frequent on social media than in other contexts, and whether this greater frequency in turn
would be associated with more negative social media use outcomes.
Social Media and Upward Comparison Extremity
We propose that social media will exert a negative impact not only by eliciting a greater
number of comparisons to superior others, but also by prompting individuals to make
comparisons that are especially “upward.” According to the selective accessibility model
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
9
(Mussweiler, 2003; Mussweiler & Strack, 1999), individuals first assess whether they are similar
or dissimilar to a superior other at a holistic level; they then go on to test their specific hypothesis
either that they are similar or dissimilar to the target. Social media posts are especially likely to
highlight large discrepancies between the comparer and the poster, leading to a holistic
assessment of dissimilarity, and a subsequent test for evidence of dissimilarity. The
characteristics of social media will yield particularly compelling evidence that other people are
very superior to the self. Posters tend to focus on particularly positive events, showcasing a
“newsreel highlight” of their lives (Steers et al., 2014; Zhao, Grasmuck, & Martin, 2008); for
example, individuals do not merely post albums of vacation photos, but rather carefully choose,
and often digitally enhance, a few select photos that indicate that their vacation was spectacular
(Lo & McKercher, 2015). Moreover, such posts are likely to present a strong contrast with the
immediate experiences of post viewers, who are, by definition, staring at an electronic device,
often while engaged in mundane activities (Tien & Aynsley, 2019). Thus, we argue that
comparisons on social media are often to superior others who are not simply better off than the
self, but rather who appear to be much better off than the self. These more extreme comparisons
may in turn be particularly threatening: To the extent that individuals perceive these highly
positive outcomes to be unattainable, they will feel worse about themselves (Lockwood &
Kunda, 1997). Consistent with this argument, one study on physical appearance comparisons
found that women made more extreme upward comparisons to social media targets than to in-
person targets, and subsequently evaluated their attractiveness more negatively (Fardouly et al.,
2017). It is unclear, however, whether this effect extends to domains beyond physical
appearance. In sum, the present studies are the first to assess whether individuals make more
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
10
extreme upward comparisons when using social media than in other contexts, with a negative
impact on self-evaluations, mood, and life satisfaction.
Self-Esteem and Social Media Comparisons
The frequency and extremity of upward comparisons may also explain why social media
exerts an especially negative impact on individuals with low self-esteem. To the extent that low
self-esteem individuals chronically view themselves as of lower worth than other individuals, it
seems likely that they will be especially prone to view successful others as upward comparison
targets. Consistent with this possibility, a number of studies indicate that lower self-esteem is
indeed associated with more frequent upward comparisons (Locke & Nekich, 2000; Vohs &
Heatherton, 2004; Wayment & Taylor, 1995; for a review, see Wood & Lockwood, 1999).
Other studies suggest, however, that low self-esteem individuals suffer not because they find
many instances in which they are inferior to others but because they find few instances in which
they are superior to others (Locke, 2005; Wheeler & Miyake, 1992). Thus, the literature on self-
esteem and upward comparison frequency in offline contexts is mixed. Research focused
specifically on social media contexts has found that individuals lower in self-esteem report a
greater tendency to make Facebook comparisons in general (Cramer et al., 2016; Jang et al.,
2016); however, these studies did not directly examine the role of self-esteem in predicting the
actual frequency of upward relative to downward social media comparisons, nor did they
compare frequency of social comparisons across different contexts.
For low self-esteem individuals, social media offers especially fertile ground for making
upward comparisons. Given their negative self-perceptions, they may find it difficult to construe
themselves as being “in the same league” as someone experiencing success (Collins, 1996); as a
result, social media posts about even modestly positive achievements may be perceived as
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
11
upward comparisons. Thus, although low self-esteem individuals will make more upward
comparisons than will higher self-esteem individuals in general, this difference in frequency may
be amplified in social media contexts, where opportunities for comparisons abound, resulting in
especially negative consequences for the self.
In addition, to the extent that low self-esteem individuals chronically view themselves as
having lower self-worth than others, social media posts may yield especially extreme
comparisons. Low self-esteem individuals may be habitually likely to make a holistic evaluation
that they are dissimilar from successful others. Consistent with the selective accessibility model
(Mussweiler, 2003), this initial judgement will trigger a search for evidence that they are inferior
successful others. The exaggerated positivity of social media posts (Walther, 2007; Wilson et
al., 2012), in combination with the already impoverished self-evaluations of low self-esteem
individuals, should lead to perceptions of an especially large discrepancy between the self and
the poster – and thus a more extreme upward comparison. Consistent with this possibility, one
study found that participants with more contingent self-esteem were more likely to make more
extreme upward comparisons and subsequently experience worse mental health outcomes;
however, this research examined only physical appearance comparisons in non-social-media
contexts, and focused on contingent rather than low self-esteem (Patrick, Neighbors, & Knee,
2004). We argue that although individuals in general will make more extreme social
comparisons when using social media relative to other contexts, this will be especially true of
low self-esteem individuals; consequently, they will experience the greatest reduction in their
self-evaluations, mood, and life satisfaction following social media use.
Present Research
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
12
In sum, the present research provides the first evidence identifying the characteristics of
social comparisons on social media – heightened frequency and extremity – that differ from
social comparisons in other contexts. We also provide the first evidence that these particular
characteristics in turn contribute to social media’s harmful effects on the self, particularly for
individuals lower in self-esteem. In Study 1, we examined low and high self-esteem individuals’
actual comparison behavior in real time by assessing their reactions to posts in their own social
media news feeds. This enabled us to assess the direction, extremity, and impact of each
comparison made, as well as the cumulative effects of the frequency and extremity of these
comparisons at the end of the browsing session. In Study 2, we experimentally manipulated post
content; this enabled us to assess whether self-esteem would determine the perceived extremity
and impact of comparisons, while holding post content constant. In Study 3, we manipulated
context by asking participants to use social media or engage in other online activities on their
smartphone, and then assessed social comparison activity and outcomes. In Study 4, we used an
experience sampling methodology to examine, in a naturalistic setting, whether the frequency,
direction, and impact of comparisons on social media would differ from those in other contexts.
Across studies, we predicted that participants would make more frequent upward than
downward social media comparisons, which in turn would negatively impact state self-esteem,
mood, and life satisfaction (Studies 1-4). We also predicted that these upward comparisons
would be to more extremely superior others, and that, for each comparison, extremity would be
associated with a more negative impact on self-evaluations (Studies 1-4). Further, we predicted
that the frequency and extremity of upward comparisons would be greater in social media than in
other contexts, and so would have more negative outcomes (Studies 3 and 4). In addition, we
predicted that low self-esteem individuals would make more frequent and extreme upward social
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
13
comparisons than high self-esteem individuals, and consequently would experience the greatest
comparison threat (Studies 1-3); we predicted that these self-esteem effects would be especially
potent among individuals using social media relative to other contexts (Study 3).
Study 1
In Study 1, we assessed social comparisons among participants browsing their social media
news feeds on either Facebook or Instagram. After viewing each post, participants indicated
whether they had made a social comparison, and if so, the comparison domain, direction, and
impact on their self-evaluations. In addition, to assess the cumulative effects of the posts
viewed, we measured participants’ mood, state self-esteem, and life satisfaction after the
browsing session. Whereas past studies have used global retrospective self-reports regarding
social comparison activity on social media, participants in the present study reported on the
social comparisons they made while browsing their news feed in real time in the lab; this enabled
us to more accurately measure the frequency, direction, and extremity of social media
comparisons, while reducing potential bias in recall. We predicted that the more extreme the
upward comparison, the more negative the immediate impact on self-evaluations. In addition, we
predicted that individuals would make more upward comparisons than downward or lateral
comparisons, and these upward comparisons would have a cumulative negative impact on their
self-evaluations, mood, and life satisfaction.
Study 1 also allowed us to examine the role of self-esteem in determining the frequency,
extremity, and outcomes of comparisons occurring during an actual social media session. We
predicted that lower self-esteem participants would make more extreme upward comparisons
than high self-esteem individuals, and thus report feeling worse about themselves after each
comparison. In addition, we predicted that low self-esteem individuals would make more
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
14
frequent upward comparisons, which in turn would result in more negative mood, self-
evaluations, and life satisfaction at the end of the session.
Participants browsed either their Facebook or Instagram news feed on their smartphones
and answered questions about the first 20 posts on a desktop computer. We examined
comparisons on two popular social media platforms (Greenwood, Perrin, & Dugan, 2016) to
confirm that our results would generalize to more than one site.
Method
Participants
We recruited 251 introductory psychology students for a study on social media use.
Thirty-eight were excluded from our analyses: Nine participants experienced technical problems
with the survey, 18 participants did not complete the survey within the allotted time, four
participants indicated at the end of the session that they did not understand the instructions, four
participants behaved in a manner indicating that they did not take the study seriously (e.g., stated
that they gave random answers to finish the study faster), and three were unable to use their
smartphones to browse their social media feeds. Our analyses included 213 introductory
psychology students (157 women and 56 men; Mage=18.98 years, SD=1.64 years) who
participated for course credit. We collected sufficient data (i.e., at least 85 observations; Cohen,
1992) to detect a small effect at both levels of our multilevel models (NL1=1796; NL2=206).
Post-hoc power analyses revealed that we had at least 81.06% power for our primary multilevel
results. For our other analyses, sensitivity analysis revealed that we had sufficient power to
detect a small-to-medium effect (r=.19).
1
1
Across all studies, we had relatively small samples of males and thus limited statistical power to detect significant
gender effects.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
15
Procedure
Participants who use both Facebook and Instagram were invited to take part in a study on
social media use. Upon arrival at the lab, participants first completed the 10-item Rosenberg
Self-Esteem Scale (e.g., “I take a positive attitude toward myself”; α=.87; Rosenberg, 1965).
Participants rated themselves on a 7-point scale with endpoints ranging from 1 (strongly
disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The pretest also included questions about frequency of
Facebook and Instagram use; all participants indicated using both Facebook and Instagram at
least once per week. Finally, to ensure that participants understood what a social comparison
was and could complete the task successfully, they each completed a brief training session in
which they were provided with a detailed description of what does (and does not) constitute a
social comparison, and practiced identifying social comparisons in sample scenarios.
2
After the training session, participants were randomly assigned to either the Facebook or
Instagram condition and were asked to open the corresponding app on their smartphone. To
ensure that participants using both platforms were following a similar procedure, we asked them
to answer questions about the first 20 posts in their news feed. Participants were asked to
complete the questionnaire without navigating away from their news feed. For each post,
participants indicated the extent to which they had made a social comparison while viewing the
post (1=not at all; 7=completely). If participants had made a comparison (i.e., answered 2 or
above), they answered additional questions about the comparison.
Comparison questions. For posts that led to comparisons, participants first indicated in
which domain(s) the comparison occurred (from a list based on options provided in an earlier
study; Wheeler & Miyake, 1992), with the option of selecting one or multiple domains, or
2
The full version of the training session, including our comparison scenarios, is available in our study materials on
OSF (https://osf.io/acyzu/).
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
16
selecting ‘other’ and providing their own domain label (see Table 1 for a list of domain options).
Participants then reported the comparison direction, by indicating whether the comparison was to
someone worse- or better-off than themselves on a 7-point scale with endpoints ranging from -3
(much worse off than me) to +3 (much better off than me). This item enabled us to assess not
only the direction of the comparison, but also the extremity. For example, a score of either +1 or
+3 would indicate an upward comparison, but the +3 indicates a more extreme upward
comparison (Patrick et al., 2004). They then rated themselves on two self-evaluation items
(“After making this comparison, I felt better about myself” and “After making this comparison, I
felt worse about myself” [reverse-scored]) on a 7-point scale with endpoints ranging from 1
(strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Responses for the two self-evaluation items (r=-.71,
p<.001) were averaged to create a composite score of post-comparison self-evaluations.
Post-social media questionnaire. After answering questions about 20 posts, participants
were instructed to put down their smartphones. They then completed measures assessing their
affect, self-esteem, and life satisfaction.
State affect. Participants first completed the 20-item Positive and Negative Affect
Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). Ratings were made on a 5-point scale
(1=not at all, 5=extremely; α=.82).
State self-esteem. Participants then completed a state self-esteem measure (Heatherton &
Polivy, 1991), indicating how true a series of 20 statements were for them “right now” on a 5-
point scale (1=not at all, 5=extremely; α=.92).
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
17
Life satisfaction. Finally, participants completed the five-item Satisfaction with Life
Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985), indicating their agreement with each item on a
7-point scale (1=strongly disagree, 7=strongly agree; α=.85).
3
Results
Overview of Analyses
We first analyzed our data at the comparison-level, examining the domain, direction,
extremity, and impact of individual comparisons. We next examined our data at the session-
level, in order to assess whether frequency of upward comparisons predicted post-session
outcomes. Unless otherwise noted, we analyzed our data using R 3.6.0.
4
Individual Comparisons
Domains. Both Facebook and Instagram comparisons occurred in a variety of domains
(see Table 1). For both platforms, the top three domains of comparison were
looks/attractiveness, popularity/friendship, and vacations/activities/lifestyle.
Direction, extremity, and self-evaluations. Participants made comparisons that were,
on average, upward in direction (M=1.04, 95%CI [0.97, 1.11]
5
, SD=1.54). An intercept-only
multilevel model indicated this was significantly greater than the scale midpoint of 0, b=1.04,
95% CI [0.93, 1.15], SE=0.06, t(193.13)=17.99, p<.001. Moreover, a multilevel model with
direction separated into its between- and within-person components and a random slope of
comparison direction revealed that when individuals made comparisons that were more upward
in direction than usual, they reported lower self-evaluations after the comparison b=-0.64, 95%
3
Participants in this and subsequent studies answered additional questions not analyzed for this set of studies. The
complete questionnaires and datasets for these studies are available by emailing the authors.
4
Syntax for all manuscript analyses are available on OSF (https://osf.io/2k6vd/).
5
All 95% confidence intervals are calculated using the bootstrap percentile method.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
18
CI [-0.72, -0.55], SE=0.04, t(175.25)=-14.98, p<.001.
6
That is, for each participant, their more
“upward” comparisons (relative to that individual’s average comparison direction) resulted in
more negative outcomes than their less “upward” comparisons.
Self-esteem and self-evaluations. We then tested whether social media comparisons are
especially damaging for individuals lower in self-esteem because they make more extreme
comparisons. To test this hypothesis, we conducted a 2-1-1 multilevel mediation model because
the predictor (i.e., self-esteem) varied only at the level of the person, but the mediator (i.e.,
comparison extremity) and outcome variable (i.e., self-evaluations) varied across all posts, which
were nested within individuals. Thus, the person-level average of comparison extremity was
included as a covariate in the final model and direction was person-centered (Zhang, Zyphur, &
Preacher, 2009). This mediational hypothesis was tested using a variant of the bootstrap
procedure (Preacher & Hayes, 2008) amended for 2-1-1 multilevel mediation using the indirect
function in R (indirect.mlm; Page-Gould & Sharples, 2016) with 5,000 bootstrapped samples to
accurately estimate the indirect effect and its 95% confidence interval. Furthermore, because the
relationship between comparison extremity and self-evaluations could vary from person-to-
person, we modeled this path (i.e., b path) as a random slope (see Figure 1). Our analysis
revealed that trait self-esteem was associated with lower self-evaluations as a function of its
relationship with comparison extremity, abwithin=0.01, 95% CI [0.006, 0.02], abbetween=0.01,
95%CI [0.007, 0.02]: Individuals with lower self-esteem made comparisons that were more
upward, a=-0.03 [-0.04, -0.02], which in turn made them feel worse about themselves, bwithin=-
0.42, 95% CI [-0.45, -0.38], bbetween= -0.48, 95% CI [-0.54, -0.39]. Although the effect of trait
6
We compared a full model that included platform, trait self-esteem, person-mean comparison direction, person-
centered comparison direction, and their interactions to a model that included main effects only. Platform did not
moderate this effect,
c
2(2)=2.15, p=.34.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
19
self-esteem on self-evaluation was significant, c=0.06, 95% CI [0.05, 0.07], the direct effect of
self-esteem on self-evaluation was reduced when the indirect path through comparison extremity
was taken into account, c’=0.04, 95% CI [0.03, 0.06]. Thus, compared to individuals with higher
self-esteem, those with lower self-esteem felt worse after social media comparisons, at least in
part because these comparisons were more extreme in their upwardness.
Overall Session
Comparison frequency and post-session outcomes. Of the 20 posts viewed by
participants, an average of 8.42 (Mdn=8.00, SD=5.07) resulted in a social comparison.
Furthermore, a one-way repeated-measures ANOVA corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser
estimates (e=.78) revealed a significant effect of comparison type on comparison frequency,
F(1.56, 329.53) = 140.60, p<.001. Participants made more upward (M=5.32, SD=3.85) than
downward (M=1.20, SD=1.55; t(211)=14.80, p<.001) or lateral (M=1.90, SD=2.46;
t(211)=11.05, p<.001) comparisons. They also made more lateral than downward comparisons,
t(211)=3.78, p<.001.
7
We then examined whether comparison behavior over all 20 posts influenced subsequent
reports of mood, state self-esteem, and life satisfaction. To account for correlations between the
outcome measures, we conducted a multivariate regression in which mood, state self-esteem, and
life satisfaction were regressed simultaneously on the number of upward, downward, and lateral
comparisons made by participants. The total number of upward comparisons predicted
outcomes, F(3, 206)=8.95, p<.001, but number of lateral, F(3,206)=1.81, p=.15, or downward
7
Platforms did not differ in terms of number of comparisons reported, b=-0.02, SE=0.02, z=-0.96, p=.34. However,
relative to participants using Facebook, participants using Instagram were especially likely to make upward
comparisons relative to any other type of comparison, b=-0.54, SE=0.18, z=-3.03, p=.002, Odds Ratio=1.71:1.
Participants who used Instagram had a 71.39% chance of making an upward comparison, whereas participants who
used Facebook had a 59.36% chance of making an upward comparison.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
20
comparisons did not, F(3,206)=0.87, p=.46. Univariate analyses indicated that making a greater
number of upward comparisons was associated with less positive affect, b=-0.02, 95% CI [-0.04,
-0.002], SE=0.01, t(208)=-2.18, p=.030, r=.15, lower state self-esteem, b=-1.26, 95% CI [-1.84,-
0.70], SE=0.25, t(208)=-4.96, p<.001, r=.33, and lower life satisfaction, b=-0.10, 95% CI [-0.14,-
0.05], SE=0.02, t(208)=-4.22, p<.001, r=.28. There were no effects of number of lateral or
downward comparisons, ts<1.87, p>.06. Thus, regardless of the number of downward and lateral
comparisons participants made while viewing their news feeds, making more upward
comparisons predicted worse mood, lower state self-esteem, and diminished life satisfaction after
individuals viewed the 20 posts.
Self-esteem and post-session outcomes. Next, we tested whether individuals lower in
trait self-esteem would have lower state self-esteem, life satisfaction, and mood after browsing
their news feeds as a result of making more upward comparisons during the session. First, we
regressed number of upward comparisons on trait self-esteem using a Poisson regression with a
log link function to account for the fact that the number of upward comparisons represented
frequency counts and thus violated the normality assumption required for traditional regression.
Consistent with our hypothesis, low self-esteem individuals made more upward comparisons,
b=-0.04 [-0.06, -0.02], SE=0.01, z=-6.70, p<.001.
8
We then conducted three mediation analyses, one for each outcome, using a
bootstrapping procedure (Hayes, 2013) with 5,000 resamples and generating 95% confidence
8
There was a main effect of platform, b=-0.10, SE=0.03, z=-3.21, p=.001: Participants made more upward
comparisons on Instagram than on Facebook; this effect was qualified by a significant trait self-esteem by platform
interaction, b=0.02, SE=0.006, z=3.12, p=.002. Although lower self-esteem predicted making more upward
comparisons on both Facebook, b=-0.02, SE=0.01, z=-2.42, p=.02, and Instagram, b=-0.06, SE=0.008, z=-7.21,
p<.001, this effect was much larger on Instagram than on Facebook. Thus, low self-esteem individuals’ tendency to
make upward comparisons is exacerbated when they browse Instagram relative to when they browse Facebook.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
21
intervals for the indirect effects.
9
Number of upward comparisons mediated the positive
association between trait self-esteem and state self-esteem, ab=0.13, 95% CI [0.03, 0.26],
SE=0.06. Although the total effect of trait self-esteem on state self-esteem was significant,
c=2.23, 95% CI [1.90, 2.54], SE=0.16, the direct effect of trait self-esteem on state self-esteem
was reduced when the indirect path through number of upward comparisons was taken into
account, c’=2.10, 95% CI [1.77, 2.44], SE=0.16 (see Figure 2 Panel A). Number of upward
comparisons also mediated the positive association between trait self-esteem and life satisfaction,
ab=0.01, 95% CI [0.001, 0.02], SE=0.01. Although the total effect of trait self-esteem on life
satisfaction, c=0.16, 95% CI [0.12, 0.19], SE=0.02, was significant, the direct effect of self-
esteem on life satisfaction was reduced when the indirect path through number of upward
comparisons was taken into account, c’=0.14, 95% CI [0.11, 0.18], SE=0.02 (see Figure 2 Panel
B). Finally, we tested whether number of comparisons would mediate the positive association
between trait self-esteem and affect. Although this indirect effect was in the predicted direction,
it was not reliable, ab=0.001, 95% CI [-0.003, 0.005], SE=0.002.
10
Thus, when using either
Facebook or Instagram, participants with lower trait self-esteem reported making a greater
number of upward comparisons, and thus experienced significantly lower state self-esteem and
life satisfaction after the social media session.
Discussion
9
In the reported models, we treated number of upward comparisons as a continuous variable. However, we tested
additional models that treated number of upward comparisons as a count variable (Geldhof, Anthony, Selig, &
Mendez-Luck, 2018). These results were consistent with those reported in the manuscript and are reported in greater
detail in supplementary materials (https://osf.io/9vbw6/).
10
We tested whether there was a self-esteem by social media platform effect for any of the post-session outcomes
measured. There were no platform effects for affect or life satisfaction, ts<.66, ps>.51; however, there was a
significant trait self-esteem by platform interaction for state self-esteem, b=-0.42 [-0.74, -0.12], SE=0.15, t(208) =-
2.70, p=.01, r=.18: This effect was much larger for participants using Instagram, b=2.67 [2.31, 3.05], SE=0.22,
t(208)=11.90, p<.001, r=.64, than for those using Facebook, b=1.84 [1.31, 2.34], SE=0.21, t(208)=8.63, p<.001,
r=.51. Thus, low self-esteem individuals feel worse about themselves after browsing Instagram than after browsing
Facebook.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
22
Study 1 demonstrates a direct link between comparison behaviors and their immediate
consequences when browsing social media. Participants made comparisons that were primarily
upward, and many individuals made multiple comparisons in a single session, with a median of 8
comparisons in 20 posts. Across both social media platforms, making more upward comparisons
while viewing posts from others was associated with lower state self-esteem and life satisfaction
following the social media session, regardless of the number of downward and lateral
comparisons individuals had also made.
Additionally, we found that, compared to higher self-esteem individuals, those with lower
self-esteem reported making more extreme upward comparisons, which predicted lower self-
evaluations after each comparison. Furthermore, individuals with lower self-esteem reported a
greater number of upward comparisons, which predicted more negative state self-esteem and life
satisfaction after the session. This provides evidence that low self-esteem individuals may be
susceptible to making more frequent and more extreme upward comparisons, which are both
associated with more negative outcomes for the self.
Our results indicate that comparisons on social media occurred in a wide range of
domains. Whereas past research examining offline comparisons found that individuals tended to
make the most comparisons about academics and personality followed by physical appearance
and lifestyle (Wheeler & Miyake, 1992), we found that individuals made more comparisons
about attractiveness, popularity, and vacations and leisure activities when using social media.
Indeed, only 10% of comparisons made on social media were in the domains of personality and
academics (despite the fact that all participants were students). Thus, it appears that, with the
rise of social media, the domains in which individuals make comparisons may have shifted, with
a greater focus on physical appearance, popularity, and recreation activities. Although
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
23
comparison domain was not a focus of these studies, it is nevertheless useful to consider that
social media may prompt comparisons that differ in domain as well as frequency and extremity.
We note, however, that we did not directly compare comparisons on social media with those in
other contexts; we examine this in Studies 3 and 4.
Study 2
Study 1 provides evidence that self-esteem is associated with both more frequent and
more extreme upward comparisons while using social media, which in turn are associated with
more negative self-evaluations. We note, however, that this study used a correlational design. It
is possible that low self-esteem individuals are simply viewing different content than are their
higher self-esteem peers. For example, it may be that low self-esteem individuals have negative
self-perceptions because they have many superior friends, in which case the posts they view
from those friends on social media may be more positive and threatening, resulting in more
extreme upward comparisons. In Study 2, we assessed participants’ responses to a set of
Facebook posts we created for the purpose of the study, thus holding the valence of post content
constant for low and high self-esteem individuals. This design is similar to that employed by
Vogel et al. (2014) in which participants viewed a post with more or fewer likes, a manipulation
of popularity. In our study, we instead manipulated the content of the posts to determine the
impact of content valence.
Participants were a community sample recruited via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk
(MTurk); all viewed social media posts in which individuals described events that varied in
valence (positive, negative, neutral). Participants were told that posts were real examples taken
from the social media pages of participants who had taken part in a previous study; in fact, the
posts were created by the experimenters for the purposes of the study. After each post,
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
24
participants indicated whether they made a comparison and how they felt about themselves as a
result of the comparison.
As in Study 1, we examined whether self-esteem would predict comparison extremity.
Here, however, by exposing all participants to the same positive, negative, and neutral posts, we
can also rule out the possibility that low self-esteem individuals report making more upward
social media comparisons simply because they have friends who post a disproportionate amount
of positive content online. We predicted that, after viewing the same posts as people with higher
self-esteem, participants with lower self-esteem would report making upward comparisons that
were more upward in direction (i.e., more extreme), which in turn would lead to more negative
self-evaluations.
Method
Participants
Through MTurk, we recruited 103 individuals who were paid $1.00 USD. Participants
were eligible for the study if they used Facebook at least once per month and passed two
standard attention checks (Maniaci & Rogge, 2014). Nine participants failed one or both
attention checks, and three participants indicated they used Facebook less than once per month.
Our analyses included 91 participants (63 women, 27 men, 1 person of other/undisclosed gender;
Mage=32.95, SD=10.19 years). As in Study 1, we were interested in within- and between-person
effects and thus collected sufficient data to detect a small effect (r=.10) at each level (at least 85
observations; Cohen, 1992); post-hoc power analyses revealed that for all analyses (except one
which had .70 power), our final sample sizes (NL1=364; NL2=91) had at least .81 power.
Procedure
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
25
Participants were invited to take part in a study regarding social perceptions on Facebook.
First, participants completed the same self-esteem scale (α=.93) used in Study 1. Participants
were then presented with four posts, ostensibly written by past participants. Posts were
presented one at a time. After each one, participants answered a series of questions. All
participants saw two neutral posts that described everyday personal experiences (see Table 2);
they viewed these as the first and last of the four posts. Participants were then randomly
assigned to see either a positive then a negative post or a negative then a positive post. The
positive post described either a personal achievement or a pleasant outcome (i.e., getting a good
job or a positive relationship experience), and the negative post described a personal negative
experience (i.e., a lay-off or a break-up). In sum, all participants viewed four posts – two
neutral, one negative and one positive. We used two domains for each condition to ensure that
results were not limited to one domain only.
Facebook post questions. In line with the study’s cover story, participants read each
post and answered a series of questions about their perception of the posters’ personality. The
last question was the comparison direction measure (“To what extent do you feel this person is
worse-off or better-off than you?”); participants responded on a 7-point scale with endpoints
ranging from -3 (much worse off than me) to +3 (much better off than me) with a midpoint of 0
(neither worse off nor better off than me). Participants then completed a two-item self-
evaluation measure (r = .69) similar to the one used in Study 1; they indicated the extent to
which they felt worse about themselves and better about themselves on a 7-point scale ranging
from -3 (strongly disagree) to +3 (strongly agree).
Results
Overview of Analyses
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
26
We first examined whether participants were more likely to make an upward comparison
after viewing a positive (vs. neutral or negative) post and thus report lower self-evaluations. We
then examined whether lower self-esteem participants were more likely than their higher self-
esteem peers to make upward comparisons in response to positive posts. Finally, across all
posts, we examined whether, as in Study 1, participants with lower self-esteem made
comparisons that were more extremely upward and consequently experienced worse post-
comparison outcomes. Unlike Study 1, all analyses in Study 2 were conducted at the level of the
individual posts and comparisons, as participants viewed too few posts to assess frequency of
comparisons as an outcome measure.
Order effects. There was a significant effect of order on comparison extremity, b=0.24,
95% CI [0.09, 0.39], SE=0.08, t(346)=3.17, p<.001: Participants who saw the positive post
before the negative post (i.e., made an upward comparison first) rated comparisons to be more
upward than participants who saw the negative post before the positive post (i.e., made a
downward comparison first). Thus, although there was no effect of order on self-evaluation, b=-
0.03, 95% CI [-0.17, 0.10], SE=0.07, t(97.88)=-0.50, p=.62, we controlled for the effect of order
in all analyses.
Comparison Direction and Extremity
We then examined whether participants made different types of comparisons in response
to the positive, neutral, and negative posts using a one-way repeated measures ANOVA
corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser estimates (e=.76) because post type was nested within
person. Comparisons made in response to the positive posts (M=0.77, SD=1.46) were more
upward than those made to the neutral posts (M=0.16, SD=0.77), which were, in turn, more
upward than comparisons made to the negative posts, (M=-1.46, SD=1.19; F(1.51,
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
27
135.94)=108.80, p<.001, ts>3.81, ps<.001). This suggests that our manipulation of post content
was indeed effective, with positive content posts leading to upward comparisons, and negative
content posts leading to downward comparisons.
Self-esteem and comparison extremity. We then examined whether self-esteem
influenced comparison extremity for each post type. Comparison direction was modelled as a
function of self-esteem (grand-mean centered continuous variable), post valence (two dummy-
coded variables), and their interaction while controlling for the effect of order. There was a main
effect of post valence, c2(2)=162.41, p<.001: Positive posts resulted in more upward
comparisons (M=0.79, SE=0.12) than neutral (M=0.18, SE=0.09) and negative posts (M=-1.45,
SE=0.12). There was also a main effect of self-esteem, b=-0.20, SE=0.08, t(93.20)=-2.63, p=.01:
Individuals lower in self-esteem made more extreme upward comparisons. The post valence by
self-esteem interaction was not significant, c2(2)=5.27, p=.07. Although the overall interaction
did not reach significance, we tested the effect of self-esteem for each post type because of our a
priori hypothesis that individuals with lower self-esteem would report comparisons that were
more upward after viewing a positive post (Howell, 2013). To test this hypothesis, we recoded
the post valence variables so that each post valence was the reference group (Aiken & West,
1991), resulting in three 2-level multilevel models with random intercepts estimated using an
unstructured covariance matrix and the Satterthwaite method of estimating degrees of freedom.
Self-esteem effect sizes for each type of post is estimated using semi-partial R2 (Edwards,
Muller, Wolfinger, Qaqish, & Schabenberger, 2008).
For positive posts, there was a significant effect of self-esteem, b=-0.40, 95% CI [-0.63, -
0.16], SE=0.12, t(315.30)=-3.28, p=.001, semi-partial R2=0.03 (see Figure 3). In contrast, there
was no effect of self-esteem for either neutral, b=-0.15, 95% CI [-0.33, 0.04], SE=0.09,
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
28
t(186.14)=-1.57, p=.12, semi-partial R2=0.01, or negative posts, b=-0.06, 95% CI [-0.29, 0.19],
SE=0.12, t(315.30)=-0.45, p=.65, semi-partial R2=0.001. Thus, consistent with our hypothesis,
when exposed to the same positive posts, individuals lower in self-esteem tended to make more
extreme upward comparisons than individuals higher in self-esteem; however, lower self-esteem
individuals did not differ from higher self-esteem individuals in comparison extremity after
being exposed to neutral or negative posts. Because the overall interaction was not significant,
we note that these results must be interpreted with caution.
Self-Evaluations
To test whether participants felt worse after viewing the positive posts, relative to other
posts, as a result of making more extreme upward comparisons, we conducted a bootstrapped 1-
1-1 multilevel mediation using a similar analytic strategy as Study 1. Because we were primarily
interested in the occurrence of upward comparisons, and thus the effect of positive posts, we
entered post valence as two dummy-coded variables, one that compared positive to neutral posts
(0=positive, 1=neutral, 0=negative), and one that compared positive to negative posts
(0=positive, 0=neutral, 1=negative). The predictor (i.e., post valence) varied within participants
only. Thus, we did not enter the person-level average of post valence as a covariate in the final
model. Furthermore, the relationships between post valence and direction as well as direction
and self-evaluations could vary from person-to-person, so we modeled these paths (i.e., a and b
paths) as random slopes. Because the function we used allowed us to specify only a single
independent variable, we included the other dummy code as a covariate and ran the bootstrap
analysis twice, allowing each dummy code to be the independent variable once and the covariate
once (Hayes & Preacher, 2014). We also specified the same starting value (i.e., seed) to ensure
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
29
that the same bootstrap samples were used for both analyses. Finally, we controlled for the order
in which the posts were presented.
Our first analysis revealed that the difference between positive and neutral posts affected
self-evaluations as a function of its relationship with comparison direction, abwithin = 0.23, 95%
CI [0.09, 0.34], abbetween = 0.20, 95% CI [0.10, 0.35]: When participants read a positive post,
relative to a neutral one, they were more likely to make an upward comparison, awithin=-0.60,
95% CI [-0.85, -0.32], which in turn made them feel worse about themselves, bwithin -0.38, 95%
CI [-0.45, -0.19], bbetween= -0.33, 95% CI [-0.50, -0.23]. Although the difference between
positive and neutral posts on self-evaluations was significant, c= 0.30 [0.07, 0.53], the direct
effect of post valence was not significant when the indirect path through comparison direction
was taken into account, c’= 0.01, 95% CI [-0.21, 0.23] (see Figure 4 Panel A). The population
covariance for this model was estimated to be sab=0, 95% CI [-0.03, 0.07]. This implies that the
mediational model was consistent across individuals. Thus, posts appear to exert their impact on
self-evaluations through social comparisons.
Our second analysis revealed that the difference between positive and negative posts also
affected self-evaluations as a function of its relationship with comparison direction, abwithin=0.93,
95% CI [0.70, 1.32], abbetween=0.73, 95% CI [0.53, 1.20]: When participants saw a positive post,
relative to a negative one, they were more likely to make an upward comparison, awithin=-2.24,
95% CI [-2.53, -1.95], which in turn made them feel worse about themselves, bwithin=-0.42, 95%
CI [-0.55, -0.32], bbetween=-0.33, 95% CI [-0.52, -0.24]. Although the difference between positive
and negative posts on self-evaluations was significant, c= 0.88, 95% CI [0.63, 1.12], the direct
effect of post type was not significant when the indirect path through comparison direction was
taken into account, c’= -0.09, 95% CI [-0.54, 0.11] (see Figure 4 Panel B). As in the first model,
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
30
the population covariance for this model implies that that the mediational model was consistent
across individuals, sab=0.002, 95% CI [-0.02, 0.08]. Thus, consistent with our hypothesis,
participants felt worse about themselves after being exposed to positive posts, relative to
negative or neutral posts, because they made comparisons that were more upward in direction.
Self-esteem and self-evaluations. Finally, we tested whether, as in Study 1, self-esteem
predicted worse self-evaluations after viewing individual social media posts as a result of making
more extreme upward comparisons. We conducted a bootstrapped 2-1-1 multilevel mediation
using the same analytic strategy described in Study 1. Furthermore, the relationship between
comparison extremity and self-evaluations could vary from person-to-person; thus, we modeled
this path (i.e., b path) as a random slope (see Figure 5) as we did in Study 1. We also controlled
for order in this model. Our analysis revealed that trait self-esteem was associated with lower
self-evaluations as a function of its relationship with comparison extremity, abwithin=0.11, 95%
CI [0.03, 0.20], abbetween=0.07, 95% CI [0.02, 0.19]: Individuals with lower self-esteem made
comparisons that were more upward, a=-0.29, 95% CI [-0.53, -0.08], which in turn made them
feel worse about themselves, bwithin=-0.37, 95% CI [-0.43, -0.25], bbetween= -0.24, 95% CI [-0.46,
-0.18]. Although the effect of trait self-esteem on self-evaluation was significant, c=0.45, 95%
CI [0.29, 0.62], the direct effect of self-esteem on self-evaluation was reduced when the indirect
path through comparison extremity was taken into account, c’=0.39, 95% CI [0.20, 0.49]. Thus,
consistent with our hypothesis, lower self-esteem participants felt worse about themselves after
viewing social media posts at least in part because they made upward comparisons that were
more extreme in upwardness than those of higher self-esteem participants.
Discussion
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
31
In sum, posts that were positive in valence did lead participants to make upward
comparisons and, thus, feel worse about themselves. The more extreme the upward comparison,
the more negative the impact on the self. Furthermore, although all participants were likely to
make an upward comparison in response to the positive posts, lower self-esteem participants
interpreted these posts as more upward than did higher self-esteem participants. Indeed, we
replicated our first mediation model from Study 1: Compared to participants with higher self-
esteem, those with lower self-esteem tended to make more extreme upward comparisons and, as
a result, experienced greater decreases in their self-evaluations after being exposed to the same
content. Thus, low self-esteem individuals do not merely have better memory for upward
comparisons than their higher self-esteem peers, or view posts with more positive content.
Moreover, this effect was limited to positive posts only: Low self-esteem individuals perceived
individuals in negative and neutral posts similarly to higher self-esteem individuals; they were
not less likely to see the worse-off others as downward comparisons and were not more likely to
see neutral posts as upward comparisons. Therefore, these data provide initial evidence
suggesting that it is not any comparison behavior in general (Steers et al., 2014), but rather
positive posts resulting in more extreme upward comparisons that are a key contributor to low
self-esteem individuals’ more negative outcomes following social media use. Because the
overall interaction between post valence and self-esteem was not significant, however, we note
that the findings related to extremity and self-esteem must be interpreted with caution.
In Studies 1 and 2, participants reported on comparisons throughout the time they spent
viewing social media posts. Although this provided information about participants’ responses to
each post they viewed, we note that this procedure may also have created demand characteristics,
in that participants may have been especially likely to notice and report on social comparisons.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
32
It may be that participants make fewer actual social comparisons when they are not prompted to
think about them in this way. Accordingly, in Study 3, instead of asking participants to report on
each comparison as it occurred, we instead asked them to report on comparisons at the end of the
session, without alerting them in advance to the focus on comparison behavior.
Study 3
We argue that social media may be especially likely to elicit upward comparisons, and
that these comparisons will have a negative impact, particularly among individuals low in self-
esteem, who make more frequent (Study 1) and more extreme (Studies 1 and 2) upward
comparisons. Up to this point, however, we have not directly compared social media
comparisons to those that occur in other contexts. In Study 3, we experimentally manipulated
context to examine whether social media would indeed be especially likely to elicit threatening
upward comparisons. Specifically, participants were randomly assigned to use their smartphone
either to access social media or for any other purposes (e.g., surf the net, text, watch online
videos) for 10 minutes; they then reported their self-evaluations and information about any
comparisons they had made. This manipulation allowed us to examine whether social media
comparisons differ from other technology-based comparisons, and to assess whether people feel
worse after using social media relative to engaging in other technology-based activities as a
result of the type of social comparisons they make. Finally, we evaluated whether low self-
esteem individuals would be especially likely to make upward comparisons, and consequently be
negatively affected, on social media relatively to other online contexts.
We also used this study to examine two variables found in previous research (Tesser,
1988) to be implicated in comparison outcomes: closeness of the comparison target and domain
relevance. Past research indicates that individuals are more threatened when outperformed by a
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
33
close other in a domain that is important to them (Tesser, Millar, & Moore, 1988). Many social
media contacts, however, are more distant and may even be past classmates or celebrities with
whom one has no direct contact. Moreover, individuals often view posts about leisure activities
or activities that may not be directly relevant to the self. This would suggest that, according to
the self-evaluation maintenance (SEM) model (Tesser, 1988), social media comparisons might
be less threatening than comparisons in other contexts. Accordingly, we measured both
closeness and domain importance. Studies 1 and 2 provided initial evidence that comparisons on
social media would be threatening to the self. Accordingly, we expected that, despite lower
closeness and domain importance, social media comparisons would nevertheless pose a greater
threat to the self than those in other contexts, due to their greater frequency and extremity.
Method
Participants
In total, 482 participants who indicated in a pre-screen survey that they owned a
smartphone and currently used either Facebook, Instagram, or both completed the study for
$1.50 USD. We excluded 67 participants from the analyses: Thirty-five participants did not
follow the instructions for the browsing session (i.e., went on Facebook and/or Instagram in the
no social media condition or did not go on Facebook and/or Instagram in the social media
condition), 4 participants failed our comparison training session
11
, and 28 participants submitted
the survey more than once. For duplicate responses, we retained the first completed response
and deleted subsequent responses.
Our analyses included 415 MTurk workers (245 women, 168 men, and 2 persons of
other/undisclosed gender; Mage=37.55 years, SD=12.79 years). We collected sufficient data (i.e.,
11
We considered answering fewer than half of the questions correctly as failing the comparison training session.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
34
at least 85 observations; Cohen, 1992) to detect a small effect at both levels of our multilevel
models (NL1= 255; NL2=124). For our other analyses, sensitivity analysis revealed that we had
sufficient power to detect a small-to-medium effect (r=.14).
Procedure
Pre-screen survey. We invited participants to complete a 5-minute pre-screen eligibility
survey that included questions about their technology and social media use. To be eligible for
the study, participants had to indicate they owned a smartphone, which they would use during
the experimental manipulation, and a second device they could use to complete the actual survey
(i.e., laptop or desktop computer); in addition, they had to indicate that they used either
Facebook, Instagram, or both platforms. As part of this pre-screen survey, participants
completed the same self-esteem measure used in Studies 1 and 2 (a=.93).
Study questionnaire. We asked participants to use their smartphones for 10 minutes,
specifying that they either refrain from using social media (no social media condition) or spend
the entire 10 minutes using Facebook and/or Instagram (social media condition). At the end of
the browsing session, participants indicated what they did during the browsing session (see Table
3). To encourage honest reporting, we told participants that their compensation would not be
affected by whether or not they followed the manipulation instructions. Next, participants in
both groups completed the same comparison training session used in Study 1 and then reported
whether they had made any comparisons during the 10-minute browsing session. If they
reported making at least one comparison, they were asked to enter the first names of each target
to whom they compared themselves. For each target they listed, participants were asked
additional questions about the comparison. As in Studies 1 and 2, we asked in what domain the
comparison occurred and the extent to which the comparison target was doing better or worse
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
35
than the self. In Study 3, we also asked participants to indicate how close they felt to the
comparison target on a 7-point scale with endpoints ranging from 0 (not at all) to 6 (extremely).
Participants also rated how important the domain was to them using a 7-point scale with
endpoints ranging from 0 (not at all important) to 6 (extremely important). Finally, participants
reported their state self-evaluations using a one-item measure (“Right now, how do you feel
about yourself?”) rated on a 7-point scale with endpoints ranging from -3 (much worse about
myself than usual) to +3 (much better about myself than usual).
Results
Overview of Analyses
As in Study 1, we first present our analyses at the comparison-level, examining whether
comparisons on social media differed from comparisons in other computer-mediated contexts in
terms of domain, direction, and impact. We also examine whether, as in Studies 1 and 2,
comparisons made by individuals lower in self-esteem were more upward in direction than those
made by individuals with higher self-esteem. We then present session-level analyses in which
we first examine whether context and self-esteem were associated with frequency of upward
comparisons during the 10-minute session. In addition, we examine whether spending time on
social media (vs. not using social media) predicted greater likelihood of making one or more
upward comparisons and thus lower self-evaluations at the end of the session. Finally, we
assessed whether this session-level effect was stronger for individuals with lower self-esteem:
We assessed whether low self-esteem individuals using social media were especially likely to
make one or more upward comparisons, and thus experience the most negative consequences to
the self.
Individual Comparisons
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
36
Domains. Consistent with Study 1, we found that social media comparisons occurred in
a variety of domains (see Table 1), and the most common domains of comparison in this context
again included looks/attractiveness and vacations/activities/lifestyle. In contrast, the most
common comparison domains in other technology-based contexts were health/physical fitness,
personality/morality, and skills/abilities. A series of logistic multilevel models, however,
indicated that comparisons made on social media (0) compared to those in the no social media
condition (1) were not significantly more likely to be about looks/attractiveness, b=-0.42,
SE=0.66, z=-0.63, p=.53, or vacations/activities/lifestyle, b=-0.43, SE=0.54, z=-0.79, p=.43, but
were less likely to be about personality/morality, b=1.12, SE=0.002, z=598.98, p<.001.
Domain importance. We next examined whether domain importance ratings differed by
experimental condition using a multilevel model. We modeled domain importance ratings as a
function of comparison context (effects-coded: -1=no social media condition; + 1= social media
condition) with a random intercept for each person. Experimental conditions did not differ in
terms of domain importance, b=-0.01, 95% CI [-0.29, 0.26], SE=0.14, t(112.79)=-0.10, p=.92.
Domains of comparisons made on social media (M=3.88, SE=0.16) and in other technology-
based contexts (M=3.91, SE=0.22) were both rated as moderately important. Thus, we found no
evidence that social media comparisons involve domains that are more (or less) personally
relevant than comparisons in other technology-based contexts.
Closeness to target. We then examined whether closeness to the comparison target
differed depending on the context in which a comparison was made; we used the same multilevel
model as we used for domain importance. Experimental conditions did not differ in terms of
target closeness, b=-0.04, 95% CI [-0.39, 0.31], SE=0.18, t(119.37)=-0.24, p=.81. Targets in
social media comparisons (M=2.10, SE=0.21) and in other technology-based contexts (M=2.18,
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
37
SE=0.29) were rated as relatively low in closeness. In sum, we found no evidence that
comparisons on social media were in domains higher (or lower) in importance, or to targets with
greater (or lesser) closeness to the self. This suggests that neither closeness nor importance can
account for any differences we observed in comparison outcomes between social media and
other online contexts.
12
Direction, extremity, and self-evaluations. Participants’ self-evaluations after
comparisons were modelled as a function of comparison context condition with a random
intercept for each person, b=-0.18, 95% CI [-0.41, 0.05], SE=0.12, t(130.58)=-1.51, p=.13.
Although this effect was not significant, it was in the expected direction: Compared to other
contexts, comparisons made while using social media were associated with feeling worse about
the self after the comparison. Next, extremity of comparison direction was modelled as a
function of comparison context with a random intercept for each person. This analysis revealed
that social media comparisons were not more upward in direction than those in other technology-
based contexts, b=0.07, 95% CI [-0.19, 0.33], SE=0.13, t(127.04)=0.52, p=.60. Additionally,
unlike in Studies 1 and 2, lower self-esteem did not predict making more extreme upward
comparisons, b=-0.08, 95% CI [-0.34, 0.18], SE=0.13, t(132.10)=-0.59, p=.56. We review
possible explanations for these findings below.
Overall Session
Comparison frequency. Next, we conducted a series of Poisson regressions to examine
whether conditions differed in terms of number of comparisons. Participants assigned to the
social media condition made more comparisons (M=0.84, SD=1.50) than those assigned to the no
12
We did not have sufficient power to test the full SEM model (i.e. a potential 3-way interaction between domain
importance, closeness, and context). However, differences in closeness across the two contexts did not mediate the
difference in comparison outcomes between contexts.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
38
social media condition (M=0.40, SD=1.20), b=0.36, 95% CI [0.14, 0.61], SE=0.07, z=5.50,
p<.001. Furthermore, participants in the social media condition made a greater number of
upward, b=0.35, 95% CI [0.11, 0.64], SE=0.08, z=4.58, p<.001, and lateral comparisons, b=0.48,
95% CI [0.08, 0.99], SE=0.19, z=2.59, p=.01, than those in the no social media condition. There
was no difference between the two conditions for number of downward comparisons, b=0.31,
95% CI [-0.17, 1.04], SE=0.18, z=1.69, p=.09. Thus, individuals using social media made more
comparisons than those simply browsing the Internet. Moreover, social media use was
associated with an increased number of upward and lateral comparisons, but not downward
comparisons. We note that participants reported less frequent comparisons relative to Studies 1
and 2. This is not surprising: Because participants in the social media condition were free to use
the platforms in any way they chose (e.g., navigate away from their news feed to read an article,
engage in other activities simultaneously such as listening to music or a podcast) rather than
viewing only news feed posts (as in Studies 1 and 2), and reported all comparisons at the end of
the social media session, rather than after each post (as in Studies 1 and 2), they likely had fewer
opportunities to make and/or take note of their individual social comparisons. Although the
overall frequency of comparison was lower, we nevertheless observed the predicted difference,
with more upward comparisons in the social media condition. See Table 4 for comparison
frequency overall and within each condition.
Self-esteem and upward comparison frequency. We then tested whether self-esteem
was associated with number of upward comparisons made during the 10-minute session. As
predicted, participants with lower self-esteem made a greater number of upward comparisons
than participants with higher self-esteem, b=-0.38 [-0.52, -0.24], SE=0.06, z=-6.63, p<.001.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
39
Post-session self-evaluation. Next, we tested whether context influenced how
participants felt about themselves after the 10-minute smartphone session. Participants assigned
to the social media condition (M=0.36, SE=0.08) reported lower self-evaluations at the end of the
study than those in the no social media condition (M=0.58, SE=0.08), b=-0.11, 95% CI [-0.22, -
0.005], SE=0.06, t(413)=-2.03, p=.04. We then tested whether making one or more upward
comparisons (vs. no upward comparisons)
13
mediated this difference between the two
experimental conditions (social media vs. no social media; see Figure 6).
14
Given the non-
normal distribution of presence of upward comparisons, our mediator, we conducted a nonlinear
mediation analysis based on the generalized linear model by specifying a path model with
binomial and Gaussian (normal) distributions (Geldhof et al., 2018). We used nonlinear
mediation analysis (Hayes & Preacher, 2010) to calculate conditional indirect effects (i.e.,
indirect effects at specific values of X) and used a bootstrapping procedure to calculate
confidence intervals with 5000 resamples using MPlus 7.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-2012)
15
.
Consistent with our hypothesis, the indirect effect through presence vs. absence of upward
comparisons was significant, ab=-0.16, 95%CI [-0.47, -0.04], SE=0.12, and the direct effect
became nonsignificant, c’=-0.14, 95% CI [-0.37, 0.08], SE=0.11, once this indirect effect was
included. Taken together, these findings suggest that individuals felt worse after using social
media, relative to those who browse the Internet without using social media, because they were
more likely to make one or more upward comparisons.
13
Given the limited variance in number of upward comparisons (1.24), we transformed our count variable to a
binary variable to test whether making at least one upward comparison mediated the effect of self-esteem on self-
evaluations. Our indirect effects were nonsignificant when we treated number of upward comparisons as a count
variable; however, based on the results of Study 1, we would predict that making more upward comparisons would
predict worse outcomes.
14
Our results for our simple and moderated mediation models remained consistent when we controlled for number
of downward comparisons and number of lateral comparisons.
15
We describe this and other analyses in greater detail in our supplementary materials (https://osf.io/9vbw6/).
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
40
Self-esteem and post-session self-evaluation. Finally, we tested a moderated mediation
model in which the association between dispositional self-esteem and self-evaluation at the end
of the study was mediated by making one or more upward comparisons (see Figure 7). We
expected this indirect effect to be larger in the social media condition than in the no social media
condition. Given the non-normal distribution of the mediator, we used a similar analytic strategy
as that described above; however, we amended this approach for a moderated mediation (Hayes,
2015). That is, we calculated conditional indirect effects at specific values of the predictor, self-
esteem (i.e., one standard deviation above and below the mean), and the moderator, experimental
condition. These conditional indirect effects are reported in Table 5. Consistent with our
hypothesis, the direct effect, c’=0.10, 95% CI [0.008. 0.19], SE=0.05, z=2.19, p=.03, was
reduced once we accounted for the indirect effect through presence or absence of upward
comparisons. Then, we conducted an omnibus test to determine whether these conditional
indirect effects at various levels of self-esteem differed between experimental conditions. This
test indicated that this trend was not reliable,
c
2(2)=5.84, p=.054. Nonetheless we take this trend
as tentative evidence that our indirect effect was moderated by context. Taken together, these
findings suggest that individuals lower in dispositional self-esteem may be more likely to make
more upward comparisons than those higher in self-esteem, particularly when using social
media; this greater number of comparisons on social media, in turn, is associated with lower self-
evaluations for low self-esteem individuals following social media use.
Discussion
Study 3 demonstrates that spending 10 minutes on social media, relative to other online
activities, increases the chances of making one or more upward social comparisons and thus
feeling worse about the self. Furthermore, the increased likelihood of making upward
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
41
comparisons mediated the link between social media use and lowered self-evaluations:
Individuals using social media feel worse about themselves because they are likely to make more
frequent upward comparisons. Moreover, this effect occurs despite the fact that social media
comparisons involve targets no closer to the self and in domains that are no more personally
relevant than comparisons made in other online contexts. This finding is theoretically significant
because it suggests that social media comparisons may not play by the same rules as other
comparisons. Past studies, including research supporting the self-evaluation maintenance model,
indicates that comparisons to less close others are less likely to result in either a threatening
contrast effect or a positive basking in reflected glory effect (e.g., Tesser, 1988). Social media
comparison targets are no more close than other online comparison targets, yet they lead to more
threatening upward social comparisons. It may be that social media contacts, while less
psychologically close than individuals with whom one has more recent or in-person contact, may
nevertheless represent important standards against which one measures one’s life successes. One
may not feel close to a former high school classmate, but one may nevertheless feel a pang when
one sees a post from the classmate highlighting their superior career accomplishments, their
recent engagement, or their children’s academic successes. Our findings suggest that further
research will be important to assess whether closeness is the best variable to evaluate the
relevance of social media contacts as comparison others.
This study also provides important evidence that low self-esteem individuals are
especially vulnerable to self-esteem threats as a result of their social comparisons. They were
more likely to make upward comparisons than were high self-esteem individuals, and this self-
esteem effect tended to be more pronounced among participants using social media. Contrary to
our predictions, however, we did not find that individuals made more extreme upward
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
42
comparisons while using social media or that low self-esteem individuals made especially
extreme upward comparisons. One possible explanation is that participants were reporting on all
comparisons at the end of the session, and thus were unable to identify or report subtle
differences in comparison extremity. In addition, we note that many individuals in the no social
media condition were viewing videos or other online material in which they may have been
exposed to celebrities or other very positive content. Thus, it is possible that the relatively
smaller number of comparisons made in the no-social-media contexts were as extremely upward
as the more numerous upward comparisons on social media. That is, it may be that upward
comparisons are more extreme in all online media, whether social media, videos, or other
websites.
In Study 3, we asked participants about their social comparisons at the end of the 10
minutes that participants spent on their phones, rather than after each opportunity for
comparison, as in Studies 1 and 2. This methodology reduces the likelihood that individuals
were reporting many comparisons because they felt prompted to do so after each post viewed.
Indeed, participants reported fewer comparisons overall in Study 3 compared to Study 1, and
although this may be partially due to spending less time on social media in Study 3, it is also
likely due to reduced demand characteristics. Although the total number of comparisons made
was lower, however, we nevertheless observed a greater frequency of social comparisons on
social media than in other contexts; this difference across contexts cannot be attributed solely to
demand characteristics because both conditions received the same instructions after using their
phone and before reporting any social comparisons they made.
Study 4
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
43
Study 3 provides important evidence that social media comparisons differ from those
made in other technology-based contexts. It did not, however, include comparisons that occur in
offline contexts, such as face-to-face interactions. Thus, in Study 4, we conducted an experience
sampling study to examine participants’ social comparison behavior across all contexts.
Participants installed a custom-made experience sampling app on their smartphone for two
weeks that prompted them six times a day to complete a short survey about any comparisons
they had made since the previous report. This enabled us to: 1) examine the direction,
frequency, extremity, and outcomes of social media comparisons as they occur in daily life; 2)
compare social comparisons made on social media with those in all other contexts; and 3) assess
whether the amount of time spent on social media predicts upward comparison frequency.
Whereas the between-subjects design of Study 3 allowed us to assess whether individual
differences in self-esteem predict different outcomes in different contexts (i.e., social media vs.
other technology-based activities), Study 4 was designed to further investigate within-person
effects of context on social comparison outcomes. That is, we were able to assess whether
individuals make more frequent and more extreme upward comparisons when spending time on
social media than they do when spending time in other contexts.
Method
Participants
We recruited 87 undergraduate students from a university in a large urban center to
participate in a two-week long study on daily experiences
16
. Two participants were unable to
participate because the smartphone app was incompatible with their devices, and six participants
16
At the outset of the study, we aimed to recruit as many participants as possible, up to a maximum of 100. We
ended recruitment at the conclusion of the semester, which coincided with the end of the academic year. We decided
to end recruitment because we anticipated being unable to follow-up with a significant portion of participants during
the summer months.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
44
did not complete any experience sampling questionnaires after attending the intake session. In
our final dataset, we had experience sampling data from 79 participants (51 women, 26 men, and
2 person of other/undisclosed gender; Mage=20.15 years, SD=2.40 years). Only 66 participants
(83.54%) returned for the exit session and thus completed all three components of the study.
Participants were invited to take part only if they owned a smartphone and reported that they
used social media. We compensated participants up to $60, depending on the extent to which
they participated in the study ($16 for the intake session, $2 per day that they completed 4 or
more surveys, $3 for each completed week, and $10 for the exit session). On average,
participants provided 62.51 surveys (SD=24.78) for 12 of the 14 survey days (SD=4.0; median
response rate= 100), for a total of 4382 completed surveys.
Procedure
Participants first came to the lab for an intake session, during which they were asked to
install the experience sampling app (ExperienceSampler, Thai & Page-Gould, 2018); a research
assistant then instructed them on how to use the app and how to recognize and accurately report
on social comparisons. Notifications were customized to participants’ sleep and wake times and
included an opportunity to specify different times for weekdays and weekends. For the following
two weeks, the app randomly prompted participants to complete a short survey six times per
day
17
, in which they indicated the time they had spent on various activities (e.g., face-to-face
interactions, texting, social media) and whether they had made a comparison since the previous
survey. Whenever participants indicated they had made a comparison, they then reported to
whom they had compared themselves (i.e., close friend, ordinary friend, acquaintance, past
17
In accordance with recommendations outlined in Wheeler & Reis (1991), we opted for signal-contingent
sampling, as we expected social comparisons to occur frequently (i.e., numerous times per day) and were interested
in their relative distribution across a variety of domains.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
45
friend, stranger, famous person, fictional character, sibling, other family member, romantic
partner, or other) and their closeness to that person on a 7-point scale with endpoints ranging
from 0 (not at all close) to 6 (extremely close). Participants then indicated the primary
comparison domain from a list (see Table 1) and indicated the importance of that domain to them
on a 7-point scale with endpoints ranging from 0 (not at all important) to 6 (extremely
important). Next, they indicated in what context that comparison took place (i.e., in person/face-
to-face, video call, voice-only phone call, other voice chat, email, texting/SMS, social media,
dating app/website, other media/online context, or in a thought/daydream). If participants
indicated the comparison was made while using social media, they were also asked to indicate
the platform (e.g., Instagram, Facebook).
18
Finally, participants indicated the direction and
outcomes of the comparison, using the same questions as in Studies 1-3. At the end of the
survey, regardless of comparison behavior, participants reported their current self-evaluations on
a 7-point scale with endpoints ranging from -3 (Much worse about myself than usual) to +3
(Much better about myself than usual). After the two-week experience sampling period,
participants returned to the lab for compensation and debriefing.
Results
Overview of Analyses
As in Studies 1 and 3, we first present analyses conducted at the individual comparison-
level, contrasting social media comparisons to those made in other contexts (e.g. those made
face-to-face, in a thought/daydream, or in any other computer-mediated context other than social
18
Participants were also asked to indicate which aspect of the social media platform (i.e., public feed, private
chat/message, or group chat/message) they were using at the time of the comparison, to distinguish comparisons
made in private messages from those made while browsing news feeds. Given that the news feed is a unique feature
of social media that does not exist in other contexts and our focus on the effects of news feed posts on comparison
behaviour in Studies 1-2, we compared news feed comparisons to comparisons made in all other contexts.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
46
media). We then present analyses for the overall experience sampling period, examining the
total number of comparisons participants made on social media compared to in other contexts.
19
Individual Comparisons
Domains. Consistent with Studies 1 and 3, the two most common domains of
comparisons on social media were looks/attractiveness and vacations/activities/lifestyle. In this
study, unlike Study 3, looks/attractiveness was also a common domain in contexts other than
social media, second only to academics/careers (see Table 1). This difference may be due to the
samples involved in each study: Study 3 consisted of primarily middle-aged adults, whereas
Study 4 consisted of primarily undergraduate students. Compared to middle-aged adults,
undergraduate students are less likely to have established careers and be in long-term
relationships, both of which may increase their interest in making comparisons in the
academics/careers and looks/attractiveness domains. In the present study, a logistic multilevel
model revealed that looks/attractiveness comparisons were 77.50% times more likely to occur on
social media than in other contexts, b=1.24, SE=0.001, z=1311.77, p<.001, Odds Ratio=3.44:1.
That is, although looks/attractiveness comparisons were common across all contexts, they were
especially likely when individuals were using social media.
Domain importance. Relative to comparisons in other contexts, comparisons made
while using social media were in domains that participants rated as less personally important, b=-
0.10, 95% CI [-0.19, -0.01], SE=0.05, t(999.01)=-2.06, p=.039. Thus, consistent with Study 3,
we found that social media comparisons did not involve domains that were more personally
important to participants than comparisons made in other contexts.
19
In contrast to Studies 1-3, we did not have the required number of participants to test for small effects at the
person-level (i.e. 85; Cohen, 1992), so we did not examine the role of self-esteem in this study.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
47
Closeness to target. Relative to comparisons in other contexts, comparisons made while
using social media involved targets that participants rated as less close to the self, b=-0.36, 95%
CI [-0.49, -0.22], SE=0.07, t(1135.62)=-5.23, p<.001. These lower closeness targets
nevertheless elicited a more negative impact on the self, as will be discussed next.
20
Comparison direction, extremity, and self-evaluations. Next, we tested a mediational
pathway such that comparisons made while browsing one’s social media news feed, relative to
comparisons made in other contexts (0= other contexts; 1= social media news feed), were more
extreme (i.e., more upward), resulting in worse post-comparison self-evaluations. We conducted
a bootstrapped 1-1-1 multilevel mediation using a similar analytic strategy as Studies 1 and 2
(see Figure 8). Because all predictors varied within person, we separated the predictor (context)
and mediator (comparison extremity) into their between- and within-person components, but we
report the within-person components only because we are primarily interested in the within-
person differences in context. We also included time as a covariate in our model. Finally,
because the relationship between context and comparison extremity (i.e., a path) as well as the
relationship between comparison extremity and self-evaluations (i.e., b path) could vary from
person-to-person, we modeled these paths as random slopes. Our analysis revealed that
comparison context affected self-evaluations as a function of its relationship with comparison
extremity, ab=-0.26, 95% CI [-0.44, -0.13]: Comparisons made on social media news feeds,
relative to other contexts, did indeed result in comparisons that were more extreme in their
upwardness, a= 0.74, 95% CI [0.37, 1.25], which in turn made participants feel worse about
themselves, b=-0.35, 95% CI [-0.40, -0.32]. Although the difference between comparison
20
As in Study 3, we did not have enough power to test the full SEM model (i.e., a potential 3-way interaction
between domain importance, closeness, and context). Furthermore, neither decreased closeness to the comparison
target nor lower domain importance mediated the effect of context on comparison outcomes, indicating that these
variables may not play as much of a role in social media comparisons as in other contexts.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
48
contexts on self-evaluations was significant, c=-0.44, 95% CI [-0.76, -0.16], the direct effect of
context was not significant when the indirect path through comparison extremity was taken into
account, c’=-0.02, 95% CI [-0.31, 0.21]. The population covariance for this model was
estimated to be sab=0, 95% CI [-0.015, 0.015]. This implies that the mediational model was
consistent across individuals. Thus, participants felt worse about themselves after making
comparisons on social media relative to comparisons in other contexts, at least in part because
these comparisons were more extreme in their upwardness.
21
Overall Experience Sampling Period
Context and comparison likelihood. We first examined whether time spent on each
activity predicted the likelihood of making a social comparison for any given survey using a
logistic multilevel model with a random intercept, the within- and between-person components
of time spent on each activity, and time as a covariate. When individuals spent more time on
social media than they usually did, they were more likely to report making a social comparison
when they were signalled, b=0.01, 95% CI [0.008, 0.013], SE=0.002, z=5.07, p<.001. No other
within-person effects were significant, zs<1.52, ps>.12. In addition, individuals who used social
media more, relative to other participants, were more likely to report making a social comparison
when they were signalled, b=0.05, 95% CI [0.004, 0.07], SE=0.01, z=3.28, p=.001. No other
between-person effects were significant, zs<1.28, ps>.20.
21
We tested whether comparisons made on Facebook and Instagram, the two most common social media platforms,
differed in extremity. When individuals reported making a comparison on Instagram, relative to a comparison on
Facebook, they reported a comparison that was more extreme in its upwardness; however, this within-person effect
was not significant, b=0.66, SE=0.39, t(149.07)=1.67, p=.09. We also tested whether comparisons differed in terms
of likelihood of being upward depending on the platform. Facebook and Instagram comparisons were equally likely
to be upward, relative to other types of comparisons, b=0.84, SE=0.66, z=1.29, p=.20.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
49
Context and comparison frequency. Next, we examined whether total time spent on
each activity predicted total number of comparisons.
22
On average, participants reported making
1.44 comparisons per day (SD=1.27), of which fewer were made on social media (M=0.22,
SD=0.42) than in other contexts (M=1.26, SD=1.08; t=-8.80, p<.001). However, a Poisson
regression that included total time spent on various activities during the study and number of
surveys completed revealed that total minutes spent on social media, b=0.0004, 95% CI [0.0002,
0.0008], SE=0.00004, z=9.87, p<.001, and dating apps, b=0.01, 95% CI [-0.02, 0.02], SE=0.002,
z=2.99, p=.003, were positively associated with number of comparisons, whereas more time
spent face-to-face, b=-0.00004, 95% CI [-0.0002, 0.00004], SE=0.00001, z=-3.07, p=.002, and
on email, b=-0.003, 95% CI [-0.01, 0.01], SE=0.0007, z=-4.37, p<.001, were negatively
associated with number of comparisons. Time spent on all other activities did not predict
number of comparisons, zs<1.75, ps>.08.
Furthermore, additional Poisson regressions revealed that number of social media
minutes were associated with a greater number of upward
23
, b=0.004, 95% CI [0.0001, 0.001],
SE= 0.0001, z= 6.27, p<.001, lateral, b= 0.0005, 95% CI [0.00003, 0.001], SE= 0.0001, z=4.14,
p<.001
24
, and downward comparisons, b=0.0005, 95% CI [0.0001, 0.001], SE=0.0001, z= 5.54,
p<.001
25
. Paired t-tests indicated that upward comparisons were, however, more common on
22
The data for total time spent on each activity (except social media, skewness =1.61) were highly positively
skewed, skewness > 2.90. Thus, the bootstrapped confidence intervals for these coefficients may include 0.
23
Time spent on dating apps was the only other activity that predicted making more upward comparisons, b=0.008
[-0.02, 0.02], SE=0.002, z=3.14, p=.002. More time spent emailing, b=-0.003, 95% CI [-0.01, 0.007], SE=0.001, z=-
3.63, p<.001, face-to-face, b=-0.00004, 95% CI [-0.0003, 0.0001], SE=0.00002, z=-2.29, p=.02, on video calls, b=-
0.0006, 95% CI [-0.004, -0.0003], SE=0.0002, z=-3.39, p=.001, and on other voice chat, b=-0.0007, 95% CI [-0.005,
0.001], SE=0.0004, z=-2.05, p=.04, all predicted making fewer upward comparisons. No other effects were
significant, zs < 0.70, ps>.49.
24
Time spent on dating apps was the only other activity that predicted making more lateral comparisons, b=0.01,
95% CI [-0.02, 0.03], SE=0.005, z=2.40, p=.02. More time spent face-to-face, b=-0.0001, 95% CI [-0.0006,
0.00003], SE=0.0001, z=-2.14, p=.03, predicted making fewer lateral comparisons. No other effects were significant,
zs < 1.84, ps>.066.
25
More time spent emailing, b=-0.004, 95% CI [-0.01, 0.009], SE=0.001, z=-3.05, p=.002, predicted making fewer
downward comparisons. No other effects were significant, zs < 1.81, ps>.07.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
50
social media than both lateral, t(78)=3.14, p=.002, and downward comparisons, t(78)=3.03,
p=.003. In other words, spending more time on social media was associated with making more
social comparisons, and these social comparisons were most likely to be upward.
The total number of comparisons made on social media was less than one might expect
from Studies 1-3. It is possible that participants in this experience sampling study were simply
forgetting about the less consequential comparisons made, or reporting relatively few to avoid
completing additional questions on the app. Indeed, we allowed participants to report multiple
comparisons each time they were signaled; thus, survey length increased if participants reported
more comparisons. Past research has shown that participants are more likely to satisfice (i.e.,
impose a limit on how much effort they will apply to the survey) when responding to more time-
consuming diary protocols (Barta, Tennen, & Litt, 2012). Regardless, it is noteworthy that
participants made over 10% of their social comparisons on social media. Furthermore, other
than spending time on dating apps (another context in which people engage in strategic self-
presentation; for a review, see Finkel, Eastwick, Karney, Reis, & Sprecher, 2012), spending time
on social media was the only recorded activity that predicted making more frequent comparisons.
These results are consistent with our hypothesis that individuals are especially likely to make
social comparisons when using social media.
Discussion
In sum, consistent with Study 3, Study 4 demonstrates that individuals are more likely to
make upward than downward or lateral comparisons when using social media, relative to other
contexts. Further, we found evidence that comparisons on social media are more threatening
and, in turn, result in worse self-evaluations than those in other contexts, despite being to less
close others and in domains that are rated as less important. Additionally, we demonstrated that,
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
51
for any given individual, spending more time on social media was associated with making more
social comparisons. This within-person finding is significant because although previous studies
have demonstrated associations between time spent on social media and global, retrospective
reports of comparison frequency (e.g., de Vries & Kühne, 2015), it was previously unclear
whether this finding was due to the possibility that people who report making more comparisons
also spend more time on social media. In this study, we show that, regardless of whether an
individual spends more or less time on social media compared to other people, when that
individual spends more time on social media, relative to her or his other daily activities,
comparisons are more likely. In addition, relative to other contexts, those comparisons made on
social media are more extremely upward and thus result in more negative self-evaluations. In
sum, this study provides the first clear evidence that time spent on social media results in a
greater number of upward comparisons, comparisons that are more extreme in their upwardness,
and, consequently, more negative self-evaluations.
General Discussion
Taken together, these studies provide the first comprehensive analysis of how individuals
make and respond to social comparisons on social media and the characteristics of these
comparisons that differ from those made in other contexts. Indeed, this research provides
compelling evidence that social media is associated with frequent and extreme upward
comparisons, which in turn have a negative impact on individuals’ self-evaluations, mood, and
life satisfaction.
Upward Comparison Frequency on Social Media
First, we found that social comparisons on social media are frequent, and especially likely
to be upward. Although past studies identified this upward direction as a significant feature of
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
52
comparisons on social media, they did not separately assess the relative frequency of upward and
downward comparisons, instead focusing only on upward comparison frequency (e.g., de Vries
& Kühne, 2015; Hanna et al., 2017; Vogel et al., 2014;) or general comparison frequency (e.g.,
Cramer et al., 2016; Feinstein et al., 2013; Gerson et al., 2016; Jang et al., 2016; Lee, 2014;
Steers et al., 2014). The present studies directly compared the frequency of upward and
downward comparisons made by individuals while browsing their own social media news feeds
(Study 1), immediately after using social media (Study 3), and in an experience sampling
paradigm during a two-week period (Study 4), confirming that upward comparisons consistently
outnumber downward comparisons on social media. The preponderance of upward comparisons
is consistent with past studies that have found that people are generally opt for superior or
upward comparison targets (Gerber et al., 2018). However, the present studies are the first to
demonstrate that upward comparisons are more frequent when individuals are using social media
than when they are using other online technologies (Studies 3) or indeed than when they are
engaging in most other daily activities (Study 4).
These studies are also the first to demonstrate that the more frequent upward comparisons
that individuals experience while using social media are associated with more negative outcomes
for the self, including worse mood, lower self-esteem, and decreased life-satisfaction. We show
that the upward comparisons that individuals make while using social media not only have an
immediate negative impact on their self-evaluations (Studies 1-4) but also cumulative negative
effects on their self-esteem, mood, and life-satisfaction (Studies 1 and 3). In past research,
investigators typically have examined the outcome of a single comparison, or have compared an
upward to a downward comparison on various outcomes such as self-evaluation (e.g., Morse &
Gergen, 1970), motivation (e.g., Lockwood, Marshall, & Sadler, 2005; Lockwood & Pinkus,
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
53
2008; Thai, Lockwood, & Boksh, 2020) affect (e.g. Buunk, Collins, Taylor, VanYperen, &
Dakof, 1990; Gibbons & Gerrard, 1989; Pinkus, Lockwood, Marshall, & Yoon, 2012; Salovey &
Rodin, 1984), closeness (e.g., Thai, Lockwood, Pinkus, & Chen, 2016), or domain relevance
(e.g., Thai & Lockwood, 2015). In the present studies, we were able to test the cumulative
effects of a series of comparisons, examining the relative impact of multiple upward and
downward comparisons on state-self-esteem, life satisfaction, and mood. Our studies
demonstrate that upward rather than downward comparisons have the greatest impact on
individuals, and that this impact is overwhelmingly negative. Indeed, any downward or lateral
comparisons did little to mitigate the sting of the more prevalent upward comparisons. Further,
because upward comparisons are more frequent on social media relative to other contexts, these
negative outcomes are more pronounced when individuals are on social media relative to other
online contexts (Study 3) or engaging in other daily activities more generally (Study 4).
The present studies also provide the first evidence that, compared to other activities,
spending time on social media increases the likelihood of making upward comparisons (Studies
3 and 4). This research thus provides new evidence to explain the negative outcomes that have
been associated with increased social media use (Best et al., 2014) and may also at least partially
account for the associations between smartphone use and lower well-being, especially among
young adults (Boumosleh & Jaalouk, 2017; Twenge, 2017). When using social media,
individuals are especially likely to compare themselves to superior, rather than inferior, others
(Studies 1, 3, and 4), and they subsequently feel worse about themselves and less satisfied with
their lives. The more time they spend on social media, the more upward comparisons they make
(Study 4), and the worse they subsequently feel about themselves (Studies 1-4). In sum, these
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
54
studies suggest that social media may be leading to changes in daily social comparison behavior;
individuals now make more upward comparisons that are more threatening to the self.
Upward Comparison Extremity on Social Media
These studies provide evidence that upward comparisons are not only more frequent on
social media, they are also more extreme in their upwardness. Because these studies allowed us
to assess, for the first time, the impact of upward comparison extremity within each individual,
we were able to evaluate the degree to which the “upwardness” of each individual comparison
would determine its impact. We found that, for any given individual, a comparison that was
more extremely upward (than their other comparisons) resulted in a more negative effect on their
self-evaluations immediately following the comparison (Studies 1, 2, and 4). Additionally, by
comparing extremity across context, we were able to show that upward comparisons are more
extreme on social media than in other daily activities (Study 4), and that this greater extremity
accounts, in part, for the more negative outcomes experienced by individuals using social media.
We note that the extremity results in Study 3 did not reach significance; it is possible that we did
not have sufficient power to detect this effect. Alternatively, it may be that comparisons that
take place in any online context are often more extreme in upwardness, given that much online
content, whether on social media, dating platforms, videos, or celebrity news stories, may
include carefully curated and predominantly positive content. In the future, it will be important
to examine comparisons in additional contexts and evaluate their impact.
Self-esteem and Comparisons on Social Media
Up to now, we have discussed the contribution of this research to understanding the
negative impact of more frequent and extreme upward comparisons on social media. This
research also provides valuable insights into the role of self-esteem in determining comparison
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
55
outcomes, in any context. Past research on self-esteem and comparisons has yielded mixed
results: Some research indicates that lower self-esteem individuals are especially prone to
making upward comparisons (e.g., Locke & Nekich, 2000), whereas other theory and research
suggests that low self-esteem individuals may be especially likely to avoid upward comparisons
(e.g., Wood, Giordano-Beech, Taylor, Michela, & Gaus, 1994; Wood, Michela, & Giordano,
2000). Furthermore, other research has focused primarily on this relationship in the reverse
causal direction, suggesting that more upward comparisons lead to lower self-esteem (e.g., de
Vries & Kühne, 2015; Hanna et al., 2017; Leahey, Crowther, & Mickelson, 2007; Vogel et al.,
2014; Wood, 1989).
The present studies focused on the role of self-esteem in determining comparison
frequency and extremity. Specifically, we found that individuals lower in trait self-esteem made
more frequent and more extreme upward comparisons, and as a result, reported greater declines
in state self-esteem, life satisfaction, and self-evaluations, than individuals with higher self-
esteem. Moreover, we found evidence that lower self-esteem individuals actually interpret
information in a way that yields a more extreme upward comparison: Even when information in
a social media post was held constant (Study 2), lower self-esteem individuals were more likely
to see the poster as superior, and as more extremely superior, than were higher self-esteem
individuals. Finally, the more frequent upward comparisons made by low self-esteem
individuals tended to lead to negative self-evaluations among individuals in social media, rather
than other online, contexts (Study 3). Because social media has dramatically increased
opportunities for social comparison, and upward comparison in particular, a browse through a
news feed appears to pose a higher risk for individuals lower in self-esteem. We note that we
had insufficient power to test the moderating impact of self-esteem in Study 4; accordingly, it
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
56
will be important to examine how self-esteem influences reactions to social comparisons in a
variety of contexts beyond the online contexts evaluated in Study 3.
Domain Importance and Closeness
We found that comparisons on social media were especially distressing; this was not,
however, due to the greater importance of the comparison domains on social media. Social
media comparisons were in domains similar in importance to those in other online contexts
(Study 3) and were actually in domains less important than those in offline contexts more
generally (Study 4). This suggests that social media comparisons are exerting a powerful impact
despite the fact that they take place in less consequential domains. It may be that the carefully
packaged information provided in social media posts packs a punch even when it is focused on
relatively trivial domains such as restaurant meals or entertainment viewed, or even when posts
simply showcase random (but attractively presented) selfies taken throughout the day. We note,
however, that we assessed domain importance after the comparisons had been made, and a
possible reaction to threatening upward comparisons is devaluing the comparison domain
(Tesser, 1988; Tesser & Paulhus, 1983). Thus, future studies should examine prior domain
importance when examining the impact of social media comparisons on self-evaluations.
Similarly, the more negative impact of social media comparisons was not due to greater
closeness of the comparison others. Indeed, although individuals compare to less close others on
social media than in other contexts, they nevertheless react more negatively to these comparisons
(Study 4). Social media has opened up a vast array of individuals with whom one can compare,
and this broad pool of comparison targets appears to intensify rather than dilute the outcome of
comparisons. Instead of making occasional comparisons to people from one’s past, at a reunion,
wedding or other event, one is forced to view the achievements of less close contacts, whose
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
57
positive outcomes are carefully presented for maximum impact, whenever one is browsing social
media. Every day has become like a high school reunion, in which one is forced to confront the
apparently more successful lives of one’s former friends and acquaintances, and comparisons to
these remote contacts are no less threatening for their psychological distance.
Future Directions and Conclusions
The present studies focused on social media comparisons in the context of Facebook
(Studies 1-4) and Instagram (Studies 1, 3, and 4), currently the most popular social media
platforms worldwide (Pew Research Center, 2019)
26
. Facebook, in particular, is used by
individuals of all ages for both social contact and business (Greenwood et al., 2016). In recent
years, however, a number of other social media platforms have increased in popularity (Pew
Research Center, 2019), and it will be important to examine the nature and impact of
comparisons made on these other platforms. Given that all social media platforms afford
individuals an opportunity to carefully manage the self-image they present to the world, we
would expect to find a similar preponderance of upward social comparisons.
Further, to the extent that different platforms are popular among different age groups, it
will be important to examine how social media use may lead to different comparison experiences
and consequences among younger and older individuals. Indeed, we note that two of our four
studies used first-year psychology student participants. It is possible that the type of posts that
young adults see and their reactions to them differ systematically from other populations, such as
older adults or those in a different stage of life. First-year undergraduates may be especially
likely to compare on social media in general. They are in a period of transition (i.e., from high
26
In this factsheet, YouTube is listed as the most popular social media channel; however, there is debate over
whether it is, primarily, a social media platform (e.g., Abramovich, 2015). Furthermore, because we were interested
in comparisons occurring in social media news feeds, we focused on the two most popular platforms with this
feature.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
58
school to university) and thus may be more interested in how well they are adjusting to their new
life by comparing themselves to others (Lockwood, Shaughnessy, Fortune, & Tong, 2012),
evaluating what they need to do to succeed (Blakemore & Mills, 2014; Suls, Martin, & Wheeler,
2002; Wheeler, Martin, Suls, 1997). The results of Studies 2 and 3, which involved online
community samples, suggest that our findings are not limited to one age group. Nevertheless,
future studies should examine social media comparisons across age groups and life stages.
In addition, we note that the present studies focused on vertical or status comparisons
(upward vs. downward) rather than horizontal comparisons (contrastive vs. connective; Locke,
2003). It is possible that some individuals will use social media to seek out lateral or similarity-
based comparisons that would result in more communal or positive outcomes (Locke & Nekich,
2000). Alternatively, it may be that opportunities for horizontal comparisons are more plentiful
offline than online, as the former context may provide greater access to information about shared
attributes, across multiple domains. For example, joint participation in an activity or discussion
with a friend could make common interests or opinions as salient as observations about
differences in appearance or particular skills. In future research, it will be important to consider
the full range of comparisons, both vertical and horizontal, that may occur through social media
and whether this differs from other contexts.
Social media platforms offer individuals unprecedented opportunities to connect with
friends, stay in touch with family, share accomplishments, and feel part of a community. Indeed,
past research suggests that Facebook can have positive outcomes such as helping maintain
relationships (Ellison et al., 2007). The benefits of these platforms, however, also come with
potential costs. The present studies reveal that upward comparisons on social media are
commonplace and have both immediate and cumulative negative outcomes, especially for
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
59
individuals with low self-esteem. With ongoing growth of social media sites, continued research
on how these platforms affect their users – especially those at vulnerable ages or life stages – is
essential.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
60
References
Abramovich, G. (2015, March 20). CMO.com wants to know: Is YouTube a social media
platform or a media play? CMO.com. Retrieved from
https://www.cmo.com/features/articles/2015/3/18/cwtk-youtube-social-platform-or-
media-play.html
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
Alfasi, Y. (2019). The grass is always greener on my Friends’ profiles: The effect of Facebook
social comparison on state self-esteem and depression. Personality and Individual
Differences, 147, 111–117.
Appel, H., Crusius, J., & Gerlach, A.L. (2015). Social comparison, envy, and depression on
Facebook: A study looking at the effects of high comparison standards on depressed
individuals. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 34, 277-289.
Appel, H., Gerlach, A. L., & Crusius, J. (2016). The interplay between Facebook use, social
comparison, envy, and depression. Current Opinion in Psychology, 9, 44–49.
Back, M., Stopfer, J., Vazire, S., Gaddis, S., Schmukle, S., Egloff, B., & Gosling, S. (2010).
Facebook profiles reflect actual personality, not self-idealization. Psychological Science,
21, 372–374.
Barta, W. D., Tennen, H., & Litt, M. D. (2012). Measurement reactivity in diary research. In M.
R. Mehl & T. S. Conner (Eds.), Handbook of Research Methods for Studying Daily Life.
(pp. 108–123). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
Best, P, Manktelow, R., & Taylor, B. (2014). Online communication, social media and
adolescent wellbeing: A systematic narrative review. Children and Youth Services
Review, 41, 27-36.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
61
Blakemore, S. J., & Mills, K. L. (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural
processing? Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 187–207.
Boumosleh, J. M., & Jaalouk, D. (2017). Depression, anxiety, and smartphone addiction in
university students – A cross-sectional study. PLoS one, 12, e0182239.
Burke, M., Marlow, C., & Lento, T. (2010). Social network activity and social well-being. In
Proceedings of the 28th Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. Atlanta,
GA.
Buunk, B.P., Collins, R.L., Taylor, S.E., VanYperen, N.W., & Dakof, G.A. (1990). The affective
consequences of social comparison: Either direction has its ups and downs. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1238–1249.
Chatard, A., Bocage-Barthélémy, Y., Selimbegović, L., & Guimond, S. (2017). The woman who
wasn’t there: Converging evidence that subliminal social comparison affects self-
evaluation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 73, 1–13.
Chou, H.-T., & Edge, N. (2012). “They are happier and having better lives than we am”: the
impact of using Facebook on perceptions of others’ lives. Cyberpsychology, Behavior
and Social Networking, 15, 117–121.
Clement, J. (2020). Daily social media usage worldwide 2012-2019. Retrieved from Statista
website: https://www.statista.com/statistics/433871/daily-social-media-usage-
worldwide/#statisticContainer
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159.
Collins, R. L. (1996). For better or worse: The impact of upward social comparison on self-
evaluations. Psychological Bulletin, 119(1), 51–69.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
62
Cramer, E. M., Song, H., & Drent, A. M. (2016). Social comparison on Facebook: Motivation,
affective consequences, self-esteem, and Facebook fatigue. Computers in Human
Behavior, 64, 739-746.
Davis, K. (2012). Friendship 2.0: Adolescents’ experiences of belonging and self-disclosure
online. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 1527–1536.
de Vries, D., & Kühne, R. (2015). Facebook and self-perception: Individual susceptibility to
negative social comparison on Facebook. Personality and Individual Differences, 86,
217–221.
de Vries, D. A., Möller, A. M., Wieringa, M. S., Eigenraam, A. W., & Hamelink, K. (2018).
Social comparison as the thief of joy: Emotional consequences of viewing strangers’
Instagram posts. Media Psychology, 21(2), 222–245.
https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2016.1267647
Diener, E., Emmons, R., Larsen, R., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75.
Dorethy, M.D., Fiebert, M.S., & Warren, C.R. (2014). Examining social networking site
behaviors: Photo sharing and impression management on Facebook. International Review
of Social Sciences and Humanities, 6, 111-116.
Edwards, L., Muller, K., Wolfinger, R., Qaqish, B., & Schabenberger, O. (2008). An R2 statistic
for fixed effects in the linear mixed model. Statistics in Medicine, 27, 6137–6157.
Ellison, N.B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Facebook “friends:” Social
capital and college students’ use of online social network sites. Journal of Computer-
Mediated Communication, 12, 1143-1168.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
63
Fardouly, J., Pinkus, R. T., & Vartanian, L. R. (2017). The impact of appearance comparisons
made through social media, traditional media, and in person in women’s everyday lives.
Body Image, 20, 31–39.
Fardouly, J., & Vartanian, L. R. (2015). Negative comparisons about one’s appearance mediate
the relationship between Facebook usage and body image concerns. Body Image, 12, 82–
88.
Feinstein, B., Hershenberg, R., Bhatia, V., Latack, J., Meuwly, N., & Davila, J. (2013). Negative
social comparison on Facebook and depressive symptoms: Rumination as a mechanism.
Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 2, 161-170.
Finkel, E. J., Eastwick, P. W., Karney, B. R., Reis, H. T., & Sprecher, S. (2012). Online dating:
A critical analysis from the perspective of psychological science. Psychological Science
in the Public Interest, 13(1), 3–66.
Geldhof, G. J., Anthony, K. P., Selig, J. P., & Mendez-Luck, C.A. (2018). Accommodating
binary and count variables in mediation: A case for conditional indirect effects.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 42, 300-308.
Gerber, J. P., Wheeler, L., & Suls, J. (2018). A social comparison theory meta-analysis 60+ years
on. Psychological Bulletin, 144, 177-197.
Gilbert, D. T., Giesler, R. B., & Morris, K. A. (1995). When comparisons arise. Journal of
Personality & Social Psychology, 69(2), 227–236.
Gerson, J., Plagnol, A.C., & Corr, P.J. (2016). Subjective well-being and social media use: Do
personality traits moderate the impact of social comparison on Facebook? Computers in
Human Behavior, 6, 813-822.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
64
Gibbons, F. X., & Buunk, B. P. (1999). Individual differences in social comparison:
Development of a scale of social comparison orientation. Journal of Personality & Social
Psychology, 76(1), 129–142.
Gibbons, F.X., & Gerrard, M. (1989). Effects of upward and downward social comparisons on
mood states. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 14-31.
Greenwood, S., Perrin, A., & Duggan, M. (2016, November 11). Social Media Update 2016.
Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.org/2016/11/11/social-media-update-2016/
Hanna, E., Ward, L., Seabrook, R., Jerald, M., Reed, L., Giaccardi, S., & Lippman, J. (2017).
Contributions of social comparison and self-objectification in mediating associations
between Facebook use and emergent adults’ psychological well-being. Cyberpsychology,
Behavior, and Social Networking, 20, 172–179.
Hayes, A.F., (2015). An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivariate Behavioral
Research, 50, 1-22.
Hayes, A.F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A
regression-based approach. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Hayes, A.F., & Preacher, K.J. (2010). Quantifying and testing indirect effects in simple
mediation models when the constituent paths are nonlinear. Multivariate Behavioral
Research, 45, 627-660.
Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2014). Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical
independent variable. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 67,
451-470.
Heatherton, T.F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of a scale for measuring state
self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 895–910.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
65
Howell, D. (2013). Statistical methods for psychology (8th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth
Cengage Learning.
Hwang, J., Cheong, P., & Feeley, T. (2009). Being young and feeling blue in Taiwan: examining
adolescent depressive mood and online and offline activities. New Media & Society, 11,
1101–1121.
Jang, K., Park, N., & Song., H. (2016). Social comparison on Facebook: Its antecedents and
psychological outcomes. Computers in Human Behavior, 62, 147-154.
Kalpidou, M., Costin, D., & Morris, J. (2011). The relationship between Facebook and the well-
being of undergraduate college students. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social
Networking, 14, 183–189.
Ko, H.-C., & Kuo, F.-Y. (2009). Can blogging enhance subjective well-being through self-
disclosure? CyberPsychology & Behavior, 12, 75–79.
Krasnova, H., Wenninger, H., Widjaja, T., & Buxmann, P. (2013). Envy on Facebook: A hidden
threat to users’ life satisfaction? 11th International Conference on Wirtschaftsinformatik,
1–16. Leipzig: Wirtschaftsinformatik.
Kross, E., Verduyn, P., Demiralp, E., Park, J., Lee, D., Lin, N., … Ybarra, O. (2013). Facebook
Use Predicts Declines in Subjective Well-Being in Young Adults. PLoS ONE, 8(8).
Leahey, T. M., Crowther, J. H., & Mickelson, K. D. (2007). The frequency, nature, and effects of
naturally occurring appearance-focused social comparisons. Behavior Therapy, 28, 132-
143.
Lee, S. Y. (2014). How do people compare themselves with others on social network sites? The
case of Facebook. Computers in Human Behavior, 32, 253-260.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
66
Liu, D., Ainsworth, S. E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2016). A meta-analysis of social networking
online and social capital. Review of General Psychology, 20(4), 369–391.
Liu, Q.-Q., Zhou, Z.-K., Yang, X.-J., Niu, G.-F., Tian, Y., & Fan, C.-Y. (2017). Upward social
comparison on social network sites and depressive symptoms: A moderated mediation
model of self-esteem and optimism. Personality and Individual Differences, 113, 223–
228.
Lo, I. S., & McKercher, B. (2015). Ideal image in process: Online tourist photography and
impression management. Annals of Tourism Research, 52, 104–116.
Locke, K. D. (2003). Status and solidarity in social comparison: Agentic and communal values
and vertical and horizontal directions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
84(3), 619–631.
Locke, K. D. (2005). Connecting the horizontal dimension of social comparison with self-worth
and self-confidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(6), 795–803.
Locke, K. D., & Nekich, J. C. (2000). Agency and communion in naturalistic social comparison.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(7), 864–874.
Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1997). Superstars and me: Predicting the impact of role models on
the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 91–103.
Lockwood, P., Marshall, T. C., & Sadler, P. (2005). Promoting success or preventing failure:
Cultural differences in motivation by positive and negative role models. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 379-392.
Lockwood, P., & Pinkus, R. T. (2008). The impact of social comparisons on motivation. In J. Y.
Shah & W. L. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of Motivation Science (pp. 251-264). New
York: Guilford Press.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
67
Lockwood, P., Shaughnessy, S., Fortune, J., & Tong, M.-O. (2012). Social comparisons in novel
situations: Finding inspiration during life transitions. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 38, 985–996.
Maniaci, M.R., & Rogge, R.D. (2014). Caring about carelessness: Participant inattention and its
effects on research. Journal of Research in Personality, 48, 61–83.
Morse, S., & Gergen, K. J., (1970). Social comparison, self-consistency, and the concept of the
self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 148-156.
Muise, A., Christofides, E., & Desmarais, S. (2009). More information than you ever wanted:
Does Facebook bring out the green-eyed monster of jealousy? CyberPsychology &
Behavior, 12, 441-444.
Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judgment: Mechanisms and
consequences. Psychological Review, 110(3), 472–489.
Mussweiler, T., Rüter, K., & Epstude, K. (2004). The ups and downs of social comparison:
Mechanisms of assimilation and contrast. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
87(6), 832–844.
Mussweiler, T., & Strack, F. (1999). Hypothesis-consistent testing and semantic priming in the
anchoring paradigm: A selective accessibility model. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 35(2), 136–164.
Muthén, L.K., & Muthén, B.O. (1998-2012). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA:
Muthén & Muthén.
Page-Gould, E. & Sharples, A. (2016). Bootstrapping accurate indirect effects in multilevel
models. Retrieved from http://www.page-gould.com/r/indirectmlm/
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
68
Patrick, H., Neighbors, C., & Knee, C. R. (2004). Appearance-related social comparisons: The
role of contingent self-esteem and self-perceptions of attractiveness. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 501-514.
Pinkus, R.T., Lockwood, P., Marshall, T.C., & Yoon, H.M., (2012). Responses to comparisons
in romantic relationships: Empathy, shared fate, and contrast. Personal Relationships, 19,
182-201.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and
comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40,
879-891.
Pew Research Center (2019). Social Media Fact Sheet. Retrieved from
http://www.pewinternet.org/fact-sheet/social-media/
Qiu, L., Lin, H., Leung, A. K., & Tov, W. (2012). Putting their best foot forward: Emotional
disclosure on Facebook. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 15(10),
569–572.
Rosenberg, M. (1965) Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
Press.
Salovey, P. & Rodin, J. (1984). Some antecedents and consequences of social-comparison
jealousy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(4), 780-792.
Seidman, G. (2013). Self-presentation and belonging on Facebook: How personality influences
social media use and motivations. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(3), 402–
407.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
69
Stapleton, P., Luiz, L., & Chatwin, H. (2017). Generation validation: The role of social
comparison in use of Instagram among emerging adults. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and
Social Networking, 20, 142–149.
Steers, M.N., Wickham, R.E., & Acitelli, L.K. (2014). Seeing everyone else’s highlight reels:
How Facebook usage is linked to depressive symptoms. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 33, 701–731.
Suls, J., Martin, R., & Wheeler, L. (2002). Social comparison: Why, with whom, and with what
effect?. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 159-163.
Tandoc, E. C., Ferrucci, P., & Duffy, M. (2015). Facebook use, envy, and depression among
college students: Is Facebooking depressing?. Computers in Human Behavior, 43, 139-
146.
Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self-evaluation maintenance model of social behavior. In L.
Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 181–227).
New York: Academic Press.
Tesser, A., Millar, M., & Moore, J. (1988). Some affective consequences of social comparison
and reflection processes: The pain and pleasure of being close. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 54(1), 49.
Tesser, A., & Paulhus, D. (1983). The definition of self: Private and public self-evaluation
management strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 672–682.
Thai, S., & Lockwood, P. (2015). Comparing you = comparing me: Social comparisons of the
expanded self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(7), 989–1004.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
70
Thai, S., Lockwood, P., & Boksh, R. J. (2020). Committed to succeed: Commitment determines
reactions to upward relationship comparisons. Personal Relationships. Advance online
publication. https://doi.org/10.1111/pere.12316
Thai, S., Lockwood, P., Pinkus, R. T., & Chen, S. Y. (2016). Being better than you is better for
us: Attachment avoidance and social comparisons within romantic relationships. Journal
of Social and Personal Relationships, 33(4), 493–514.
Thai, S., & Page-Gould, E. (2018). ExperienceSampler: An Open-Source Scaffold for Building
Smartphone Apps for Experience Sampling. Psychological Methods, 23, 729-739.
Tien, S., & Aynsley, M. (2019, July 15). The best time to post on Facebook, Instagram, Twitter,
and LinkedIn. Retrieved May 23, 2020, from Hootsuite website:
https://blog.hootsuite.com/best-time-to-post-on-facebook-twitter-instagram/
Twenge, J. (2017). IGen: Why today’s super-connected kids are growing up less rebellious,
more tolerant, less happy--and completely unprepared for adulthood--and what that
means for the rest of us. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.
Valkenburg, P., Peter, J., & Schouten, A. (2006). Friend networking sites and their relationship
to adolescents’ well-being and social self-esteem. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 9, 584-
590.
Verduyn, P., Lee, D. S., Park, J., Shablack, H., Orvell, A., Bayer, J., … Kross, E. (2015). Passive
Facebook usage undermines affective well-being: Experimental and longitudinal
evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(2), 480–488.
Verduyn, P., Ybarra, O., Résibois, M., Jonides, J., & Kross, E. (2017). Do social network sites
enhance or undermine subjective well-being? A critical review. Social Issues and Policy
Review, 11(1), 274–302.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
71
Vogel, E. A., Rose, J. P., Okdie, B. M., Eckles, K., & Franz, B. (2015). Who compares and
despairs? The effect of social comparison orientation on social media use and its
outcomes. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 249–256.
Vogel, E.A., Rose, J.P., Roberts, L.R., & Eckles, K. (2014). Social comparison, social media,
and self-esteem. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 3, 206-222.
Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2004). Ego threats elicits different social comparison process
among high and low self-esteem people: Implications for interpersonal perceptions.
Social Cognition, 22, 168-191.
Walther, J.B. (2007). Selective self-presentation in computer-mediated communication:
Hyperpersonal dimensions of technology, language, and cognition. Computers in Human
Behavior, 23, 2538-2557.
Wang, J.-L., Wang, H.-Z., Gaskin, J., & Hawk, S. (2017). The mediating roles of upward social
comparison and self-esteem and the moderating role of social comparison orientation in
the association between social networking site usage and subjective well-being. Frontiers
in Psychology, 8.
Watson, D., Clark, L.A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures
of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 54, 1063-1070.
Wayment, H. A., & Taylor, S. E. (1995). Self-evaluation processes: Motives, information use,
and self-esteem. Journal of Personality, 63, 729-757.
Wheeler, L., Martin, R., & Suls, J. (1997). The proxy model of social comparison for self-
assessment of ability. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 54-61.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
72
Wheeler, L., & Miyake, K. (1992). Social comparison in everyday life. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 62, 760–773.
Wheeler, L., & Reis, H. T. (1991). Self-recording of everyday life events: Origins, types, and
uses. Journal of Personality, 59(3), 339–354.
Wilson, R.E., Gosling, S.D., & Graham, L.T. (2012). A review of Facebook research in the
social sciences. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 203-220.
Wood, J. V. (1989). Theory and research concerning social comparisons of personal attributes.
Psychological Bulletin, 106, 231-248.
Wood, J. V., Giordano-Beech, M., Taylor, K. L., Michela, J. L., & Gaus, V. (1994). Strategies of
social comparison among people with low self-esteem: Self-protection and self-
enhancement. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 67(4), 713–731.
Wood, J. V., & Lockwood, P. (1999). Social comparisons in dysphoric and low self-esteem
people. In R. Kowalski & M. Leary (Eds.), The social psychology of emotional and
behavioral problems: Interfaces of social and clinical psychology (pp. 97–135).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Wood, J.V., Michela, J.L., & Giordano, C. (2000). Downward comparison in everyday life:
Reconciling self-enhancement models with the mood–cognition priming model. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 563–579.
Woods, H. C., & Scott, H. (2016). #Sleepyteens: Social media use in adolescence is associated
with poor sleep quality, anxiety, depression and low self-esteem. Journal of Adolescence,
51, 41–49.
Zhang, Z., Zyphur, M.J., & Preacher, K.J. (2009). Testing multilevel mediation using
hierarchical linear models. Organizational Research Methods, 12, 695–719.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
73
Zhao, S., Grasmuck, S., & Martin, J. (2008). Identity construction on Facebook: Digital
empowerment in anchored relationships. Computers in Human Behavior, 24(5), 1816–
1836.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
74
Table 1. Comparison Domains.
Comparison
Domain
Study 1
(N=1795)
Study 3
(N=250)
Study 4
(N=1211)
Instagram
(n=941)
Facebook
(n=854)
Social
media
(n=169)
Other
contexts
(n=86)
Social
media
(n=124)
Other
contexts
(n=1087)
Looks /
Attractiveness
263
(27.9%)
163
(19.1%)
23
(13.6%)
10
(11.6%)
41
(33.1%)
167
(15.4%)
Academics/
Career
54
(5.7%)
89
(10.4%)
14
(8.3%)
5
(5.8%)
11
(8.9%)
254
(23.4%)
Dating /
Relationships
48
(5.1%)
47
(5.5%)
5
(3.0%)
6
(7.0%)
7
(5.6%)
65
(6.0%)
Popularity /
Friendships
98
(10.4%)
119
(13.9%)
15
(8.9%)
5
(5.8%)
3
(2.4%)
46
(4.2%)
Vacations /
Activities /
Lifestyle
219
(23.3%)
162
(19.0%)
26
(15.4%)
9
(10.5%)
16
(12.9%)
83
(7.6%)
Personality /
Morality
38
(4.0%)
86
(10.1%)
12
(7.1%)
9
(10.5%)
3
(2.4%)
112
(10.3%)
Skills /
Abilities
75
(8.0%)
63
(7.4%)
14
(8.3%)
13
(15.1%)
12
(9.7%)
126
(11.6%)
Health /
Physical Fitness
65
(6.9%)
62
(7.3%)
20
(11.8%)
13
(15.1%)
16
(12.9%)
80
(7.4%)
Wealth /
Finances
31
(3.3%)
22
(2.6%)
9
(5.3%)
4
(4.7%)
1
(0.8%)
19
(1.7%)
Family
21
(2.2%)
19
(2.2%)
10
(5.9%)
6
(7.0%)
0
(0%)
17
(1.6%)
Other
29
(3.1%)
22
(2.6%)
13
(7.7%)
4
(4.7%)
1
(0.8%)
16
(1.5%)
Multiple domains
-
-
8
(4.7%)
2
(2.3%)
13
(10.5%)
102
(9.4%)
Note: N is the number of comparisons reported across all participants. In Study 1, participants
could select multiple domains; thus, the sum of all domains in Study 1 exceeds the total.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
75
Table 2. Content of Facebook posts viewed by participants (Study 2).
Post ID
Post Content
Neutral 1
Any suggestions for good lunch spots? In the mood to try something new :P
Neutral 2
OMGGG to the season finale of Walking Dead! Who else was shocked?!?
Negative 1
got some bad news, I’m unexpectedly out of a job… but we guess I’ve got to
pick myself up and move forward.. would really appreciate anyone that knows
someone who is hiring!
Negative 2
Heartbroken – trust me everyone, never fall in love… even when you think you
know someone, it’s impossible to actually know what is in their head – it is
SUCH a myth that two people can become one…
Positive 1
Finally got my dream job at John Hopkins Hospital!! Thank you to everyone
who helped me get here…this is PROOF that hard work, tirelessness, and
determination does pay off – you can all reach your dreams :)
Positive 2
So thankful to be celebrating 1 year with my babe today…you make me smile
like no one else can& make my life so much better. We can’t wait for what is
next for us.. love you xxxooo
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
76
Table 3. Frequencies of participants’ online activities (Study 3).
No Social Media
Condition
(n=213)
Social Media
Condition
(n=202)
Made a phone call
20
5
Answered a phone call
12
5
Read a text/message
100
32
Wrote a text/message
67
16
Read an email
96
11
Wrote an email
24
4
Read an article
64
15
Read a book
10
1
Listened to music
26
7
Listened to the radio
7
5
Listened to a podcast
6
3
Watched a video
56
23
Watched TV or a movie
12
6
Used a dating app/website
5
2
Checked Facebook
0
172
Checked Instagram
0
90
Used other social media
36
5
- Used Snapchat
7
1
- Used Twitter
9
1
Other activity
85
6
Multiple activities
171
109
Note: Frequencies do not total condition sample size because participants were able to report
multiple activities.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
77
Table 4. Frequencies of participants’ comparison activity (Study 3).
Comparison Frequency
Total # of
Participants
1
2
3
3+
Whole Sample
(n=415)
Upward
63
22
11
7
103
Downward
15
3
4
-
22
Lateral
23
4
-
1
28
No social media
condition
(n=213)
Upward
22
9
2
2
35
Downward
3
-
3
-
6
Lateral
8
1
-
-
9
Social media
condition
(n=202)
Upward
41
13
9
5
68
Downward
12
3
1
-
16
Lateral
15
3
-
1
19
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
78
Table 5. Conditional Indirect Effects Through Number of Upward Comparisons on Self-
Evaluation at Specific Values of Self-Esteem in Each Condition (Study 3).
Self-Esteem
Values
Experimental
Condition
Indirect Effect
Estimate
SE
95% CI
Low
(-1 SD)
No Social Media
0.080
0.05
[0.018, 0.195]
Social Media
0.113
0.04
[0.039, 0.194]
High
(+1 SD)
No Social Media
0.034
0.01
[0.01, 0.057]
Social Media
0.099
0.04
[0.032, 0.193]
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
79
Figure 1. The association between self-esteem and self-evaluation is mediated by differences in
comparison extremity (Study 1). Unstandardized regression coefficients and 95% bootstrapped
confidence intervals are reported along the paths they model. Statistics reported within
parentheses represent the total effect. Values with the subscript within represent the within-
subject effects, and values with the subscript between represent between-subject effects. The
direct and total effects have not been parsed into within- and between-person components.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
80
Figure 2. The association between self-esteem and post-browsing session outcomes are
mediated by number of upward comparisons (Study 1). Panel A depicts the mediational model
for state self-esteem, and Panel B depicts the mediational model for life satisfaction.
Unstandardized regression coefficients and 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals are reported
along the paths they model. Statistics reported within parentheses represent the total effect.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
81
Figure 3. The effect of self-esteem on comparison extremity for each post type (Study 2) while
controlling for order. Errors bars represent standard errors. Greater scores on the y-axis indicate
a more extreme upward comparison.
-2
-1.5
-1
-0.5
0
0.5
1
1.5
2
Low High
Comparison Extremity
Self-Esteem
Positive Neutral Negative
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
82
Figure 4. The association between post valence and self-evaluation is mediated by differences in
comparison extremity (Study 2). Panel A depicts the mediational model for the difference
between positive and neutral posts, and Panel B depicts the mediational model for the difference
between positive and negative posts. Unstandardized regression coefficients and 95%
bootstrapped confidence intervals are reported along the paths they model. Statistics reported
within parentheses represent the total effect. The direct and total effects have not been parsed
into within- and between-person components.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
83
Figure 5. The association between self-esteem and self-evaluation is mediated by differences in
comparison extremity (Study 2). Unstandardized regression coefficients and 95% bootstrapped
confidence intervals are reported along the paths they model. Statistics reported within
parentheses represent the total effect. The direct and total effects have not been parsed into
within- and between-person components.
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
84
Figure 6. The association between comparison context (experimental condition: social media vs.
other online contexts) and self-evaluation is mediated by greater likelihood of making one or
more upward comparisons (Study 3). Statistics are reported in body of text given the non-normal
distribution of the mediator (presence/absence of upward comparisons).
Comparison
Context
1+ Upward
Comparisons
(vs. 0)
Self-Evaluation
at End of Study
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
85
Figure 7. The moderated mediation model we tested. The association between self-esteem and
self-evaluation is mediated by greater likelihood of making one or more upward comparisons,
and this indirect effect differs depending on the comparison context (Study 3).
Self-
Esteem
1+ Upward
Comparisons
(vs. 0)
Self-Evaluation
at End of Study
Comparison
Context
SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
86
Figure 8. The association between comparison context (social media news feeds vs. all other
contexts) and self-evaluation is mediated by differences in comparison extremity (Study 4).
Unstandardized regression coefficients and 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals are reported
along the paths they model. Statistics reported within parentheses represent the total effect. The
direct and total effects have not been parsed into within- and between-person components.