ArticlePublisher preview available
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

A classic phenomenon known as prototype preference effect (PPE) or beauty-in-averageness effect is that prototypical exemplars of a neutral category are preferred over atypical exemplars. This PPE has been explained in terms of deviance avoidance, hedonic fluency, or preference for certainty and familiarity. However, typicality also facilitates greater activation of category-related information. Thus, prototypes rather than atypical exemplars should be more associated with the valence of the category, either positive or negative. Hence, we hypothesize that the evaluation of a prototype depends on the valence of its category. Results from three experiments crossing a standard PPE paradigm with an evaluative conditioning procedure support our hypothesis. We show that for positive categories, greater typicality increases liking. Critically, for negative categories, greater typicality decreases liking. This pattern of results challenges dominant explanations of prototype evaluation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
BRIEF REPORT
The Architecture of Prototype Preferences: Typicality, Fluency,
and Valence
Tobias Vogel and Moritz Ingendahl
University of Mannheim
Piotr Winkielman
University of California, San Diego, and SWPS University of
Social Sciences and Humanities
A classic phenomenon known as prototype preference effect (PPE) or beauty-in-averageness effect is that
prototypical exemplars of a neutral category are preferred over atypical exemplars. This PPE has been
explained in terms of deviance avoidance, hedonic fluency, or preference for certainty and familiarity.
However, typicality also facilitates greater activation of category-related information. Thus, prototypes
rather than atypical exemplars should be more associated with the valence of the category, either positive
or negative. Hence, we hypothesize that the evaluation of a prototype depends on the valence of its
category. Results from three experiments crossing a standard PPE paradigm with an evaluative condi-
tioning procedure support our hypothesis. We show that for positive categories, greater typicality
increases liking. Critically, for negative categories, greater typicality decreases liking. This pattern of
results challenges dominant explanations of prototype evaluation.
Keywords: beauty-in-averageness, categorization, evaluative conditioning, fluency, valence
Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000798.supp
A classic phenomenon in psychology is the prototype prefer-
ence effect (PPE): preference for typical category exemplars over
atypical exemplars. Closely related is the beauty-in-averageness
effect: preference for the category average over its constituent
exemplars (Galton, 1879;Langlois & Roggman, 1990). The PPE
occurs with natural and artificial stimuli, across different popula-
tions, ages, and even species. As reviewed next, the phenomenon
has been explained by multiple influential frameworks across
psychology. Building on categorization theory, we present an
account in which the PPE crucially depends on the valence of
attributes associated with prototypes. This account generates a
novel empirical prediction—a reversal of the classic effect, with
lower preference for typical than atypical exemplars within nega-
tively valenced categories.
Prototype Evaluation: Major Explanations
PPE From Deviance Detection
Evolutionary accounts suggest that the PPE results from “devi-
ance detection”—negative sexual selection for atypical organisms
(Langlois & Roggman, 1990;Rhodes & Tremewan, 1996;Sy-
mons, 1979;Thornhill & Gangestad, 1993). However, the PPE
also occurs with objects (e.g., Halberstadt & Rhodes, 2003), in-
cluding consumer goods (e.g., Landwehr, Labroo, & Herrmann,
2011), and even abstract graphical patterns (e.g., Winkielman,
Halberstadt, Fazendeiro, & Catty, 2006). Accordingly, many
frameworks focus on basic cognitive and affective processes.
PPE From Preference for Certainty, Familiarity, or
Efficient Coding
Note that the aggregate similarity to all category exemplars is
highest for prototypes and they are most unambiguously identified
as category members (Markman & Ross, 2003). As such, proto-
types are the most certain, most “familiar” category members. The
PPE could then occur because uncertainty triggers negative affect
(Friston, Adams, & Montague, 2012;Hsu & Preuschoff, 2015;
Klein, Cosmides, Tooby, & Chance, 2002). Or because familiarity
triggers positive affect (Titchener, 1915). Related arguments em-
phasize that prototypes are statistically typical and thus more
efficiently coded (Dotsch, Hassin, & Todorov, 2016;Vogel, Carr,
Davis, & Winkielman, 2018). Thus, prototypes could be preferred
because they are simpler and less energy-demanding (Ryali & Yu,
2018).
This article was published Online First June 25, 2020.
XTobias Vogel and XMoritz Ingendahl, Department of Consumer
and Economic Psychology, University of Mannheim; Piotr Winkielman,
Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, and De-
partment of Psychology, SWPS University of Social Sciences and Human-
ities.
The authors want to thank Andy Arnold for helpful comments on the
article. Additional information is available at https://osf.io/qyf58/. Findings
from Experiment 1 have been presented at the Tagung experimentell
arbeitender Psychologen (TeaP) 2017 in Dresden, Germany.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Tobias
Vogel, Department of Consumer and Economic Psychology, University of
Mannheim, A5, 6 B117, 68159 Mannheim, Germany. E-mail: tobias
.vogel@uni-mannheim.de
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
© 2020 American Psychological Association 2021, Vol. 150, No. 1, 187–194
ISSN: 0096-3445 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000798
187
... In fact, EC effects might even require that memory retrieval is consciously accessible to the individual (Stahl et al., 2024). Third, other sources of metacognitive feelings than memory retrieval have been shown to modulate EC, primarily, the perceptual ease of processing a stimulus (Landwehr & Eckmann, 2020;Vogel et al., 2021). Thus, it is likely that ease from memory retrieval contributes to EC effects as well. ...
... We did not control for these factors in our paradigm. Thus, future research might consider either measuring or manipulating people's naïve beliefs of what the experience of easy/difficult recall signals in an EC paradigm (e.g., typicality; Vogel et al., 2021). In addition, people are less likely to rely on their ease-of-retrieval if they have high cognitive capacity (Greifeneder & Bless, 2007) or if they are in a mindset of abstract thinking (Tsai & Thomas, 2011)both structural aspects that we did not control for in our paradigm. ...
Article
Full-text available
Evaluative conditioning (EC), the change in the liking of a conditioned stimulus due to its pairing with a positive or negative stimulus, is a key effect in attitude formation. Current theories on EC emphasize the role of memory in EC, assuming that explicit memory on the stimulus pairings strengthens the EC effect. In this paper, we extend the scope of memory’s role in EC by focusing on whether the metacognitive experience of ease during memory retrieval influences EC effects beyond the effects of the retrieved memory content. In two preregistered experiments (total N = 392), we tested for ease-of-retrieval effects in EC by letting participants recall few vs. many unconditioned stimuli before giving an evaluative judgment. Although the manipulation was successful in manipulating ease-of-retrieval, we found no evidence for ease-of-retrieval effects resulting from the experimental manipulation. However, the subjectively perceived ease of retrieval was indeed associated with stronger EC effects. Overall, these findings show no evidence that ease resulting from an instruction to recollect more/less US information contributes to EC effects but leave open whether ease-of-retrieval from other sources contributes to EC effects.
... This phenomenon of categorical divergence may be linked to the polysemantic nature of neuronal concepts [64,39,9,16], distinguishing it from traditional human categorical approaches [49,149,150,151,152,153,154,155]. ...
... This aligns with the fundamental characteristic of thought categories being ad hoc [5,156,153], meaning they serve a purpose, which, in synthetic categories, might involve minimal alignment with human thought categories and partial convergence toward human-like semantic elements. Furthermore, the tokens specifically involved in this very high activation and categorical convergence process could be interpreted as corresponding to categorical prototypes [149,150]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
This study investigates the ability of perceptron-type neurons in language models to perform intra-neuronal attention; that is, to identify different homogeneous categorical segments within the synthetic thought category they encode, based on a segmentation of specific activation zones for the tokens to which they are particularly responsive. The objective of this work is therefore to determine to what extent formal neurons can establish a homomorphic relationship between activation-based and categorical segmentations. The results suggest the existence of such a relationship, albeit tenuous, only at the level of tokens with very high activation levels. This intra-neuronal attention subsequently enables categorical restructuring processes at the level of neurons in the following layer, thereby contributing to the progressive formation of high-level categorical abstractions.
... They allow individuals to simplify groups into overarching mental representations (Brewer, 1988;Rosch, 1978;Turner et al., 1987). Although prototypes may be cognitively useful, holding particularly narrow prototypes carries serious implications for those deemed not prototypical, such that individuals who do not match the prototype of their social group are often disliked, forgotten, or punished (Phelan & Rudman, 2010;Purdie-Vaughns & Eibach, 2008;Schug et al., 2017;Sesko & Biernat, 2010;Vogel et al., 2021). An intersectional approach to prototypes (Crenshaw, 1989) provides another avenue for understanding how prototypes can be detrimental. ...
... Research indicates that East Asians are perceived to be the most prototypically Asian group in the US (Goh & McCue, 2021). Substantial research has focused on the negative consequences of narrow prototypes (Phelan & Rudman, 2010;Purdie-Vaughns & Eibach, 2008;Schug et al., 2017;Sesko & Biernat, 2010;Vogel et al., 2021), however, limited research examines how these consequences apply to broad racial movements. The current work sought to fill this gap in the literature by examining whether people have narrow beliefs about which Asian subgroups are represented by the Stop Asian Hate movement and potential implications of such a prototypicality bias for several victim reporting metrics. ...
Article
Full-text available
The Stop Asian Hate movement is a collective for several anti-Asian-violence rallies and organizations in the United States (US). Research indicates that when asked to think about who is Asian, Americans’ prototype primarily comprises East Asian individuals (e.g., people from China, Japan, Korea) at the exclusion of people from other regions of Asia (e.g., South Asia). The current work extends this prototypicality research to examine implications for social justice movements. We focused on the Stop Asian Hate movement, which was designed to raise awareness and protest racial discrimination directed towards Asian Americans, particularly in light of COVID-19. Three studies tested whether people’s prototypes regarding who is Asian influenced who they believe is represented by the Stop Asian Hate movement, as well as potential implications of this bias. Compared to South Asians, people judged East Asians as more represented by the Stop Asian Hate movement (Study 1). When described as being the victim of a hate crime, participants perceived East Asian targets to be more credible, more traumatized, and their reporting of the crime on the SAAPI website was deemed more appropriate, compared to South Asian targets (Studies 2–3), effects that were mediated by judgments of prototypicality (Study 3).
... In the realm of human thought, categorization is essential in various cognitive activities, such as classification, object identification, understanding, reasoning, problem-solving, memory, inference, prediction, and conceptualization (Sternberg, 2007; Roads et al., 2024). Rosch (1975) proposes an approach to categorization based on the resemblance of an object to a prototype of the category, noting that individuals tend to state characteristic features rather than determinative properties (Rosch & Mervis, 1975); the prototype being the most representative example of the category (Singh et al., 2020;Vogel et al., 2021). The exemplar theory of categorization (Medin & Schaffer, 1978;Nosofsky, 1992;Nosofsky et al., 2022), as for it, suggests that objects are compared to typical examples stored in memory, with the most typical exemplar being the one that most closely resembles the known exemplars. ...
... To conclude this line of inquiry, tokens with (very) high activations can be thought of as being or tending towards the prototypes, in the sense of Rosch (1975) (and also Singh et al., 2020;Vogel et al., 2021), of their respective neurons (the true ultimate prototype being, for a given neuron, the fictitious token that maximizes the aggregation function associated with that neuron). This is because, again by the mathematical construction of the aggregation function, highly activated tokens best satisfy the various sub-dimensional categories (from the input vector space) of their associated neurons. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
We propose a neuropsychological approach to the explainability of artificial neural networks, which involves using concepts from human cognitive psychology as relevant heuristic references for developing synthetic explanatory frameworks that align with human modes of thought. The analogical concepts mobilized here, which are intended to create such an epistemological bridge, are those of categorization and similarity, as these notions are particularly suited to the categorical "nature" of the reconstructive information processing performed by artificial neural networks. Our study aims to reveal a unique process of synthetic cognition, that of the categorical convergence of highly activated tokens. We attempt to explain this process with the idea that the categorical segment created by a neuron is actually the result of a superposition of categorical sub-dimensions within its input vector space.
... The question of abstraction is, de facto, epistemologically closely linked to that of categorization [103,104]. In the field of cognitive and developmental psychology, various approaches to the nature of the content and organization of concepts are explored, each focusing on specific (complementary or divergent) aspects of conceptual entities : core knowledge [21,121], exemplars and prototypes as nodal entities calibrating concepts [92,115,123,138,90], semantic networks defining the structuring of concepts among themselves [25,26,60], the finalization of categories from a situated cognition perspective [10,48,73], the metaphorized dimension of concepts [68], concepts as mental models rooted in perceptual activity [69], and the relationship of concepts to a language of thought within a probabilistic stochastic approach [52]. These various approaches each highlight different modalities, functionalities, or purposes of abstraction in its relationship to categorization, but it is arguably Vergnaud [133,134] who connects them most directly by emphasizing that conceptual categorization is fundamentally an activity of abstraction and extraction of characteristics deemed relevant (concepts-in-act) or true (theorems-in-act) within the context of a given activity. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
This article investigates, within the field of neuropsychology of artificial intelligence, the process of categorical segmentation performed by language models. This process involves, across different neural layers, the creation of new functional categorical dimensions to analyze the input textual data and perform the required tasks. Each neuron in a multilayer perceptron (MLP) network is associated with a specific category, generated by three factors carried by the neural aggregation function: categorical priming, categorical attention, and categorical phasing. At each new layer, these factors govern the formation of new categories derived from the categories of precursor neurons. Through a process of categorical clipping, these new categories are created by selectively extracting specific subdimensions from the preceding categories, constructing a distinction between a form and a categorical background. We explore several cognitive characteristics of this synthetic clipping in an exploratory manner: categorical reduction, categorical selectivity, separation of initial embedding dimensions, and segmentation of categorical zones.
... 252), particularly when not engaged in goal-directed behaviour. However, no simple explanation should be expected for aesthetic preference, and there is also evidence that familiar stimuli with salient features are often considered more pleasing than unfamiliar stimuli (Martindale & Moore, 1988;Vogel et al., 2021). Symmetry is one form of informative structure that has long been considered important in the production and appreciation of art (Gardner & Kleiner, 2020), and perhaps Acheulean hand axes (Hodgson, 2011). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The majority of Pleistocene figurative cave art in Western Europe consists of line drawings depicting large herbivores from the side view, and outlines were sometimes abbreviated to the head-neck-dorsal line. It is often assumed that the side view and abbreviation were used because they facilitate animal recognition compared to other views and to complete outlines, respectively. We investigate these claims grounded on the ecological approach to picture perception. We present the implications of the ecological approach for the study of cave art and report an experiment conducted to examine the roles of perspective and abbreviation in cave art in relation to two roles: communicating about specific animals and inducing aesthetic appreciation. Participants were shown outlines of animals (bison, horse, hind, and ibex), which varied in terms of perspective (frontal, fronto-oblique, side, rear-oblique, and rear view) and abbreviation (complete or abbreviated). They were instructed to quickly identify them and to rate their aesthetic value. We found that side and oblique views provide equivalent information, equally facilitating recognition and inducing aesthetic appreciation; and did so more than frontal and rear views. We found that complete outlines facilitate recognition and induce more aesthetic appreciation compared to abbreviated outlines. Contrary to common assumptions, side views are not simply motivated by ease of recognition. Facts of ecological optics, production effort, and available drawing techniques must also be considered. Abbreviation may also be contingent on participation in a shared history of communicative practices and production effort, as its prevalence further from cave entrances might partly be motivated by the need to draw quickly as the light was scarce. Our results point to a complex interplay of perceptual, technical, and cultural factors in the development of early human art and show how an ecological approach to (picture) perception can bring new insights to inform these discussions.
... However, contemporary understandings of social prototypes demonstrate a resistance to sociocultural changes. Not only do we tend to categorize individuals according to how well they align with our ideas of prototypical representations, but we also tend to prefer prototypical members over nonprototypical members (Vogel et al., 2020). Critically, our use of prototypes to classify others is not benign. ...
Article
Full-text available
Black individuals with phenotypically African features tend to experience heightened discrimination and mistreatment. The current research examined how racial phenotypicality and prototypicality effect hate crime reporting metrics and beliefs about who evaluators are represented #BlackLivesMatter. Across five studies (N = 876), results indicate that, compared to low racially phenotypic Black targets, high phenotypic targets were seen as more represented by #BlackLivesMatter (Study 1). When depicted as being the victim of a hate crime, high phenotypic targets were deemed more credible and that it was more appropriate for them to report their victimization on the #BlackLivesMatter website compared to their low phenotypic counterparts by White (Study 2a and 2c) and Black participants (Study 2b and 2c). Black (Study 2b and 2c) and White (Study 3) participants showed differences in perceptions of harm following hate crime victimization. Study 3 extended these findings to a separate manipulation of prototypicality and used a more ecologically valid context. These findings provide support for the problematic exclusivity of narrow prototypes by demonstrating their effect on beliefs about who social justice movements represent, and how they influence beliefs about victim reporting metrics.
Article
Purpose Racial and ethnic minorities have long been underrepresented in the superhero genre. Empirical examination of the effects of this underrepresentation, however, has been limited. The purpose of this research was to identify if “being White” is a component of the superhero prototype. Design/methodology/approach Across three different studies, participants recruited through CloudResearch’s Connect platform and Amazon’s Mechanical Turk viewed different versions of comic panel compilations and responded to questions about their perceptions of characters shown in the panels. Findings When using measurements similar to those typically used to test for prototypicality, we found no evidence that “being White” is a component of the superhero prototype. However, when using less conspicuous questions, we found that participants exposed to a not-White character were more likely to recall the character’s race and correctly identify the purpose of the research, providing some indication that “being White” is a component of the superhero prototype. Our findings suggest that when studying race, researchers may need to transition from traditional experimental designs to less conspicuous methods. Originality/value In a time where research on diversity, equity and inclusion (DEI) has been confounded by social desirability bias, our findings demonstrate how researchers can use less conspicuous methods within traditional survey methodology to examine DEI questions.
Article
Purpose This study examines whether design typicality and the communication of the zero-waste concept as a sustainable practice impact consumers’ aesthetic preferences and purchase intentions for zero-waste apparel. Design/methodology/approach The study employed a 2 (dress design: typical vs atypical) × 2 (dress length: long vs short) × 2 (zero-waste concept communication: present vs absent) mixed factorial experimental design with an online survey of 137 female consumers, ages 19–34. Findings Respondents rated typical zero-waste design dresses significantly higher than atypical dresses for aesthetic preferences and purchase intentions. Further, the zero-waste design concept did not affect this typicality-based preference or purchase intention for zero-waste dresses. They also demonstrated greater overall aesthetic preferences for long than short zero-waste dresses. Design typicality moderated this effect such that aesthetic preferences and purchase intentions were greater for long than short-length dresses when the zero-waste dress design was typical. When the design was atypical, purchase intentions were greater for short than long dresses. Research limitations/implications Typicality is critical in consumers’ aesthetic preferences and purchase intentions for zero-waste apparel. Originality/value The study focused on zero-waste dress typicality as a critical factor in consumers’ preference formation and purchase intentions. Additionally, it investigated dress length preferences within typical and atypical designs.
Conference Paper
The hierarchy of semantic networks can also be observed in the functioning of economic systems. There are uncertainties in semantic networks, meaning that the classification of different attributes is not always clear. The same uncertainty is also present, for example, in the design of logistics strategies as a sub-strategy of the economy, which can lead to inconsistencies between the parameters of the system. It is important for a system to be resilient to both internal and external influences, and it is therefore necessary to develop a hierarchy of system parameters based on the semantic networks method and to examine the relationship between parameters in order to achieve resilience and long-term sustainability.
Article
Full-text available
The eminent role of processing fluency in judgment and decision-making is undisputed. Not only is fluency affected by sources as diverse as stimulus repetition or visual clarity, but it also has an impact on outcomes as diverse as liking for a stimulus or the subjective validity of a statement. Although several studies indicate that sources and outcomes are widely interchangeable, recent research suggests that judgments are differentially affected by conceptual and perceptual fluency, with stronger effects of conceptual (vs. perceptual) fluency on judgments of truth. Here, we propose a fluency-specificity hypothesis according to which conceptual fluency is more informative for content-related judgments, but perceptual fluency is more informative for judgments related to perception. Two experimental studies in which perceptual and conceptual fluency were manipulated orthogonally show the superiority of content repetition on judgments of truth but the superiority of visual contrast on aesthetic evaluations. The theoretical implications are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
Preprint
Full-text available
Understanding how humans perceive the likability of high-dimensional ``objects'' such as faces is an important problem in both cognitive science and AI/ML. Existing models of human preferences generally assume these preferences to be fixed. However, human assessment of facial attractiveness have been found to be highly context-dependent. Specifically, the classical Beauty-in-Averageness (BiA) effect, whereby a face blended from two original faces is judged to be more attractive than the originals, is significantly diminished or reversed when the original faces are recognizable, or when the morph is mixed-race/mixed gender and the attractiveness judgment is preceded by a race/gender categorization. This effect, dubbed Ugliness-in-Averageness (UiA), has previously been attributed to a disfluency account, which is both qualitative and clumsy in explaining BiA. We hypothesize, instead, that these contextual influences on face processing result from the dependence of attractiveness perception on an element of statistical typicality, and from an attentional mechanism that restricts face representation to a task-relevant subset of features, thus redefining typicality within that subspace. Furthermore, we propose a principled explanation of why statistically atypical objects are less likable: they incur greater encoding or processing cost associated with a greater prediction error, when the brain uses predictive coding to compare the actual stimulus properties with those expected from its associated categorical prototype. We use simulations to show our model provides a parsimonious, statistically grounded, and quantitative account of contextual dependence of attractiveness. We also validate our model using experimental data from a gender categorization task. Finally, we make model predictions for a proposed experiment that can disambiguate the previous disfluency account and our statistical typicality theory.
Article
Full-text available
Stimuli that capture the central tendency of presented exemplars are often preferred-a phenomenon also known as the classic beauty-in-averageness effect. However, recent studies have shown that this effect can reverse under certain conditions. We propose that a key variable for such ugliness-in-averageness effects is the category structure of the presented exemplars. When exemplars cluster into multiple subcategories, the global average should no longer reflect the underlying stimulus distributions, and will thereby become unattractive. In contrast, the subcategory averages (i.e., local averages) should better reflect the stimulus distributions, and become more attractive. In 3 studies, we presented participants with dot patterns belonging to 2 different subcategories. Importantly, across studies, we also manipulated the distinctiveness of the subcategories. We found that participants preferred the local averages over the global average when they first learned to classify the patterns into 2 different subcategories in a contrastive categorization paradigm (Experiment 1). Moreover, participants still preferred local averages when first classifying patterns into a single category (Experiment 2) or when not classifying patterns at all during incidental learning (Experiment 3), as long as the subcategories were sufficiently distinct. Finally, as a proof-of-concept, we mapped our empirical results onto predictions generated by a well-known computational model of category learning (the Generalized Context Model [GCM]). Overall, our findings emphasize the key role of categorization for understanding the nature of preferences, including any effects that emerge from stimulus averaging. (PsycINFO Database Record
Article
Full-text available
Human preferences can be shaped by evaluative conditioning (EC), which describes observed changes in liking of an initially neutral stimulus (conditioned stimulus) due to repeated paired presentations with an inherently positive or negative stimulus (unconditioned stimulus). The experimental procedure of EC implies that participants are repeatedly exposed to the conditioned stimulus. Prior research suggests that repeated exposure to stimuli facilitates their processing. Furthermore, the resulting experience of processing fluency is known to shape human preferences through its inherent positive valence. Surprisingly, however, the role of processing fluency due to repeated stimulus exposure has never been directly investigated within the context of EC. The present research extends current conceptualizations of EC by incorporating processing fluency. In particular, it presents the first study that differentiates between a direct effect of stimulus pairing and a fluency-mediated effect of stimulus repetition on liking in a standard EC procedure. This approach helps to answer the open theoretical questions of why negative EC (i.e., EC applying negative unconditioned stimuli) tends to produce smaller effects than positive EC (i.e., EC applying positive unconditioned stimuli), and why positive EC is less susceptible to extinction than negative EC. On this basis, we strongly recommend considering processing fluency in theoretical models and empirical studies on EC and other forms of evaluative learning.
Chapter
Full-text available
Review of research into the consequences of easy vs difficult processing; traces fluency effects to the hedonic quality of the processing experience.
Article
The belief in physiognomy—the art of reading character from faces—has been with us for centuries1, 2, 3. People everywhere infer traits (for example, trustworthiness) from faces, and these inferences predict economic, legal and even voting decisions2,4. Research has identified many configurations of facial features that predict specific trait inferences2,5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, and detailed computational models of such inferences have recently been developed5, 6, 7,15, 16, 17. However, these configurations do not fully account for trait inferences from faces. Here, we propose a new direction in the study of inferences from faces, inspired by a cognitive–ecological18, 19, 20 and implicit-learning approach21,22. Any face can be positioned in a statistical distribution of faces extracted from the environment. We argue that understanding inferences from faces requires consideration of the statistical position of the faces in this learned distribution. Four experiments show that the mere statistical position of faces imbues them with social meaning: faces are evaluated more negatively the more they deviate from a learned central tendency. Our findings open new possibilities for the study of face evaluation, providing a potential model for explaining both individual and cross-cultural variation, as individuals are immersed in varying environments that contain different distributions of facial features.
Article
Because rewards and punishments available are rarely known for certain, the ability to sense, respond, and resolve uncertainty at different levels has important value for survival and reproduction. Recent neuroscientific evidence indicates that distinct measures of uncertainty are represented in a distributed set of regions, in ways that are consistent with predictions of formal mathematical models of uncertainty from decision and reinforcement learning theories. We provide a review of this evidence as well as discussion of outstanding questions in this area.