ArticlePDF Available

COVID-19 is rapidly changing: Examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to this evolving pandemic

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background: Since the emergence of SARS-CoV-2, the virus that causes coronavirus disease (COVID-19) in late 2019, communities have been required to rapidly adopt community mitigation strategies rarely used before, or only in limited settings. This study aimed to examine the attitudes and beliefs of Australian adults towards the COVID-19 pandemic, and willingness and capacity to engage with these mitigation measures. In addition, we aimed to explore the psychosocial and demographic factors that are associated with adoption of recommended hygiene-related and avoidance-related behaviors. Methods: A national cross-sectional online survey of 1420 Australian adults (18 years and older) was undertaken between the 18 and 24 March 2020. The statistical analysis of the data included univariate and multivariate logistic regression analysis. Findings: The survey of 1420 respondents found 50% (710) of respondents felt COVID-19 would 'somewhat' affect their health if infected and 19% perceived their level of risk as high or very high. 84·9% had performed ≥1 of the three recommended hygiene-related behaviors and 93·4% performed ≥1 of six avoidance-related behaviors over the last one month. Adopting avoidance behaviors was associated with trust in government/authorities (aOR: 6.0, 95% CI 2.6-11·0), higher perceived rating of effectiveness of behaviors (aOR: 4·0, 95% CI: 1·8-8·7), higher levels of perceived ability to adopt social distancing strategies (aOR: 5.0, 95% CI: 1·5-9.3), higher trust in government (aOR: 6.0, 95% CI: 2.6-11.0) and higher level of concern if self-isolated (aOR: 1.8, 95% CI: 1.1-3.0). Interpretation: In the last two months, members of the public have been inundated with messages about hygiene and social (physical) distancing. However, our results indicate that a continued focus on supporting community understanding of the rationale for these strategies, as well as instilling community confidence in their ability to adopt or sustain the recommendations is needed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
COVID-19 is rapidly changing: Examining
public perceptions and behaviors in response
to this evolving pandemic
Holly SealeID
1
*, Anita E. Heywood
1
, Julie Leask
2,3
, Meru SheelID
4
, Susan Thomas
5
, David
N. Durrheim
5
, Katarzyna Bolsewicz
5
, Rajneesh Kaur
2,6
1School of Public Health and Community Medicine, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW,
Australia, 2Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia, 3National
Centre for Immunisation Research and Surveillance, Kids Research, Sydney Children’s Hospitals Network,
Westmead, NSW, Australia, 4National Centre for Epidemiology and Population Health, Research School of
Population Health, ANU College of Health and Medicine, The Australian National University, Acton, ACT,
Australia, 5School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Newcastle, Wallsend, NSW, Australia,
6Office of Medical Education, Faculty of Medicine, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia
*h.seale@unsw.edu.au
Abstract
Background
Since the emergence of SARS-CoV-2, the virus that causes coronavirus disease (COVID-
19) in late 2019, communities have been required to rapidly adopt community mitigation
strategies rarely used before, or only in limited settings. This study aimed to examine the
attitudes and beliefs of Australian adults towards the COVID-19 pandemic, and willingness
and capacity to engage with these mitigation measures. In addition, we aimed to explore the
psychosocial and demographic factors that are associated with adoption of recommended
hygiene-related and avoidance-related behaviors.
Methods
A national cross-sectional online survey of 1420 Australian adults (18 years and older) was
undertaken between the 18 and 24 March 2020. The statistical analysis of the data included
univariate and multivariate logistic regression analysis.
Findings
The survey of 1420 respondents found 50% (710) of respondents felt COVID-19 would
‘somewhat’ affect their health if infected and 19% perceived their level of risk as high or very
high. 849% had performed 1 of the three recommended hygiene-related behaviors and
934% performed 1 of six avoidance-related behaviors over the last one month. Adopting
avoidance behaviors was associated with trust in government/authorities (aOR: 6.0, 95% CI
2.6–110), higher perceived rating of effectiveness of behaviors (aOR: 40, 95% CI: 18–
87), higher levels of perceived ability to adopt social distancing strategies (aOR: 5.0, 95%
CI: 15–9.3), higher trust in government (aOR: 6.0, 95% CI: 2.6–11.0) and higher level of
concern if self-isolated (aOR: 1.8, 95% CI: 1.1–3.0).
PLOS ONE
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112 June 23, 2020 1 / 13
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Seale H, Heywood AE, Leask J, Sheel M,
Thomas S, Durrheim DN, et al. (2020) COVID-19 is
rapidly changing: Examining public perceptions
and behaviors in response to this evolving
pandemic. PLoS ONE 15(6): e0235112. https://doi.
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112
Editor: Wen-Jun Tu, Chinese Academy of Medical
Sciences and Peking Union Medical College,
CHINA
Received: April 6, 2020
Accepted: June 2, 2020
Published: June 23, 2020
Copyright: ©2020 Seale et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: The data is available
at https://figshare.com/articles/COVID-19_
AustSurvey_xlsx/12298844.
Funding: No funding was received for this specific
study. MS is supported by a fellowship from the
Westpac Scholars Trust.
Competing interests: The authors have read the
journal’s policy and the authors of this paper have
the following competing interests: HS has
previously received funding from drug companies
Interpretation
In the last two months, members of the public have been inundated with messages about
hygiene and social (physical) distancing. However, our results indicate that a continued
focus on supporting community understanding of the rationale for these strategies, as well
as instilling community confidence in their ability to adopt or sustain the recommendations is
needed.
Introduction
In the course of four months, since the first reports about a novel strain of coronavirus, severe
acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) emerging in December 2019 [1,2],
countries around the world have introduced a range of community mitigation strategies with
the aim to lower the trajectory of this pandemic by reducing transmission, and avoid over-
whelming health services. Community mitigation strategies refer to measures that people, and
communities can take to slow the spread of infection during a period when vaccines and/or
medical treatments that are not available [3]. They include the use of personal protective mea-
sures for everyday use (e.g., voluntary home isolation of ill persons, respiratory etiquette, and
hand hygiene); community measures aimed at increasing social distancing (e.g., maintaining a
physical distance of 15–20 meters between people, staying at home and postponing or cancel-
ling gatherings); and environmental measures (e.g., routinely disinfecting surfaces). In some
settings these strategies are voluntary, whereas in others they are now enforced (such as, via
fines and/or jail time).
Governments are implementing strategies at large-scale that have previously been used in
limited ways and for limited time periods i.e. during Ebola, avian influenza outbreaks and
SARS. This means a large proportion of the population do not have prior experience undertak-
ing these strategies. People’s ability to comply with recommendations during emergency situa-
tions is influenced by a range of modifiable and nonmodifiable factors including: (1) what
people perceive their susceptibility to infection to be [4]; (2) whether they perceive the infec-
tion to be serious, if acquired; (3) whether they have the necessary capacity, confidence and
resources to comply with the strategies [5]; and (4) their sociodemographic status [6]. Effective
control of this pandemic requires an understanding of people’s perceptions about their will-
ingness, motivation and ability/capacity to adopt strategies and how this relates to their per-
ceived risk [7]. Perceived costs, perceptions about the benefits of the behaviors and the
perceived impact of an individual’s behavior on another’s health will also influence engage-
ment with these behaviors [8,9].
In Australia, the government has recommended specific behaviors that can be classed as
hygiene-related and avoidance related. To engage in these behaviors, people will weigh up the
perceived costs and benefits related to themselves and others. It is therefore important to
understand community perceptions and behaviors in order to develop effective messages.
Accordingly, we carried out a cross sectional online survey of a large, demographically repre-
sentative sample of the population of Australia in March 2020.
Materials and methods
Cross sectional online survey
We conducted an online survey of Australian residents via a market research company (Qual-
ity Online Research (QOR)) between 18 and 24 March 2020. This sample size provided us
PLOS ONE
Examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112 June 23, 2020 2 / 13
for investigator driven research and consulting fees
to present at conferences/workshops and develop
resources (bio-CSL/Sequiris, GSK and Sanofi
Pasteur). She has also participated in advisory
board meeting for Sanofi Pasteur. This does not
alter our adherence to PLOS ONE policies on
sharing data and materials. There are no patents,
products in development or marketed products
associated with this research to declare.
with a sample error of ±3%. Proportional quota sampling was used to ensure that respondents
were demographically representative of the general public, with quotas based on age, gender
and state/territory. Respondents were required to be 18 years or older and to speak English.
Respondents earned points for completing the survey.
Ethics statement
Ethics approval for the study was obtained from the University of New South Wales HREAP
G: Health, Medical, Community and Social (HC200190). After reading the participant infor-
mation, consent was implied if the person completed the survey and submitted it via the QOR
website. No personal identifiers were collected.
Survey design
The questions for this survey were adapted from published studies by HS during the 2009
influenza H1N1/A pandemic [10,11]. The study tool is available upon request. Two primary
outcome variables used were hygiene-related and avoidance-related behaviors. (see Table 1).
Ten items were used to assess respondent perceptions about the COVID-19 pandemic, includ-
ing perceived risk level and impact on health (if infected). 8/10 items were phrased as state-
ments, with Likert response options scored as 5 for strongly agree through to 1 for strongly
disagree. Two items measuring participants level of worry about current Covid-19 were used
on a 5 point Likert scale ranging from 1 for strongly disagree to 5 for strongly agree, these were
combined and changed into a dichotomous scale of high and low.
Respondents were asked to rate the perceived level of effectiveness of 13 items in reducing
the risk from COVID-19 on a 5-point scale These items included those promoted by the gov-
ernment and those that were not (mask use when not symptomatic, taking antibiotics). The
Table 1. Adoption of hygiene-related and avoidance-related behaviors.in response to COVID-19.
Hygiene-related behaviors: actions taken over last month due to COVID-
19 (Cronbach’s alpha = 0701)
Number (%) Standardised
loading
Increased the time I spent cleaning or disinfecting things I might touch,
such as door knobs
537/1420
(378)
0.740
Washed my hands with soap and water more often than usual 1088/1420
(766)
0.763
Used alcoholic hand gel or hand sanitizer more than usual 806/1420
(568)
0.807
Avoidance-related behaviors: Actions taken over last month due to
COVID-19 (Cronbach’s alpha = 0797)
Deliberately cancelled or postponed a social event 519/1161
(447)
0.814
Cancelled or delayed travelling overseas 407/811
(502)
0.745
Reduced my use of public transport 448/880
(509)
0.816
Kept away from crowded places generally 889/1332
(667)
0.778
Performed 1 of three recommended behaviors 1205/1420
(849)
Performed 1 of six avoidance behaviors 1326/1420
(934)
Total number of participants lower than overall total of 1420 due to exclusion of participants who ticked ‘not
applicable’ options.
#
587 participants had children of school or childcare going age.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112.t001
PLOS ONE
Examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112 June 23, 2020 3 / 13
strategies were grouped into: (1) hygiene related behaviors (hand washing/sanitizing, cleaning
surfaces) and (2) avoidance-related behaviors (avoiding crowds, public transport, and comply-
ing with quarantine restrictions). Given the relative novelty of social distancing for the Austra-
lian community, we also included a question that assessed the respondent’s ability to adopt 6
different social distancing strategies (working from home, keeping children home from school,
avoiding travelling, avoiding large crowds, quarantine if exposed, and isolation if symptom-
atic) with possible response options scored as 1 for very high and 5 for very low. The last sec-
tion of the survey included six items focused on self-isolation. Respondents were asked to
comment on their willingness to comply, their level of concern regarding the impact on being
placed into self-isolation (at home), their ability to comply, their access to assistance from fam-
ily/friends and issues they have with the strategy. All predictor variables and the items and
scales are described in the supplementary materials.
We collected data on gender, age, education and employment status, children (including
attendance at childcare/school), country of birth/language spoken at home, whether they iden-
tify as Aboriginal and/or Torres Strait Islander, international travel patterns since 1 January
2020, private healthcare insurance coverage, income protection insurance, the presence of any
chronic illness and self-reported health status (very good, good, moderate, poor, very poor).
Due to the promotion of social distancing and working from home by the Australian Govern-
ment, we also included two items that assessed access to internet and a computer at home.
Analysis. Correlation matrix of all scales was studied. Items of scales with low correlation
with the rest of the items of a scale were excluded. This was followed by Principle Component
analyses using direct Oblimin rotation to determine the number of components to retain and
loading of items for all scales. Cohen alpha was used to check the internal consistency of all
items of the scale (See S1 Table). Univariate associations were tested between primary outcome
measures and demographic factors. Univariate associations between worry and outcome mea-
sures was also assessed using univariate logistic regression. Two separate multivariate logistic
regression models were used to measure the associations of perception factors with each out-
come factor after adjusting for independent variables. Demographic variables with a P<0.25 in
the univariate analysis were used to adjust the models. In Model one hygiene behaviour was
tested as the outcome and was adjusted for gender, country of birth, travelled overseas and
worry variables. Model two tested avoidance behaviour as the outcome and was adjusted for
age, gender, country of birth, employment, current health conditions and worry variables. For
all analyses, P values of less than 0.05 were considered statistically significant. Data were ana-
lyzed using the SPSS software version 26.0 (SPSS Science, Chicago, IL, USA).
Correlation matrix of hygiene related behaviour items demonstrated a moderate correlation
of included three items (rs ranged between 0.34 and 0.405). Overall KMO value of 0.65 was
adequate for exploratory factor analysis. Correlation matrix for the avoidance behaviour scale
revealed that the third and the last items were outliers and were not related to the other items
in the scale (rs = 0. 19 and 0.10 respectively) and were therefore excluded. The correlation of
the remaining items showed moderate correlation (rs ranged between 0.33 and 0.39). Overall
KMO value was 0.66. Standardised loading of items included in these scales is shown in
Table 1.
From the rating of level of effectiveness to reduce risk of COVID, fourth and seventh items
of the scale demonstrated a low correlation (0.04 and 0.14) with other items of the scale and
were therefore excluded. The remaining items of the scale showed moderate to high correla-
tion (rs between 0.49–0.67). Similarly, second item from the rating of ability to adopt social
distancing strategies scale was excluded due to its low correlation with rest of the scale items
(rs = 0.16). The remaining items of the scale showed moderate correlation (rs between 0.34–
0.55). From the variable ‘rating of level of concern if self-isolated’ third item was excluded due
PLOS ONE
Examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112 June 23, 2020 4 / 13
to low correlation with other items of the scale (rs = 0.15). The rest of the items of the scale
showed moderate correlation (rs between 0.32–0.44). The factor loading of items of these three
scales is shown in S1 Table. Combined items for all variables demonstrated a moderate (Cron-
bach’s α= 0.733) to high (Crohn’s α= 0.877) internal consistency and were therefore retained.
Results
Of the 1420 respondents, 740 (52%), were female, 47 (33%) identified as Aboriginal and/or
Torres Strait Islanders, 830 (585%) had private health insurance, 792 (55.8%) had children
with 211/792 (267%) attended childcare/school. Of the respondents, 37 (%) reported knowing
of a COVID-19 case amongst their family or friends. Television news was the primary source
of information about COVID-19 (n = 724, 51%), followed by government websites
(n = 241,17%) and social media (n = 198, 14%). When asked about the level of trust they had
in the information coming from the Government, 667 (47%) stated high to very high.
Perceptions about susceptibility and severity
Respondents ranked their risk of acquiring COVID-19 as very high (n = 71, 5%); high
(n = 198, 14%), intermediate (n = 497, 35%), low (n = 397, 28%) and very low (n = 156, 11%).
The remaining 100 respondents reported not knowing what their risk was. When it came to
perceived impact on their health, 710 (50%) reported that COVID-19 would ‘somewhat’ affect
it, while the remaining respondents reported it as: extremely (n = 170, 12%), seriously
(n = 326, 23%), not at all (n = 85, 6%) or don’t know (n = 113, 8%). Fifty percent reported
changing their personal perception of risk after reading or hearing information in the media
or on social media.
Perceived effectiveness of social mitigation strategies and ability to adopt
Fig 1 shows the perceptions towards the degree of effectiveness of measures to reduce personal
risk from COVID-19. Self-quarantine of anyone who has travelled into Australia from overseas
was considered to have high to very high effectiveness (n = 1171, 825%), followed by avoiding
people who have travelled overseas (n = 1155, 814%). Whereas, only 525 (37%) thought that
shutting the restaurants/bars after 6pm would have a high/very high effect and 696 (49%) stated
that wearing a mask (when not symptomatic) would be effective. Taking antibiotics was consid-
ered to have low to very low effectiveness by most (n = 908, 64%). Beyond perceived effectiveness,
respondents were asked to comment on their ability (self-efficacy) to carry out social distancing
strategies, 596 (42%) respondents rated their ability to work from home as high/very high.
Practice of recommended measures/behavior
The most common hygiene-related behavior adopted was washing hands with soap and water
(n = 1087, 766%), whereas keeping away from crowded places generally was the most com-
mon avoidance behavior (n = 947, 667%). Overall, 1205 (849%) respondents reported
undertaking 1 of three hygiene-related behaviors and 1326 (934%) performed 1 of six
avoidance-related behaviors (Table 1).
Table 2 shows association between demographic characteristics and reported behaviors dur-
ing COVID-19 pandemic. Five hundred and five (363%) respondents considered the hygiene-
related and avoidance-related behaviors as ‘the right thing to do’ as their main motivation to
comply. Close to 80% of respondents (n = 1127) who reported being worried about COVID-19
(high-very high) were found to have higher engagement with hygiene-related behaviors (OR
4.2, 95% CI: 3.1–58) and avoidance-related behaviors (OR 4.0, 95% CI: 26–6.2).
PLOS ONE
Examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112 June 23, 2020 5 / 13
Controlling for demographic and worry variables, there was a higher association between
performance of hygiene-related behaviors and trust in government/authorities (aOR: 27, 95%
CI 14–5.1), perceived high severity if infected (aOR: 14, 95% CI 12–3.0), higher levels of
belief in the effectiveness of behaviors (aOR 32, 95% CI: 1.4–7.2), higher ability to adopt social
distancing strategies (aOR: 3.6,95% CI 16–7.0), higher levels of concern if self-isolated (aOR:
24 95% CI: 11–40) and intermediate to higher level of risk perception (aOR: 1.6, 95% CI:
11–20, aOR: 2.0, 95% CI: 12–3.5) led to performance of recommended behaviors. Reporting
the use of avoidance behaviors was more likely in respondents who: trusted government/
authorities (aOR: 6.0, 95% CI 2.6–110), rated effectiveness of behaviors higher (aOR: 4.0, 95%
CI: 1.3–12.7), and indicated a higher ability to adopt social distancing strategies (aOR: 5.0, 95%
CI 15–13.6), perceived high severity if infected (aOR: 1.8, 95% CI: 1.1–3.0) (Table 3).
Six questions focused on self-isolation as a strategy. The majority (n = 1349, 95%) agreed
that they could self-isolate if necessary and that they had a family member or friend who could
assist them in the event of isolation (n = 1178, 83%). However, respondents did have concerns
(high/very high) about not being able to access shops for food/supplies (n = 681, 48%) and not
being able to access a primary care provider (n = 553, 39%). Amongst those who felt they
could not manage self-isolation at home (n = 122, 8%), the main concerns were centered
around carers responsibilities for children, elderly parents and disabled family members.
Discussion
Our results suggest that a large proportion of respondents have adopted one or more of either
the hygiene-related and/or avoidance-related behaviors that had been recommended by the
Australian Government. Considering the intense media coverage and government
Fig 1. Rating of level of effectiveness of strategies to control Covid-19 outbreak.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112.g001
PLOS ONE
Examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112 June 23, 2020 6 / 13
information, it is not surprising that there was a gravitation towards actual (or willingness for)
adoption of hygiene strategies including hand washing/sanitizing. While anxiety levels were
moderate, concerns were raised about accessing food and medical supplies if placed into self-
Table 2. Association between demographic characteristics and adoption of preventive/avoidance strategies during COVID-19 pandemic.
Variable No (%) of
participants
No (%) using hygiene
behaviors
Odds ratio (95%
CI)
No (%) using avoidance
behavior
Odds ratio (95%
CI)
Sex
Women 740 (521) 646 (873) 15 (11–21) 707 (955) 0.7(0.5–1)
Men 678 (477) 559 (824) Ref 618 (912) Ref
Other
#
2 (01) 0 (0) - 1 (50) -
Age group
18–49 803 (565) 685 (853) Ref 739 (92) Ref
50 617 (435) 520 (843) 09 (07–12) 587 (951) 10 (0.7–1.6)
Aboriginal and/or Torres
Strait Islander
Yes 47 (33) 40 (851) 1.0 (05–2.2) 42 (894) 0.8 (03–2.1)
No 1373 (967) 1165 (849) Ref 1284 (935) Ref
Country of birth
Australia 1096 (772) 919 (839) Ref 1011 (922) Ref
Other 324 (228) 286 (883) 07 (05–10) 315 (972) 3.1 (17–60)
Working status
Not working 591 (416) 497 (841) 12 (09–16) 567 (959) 0.7 (0.5–1.2)
Working full/part time 829 (584) 708 (854) Ref 759 (916) Ref
Educational attainment 30 (21)
None 131 27 (90) Ref 28 (933) Ref
School certificate (year 10) (92)235 108 (924) 05 (02–19) 119 (908) 07 (02–2.7)
Leaving certificate (year 12) (165) 197 (838) 06 (02–21) 219 (932) 12 (03–43)
Trade/apprenticeship/cert 483 (34) 405 (839) 06 (02–2.0) 449 (93) 09 (03–31)
Bachelor’s degree 379 (267) 328 (865) 08 (02–27) 359(947) 2.0 (06–6.7)
Masters or higher 162 (114) 140 (864) 08 (02–27) 152(938) 2.6 (07–9.4)
Children in household
Attending childcare/school 212 (149) 184 (866) 1.2 (08–19) 195 (92) 1.4 (08–2.5)
Not attending childcare/school or no
children
1208 (851) 1021 (845) Ref 1131(936) Ref
Travelled overseas in 2020 222 (156) 201 (905) 1.9(12–3.1)206 (928) 2.3 (1.4–3.9
No 1198 (844) 1004(838) Ref 1120 (935) Ref
Have private health 830 (585) 712(85.8) 1.2(09–17) 1. (93) 1.2 (08–19)
Insurance
No 590 (415) 493(836) Ref 554 (939) Ref
Health rating
Very good/good 1009 (711) 861 (8)69 Ref 945 (937) Ref
Moderate 294 (207) 241 (82) 06 (04–1.1) 271 (922) 11 (05–27)
Poor/very poor 117 (82) 95 (81.2) 07 (0.5–1.0) 110 (94) 08 (05–13)
Chronic health condition
Present 363 (256) 317 (873) 1.5(07–3.3) 345 (95) 12 (04–3.2)
None 1057 (744) 888 (84) Ref 981(928) Ref
P<005
# Not included in OR calculations.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112.t002
PLOS ONE
Examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112 June 23, 2020 7 / 13
isolation. Given the limited community behavioral COVID-19 studies published, we have
compared our findings to two ongoing community surveys. Firstly, results from an online poll
of 14,000 respondents from 14 countries (conducted at the same time point as our study),
reported that their respondents in 8/14 countries expressed a belief that social distancing mea-
sures such as travel bans, and self-isolation would not prevent the spread of the virus, including
participants from Australia (52%) [12]. Secondly, a German survey (conducted a week earlier
then our study) identified that respondents had high levels of knowledge, but adoption of
important protection behaviors was very low, and risk perceptions were especially low among
the elderly [13]. Lastly, a survey from Malaysia reported that a high proportion of respondents
were already adopting precautions such as avoiding crowds (83.4%) and practicing proper
hand hygiene (87.8%) at the time of their study in late March. However, the wearing of face
masks was less common (51.2%) [14].
Amongst our participants, perceived susceptibility to COVID-19 was at an intermediate
level. This aligns with results found in a similar study conducted in the UK, which reported
that just under half of their cohort (n = 2,108) were likely to acquire COVID-19, while 56% felt
that it would have a moderate impact (i.e. would require them to self-care and rest in bed)
[15]. In addition, a separate survey of Australian residents also found that two thirds of respon-
dents were at least moderately worried about a widespread COVID-19 outbreak in Australia
Table 3. Logistic regression models testing association between perception variables and adoption of hygiene/avoidance strategies during COVID-19 outbreak.
Association with carrying or 1 preventive
behavior
Association with carrying out 1 avoidance
behavior
Variables No. (%) OR (95% CI) Adjusted OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) Adjusted OR (95% CI)
Trust in government/authorities
High 1315/1401 (939) 4.2 (26–67)27 (14–5.1)5.8 (3.2–10.7)60 (2.6–11.0)
Low 86/1401 (6.1) Ref Ref Ref Ref
Perceived Severity
High 1219/1400 (858) 26 (18–37)14 (11–23)20 (12–33)15 (07–3.2)
Low 181/1400 (12.7) Ref Ref Ref Ref
Rating of level of effectiveness of behaviors~
High 1150 (81) 8.8 (5.0–15.5)32 (1.4–7.2)14.3 (6.0–24.3)40 (1.88.7)
Low 270 (19) Ref Ref Ref Ref
Ability to adopt social distancing strategies
High 740/1293 (52.1) 5.7 (3.2–10.4)3.6 (1.6–7.0)15.7 (8.6258)5.0 (1.5–9.3)
Low 553/1293 (38.9) Ref Ref Ref Ref
Level of concern if self-isolated
High 274/1359 (193) 1.4 (11–2.0)24 (11–40)15 (08–29 18 (1.1–30)
Low 1085/1359 (76.4) Ref Ref Ref Ref
Level of Risk
Very low/low 553/1320 (419) Ref Ref Ref Ref
Intermediate 496 /1320 (376) 20 (14–28)16 (11–20)15 (1.0–24) 1.1 (06–17)
Very high/high 271/1320 (205) 31 (19–49)20 (12–35)3.0 (17–5.4) 17 (08–3.4)
Impact on health
No/somewhat 806/1304 (618) Ref Ref Ref Ref
Serious/Extreme 498/1304 (382) 16 (12–23)10 (06–15) 3.2 (2.0–5.2)16 (09–2.9)
P<0.05, Two logistic regression models: Model one: Hygiene related behaviour as the outcome, model adjusted for gender, country of birth, travelled overseas, private
health insurance, rating of current health and worry, variables with P<0.25 in the univariate analysis. Model two: avoidance behaviours as the outcome, model adjusted
for gender, country of birth, employment, ATSI status, travelled overseas, children attending childcare/school or not, and worry, variables with P<0.25.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112.t003
PLOS ONE
Examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112 June 23, 2020 8 / 13
in early March [16]. Risk perceptions were also found to be moderate (median 5 out of 10)
based on an online poll of US residents undertaken in early February [17]. There may be sev-
eral factors at play here that account for the perceived level of risk. Firstly, people may be
unaware of true risk since little reporting has focused on attack rates during the time of the
study. Secondly, people may be subject to optimism bias–a phenomenon where people down-
play their own risk of an outcome [18]. Thirdly, it is likely that some people assess their risk as
being low due to already factoring in a change towards anticipated or already accomplished
protective behavior [19]. During events that people deem ‘familiar’, we often see unrealistic
optimism because the risk is perceived to be under control, as was the case in 2009 with the
influenza pandemic, when adoption of precautions was low and there was a sense of personal
security [4]. However, COVID-19 presents as an unfamiliar risk (for the large majority of the
population had not experienced outbreaks of SARS or MERS) making the risk less tolerable
for those who perceive the situation as uncontrollable [20]. When it comes to perceptions of
risk, there are numerous studies documenting how they are associated with the uptake of pre-
ventive and/or avoidant behaviors. Studies conducted during/after the 2003 SARS outbreak
reported that higher levels of perceived risk/susceptibility of SARS was associated with the
adoption of preventive behaviors and also avoidance behaviors [2124].
While understanding a person’s perception of risk is important, it is not the only condition
needed for engagement. Higher risk perceptions may only predict protective behavior when
people believe that effective protective actions are available (response efficacy) and when they
are confident that they can engage in such protective actions (self-efficacy) [25]. According to
Bandura social cognitive theory, an individual’s self-efficacy plays a crucial role on the individ-
ual’s likelihood to engage in a desired behavior. If an individual does not believe that he/she
can carry out the behavior (i.e. physically distance themselves), there is little motivation to
engage [26]. Three-quarters of the study respondents agreed that they could adopt the avoid-
ance-related strategies, with lower scores for working from home and self-isolation at home.
When asked whether they had adopted any of the hygiene related strategies, washing or sani-
tizing hands were the most common responses. These findings have also been replicated in an
online survey of 5974 residents from the US and UK, that found that 92% of the cohort would
adopt hygiene related behaviors [27]. These represent more readily adoptable strategies, as
people in the community understand how to engage in them, believe that the strategy will pro-
tect them, and usually have the resources to carry them out. These easy to adopt actions have
also been a focus of government mass media messages.
When it came to avoidance behaviors, our respondents were less inclined to rate them as
being effective or to have adopted them, in comparison to the preventive behaviors listed
above. Perceptions regarding the efficacy of the strategy (as opposed to self-efficacy) have also
been found to impact on intentions/likelihood to adopt or actual uptake [23,28]. It is not sur-
prising that some strategies including social distancing, scored low as people may not under-
stand what the strategy entails, the rationale for its use, or what impact it may have on one’s
health. It should also be noted that individuals may not have the capacity or resources to com-
ply with physical distancing measures because they: (1) have extended families living in their
households; (2) they have a responsibility to provide care for someone outside of their home;
(3) they may reside in share accommodation; (4) may not have access to internet/computer in
the home setting or (5) because of the type of job they have they cannot simply shift to working
from home. In these settings, providing mass media education is not going to suffice. What is
needed is pragmatic solutions that support people financially and socially to participate. Exam-
ples including increases in social support and charities to assist with delivery of groceries and
meals, home delivery from chemists, telehealth consultations bulk billed, drive through vac-
cine clinics etc.
PLOS ONE
Examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112 June 23, 2020 9 / 13
It has been suggested that we start to prime people about what additional strategies may still
need to be introduced [29]. This would entail talking with them about why the strategy would
be implemented; the end-goal of implementing it; what could be the potential impacts; how
members of the public engage; and the criteria for its de-escalation. In order to promote cooper-
ation with social (physical distancing) strategies, governments may need to use realistic portray-
als (community stories) and role modelling by influential actors in social networks. Observing
competent role models perform actions that result in success conveys information to observers
about the sequence of actions to use to be successful [30]. Motivation may be helped by creating
media campaigns that foster awareness of the recommended behaviors and encourage people to
share their strategies for complying with self-isolation and working from home.
Amongst our respondents, older age was associated with the adoption of precautionary behav-
iors, which aligns with the findings from Singapore and Hong Kong during the 2003 SARS out-
break [7,23] and some studies during the 2009 H1N1/A pandemic [31,32]. However, the pattern
of age is not straight forward. In contrast to the above studies, others have reported higher levels
of adoption of preventive behaviors amongst younger people (18–24 years) in the context of the
2009 influenza pandemic. When it comes to gender, we found that females were more likely to
report uptake of both preventive and avoidance behaviors, consistent with studies during SARS
and H1N1 pandemic influenza [4,7,23,33]. Earlier studies have indicated that women are more
likely to perceive themselves to be susceptible and hence adopt the behaviors [21,34]. When it
comes to country of birth, we found that people born outside of Australia were less likely to
adopt behaviors. This finding may relate to the capacity to access information, which at the time,
was being disseminated in English and largely through mainstream media conferences and health
department websites rather than community and language groups. Further work is needed to
explore the associations between country of birth and pandemic-related behaviors.
When asked what would motivate respondents to comply with a social distancing strategy,
they nominated ‘I believe it is the right thing to do’ as the primary response. While this answer
did not have any significant relationship with the outcome measures reviewed, it is still rele-
vant when it comes to planning communication messages. It suggests that respondents may be
influenced by a desire for social approval from others, an idea linked to the model of moral
motivation. The model, developed by Brekke et al. (2003), assumes that individuals have pref-
erences for achieving and maintaining a self-image as a socially responsible person [35]. In
Brekke et al.’s model, self-image improves when the individual’s actual behavior gets closer to
her/his view of the “morally ideal” behavior (i.e. the behavior that would maximize social wel-
fare if chosen by every member of society). However, individual’s participation can be condi-
tional on whether they think others are also contributing [36]. Mass media campaigns that
frame their messages around a social collective action/power or the inclusion of the general
public within a team to assist the community response may be effective. The promotion of pro-
social behaviors has been shown to be effective in vaccination uptake and could be adapted in
promoting COVID-19 mitigation behaviors, such as how one’s actions can contribute to pro-
tecting their grandparents [37]. This idea has been picked up by celebrities and the wider com-
munity on twitter under the #LockDownForLove, with people nominating who they are social
distancing for. Whether these strategies work, needs to be further examined.
Early results from a study conducted across the US, UK and Germany has suggested that
inducing empathy for those most vulnerable to the virus promotes the motivation to adhere to
physical distancing [38]. The use of empathy in messaging is not a new concept and has been
applied in a range of ways from the promotion of testing/treatment for STIs, through to increas-
ing our acceptance of robots [39,40]. Empathy is a skill which enables understanding of another
person’s experience. Here, people could be asked to imagine the perceptions, needs and impact
(health, financial, social) of pandemic COVID-19 amongst our family members/friends.
PLOS ONE
Examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112 June 23, 2020 10 / 13
Our study includes a large, representative cross-section of the adult Australian population.
People who could not communicate in English were excluded from the sample, which may
have affected representation of ethnic minorities. We also had under-representation of
Aboriginal Australians and Torres Strait Islanders and from those in remote settings. Sec-
ondly, as participation in our study was on a voluntary basis, this study has potential for self-
selection bias by community members who are particularly concerned about this pandemic.
We relied on self-reports of behaviors which may have led to over-reported (social desirability
bias) However, this may have minimized as the survey was self-complete and anonymous.
Lastly, we did not collect information about income level and so unable to comment on varia-
tions in behaviors by income.
Based on the available data, it appears that older individuals (aged >60 years) and people
with chronic underlying health conditions are particularly susceptible to severe disease. This
presents a challenging situation. In the media there is reporting that ‘COVID-19 is causing
mild illness’ in the majority but it’s in the best interest of the country to stay home in order to
‘flatten the curve’. This will cause two responses–those who continue with their normal prac-
tice (not adopting or complying with the recommendations around social distancing/mitiga-
tion strategies) as identified in a proportion of our respondents. Motivating this group
(especially those less likely to be at risk or suffer the health impact of COVID-19) to adopt
behaviors that require marked change in their routines, beyond those related to personal
hygiene. It may prove difficult unless people understand the required behavior, the rationale
for it, are given clear and sufficient information about how to comply, and they believe the
strategy will have an impact and are motivated to act [9,23,41]. They also need to have capac-
ity and opportunity to comply with new behaviors for another 4 to 6 months. In order to
engage a community, they need to feel like they are a valued part of a team, and that their con-
tributions are valued and key to the response. Lastly, it is essential that governments ensure
that resources, legalization and support measures are in place in order to facilitate community
participation in community mitigation strategies.
Supporting information
S1 Table. List of original and recoded predictor variables.
(DOCX)
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the respondents for their time in participating in the research study.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Holly Seale.
Data curation: Holly Seale.
Formal analysis: Holly Seale, Anita E. Heywood, Rajneesh Kaur.
Methodology: Holly Seale, Anita E. Heywood, Julie Leask, Meru Sheel, Susan Thomas, David
N. Durrheim, Katarzyna Bolsewicz, Rajneesh Kaur.
Resources: Susan Thomas.
Writing – original draft: Holly Seale, Anita E. Heywood, Julie Leask, Meru Sheel, Susan
Thomas, David N. Durrheim, Katarzyna Bolsewicz, Rajneesh Kaur.
PLOS ONE
Examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112 June 23, 2020 11 / 13
References
1. Anastassopoulou C, Russo L, Tsakris A, Siettos C. Data-based analysis, modelling and forecasting of
the COVID-19 outbreak. PloS one. 2020; 15:e0230405. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230405
PMID: 32231374
2. Guan WJ, ZY, Hu Y, Liang WH, Ou CQ, He JX, et al. Clinical Characteristics of Coronavirus Disease
2019 in China. N Engl J Med. 2020; 382:1708–20. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa2002032 PMID:
32109013
3. Anderson RM, Heesterbeek H, Klinkenberg D, Hollingsworth TD. How will country-based mitigation
measures influence the course of the COVID-19 epidemic? The Lancet. 2020; 395:931–4.
4. Rubin GJ, Amlo
ˆt R, Page L, Wessely S. Public perceptions, anxiety, and behaviour change in relation to
the swine flu outbreak: cross sectional telephone survey. Br Med J. 2009; 339:b2651.
5. Lau JT, Griffiths S, Choi KC, Lin C. Prevalence of preventive behaviors and associated factors during
early phase of the H1N1 influenza epidemic. Am J Infect Control. 2010; 38(5):374–80. Epub 2010/06/
24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajic.2010.03.002 PMID: 20569849.
6. Blake KD, Blendon RJ, Viswanath K. Employment and compliance with pandemic influenza mitigation
recommendations. Emerging infectious diseases. 2010; 16(2):212–8. https://doi.org/10.3201/eid1602.
090638 PMID: 20113549.
7. Leung GM, Lam TH, Ho LM, Ho SY, Chan BHY, Wong IOL, et al. The impact of community psychological
responses on outbreak control for severe acute respiratory syndrome in Hong Kong. Journal of epidemi-
ology and community health. 2003; 57:857. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech.57.11.857 PMID: 14600110
8. Hershey JC, Asch DA, Thumasathit T, Meszaros J, Waters VV. The Roles of Altruism, Free Riding, and
Bandwagoning in Vaccination Decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes.
1994; 59(2):177–87. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1994.1055.
9. Bish A, Michie S. Demographic and attitudinal determinants of protective behaviours during a pan-
demic: a review. British journal of health psychology. 2010; 15:797–824. https://doi.org/10.1348/
135910710X485826 PMID: 20109274
10. Seale H, McLaws M- L, Heywood AE, Ward KF, Lowbridge CP, Van D, et al. The community’s attitude
towards swine flu and pandemic influenza. Medical Journal of Australia. 2009; 191:267–9. PMID: 19740048
11. Seale H, Heywood A, McLaws M, Ward K, Lowbridge C, Van D, et al. Why do I need it? I am not at risk!
Public perceptions towards the pandemic (H1N1) 2009 vaccine. BMC Infectious Diseases. 2010; 10
(99):1–9.
12. Public divided on whether isolation, travel bans prevent COVID-19 spread; border closures become
more acceptable–Ipsos survey [Internet]. Toronto, Canada 2020
13. Betsch C. How behavioural science data helps mitigate the COVID-19 crisis. Nature Human Behaviour.
2020.
14. Azlan AA, Hamzah MR, Sern TJ, Ayub SH, Mohamad E. Public knowledge, attitudes and practices
towards COVID-19: A cross-sectional study in Malaysia. PloS one. 2020; 15(5):e0233668. https://doi.
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0233668 PMID: 32437434
15. Atchison CJ, Bowman L, Vrinten C, Redd R, Pristera P, Eaton JW, et al. Perceptions and behavioural
responses of the general public during the COVID-19 pandemic: A cross-sectional survey of UK Adults.
medRxiv. 2020:2020.04.01.20050039.
16. Faasse K, Newby JM. Public perceptions of COVID-19 in Australia: perceived risk, knowledge, health-
protective behaviours, and vaccine intentions. medRxiv. 2020:2020.04.25.20079996. https://doi.org/
10.1101/2020.04.25.20079996
17. McFadden SM, Malik AA, Aguolu OG, Willebrand KS, Omer SB. Perceptions of the adult US population
regarding the novel coronavirus outbreak. PloS one. 2020; 15(4):e0231808. https://doi.org/10.1371/
journal.pone.0231808 PMID: 32302370
18. Sharot T. The optimism bias. Current biology: CB. 2011; 21:R941–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.
2011.10.030 PMID: 22153158.
19. Brewer NT, Chapman GB, Gibbons FX, Gerrard M, McCaul KD, Weinstein ND. Meta-analysis of the
relationship between risk perception and health behavior: the example of vaccination. Health Psychol.
2007; 26:136–45. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.26.2.136 PMID: 17385964.
20. Brug J, Aro AR, Richardus JH. Risk Perceptions and Behaviour: Towards Pandemic Control of Emerg-
ing Infectious Diseases. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2009; 16(1):3. https://doi.org/10.
1007/s12529-008-9000-x PMID: 19127440
21. Brug J, Aro AR, Oenema A, de Zwart O, Richardus JH, Bishop GD. SARS risk perception, knowledge,
precautions, and information sources, the Netherlands. Emerg Infect Dis. 2004; 10:1486–9. https://doi.
org/10.3201/eid1008.040283 PMID: 15496256
PLOS ONE
Examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112 June 23, 2020 12 / 13
22. Jones JH, Salathe
´M. Early Assessment of Anxiety and Behavioral Response to Novel Swine-Origin
Influenza A(H1N1). PloS one. 2009; 4:e8032. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0008032 PMID:
19997505
23. Lau JTF, Yang X, Tsui H, Kim JH. Monitoring community responses to the SARS epidemic in Hong
Kong: from day 10 to day 62. Journal of epidemiology and community health. 2003; 57:864. https://doi.
org/10.1136/jech.57.11.864 PMID: 14600111
24. Blendon RJ, Benson JM, DesRoches CM, Raleigh E, Taylor-Clark K. The public’s response to severe
acute respiratory syndrome in Toronto and the United States. Clin Infect Dis. 2004; 38(7):925–31. Epub
2004/03/23. https://doi.org/10.1086/382355 PMID: 15034821.
25. Rimal RN, Real K. Perceived Risk and Efficacy Beliefs as Motivators of Change. Human Communica-
tion Research. 2003; 29:370–99.
26. Bandura A. Social Cognitive Theory and Exercise of Control over HIV Infection. In: DiClemente RJ,
Peterson JL, editors. Preventing AIDS: Theories and Methods of Behavioral Interventions. Boston, MA:
Springer US; 1994. p. 25–59.
27. Geldsetzer P. Use of Rapid Online Surveys to Assess People’s Perceptions During Infectious Disease
Outbreaks: A Cross-sectional Survey on COVID-19. J Med Internet Res. 2020; 22:e18790. https://doi.
org/10.2196/18790 PMID: 32240094
28. Lau JTF, Yang X, Tsui H, Pang E, Kim JH. SARS preventive and risk behaviours of Hong Kong air trav-
ellers. Epidemiology and infection. 2004; 132:727–36. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0950268804002225
PMID: 15310175.
29. Leask J. Leaders can still build our trust to fight the virus—here’s how. Sydney Morning Herald. 26
March 2020.
30. Schunk DH. Social cognitive theory and self-regulated learning In: Zimmerman BJ, Schunk DH, editors.
Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theoretical perspectives. Mahwah, NJ Erlbaum;
2001. p. 125–51
31. Marshall H, Ryan P, Roberton D, Street J, Watson M. Pandemic influenza and community prepared-
ness. American journal of public health. 2009; 99 Suppl 2(2):S365–S71. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.
2008.153056 PMID: 19797751.
32. Cowling BJ, Ng DM, Ip DK, Liao Q, Lam WW, Wu JT, et al. Community psychological and behavioral
responses through the first wave of the 2009 influenza A(H1N1) pandemic in Hong Kong. J Infect Dis.
2010; 202:867–76. https://doi.org/10.1086/655811 PMID: 20677945.
33. Eastwood K, Durrheim D, Francis JL, d’Espaignet ET, Duncan S, Islam F, et al. Knowledge about pan-
demic influenza and compliance with containment measures among Australians. Bulletin of the World
Health Organization. 2009; 87:588–94. https://doi.org/10.2471/blt.08.060772 PMID: 19705008.
34. Bish A, Michie S. Demographic and attitudinal determinants of protective behaviours during a pan-
demic: A review. British journal of health psychology. 2010; 15:797–824. https://doi.org/10.1348/
135910710X485826 PMID: 20109274
35. Brekke KA, Kverndokk S, Nyborg K. An economic model of moral motivation. Journal of Public Econom-
ics. 2003; 87:1967–83.
36. Bardsley N, Sausgruber R. Conformity and reciprocity in public good provision. Journal of Economic
Psychology. 2005; 26:664–81.
37. Brewer NT, Chapman GB, Rothman AJ, Leask J, Kempe A. Increasing Vaccination: Putting Psychologi-
cal Science Into Action. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 2017; 18:149–207. https://doi.org/
10.1177/1529100618760521 PMID: 29611455
38. Pfattheicher S, Nockur L, Bo
¨hm R, Sassenrath C, Petersen M. The emotional path to action: Empathy
promotes physical distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic. 2020
39. Grow JM, Christopher SA. Breaking the Silence Surrounding Hepatitis C by Promoting Self-Efficacy:
Hepatitis C Public Service Announcements. Qualitative Health Research. 2008; 18:1401–12. https://
doi.org/10.1177/1049732308322603 PMID: 18832769
40. Pereira A, Leite I, Mascarenhas S, Martinho C, Paiva A, editors. Using Empathy to Improve Human-
Robot Relationships. Human-Robot Personal Relationships; 2011; Berlin, Heidelberg.
41. Tang CS, Wong CY. An outbreak of the severe acute respiratory syndrome: predictors of health behav-
iors and effect of community prevention measures in Hong Kong, China. Am J Public Health. 2003;
93:1887–8. https://doi.org/10.2105/ajph.93.11.1887 PMID: 14600058; PubMed Central PMCID:
PMC1448068.
PLOS ONE
Examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235112 June 23, 2020 13 / 13
... However, it is still dangerous, and it significantly affects social behaviour. Changes in social behaviour are being studied in many countries, including Australia, France, and Belgium, among others [8]. The risk perceived by the public, regardless of the area, is a subjective assessment made by people in relation to their knowledge and experiences and interacting stimuli in their environment [9]. ...
... Studies in many countries have shown that changing habits in Australia, for example, regarding more frequent hand washing, may have significantly impacted higher water consumption. The use of appropriate washing products was considered to reduce the personal risk of contracting COVID-19 [8]. Increased household water consumption was observed in southern Brazil. ...
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic has seriously affected many world regions' societies and economies. The pandemic and the restrictions introduced in response to it have impacted social behaviour and the operation of businesses in various ways. The present study aimed to verify the impact of information on the COVID-19 outbreak in Poland on the frequency with which residents of Rybnik and the surrounding areas chose to stay in their houses or apartments, as well as the functioning of businesses. The originality of the study lies in the use of mediation modelling, in which the following were used as variables describing the epidemic in Poland: new cases of SARS-CoV-2 infection in Poland; the number of deaths caused by SARS-CoV-2 infection in Poland; and the number of people quarantined due to the COVID-19 epidemic in Poland. On the other hand, data on tap water consumption were used as variables describing the frequency with which residents of Rybnik and the surrounding areas chose to stay in their houses or apartments, the operation of businesses in and around the city, and the operation of government and local government institutions. The study's conclusions confirm that publicly available information about the COVID-19 outbreak in Poland affected the frequency of residents of Rybnik and the surrounding areas staying in their houses or apartments as well as the functioning of businesses in Rybnik and the surrounding areas. The increasing number of people who died due to SARS-CoV-2 infection was associated with the limited functioning of government and local government institutions in and around Rybnik, which contributed to restrictions on the functioning of service industry companies in the city and surrounding areas.
... While numerous studies have investigated attitudes and perceptions surrounding COVID-19 vaccination in adults (11)(12)(13)(14)(15)(16)(17), fewer studies have examined parental considerations for COVID-19 vaccination of children in the setting of recent authorization of a pediatric vaccine (3,5,6,(18)(19)(20)(21)(22)(23)(24)(25)(26). To our knowledge, one prior study has explored vaccine willingness and hesitancy in the context of pediatric cancer, targeting the views of U.S.-based caregivers of childhood cancer survivors; within this cohort, 29% of caregivers expressed vaccine hesitancy, and confidence in COVID-19 vaccination and its value for childhood cancer survivors emerged as a prominent theme (27). ...
Article
Full-text available
SARS-CoV-2 has led to an unprecedented pandemic where vulnerable populations, such as those with childhood cancer, face increased risk of morbidity and mortality. COVID-19 vaccines are a critical intervention to control the pandemic and ensure patient safety. This study explores global caregiver's perspectives related to COVID-19 immunization in the context of pediatric cancer management. A mixed methods survey was developed based on consensus questions with iterative feedback from global medical professional and caregiver groups and distributed globally to caregivers of childhood cancer via electronic and paper routes. We present qualitative findings through inductive content analysis of caregiver free-text responses. A total of 184 participants provided qualitative responses, 29.3% of total survey respondents, with a total of 271 codes applied. Codes focused on themes related to safety and effectiveness (n = 95, 35.1%), logistics (n = 69, 25.5%), statements supporting or opposing vaccination (n = 55, 20.3%), and statements discussing the limited availability of information (n = 31, 11.4%). Within the theme of safety and effectiveness, safety itself was the most commonly used code (n = 66, 24.4% of total segments and 69.5% of safety and effectiveness codes), followed by risks versus benefits (n = 18, 18.9% of safety and effectiveness codes) and efficacy (n = 11, 11.6%). This study provides insights to guide healthcare professionals and caregiver peers in supporting families during the complex decision-making process for COVID-19 vaccination. These findings highlight the multidimensionality of concerns and considerations of caregivers of children with cancer regarding COVID-19 vaccination and suggest that certain perspectives transcend borders and cultures.
... This finding is in line with the study conducted in Pakistan found that people carry hand sanitiser during the pandemic around 56.8% [6]. A study in Australia found that people fit in in one or more behaviour-related hygiene recommended by their government [30]. As far as we know, our respondents are not too familiar with hand sanitiser because water and soap are easily found in their surroundings. ...
... An Australian study, also undertaken during the early phase, found that even when level of risk was perceived as low, adopting avoidance behaviors such as cancelling social events or reducing ones' use of public transport, was associated high perceived rating of effectiveness of behaviors and high level of perceived ability to adopt social distancing strategies (Seale et al., 2020). This is highly relevant, since this indicates the potential of strengthening efficacy beliefs in the general population, not least in groups who perceive low susceptibility and severity of disease. ...
Article
Full-text available
To minimize the spread of Covid-19, changing every-day behavior has been key. Trust in the effectiveness of individual protective measures (response efficacy) and confidence in collective safeguarding measures (strategy efficacy), offers an incitement for acting adequately. Efficacy beliefs of protective measures might be especially relevant to study in the Swedish context, since Sweden, in contrast to countries facing hard lock-downs, launched safeguarding measures based on individual responsibility and voluntary actions. We aimed to assess associations between on the one hand, response efficacy and strategy efficacy, and on the other hand, propensity for behavior change and support of protective measures. Furthermore, to assess associations between the efficacy beliefs and comprehension of and confidence in information about the virus, prosocial beliefs and worry of Covid-19. Reactions were assessed in a Swedish sample close in time to experiences via the SEMA3 app from March 25th to May 17th 2020. Study participants had replied to questions on strategy efficacy (n = 175) or response efficacy (n = 157) and 146 participants had replied to both. High response efficacy was associated with propensity for behavior change, support of protective measures and confidence in Covid-19 information. Low strategy efficacy was associated with lower comprehension of and confidence in information about Covid-19. The results suggest that strengthening efficacy beliefs can be a way to promote protective behaviors. Furthermore, the result underscores the importance of information being easy to understand and trustworthy. Finding ways to increase public understanding of the effectiveness of protective measures, including vaccination, seems crucial in pandemic times.
... The instrument was a three-part questionnaire with 35 items constructed by the research team. To develop the questionnaire, similar studies [58,59] and the WHO guideline [60] were reviewed as well as the instructions provided on Iran's Ministry of health (MOH) homepage [61]. The first part of the questionnaire included nine items exploring demographic variables such as age, sex, educational level, and one's own or a family member's history of COVID-19 infection. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Electronic health (e-health) technologies play an important role in improving public knowledge and behavior to control the COVID-19 pandemic. The present study aimed to investigate the role of e-health on the public knowledge and behavior in preventing COVID-19 in Kerman, a city in Iran. Methods: The present descriptive cross-sectional study used an online survey in Kerman in November 2021. The research instrument was a tripartite questionnaire that included demographic information, the level of respondents' knowledge about COVID-19, the extent of change in respondents' behavior influenced by e-health. Negative binomial regression analysis was run to test the relationship between individual characteristics and the research variables with knowledge and behavior. Spearman correlation test was used to measure the correlation between the two main parts of the survey. Results: As the negative binomial regression analysis results showed, the relationship between no demographic variable and the public knowledge and behavior was statistically significant (p < 0.001). The mean knowledge and behavior scores were 5.84 ± 2.55 and 11.95 ± 5.09, respectively, showing that people who used e-health had a high level of knowledge, and taking preventive measures was at a high level. The Spearman correlation coefficient test results showed a positive association between knowledge and behavior (r = 0.71). Conclusion: E-health played a decisive role in increasing knowledge and improving behavior in preventing COVID-19. The results of our research can encourage the use of e-health to improve the public knowledge and behavior in pandemics such as COVID-19.
... m between people, staying at home and postponing or cancelling gatherings), and environmental protection measures (e.g., routine surface disinfection). In some situations, these actions are voluntary, while in others, they are enforced by law (Seale et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
The article recounts the changes in the security perception of Poles after the outbreak of the coronavirus (SARS-CoV-2) pandemic. Upon utilisation of comparative empirical research carried out in 2020 and 2021, using the diagnostic survey method with the survey techniques, the conclusion confirmed that Poles’ sense of security decreased in 2020 and 2021. According to the respondent’s answers, the most frequent explanation for declining security perception coincides with the threat of contracting the coronavirus. Those include losing a job due to the restrictions introduced in economic activity, determined by the appearance of the coronavirus, and the introduction of restrictions on movement in connection with the occurrences of COVID-19. In addition, the study found that the respondents’ most frequently chosen methods of improving their sense of security include using personal protective equipment, avoiding contact with other people, and refraining from leaving their homes.
... Considering as a starting point the scientific literature reviewed, shows that 52.34% of schoolchildren from this sample present high and very high levels of social anxiety, a very significant data which provides information on factors such as speaking in public or interacting with teachers; interacting with the opposite sex; being in evidence or ridicule; assertive expression of annoyance or anger; interacting with strangers or acting in public. All these aspects have been impaired by the pandemic situation, which coincides with several studies reviewed in which the deterioration of emotional stability in the population worldwide is evident, adding to the factors that impact the physical and mental health of people after the time lived in lockdown [27][28][29]. Some studies suggest the inclusion in the curriculum of emotional education to address and counteract social anxiety in order to provide children, youth, parents and teachers with skills, attitudes and behaviors necessary to stay healthy and positive, explore their emotions, practice mindful engagement, exhibit prosocial behavior and deal with daily challenges [30,31]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of the present study was to determine the perception of schoolchildren whether their academic performance improved or worsened during the pandemic, analyzing their social anxiety, gender, use of masks in the classroom, and school year. The total sample was 107 primary school students (25 in the fourth, 40 in the fifth and 42 in the sixth grade), with a mean age of 10.51 years old (SD = 1). The gender were 58 girls and 49 boys, from a school in the province of La Coruña (Spain). The study was based on a quantitative methodology , and the design was cross-sectional, descriptive, observational and correlational. The social anxiety questionnaire (CASO-N24) was used to assess social anxiety, and an ad hoc self-report register was elaborated to evaluate sociodemographic variables. The results indicated that 44.8% of the schoolchildren considered that the pandemic had neither improved nor worsened their academic performance. Although 38.3% considered that high and very high social anxiety increased progressively as the school year progressed, both in boys and girls. Besides, the schoolchildren who presented very low and low social anxiety improved their grades in Physical Education, while those who presented high social anxiety worsened them. In conclusion, having a low social anxiety, lower grades before the pandemic and higher grades after, makes children perceive an improvement in their academic performance during the pandemic.
Article
Background: There are no psychometrically validated measures of willingness to engage in public health screening and prevention efforts, particularly mHealth-based tracking, that can be adapted to future crises post-COVID-19. Objective: We tested the psychometric properties of a novel measure of willingness to participate in pandemic-related screening and tracking, including willingness to use pandemic-related mHealth tools. Methods: Data were from a cross-sectional, national probability survey deployed in three cross-sectional stages several weeks apart to adult residents of the United States (N1=2,190, N2=2,238, and N3=2,047) from the AmeriSpeak probability-based research panel covering approximately 97% of the US household population. Five items asked about willingness to use mHealth tools for COVID-related screening and tracking and provide biological specimens for COVID-19 testing. Results: In the first, exploratory sample, three of five items loaded onto one underlying factor, willingness to use pandemic-related mHealth tools, based on exploratory factor analysis. A two-factor solution, including the three-item factor, fit the data (RMSEA=0.038, CFI=1.000, SRMR=0.005), and the factor loadings for the three items ranged from 0.849 to 0.893. In the second, validation sample, the reliability of the three-item measure was high (Cronbach's alpha=0.90) and one underlying factor for the three items was confirmed using confirmatory factor analysis (RMSEA=0.000, CFI=1.000, SRMR=0.000 [a saturated model]; factor loadings ranged from 1.000 to 0.962). The factor was independently associated with COVID-19-preventive behaviors (e.g., "worn a face mask": r=0.313, SE=0.041, p<.001; "kept six feet distance from those outside my household": r=0.282, SE=0.050, p<.001) and willingness to provide biological specimens for COVID-19 testing (i.e., swab to cheek or nose: r=0.709, SE=0.017, p<.001; small blood draw: r=0.684, SE=0.019, p<.001). In the third, multiple-group sample, the measure was invariant, or measured the same thing in the same way (i.e., difference in CFI (ΔCFI)<0.010 across all grouping categories), across age groups, gender, racial/ethnic groups, education levels, United States geographic region, and population density (i.e., rural, suburban, urban). When repeated across different samples, factor-analytic findings were essentially the same. Additionally, there were mean differences (ΔM) in willingness to use mHealth tools across samples, mainly based on race or ethnicity and population density. For example, in standard-deviation units, suburban (ΔM=-0.30, SE=0.13, p=.001) and urban (ΔM=-0.42, SE=0.12, p<.001) adults showed less willingness to use mHealth tools than rural adults in the third sample collected May 30-June 8, 2020, but no differences were detected in the first sample collected April 20-26, 2020. Conclusions: Findings showed that the screener is psychometrically valid. It can also be adapted to future public health crises. Racial- and ethnic-minority adults showed greater willingness to use mHealth tools than White adults. Rural adults showed more mHealth willingness than suburban and urban adults. Findings have implications for public health screening and tracking and understanding digital health inequities, including lack of uptake.
Article
Full-text available
In an effort to mitigate the outbreak of COVID-19, many countries have imposed drastic lockdown, movement control or shelter in place orders on their residents. The effectiveness of these mitigation measures is highly dependent on cooperation and compliance of all members of society. The knowledge, attitudes and practices people hold toward the disease play an integral role in determining a society’s readiness to accept behavioural change measures from health authorities. The aim of this study was to determine the knowledge levels, attitudes and practices toward COVID-19 among the Malaysian public. A cross-sectional online survey of 4,850 Malaysian residents was conducted between 27th March and 3rd April 2020. The survey instrument consisted of demographic characteristics, 13 items on knowledge, 3 items on attitudes and 3 items on practices, modified from a previously published questionnaire on COVID-19. Descriptive statistics, chi-square tests, t-tests and one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) were conducted. The overall correct rate of the knowledge questionnaire was 80.5%. Most participants held positive attitudes toward the successful control of COVID-19 (83.1%), the ability of Malaysia to conquer the disease (95.9%) and the way the Malaysian government was handling the crisis (89.9%). Most participants were also taking precautions such as avoiding crowds (83.4%) and practising proper hand hygiene (87.8%) in the week before the movement control order started. However, the wearing of face masks was less common (51.2%). This survey is among the first to assess knowledge, attitudes and practice in response to the COVID-19 pandemic in Malaysia. The results highlight the importance of consistent messaging from health authorities and the government as well as the need for tailored health education programs to improve levels of knowledge, attitudes and practices.
Preprint
Full-text available
Widespread and sustained engagement with health-protective behaviours (i.e., hygiene and distancing) is critical to successfully managing the COVID-19 pandemic. Evidence from previous emerging infectious disease outbreaks points to the role of perceived risk, worry, media coverage, and knowledge in shaping engagement with health-protective behaviours as well as vaccination intentions. The current study examined these factors in 2,174 Australian residents. An online survey was completed between 2-9 March 2020, at an early stage of the COVID-19 outbreak in Australia. Results revealed that two thirds of respondents were at least moderately worried about a widespread COVID-19 outbreak in Australia (which subsequently occurred). Worry about the outbreak and closely following media coverage were consistent predictors of health-protective behaviours (both over the previous month, and intended behaviours in the case of a widespread outbreak) as well as vaccination intentions. Health-behaviour engagement over the previous month was lower in some demographic groups, including males and younger individuals (18-29 age group). These was a substantial mismatch between respondents' expected symptoms of infection and emerging evidence that a meaningful proportion of people who contract the novel coronavirus will experience asymptomatic infection (i.e., they will not experience symptoms associated with COVID-19). Only 0.3% of those in the current study believed that they personally would not experience any symptoms if they were infected. Uncertainty and misconceptions about COVID-19 were common, including one third of respondents who reported being unsure whether people are likely have natural or existing immunity. There was also uncertainty around whether specific home remedies (e.g., vitamins, saline rinses) would offer protection, whether the virus could spread via the airborne route, and whether the virus was human made and deliberately released. Such misconceptions are likely to cause concern for members of the public. These results point to areas of uncertainty that could be usefully targeted by public education campaigns, as well as psychological and demographic factors associated with engagement with health-protective behaviours. These findings offer potential pathways for interventions to encourage health-protective behaviours to reduce the spread of COVID-19.
Article
Full-text available
The Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) outbreak is spreading globally. Although COVID-19 has now been declared a pandemic and risk for infection in the United States (US) is currently high, at the time of survey administration the risk of infection in the US was low. It is important to understand the public perception of risk and trust in sources of information to better inform public health messaging. In this study, we surveyed the adult US population to understand their risk perceptions about the COVID-19 outbreak. We used an online platform to survey 718 adults in the US in early February 2020 using a questionnaire that we developed. Our sample was fairly similar to the general adult US population in terms of age, gender, race, ethnicity and education. We found that 69% of the respondents wanted the scientific/public health leadership (either the CDC Director or NIH Director) to lead the US response to COVID-19 outbreak as compared to 14% who wanted the political leadership (either the president or Congress) to lead the response. Risk perception was low (median score of 5 out of 10) with the respondents trusting health professionals and health officials for information on COVID-19. The majority of respondents were in favor of strict infection prevention policies to control the outbreak. Given our results, the public health/scientific leadership should be at the forefront of the COVID-19 response to promote trust.
Article
Full-text available
Since the first suspected case of coronavirus disease-2019 (COVID-19) on December 1st, 2019, in Wuhan, Hubei Province, China, a total of 40,235 confirmed cases and 909 deaths have been reported in China up to February 10, 2020, evoking fear locally and internationally. Here, based on the publicly available epidemiological data for Hubei, China from January 11 to February 10, 2020, we provide estimates of the main epidemiological parameters. In particular, we provide an estimation of the case fatality and case recovery ratios, along with their 90% confidence intervals as the outbreak evolves. On the basis of a Susceptible-Infectious-Recovered-Dead (SIDR) model, we provide estimations of the basic reproduction number (R0), and the per day infection mortality and recovery rates. By calibrating the parameters of the SIRD model to the reported data, we also attempt to forecast the evolution of the outbreak at the epicenter three weeks ahead, i.e. until February 29. As the number of infected individuals, especially of those with asymptomatic or mild courses, is suspected to be much higher than the official numbers, which can be considered only as a subset of the actual numbers of infected and recovered cases in the total population, we have repeated the calculations under a second scenario that considers twenty times the number of confirmed infected cases and forty times the number of recovered, leaving the number of deaths unchanged. Based on the reported data, the expected value of R0 as computed considering the period from the 11th of January until the 18th of January, using the official counts of confirmed cases was found to be ∼4.6, while the one computed under the second scenario was found to be ∼3.2. Thus, based on the SIRD simulations, the estimated average value of R0 was found to be ∼2.6 based on confirmed cases and ∼2 based on the second scenario. Our forecasting flashes a note of caution for the presently unfolding outbreak in China. Based on the official counts for confirmed cases, the simulations suggest that the cumulative number of infected could reach 180,000 (with a lower bound of 45,000) by February 29. Regarding the number of deaths, simulations forecast that on the basis of the up to the 10th of February reported data, the death toll might exceed 2,700 (as a lower bound) by February 29. Our analysis further reveals a significant decline of the case fatality ratio from January 26 to which various factors may have contributed, such as the severe control measures taken in Hubei, China (e.g. quarantine and hospitalization of infected individuals), but mainly because of the fact that the actual cumulative numbers of infected and recovered cases in the population most likely are much higher than the reported ones. Thus, in a scenario where we have taken twenty times the confirmed number of infected and forty times the confirmed number of recovered cases, the case fatality ratio is around ∼0.15% in the total population. Importantly, based on this scenario, simulations suggest a slow down of the outbreak in Hubei at the end of February.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Given the extensive time needed to conduct a nationally representative household survey and the commonly low response rate of phone surveys, rapid online surveys may be a promising method to assess and track knowledge and perceptions among the general public during fast-moving infectious disease outbreaks. Objective: This study aimed to apply rapid online surveying to determine knowledge and perceptions of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) among the general public in the United States and the United Kingdom. Methods: An online questionnaire was administered to 3000 adults residing in the United States and 3000 adults residing in the United Kingdom who had registered with Prolific Academic to participate in online research. Prolific Academic established strata by age (18-27, 28-37, 38-47, 48-57, or ≥58 years), sex (male or female), and ethnicity (white, black or African American, Asian or Asian Indian, mixed, or "other"), as well as all permutations of these strata. The number of participants who could enroll in each of these strata was calculated to reflect the distribution in the US and UK general population. Enrollment into the survey within each stratum was on a first-come, first-served basis. Participants completed the questionnaire between February 23 and March 2, 2020. Results: A total of 2986 and 2988 adults residing in the United States and the United Kingdom, respectively, completed the questionnaire. Of those, 64.4% (1924/2986) of US participants and 51.5% (1540/2988) of UK participants had a tertiary education degree, 67.5% (2015/2986) of US participants had a total household income between US $20,000 and US $99,999, and 74.4% (2223/2988) of UK participants had a total household income between £15,000 and £74,999. US and UK participants' median estimate for the probability of a fatal disease course among those infected with severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) was 5.0% (IQR 2.0%-15.0%) and 3.0% (IQR 2.0%-10.0%), respectively. Participants generally had good knowledge of the main mode of disease transmission and common symptoms of COVID-19. However, a substantial proportion of participants had misconceptions about how to prevent an infection and the recommended care-seeking behavior. For instance, 37.8% (95% CI 36.1%-39.6%) of US participants and 29.7% (95% CI 28.1%-31.4%) of UK participants thought that wearing a common surgical mask was "highly effective" in protecting them from acquiring COVID-19, and 25.6% (95% CI 24.1%-27.2%) of US participants and 29.6% (95% CI 28.0%-31.3%) of UK participants thought it was prudent to refrain from eating at Chinese restaurants. Around half (53.8%, 95% CI 52.1%-55.6%) of US participants and 39.1% (95% CI 37.4%-40.9%) of UK participants thought that children were at an especially high risk of death when infected with SARS-CoV-2. Conclusions: The distribution of participants by total household income and education followed approximately that of the US and UK general population. The findings from this online survey could guide information campaigns by public health authorities, clinicians, and the media. More broadly, rapid online surveys could be an important tool in tracking the public's knowledge and misperceptions during rapidly moving infectious disease outbreaks.
Preprint
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic presents a major challenge to societies all over the globe. To curb the spread of the disease, two measures implemented in many countries are minimizing close contact between people (“physical distancing”) and wearing face masks. In the present research, we tested the idea that physical distancing and wearing face masks can be the result of a genuine prosocial emotion—empathy for those most vulnerable to the virus. In four pre-registered studies (total N = 3,718‬, Western population), we show that (i) empathy is indeed a basic motivation for physical distancing and wearing face masks, and (ii) inducing empathy for those most vulnerable to the virus promotes the motivation to adhere to these measures (whereas providing mere information about its importance is not). In sum, the present research provides a better understanding of the promoting factors underlying the willingness to follow two important measures during the COVID-19 pandemic.
Article
Full-text available
Governments will not be able to minimise both deaths from coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) and the economic impact of viral spread. Keeping mortality as low as possible will be the highest priority for individuals; hence governments must put in place measures to ameliorate the inevitable economic downturn. In our view, COVID-19 has developed into a pandemic, with small chains of transmission in many countries and large chains resulting in extensive spread in a few countries, such as Italy, Iran, South Korea, and Japan.1 Most countries are likely to have spread of COVID-19, at least in the early stages, before any mitigation measures have an impact.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Since December 2019, when coronavirus disease 2019 (Covid-19) emerged in Wuhan city and rapidly spread throughout China, data have been needed on the clinical characteristics of the affected patients. Methods: We extracted data regarding 1099 patients with laboratory-confirmed Covid-19 from 552 hospitals in 30 provinces, autonomous regions, and municipalities in China through January 29, 2020. The primary composite end point was admission to an intensive care unit (ICU), the use of mechanical ventilation, or death. Results: The median age of the patients was 47 years; 41.9% of the patients were female. The primary composite end point occurred in 67 patients (6.1%), including 5.0% who were admitted to the ICU, 2.3% who underwent invasive mechanical ventilation, and 1.4% who died. Only 1.9% of the patients had a history of direct contact with wildlife. Among nonresidents of Wuhan, 72.3% had contact with residents of Wuhan, including 31.3% who had visited the city. The most common symptoms were fever (43.8% on admission and 88.7% during hospitalization) and cough (67.8%). Diarrhea was uncommon (3.8%). The median incubation period was 4 days (interquartile range, 2 to 7). On admission, ground-glass opacity was the most common radiologic finding on chest computed tomography (CT) (56.4%). No radiographic or CT abnormality was found in 157 of 877 patients (17.9%) with nonsevere disease and in 5 of 173 patients (2.9%) with severe disease. Lymphocytopenia was present in 83.2% of the patients on admission. Conclusions: During the first 2 months of the current outbreak, Covid-19 spread rapidly throughout China and caused varying degrees of illness. Patients often presented without fever, and many did not have abnormal radiologic findings. (Funded by the National Health Commission of China and others.).
Article
Full-text available
Vaccination is one of the great achievements of the 20th century, yet persistent public-health problems include inadequate, delayed, and unstable vaccination uptake. Psychology offers three general propositions for understanding and intervening to increase uptake where vaccines are available and affordable. The first proposition is that thoughts and feelings can motivate getting vaccinated. Hundreds of studies have shown that risk beliefs and anticipated regret about infectious disease correlate reliably with getting vaccinated; low confidence in vaccine effectiveness and concern about safety correlate reliably with not getting vaccinated. We were surprised to find that few randomized trials have successfully changed what people think and feel about vaccines, and those few that succeeded were minimally effective in increasing uptake. The second proposition is that social processes can motivate getting vaccinated. Substantial research has shown that social norms are associated with vaccination, but few interventions examined whether normative messages increase vaccination uptake. Many experimental studies have relied on hypothetical scenarios to demonstrate that altruism and free riding (i.e., taking advantage of the protection provided by others) can affect intended behavior, but few randomized trials have tested strategies to change social processes to increase vaccination uptake. The third proposition is that interventions can facilitate vaccination directly by leveraging, but not trying to change, what people think and feel. These interventions are by far the most plentiful and effective in the literature. To increase vaccine uptake, these interventions build on existing favorable intentions by facilitating action (through reminders, prompts, and primes) and reducing barriers (through logistics and healthy defaults); these interventions also shape behavior (through incentives, sanctions, and requirements). Although identification of principles for changing thoughts and feelings to motivate vaccination is a work in progress, psychological principles can now inform the design of systems and policies to directly facilitate action.
Article
In the current absence of medical treatment and vaccination, the unfolding COVID-19 pandemic can only be brought under control by massive and rapid behaviour change. To achieve this we need to systematically monitor and understand how different individuals perceive risk and what prompts them to act upon it, argues Cornelia Betsch.