ArticlePublisher preview available

Does exposure to facial composites damage eyewitness memory? A comprehensive review

Wiley
Applied Cognitive Psychology
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract and Figures

Eyewitnesses often create face likenesses, which are published in the hope that potential suspects will be reported to the police. Witnesses exposed to another witness's composite, however, may be positively or negatively influenced by such composites. A good likeness may facilitate identification, but a bad likeness that resembles an innocent suspect may lead to a misidentification (“mix‐up”). We offer a theoretical review, and comprehensively summarize extant studies descriptively because most studies did not report enough statistical details to warrant a formal meta‐analysis. Some studies showed negative exposure effects, particularly when the innocent suspect and composite shared misleading features. Studies that exposed witnesses to “good” composites reported positive or no effects on lineup performance, and some highly powered studies also showed no effect. We outline suggestions for further investigations under ecologically valid conditions. We also make recommendations for investigative practice, and the evaluation of identification evidence by fact finders or courts.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Does exposure to facial composites damage eyewitness
memory? A comprehensive review
Siegfried L. Sporer
1
| Colin G. Tredoux
2,3
| Annelies Vredeveldt
4
|
Kate Kempen
2
| Alicia Nortje
2
1
Department of Psychology and Sports
Science, University of Giessen, Giessen,
Germany
2
Department of Psychology, University of
Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
3
CLLE, Université de Toulouse, CNRS, UT2J,
Toulouse, France
4
Department of Criminal Law and Criminology,
Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The
Netherlands
Correspondence
Siegfried L. Sporer, Department of Psychology,
University of Giessen, Giessen, Germany.
Email: siegfried.l.sporer@psy.jlug.de
Funding information
Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft
(Sp272/9-1)
Summary
Eyewitnesses often create face likenesses, which are published in the hope that
potential suspects will be reported to the police. Witnesses exposed to another
witness's composite, however, may be positively or negatively influenced by such
composites. A good likeness may facilitate identification, but a bad likeness that
resembles an innocent suspect may lead to a misidentification (mix-up). We offer a
theoretical review, and comprehensively summarize extant studies descriptively
because most studies did not report enough statistical details to warrant a formal
meta-analysis. Some studies showed negative exposure effects, particularly when the
innocent suspect and composite shared misleading features. Studies that exposed
witnesses to goodcomposites reported positive or no effects on lineup perfor-
mance, and some highly powered studies also showed no effect. We outline sugges-
tions for further investigations under ecologically valid conditions. We also make
recommendations for investigative practice, and the evaluation of identification evi-
dence by fact finders or courts.
KEYWORDS
eyewitness identification, eyewitness recall, face composites, misinformation effect
1|INTRODUCTION
Mistaken eyewitness identifications can have a major impact on crimi-
nal cases, and there is plenty of evidence that mistaken identifications
have been involved in wrongful convictions discovered through DNA
testing (Garrett, 2011; www.innocenceproject.org). One potential fac-
tor contributing to these misidentifications may be the production of
face composites as part of the police investigation. In cases in which
the identity of the perpetrator is at issue, the police may rely on eye-
witnesses to help produce a likeness (or composite) of the perpetra-
tor's facial appearance. By publishing the composite, police
investigators hope that a member of the public will recognize the per-
son depicted and report this to the police (Davies & Valentine, 2007;
Shepherd & Ellis, 1996). Face composites have been used by the
police across the globe for many decades, from the US to Europe and
Australia and many other countries, including Brazil, South Africa and
in former socialist and communist countries (Saraiva et al., 2018;
Schmidt & Tredoux, 2006; Shepherd & Ellis, 1996; Snetkow, Sinin, &
Delang, 1981).
Although face composites are commonly used by police, labora-
tory research indicates that composites often do not resemble the
perpetrator (Davies & Valentine, 2007; Frowd et al., 2005; Frowd
et al., 2005; Koehn & Fisher, 1997; Kovera, Penrod, Pappas, & Thill,
1997). However, Frowd, Valentine, and Davis (2015) summarize data
on new generation composite systems
1
and provide evidence that
resemblance between composites and targets may be improving.
Newer interviewing techniques developed specifically for face com-
posite construction may further improve composite accuracy (Skelton
et al., 2019; Fodarella et al., 2017; Fodarella, Kuivaniemi-Smith,
Gawrylowicz, & Frowd, 2015).
A facial composite constructed by a witness can affect later iden-
tification in at least two ways. First, the task of constructing a
Received: 18 December 2019 Revised: 3 June 2020 Accepted: 5 June 2020
DOI: 10.1002/acp.3705
1166 © 2020 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl Cognit Psychol. 2020;34:11661179.wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/acp
... Research has shown that eyewitness identification accuracy can be influenced by misleading post event information, including misleading face descriptions (Loftus and Greene, 1980) and composites (Topp-Manriquez et al., 2014;Sporer et al., 2020). The mechanism behind the incorporation of MI into the witness's memory for original event has been widely researched and the subject of numerous debates. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction This study investigated the effects of face angle congruency across stages of a misinformation paradigm on lineup discrimination accuracy. Methods In a between-subjects design, participants viewed a mock crime with the perpetrator’s face from the front or profile angle. They then read a news report featuring an innocent suspect’s image from the same or different angle as the perpetrator had been shown. A subsequent lineup manipulated perpetrator presence and viewing angle of the lineup members, who were all shown either from the front or in profile. Results No significant difference emerged in identification errors based on angle congruency between stages. However, accuracy was higher when faces were shown from the front angle, both during the initial event and the lineup, compared to the profile angle. Discussion The results of this research underscore the importance of considering viewing angles in the construction of lineups.
... In conclusion, the findings of the narrative analysis (Sporer et al. 2020) and meta-analysis (Tredoux et al. 2021) are not in line with the strong claims published in news outlets and practitioner guidelines, which suggest that viewing or creating facial composites impairs memory. Based on the current body of scientific evidence, we are not yet able to formulate sound and empirically based policy recommendations about the ways in which viewing or creating a facial composite may impact memory. ...
Chapter
Law enforcement agencies often rely on practical technologies to help witnesses and victims of crimes construct likenesses of faces from memory. These ‘face composites’ are typically circulated to law enforcement officers and made accessible to the public in the hope that someone familiar with the depicted person will recognise their likeness and thus provide the police with a suspect. We will review methods for constructing such likenesses from memory dating back to the portrait parlé of Alphonse Bertillon (Signaletic instructions including the theory and practice of anthropometrical identification. Werner Company, 1896) and the composite images of Francis Galton (Nature 18:97-100, 1878). We will also review more modern methods, ranging from the overlay techniques of Identi-Kit (McDonald, c 1959) and Photo-Fit (Penry J. The Police Journal 43:307, 1970) to feature-based computerised composite systems such as Identi-Kit 2000, FACES, and ProMat. Most early systems were based on the common-sense notion that sectioning a face is invertible: just as a face can be sectioned into components, so it can be recreated by arrangements of sections. This assumption appears to be unwarranted. The underlying problem with earlier face systems may have been the absence of a representational or computational theory. This led in the late 1990s to the development of the so-called third-generation holistic composite systems, which are based on underlying statistical and mathematical models of face images (e.g. ID [Tredoux et al. South African Computer Journal 2006:90–97, 2006], EvoFIT [Frowd CD, Hancock PJB, & Carson D. ACM Transactions on Applied Psychology (TAP) 1:1-21, 2004a], E-FIT [Gibson et al., International Conference on Visualisation, 146–151, 2003]). A special focus of the chapter will be on these newer technologies and other recent technological innovations. Our approach will be to review (i) the methods of operation, (ii) the techniques identified by psychologists and other researchers for improving the quality of information obtained from memory, and (iii) the empirical data on the effectiveness of these systems at representing faces from memory. We will consider related issues, too, including the question of whether face composites damage witness memory, and the ethics of face composition.KeywordsMemoryEyewitnessFace compositeFace likenessSynthetic face
... Whereas the literature on composite construction is reasonably homogenous methodologically, making a meta-analysis feasible, the literature on composite exposure is heterogenous, and it is infeasible to conduct a metaanalysis on it. We are preparing a narrative review of the composite exposure literature, which could be considered a companion article to the present review (Sporer et al. 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives We conducted a meta-analysis to assess whether the construction of facial composites affects witnesses’ lineup identification decisions.Methods We located 23 studies (56 effects, 2276 participants). We consider effects of constructing composites on (a) correct identifications, and (b) incorrect identifications, from target-present lineups, and (c) incorrect identifications from target-absent lineups. Log odds ratio effect sizes were entered into a random-effects meta-analysis. We also present novel signal detection theory analyses in an online supplement.ResultsThere were no significant negative effects of composite construction, but some weak evidence that composite construction reduced incorrect identifications in target-present lineups. Because effect sizes showed little hetereogeneity for any of the outcomes (after outlier removal), there were no moderator analyses. Results for SDT measures also showed no effects.Conclusions Empirical evidence suggests no effects of composite construction on identifications. We identify gaps in knowledge and make recommendations for more ecologically valid research.
Article
Full-text available
When looking for a crime suspect, the police may ask an eyewitness to construct a visual likeness (‘facial composite’) of the perpetrator, to be distributed to the public via newspaper articles, television programmes, or social media. The dissemination of facial composites can have a major impact on police investigations. It often results in a deluge of tips and could potentially influence the memory of other eyewitnesses in the case. In this article, we review research on how to interview eyewitnesses for the optimal construction of facial composites from memory. We discuss types of composite systems and their effectiveness, including the ‘gold standard’ of measuring effectiveness. We compare the question posed to the public when a facial composite is disseminated to face-matching tasks faced by immigration officials and store clerks, but then with the added difficulty of the image being a composite of unknown resemblance to the target. We also discuss the potential danger of composites contaminating other eyewitnesses’ memory, highlighting the lessons learnt from research on unconscious transference. We pose several challenges for future researchers and practitioners. We conclude that evidence-based guidance is lacking to inform the police on whether and how to use facial composites in their investigations.
Article
Full-text available
Eyewitnesses to crimes sometimes search for a culprit on social media before viewing a police lineup, but it is not known whether this affects subsequent lineup identification accuracy. The present online study was conducted to address this. Two hundred and eighty-five participants viewed a mock crime video, and after a 15–20 min delay either (i) viewed a mock social media site including the culprit, (ii) viewed a mock social media site including a lookalike, or (iii) completed a filler task. A week later, participants made an identification from a photo lineup. It was predicted that searching for a culprit on social media containing the lookalike (rather than the culprit) would reduce lineup identification accuracy. There was a significant association between social media exposure and lineup accuracy for the Target Present lineup (30% more of the participants who saw the lookalike on social media failed to positively identify the culprit than participants in the other conditions), but for the Target Absent lineup (which also included the lookalike) there was no significant association with lineup identification accuracy. The results suggest that if an eyewitness sees a lookalike (where they are expecting to see the culprit) when conducting a self-directed search on social media, they are less likely to subsequently identify the culprit in the formal ID procedure.
Article
Full-text available
Four participants constructed face composites, of familiar and unfamiliar targets, using Pro-Fit, with reference images present or from memory. The “mean” of all 4 composites, created by morphing (4-morph) was rated as a better likeness than individual composites on average and was as good as the best individual likeness. When participants attempted to identify targets from line-ups, 4-morphs again performed as well as the best individual composite. In a second experiment, participants familiar with target women attempted to identify composites, and the trend showed better recognition from multiple composites, whether combined or shown together. In a line-up task with unfamiliar participants, 4-morphs produced most correct choices and fewest false positives from target-absent or target-present arrays. These results have practical implications for the way evidence from different witnesses is used in police investigations.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: The Executive Committee of the American Psychology-Law Society (Division 41 of the American Psychological Association) appointed a subcommittee to update the influential 1998 scientific review paper on guidelines for eyewitness identification procedures. Method: This was a collaborative effort by six senior eyewitness researchers, who all participated in the writing process. Feedback from members of AP-LS and the legal communities was solicited over an 18-month period. Results: The results yielded nine recommendations for planning, designing, and conducting eyewitness identification procedures. Four of the recommendations were from the 1998 article and concerned the selection of lineup fillers, prelineup instructions to witnesses, the use of double-blind procedures, and collection of a confidence statement. The additional five recommendations concern the need for law enforcement to conduct a prelineup interview of the witness, the need for evidence-based suspicion before conducting an identification procedure, video-recording of the entire procedure, avoiding repeated identification attempts with the same witness and same suspect, and avoiding the use of showups when possible and improving how showups are conducted when they are necessary. Conclusions: The reliability and integrity of eyewitness identification evidence is highly dependent on the procedures used by law enforcement for collecting and preserving the eyewitness evidence. These nine recommendations can advance the reliability and integrity of the evidence. Public Significance Statement Mistaken eyewitness identification is a primary contributor to criminal convictions of the innocent. Pristine procedures for collecting and documenting eyewitness identification evidence can help prevent these mistakes. This scientific review paper makes nine system variable recommendations concerning eyewitness identification procedures that should be implemented by crime investigators in eyewitness identification cases.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives We conducted a meta-analysis to assess whether the construction of facial composites affects witnesses’ lineup identification decisions.Methods We located 23 studies (56 effects, 2276 participants). We consider effects of constructing composites on (a) correct identifications, and (b) incorrect identifications, from target-present lineups, and (c) incorrect identifications from target-absent lineups. Log odds ratio effect sizes were entered into a random-effects meta-analysis. We also present novel signal detection theory analyses in an online supplement.ResultsThere were no significant negative effects of composite construction, but some weak evidence that composite construction reduced incorrect identifications in target-present lineups. Because effect sizes showed little hetereogeneity for any of the outcomes (after outlier removal), there were no moderator analyses. Results for SDT measures also showed no effects.Conclusions Empirical evidence suggests no effects of composite construction on identifications. We identify gaps in knowledge and make recommendations for more ecologically valid research.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: The Executive Committee of the American Psychology-Law Society (Division 41 of the American Psychological Association) appointed a subcommittee to update the influential 1998 scientific review paper on guidelines for eyewitness identification procedures. Method: This was a collaborative effort by six senior eyewitness researchers, who all participated in the writing process. Feedback from members of AP-LS and the legal communities was solicited over an 18-month period. Results: The results yielded nine recommendations for planning, designing, and conducting eyewitness identification procedures. Four of the recommendations were from the 1998 article and concerned the selection of lineup fillers, prelineup instructions to witnesses, the use of double-blind procedures, and collection of a confidence statement. The additional five recommendations concern the need for law enforcement to conduct a prelineup interview of the witness, the need for evidence-based suspicion before conducting an identification procedure, video-recording of the entire procedure, avoiding repeated identification attempts with the same witness and same suspect, and avoiding the use of showups when possible and improving how showups are conducted when they are necessary. Conclusions: The reliability and integrity of eyewitness identification evidence is highly dependent on the procedures used by law enforcement for collecting and preserving the eyewitness evidence. These nine recommendations can advance the reliability and integrity of the evidence.
Article
Full-text available
We investigated the impact of congruency between the witness interview and method used to construct a composite face. Experiment 1, using a typical feature-by-feature composite method, revealed that aligning cognitive processes during interview and face construction enhanced the effectiveness of composites compared with composites produced following unaligned (incongruent) procedures. Experiment 2 revealed that incorporating character judgments in the witness interview substantially enhanced identification of feature-based composites when constructing the central (internal) features first, suggesting that such judgments focus attention on this region of the face. Experiment 3 explored alignment of processes using an approach based on an evolutionary algorithm, a method requiring witnesses to create a composite by selecting from arrays based on the eye-region. A combination of character judgments, first for the whole face and then for the eye region, led to best-identified composites. Overall, results indicate that more effective composites are produced when both interview and construction procedures are aligned cognitively. Results are discussed with relevance to the theory of transfer-appropriate processing (Morris, Bransford, & Franks, 1977). (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Few studies have investigated eyewitnesses' ability to predict their later lineup performance, known as predecision confidence. We applied calibration analysis in two experiments comparing predecision confidence (immediately after encoding but prior to a lineup) to postdecision confidence (immediately after a lineup) to determine which produces a superior relationship with lineup decision accuracy. Experiment 1 (N = 177) featured a multiple‐block lineup recognition paradigm featuring several targets and lineups; Experiment 2 featured an eyewitness identification paradigm with a mock‐crime video and a single lineup for each participant (N = 855). Across both experiments, postdecision confidence discriminated well between correct and incorrect lineup decisions, but predecision confidence was a poor predictor of accuracy. Moreover, simply asking for predecision confidence weakened the postdecision confidence–accuracy relationship. This implies that police should exercise caution when interviewing eyewitnesses, as they should not be asked to predict their ability to make an accurate lineup decision.
Article
Full-text available
Problems associated with eyewitness identification decisions have long been highlighted by memory researchers (e.g., Loftus, 1979), with overwhelming evidence that witnesses can err, sometimes with disastrous consequences. Guided by the rationale that witnesses have access to potentially probative memorial information not captured by the traditional categorical lineup responses, an alternative procedure was examined in 6 experiments with adult (N = 1,669) and child (N = 273) witnesses. Instead of witnesses being asked to identify the offender from the lineup, they rated their confidence in the match between the offender and each lineup member and then variations in the maximum (max) confidence values assigned (i.e., the highest rated lineup members) were examined. Specifically, we evaluated how well max confidence values predicted suspect guilt or innocence. When suspects (guilty or innocent) in a lineup received the max confidence rating, the probability of guilt increased with the max. When the suspect received a rating lower than the max, they were generally more likely to be innocent. Max confidence patterns also predicted guilt where a traditional positive identification would have been unlikely: for example, when the max was low, when the witness gave the max to multiple lineup members, or when a filler received the max but the suspect also received a high rating. The data indicate that witnesses have access to probative memorial information often not captured by the traditional lineup responses when identifying someone or rejecting the lineup. Guidelines for the use and interpretation of this theoretically informed futuristic alternative to existing lineup procedures are provided.