ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

This is a literature review of the effects of humans ́ relationships with farm animals on animal productivity and welfare, including the following topics: definition of the concept and description of different tests that have been developed to measure human-animal relationship (HAR). Temperament and tameness, which have been considered as farm animal characteristics that are important in HAR, as are stockperson attitudes. Some international farm animal welfare protocols are also described, together with negative and positive stimuli that affect farm animal welfare and productivity. In addition to some factors affecting the quality of HAR. We conclude that even with improved precision farming and automation: a) a good HAR is still fundamental to improve farm animal welfare with associated health and production benefits and b) with the numerous tests assessing fear of humans, many are not commercially applicable.
Content may be subject to copyright.
J Anim Behav Biometeorol (2020) 8:196-205
ISSN 2318-1265
doi.org/10.31893/jabb.20026
Effects of human-animal relationship on animal productivity and
welfare
Daniel Mota-Rojas Donald Maurice Broom Agustín Orihuela Antonio Velarde
Fabio Napolitano María Alonso-Spilsbury
D Mota-Rojas M Alonso-Spilsbury
Neurophysiology, behaviour and assessment of welfare in
domestic animals. Department of Animal Production and
Agriculture, Universidad Autónoma Metropolitana (UAM),
Mexico City, Mexico.
DM Broom
Department of Veterinary Medicine, University of
Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom.
A Orihuela (Corresponding author)
Facultad de Ciencias Agropecuarias, Universidad Autónoma
del Estado de Morelos, Cuernavaca, Morelos, México.
A Velarde
Institute of Agrifood Research and Technology - IRTA,
Animal Welfare Program, Veinat Sies S-N, Monells 17121,
Spain.
F Napolitano
Scuola di Scienze Agrarie, Forestali, Alimentari ed
Ambientali, Università degli Studi della Basilicata, 85100
Potenza, Italy.
email: agustin.orihuela.trujillo@gmail.com
Abstract This is a literature review of the effects of humans´
relationships with farm animals on animal productivity and
welfare, including the following topics: definition of the
concept and description of different tests that have been
developed to measure human-animal relationship (HAR).
Temperament and tameness, which have been considered as
farm animal characteristics that are important in HAR, as are
stockperson attitudes. Some international farm animal
welfare protocols are also described, together with negative
and positive stimuli that affect farm animal welfare and
productivity. In addition to some factors affecting the quality
of HAR. We conclude that even with improved precision
farming and automation: a) a good HAR is still fundamental
to improve farm animal welfare with associated health and
production benefits and b) with the numerous tests assessing
fear of humans, many are not commercially applicable.
Keywords: fear, sensitive period, tameness, wellbeing
Introduction
According to Waiblinger et al (2006) and Ellingsen et
al (2014), human-animal relationships (HAR) can be defined
as “the degree of relation or distance that exists between an
animal and a human being, perceived, developed and
expressed through their mutual behaviour”. To create a
relation between any two individuals, entails on the one
hand, repeated encounters and, on the other, certain cognitive
abilities; that is, the capacities that allow individuals to
associate the positive or negative emotional content of
interactions with another individual, and then recall it when
predicting future encounters (Sankey et al 2010).
Farm animals, may perceive interaction with humans
as: a) negative, when they fear people, avoiding contact with
them; b) neutral, when the fear level is low but animals still
avoid contact; and c) positive, when fear is absent, and
animals allow physical (Claxton 2011; des Roches et al
2016). Handling that includes abrupt movements, pushing
and the use of prods, shouts and kicks is considered negative,
while handling characterised by slow movements, whispers
and petting have positive effects on animals (Ellingsen et al
2014). Non-aggressive controlling interactions, such as
gentle stick use, gentle handling and instructive talking, are
considered neutral (Waiblinger et al 2002). Classic
conditioning processes can occur when farm animals
associate either negative or positive handling with specific
humans responsible for their care (Rushen et al 1999ab) and
may also lead them to generalise their responses to other
people (Waiblinger et al 2006).
The use of positive reinforcements, like feeding or
tactile contact, often fosters learning in farm animals
(Rochais et al 2014) and may stimulate physiological
reactions that can be interpreted as “anti-stress effects
(Lürzel et al 2015). This is one of the means of improving
the quality of HAR.
The objective of this literature review is to show the
effects of humans´ relationships with farm animals on animal
productivity and welfare.
REVIEW
Received: May 02, 2020 Accepted: May 19, 2020 Published Online: June 04, 2020
197
J Anim Behav Biometeorol (2020) 8:196-205
Measures of HAR
Evaluating the quality of HAR is an important means
of improving animal welfare. This process must consider the
behaviour of the animals towards stockpersons as well as the
behaviour of the stockpersons towards the animals. HAR is
considered in international animal welfare protocols for
monitoring welfare in production units. For example, the
Welfare Quality®protocols (WQ® 2009abc; des Roches et al
2016), as well as others included in the Animal Welfare
Indicators project (AWIN) (Caroprese et al 2016).
Assessing the quality of HAR requires the gathering
of evidence that is: a) valid (i.e., reflects what actually
occurs); b) reliable (i.e., the tendency to give consistent
results with repeated measurement); and c) viable (in terms
of time, financial resources and safety) (Napolitano et al
2011; des Roches et al 2016).
Important human factors to be considered during this
assessment are stockpersons’ attitudes, personality,
knowledge, experience and degree of work satisfaction. The
attitude towards any kind of animal will affect the quality of
interaction and the type of handling. Attitudes during animal
handling have been classified as: a) tranquil or friendly; b)
dominant; c) impatient; and d) aggressive (Waiblinger et al
2006).
Figure 1shows the model proposed by Hemsworth
and Coleman (1998) to describe the effect of HAR on animal
productivity and welfare, as well as the reciprocal
relationship between the attitude of a person who handles
farm animals and the animal’s behavioural response towards
him. Within this topic Welp et al (2004) found that dairy
cows show greater vigilance behaviour, an indicator of fear,
in the presence of people who have negative attitudes
towards them, in comparison with unfamiliar individuals and
people with whom they have positive interaction, indicating
that animals are able to discriminate among people with
distinct behaviours.
Figure 1 Model of human-animal relationship proposed by Hemsworth and Coleman (1998), adapted from Hemsworth (2003). Most studies
on this topic highlight the sequential relations among the attitudes and behaviour of operators towards their animals, fear r esponses, and the
effect of fear on the welfare and productivity of farm animals.
To evaluate general beliefs and attitudes towards
animals, and levels of work satisfaction among stockpersons,
several questionnaires have been used. For example, in the
dairy (Breuer et al 2000) and sheep industries (Napolitano et
al 2011). However, some behavioural tests are difficult to
interpret, for example, human approaches to animals can be
perceived as threatening, but the fear the animals feel may
induce flight or a freezing reaction (Bourguet et al 2015).
198
J Anim Behav Biometeorol (2020) 8:196-205
Furthermore, activities like feeding reduce the possibility to
flee so may be inhibited. The response of the animals may
also vary as a function of social rank, the gregarious nature
of the animals (Carbajal and Orihuela 2010) and the inter-
individual distances that they keep within the group (Boissy
and Le Neindre 1997). Many responses are species-
dependent and goats (Mattiello et al 2010) are usually
considered more curious than sheep because of the
exploratory behaviour they often manifest towards people.
It is also important to evaluate animals’ reactions
during handling procedures (Napolitano et al 2013) because
tests that consider this factor may be used to select animals
less afraid of humans and, therefore, easier to manage
(Windschnurer et al 2009). Animals raised in semi-natural
conditions tend to show less interest in humans, perhaps
related to the fact that their only contact with them occurs
when they are captured in the field and separated from their
mother or herd (Rochais et al 2014).
Farm animals are more likely to approach stationary
persons than persons actively moving toward them, and
humans in squatting position more than those in standing
position (Hemsworth et al 1986b; Lyons et al 1988). Farm
animal species can also recognize individual humans and are
more likely to approach those who treat them well than those
who act in an aggressive way. Similarly, the location of prior
positive or negative handling experiences can determine how
animals approach or avoid the same person in two different
places if that person treated them differently in each place
(Rushen et al 1999a). Several behavioural tests have been
used to assess fearfulness towards humans, as reviewed by
Waiblinger et al (2006).
The stockpersons´ attitudes towards farm animals can
be measured by the animal responses to humans in
standardized test situations like the reactivity to an
approaching human (Muns et al 2015; Brajon et al 2015) and
the avoidance distance test (Edwards et al 2010).
Studies on cattle, poultry and pigs have suggested that
the test of reactivity to an approaching moving human, based
on the avoidance responses of the animals, reflects the HAR
(Waiblinger et al 2006). The human approach test, assesses
fear of humans, offering also the possibility to measure the
social relationship with humans, the management quality and
the extent of individual differences (Waiblinger et al 2006).
The WQ protocols rely on human approach tests. A human
approach test is also included in the AWIN protocols for
horses and sheep (AWIN 2015ab), whereas the AWIN
protocol for goats relies on a stationary human test (AWIN,
2015c). In cattle, the test of reactivity to a stationary human
seems to be less relevant, because it is based on animal
approach behaviour towards humans and reflects not just
reactivity to humans but a mix of motivations depending on
the context of the test (de Passillé and Rushen 2005).
Conversely, this type of test seems more appropriate for
goats, which are generally more exploratory than other farm
animals.
The response of sows in stalls to approaching hand
contact and the response of free-moving sows in groups to
approach by an observer, had been validated by Scott et al
(2009) for on-farm welfare assessment in different housing
systems. However, according to Powell et al (2016), it
should be noted that the human approaching touch is a valid
measure of fear to humans in experimental settings, but
impractical for on-farm use due to the need for animals to be
individually tested in a purpose-built test arena.
The Stationary Person Test, the Avoidance Distance
Test and the Touch Test had been validated to effectively
measure the human-hen relationship in non-caged systems
(Graml et al 2008a). It is worth noting that a complex test
including stationary and moving elements has also been
validated for birds (Raubek et al 2007). The Avoidance
Distance Test has also been recommended by Windschnurer
et al (2008) for on-farm welfare assessment for dairy cows.
However, some inconsistencies in dairy calves´ responses
had also been observed (Meagher et al 2016).
Other means to assess the HAR are: (i) use an
interview about stockperson practices (Kling-Eveillard et al
2017); (ii) observing stockpersons during common
(Ellingsen et al 2014; Rueda et al 2015) or less common
handling events (Destrez et al 2018); (iii) assess farmers´
attitudes through a questionnaire (Bertenshaw and
Rowlinson 2009; Hemsworth et al 2000; Fukasawa et al
2017); or (iv) use video-recording (Johansson et al 2015).
However, according to Spoolder (2007), it is more relevant
to assess the quality of the HAR directly by looking at
farmers´ attitudes and handling practices than by assessing
fear of humans.
Regardless of the test used, there are some
confounding factors that might be considered when
interpreting the results. The nature of human contact can
significantly modulate how farm animals perceive humans,
affecting their behavioural responses (for a review, see
Boivin et al 2003; Waiblinger et al 2006; Adler et al 2019).
Effects on productivity and welfare
The nature of HAR matters as it will modulate not
only the welfare of the animal, including its health, but also
productivity and product quality (Hemsworth et al 2009;
Tallet et al 2018). Research on HAR and animal production
has been mainly focused on its effect on stress, productivity
and meat quality (Hemsworth 2003; Hemsworth et al 2009;
Zulkifli et al 2013).
Negative effects
Negative handling such as shouting and hitting, leads
to poor animal welfare, including fear, acute and chronic
199
J Anim Behav Biometeorol (2020) 8:196-205
stress (Hemsworth et al 2000). Fear responses towards
humans may affect the productivity (Rushen et al 1999ab),
health and stress physiology of farm animals (Hemsworth et
al 2000; Hemsworth 2009; Hemsworth and Coleman 2011);
as in pigs (Hemsworth 2008; Hemsworth and Coleman 2011;
Probst et al 2013), hens (Barnett et al 1994; Graml et al
2008b; Edwards et al 2010), and dairy cows (Lürzel et al
2015; Ivemeyer et al 2018; Adler et al 2019).
In Pigs
Poor handling results in high levels of fearfulness
(Hemsworth and Barnett 1991). Negative behaviours
displayed by the stockperson (e.g. slapping, hitting and
kicking) are strongly associated with a high level of fear in
pigs (Hemsworth et al 1989a).
Stressful, human handling affects pig weight gains
negatively (Gonyou et al 1986; Hemsworth and Barnett
1991). Hemsworth et al (1981) subjected gilts to either
pleasant or unpleasant human contact for three 2-min
periods/week, from 11 to 22 weeks of age. The authors noted
that the juvenile females with the pleasant handling treatment
had higher weigh gain than those with other treatments.
Lensink et al (2009a), observed that the fear response
to humans of nulliparous sows was linked with their
behaviour towards humans and nervousness around first
farrowing, and these negative interactions were mostly
related with prophylactic and therapeutic procedures
(Lensink et al 2009b).
Unpleasant physical contact with humans reduced
testicle size and delayed coordinated mating response in
boars, and reduced pregnancy rate in sows, when compared
with those receiving positive human handling (Hemsworth et
al 1986c).
A study undertaken by Sommavilla et al (2011)
showed that loud tones, making threatening postures and
forcing piglets in the creep area during the suckling period
were perceived as aversive, and increased the withdrawal
response of piglets to human approach on the day of weaning
compared with piglets that received a neutral treatment.
Similarly, Brajon et al (2015) showed that rough handling,
even without physical stress, was enough to activate a fear
response in weaned piglets.
In Poultry
High fear of humans is associated with reduced egg
production, growth, feed efficiency, product quality and
sexual activity whilst increasing aggression, handling
difficulties and immunosuppression (Barnett et al 1993;
1994; Gross and Siegel 1982; Jones 1996).
Visual or physical contact with humans can elicit
behavioural inhibition, withdrawal panic and violent escape
reactions in chickens (Jones 1996), often with associated
injury as well as adrenal responses. Fear reactions, like panic
or violent escape attempts, not only waste energy and thereby
impose a metabolic cost, but they can also result in injury or
even death when the birds run into obstacles or pile on top of
each other (Waiblinger et al 2006).
In Cattle and Buffaloes
Poor HAR is associated with reduced milk production
by cows (Seabrook 1984; Waiblinger et al 2002). Aversion
to shouts has been reported (Waynert et al 1999; Pajor et al
2000). Ellingsen et al (2014) observed that stockpersons with
a nervous handling style or who were dominant and
aggressive, induced a negative mood in more cows. des
Roches et al (2016) confirmed that cows’ fear of people is
linked to negative attitudes displayed by caretakers toward
cows and is reduced in farms where several caretakers are
present. Likewise, cattle show more intense fear responses to
humans in larger farms with higher levels of mechanization,
due to the lower frequency of contact with the stockperson
(Mattiello et al 2009). Rushen et al (1999b) reported that the
presence of an aversive stockperson at milking, who had
previously hit or occasionally used a battery-operated
prodder over a 5-day period, led cows to increase their
residual milk. Similarly, Munksgaard et al (1997) observed
that, following the aversive treatment of striking the cow
forcefully with an open hand, cows urinated and defecated
more frequently. In addition, Arias and Špinka (2005) found
that in farms with more neurotic stockpersons, the cows had
lower milk yields per standardized lactation and higher
veterinary costs. Hemsworth et al (2000) found that the use
of negative interactions with cattle by stockpersons was not
only negatively correlated with milk yield, but also with
percent protein and fat, and positively correlated with milk
cortisol concentration.
There is evidence that excitable temperament in
stockpersons increases the risk of aggressive handling and
tends to decrease labour efficiency, for example, by
increasing the time needed to perform artificial insemination
and reducing cows’ body hygiene (Rueda et al 2015). Heifers
that faced negative handling and were more reactive during
handling had higher dirtiness scores and these were
associated with lower pregnancy rates (Ceballos et al 2018).
Furthermore, cows that required more handling time in the
corral, produced fewer viable embryos (Macedo et al 2011).
Aggressive and mild-escape behaviours during head-lock
restraint had negative effects on reproductive performance of
dairy heifers (Kasimanickam et al 2018). Moreover, fear of
humans may also decrease meat quality, such as tenderness
(Ferguson and Warner 2008; Probst et al 2012).
In dairy buffaloes the number of negative interactions
performed by stock-people was positively correlated with the
number of kicks by buffaloes during milking and with the
200
J Anim Behav Biometeorol (2020) 8:196-205
number of exogenous oxytocin injections used to induce
milk let-down (Napolitano et al 2019). These results support
the hypothesis that the behaviour of stock-people and
buffaloes are related and indicate room for improvement
following appropriate stock-people training programmes.
Positive effects
In contrast, a positive relationship is notably
characterized by absence of fear reactions to humans and
animals that are easier to handle (Waiblinger et al 2006).
Positive interactions such as pats, gentle slaps or talking to
the animals showed positive effects reducing fear and human
avoidance. Also, among the interactions with humans that
can be perceived as positive, food provision is an efficient
way of attracting animals (Jago et al 1999; Tallet et al 2005;
Graml et al 2008a). Stockpersons with positive attitudes
towards animals often have animals with increased
productivity (Rushen and de Passillé 2015).
In Pigs
Human-animal interactions as short as
4h/sow/reproductive cycle, may influence both the
performance and welfare of the animals (Prunier and Tallet
2015). Janczak et al (2003) found that sows showing less fear
of humans had higher reproductive success and more
adaptive maternal behaviour.
According to Hemsworth et al (1994), human
recognition can happen in situations of intensive handling.
Intact males, that are commonly raised in stable groups in
some countries, when they were positively handled were
more socially active, both in their groups and with an
unfamiliar human (Tallet et al 2013).
In Poultry
Handling studies on poultry show that regular positive
gentle handling can enhance growth performance, feed
efficiency (Gross and Siegel 1982; Zulkifli and Siti Nor
Azah 2004), egg production (Barnett et al., 1994), disease
resistance to infection, antibody protection (Graml et al
2008a; Edwards et al 2010; Al-Aqil et al 2013) and first-
week survival (Cransberg et al 2000).
In adult laying hens, reduction in fear of humans and
a decrease in plasma corticosterone concentration were
shown following additional handling, consisting of walking,
talking, feeding and touching single birds (Barnett et al 1994;
Graml et al 2008a). Similarly, Edwards et al (2010) showed
for laying hens that 12 min/day of visual contact with
humans during rearing resulted in reduced avoidance
behaviour of humans during adulthood and there was a trend
for hens receiving positive handling to have a lower
corticosterone response to human contact than those
receiving negative handling. Visible human presence
associated with predictable approach, human voice, slow
movements and gentle handling for weighing, were effective
in inducing habituation to humans in laying hens (Bertin et al
2019).
Al-Aqil et al (2013) subjected broiler chicks to a
pleasant physical contact 30 sec/day from 1 to 28 days of age
and found that those chickens had lower
heterophil/lymphocyte ratios (h/l) and plasma corticosterone
levels than their neglected counterparts following road
transportation. Zulkifli et al (2002) suggested that allowing
broiler chickens to see the experimenter for 10 min twice
daily from 0 to 3 weeks of age, with no attempt to initiate
tactile interaction with the birds, was sufficient to alleviate
fear and stress reactions to handling and crating, and improve
their antibody response. In a study conducted by Zulkifli et al
(2004), some chicks were randomly caught, picked up and
stroked gently for 10 min twice daily, and other chicks were
picked up individually and stroked gently for 30 sec once
daily in their home pen. While positive visual contact had no
effect on performance, birds that were handled pleasantly
had improved body weight and feed conversion ratios.
In Cattle and Buffaloes
Hemsworth et al (1989b) reported that the
stockperson’s presence and positive handling during calving
of heifers led to faster approach to an experimenter in a test
situation, lower cortisol concentrations, and less stepping and
kicking responses during milking in the first weeks of
lactation than heifers that calved without human presence.
According to Ellingsen et al (2014), stockpersons who
handle their calves patiently, pet them and calmly talk to
them during handling, induce in their animals’ higher levels
of positive mood. The influence of gentle interactions on
weight gain in group-housed dairy calves has been studied by
Lürzel et al (2015). Gentle stroking in dairy cattle has been
shown to reduce heart rate increased during a veterinary
procedure (Schmied et al 2010). In addition, in beef cattle,
gentle touching at an early age seems to reduce the cortisol
release at slaughter (Probst et al 2012).
The HAR is also relevant for udder health. Ivemeyer
et al (2011; 2018) found that positive behaviour of
stockpersons during milking was associated with lower
somatic cell counts and with lower prevalence of mastitic
quarters.
As also observed in cattle, in dairy buffaloes the
number of positive interactions performed by stock-people
was positively correlated with milk production (Napolitano
et al 2019).
Factors affecting the quality of HAR
At least three aspects are important in affecting the
quality of HAR: 1) a genetic influence indicating the
201
J Anim Behav Biometeorol (2020) 8:196-205
relevance of genetic selection, 2) the development of
husbandry practices that improve animal´s perception of
humans, sometimes by habituation (Boissy et al 2002; Faure
et al 2003) and 3) early positive human contact, that could
influence future emotional experiences in animals to
stressors when in the presence of humans.
Genetic influences
Genetic dispositions contribute to personality
differences, partly explaining differences in HAR within a
herd or between breeds (Andersen et al 2006). Stockpersons
deliberately or accidentally select tame animals for breeding
stock because they are easier to handle and manage (Price
and Orihuela 2010), and tameness heritability has been
estimated to be about 22 (Le Neindre et al 1995) to 38%
(Hemsworth et al 1990).
Lankin (1997) studied the behavior of 11 breeds of
sheep and concluded that breeds subjected to intensive
selection for commercial purposes were tamer toward
humans than breeds which had not been subjected to such
selection. Also, Lyons et al (1988) examined the tameness of
dairy goats toward humans, both within and between twin
sets. One sibling of each twin set was reared by the mother
while the other was hand reared. The tameness scores of the
hand-reared goats were better than for their dam-reared twin.
Habituation
In habituation, the animal´s fear of humans is
gradually reduced by repeated exposures in a neutral context,
when a person´s presence has no obvious reinforcement
properties (Price and Orihuela 2010). For example, Jones et
al (1993) found that domestic chicks showed decreased
avoidance of humans following twice daily exposures to
humans.
Similarly, Prado et al (2001) found that bucks raised
under semi-intensive conditions but habituated to the human
presence, became easier to train for semen collection using
an artificial vagina, than bucks raised under extensive
conditions, with little or no human presence.
Early contact
Positive human animal contacts and handling can
efficiently affect the quality of the relationship when the
interactions are conducted at a young age. Indeed, Le
Neindre et al (1996) found that young range-reared cattle
increased their tolerance of human presence if exposed to
human handling just after weaning.
Early tactile stimulation influences the postnatal
development of pigs (Tanida et al 1995; de Oliveira et al
2015). Early gentle contact with humans enhances approach
behaviour to human beings (Hemsworth et al 1986a). Piglets
whose back was stroked by humans for 2 min from 5 to 35
days of age, were less fearful in a novel environment and less
fearful of being handled by people (de Oliveira et al 2015).
Muns et al (2015) found that positive contact reduced the
duration of escape behaviour of piglets to tail-docking on day
15. Furthermore, according to Büttner et al (2018) positive
HAR (e.g. calm speech, petting, food provision), carried out
3 times/week by one person for 15 min in each pen during
the rearing period, can reduce the occurrence of tail-biting in
weaned piglets.
Several authors have showed that previous positive
handling may improve ease of handling later in life in several
species. For example, favoring ease of handling while
loading calves for transport (Lensink et al 2001), and
reducing fear related reactions at the abattoir in beef cattle,
which can be the reason for improved tenderness of meat
(Probst et al., 2012); reducing vocalizations in unfamiliar
environments (Boivin et al 2000; Tallet et al 2008), heart
rate, cortisol concentration (Tosi and Hemsworh 2002) and
flight distance in lambs (Markowitz et al 1998); and reducing
kicking in dairy cows during rectal palpation (Waiblinger et
al 2004) and fear of humans in chickens (Jones and
Waddington 1993).
Domestic horse foals showed less fear of humans if
they had contact with humans during their first 5 days of age,
even if they were just observing their mothers being fed by
hand and brushed (Hausberger et al 2008) or when exposed
to motionless humans (Henry et al 2006).
There is some evidence that mothers may be an
important social model, modulating or buffering the
behavioral and physiological responses associated with the
development of HAR in their young (Waiblinger 2017;
2019).
Final Considerations
We conclude that even with improved precision
farming and automation: a) a good HAR is still fundamental
to improve farm animal welfare with associated health and
production benefits and b) with the numerous tests assessing
fear of humans, many are not commercially applicable.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that there are no conflicts of
interest with this work.
References
Adler F, Christley R, Campe A (2019) Examining farmers'
personalities and attitudes as possible risk factors for dairy cattle
health, welfare, productivity, and farm management: A systematic
scoping review. Journal of Dairy Science 102:3805-3824.
Al-Aqil A, Zulkifli I, Hair Bejo M, Sazili AQ, Rajion MA, Somchit
MN (2013) Changes in heat shock protein 70, blood parameters and
202
J Anim Behav Biometeorol (2020) 8:196-205
fear-related behavior in broiler chickens as affected by pleasant and
unpleasant human contact. Poultry Science 92:33-40.
Andersen IL, Berg S, Bøe KE, Edwards SA (2006) Positive
handling in late pregnancy and the consequences for maternal
behavior and production in sows. Applied Animal Behaviour
Science 99:64-76.
Arias JLP, Špinka M (2005) Associations of stockpersons'
personalities and attitudes with performance of dairy cattle herds.
Czech Journal of Animal Science 50: 226-34.
AWIN (2015a) AWIN Welfare Assessment Protocol for Sheep.
doi:10.13130/AWIN_sheep_2015.
AWIN (2015b) AWIN Welfare Assessment Protocol for Horses.
doi: 10.13130/awin_horses_2015.
AWIN (2015c) AWIN Welfare Assessment Protocol for Goats.
doi:10.13130/AWIN_goats_2015.
Barnett JL, Hemsworth PH, Jones RB (1993) Behavioral responses
of commercially farmed laying hens to humans: evidence of
stimulus generalization. Applied Animal Behaviour Science
37:139-146.
Barnett JL, Hemsworth PH, Hennessy DP, McCallum TH,
Newmann EA (1994) The effects of modifying the amount of
human contact on behavioral, physiological and production
responses of laying hens. Applied Animal Behaviour Science
41:87-100.
Bertenshaw C, Rowlinson P (2009) Exploring stock managers'
perceptions of the human-animal relationship on dairy farms and an
association with milk production. Anthrozoos 22:59-69.
Bertin A, Mocz F, Calandreau L, Palme R, Lumineau S,
Darmaillacq S, Houdelier C (2019) Human behavior at the origin of
maternal effects on offspring behavior in laying hens (Gallus gallus
domesticus). Physiology Behaviour 201:175-183.
Boissy A, Le Neindre P (1997) Behavioral, cardiac and cortisol
responses to brief peer separation and reunion in cattle. Physiology
Behavior 61:693-699.
Boissy A, Fisher A, Bouix J, Boivin X, Le Neindre P (2002,
August) Genetics of fear and fearfulness in domestic herbivores.
Paper presented at the Seventh World Congress on Genetics
Applied to Livestock Production, Montpellier, FR.
Boivin S, Tournadre H, Le Neindre P (2000) Hand-feeding and
gentling influence early weaned lambs´ attachment responses to
their stock person. Journal of Animal Science 78:879-884.
Boivin X, Lensink BJ, Tallet C, Veissier I (2003) Stockmanship and
farm animal welfare. Animal Welfare 12:479-92.
Bourguet C, Deiss V, Boissy A, Terlouw EMC (2015) Young
Blond d'Aquitaine, Angus and Limousin bulls differ in emotional
reactivity: Relationships with animal traits, stress reactions at
slaughter and post-mortem muscle metabolism. Applied Animal
Behaviour Science 164:41-55.
Brajon S, Laforest JP, Bergeron R, Tallet C, Hötzel MJ, Devillers N
(2015) Persistency of the piglet's reactivity to the handler following
a previous positive or negative experience. Applied Animal
Behaviour Science 162:9-19.
Breuer K, Hemsworth PH, Barnett JL, Matthews LR, Coleman GL
(2000) Behavioral response to humans and the productivity of
commercial dairy cows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science
66:273-88.
Büttner K, Czycholl I, Basler H, Krieter J (2018) Effects of an
intensified human-animal interaction on tail biting in pigs during
the rearing period. Journal of Agricultural Science 156:1039-46.
Carbajal S, Orihuela A (2010) Minimal number of conspecifics
needed to minimize the stress response of isolated mature ewes.
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 4:249-255.
Caroprese M, Napolitano F, Mattiello S, Fthenakis GC, Ribó O,
Sevi A (2016) On-farm welfare monitoring of small ruminants.
Small Ruminant Research 135:20-25.
Ceballos MC, Sant'Anna AC, Góis KCR, Ferraudo AS, Negrao JA,
da Costa MJRP (2018) Investigating the relationship between
human-animal interactions, reactivity, stress response and
reproductive performance in Nellore heifers. Livestock Science
217:65-75.
Claxton AM (2011) The potential of the humananimal relationship
as an environmental enrichment for the welfare of zoo-housed
animals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 133:1-10.
Cransberg PH, Hemsworth PH, Coleman GJ (2000) Human factors
affecting the behavior and productivity of commercial broiler
chickens. British Poultry Science 41:272-79.
de Oliveira D, Paranhos da Costa MJR, Zupan M, Rehn T, Keeling
LJ (2015) Early human handling in non-weaned piglets: Effects on
behavior and body weight. Applied Animal Behaviour Science
164:56-63.
de Passillé AM, Rushen J (2005) Can we measure humananimal
interactions in on-farm animal welfare assessment? Some
unresolved issues. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 92:193-209.
des Roches AB, Veisser I, Boivin X, Gilot-Fromont E, Mounier L
(2016) A prospective exploration of farm, farmer, and animal
characteristics in human-animal relationships: An epidemiological
survey. Journal of Dairy Science 99:5573-85.
Destrez A, Haslin E, Boivin X (2018) What stockperson behavior
during weighing reveals about the relationship between humans and
suckling beef cattle: A preliminary study. Applied Animal Behavior
Science 209:8-13.
Edwards LE, Botheras NA, Coleman GJ, Hemsworth PH (2010)
Behavioral and physiological responses of laying hens to humans.
Animal Production Science 50:557-59.
Ellingsen K, Coleman GJ, Lund V, Mejdell CM (2014) Using
qualitative behavior assessment to explore the link between
stockperson behavior and dairy calf behavior. Applied Animal
Behavior Science 153:10-17.
Faure JM, Bessei W, Jones RB (2003) Direct selection for
improvement of animal well-being. In: Muir WM, Aggrey
SE (Eds.), Poultry Genetics, Breeding and Biotechnology (pp. 221-
245). Wallingford, UK: CABI Publishing.
Ferguson DM, Warner RD (2008) Have we underestimated the
impact of pre-slaughter stress on meat quality in ruminants? Meat
Science 80:12-19.
Fukasawa M, Kawahata M, Higashiyama Y, Komatsu T (2017)
Relationship between the stockperson's attitudes and dairy
productivity in Japan. Animal Science Journal 88:394-400.
Gonyou HW, Hemsworth PH, Barnett JL (1986) Effects of frequent
interactions with humans on growing pigs. Applied Animal
Behaviour Science 16:269-78.
Graml C, Waiblinger S, Niebuhr K (2008a) Validation of tests for
on-farm assessment of the hen-human relationship in non-cage
systems. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 111:301-310.
203
J Anim Behav Biometeorol (2020) 8:196-205
Graml C, Niebuhr K, Waiblinger S (2008b) Reaction of laying hens
to humans in the home or a novel environment. Applied Animal
Behaviour Science 113:98-109.
Gross WB, Siegel PB (1982) Socialization as a factor in resistance
to disease, feed efficiency and response to antigen in chickens.
American Journal of Veterinary Research 43:2010-12.
Hausberger M, Roche H, Henry S, Visser EK (2008) A review of
the human-horse relationship. Applied Animal Behaviour Science
109:1-24.
Hemsworth PH, Barnett JL, Hansen C (1981) The influence of
handling by humans on the behavior, growth and corticosteroids in
the juvenile female pig. Hormones and Behavior 15:396-403.
Hemsworth PH, Barnett JL, Hansen C, Gonyou HW (1986a) The
influence of early contact with humans on subsequent behavioral
response of pigs to humans. Applied Animal Behaviour Science
15:55-63.
Hemsworth PH, Gonyou HW, Dziuk PJ (1986b) Human
communication with pigs: the behavioral response of pigs to
specific human signals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 15:45-
54.
Hemsworth PH, Barnett JL, Hansen C (1986c) The influence of
handling by humans on the behavior, reproduction and
corticosteroids of male and female pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour
Science 15:303-14.
Hemsworth PH, Barnett JL, Hansen C (1987) The influence of
inconsistent handling by humans on the behavior, growth and
corticosteroids of young pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science
17:245-52.
Hemsworth PH, Barnett JL, Coleman GJ, Hansen C (1989b) A
study of the relationships between the attitudinal and behavioral
profiles of stockpersons and the level of fear of human and
reproductive performance of commercial pigs. Applied Animal
Behaviour Science 23:301-14.
Hemsworth PH, Barnett JL, Tilbrook AJ, Hansen C (1989a) The
effects of handling by humans at calving and during milking on the
behavior and milk cortisol concentrations of primiparous dairy
cows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 22:313-26.
Hemsworth PH, Barnett JL, Treacy D, Madgwick P (1990) The
heritability of the trait fear of humans and the association between
this trait and subsequent reproductive performance of gilts. Applied
Animal Behaviour Science 25:85-95.
Hemsworth PH, Barnett JL (1991) The effects of aversively
handling pigs, either individually or in groups, on their behavior,
growth and corticosteroids. Applied Animal Behaviour Science
30:61-72.
Hemsworth PH, Coleman GJ, Cox M, Barnett JL (1994) Stimulus
generalization: the inability of piglets to discriminate between
humans on the basis of their previous handling experience. Applied
Animal Behaviour Science 40:129-42.
Hemsworth PH, Coleman GJ (1998) Human-Livestock Interactions:
The Stockperson and the Productivity and Welfare of Intensively
Farmed Animals. Wallingford, UK: CAB International.
Hemsworth PH, Coleman GJ, Barnett JL, Borg S (2000)
Relationships between human-animal interactions and productivity
of commercial dairy cows. Journal of Animal Science 78:2821-31.
Hemsworth PH (2003) Human-animal interactions in livestock
production. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 81:185-98.
Hemsworth PH (2008) Human-pig relationships. In: Faucitano L,
Schaefer AL (Eds.), Welfare of Pigs from Birth to Slaughter (pp.
271-288). Wageningen, NL: Academic Publishers.
Hemsworth PH (2009) Impact of human-animal interactions on the
health, productivity and welfare of farm animals. In: Aland A,
Madec F (Eds.), Sustainable Animal Production: The Challenges
and Potential Developments for Professional Farming (pp. 57-68),
Wageningen, NL: Academic Publishers.
Hemsworth PH, Barnett JL, Coleman GJ (2009) The integration of
human-animal relations into animal welfare monitoring schemes.
Animal Welfare 18:335-45.
Hemsworth PH, Coleman GJ (2011) Human-animal interactions
and animal productivity and welfare. In: Human-livestock
interactions. The Stockperson and the Productivity of Intensively
Farmed Animals (pp. 47-83). Oxford, UK: CAB International.
Henry S, Richard-Yris MA, Hausberger M (2006) Influence of
various early human-foal interferences on subsequent human-foal
relationship. Developmental Psychobiology 48:712-718.
Ivemeyer S, Knierim U, Waiblinger S (2011) Effect of human-
animal relationship and management on udder health in Swiss dairy
herds. Journal of Dairy Science 94:5890-5902.
Ivemeyer S, Simantke C, Ebinghaus A, Poulsen PH, Sorensen JT,
Rousing T, Knierim U (2018) Herd-level associations between
human-animal relationship, management, fecal cortisol metabolites,
and udder health of organic dairy cows. Journal of Dairy Science
101:7361-74.
Jago JG, Krohn CC, Matthews LR (1999) The influence of feeding
and handling on the development of the humananimal interactions
in young cattle. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 62:137-51.
Janczak AM, Pedersen LJ, Rydhmer L, Bakken M (2003) Relation
between early fear- and anxiety-related behavior and maternal
ability in sows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 82:121-35.
Johansson A, Blokhuis HJ, Berckmans D, Butterworth A (2015)
Development of an automatic method to assess the human-animal
relationship in broilers at flock level. Paper presented at the Seventh
European Conference on Precision Livestock Farming, Milan, ITL.
Jones RB, Waddington D (1993) Attenuation of the domestic
chick´s fear of human beings via regular handling: in search of a
sensitive period. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 36:185-195.
Jones RB, Hemsworth PH, Barnett JL (1993, September) Fear of
humans and performance in commercial broiler flocks. In: Savory
CJ, Hughes BO (Eds.), Proceedings of the 4th UFAW European
Symposium on Poultry Welfare, Potters Bar, UK.
Jones RB (1996) Fear and adaptability in poultry: insights,
implications and imperatives. World Poultry Science Journal
52:133-74.
Kasimanickam VR, Staker C, Williams HM, Kastelic JP,
Kasimanickam R (2018) Aggressive attempted escape behavior
during head-lock restraint reduced reproductive performances in
Holstein heifers. Theriogenology 121:147-152.
Kling-Eveillard F, Hostiou N, Ganis E, Philibert A (2017,
September) The effects of PLF on human-animal relationships on
farms. Paper presented at the 8th European Conference on Precision
Livestock Farming, Nantes, FR.
Lankin V (1997) Factors of diversity of domestic behavior in sheep.
Genetics Selection Evolution 29:73-92.
Le Neindre
P,
Boivin X, Boissy A (1996) Handling of extensively
kept animals. Applied Animal Behaviour
Science
49:7381.
204
J Anim Behav Biometeorol (2020) 8:196-205
Le Neindre P, Trillat C, Sapa J, Mnissier F, Bonnet JN, Chupin J
(1995) Individual differences in docility in Limousin cattle. Journal
of Animal Science 73:22492253.
Lensink BJ, Leruste H, Le Roux T, Bizeray-Filoche D (2009a)
Relationship between the behavior of sows at 6 months old and the
behavior and performance at farrowing. Animal 3:128-134.
Lensink BJ, Leruste H, de Bretagne T, Bizeray-Filoche D (2009b)
Sow behavior towards humans during standard management
procedures and their relationship to piglet survival. Applied Animal
Behaviour Science 119:151-157.
Lürzel S, Münsch C, Windschnurer I, Futschik A, Palme R,
Waiblinger S (2015) The influence of gentle interactions on
avoidance distance towards humans, weight gain and physiological
parameters in group-housed dairy calves. Applied Animal
Behaviour Science 172:9-16.
Lyons DM, Price EO, Moberg GP (1988) Individual differences in
temperament of domestic dairy goats: constancy and change.
Animal Behaviour 36:1323-1333.
Macedo GG, Zúccari CE, de Abreu UG, Negrão JA, da Costa e
Silva EV (2011) Human-animal interaction, stress, and embryo
production in Bos indicus embryo donors under tropical conditions.
Tropical Animal Health and Production 43:1175-1182.
Markowitz TM, Dally MR, Gursky K, Price EO (1998) Early
handling increases lamb affinity for humans. Animal Behaviour
55:573-587.
Mattiello S, Klotz C, Baroli D, Minero M, Ferrente V, Canali E
(2009) Welfare problems in alpine cattle farms in Alto Adige
(Eastern Italian Alps). Italian Journal of Animal Science 8:628-30.
Mattiello S, Battini M, Andreoli E, Minero M, Barbieri S, Canali E
(2010) Avoidance distance test in goats: A comparison with its
application in cows. Small Ruminant Research 91:215-218.
Meagher RK, von Keyserlingk MAG, Atkinson D, Weary DM
(2016) Inconsistency in dairy calves’ responses to tests of
fearfulness. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 185:15-22.
Munksgaard L, de Passillé AM, Rushen J, Thodberg K, Jensen MB
(1997) Discrimination of people by dairy cows based on handling.
Journal of Dairy Science 80:1106-1112.
Muns R, Rault JL, Hemsworth P (2015) Positive human contact on
the first day of life alters the piglet's behavioral response to humans
and husbandry practices. Physiology Behavior 151:162-167.
Napolitano F, De Rosa G, Girolami A, Scavone M, Braghieri A
(2011) Avoidance distance in sheep: Test retest reliability and
relationship with stockmen attitude. Small Ruminant Research
99:81-86.
Napolitano F, Pacelli C, Grasso F, Braghieri A, De Rosa G (2013)
The behavior and welfare of buffaloes (Bubalus bubalis) in modern
dairy enterprises. Animal 7:1704-1713.
Napolitano F, Serrapica M, Braghieri A, Masucci F, Sabia E, De
Rosa G (2019) Human-animal interactions in dairy buffalo farms.
Animals 9:246.
Pajor EA, Rushen J, de Passillé AMB (2000). Aversion learning
techniques to evaluate dairy cattle handling practices. Applied
Animal Behaviour Science 69:89-102.
Powell C, Hemsworth LM, Rice M, Hemsworth PH (2016)
Comparison of methods to assess fear of humans in commercial
breeding gilts and sows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science
181:70-75.
Prado VM, Orihuela A, Lozano TS, Pérez-León MI (2001) Effect of
the breeding system (extensive vs. semi-intensive) of male goat, on
obtaining semen via artificial vagina. Veterinaria México 32:297-
299.
Price EO, Orihuela A (2010) Conducta Animal Aplicada al Cuidado
y Producción Pecuaria. (Animal Behavior Applied to Livestock
Care and Production). México, D.F.: Editorial Trillas.
Probst JK, Neff AS, Leiber F, Kreuzer M, Hillmann E (2012)
Gentle touching in early life reduces avoidance distance and
slaughter stress in beef cattle. Applied Animal Behaviour Science
139:42-49.
Probst JK, Hillmann E, Leiber F, Kreuzer M, Spengler NA (2013)
Influence of gentle touching applied few weeks before slaughter on
avoidance distance and slaughter stress in finishing cattle. Applied
Animal Behaviour Science 144:14-21.
Prunier A, Tallet C (2015) Endocrine and behavioural responses of
sows to human interactions and consequences on reproductive
performance. In: Farmer C (Ed.), The Gestating and Lactating Sow
(279-296). Wageningen, NL: Academic Publishers.
Raubek J, Niebuhr K, Waiblinger S (2007) Development of on-farm
methods to assess the animal-human relationship in laying hens
kept in non-cage systems. Animal Welfare 16:173-175.
Rochais C, Henry S, Sankey C, Nassur F, Goracka-Bruzda A,
Hausberger M (2014) Visual attention, an indicator of human-
animal relationships? A study of domestic horses (Equus caballus).
Frontiers in Psychology 5:108.
Rueda PM, Sant'Anna AC, Valente TS, da Costa MJRP (2015)
Impact of the temperament of Nellore cows on the quality of
handling and pregnancy rates in fixed-time artificial insemination.
Livestock Science 177:189-95.
Rushen J, Taylor AA, de Passill AM (1999a) Domestic animals’
fear of humans and its effect on their welfare. Applied Animal
Behaviour Science 65:285-303.
Rushen J, de Passillé AM, Munksgaard L (1999b) Fear of people by
cows and effects on milk yield, behaviour and heart rate at milking.
Journal of Dairy Science 82:720-27.
Rushen J, de Passillé AM (2015) The importance of good
stockmanship and its benefits to animals. In: Grandin T (Ed.),
Improving Animal Welfare: A Practical Approach (pp. 125-138),
Wallingford, UK: CAB International.
Sankey C, Richard-Yris MA, Leroy H, Henry S, Hausberger M
(2010) Positive interactions lead to lasting positive memories in
horses, Equus caballus. Animal Behavior 79:869-875.
Schmied C, Boivin X, Scala S, Waiblinger S (2010) Effect of
previous stroking on reactions to a veterinary procedure behavior
and heart rate of dairy cows. Interaction Studies 11:467-81.
Scott K, Laws DM, Courboulay V, Meunier-Salaün MC, Edwards
SA (2009) Comparison of methods to assess fear of humans in
sows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 118:36-41.
Seabrook MF (1984) The psychological interaction between the
stockman and his animals and its influence on performance of pigs
and dairy cows. Veterinary Science 115:8487.
Sommavilla R, Hötzel MJ, Dalla Costa OA (2011) Piglets’ weaning
behavioral response is influenced by quality of human-animal
interactions during suckling. Animal 5:1426-1431.
Spoolder H (2007) Fear of humans. In: Velarde A, Geers R (Eds.),
On Farm Monitoring of Pig Welfare (pp. 33-39), Wageningen, NL:
Academic Publications.
205
J Anim Behav Biometeorol (2020) 8:196-205
Tallet C, Veissier I, Boivin X (2005) Human contact and feeding as
rewards for the lamb's affinity to their stockperson. Applied Animal
Behaviour Science 94:59-73.
Tallet C, Veissier I, Boivin X (2008) Temporal association between
food distribution and human caregiver presence and the
development of affinity to humans in lambs. Developmental
Psychobiology 50:147-159.
Tallet C, Brilloüet A, Meunier-Salaün MC, Paulmier V, Guérin C,
Prunier A (2013) Effects of neonatal castration on social behavior,
human-animal relationship and feeding activity in finishing pigs
reared in a conventional or an enriched housing. Applied Animal
Behaviour Science 145:70-83.
Tallet C, Brajon S, Devillers N, Lensink J (2018) Pig-human
interactions: Creating a positive perception of humans to ensure pig
welfare. In: Špinka M (Ed.). Advances in Pig Welfare, (pp. 381-
398). Amsterdam, NL: Elsevier Science.
Tanida H, Miura A, Tanaka T, Yoshimoto T (1995) Behavioral
response to humans in individually handled weaning pigs. Applied
Animal Behaviour Science 42:249-59.
Tosi MV, Hemsworth PH (2002) Stockperson-husbandry
interactions and animal welfare in the extensive livestock industries.
Paper presented at the 36th International Society for Applied
Ethology Congress, Wageningen NL.
Waiblinger S, Menke C, Coleman G (2002) The relationship
between attitudes, personal characteristics and behavior of
stockpeople and subsequent behavior and production of dairy cows.
Applied Animal Behaviour Science 79:195-219.
Waiblinger S, Menke C, Korff J, Bucher A (2004) Previous
handling and gentle interactions affect behavior and heart rate of
dairy cows during a veterinary procedure. Applied Animal
Behaviour Science 85, 3142.
Waiblinger S, Boivin X, Pedersen V, Tosi MV, Janczak AM, Visser
EK, Jones RB (2006) Assessing the human-animal relationship in
farmed species: a critical review. Applied Animal Behaviour
Science 101:185-242.
Waiblinger S (2017) Human-animal relations. In: Jensen P (Ed.),
The Ethology of Domestic Animals: An Introductory Text, (pp.
135-146). Wallingford UK: CAB International,
Waiblinger S (2019) Agricultural animals. In: Hosey G and Melfi V
(Eds.), Anthrozoology: Human-Animal Interactions in
Domesticated and Wild Animals, (pp. 32-58). Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press.
Waynert DF, Stookey JM, Schwartzkopf-Genswein KS, Watts JM,
Waltz CS (1999) The response of beef cattle to noise during
handling. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 62:27-42.
Welfare Quality® (2009a) Assessment Protocol for Cattle. Welfare
Quality® Consortium, Lelystad, The Netherlands.
Welfare Quality® (2009b) Assessment Protocol for Pigs. Welfare
Quality® Consortium, Lelystad, The Netherlands.
Welfare Quality® (2009c) Assessment Protocol for Poultry.
Welfare Quality® Consortium, Lelystad, The Netherlands.
Welp T, Rushen J, Kramer DL, Festa-Bianchet M, de Passillé AM
(2004) Vigilance as a measure of fear in dairy cattle. Applied
Animal Behaviour Science 87:1-13.
Windschnurer I, Schmied C, Boivin X, Waiblinger S (2008)
Reliability and inter-test relationship of tests for on-farm assessment
of dairy cows' relationship to humans. Applied Animal Behaviour
Science 114:37-53.
Windschnurer I, Boivin X, Waiblinger S (2009) Reliability of an
avoidance distance test for the assessment of animals’
responsiveness to humans and a preliminary investigation of its
association with farmers’ attitudes on bull fattening farms. Applied
Animal Behaviour Science 117:117-127.
Zulkifli I, Gilbert J, Liew PK, Ginsos J (2002) The effects of regular
visual contact with human beings on fear, stress, antibody and
growth responses in broiler chickens. Applied Animal Behaviour
Science 79:103-112.
Zulkifli I, Siti Nor Azah A (2004) Fear and stress reactions, and the
performance of commercial broiler chickens subjected to regular
pleasant and unpleasant contacts with human being. Applied
Animal Behaviour Science 88:77-87.
Zulkifli I (2013) Review of human-animal interactions and their
impact on animal productivity and welfare. Journal of Animal
Science and Biotechnology 4:25.
... The reliability of such systems and algorithms in clinical practice is critical, but the process can be a lengthy and costly endeavor. The same authors pointed out that in their studies of methods for assessing animal welfare, attention was paid to the need for new technologies' pilot data [92]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Remote vital sensing in veterinary medicine is a relatively new area of practice, which involves the acquisition of data without invasion of the body cavities of live animals. This paper aims to review several technologies in remote vital sensing: infrared thermography, remote photoplethysmography (rPPG), radar, wearable sensors, and computer vision and machine learning. In each of these technologies, we outline its concepts, uses, strengths, and limitations in multiple animal species, and its potential to reshape health surveillance, welfare evaluation, and clinical medicine in animals. The review also provides information about the problems associated with applying these technologies, including species differences, external conditions, and the question of the reliability and classification of these technologies. Additional topics discussed in this review include future developments such as the use of artificial intelligence, combining different sensing methods, and creating monitoring solutions tailored to specific animal species. This contribution gives a clear understanding of the status and future possibilities of remote vital sensing in veterinary applications and stresses the importance of that technology for the development of the veterinary field in terms of animal health and science.
... Some common practices that the farm animals are subjected to include, "debeaking, dehorning, castration without anesthesia, repeatedly and forcibly artificially inseminating animals, mulesing, overcrowding, overloading, filthy living conditions, separating mother and child, and genetic mutilating" (Narayanan, 2016). Poor human-animal relationships (HAR) tend to induce negative productivity outcomes (Waiblinger et al., 2006;Mota-Rojas et al., 2020). A sequential association between the attitudes and behavior of the stock person tends to influence fear, stress, and welfare and productivity (Acharya et al., 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
The relationship between sustainable agriculture and animal welfare is explored. Agricultural practices that promote economic prosperity and ecological balance are discussed. Ethical considerations and legal aspects that recognize animals with legal rights are discussed. Global practices in farm animal welfare are compared with those in India.
... De igual forma, se sugiere realizar investigaciones alrededor de la importancia de la personalidad en la lateralización, y desarrollar más estudios donde se evalúe en conjunto indicadores conductuales y fisiológicos, empleando herramientas no invasivas como la termografía infrarroja, que también detecta alteraciones asociadas con cambios en la valencia del estado emocional 98 . Una completa comprensión de esta función cerebral y su modulación permitirá identificar y establecer formas de crianza que aumenten la probabilidad de que los animales tengan un sesgo cognitivo positivo y, en caso de que se encuentre una forma de trasladar el control al hemisferio izquierdo, se pueda ayudar a mejorar el bienestar de aquellos animales que sufren de estrés crónico [99][100][101][102][103][104][105][106][107][108][109][110][111][112] ...
... In comparison to other livestock, a dairy cow is kept for a long time and across lactations involving regular, intense, and long-term human-animal contact, promoting relationships not only with individual cows but also across generations (Waiblinger et al., 2006, Waiblinger andLürzel, 2023). Previous research has suggested that the human-animal relationship is believed to have a positive effect, with mutually beneficial effects on the health and well-being of both humans and animals (Mota-Rojas et al., 2020, Prato-Previde et al., 2022. However, contradictory results exist suggesting that the relationship may vary from highly positive to highly negative outcomes (Herzog, 2011, Andrade andAnneberg, 2014). ...
Article
The cattle industry has made considerable efforts to adopt more sustainable beef production practices. Still, the social pillar of sustainability—especially workforce well-being—often receives less attention than the environmental and economic pillars. There is also limited information about the perspectives of U.S. cattle transporters, who play a critical role in ensuring animal welfare during the final stages of the beef supply chain. This study explored cattle transporters’ perspectives on management practices related to cattle care and well-being, training, work environment, and community. Cattle transporters (N = 74) from three trucking companies operating in Colorado, Kansas, Nebraska, and Texas participated in focus group discussions and completed a short, written survey. In total, 12 focus groups were conducted. In the written survey, 98.7% (n = 73) of survey respondents agreed with the statement “I have pride in the job I do,” and 97.3% (n = 72) agreed with the statement “Animal well-being is a critical component of my job.” Additionally, 96% (n = 71) of respondents agreed with the statement, “The training I have received at this job has prepared me well for transporting cattle,” highlighting the success of current education and training programs. Thematic analysis of focus group transcripts identified ten themes, including (1) Animal Welfare and Safety; (2) Feelings; (3) Human Welfare and Safety; (4) Learning and Training; (5) Money; (6) Opportunities for Improvement Throughout the Supply Chain; (7) Pride, Responsibility, and Care in Job; (8) Specialized Knowledge, Skills, and Commitment; (9) Unique, Positive Characteristics of the Job; and (10) Work Environment. Transporters expressed a strong sense of pride and responsibility in their work and viewed their role as essential to ensuring the well-being of cattle during transportation. However, they also identified several challenges, including ensuring human and animal safety, dealing with inadequate facilities, and a lack of cohesion among different supply chain sectors. As vital links between supply chain sectors, transporters offer unique insights. Listening to their daily observations could lead to improvements in operational efficiency and cattle welfare.
Article
Full-text available
L'élevage assume une multitude rôles au sein des sociétés humaines. La relation entre l'Homme et les animaux de ferme a toujours été privilégiée dans la constitution de l'identité même de ces sociétés. La montée en puissance récente d'enjeux nouveaux, comme les risques induits par le changement climatique, les envies d'une alimentation avec de moindres effets environnementaux ou encore la considération du bien-être animal, ont aujourd'hui des effets directs sur la manière avec laquelle la science considère l'élevage. Dans cet article, un état des lieux des avancées du savoir par rapport à cinq enjeux sociaux émergents de l'élevage est dressé, à partir d'une bibliographie sélective : i) élevage et innovation sociale, ii) gestion de la contrainte travail en élevage, iii) insectes comme source de nutriments pour les animaux d'élevage et pour les humains, iv) bien-être animal, et v) rôles des aliments d'origine animale dans la diète humaine. Ceci permet d'évaluer les complexités des interventions futures des sciences animales, dans leurs dimensions les plus larges, et les portées de leurs champs de recherche. Abstract-Livestock assumes a multitude of roles within human societies. The relationship between humans and farm animals has always been privileged in the constitution of the very identity of human societies. The recent emergence of new issues, such as the risks induced by climate change, the desire for food with less environmental harms or the consideration of animal welfare, have nowadays direct effects on the way with which science considers livestock rearing. In this article, an inventory of advances in knowledge in relation to emerging social stakes linked to livestock farming is drawn up, based on a selective bibliography. These are related to five topics identified as priorities: i) breeding and social innovation, ii) management of work constraints within livestock farms, iii) insects as a source of nutrients for animals and humans, iv) animal welfare and v) the roles of animal source foods in the human diet. This makes it possible to assess the complexities of future investigations in animal sciences, in their broadest dimensions, and the scopes of their research fields.
Article
Full-text available
The analysis of animal behavior, in particular suckling sows, allows us to identify problems of their well-being and health at the initial stages, which ensures an increase in reproductive productivity and profitability of the herd. Most often, the individual characteristics of animals associated with the type of higher nervous activity remain beyond the attention of breeders. The assessment of behavioral reactions of suckling sows with different levels of reproductive qualities (multiple fertility, lactation (nest weight at 21 days), the number of piglets in the nest at 2 months) during the period of the highest daily activity was carried out. The time spent by the sow on feeding piglets, motor activity, rest, feeding, research behavior, urination and defecation were recorded. As a result of the studies, no behavioral abnormalities were found in lactating sows that reduce their well-being, as indicated by the percentage distribution of animal activity types. A significant share in the ethological profile of animals was occupied by rest, the values of which ranged from 33.3 to 45.8%. Motor activity accounted for 10.4 to 20.8% of the studied time budget. On average, a sow spent a third of the daily activity budget on feeding and feeding piglets. The tendency to an advantage in feeding time of piglets of more fertile and dairy sows over the rest is shown. Suckling sows with productivity indicators above the average values in the sample were characterized by slightly less research activity, which probably reflects their individual characteristics.
Article
Full-text available
Simple Summary The quality of the human-animal relationship plays a central role in determining animal welfare. In this study, we assessed the relationship between stockperson behavior and buffalo behavior. In particular, during milking, we recorded the behavior of stockpeople in terms of quality and quantity of interactions, and we recorded the behavior of animals in terms of restlessness, whereas at the feeding place, we measured the avoidance distance. Avoidance distance of an animal can be defined as the distance to which the animal will allow an unknown person to approach before moving to the side or away. We found that a high percentage of negative stockperson interactions (shouting, talking impatiently, slapping, and handling forcefully) were associated with a high avoidance distance at the feeding place and restlessness during milking. Therefore, appropriate stockpeople training should be conducted to improve the human-animal relationship with positive effects on animal welfare, productivity, and stockpeople safety. Abstract This study aimed to assess the relationship between stockperson behavior and buffalo behavior. The research was carried out in 27 buffalo farms. The behavior of stockpeople and animals during milking and the avoidance distance at the feeding place were recorded. Recordings were repeated within one month to assess test-retest reliability. A high degree of test-retest reliability was observed for all the variables with Spearman rank correlation coefficients (rs) ranging from 0.578 (p = 0.002, df = 25) for the number of kicks performed during milking to 0.937 (p < 0.001, df = 25) for the percentage of animals moving when approached by ≤ 0.5 m. The number of negative stockperson interactions correlated positively with the number of kicks during milking (rs = 0.421, p < 0.028, df = 25) and the percentage of animals injected with oxytocin (rs = 0.424, p < 0.027), whereas the percentage of negative stockperson interactions correlated positively with the percentage of buffaloes moving when approached at a distance >1 m (rs = 0.415, p < 0.031, df = 25). In a subsample of 14 farms, milk yield was correlated positively with the number of positive interactions (rs = 0.588, p < 0.027, df = 12) and correlated negatively with the number of steps performed by the animals during milking (rs = −0.820, p < 0.001, df = 12). This study showed that the quality of stockpeople interactions may affect buffalo behavior and production.
Chapter
This book contains articles on the quantitative and molecular genetics of poultry, the uses of transgenic technology, and genetics of disease resistance and transmission. It is divided into 4 parts: problems and issues associated with poultry breeding (part 1; 6 chapters); breeding strategies and objectives (part 2; 9 chapters); disease resistance and transmission (part 3; 7 chapters); and the use of genomics and bioinformatics in poultry (part 4; 12 chapters).
Chapter
The second edition of this book contains chapters that discuss the role of the stockperson in animal welfare, including attitudes, human-animal interactions, human and animal behaviour and improvement of human-animal interactions in animal production. This book is intended for those with an interest in human-animal interactions, including trainers, livestock farm managers, students and academics.
Chapter
This third edition book with 19 chapters on domestic animal ethology is divided into two parts: Part A contains the basic elements of animal behaviour (chapter I, the study of animal behaviour and its application; chapter II, behaviour genetics, evolution and domestication; chapter III, behaviour and physiology; chapter IV, motivation and the organization of behaviour; chapter V, learning and cognition; chapter VI, social and reproductive behaviour; chapter VII, play behaviour; chapter VIII, introduction to animal personality; chapter IX, animal behaviour, stress and welfare; and chapter X, human-animal relations) and Part B discusses species-specific behaviour of some important domestic animals (chapter XI, behaviour of domesticated birds: chickens, turkeys and ducks; chapter XII, behaviour of horses; chapter XIII, behaviour of cattle; chapter XIV, behaviour of sheep and goats; chapter XV, behaviour of pigs; chapter XVI, behaviour of dogs; chapter XVII, behaviour of cats; chapter XVIII, behaviour of foxes and minks kept for fur production; and chapter XIX, behaviour of laboratory mice and rats). The book is targeted towards veterinarians, veterinary nurses and technicians, farm owners and managers, animal scientists, animal behaviourists and students.
Article
We aimed to determine how research regarding farmers' personalities and attitudes as risk factors is reported (methodological approaches to assessing, extracting, and processing data and analyzing risk factors) and to explore evidence for the effect of farmers' attitudes and personalities on dairy cattle health, welfare, productivity, and management. Therefore, we conducted a systematic review of studies on personality and attitude as risk factors for dairy cattle health, welfare, productivity, and farm management. Database searches captured 1,144 records, and 38 were finally included in the review. Thirty-three manuscripts assessed farmers' attitudes, 1 assessed their personalities, and 4 assessed both as risk factors. These potential risk factors were checked for relationships with more than 50 different outcome variables regarding farm management (17 manuscripts), animal health (13 manuscripts), animal productivity (11 manuscripts), and animal welfare (4 manuscripts). The approaches to assessing risk factors and processing and interpreting data varied greatly; thus, drawing conclusions regarding the effects of attitude and personality as risk factors is impeded because manuscripts are difficult to compare. Our findings highlight the need for harmonization of attitudes and personality assessments in future research. Furthermore, researchers should carefully consider which depth of detail to apply when planning and evaluating related research. Nevertheless, results highlight the importance of the effect of personality and attitude on outcomes. Farmers' personality and attitudes are associated with dairy cattle health, welfare, productivity, and management. In general, attitudes indicating higher degrees of technical knowledge, affection with problems, perceived responsibility, perception of control of a situation, a better human-animal relationship, or a positive evaluation of the benefits of management decisions tended to affect outcomes in a beneficial way. “Agreeableness” and “conscientiousness” were shown to promote better farm performance, whereas “neuroticism” had a negative effect. Therefore, further research on attitude and personality and their consideration by professionals and decision-makers within the dairy sector and politics is strongly recommended. This might provide the chance to better understand the needs of dairy farmers and therefore develop tailored advice and support strategies to improve both satisfactory and constructive cooperation.
Chapter
Farm animals have had a crucial role in human development and have been of high importance until now in many diverse farming systems. For the future, animal production poses substantial opportunities as well as risks, regarding both food security of the growing human population, as well as climate effects, depending largely on the farming system. Human–animal interactions inevitably occur throughout the lives of farm animals. However, the frequency and types of interactions are extremely variable, both between and within husbandry systems. This chapter reviews the variation in interactions and relationships, the causes for these differences and the implications for animal and human welfare. It shows evidence not only for the direct effects of the stockpersons’ relationship towards the animals in interactions and thus animal welfare, but also for the indirect effects via decision making. Lastly, societal implications and future research areas are discussed.
Article
Regular visual presence of humans is known to reduce chickens’ human-generated stress responses. Here we questioned whether, more than mere visual presence, human behaviour affects laying hen behaviour and subsequently their offspring’s behaviour. We hypothesized that human behaviour triggers maternal effects via variations in yolk hormone levels. For five consecutive weeks, two groups of hens were exposed to the same durations of human presence (30 min twice a day, five days a week) but the behaviour of the human differed between groups. The first group (H+) was exposed to predictable arrival of the experimenter, slow movements combined with static presence, stroking during handling and human voice. Whereas the second group of hens (H-) was exposed to unpredictable arrival of the experimenter which remained silent, in motion, and did not provide stroking during handling. At the end of the treatment, we evaluated egg quality and offspring behaviour. We found that avoidance of the experimenter by H+ hens but not by H- hens decreased significantly. Fertility rates and concentrations of yolk progesterone and estradiol in H+ hens’ eggs were higher than in H- hens’ eggs. Fear of humans, neophobia or the capacity to solve a detour task did not differ significantly between H+ and H- chicks. Social discrimination tests showed that H+ chicks but not H- chicks typically preferred a familiar conspecific to a stranger. These results show that, with the same duration in the presence of the birds, humans through their behaviour engender variations in fertility rates, yolk hormone levels and transgenerational effects on social skills. Rarely explored, our data suggest that maternal effects influence filial imprinting. These data have broad implications for laboratory, commercial systems and conservatory programs where the inevitable presence of humans could trigger maternal effects on offspring phenotype.