ArticlePDF Available

Stroboscopic Vision as a Dynamic Sensory Reweighting Alternative to the Sensory Organization Test

Authors:
  • Houston Methodist Research Institute

Abstract and Figures

Context: The sensory organization test (SOT) is a standard for quantifying sensory dependence via sway-referenced conditions (sway-referenced support and sway-referenced vision [SRV]). However, the SOT is limited to expensive equipment. Thus, a practical version of the SOT is more commonly employed-the clinical test for sensory integration in balance; however, it fails to induce postural instability to the level of SRV. Objective: Determine if Stroboscopic vision (SV), characterized by intermittent visual blocking, may provide an alternative to the SRV for assessing postural stability. Design: Descriptive laboratory study. Setting: Research laboratory. Participants: Eighteen participants (9 males, 9 females; age = 22.1 [2.1] y, height = 169.8 [8.5] cm, weight = 66.5 [10.6] kg). Intervention: Participants completed the SOT conditions, and then repeated SOT conditions 2 and 5 with SV created by specialized eyewear. Main outcome measures: A repeated-measures analysis of variance was completed on the time-to-boundary metrics of center-of-pressure excursion in the anteroposterior and mediolateral directions in order to determine the difference between the full-vision, SV, and SRV conditions. Results: Postural stability with either SRV or SV was significantly worse than with full vision (P < .05), with no significant difference between SV and SRV (P > .05). Limits of agreement analysis revealed similar effects of SV and SRV except for unstable surface mediolateral time-to-boundary. Conclusions: In general, SV was found to induce a degree of postural instability similar to that induced by SRV, indicating that SV could be a portable and relatively inexpensive alternative for the assessment of sensory dependence and reweighting.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Stroboscopic Vision as a Dynamic Sensory Reweighting Alternative
to the Sensory Organization Test
Kyung-Min Kim, Joo-Sung Kim, Jeonghoon Oh, and Dustin R. Grooms
Context:The sensory organization test (SOT) is a standard for quantifying sensory dependence via sway-referenced
conditions (sway-referenced support and sway-referenced vision [SRV]). However, the SOT is limited to expensive
equipment. Thus, a practical version of the SOT is more commonly employedthe clinical test for sensory integration in
balance; however, it fails to induce postural instability to the level of SRV. Objective:Determine if Stroboscopic vision
(SV), characterized by intermittent visual blocking, may provide an alternative to the SRV for assessing postural stability.
Design:Descriptive laboratory study. Setting:Research laboratory. Participants:Eighteen participants (9 males, 9 females;
age = 22.1 [2.1] y, height = 169.8 [8.5] cm, weight = 66.5 [10.6] kg). Intervention:Participants completed the SOT
conditions, and then repeated SOT conditions 2 and 5 with SV created by specialized eyewear. Main Outcome Measures:
A repeated-measures analysis of variance was completed on the time-to-boundary metrics of center-of-pressure excursion in
the anteroposterior and mediolateral directions in order to determine the difference between the full-vision, SV, and SRV
conditions. Results:Postural stability with either SRV or SV was signicantly worse than with full vision (P<.05), with no
signicant difference between SV and SRV (P>.05). Limits of agreement analysis revealed similar effects of SV and SRV
except for unstable surface mediolateral time-to-boundary. Conclusions:In general, SV was found to induce a degree of
postural instability similar to that induced by SRV, indicating that SV could be a portable and relatively inexpensive
alternative for the assessment of sensory dependence and reweighting.
Keywords:balance/posture, biomechanics, instrument-assisted interventions, sensory integration
Sensory dependence is the preferential contribution of visual,
vestibular, and somatosensory feedback for completing a motor
task or maintaining stability and can be modied by pathologies,
task conditions, or training.
1,2
The sensory organization test (SOT)
is the gold standard for quantifying sensory dependence by utiliz-
ing sway-referenced conditions. Specically, the SOT engages a
sway-referenced support condition to knockdown the somatosen-
sory system and a sway-referenced vision (SRV) condition to
knockdown the visual system. The SOT provides a standardized
assessment of sensory contributions to postural stability and has
been used extensively to evaluate concussion and fall risk and
patients with balance disorders in order to inform targeted therapies
for specic sensory decits.
1,3,4
However, the SOT is limited to expensive equipment; thus, the
clinical test for sensory integration in balance (CTSIB) was
developed.
5
In the CTSIB, the sway-referenced condition of the
SOT is imitated using a foam pad, and SRV is imitated using a
white dome over the head.
5
The foam-induced postural instability
is comparable to the sway-referenced support condition, but the
dome is unable to induce instability to the level of the SRV.
68
Thus, there remains a need to develop an adequate surrogate for
SRV conditions for the CTSIB.
Stroboscopic vision (SV) may provide an alternative to SRV
and is characterized by intermittent visual blocking via glasses
or goggles. SV has recently been shown to modify visual depen-
dence and postural instability in both stable and unstable surface
conditions.
9
However, a comparison to the gold standard sensory
dependence for vision SRV condition has yet to be examined. If SV
induces postural instability similar to that induced by SRV, then it
may offer a viable option for a visually perturbed sensory depen-
dence assessment. Thus, the purpose of this study was to compare
the postural instability effects of SV against SRV on both stable
and unstable surfaces. We hypothesized that both SV and SRV
would signicantly increase postural instability relative to full
vision, but postural instability between SV and SRV would not
be different and have high agreement.
Methods
Eighteen young adults (9 males, 9 females; age = 22.1 [2.1] y,
height = 169.8 [8.5] cm, weight = 66.5 [10.6] kg) without any
history of musculoskeletal injury in the past 6 months or of vision
or balance disorders participated. The University of Miami
Institutional Review Board approved the study protocol requiring
informed consent.
All participants completed the SOT with the NeuroCom
EquiTest (Neurocom International Inc, Clackamas, OR), consistent
with the testing protocol described previously.
4
However, SOT
conditions 2 and 5, in which participants are asked to close their
eyes during testing, were replaced with SV created by specialized
eyewear (Nike SPARQ Vapor Strobes; Nike Inc, Beaverton, OR)
K.-M. Kim, J.-S. Kim, and Oh are with the Department of Kinesiology and Sport
Sciences, University of Miami, Miami, FL, USA. Grooms is with the Division of
Athletic Training, School of Applied Health Sciences and Wellness, College of
Health Sciences and Professions; and the Ohio Musculoskeletal & Neurological
Institute, Ohio University, Athens, OH, USA. Grooms (groomsd@ohio.edu)is
corresponding author.
1
Journal of Sport Rehabilitation, (Ahead of Print)
https://doi.org/10.1123/jsr.2019-0466
© 2020 Human Kinetics, Inc. TECHNICAL REPORT
that intermittently cycles between opaque and transparent for 100
milliseconds at a time.
9
Participants performed bipedal stances that
lasted for 20 seconds and were repeated 3 times. The averages were
used for statistical analysis. The study outcomes were time-to-
boundary (TTB) parameters of center-of-pressure excursion. We
computed TTB measures such as mean and SD of the minima in the
anteroposterior (AP) and mediolateral (ML) directions using pre-
viously reported methods.
10
A lower TTB value reects poorer
postural stability.
For each of the TTB measures, a repeated-measures analysis
of variance was performed to determine if there was a difference
in postural stability on the xed (stable) surface between 3 visual
conditions as follows: (1) full vision (SOT condition 1), (2) SV
(SOT condition 2), and (3) SRV (SOT condition 3). The same
analysis was repeated for these conditions on the sway-
referenced (unstable) surface (SOT conditions 46). In addition,
Cohen deffect sizes and associated 95% condence intervals
(CIs) were calculated to provide the magnitude of postural
instability with either SV or SRV compared to full vision using
pooled SD. Cohen guidelines were used to determine the
strength of the effect size as follows: (1) less than 0.2 = trivial,
(2) from 0.21 to 0.5 = small, (3) from 0.51 to 0.8 = moderate, and
(4) greater than 0.8 = large.
11
To assess agreement between SRV and SV, we completed a
one-sample ttest to determine difference from zero for each
balance measure, BlandAltman plots to assess agreement for
each measure, and Pearson rvalues with 95% CIs between
conditions for all measures. We also calculated R
2
values to
determine the variance of SRV performance explained by SV.
All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS statistical
software (version 24.0; IBM Corp, Armonk, NY) with the alpha
level set a priori at P.05.
Results
Descriptive data for the 3 visual conditions with the xed and
sway-referenced surfaces are presented in Tables 1and 2,respec-
tively. For postural stability on a xed surface, there were
signicant differences between the 3 visual conditions for the
AP mean and SD measures (Table 1). Postural stability with either
SRV or SV in the AP direction was signicantly worse than with
full vision, but there were no signicant differences between SRV
and SV. The disruptive effects of SRV were small (close to
moderate), and SV effects were moderate, but there was no
difference in effect size between SRV and SV relative to FV,
because their respective 95% CI overlapped. In contrast to AP
postural instability, ML stability was unaffected by SRV or SV on
the stable surface.
Thereweresignicant differences for all TTB measures for
postural stability on the unstable (sway-referenced) surface
(Table 2). Postural stability with SRV or SV was signicantly
worse relative to full vision, but SV again did not signicantly
differ from SRV. Postural instability with SV or SRV in the
AP direction was of large magnitude relative to FV. For ML
postural instability, SV also had large effects relative to FV,
while SRV had small to moderate effects. Again, relative to FV,
the 95% CIs of both SV and SRV overlapped, indicating no
effect differences.
The limits of agreement analysis also largely supported a
similar effect of SV relative to SRV. On the xed surface, all
TTB metrics had good agreement (Figure 1) and moderate to
high correlation (Table 1,r=.49.77). On the unstable surface,
the TTB mean and SD in the AP direction had good agreement
(Figure 2), with moderate to high correlation (Table 2,
r=.65.75). However, TTB mean and SD in the ML direction
had condition differences greater than zero but still had
high correlations (Table 2,r=.79.82). The R
2
values for
each variable between SRV and SV are reported in Tables 1
and 2.
Discussion
We found both SRV and SV signicantly impaired standing
balance relative to full vision, with no signicant difference
between SV and SRV. Specically, we observed small to mod-
erate effects for AP postural instability on a stable surface, with
no ML effect and moderate to large effects for AP and ML
postural instability on an unstable surface with both visual
conditions. The limits of agreement analysis indicated that SV
and SRV induced comparable effects in all conditions for all
outcome metrics except for the ML direction during the unstable
surface condition. This lack of agreement may have been due to
the different mechanisms of visual knockdown. The SV condi-
tion reduces visual feedback by 50% (strobe rate of 100-ms
transparent\100-ms opaque) and is not specictoanAPorML
corrective strategy, whereas the SRV produces inaccurate visual
feedback primarily in the AP direction. Nonetheless, SV ac-
counted for approximately 62% to 67% of the variance in SRV
for ML TTB mean and SD; the remaining 33% to 38% can
potentially be attributed to differences in the ways in which
visual information was manipulated. The different mechanisms
of visual perturbation, while resulting in generally similar pos-
tural instability, should be considered for SOT in patients with
postural instability depending on an origin of postural instability
(reduced sensory feedback versus inability to recognize accurate
sensory input).
Stroboscopic vision can be utilized dynamically and is
portable and relatively inexpensive ($200 or less), making it
an attractive alternative for sensory reweighting assessment.
The need for such a tool is especially apparent in settings dealing
with highly active patients, such as in sports medicine, with
emerging evidence indicating that common musculoskeletal con-
ditions induce sensory reweighting.
1215
SOT may not be effec-
tive in athletic populations due to its reliance on a static bipedal
stance not being challenging enough to discover postural insta-
bility that may be manifested during more aggressive athletic
maneuvers. Thus, SV could be an alternative for progressive
sensory reweighting in more active populations. Of note, our
ndings are limited to individuals without current injury and to
the NeuroCom system. Patients, especially those with disorders
affecting balance, may respond to SV differently; this requires
future research.
Conclusions
Stroboscopic vision was found to induce a degree of postural
instability similar to that induced by SRV and can be used in the
assessment of sensory reweighting as part of CTSIB, allowing future
investigators and clinicians to engage in more dynamic postural
stability assessments.
(Ahead of Print)
2K.-M. Kim et al
Table 1 TTB Measures of Center-of-Pressure Excursion During 20-second Bipedal Stance on Fixed (Stable) Surface
TTB
parameters RM ANOVA FV SRV SV
Effect size
a
One-sample
ttest
b
Pearson r
c
R
2
FV and SRV FV and SV SRV and SV
ML mean (s) F
2,34
= 1.52;
P= .23
64.82
(15.15)
62.27
(12.01)
59.45
(12.62)
0.19
(0.84 to 0.47)
0.38
(1.04 to 0.28)
0.22
(0.43 to 0.88)
t
17
= 1.24;
P= .23
.71 (.36, .88);
P<.01**
.50
(.13, .77)
AP mean (s) F
2,34
= 3.73;
P= .03*
35.25
(12.60)
30.01
(9.87)
26.86
(13.56)
0.46
(1.12 to 0.20)
0.64
(1.31 to 0.03)
0.27
(0.93 to 0.38)
t
17
= 1.56;
P= .14
.77 (.47, .91);
P<.01**
.59
(.22, .83)
ML SD (s) F
2,34
= 1.47;
P= .24
40.14
(9.14)
38.19
(7.46)
36.08
(10.82)
0.23
(0.89 to 0.42)
0.41
(1.07 to 0.25)
0.29
(0.37 to 0.95)
t
17
= .92;
P= .37
.49 (.30, .78);
P= .04**
.24
(.09, .61)
AP SD (s) F
2,34
= 3.63;
P= .04*
26.60
(10.68)
22.49
(7.34)
20.04
(9.38)
0.45
(1.11 to 0.21)
0.65
(1.32 to 0.02)
0.23
(0.43 to 0.88)
t
17
= 1.69;
P= .11
.75 (.44, .90);
P<.01**
.56
(.19, .81)
Abbreviations: AP, anteroposterior; FV, full vision; ML, mediolateral; RM ANOVA, repeated-measures analysis of variance; SRV, sway-referenced vision; SV, stroboscopic vision; TTB, time-to-boundary. Note: Bolded R
2
values
were calculated from the Pearson correlation coefcients.
a
Cohensdestimate of effect size was calculated between 2 of 3 visual conditions using pooled standard deviation, along with its associated 95% condence interval. A lower TTB measure indicates poorer balance; a negative value
of effect size represents a decline in balance performance.
b
One-sample ttests were performed for each measure to determine the difference between SRV and SV from zero.
c
Pearson correlation coefcients (r) were estimated
between SRV and SV conditions for each measure, along with their 95% condence intervals.
*AP postural control with SRV or SV was signicantly worse than postural control with full vision; SV did not signicantly differ from SRV. **Signicant correlation between SRV and SV conditions.
(Ahead of Print) 3
Table 2 TTB Measures of Center-of-Pressure Excursion During 20-Second Bipedal Stance on Sway-Referenced (Unstable) Surface
TTB
parameters
RM
ANOVA FV SRV SV
Effect size
a
One-sample
ttest
b
Pearson r
c
R
2
FV and SRV FV and SV SRV and SV
ML mean (s) F
2,34
= 7.77;
P<.01*
50.69
(10.18)
44.14
(10.95)
34.99
(17.65)
0.62
(1.29 to 0.05)
1.09
(1.79 to 0.39)
0.62
(1.29 to 0.05)
t
17
= 3.44;
P<.01
.79 (.51, .92);
P<.01**
.62
(.26, .85)
AP mean (s) F
2,34
= 16.56;
P<.01*
17.64
(4.86)
12.17
(3.94)
12.35
(4.43)
1.24
(1.97 to 0.50)
1.14
(1.86 to 0.41)
0.04
(0.61 to 0.70)
t
17
=.16;
P= .88
.75 (.44, .90);
P<.01**
.56
(.19, .81)
ML SD (s) F
2,34
= 5.77;
P<.01*
30.76
(5.92)
28.11
(7.03)
21.98
(10.91)
0.41
(1.07 to 0.25)
1.00
(1.69 to 0.31)
0.67
(1.34 to 0.01)
t
17
= 3.95;
P<.01
.82 (.57, .93);
P<.01**
.67
(.32, .86)
AP SD (s) F
2,34
= 7.34;
P<.01*
15.19
(4.62)
11.14
(3.12)
9.58
(5.70)
1.03
(1.72 to 0.33)
1.08
(1.78 to 0.38)
0.34
(1.00 to 0.32)
t
17
= 1.51;
P= .15
.65 (.26, .86);
P<.01**
.42
(.07, .74)
Abbreviations: AP, anteroposterior; FV, full vision; ML, mediolateral; RM ANOVA, repeated-measures analysis of variance; ML, mediolateral; SRV, sway-referenced vision; SV, stroboscopic vision; TTB, time-to-boundary. Note:
Bolded R
2
values were calculated from the Pearson correlation coefcients.
a
Cohensdestimate of effect size was calculated between 2 of 3 visual conditions using pooled standard deviation, along with its associated 95% condence interval. A lower TTB measure indicates poorer balance; a negative value of
effect size represents a decline in balance performance.
b
One-sample ttests were performed for each measure to determine the difference between SRV and SV from zero.
c
Pearson correlation coefcients (r) were estimated between
SRV and SV conditions for each measure, along with their 95% condence intervals.
*AP postural control with SRV or SV was signicantly worse than postural control with full vision, while SV did not signicantly differ from SRV. **Signicant correlation between SRV and SV conditions.
4(Ahead of Print)
Figure 1 BlandAltman plots for time-to-boundary (TTB) measures of center-of-pressure excursion during 20-second bipedal stance on xed (stable) surface; the dotted line indicates
zero, the solid line represents the average difference (bias line), and the dashed lines show the 95% condence intervals of the average differences. AP indicates anteroposterior; ML, mediolateral;
SRV, sway-referenced vision; SV, stroboscopic vision; TTB, time-to-boundary.
(Ahead of Print) 5
Figure 2 BlandAltman plots for time-to-boundary (TTB) measures of center-of-pressure excursion during 20-second bipedal stance on sway-referenced (unstable) surface; the dotted line
indicates zero, the solid line represents the average difference (bias line), and the dashed lines show the 95% condence intervals of the average differences. AP indicate anteroposterior; ML,
mediolateral; SRV, sway-referenced vision; SV, stroboscopic vision; TTB, time-to-boundary.
6(Ahead of Print)
Acknowledgments
This research did not receive any specic grant from funding agencies in
the public, commercial, or not-for-prot sectors. The authors have no
conicts of interest associated with this work.
References
1. Peterka RJ. Sensory integration for human balance control. Handb
Clin Neurol. 2018;159:2742. PubMed ID: 30482320 doi:10.1016/
B978-0-444-63916-5.00002-1
2. Forbes PA, Chen A, Blouin JS. Sensorimotor control of standing
balance. Handb Clin Neurol. 2018;159:6183. PubMed ID:
30482333 doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-63916-5.00004-5
3. Pierchala K, Lachowska M, Wysocki J, Morawski K, Niemczyk K.
Evaluation of the Sensory Organization Test to differentiate non-
fallers from single- and multi-fallers. Adv Clin Exp Med. 2019;28:
3543. PubMed ID: 30058785 doi:10.17219/acem/76374
4. Vander Vegt CB, Register-Mihalik JK, Ford CB, Rodrigo CJ,
Guskiewicz KM, Mihalik JP. Baseline concussion clinical measures
are related to sensory organization and balance. Med Sci Sports Exerc.
2019;51(2):264270. PubMed ID: 30239494 doi:10.1249/MSS.
0000000000001789
5. Shumway-Cook A, Horak FB. Assessing the inuence of sensory
interaction of balance. Suggestion from the eld. Phys Ther.1986;
66(10):15481550. PubMed ID: 3763708 doi:10.1093/ptj/66.10.1548
6. Cohen H, Blatchly CA, Gombash LL. A study of the clinical test
of sensory interaction and balance. Phys Ther. 1993;73(6):346351;
discussion 5154. PubMed ID: 8497509 doi:10.1093/ptj/73.6.346
7. Hong SK, Park JH, Kwon SY, Kim JS, Koo JW. Clinical efcacy
of the Romberg test using a foam pad to identify balance problems:
a comparative study with the sensory organization test. Eur Arch
Otorhinolaryngol. 2015;272(10):27412747. PubMed ID: 25200890
doi:10.1007/s00405-014-3273-2
8. Wrisley DM, Whitney SL. The effect of foot position on the modied
clinical test of sensory interaction and balance. Arch Phys Med
Rehabil. 2004;85(2):335338. PubMed ID: 14966723 doi:10.1016/
j.apmr.2003.03.005
9. Kim KM, Kim JS, Grooms DR. Stroboscopic vision to induce sensory
reweighting during postural control. J Sport Rehabil. 2017;26(5):1
11. PubMed ID: 28605310 doi:10.1123/jsr.2017-0035
10. Hertel J, Olmsted-Kramer LC, Challis JH. Time-to-boundary mea-
sures of postural control during single leg quiet standing. J Appl
Biomech. 2006;22(1):6773. PubMed ID: 16760569 doi:10.1123/
jab.22.1.67
11. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd
ed. Hillsdale, MI: Lawrence Erlbaum Association; 1988.
12. Song K, Burcal CJ, Hertel J, Wikstrom EA. Increased visual use in
chronic ankle instability: a meta-analysis. Med Sci Sports Exerc.
2016;48(10):20462056. PubMed ID: 27635773 doi:10.1249/MSS.
0000000000000992
13. Wikstrom EA, Song K, Pietrosimone BG, Blackburn JT, Padua DA.
Visual utilization during postural control in anterior cruciate
ligament- decient and -reconstructed patients: systematic reviews
and meta-analyses. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2017;98(10):20522065.
PubMed ID: 28483655 doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2017.04.010
14. Claeys K, Brumagne S, Dankaerts W, Kiers H, Janssens L. Decreased
variability in postural control strategies in young people with non-
specic low back pain is associated with altered proprioceptive
reweighting. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2011;111(1):115123. PubMed
ID: 20824281 doi:10.1007/s00421-010-1637-x
15. Kim KM, Kim JS, Oh J, Lee SY. Time-to-boundary analysis of postural
control following acute lateral ankle sprain. Gait Posture. 2019;67:151
153. PubMed ID: 30340127 doi:10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.10.002
(Ahead of Print)
Strobes and SOT 7
... The CNS adjusts the weight of sensory inputs based on factors like movement complexity, environmental features, and signal reliability [3]. For example, during predictable steady-state movements, the CNS may down-weight the contribution of sensory afferents broadly [4]. Also, in response to disruptions in visual or somatosensory input, the CNS may reweight the contribution of individual sensory inputs [5,6]. ...
... The technology provides potential for expanding the scope of investigation into the influence of continuous visual input on motor control during DJs. By reducing the reliability of visual information, stroboscopic vision might induce a compensatory up-regulation of other sensory modalities to support the execution of a desired task [4]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Researchers commonly use the ‘free-fall’ paradigm to investigate motor control during landing impacts, particularly in drop landings and depth jumps (DJ). While recent studies have focused on the impact of vision on landing motor control, previous research fully removed continuous visual input, limiting ecological validity. The aim of this investigation was to evaluate the effects of stroboscopic vision on depth jump (DJ) motor control. Ground reaction forces (GRF) and lower-extremity surface electromyography (EMG) were collected for 20 young adults (11 male; 9 female) performing six depth jumps (0.51 m drop height) in each of two visual conditions (full vision vs. 3 Hz stroboscopic vision). Muscle activation magnitude was estimated from EMG signals using root-mean-square amplitudes (RMS) over specific time intervals (150 ms pre-impact; 30–60 ms, 60–85 ms, and 85–120 ms post-impact). The main effects of and interactions between vision and trial number were assessed using two-way within-subjects repeated measures analyses of variance. Peak GRF was 6.4% greater, on average, for DJs performed with stroboscopic vision compared to full vision (p = 0.042). Tibialis anterior RMS EMG during the 60–85 ms post-impact time interval was 14.1% lower for DJs performed with stroboscopic vision (p = 0.020). Vastus lateralis RMS EMG during the 85–120 ms post-impact time interval was 11.8% lower for DJs performed with stroboscopic vision (p = 0.017). Stroboscopic vision altered DJ landing mechanics and lower-extremity muscle activation. The observed increase in peak GRF and reduction in RMS EMG of the tibialis anterior and vastus lateralis post-landing may signify a higher magnitude of lower-extremity musculotendinous stiffness developed pre-landing. The results indicate measurable sensorimotor disruption for DJs performed with stroboscopic vision, warranting further research and supporting the potential use of stroboscopic vision as a sensorimotor training aid in exercise and rehabilitation. Stroboscopic vision could induce beneficial adaptations in multisensory integration, applicable to restoring sensorimotor function after injury and preventing injuries in populations experiencing landing impacts at night (e.g., military personnel).
... This may lead to increased sensitisation and better visual skills when they return to normal visual conditions, as well as resulting in more effective utilisation of other sensory modalities, such as kinaesthetic awareness and auditory information [1]. Indeed, stroboscopic visual perturbation has been shown to be a valid assessment method for sensory dependence, promoting sensory reweighting and improving balance control [19][20][21]. ...
... Only original research articles using human participantsAU : PleasenotethatasperPLOSstyle; dono were included, utilising stroboscopic eyewear during a defined training period to produce intermittent vision and impact on visual and/or motor performance. Studies were excluded if they investigated the immediate influence of SV on task performance [19][20][21][25][26][27][28][29][30][31], if SVT was applied as part of a multifaceted visual training intervention [32][33][34], if the study used alternative methods of delivering strobe effects (e.g., vertical cyclinders) [35], or if it investigated effects on motion sickness without a visual or functional motor outcome [12]. Three studies were excluded on the basis that they were theses generated from graduate projects and not fully peer reviewed [14,36,37]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Visual problems are common in people who have neurological injury or disease, with deficits linked to postural control and gait impairment. Vision therapy could be a useful intervention for visual impairment in various neurological conditions such as stroke, head injury, or Parkinson’s disease. Stroboscopic visual training (SVT) has been shown to improve aspects of visuomotor and cognitive performance in healthy populations, but approaches vary with respect to testing protocols, populations, and outcomes. The purpose of this structured review was to examine the use of strobe glasses as a training intervention to inform the development of robust protocols for use in clinical practice. Within this review, any studies using strobe glasses as a training intervention with visual or motor performance–related outcomes was considered. PubMed, Scopus, and ProQuest databases were searched in January 2023. Two independent reviewers (JD and RM) screened articles that used strobe glasses as a training tool. A total of 33 full text articles were screened, and 15 met inclusion/exclusion criteria. Reported outcomes of SVT included improvements in short–term memory, attention, and visual response times, with emerging evidence for training effects translating to balance and physical performance. However, the lack of standardisation across studies for SVT protocols, variation in intervention settings, duration and outcomes, and the limited evidence within clinical populations demonstrates that further work is required to determine optimal strobe dosage and delivery. This review highlights the potential benefits, and existing research gaps regarding the use of SVT in clinical practice, with recommendations for clinicians considering adopting this technology as part of future studies in this emerging field.
... People with PD (and older adults) become increasingly reliant on visuo-cognition for mobility, which may require sensory reweighting training to overcome functional problems and improve performance. Stroboscopic visual perturbation has been shown to be a valid assessment method for sensory dependence and reweighting [7]. In addition to offering the SSS, Senaptec produce glasses which have liquid crystal lens technology that allows the lenses to strobe between clear and opaque ( Figure 1b). ...
... Studies on healthy individuals have shown that visuo-cognitive outcomes can predict real-world task performance, such as goal scoring ability in hockey players [4] and collision avoidance in football players [8]. Additionally, the use of Strobe Glasses as an intervention/training tool has been shown to improve eye-hand co-ordination, sensory weighting, and attentional capabilities in healthy populations [7,9,10]. ...
... SV may also facilitate sensory reweighting by causing greater reliance on non-visual inputs. Consequently, postural training combined with SV is presumed to enhance proprioceptive awareness and improve sensory integration [19,20]. For movements relying on internal body awareness, such as posture adjustments or blindfolded reaching, SV may improve the accurate estimation of internal body states within the egocentric reference frame by enhanced proprioceptive inputs [21]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Stroboscopic vision (SV), known for providing intermittent visual input, has been recently integrated into postural training to improve proprioceptive awareness. This research examined the impact of SV on cortico-posture coupling in older adults, along with the related changes in postural control throughout a spectrum of feedback and feedforward processes. Methods A total of thirty-three adults, averaging 66.1 ± 2.5 years of age, were tasked with maintaining an upright posture on a stabilometer, utilizing either complete or intermittent visual guidance. Stabilogram diffusion analysis (SDA) was employed to assess balance strategies based on postural sway, while phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) between postural fluctuations and scalp EEG provided insights into the associated neural control mechanisms. Results SV resulted in significantly increased postural sway as compared with that of full-vision feedback (p < 0.001). SDA results indicated greater critical point displacement (CD) (p < 0.001), short-term diffusion coefficients (Ds) (p < 0.001), and scaling exponents (Hs) (p = 0.014) under SV conditions. PAC analysis revealed that the coupling between the postural fluctuation phase and cortical oscillation amplitude in the theta and alpha bands of the fronto-central area was significantly greater in the SV condition than in the full-vision condition (p < 0.001). Additionally, SV led to increased beta PAC in the frontal and sensorimotor areas compared to that of full vision (p < 0.001), which negatively correlated to SV-dependent changes in open-loop gain (Hs) (p < 0.05). Conclusions SV transitions postural sway towards an open-loop process and influences cortico-posture interactions in older adults, emphasizing a neuromotor adaptation to the uncertainty in feedforward predictions when utilizing intermittent visual feedback.
... Evidence indicates that when either visual or proprioceptive input is restricted, the central nervous system recalibrates the contribution of individual sensory inputs [36]. This recalibration is likely more pronounced during stroboscopic training, as opposed to the reduced reliance on sensory inputs observed during steady-state movements [37]. The additional visual load introduced during training induces adaptive modifications in sensory integration, which subsequently enhances motor performance through the refinement or optimisation of sensory integration processes [38]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: In elite curling, precise time perception, speed control, and accuracy are critical components of performance. Stroboscopic training enhances visual processing speed, reaction time, motor skill control, and cognitive abilities by challenging the brain to make quick decisions with limited visual information. Purpose: This study aimed to investigate the impact of stroboscopic visual conditions on the key performance aspects of elite athletes in curling to determine whether these effects can be leveraged in long-term training to enhance elite curling performance. Methods: This study involved the participation of 32 national-level male curling athletes (n = 32, age: 19.9 ± 2.2 years, height: 178.0 ± 6.2 cm, body mass: 71.9 ± 10.6 kg, and training age: 2.7 ± 0.9 years). A cross-over controlled experiment was conducted, with participants randomly assigned to either a stroboscopic-first group (n = 16) or a control-first group (n = 16). Each participant completed tests under both stroboscopic and normal visual conditions, including assessments of time perception error, speed control error, and curling accuracy. Paired sample t-tests were employed to analyse performance differences across conditions, and two-factor ANOVA was used to analyse sequence effects. Bonferroni post-hoc tests were used to compare differences if the main effect was significant. Cohen’s d was used for two-group comparisons, whereas ηp² and Cohen’s f were used for comparisons involving three or more groups. Results: under stroboscopic conditions, participants experienced increased errors in time perception (p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.143), delivery speed control (p = 0.016, Cohen’s d = 0.448), and reduced accuracy (p = 0.029, Cohen’s d = 0.404). The sequence main effect on speed control error was significant (p = 0.025, ηp² = 0.081, Cohen’s f = 0.297). Conclusions: Stroboscopic visual conditions negatively impacted cognition (especially time perception) and delivery performance focused on speed control and accuracy in elite curling, highlighting the potential and feasibility of using stroboscopic training to enhance elite curling performance.
... This approach provides a refined method for creating postural stability tasks, offering insights into the predominant sensory system required for balance maintenance on unstable surfaces like foam [17]. By simulating less consistent visual environments, this innovative approach not only detects minor impairments but also offers a portable, cost-effective solution for widespread clinical use [19]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study explores the efficacy of integrating stroboscopic glasses with smartphone-based applications to evaluate postural control, offering a cost-effective alternative to traditional forceplate technology. Athletes, particularly those with visual and visuo-oculomotor enhancements due to sports, often suffer from injuries that necessitate reliance on visual inputs for balance—conditions that can be simulated and studied using visual perturbation methods such as stroboscopic glasses. These glasses intermittently occlude vision, mimicking visual impairments that are crucial in assessing dependency on visual information for postural stability. Participants performed these tasks under three visual conditions: full vision, partial vision occlusion via stroboscopic glasses, and no vision (eyes closed), on foam surfaces to induce postural instability. The use of a smartphone app to measure postural sway was validated against traditional force plate measurements, providing a comparative analysis of both tools under varied sensory conditions. We investigated postural parameters like anterior–posterior and medial–lateral sway ranges, root mean square values, 95% confidence ellipse area, and sway velocity and median dominant sway frequency from both the smartphone and the force plates. Our findings indicate that force plates exhibit high sensitivity to various visual conditions, as evidenced by significant differences observed in certain postural parameters, which were not detected by smartphone-based measurements. Overall, our findings indicate that smartphones show promise as a cost-effective alternative to force plate measurements for routine monitoring of postural control in sports, although they may not achieve the same level of accuracy as force plates. The integration of stroboscopic glasses further refined the assessment by effectively simulating visual impairments, thereby allowing precise evaluation of an individual’s ability to maintain balance under visually perturbed conditions.
... This response reflects a compensatory postural strategy. In another study [37], a comparative evaluation of tests that are used to assess sensory dependence was conducted. The sensory organization test (SOT) is a standard for quantifying sensory dependence via sway-referenced conditions (sway-referenced support and sway-referenced vision (SRV). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and Study Aim. Monitoring the condition of athletes is one of the leading tasks in sports science. Predicting success and skill growth is an integral part of monitoring. The fulfillment of this task requires the use of modern methods and the sensory integration analysis occupies an important place. The purpose of this article is to analyze publications on sensory integration and identify priority research areas in this field. Material and Methods. The bibliometric database Web of Science Core Collection (WoS) was analyzed. 388 sources that met the search criteria for 2013-2023 were selected for primary analysis. Bibliometric methods were used for processing the obtained information. The program VOSviewer 1.6.18 was used for keyword analysis and direct citation analysis with the construction of bibliometric maps, visualization of cluster density, and weight – citations. Results. The leading publication categories in WoS were Neurosciences, Sport Sciences, Psychology, Psychology Experimental, Physiology, Rehabilitation. The distribution of publications by years was fairly evenly. Most publications were related to Germany and the USA. The most frequent institutions were German Sport University Cologne, Universidade de Sao Paulo, Shanghai University of Sport, Institute National de la Sante et de la Recherche Medicale Inserm, University of Freiburg. The most cited authors were identified. The constructed bibliometric maps allowed us to identify the leading thematic areas of research and current research directions in the field. The two most important clusters were identified. The largest cluster reflects research on balance and posture and the use of special exercises for rehabilitation in different population groups. The second cluster includes the most significant keyword in the whole map – "sensorimotor integration". The works reflect studies of sensorimotor integration, the study of movement features, and coordination and control of movement at different levels. Conclusions. The conducted bibliometric analysis of publications confirms the relevance of sensorimotor integration. A stable interest in this problem has been determined. Research in this area has neurophysiological, sports-recreational, and medical-rehabilitation directions. Neurophysiological research is aimed at analyzing balance and equilibrium and the application of various tests and functional samples. The sport context studies sensory integration in different sports. Recreational publications have investigated sensory integration as a criterion of health. The therapeutic and rehabilitation focus explores the use of sensory integration as a criterion for treatment and rehabilitation. A characteristic feature is the intersection of these directions. There is a lack of studies devoted to sensory integration in sport dancing. There is a lack of studies aimed at studying the psychophysiological state. These types of research should be assessed as relevant.
... First, limiting the amount of visual input forces the sensorimotor system to be more efficient at integrating the minimal visual samples that are available, which ultimately leads to improved performance when vision is restored fully (Appelbaum et al., 2011;Wilkins & Appelbaum, 2020;Wilkins & Gray, 2015). The alternate view is that by limiting the amount of visual information that can be used to control a movement, the CNS is required to rely on other sensory sources such as proprioception or somatosensory feedback, which then lead to improved performance when combined with complete vision (Kim et al., 2017(Kim et al., , 2020Lee et al., 2022). While it is likely that both mechanisms are at play when training with stroboscopic glasses, in this section we will focus on the latter perspective due to the focus of this paper. ...
Article
Full-text available
Historically, research aimed at improving motor performance has largely focused on the neural processes involved in motor execution due to their role in muscle activation. However, accompanying somatosensory and proprioceptive sensory information is also vitally involved in performing motor skills. Here we review research from interdisciplinary fields to provide a description for how somatosensation informs the successful performance of motor skills as well as emphasize the need for careful selection of study methods to isolate the neural processes involved in somatosensory perception. We also discuss upcoming strategies of intervention that have been used to improve performance via somatosensory targets. We believe that a greater appreciation for somatosensation's role in motor learning and control will enable researchers and practitioners to develop and apply methods for the enhancement of human performance that will benefit clinical, healthy, and elite populations alike.
Article
OBJECTIVE: To evaluate if stroboscopic vision significantly affected dynamic balance performance on the YBT-LQ in athletes aged 12-18 years. DESIGN: Single group pre-test post-test quasi experimental study METHODS: Two hundred seventy-two participants aged 12-18 years, who participated in a school- or club-sponsored sport, performed the Y-Balance Test Lower Quarter (YBT-LQ) without and with stroboscopic vision. Reach distances were recorded for each participant with and without stroboscopic vision. Main effects were analyzed to interpret the effect of stroboscopic vision on YBT-LQ performance, and secondary effects were analyzed to interpret the effect on individual reach directions and composite scores. RESULTS: There were significant main effects between YBT-LQ scores with and without stroboscopic vision ( p = <.001). Post hoc analyses identified a significant decrease in performance when participants performed the YBT-LQ with stroboscopic vision compared to the YBT-LQ under standard conditions in each reach direction, for left and right lower extremities. There was a significant decrease in composite scores when controlling for leg length of the participants. CONCLUSION: Performance of the YBT-LQ decreases when youth athletes’ vision is perturbed with stroboscopic vision.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Among the elderly, instability leading to falls (and consequences of them) is one of the most important problems. The etiology of falls is usually complex, but balance, posture and gait problems are considered to be the most important risk factors. Objectives: The objective of this study was to assess the usefulness of the Sensory Organization Test (SOT) in differentiating multi-fallers from single-fallers. Material and methods: The studied group included 92 patients aged >60 years with balance disorders and falls in their history. The patients were divided into 2 groups: multi-fallers and single-fallers. The control group (non-fallers) included 21 individuals. The SOT was performed on the 1st day (SOT1) and on the last day (SOT2) of rehabilitation. Mean equilibrium score (ES) of 1-3 and 4-6 sensory conditions and composite score (CS) of the SOT1 and SOT2 were analyzed. The falls were analyzed as a total number of falls while performing SOT and a number of falls in all 3 repetitions of both conditions 5 and 6 separately. In SOT conditions 1-4 there were no falls observed. Results: The importance of SOT to differentiate fallers from non-fallers and single-fallers from multi-fallers is ambiguous. Conclusions: The SOT may or may not indicate the differences between the groups - it does not fully explain those differences. It shows only postural dysfunction without indicating any localization in particular part of vestibular organ. The basic diagnostic evaluation in the elderly with a proneness to falls should include clinical examination and the Dix-Hallpike maneuver, supplemented with a videonystagmography (VNG), which would assess the structure of damage in vestibular organ. Posturography is of less validity in the differentiation of fallers from non-fallers.
Article
Full-text available
Context: Patients with somatosensory deficits have been found to rely more on visual feedback for postural control. However, current balance tests may be limited in identifying increased visual dependence (sensory reweighting to the visual system), as options are typically limited to eyes open or closed conditions with no progressions between. Objective: To assess the capability of stroboscopic glasses to induce sensory reweighting of visual input during single-leg balance. Design: Descriptive Setting: Laboratory Participants: Eighteen healthy subjects without vision or balance disorders or lower extremity injury history (9 females; age=22.1±2.1 years; height=169.8±8.5cm; mass=66.5±10.6kg) participated. Interventions: Subjects performed 3 trials of unipedal stance for 10 seconds with eyes open (EO) and closed (EC), and with stroboscopic vision (SV) that was completed with specialized eyewear that intermittently cycled between opaque and transparent for 100 milliseconds at a time. Balance tasks were performed on firm and foam surfaces, with the order randomized. Main outcome measures: Ten center-of-pressure parameters were computed. Results: Separate ANOVAs with repeated measures found significant differences between the 3 visual conditions on both firm (Ps=<.001) and foam (Ps=<.001 to .005) surfaces for all measures. For trials on firm surface, almost all measures showed that balance with SV was significantly impaired relative to EO, but less impaired than EC. On the foam surface, almost all postural stability measures demonstrated significant impairments with SV compared with EO, but the impairment with SV was similar to EC. Conclusions: SV impairment of single-leg balance was large on the firm surface, but not to the same degree as EC. However, the foam surface disruption to somatosensory processing and sensory reweighting to vision lead to greater disruptive effects of SV to the same level as EC. This indicates that when the somatosensory system is perturbed even a moderate decrease in visual feedback (SV) may induce an EC level impairment to postural control during single-leg stance.
Article
Full-text available
The modified Romberg test using a foam pad (“MRuFP”) as a bedside examination has been used to assess the function of the complex sensory input needed for upright stance. The objective of this study was to assess its clinical value detecting vestibular falls in comparison with the sensory organization test (SOT), the gold standard. In total, 80 patients who had undergone the MRuFP, SOT, and bithermal caloric tests were included in this study. The MRuFPs were performed on two (height 12 cm, MRu2FP) or three (18 cm, MRu3FP) layers of foam pads. The odds ratios of falling on SOT were calculated. Iterative algorithms were used for linear curve fitting between the balance time on the MRuFP and SOT equilibrium score (ES). The diagnostic performance of MRuFP under different conditions was poor, with low sensitivity (0.07–0.63), when the results of SOT were used as the gold standard. However, the odds ratios of failing SOT condition 5 were 6.78 (95 % CI = 1.26–36.50) for patients with abnormal findings on eyes closed (EC)–MRu2FP and 10.91 (95 % CI = 2.58–46.11) for those on the EC–MRu3FP in patients without caloric weakness. In patients with caloric weakness, the odds ratio of failing SOT condition 5 for patients with abnormal findings on EC–MRu2FP was 7.0 (95 % CI = 0.69–70.74, p > 0.05), and 32.0 for those on EC-MRu3FP (95 % CI = 2.81–364.7). A linear equation was presented as the model fit (adjusted R 2 = 0.355) predicting the SOT condition 5 ES according to the balance time on EC–MRu3FP. In conclusion, the EC–MRu3FP, as a bedside examination, correlated well with SOT condition 5 as an objective measure.
Chapter
The upright stance position is inherently unstable since the smallest deviation from a perfect upright orientation produces forces due to gravity that accelerate the body toward the ground. Stability is achieved by generating appropriate joint torques that correct for deviations from a desired orientation with orientation changes detected by sensory systems (primarily somatosensory/proprioceptive, visual, and vestibular systems). Functionally, balance control can be viewed as a closed-loop feedback control system with the integration of different sources of sensory orientation information being one component of the overall system, but with the system's feedback nature placing constraints on the sensory integration process. Analysis of body sway evoked by balance perturbations allows for the measurement of “sensory weights” that represent the relative contributions of different sensory systems to an internal estimate of orientation that, in turn, is used to generate corrective actions. Experiments reveal that sensory weights are not fixed quantities, but vary as a function of environmental and experimental conditions as well as neurologic disorders that affect the quality of sensory information available from different sensory systems. Because environmental conditions can change rapidly, sensory reweighting must also occur rapidly enough to prevent instability due to an under- or overproduction of corrective action.
Chapter
For most individuals, balancing upright is a simple task that requires little effort. The inherent difficulties associated with standing balance are not revealed until a pathology or injury impairs its control. Fundamentally, standing upright requires us to balance our unstable whole-body load within a small base of support. Small movements of the upright body are detected by various sensory receptors, all encoding these movements through their own coordinate system with specific dynamics. The balance controller filters, processes, and integrates sensory cues of body motion to produce an error signal between predicted and actual sensory consequences of balance-related movements. Compensatory motor commands are generated in response to this error to maintain upright standing. In the present review, we first briefly describe the biomechanics and sensor dynamics of standing balance. We further review sensorimotor and perceptual approaches revealing operational principles of the balance system, along with computational approaches that explore control processes underlying upright stance. Finally, we present robotic tools that virtualize the sensory consequences, biomechanics, and/or environmental factors inherent to the standing balance task. Throughout, we emphasize works that combine sensorimotor, computational, and/or robotics approaches to highlight the task dependency, multisensory cue combinations, cortical-subcortical contributions, and internal representations underpinning balance control.
Article
Background: Acute lateral ankle sprain (ALAS) impairs unipedal balance both with the injured and uninjured limb, suggesting that balance during bipedal stance may also be compromised. However, a previous study failed to find such impairment because of poorly sensitive balance outcomes. Time-to-boundary (TTB) analysis may be sensitive enough for detecting latent deficits in bipedal balance following ALAS. Research question: We aimed to examine postural stability during bipedal stance in patients with ALAS using TTB outcomes, and to determine bilateral deficits in unipedal balance. Methods: Twenty-seven patients with ALAS and 26 persons without a history of ALAS participated. ALAS was operationally defined as a traumatic injury to the lateral ligaments of the ankle joint occurring within 24-72 h. Both limbs of the control group were side-matched to those of the patients as either injured or uninjured limbs. All participants performed 3 trials of bipedal stance with eyes open and closed. Next, they completed 3 trials of unipedal stance on both the injured and uninjured limbs in both visual conditions. Order of limb and visual condition for each limb was randomly selected. Means and standard deviations of TTB minima in the anteroposterior and mediolateral directions were computed to assess balance, with lower values indicating poorer balance. Results: Independent t-tests revealed significant group differences for almost all measures (p=<0.001 to 0.021), indicating that the ALAS group presented poorer bipedal balance. For unipedal balance, there were no significant group-by-limb interactions for all measures (p > 0.05), indicating no side-to-side differences in the ALAS group. However, group main effects were found for all measures (p=<0.001 to 0.048), showing poorer unipedal balance in the ALAS group. Significance: TTB analysis revealed impaired balance during both unipedal and bipedal stance conditions following ALAS. These results support the emerging hypothesis that centrally mediated changes in postural control may occur following ALAS.
Article
Purpose: To examine relationships among baseline demographics, symptom severity, computerized neurocognitive outcomes, and balance performance in collegiate athletes. Methods: Collegiate varsity athletes (N=207, age=19.3 ± 1.0 years) participating in an ongoing clinical research program who completed concussion baseline assessments including a demographic questionnaire, graded symptom checklist, neurocognitive assessment, and the Sensory Organization Test (SOT) were included in this study. The SOT composite equilibrium score (COMP) and three sensory ratio scores-vestibular (VEST), visual (VIS), and somatosensory (SOM)-were used to describe athletes' overall sensory organization and ability to utilize input from each sensory system to maintain balance. Separate stepwise multiple linear regression models were performed for each SOT outcome. Total symptom severity level and CNS Vital Signs domain scores served as predictor variables. Results: Stepwise regression models for COMP (R = 0.18, F4,201 = 11.29, P <0.001), VEST (R = 0.14, F4,201 = 8.16, P<0.001), and VIS (R = 0.10, F4,201 = 5.52, P <0.001) were all significant. Faster reaction times and higher executive function scores were associated with higher COMP and VEST scores in separate models. Those with faster reaction times also had significantly higher VIS scores. Conclusion: Reaction time and executive function demonstrated significant relationships with SOT balance performance. These cognitive processes may influence athletes' ability to organize and process higher order information and generate appropriate responses to changes in their environment, with respect to balance and injury risk. Future investigations should consider these relationships following injury and clinicians should be mindful of this relationship when considering concussion management strategies.
Article
Objective: To determine if anterior cruciate ligament deficient (ACL-D) individuals and individuals with a reconstructed anterior cruciate ligament (ACL-R) rely more heavily on visual information to maintain postural control. Data sources: PubMed, CINAHL, and SPORTDiscus databases were searched from their earliest available date to May 24, 2016 using the combination of key words. Study selection: Articles were included if they reported any instrumented static single leg balance outcome in both a patient and control sample. The means and standard deviations of these outcomes must have been reported with both eyes open and eyes closed. Data extraction: Sample sizes, means, and standard deviations of single leg balance measures for each group's eyes open and eyes closed testing conditions were extracted. The methodological quality of included studies was independently evaluated by multiple authors using an adapted version of the quality index. Data synthesis: Effect sizes were calculated by dividing the differences in change between eyes closed and eyes open in the ACL-D and control group and the ACL-R and control group by the pooled standard deviation from eyes closed trials for each analysis. Significant differences between the ACL-D and control group (Effect Size=-1.66, 95%CI= -2.90 to -0.41) was noted. The ACL-R and control group were not different (Effect Size=-0.61, 95%CI= -2.17 to 0.95) CONCLUSIONS: Individuals with an ACL-D but not individuals with ACL-R demonstrate a greater reliance on visual information during single leg stance compared to healthy individuals.
Article
Purpose: The underlying cause of balance impairments in chronic ankle instability (CAI) patients remains unknown but an altered utilization of sensory information has been hypothesized as a potential cause. The purpose of this systematic review with meta-analysis was to determine if CAI patients utilize somatosensory information to the same extent as uninjured controls during static single limb stance. Method: We searched PubMed, CINAHL, SPORTDiscus, and Scopus databases from origin to March 2016 using the combination of key words including: postural control, postural stability, single limb stance, single leg stance, single leg balance, single limb balance, time-to-boundary, or TTB. Eligible studies had to include instrumented single leg stance with both eyes open (EO) and closed (EC) in healthy, CAI, or both groups as well as report time-to-boundary (TTB) means and standard deviations (SD). Results: A total of 11 articles were identified. Effect sizes using EC to EO standardized mean differences (SMD) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) were calculated for all studies that were included in this investigation. Similarly, pooled estimates for each TTB outcome were compared between the CAI and uninjured control groups. The 95% CI of the mediolateral (ML) TTB mean (Control: -1.50 (-1.71 to -1.29), CAI: -2.04 (-2.31 to -1.77)), anterioposterior (AP) mean (Control: -2.19 (-2.43 to -1.96), CAI: -2.82 (-3.13 to -2.52)), and AP SD (Control: -1.81 (-2.03 to -1.58), CAI: -2.50 (-2.79 to -2.22)) did not overlap, indicating significant differences between two groups. Conclusion: On the basis of our systematic review with meta-analysis, it appears that CAI patients do not utilize somatosensory information to the same extent as uninjured controls and instead upregulate the use of visual information during single limb stance.