Available via license: CC BY-NC 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
SPECIAL ARTICLE
Fear of missing out (FOMO): overview, theoretical
underpinnings, and literature review on relations
with severity of negative affectivity and problematic
technology use
Jon D. Elhai,
1,2,3
0000-0000-0000-0000
Haibo Yang,
1
Christian Montag
4
0000-0000-0000-0000
1
Academy of Psychology and Behavior, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin, China.
2
Department of Psychology, University of Toledo, Toledo,
Ohio, USA.
3
Department of Psychiatry, University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA.
4
Department of Molecular Psychology, Institute of Psychology
and Education, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany.
This article discusses the fear of missing out (FOMO) on rewarding experiences, an important
psychological construct in contemporary times. We present an overview of the FOMO construct and
its operational definition and measurement. Then, we review recent empirical research on FOMO’s
relationship with levels of online social engagement, problematic technology and internet commu-
nication use, negative affectivity, and sociodemographic variables. Additionally, we discuss theoretical
conceptualizations regarding possible causes of FOMO and how FOMO may drive problematic
internet technology use. Finally, we discuss future directions for the empirical study of FOMO.
Keywords: Addictive behavior; anxiety; social anxiety; depression; smartphone
Introduction
The fear of missing out (FOMO) on rewarding experiences
has received increasing empirical study in recent years.
Central to FOMO is the perceived need to persistently stay
connected with one’s social network, resulting in frequent
(and for some people, excessive) use of social networking
sites (SNS) and messaging services.
1
The enhanced sci-
entific focus on FOMO coincides with growing societal
debate about whether too much digital ‘‘screen time’’ is
harmful to children and adults.
2,3
However, the empirical
literature on FOMO has not yet been synthesized into a
review paper. Our focus in this paper is to define and dis-
cuss the FOMO construct and its theoretical underpin-
nings, as well as review the recent empirical literature on
relationships between FOMO and levels of online social
engagement, problematic internet use (PIU), negative affec-
tivity, and sociodemographic characteristics.
Background, definition, and measurement of
FOMO
FOMO was first introduced in media outlets in the early
2010s.
4,5
At that time, SNS use had grown exponentially
around the world.
6,7
With the dissemination of means to
check SNS, especially the increasing ubiquity of smart-
phones, it has become easy to learn about potentially
rewarding experiences (online and offline) that one may
be missing. Incidentally, from early on FOMO was char-
acterized as an anxiety-provoking construct in popular
media.
4,5
FOMO has been defined in scientific literature as
involving two specific primary components: a) apprehen-
sion that others are having rewarding experiences from
which one is absent, and b) the persistent desire to stay
connected with people in one’s social network.
1
The first
component maps onto the cognitive aspect of anxiety
(e.g., worry, rumination, etc.). The latter component involves
a behavioral strategy aimed at relieving such anxiety –
analogous to how compulsions aim (though maladapti-
vely) to relieve anxiety in obsessive compulsive disorder.
Currently, this behavioral component of FOMO most often
involves frequent checking of SNS and messaging ser-
vices to maintain social connections and avoid missing
out on rewarding experiences.
1
The persistent online checking behavior inherent in
FOMO is not only active, i.e. when people have time to
proactively browse their internet-enabled devices such as
smartphones, but is also frequently reactive (or perhaps
passive) through the many social-related notifications
received over the course of the day – to which there is a
compulsion to respond. On one hand, social-related noti-
fications are helpful for one’s social life and are rated
favourably
8,9
because they satisfy and alleviate FOMO.
Correspondence: Haibo Yang, Academy of Psychology and Behavior,
Tianjin Normal University, No. 57-1 Wujiayao Street, Hexi District,
Tianjin, 300074, China.
E-mail: yanghaibo@tjnu.edu.cn
Submitted Jan 23 2020, accepted Mar 07 2020.
How to cite this article: Elhai JD, Yang H, Montag C. Fear of
missing out (FOMO): overview, theoretical underpinnings, and
literature review on relations with severity of negative affectivity
and problematic technology use. Braz J Psychiatry. 2020;00:000-
000. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1516-4446-2020-0870
Braz J Psychiatry. 2020 xxx-xxx;00(00):000-000
doi:10.1590/1516-4446-2020-0870
Brazilian Psychiatric Association
00000000-0002-7316-1185
Online social interaction can also enhance social capital
for many people.
10
On the other hand, interruptive smart-
phone and computer notifications (and associated check-
ing behavior) are known to have adverse effects. Such
notifications can result in a distracted and less focused
daily experience, impairing attention
11
and interrupting
work, school,
12,13
and other daily life activities
14
due to
‘‘switching costs,’’ which make it difficult to return to and
complete the task at hand.
15
Thus, FOMO can drive exces-
sive checking for and responding to SNS notifications,
making it difficult to remain productive in daily life.
16
In this
context, we also mention growing discussion on the need
to regulate the number of elements built in to social media
apps that elicit FOMO
17,18
in an attempt to prolong usage
time to harvest more personal data in the age of sur-
veillance capitalism.
19,20
Several self-report scales have been developed to meas-
ure FOMO, of which the most widely used is the 10-item
Likert-type FOMO Scale developed by Przybylski et al.
1
This scale includes items such as ‘‘I fear others have more
rewarding experiences than me,’’ and ‘‘When I miss out
on a planned get-together it bothers me.’’ Another similar
scale is Alt’s FOMO Scale, with 17-item
21
and 10-item
22
versions. A third scale added items to the Przybylski
FOMO Scale by incorporating state-based FOMO content
to distinguish it from trait-based FOMO.
23
The authors
used the two FOMO Scale items described above as
examples of trait-based FOMO, and added state-based
FOMO items such as ‘‘I am continuously online in order to
not miss out on anything’’ and ‘‘I fear not to be up-to-date
in my social networking sites.’’ Additionally, some research
has used behavioral measures to examine FOMO by
assessing the physiological distress (e.g., heart rate and
blood pressure) of being separated from a smartphone and
SNS.
24,25
Several studies have explored the latent dimensions
of FOMO by using exploratory and/or confirmatory factor
analysis to better understand this construct. Some work
has tested and found support for a single latent dimen-
sion.
26-28
Wegmann et al.
23
added the previously des-
cribed state-based content to the Przybylski et al.
1
FOMO Scale and refined it through exploratory factor
analysis with a sample of German participants. They then
validated the expanded scale with confirmatory factor
analysis in a separate German sample, revealing a two-
dimensional model involving trait- and state-based FOMO
factors.
23,29
Another study revealed two FOMO factors
involving missing out on the experiences of others, and
the use of rumination strategies for controlling one’s social
experiences.
30
Finally, other research has discovered
three FOMO factors in social, news, and commercial
information.
21,22
FOMO appears to be a universal phenomenon, having
been investigated and supported as a valid construct in
numerous countries and languages. For example, FOMO
has been studied in samples from Israel,
22
Turkey,
29
Belgium,
31
Poland,
32
the United Kingdom,
33
New Zealand,
34
Germany,
35
Italy,
30
China,
36
Bosnia,
37
India,
38
Latin
America,
39
and various regions within the United
States.
27,40,41
One paper compared the FOMO scores
of German and Spanish samples, finding that Germans
scored significantly higher on trait-based FOMO, with a
small effect (Z
p2
= 0.031).
23
FOMO’s relationships with relevant variables
We now review recent empirical research on bivariate rela-
tionships between FOMO and relevant variables, including
PIU, psychopathology and sociodemographic character-
istics. Ours is not a comprehensive systematic review,
since we focused on findings from the past couple of years.
Specifically, when reviewing FOMO’s relations with rele-
vant constructs, we only included studies published since
2018. Nevertheless, we also reviewed earlier studies when
discussing FOMO’s relations with sociodemographic vari-
ables, since few papers have reported such findings.
We organized the discussed references in an Endnote
database.
Frequency and problematic use of internet
technology
First, we will discuss research investigating FOMO in
relation to greater frequency of, but not necessarily mala-
daptive, SNS use. The majority of these studies investi-
gated only Facebook use or overall SNS use, employing
self-report methodology with a correlational, cross-
sectional survey design. Moderate to large relationships
have been discovered in several such studies involving
samples of children and youth, college students, and
adults.
35,37,42-45
For example, Dempsey et al.
43
analyzed
data from 289 American college students using a cross-
sectional web survey design with standardized, self-report
scales. The authors reported a bivariate Pearson correla-
tion of -0.19 between the Przybylski et al.
1
10-item FOMO
Scale and a five-item Facebook use frequency scale
(computed such that lower scores indicate greater Face-
book frequency).
We should provide a caveat at this juncture about healthy
vs. maladaptive internet technology (including SNS) use.
A higher level of social networking is not necessarily
maladaptive, but it can be if it becomes excessive or
‘‘problematic.’’ In fact, PIU is defined as when overuse
results in adverse effects.
46
Such adverse effects are
typically categorized as those observed in addictive dis-
orders involving drug and alcohol use, including with-
drawal when denied access, tolerance (requiring increas-
ing periods of use to feel the same level of emotional
relief), and functional impairment such as work or social
problems, hazardous use, etc.
47,48
For recent discussion
on this topic and a taxonomy of internet-related use
disorders, including SNS use disorder or problematic SNS
use, see work by Montag et al.
49
We should also point out
that only problematic use of internet gaming is currently
an official medical/mental health diagnosis
50
; for pre-
liminary empirical findings, see recent work.
51,52
Nonetheless, problematic use of other forms of internet
technology, such as smartphones and SNS, are important
because of the adverse health and functional conse-
quences that can result from overuse.
53-55
Since the
smartphone itself is only a vehicle for accessing social
Braz J Psychiatry. 2020;00(00)
2JD Elhai et al.
media apps etc., the construct of problematic smartphone
use (PSU) (alternatively, smartphone addiction or smart-
phone use disorder) is frequently debated.
49,56
Recent
reviews of PIU have included smartphone
57-59
and SNS
use,
60,61
including relations between problematic use and
psychological (including psychopathological) constructs.
The relationships between unspecified PIU and PSU have
also been investigated, showing overlap (correlations) of
around 0.50 in recent research.
62-64
The psychological
scales in the aforementioned reviews have frequently
used diagnostic criteria for drug and alcohol use that have
been modified to assess use of a specific technology
medium.
FOMO has been empirically studied in relation to
problematic SNS use in numerous studies. These studies
have almost exclusively used self-report methodology
with a correlational, cross-sectional survey design. Mode-
rate to large positive associations between FOMO and
levels of problematic SNS use have been found in several
studies of school-aged adolescents, college students,
and adults.
29,30,37,38,43,65-69
Dempsey et al.
43
discovered
a bivariate Pearson correlation of 0.32 between the
Przybylski et al.
1
FOMO Scale and the six-item Bergen
Facebook Addiction Scale.
70
Thus, FOMO has been
linked not only to greater frequency of SNS use, but also
to higher levels of problematic SNS use.
Many studies have also examined FOMO in relation to
levels of problematic smartphone use. These investiga-
tions have exclusively used self-report methods and a
correlational, cross-sectional research design. Numerous
studies with samples from all age groups have found
moderate to large positive associations between FOMO
and problematic smartphone use.
27,28,36,45,65,66,69,71-75
For example, Elhai, Yang, Fang et al.
36
analyzed data
from 1,034 Chinese university students, using a cross-
sectional, online self-report survey design with standard-
ized psychological scales. They reported a bivariate
Pearson correlation of 0.29 between the Chinese versions
of the Przybylski et al.
1
FOMO Scale and the 10-item
Smartphone Addiction Scale.
76
Associations between
FOMO and other adverse effects from smartphones have
also been investigated. FOMO has been positively
correlated with disrupted daily life activities due to smart-
phone notifications,
16
as well as to distracted pedestrian
behavior due to smartphone use.
77
Negative affectivity and demographics
FOMO has been conceptualized as a construct that
primarily involves anxiety-related psychopathology,
1
and
anxiety disorders are conceptualized as an important
aspect of underlying negative affectivity.
78
As such,
FOMO has been examined in relation to anxiety symptom
severity, including social anxiety, in studies with adoles-
cent and adult samples that used correlational, cross-
sectional designs. Across studies, FOMO has revealed
moderate to large positive relationships with anxiety
severity.
27,29,30,32,35,36,38,43,68,72,75,79
For instance, in the
previously mentioned study by Elhai, Yang, Fang et al.,
36
a bivariate Pearson correlation of 0.33 was reported
between the Przybylski et al.
1
FOMO Scale and the
seven-item anxiety subscale of the Depression Anxiety
Stress Scale-21.
80
Anxiety highly correlates (and is comorbid) with dep-
ression,
81,82
which is also a fundamental aspect of
underlying negative affectivity.
78
FOMO has also been
examined in relation to depression severity from adoles-
cence through adulthood using correlational, cross-sec-
tional methodology. Specifically, mild to moderate positive
associations have been found between FOMO and dep-
ression symptom severity.
27,35,36,38,43,72,74,79,83
Elhai,
Yang, Fang et al.
36
reported a bivariate Pearson correla-
tion of 0.29 between the Przybylski et al.
1
FOMO Scale
and seven-item depression subscale of the Depression
Anxiety Stress Scale-21.
80
Other indices of negative affectivity have been sup-
ported in connection with FOMO through correlational,
cross-sectional methodology. Moderate to large positive
associations have been found for FOMO with rumina-
tion,
27,43
as well as for FOMO with negative affect and
mood.
72,75,79
Additionally, a moderate positive correlation
has been found between FOMO and proneness to expe-
rience boredom
27,74
; in fact, boredom proneness is
conceptualized as a negative affectivity construct that
additionally involves impaired attention.
84
FOMO has also been investigated for relations with
variables involving the opposite of negative affectivity –
namely, perceived quality of life. These studies have used
cross-sectional methods similar to those of most of the
previously mentioned studies. Specifically, FOMO has
shown mild to moderate inverse correlations with life
satisfaction.
26,66,69
Furthermore, mild to moderate inverse
associations have been found between FOMO and
emotional well-being.
42,85
Finally, FOMO has been associated with particular
demographic characteristics in a small number of studies
with correlational, cross-sectional designs. FOMO has
been correlated with younger age in some studies,
27,32,86
and others have found it to be more related to females
than males.
27,31,87
One study of North Americans found
that FOMO was more related to Caucasians than racial
minorities.
27
Theoretical underpinnings of FOMO
FOMO was first conceptualized using self-determination
theory (SDT), which was developed by Ryan & Deci
88
and
applied by Przybylski et al. to understanding what drives
FOMO.
1
SDT attempts to explain how personality is
formed and the psychological needs that drive persona-
lity formation. SDT proposes that intrinsic (rather than
extrinsic) motivation for reward is essential in promoting
mental health, and that intrinsic motivation is best
promoted when one feels socially connected to others.
Therefore, in SDT, social relatedness can drive intrinsic
motivation, which in turn can encourage positive mental
health.
89
Przybylski et al. applied SDT to FOMO, pro-
posing that FOMO is a negative emotional state resulting
from unmet social relatedness needs.
1
The conceptuali-
zation that FOMO involves negative affect from unmet
social needs is similar to theories about the negative
emotional effects of social ostracism.
90
Braz J Psychiatry. 2020;00(00)
FOMO review 3
In this context, research involving personality psychol-
ogy should also be mentioned. FOMO has been linked to
the personality trait of neuroticism,
29,86
one of the most
well-known risk factors for developing a mood disorder.
91
Furthermore, narcissism likely plays a role in FOMO.
Vulnerable narcissists in particular have unmet social
relatedness needs (similar to those with severe FOMO)
and more often engage in problematic SNS use.
92
There-
fore, FOMO may have a mediational role between narcis-
sism and problematic SNS use.
Subsequent investigations have assessed whether
FOMO influences negative affectivity, such as depression
and anxiety, or whether negative affectivity influences
FOMO. For instance, several papers have conceptualized
FOMO as a driving factor for negative affectivity.
31,79,93
However, other papers have conceptualized negative
affectivity as an antecedent of FOMO.
23,39,94
It is not yet
clear whether FOMO causes negative affectivity, whether
negative affectivity causes FOMO, or whether there is
a bidirectional effect. Repeated measures, longitudi-
nal designs, and experiments may eventually provide
answers to this question. Studies with repeated measures
designs have found some initial support that FOMO
drives negative affect over short (1-week) periods of
time.
79,93
FOMO is also widely considered a driving mechanism
for PIU, as discussed above. As such, FOMO has been
theorized for its role in PIU. The Interaction of Person-
Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model of PIU con-
ceptualizes risk factors for PIU, both in early
95
and later
stages of excessive technology use.
96
I-PACE proposes
that background personal variables, such as psycho-
pathology, personality, and biology (including genetics),
can influence problematic use. I-PACE additionally sug-
gests that responses to background variables also play a
role in PIU as mediating mechanisms between back-
ground variables and problematic use. Such response
variables include coping strategies, cognitive bias, ability
to inhibit impulsive behavior, craving and expectations
about internet use.
96
FOMO has been conceptualized as an internet-related
maladaptive cognitive bias within the I-PACE model’s
response variables.
23,94
Elhai et al. suggested that
because depression and anxiety involve social isolation,
FOMO can be a natural consequence, in turn driving
PIU.
94
In fact, consistent with I-PACE, several papers
have found FOMO to mediate relations between psycho-
pathological symptoms (i.e., depression and anxiety)
and PIU levels.
27,28,36,39,43,73,74
Thus, FOMO may be a
mechanism that explains how some depressed/anxious
people develop PIU.
Conclusions and future directions
FOMO is an important psychological construct in the
digital age. FOMO has been examined and validated
globally with several self-report psychological scales, as
well as with physiological monitoring. Support has been
established for FOMO in relation to greater frequency of
SNS use, higher levels of problematic SNS and smart-
phone use, more severe anxiety, depression and negative
affectivity, and lower levels of perceived quality of life.
Preliminary evidence indicates that FOMO is more related
to younger age and female sex.
Future research could explore unanswered questions
regarding the FOMO construct. Nearly all work assessing
the relations between FOMO and high or problematic
levels of technology use has involved self-reported beha-
vior as the dependent variable. However, self-reported
rates of internet use differ from objective measures of use
(e.g. device logs).
97-99
In particular, digital phenotyping
could help the psychological sciences overcome some of
the problems arising from self-report methods, such as
problems self-assessing a construct such as FOMO and
the tendency to answer questions in socially desirable
ways.
100
We are aware of only one FOMO study that
has objectively measured internet technology use (smart-
phone), finding that FOMO was related to higher level of
use.
83
More work in this area is needed. Furthermore,
the vast majority of research on FOMO has used cross-
sectional research methodology. We encourage resear-
chers to use repeated measures, longitudinal, daily diary
and/or experience sampling designs to further assess
FOMO. Such designs can attempt to answer whether
FOMO drives negative affectivity or vice-versa, which has
been done thus far in only two papers,
79,93
and whether
FOMO drives problematic smartphone/SNS use or vice-
versa, which is as yet unexplored.
Additionally, the research on FOMO has been exclu-
sively variable-centered. No studies have used person-
centered analyses to examine possible heterogeneity
in the experiences/symptoms of FOMO across indivi-
duals with mixture modeling such as cluster, latent
class, or latent profile analysis. Furthermore, FOMO and
other negative affectivity variables are all correlated with
PIU and with each other. Therefore, statistical methods,
such as machine learning algorithms that can address
collinearity and relative variable importance and can
shrink regression coefficients among collinear predic-
tors to realistic levels, are advisable for further FOMO
investigation.
72
When writing the present paper, we were also surprised
that FOMO has not yet been investigated with neuros-
cientific tools to better understand which neural processes
underlie the relevant construct. This is a glaring omission,
since studies have increasingly used such tools to
investigate the effects of excessive social network use/
smartphone use
101-103
or the power of likes on platforms
such as Instagram.
104-106
This less-traveled path will be of
tremendous importance for gaining deeper insights into
the actual nature of FOMO. It will also be important to
disentangle more state/trait effects of FOMO, given that
Balta et al.
29
found that associations with neuroticism
are stronger for trait FOMO (r = 0.29) than state FOMO
(r = 0.15). See also Wegmann et al.
23
on the distinction
between state vs. trait FOMO.
Finally, other psychological constructs that have pre-
liminary associations with FOMO should be further
examined in subsequent studies, including behavioral
activation, which is important for treating major depres-
sive disorder,
107
as well as the need for physical touch.
108
Another important area to study is app design that
Braz J Psychiatry. 2020;00(00)
4JD Elhai et al.
reduces FOMO by batching interruptive smartphone
notifications.
109
We hope that advances in methodologi-
cal design will further our understanding of FOMO and its
relationship with relevant psychological variables.
Acknowledgements
JDE is a paid, full-time faculty member at University of
Toledo and a paid, visiting scientist at Tianjin Normal
University; he receives grant research funding from the
U.S. National Institutes of Health and the Department of
Defense. CM has received (from Ulm University and the
University of Bonn) grants from the German Research
Foundation (DFG) and the German Federal Ministry for
Research and Education.
Disclosure
JDE receives royalties for several books published on post-
traumatic stress disorder (PTSD); occasionally serves as a
paid, expert witness on PTSD legal cases. CM has perfor-
med grant reviews for several agencies; has edited journal
sections and articles; has given academic lectures in clini-
cal or scientific venues or companies; has generated books
or book chapters for publishers of mental health texts (for
some of these activities he received royalties, but never
from the gaming or social media industry); is part of a dis-
cussion circle (Digitalita
¨t und Verantwortung: https://about.
fb.com/de/news/h/gespraechskreis-digitalitaet-und-verant
wortung/) debating ethical questions linked to social media,
digitalization and society/democracy at Facebook (no
salary); and is currently on the scientific advisory board
of the Nymphenburg Group (nymphenburg.de; for this
activity he is financially compensated). The other author
reports no conflicts of interest.
References
1 Przybylski AK, Murayama K, DeHaan CR, Gladwell V. Motivational,
emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Comput
Human Behav. 2013;29:1841-8.
2 Orben A, Przybylski AK. The association between adolescent well-
being and digital technology use. Nat Hum Behav. 2019;3:173-82.
3 Elhai JD, Dvorak RD, Levine JC, Hall BJ. Problematic smartphone
use: a conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with
anxiety and depression psychopathology. J Affect Disord. 2017;207:
251-9.
4 Fake C. FOMO and social media [Internet]. 2011 Mar 15 [cited 2020
Mar 19]. www.caterina.net/2011/03/15/fomo-and-social-media/
5 Morford M. Oh my God you are so missing out [Internet]. 2010 Aug
[cited 2020 Mar 19]. www.sfgate.com/entertainment/morford/article/
Oh-my-God-you-are-so-missing-out-2536241.php
6 Hitlin P. Internet, social media use and device ownership in U.S.
have plateaued after years of growth [Internet]. 2018 Sep 28 [cited
2020 Mar 19]. www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2018/09/28/internet-
social-media-use-and-device-ownership-in-u-s-have-plateaued-after-
years-of-growth/
7 Poushter J, Bishop C, Chwe H. Social media use continues to rise in
developing countries but plateaus across developed ones [Internet].
2018 Jun 19 [cited 2020 Mar 19]. www.pewglobal.org/2018/06/19/
social-media-use-continues-to-rise-in-developing-countries-but-pla
teaus-across-developed-ones/
8 Paul CL, Komlodi A, Lutters WG. Again?!! The emotional experience
of social notification interruptions. Human-Computer Interaction-
INTERACT 2011, Part 2: 13th IFIP TC 13 International Conference,
Lisbon, Portugal, September 5-9, 2011, Proceedings. p. 471-8.
9 Paul CL, Komlodi A, Lutters WG. Interruptive notifications in support
of task management. Int J Hum Comput Stud. 2015;79:20-34.
10 Cheng C, Wang HY, Sigerson L, Chau CL. Do the socially rich get
richer? A nuanced perspective on social network site use and online
social capital accrual. Psychol Bull. 2019;145:734-64.
11 Stothart C, Mitchum A, Yehnert C. The attentional cost of receiving a
cell phone notification. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2015;
41:893-7.
12 Kushlev K, Proulx JD, Dunn E. ‘‘Silence your phones’’: smartphone
notifications increase inattention and hyperactivity symptoms. Con-
ference: CHI’ 2016, At San Jose, CA, USA. p. 1011-20.
13 Duke E, Montag C. Smartphone addiction, daily interruptions and
self-reported productivity. Addict Behav Rep. 2017;6:90-5.
14 Elhai JD, Rozgonjuk D, Alghraibeh AM, Yang H. Disrupted daily
activities from interruptive smartphone notifications: relations with
depression and anxiety severity and the mediating role of boredom
proneness [Internet]. First Published June 18, 2019 [cited 2020 Mar
19]. www.journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0894439319858008
15 Salvucci DD, Taatgen NA. Threaded cognition: an integrated theory
of concurrent multitasking. Psychol Rev. 2008;115:101-30.
16 Rozgonjuk D, Elhai JD, Ryan T, Scott G. Fear of missing out is asso-
ciated with disrupted activities from receiving smartphone notifica-
tions and surface learning in college students. Comput Educ. 2019;
140:103590.
17 Alutaybi A, McAlaney J, Stefanidis A, Phalp K, Ali R. Designing
social networks to combat fear of missing out. In the 32nd Human
Computer Interaction Conference (British HCI’18) – Position Papers
Track. 02-06 Jul, 2018., At In the 32nd Human Computer Interaction
Conference (British HCI’18) – Position Papers Track. 02-06 Jul,
2018. Belfast, Northern Ireland.
18 Montag C, Lachmann B, Herrlich M, Zweig K. Addictive features of
social media/messenger platforms and freemium games against the
background of psychological and economic theories. Int J Environ
Res Public Health. 2019 Jul 23;16(14). pii: E2612. doi: https://doi.
org/10.3390/ijerph16142612
19 Zuboff S. Big other: surveillance capitalism and the prospects of an
information civilization. J Inf Technol. 2015;30:75-89.
20 Zuboff S. The age of surveillance capitalism: The fight for a human
future at the new frontier of power. London: Profile Books; 2019.
21 Alt D. College students’ academic motivation, media engagement
and fear of missing out. Comput Human Behav. 2015;49:111-9.
22 Alt D. Students’ wellbeing, fear of missing out, and social media
engagement for leisure in higher education learning environments.
Curr Psychol. 2018;37:128-38.
23 Wegmann E, Oberst U, Stodt B, Brand M. Online-specific fear
of missing out and Internet-use expectancies contribute to symp-
toms of Internet-communication disorder. Addict Behav Rep. 2017;
5:33-42.
24 Cheever NA, Rosen LD, Carrier LM, Chavez A. Out of sight is not
out of mind: the impact of restricting wireless mobile device use on
anxiety levels among low, moderate and high users. Comput Human
Behav. 2014;37:290-7.
25 Clayton RB, Leshner G, Almond A. The extended iSelf: the impact of
iPhone separation on cognition, emotion, and physiology. J Comput
Mediat Commun. 2015;20:119-35.
26 Can G, Satici SA. Adaptation of fear of missing out scale (FoMOs):
Turkish version validity and reliability study. Psicol Reflex Crit.
2019;32:3.
27 Elhai JD, Levine JC, Alghraibeh AM, Alafnan AA, Aldraiweesh AA,
Hall BJ. Fear of missing out: testing relationships with negative
affectivity, online social engagement, and problematic smartphone
use. Comput Human Behav. 2018;89:289-98.
28 Servidio R. Self-control and problematic smartphone use among
Italian University students: the mediating role of the fear of missing
out and of smartphone use patterns. Curr Psychol. 2019; https://doi.
org/10.1007/s12144-019-00373-z
29 Balta S, Emirtekin E, Kircaburun K, Griffiths MD. Neuroticism, trait
fear of missing out, and phubbing: The mediating role of state fear of
missing out and problematic instagram use [Internet]. 2018 [cited
2010 Mar 19]. www.link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11469-018-
9959-8
30 Casale S, Fioravanti G. Factor structure and psychometric proper-
ties of the Italian version of the fear of missing out scale in emerging
adults and adolescents. Addict Behav. 2020;102:106179.
Braz J Psychiatry. 2020;00(00)
FOMO review 5
31 Beyens I, Frison E, Eggermont S. ‘‘I don’t want to miss a thing’’:
Adolescents’ fear of missing out and its relationship to adolescents’
social needs, Facebook use, and Facebook related stress. Comput
Human Behav. 2016;64:1-8.
32 B"achnio A, Przepio
´rka A. Facebook intrusion, fear of missing out,
narcissism, and life satisfaction: a cross-sectional study. Psychiatry
Res. 2018;259:514-9.
33 Buglass SL, Binder JF, Betts LR, Underwood JD. Motivators of
online vulnerability: The impact of social network site use and
FOMO. Comput Human Behav. 2017;66:248-55.
34 Riordan B, Flett J, Hunter J, Scarf D, Conner TS. Fear of missing out
(FoMO): the relationship between FoMO, alcohol use, and alcohol-
related consequences in college students. J Psychiatry Brain Funct.
2015;2.
35 Reer F, Tang WY, Quandt T. Psychosocial well-being and social
media engagement: the mediating roles of social comparison orien-
tation and fear of missing out. New Media Soc. 2019;21:1486-505.
36 Elhai JD, Yang H, Fang J, Bai X, Hall BJ. Depression and anxiety
symptoms are related to problematic smartphone use severity in
Chinese young adults: fear of missing out as a mediator. Addict
Behav. 2020;101:105962.
37 Tomczyk ˇ
ˇL, Selmanagic-Lizde E. Fear of Missing Out (FOMO)
among youth in Bosnia and Herzegovina: scale and selected
mechanisms. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2018;88:541-9.
38 Dhir A, Yossatorn Y, Kaur P, Chen S. Online social media fatigue
and psychological wellbeing: a study of compulsive use, fear of
missing out, fatigue, anxiety and depression. Int J Inf Manage.
2018;40:141-52.
39 Oberst U, Wegmann E, Stodt B, Brand M, Chamarro A. Negative
consequences from heavy social networking in adolescents: the
mediating role of fear of missing out. J Adolesc. 2017;55:51-60.
40 Baker ZG, Krieger H, LeRoy AS. Fear of missing out: relationships
with depression, mindfulness, and physical symptoms. Transl Issues
Psychol Sci. 2016;2:275-82.
41 Scalzo AC, Martinez JA. Not all anxiety is the same: how different
‘‘types’’ of anxiety uniquely associate with college students’ drinking
intentions. J Coll Stud Dev. 2017;58:943-7.
42 Chai HY, Niu GF, Lian SL, Chu XW, Liu S, Sun XJ. Why social
network site use fails to promote well-being? The roles of social
overload and fear of missing out. Comput Human Behav. 2019;100:
85-92.
43 Dempsey A, E., O’Brien KD, Tiamiyu MF, Elhai JD. Fear of miss-
ing out (FoMO) and rumination mediate relations between social
anxiety and problematic Facebook use. Addict Behav Rep. 2019;9:
100150.
44 Franchina V, Abeele MV, van Rooij AJ, Lo Coco G, De Marez L.
Fear of missing out as a predictor of problematic social media use
and phubbing behavior among Flemish adolescents. Int J Environ
Res Public Health. 2018;15:2319.
45 Tras¸Z. Examining the relationships between Facebook intensity, fear
of missing out, and smartphone addiction. Addicta. 2019;6:91-113.
46 Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD, Karila L, Billieux J. Internet addiction:
a systematic review of epidemiological research for the last decade.
Curr Pharm Des. 2014;20:4026-52.
47 Billieux J, Maurage P, Lopez-Fernandez O, Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD.
Can disordered mobile phone use be considered a behavioral
addiction? An update on current evidence and a comprehensive
model for future research. Curr Addict Rep. 2015;2:156-62.
48 Gutierrez J, de Fonseca FR, Rubio G. Cell-phone addiction:
a review. Front Psychiatry. 2016;7:175.
49 Montag C, Wegmann E, Sariyska R, Demetrovics Z, Brand M. How
to overcome taxonomical problems in the study of Internet use
disorders and what to do with ‘‘smartphone addiction’’? J Behav
Addict; 2019 Oct 31:1-7. doi: http://10.1556/2006.8.2019.59. [Epub
ahead of print]
50 World Health Organization (WHO). 6C51.0 Gaming disorder, pre-
dominantly online [Internet]. 2019 Apr [cited 2010 Mar 19]. www.icd.
who.int/browse11/l-m/en#/http://idwho.int/icd/entity/338347362
51 Montag C, Schivinski B, Sariyska R, Kannen C, Demetrovics Z,
Pontes HM. Psychopathological symptoms and gaming motives in
disordered gaming: a psychometric comparison between the WHO
and APA diagnostic frameworks. J Clin Med. 2019;8(10). pii: E1691.
doi: http://10.3390/jcm8101691
52 Pontes HM, Schivinski B, Sindermann C, Li M, Becker B, Zhou M,
et al. Measurement and conceptualization of gaming disorder
according to the World Health Organization framework: the devel-
opment of the gaming disorder test. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2019
Jun 03. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-019-00088-z. [Epub ahead
of print]
53 Grant JE, Lust K, Chamberlain SR. Problematic smartphone use
associated with greater alcohol consumption, mental health issues,
poorer academic performance, and impulsivity. J Behav Addict.
2019;8:335-42.
54 Luria G. The mediating role of smartphone addiction on the rela-
tionship between personality and young drivers’ smartphone use
while driving. Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav. 2018;59:
203-11.
55 Yang G, Cao J, Li Y, Cheng P, Liu B, Hao Z, et al. Association
between internet addiction and the risk of musculoskeletal pain in
Chinese college freshmen – a cross-sectional study. Front Psychol.
2019;10:1959.
56 Panova T, Carbonell X. Is smartphone addiction really an addiction?
J Behav Addict. 2018;7:252-9.
57 Elhai JD, Levine JC, Hall BJ. The relationship between anxiety
symptom severity and problematic smartphone use: a review of the
literature and conceptual frameworks. J Anxiety Disord. 2019;62:
45-52.
58 Thome
´e S. Mobile phone use and mental health: a review of the
research that takes a psychological perspective on exposure. Int J
Environ Res Public Health. 2018 Nov 29;15(12). pii: E2692. doi:
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15122692
59 Elhai JD, Levine JC, Hall BJ. Problematic smartphone use and
mental health problems: current state of research and future direc-
tions. Dusunen Adam. 2019;32:1-3.
60 Marino C, Gini G, Vieno A, Spada MM. The associations between
problematic Facebook use, psychological distress and well-being
among adolescents and young adults: a systematic review and
meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2018;226:274-81.
61 Seabrook EM, Kern ML, Rickard NS. Social networking sites,
depression, and anxiety: a systematic review. JMIR Ment Health.
2016;3:e50.
62 Lachmann B, Duke E
´, Sariyska R, Montag C. Who’s addicted to the
smartphone and/or the Internet? Psychol Pop Media Cult. 2019;
8:182-9.
63 Lachmann B, Sindermann C, Sariyska RY, Luo R, Melchers MC,
Becker B, et al. The role of empathy and life satisfaction in Internet
and smartphone use disorder. Front Psychol. 2018;9:398.
64 Montag C, Sindermann C, Becker B, Panksepp J. An affective
neuroscience framework for the molecular study of internet addic-
tion. Front Psychol. 2016;7:1906.
65 Liu C, Ma J. Social support through online social networking sites
and addiction among college students: the mediating roles of fear of
missing out and problematic smartphone use. Curr Psychol. 2018
Nov. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-0075-5. [Epub ahead
of print]
66 Sha P, Sariyska R, Riedl R, Lachmann B, Montag C. Linking Internet
communication and smartphone use disorder by taking a closer look
at the Facebook and WhatsApp applications. Addict Behav Rep.
2019;9:100148.
67 Yin L, Wang P, Nie J, Guo J, Feng J, Lei L. Social networking sites
addiction and FoMO: the mediating role of envy and the moderating
role of need to belong. Curr Psychol. 2019 Jul 02. https://doi.org/
10.1007/s12144-019-00344-4. [Epub ahead of print]
68 Casale S, Rugai L, Fioravanti G. Exploring the role of positive
metacognitions in explaining the association between the fear of
missing out and social media addiction. Addict Behav. 2018;85:83-7.
69 Sette CP, Lima NRS, Queluz FNFR, Ferrari BL, Hauck N. The
online fear of missing out inventory (ON-FoMO): development and
validation of a new tool. J Technol Behav Sci. 2019 Jul 24. https://
doi.org/10.1007/s41347-019-00110-0. [Epub ahead of print]
70 Andreassen CS, Torsheim T, Brunborg GS, Pallesen S. Deve-
lopment of a Facebook Addiction Scale. Psychol Rep. 2012;110:
501-17.
71 Coskun S, Karayagiz Muslu G. Investigation of problematic mobile
phones use and fear of missing out (FoMO) level in adolescents.
Community Ment Health J. 2019;55:1004-14.
Braz J Psychiatry. 2020;00(00)
6JD Elhai et al.
72 Elhai JD, Yang H, Rozgonjuk D, Montag C. Using machine learning
to model problematic smartphone use severity: the significant role of
fear of missing out. Addict Behav. 2020;103:106261.
73 Wang J, Wang P, Yang X, Zhang G, Wang X, Zhao F, et al. Fear of
missing out and procrastination as mediators between sensation
seeking and adolescent smartphone addiction. Int J Ment Health
Addict. 2019;17:1049-62.
74 Wolniewicz CA, Rozgonjuk D, Elhai JD. Boredom proneness and
fear of missing out mediate relations between depression and
anxiety with problematic smartphone use. Hum Behav Emerg
Technol. 2020;2:61-70.
75 Wolniewicz CA, Tiamiyu MF, Weeks JW, Elhai JD. Problematic
smartphone use and relations with negative affect, fear of missing
out, and fear of negative and positive evaluation. Psychiatry Res.
2018;262:618-23.
76 Kwon M, Kim DJ, Cho H, Yang S. The smartphone addiction scale:
development and validation of a short version for adolescents. PLoS
One. 2013;8:e83558.
77 Appel M, Krisch N, Stein JP, Weber S. Smartphone zombies!
Pedestrians’ distracted walking as a function of their fear of missing
out. J Environ Psychol. 2019;63:130-3.
78 Watson D. Differentiating the mood and anxiety disorders: a quad-
ripartite model. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2009;5:221-47.
79 Elhai JD, Rozgonjuk D, Liu T, Yang H. Fear of missing out is related
to repeated measurements of negative affect using experience
sampling methodology. J Affect Disord. 2020;262:298-303.
80 Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of negative emotional
states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS)
with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav Res Ther.
1995;33:335-43.
81 Lamers F, van Oppen P, Comijs HC, Smit JH, Spinhoven P, van
Bolkom AJ, et al. Comorbidity patterns of anxiety and depressive
disorders in a large cohort study: the Netherlands Study of
Depression and Anxiety (NESDA). J Clin Psychiatry. 2011;72:341-8.
82 Cummings CM, Caporino NE, Kendall PC. Comorbidity of anxiety
and depression in children and adolescents: 20 years after. Psychol
Bull. 2014;140:816-45.
83 Sela Y, Zach M, Amichay-Hamburger Y, Mishali M, Omer H. Family
environment and problematic internet use among adolescents: the
mediating roles of depression and fear of missing out. Comput
Human Behav. 2020;106:106226.
84 Eastwood JD, Frischen A, Fenske MJ, Smilek D. The unengaged
mind: defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspect Psychol Sci.
2012;7:482-95.
85 Roberts JA, David ME. The Social media party: fear of missing out
(FoMO), social media intensity, connection, and well-being. Int J
Hum Comput Interact. 2020;36:386-92.
86 Blackwell D, Leaman C, Tramposch R, Osborne C, Liss M. Extra-
version, neuroticism, attachment style and fear of missing out as
predictors of social media use and addiction. Pers Individ Dif.
2017;116:69-72.
87 Stead H, Bibby PA. Personality, fear of missing out and problematic
internet use and their relationship to subjective well-being. Comput
Human Behav. 2017;76:534-40.
88 Ryan R, Deci EL. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of
intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am Psy-
chol. 2000;55:68-78.
89 Koole SL, Schlinkert C, Maldei T, Baumann N. Becoming who you
are: an integrative review of self-determination theory and person-
ality systems interactions theory. J Pers. 2019;87:15-36.
90 Williams KD. Ostracism. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:425-52.
91 Lahey BB. Public health significance of neuroticism. Am Psychol.
2009;64:241-56.
92 Casale S, Fioravanti G, Rugai L. Grandiose and vulnerable narcis-
sists: who is at higher risk for social networking addiction?
Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2016;19:510-5.
93 Milyavskaya M, Saffran M, Hope N, Koestner R. Fear of missing out:
prevalence, dynamics, and consequences of experiencing FOMO.
Motiv Emot. 2018;42:725-37.
94 Elhai JD, Yang H, Montag C. Cognitive- and emotion-related dys-
functional coping processes: transdiagnostic mechanisms explain-
ing depression and anxiety’s relations with problematic smartphone
use. Curr Addict Rep. 2019;6:410-7.
95 Brand M, Young KS, Laier C, Wolfling K, Potenza MN. Integrating
psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the
development and maintenance of specific Internet-use disorders: an
interaction of person-affect-cognition-execution (I-PACE) model.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71:252-66.
96 Brand M, Wegmann E, Stark R, Muller A, Wolfling K, Robbins TW,
et al. The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE)
model for addictive behaviors: update, generalization to addictive
behaviors beyond internet-use disorders, and specification of the
process character of addictive behaviors. Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
2019;104:1-10.
97 Elhai JD, Tiamiyu MF, Weeks JW, Levine JC, Picard KJ, Hall BJ.
Depression and emotion regulation predict objective smart-
phone use measured over one week. Pers Individ Dif. 2018;133:
21-8.
98 Montag C, Blaszkiewicz K, Lachmann B, Sariyska R, Andone I,
Trendafilov B, et al. Recorded behavior as a valuable resource for
diagnostics in mobile phone addiction: evidence from psychoinfor-
matics. Behav Sci (Basel). 2015;5:434-42.
99 Rozgonjuk D, Levine JC, Hall BJ, Elhai JD. The association between
problematic smartphone use, depression and anxiety symptom
severity, and objectively measured smartphone use over one week.
Comput Human Behav. 2018;87:10-7.
100 Montag C, Elhai JD. A new agenda for personality psychology in the
digital age? Pers Individ Dif. 2019;147:128-34.
101 Chen J, Liang Y, Mai C, Zhong X, Qu C. General deficit in inhibitory
control of excessive smartphone users: evidence from an event-
related potential study. Front Psychol. 2016;7:511.
102 Montag C, Markowetz A, Blaszkiewicz K, Andone I, Lachmann B,
Sariyska R, et al. Facebook usage on smartphones and gray
matter volume of the nucleus accumbens. Behav Brain Res. 2017;
329:221-8.
103 Montag C, Zhao Z, Sindermann C, Xu L, Fu M, Li J, et al. Internet
communication disorder and the structure of the human brain: initial
insights on WeChat addiction. Sci Rep. 2018;8:2155.
104 Montag C, Becker B. Psychological and neuroscientific advances to
understand Internet use disorder. Neuroforum. 2019;25:99-107.
105 Sherman LE, Hernandez LM, Greenfield PM, Dapretto M. What the
brain ’Likes’: neural correlates of providing feedback on social
media. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2018;13:699-707.
106 Sherman LE, Payton AA, Hernandez LM, Greenfield PM, Dapretto
M. The power of the Like in adolescence: effects of peer influence
on neural and behavioral responses to social media. Psychol Sci.
2016;27:1027-35.
107 Dimidjian S, Barrera M Jr, Martell C, Munoz RF, Lewinsohn PM. The
origins and current status of behavioral activation treatments for
depression. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2011;7:1-38.
108 Elhai JD, Levine JC, Dvorak RD, Hall BJ. Fear of missing out, need
for touch, anxiety and depression are related to problematic
smartphone use. Comput Human Behav. 2016;63:509-16.
109 Fitz N, Kushlev K, Jagannathan R, Lewis T, Paliwal D, Ariely D.
Batching smartphone notifications can improve well-being. Comput
Human Behav. 2019;101:84-94.
Braz J Psychiatry. 2020;00(00)
FOMO review 7