ArticlePDF Available

Measuring centrality in film narratives using dynamic character interaction networks

Authors:
  • Kinomatics

Abstract and Figures

The tools of social network analysis offer a promising framework for studying fictional texts and the relational activity of the characters therein. The goal of this paper is to offer both a conceptual refinement of the project of measuring the centrality of characters within narratives using network tools, as well as the proposal of a novel measure with which to do so. Conceptually, we argue that as questions of time, order and sequence are central in narratives, measures of characters’ narrative importance should be based on dynamic network representations which respect the time-ordering of narrative events. We suggest a directed dynamic measure of relative character importance based on character interactions and illustrate it through an examination of gender in the 2015 film Star Wars: The Force Awakens. We find that the measure helps illuminate important narrative dynamics which cannot be captured by static measures, and presents a platform on which future character network research can productively build.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
1
Measuring centrality in film narratives using
dynamic character interaction networks
1
Pete Jones1*, Eithne Quinn2, and Johan Koskinen3
1 School of Social Sciences, University of Manchester, United Kingdom.
2 School of Arts, Languages and Cultures, University of Manchester, United Kingdom.
3 Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Australia, and The Institute of Analytic
Sociology, Linköping University, Sweden.
*Corresponding author. E-mail: pete.jones@manchester.ac.uk
2
.
Accepted for publication 26/3/2020.
Abstract
The tools of social network analysis offer a promising framework for studying fictional texts and the relational
activity of the characters therein. The goal of this paper is to offer both a conceptual refinement of the
project of measuring the centrality of characters within narratives using network tools, as well as the proposal
of a novel measure with which to do so. Conceptually, we argue that as questions of time, order and sequence
are central in narratives, measures of characters’ narrative importance should be based on dynamic network
representations which respect the time-ordering of narrative events. We suggest a directed dynamic measure
of relative character importance based on character interactions and illustrate it through an examination of
gender in the 2015 film Star Wars: The Force Awakens. We find that the measure helps illuminate important
narrative dynamics which cannot be captured by static measures, and presents a platform on which future
character network research can productively build.
Keywords: Character networks, Dialogue networks, Dynamic centrality, Narrative, Interactions, Gender
1. Introduction
This paper aims to develop a character interaction-based approach to investigating the positions of
characters within film narratives. As we outline in the following section, several studies now exist which
recognise the potential of network analysis for offering insights into the study of fictional narrative texts and
the characters therein (e.g. Moretti 2011; Sack 2014; Waumans et al. 2015). These studies range from in-depth
textual readings of particular texts to computational interventions in literary network extraction with little to
no literary analysis. What the studies all share is the assumption that network tools might assist in the analysis
of narrative texts, an assumption largely born out of the growing field of digital humanities scholarship. In this
paper, we aim to contribute to the literature in this area by intervening in the discussion of how we relate node-
level network measures to interpretative readings of characters’ positions within narratives. Engaging with key
ideas from narratology and narrative comprehension scholarship, we argue that the dominant approach of
applying off-the-shelf centrality measures such as degree and betweenness to aggregated static character
networks limits the usefulness of the character networks approach because of the amount of narrative
1
Published version available from https://www.journals.elsevier.com/social-networks.
2
Part of this work by the first author was completed as a Visiting Researcher to the Social Networks Lab in the Melbourne
School of Psychological Sciences.
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
2
information that is lost through aggregation. Instead, we draw on recent developments in representing and
analysing temporal network dynamics to develop a dynamic measure of the relative narrative positions of
characters through time (Broccatelli et al. 2016; Everett et al. 2018; Moody 2002). Our contention is that dynamic
approaches such as the one we pursue here are needed in order to capture the way the story is told through its
characters. Thus, we contribute both a conceptual refinement to the project of measuring narrative centrality
in character networks, as well as the proposal of a novel measure for doing so. These contributions are offered
as exploratory steps towards a more analytically productive platform for future network-based study of narrative
texts.
The structure of the paper is as follows. In the next section, we provide an outline of existing approaches to
analysing fictional texts through character network models. Then, we consider the problem of how to say
something meaningful about the relative importance of nodes within the narrative, focusing on how to
appropriately represent narratives as network data given the importance of sequence in narratives. We suggest
a number of principles for measuring narrative centrality, and outline a method for configuring film texts as
character interaction networks based on the distribution of lines of dialogue between characters. Following this
discussion, we propose a new approach to measuring the narrative positions of characters in fictional texts and
define a dynamic relational measure based on the acts of speaking and being spoken to. As well as offering a
conceptual modelling framework for analysing the relative importance of characters in a narrative, some formal
properties of the proposed measure are further explored in the appendix. Because of the dynamic nature of the
measure, only some properties are analytically tractable, and we cannot provide an exhaustive formal analysis
within the scope of this paper. However, the analysis of idealised examples we present could be complemented
by simulations in the future. Finally, we offer an application of this measure to the 2015 film Star Wars: The
Force Awakens, drawing out the gendered dynamics in the film. This case study illustrates some of the things
the measure allows us to observe about the ways in which characters relate to the narrative that cannot be
inferred from static measures. In particular, through a focus on the vocal and relational disempowerment of
women in film narratives, we show that the approach allows us to use character network data to engage with
existing debates in the study of social representation in fictional narrative texts in a way which previous
character network approaches cannot. We argue that by focusing on the conceptual links between narrative
texts and the way we represent them as data, network-based measures such as this have the potential to
contribute more to character-oriented scholarship on those texts than existing approaches have been able to
offer.
2. Character networks
A character network is any network representation in which the nodes are characters in a fictional text.
There now exists a number of character network-based studies of literary texts (e.g. Agarwal et al. 2012; Elson
et al. 2010; Grayson et al. 2016; Kydros et al. 2015; Jayannavar et al 2015; Min and Park 2016; Moretti 2011;
Sack 2014; Waumans et al. 2015). These literary character network studies have primarily been concerned with
questions of network topology, the process of computational extraction of networks from literary texts, and
validating existing literary and network theories using extracted character network data. Though these studies
vary in tie definition, scope and audience, one of the common threads they share is their recognition that the
analytical tools of social network analysis can prove fruitful for answering questions of substantive interest to
scholars of fictional cultural texts. For example, Elson et al. (2010) use network analytical tools and concepts
such as clique detection, density and network size to explore hypotheses related to literary theory on the
relationship between urbanity/rurality and character interactions in Victorian novels. Network tools have also
been applied to literary texts at the node level through the application of centrality measures to the characters
in a text. In practice, this means calculating common measures such as degree, betweenness and closeness for
nodes in the character network in order to make claims about the relationship between those characters and
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
3
the narrative (e.g. Kagan et al. 2019; Kydros et al. 2015; Moretti 2011), an approach we discuss further in the
next section.
Despite the growing interest in this approach from a literary perspective, the use of character networks to
study film has taken a different tack. Work in this area has been concerned with methods for large-scale
computational extraction of networks from film corpora rather than questions of how a network-based approach
can aid the reading and analysis of particular film texts. These studies are thus conducted from a primarily
computational standpoint, aiming to develop tools that extract networks automatically from scripts (Agarwal et
al. 2014; Gil et al. 2011; Mourchid et al. 2018), video (Lv et al. 2018; Weng et al. 2009) or subtitle files (Kagan
et al. 2019). These automatic extraction methods require the construction of undirected ties based on some
definition of co-presence within a scene rather than direct interaction between characters. As such, one of the
compromises involved in pursuing a more computational character networks approach is that by defining ties
based on scene-sharing we lose the capacity to answer many character-oriented questions such as how much the
characters speak or to whom.
It is useful to contrast the character networks approach with the related but largely disjunct body of work
concerning “narrative networks” (e.g. Abell 2004; Bearman and Stovel 2000; Padgett 2018; Smith 2007). Work
on narrative networks has broadly aimed at mapping the temporal relations between narrative events to try to
build a picture of how people construct identities and shared meanings through the assembly of historical events
into narratives. As such, the narrative networks literature takes a different conceptual approach, as it is
primarily concerned with understanding narrativity as a dimension of social life, rather than the study of fictional
narrative texts. Some of the insights of this work, such as the importance of time for understanding narrative
phenomena, are directly relevant to the development of character networks scholarship and will be drawn on
in the next section. As there are some differences in aims, objectives and methods pursued in the two
approaches, it might be useful to separate character networks and narrative networks as two distinct sub-fields
of network research.
3. Relating network positions to narrative positions
One of the ways in which network tools can be brought to bear on the study of narrative texts is through the
application of measures of node importance to the characters in a text in order to explore existing debates and
theories concerning issues such as visibility, representation and the relationship between characters and
narratives. The strategy that has been pursued in existing character networks approaches has been to apply
standard network centrality measures (such as degree and betweenness) in order to make claims about the
relative positions of characters within narratives. For example, Moretti (2011) looks at the degree centrality of
characters in Hamlet, using binary and undirected data in which a tie represents the fact that any amount of
words flowed between two characters. He uses this network data to claim, for example, that Claudius is the
“second most central character in the play” because he interacts with 13 characters, one more than Horatio and
two fewer than Hamlet (Moretti 2011, 88). Kydros et al. similarly present degree, betweenness, and closeness
measures for characters in Homer’s Iliad, though they note that interpretation of the results is “not so easy”
(2015, 125). In this section, we argue that the utility of this approach is limited because it is based on network
representations that do not adequately capture narratives in ways that allow for meaningful conceptualisation
and interpretation of the relationships between characters and narratives.
Network representations
Brandes et al (2013) note that there are two steps to obtaining a network model: first, there must be an
appropriate abstraction of a phenomenon to a network concept; then, there must be an appropriate
representation of that network concept in network data. In the case of character networks, the phenomenon in
question is a narrative text. Thus, the first step is to abstract the narrative text to a network concept. As Moretti
(2011) has noted, Alex Woloch (2003) offers a compelling basis for so doing in his work on major and minor
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
4
characters in the literary novel. Woloch argues that what we see as major and minor characters is the product
of the “character-space” that those characters occupy within a broader “character-system” (Woloch 2003). In
narratives, our attention is a finite resource which can only be allocated to certain narrative elements at once.
Thus, we can think of narratives as a distribution of attention, as “[n]arrative meaning takes shape in the
dynamic flux of attention and neglect toward the various characters who are locked within the same story but
have radically different positions within the narrative (Woloch 2003, 2).
Moreover, this conceptualisation of narrative texts as a dynamic distribution of attention through a character
system is explicitly relational in Woloch’s theory, as he argues that “the emplacement of a character within the
narrative form is largely comprised by his or her relative position vis-a-vis other characters” (Woloch 2003, 18,
italics in original). Extending these ideas to more visual media such as film finds resonance with much of the
theory on film narratives, where it has been noted that the technical practices of mainstream Hollywood film
production were designed “to guide audience attention to salient narrative events from moment to moment”
(Thompson 1999, 1). Work in this area has aimed to pair an analysis of film narratives with a deep understanding
of narrative comprehension and how viewers process visual narratives, and has emphasised the role of relations
between narrative elements (including characters) in the process. For example, David Bordwell’s theory of
narrative states that “comprehending a narrative requires assigning it some coherence. At a local level, the
viewer must grasp character relations, lines of dialogue, relations between shots, and so on” (Bordwell 1985,
34). Edward Branigan notes how these local events feed into a “global interpretation of changing data measured
through sets of relationships” (Branigan 1992, 4). Moreover, Branigan argues that psychological experiments
have shown that “an individual's attention does not spread equally through a narrative text but works forward
and backward in an uneven manner in constructing large-scale, hierarchical patterns which represent a
particular story as an abstract grouping of knowledge” (Branigan 1992, 16). Thus, we can think of film narratives
as dynamic patterns of character-driven events from which we construct these abstract relational groupings of
knowledge.
These ideas suggest that thinking of narratives as systems of character relations wherein our attention as
readers or viewers of the text is distributed unevenly into hierarchical patterns seems like a useful way of looking
at a narrative text. However, the second step in obtaining the network model (the representation of this network
concept as network data) is more fraught because existing character networks approaches typically represent
the character system using static, aggregated network data. A brief consideration of how narratives have been
defined and theorised will illustrate why this kind of network representation limits the utility of such approaches.
Narrative as dynamic
Narratologists have long recognised that the narrative is by definition a dynamic and temporal phenomenon,
and the importance of temporal sequence is fundamental to almost all definitions of narrative (Abbott 2008; Bal
2017; Chatman 1980; Genette 1980; Ricœur 1980). One of the ways that narrative is frequently defined is through
its relation to story. Story is usually defined as the events that take place, while narrative is defined as how that
story is told. This opens up the question of story discourse versus narrative discourse and, crucially, story time
versus narrative time (Chatman 1980; Genette 1980). Seymour Chatman (1980) theorised this as the “double
time structuring” of narrativity – our engagement with a narrative asks us to understand the way in which story
events are ordered even while those events may be told to us in a completely different order. For example, a
simple story might involve 3 events: A = my alarm failed to go off; B = I missed the bus; and C = I was late to
work. However, I might narrativise these story elements in a different order (C-A-B) by telling the story through
the following narrative: “I was late to work because my alarm didn’t go off which made me miss the bus. What
is important here is the recognition that when we talk about narrativity, we are talking about sequence and
time. Work on narrative comprehension has argued that “the appearance and ordering of story elements in the
[narrative] discourse is critical to the process of comprehension” (Niehaus and Young 2014, 561), and that the
decision to tell a story one way or another affects the ways in which we make sense of the story and the
importance of the elements therein (Bordwell 1985; Branigan 1992). Therefore, network measures which are
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
5
invariant under permutations of sequence are missing a very central aspect of what makes a narrative a
narrative.
The importance of sequence and time to the analysis of narratives has been integral in the literature on
narrative networks, as noted in Section 2. For example, Padgett (2018) presents a complex and sophisticated
model of how William Faulkner uses narration to build multiple dimensions of time and memory assembly into
his novel Absalom, Absalom!, relating this to cognitive identity-construction processes. However, the character
networks literature has paid much less attention to these temporal considerations, and has largely continued to
treat narrative texts as static networks. For example, Moretti’s Hamlet study (2011) defines a static tie between
nodes indicating that any amount of dialogue passed between two characters, while Elson et al. (2010) and
Waumans et al. (2015) define static ties based on the identification of adjacent attributed speech for two
characters within the novel’s text. However, as Branigan notes, “recognizing the complexity and dynamism of a
text is usually more important than assigning a final, decisive label to it” (Branigan 1992, 8). Static character
network representations are therefore unlikely to offer satisfactory models of fictional texts as they compress
and flatten narratives through aggregation such that narrative time is lost in the representation
3
(Moody et al.
2005). Even when the frequency or amount of character interaction is retained through tie weights (e.g. as in
Masías et al. 2017; Ruegg and Lee 2019), the temporal sequence of these interactions is lost. As narratologist
Gérard Genette has argued, this elimination of time from the representation “is not only not sticking to the
text, but is quite simply killing it” (Genette 1980, 35).
We argue that we should therefore think instead of narrative character networks as dynamic networks, as
defined by Kontoleon et al. (2013). As with most other cases where we are interested in representing dynamic
networks based on relational activity rather than fixed, stable relations, this has direct implications for our
ability to capture centrality concepts using network analytical measures (Falzon et al. 2018; Moody 2002). Node-
level measures based on a static network representation give us a static measure of that node’s narrative
importance. However, it does not make sense to talk about a character’s position within a narrative as a fixed
quality because character positioning evolves dynamically over the course of a narrative. A character can occupy
many different narrative positions throughout the course of a narrative, and the dynamics of this ought to be of
interest in any analysis of that character’s narrative importance.
Principles for measuring relative narrative importance
The previous discussion allows us to specify some principles which an appropriate measure of the relative
importance of nodes in a narrative character network ought to satisfy. Firstly, such a measure ought to be
dynamic and based on a temporally disaggregated network representation of the narrative text. Secondly, such
a measure ought to be linked to a character’s narrative activity, in order to satisfy the idea of narratives as a
distributed field of attention between characters. When we focus on one character, we always do so at the
expense of others, and so we assume that narrative activity is always an indicator of narrative importance; the
more a character does something, the more attention they receive, the more character space they take up in
the narrative, and the more information we have with which to “qualify” them as characters (Bal 2017). Thirdly,
such a measure ought to take into account their position in the relational structure. That is, our sense of a
node’s relative importance (based dynamically on their narrative activity) should be dependent to some degree
on the relative importance of the nodes with which they interact. David Bordwell has argued that when watching
a film, one cognitive activity of the viewer is “looking for relevance, testing each event for its pertinence to the
3
It should be noted that a few papers have analysed character networks from the perspective of longitudinal networks
(Agarwal et al. 2012; Fischer et al 2017; Min and Park 2016; Prado et al. 2016). They analysed the networks as a series of
snap-shots by aggregating character interactions within each chapter and then calculating static network statistics (including
node-level measures such as degree) for each chapter. Thus, though such approaches aim to discuss narratives in terms of
temporal dynamics, many of the points made here about the appropriateness of static network measures apply also to these
papers as a great deal of information about the frequency and sequence of events within chapters is still lost through
aggregation.
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
6
action which the film (or scene, or character action) seems to be basically setting forth" (1985, 34). Based on
this, we assume that a character is more likely to be meaningfully engaging with the narrative if they are
interacting with a character who has already occupied our attention than if they interact with a character we
do not know yet. The third principle also ensures that such a measure will be relational, as we could easily
satisfy the first two principles by tracking character activity (such as lines spoken) through time without using
any relational information from the network.
4. Defining a measure of narrative positioning
Data representation
In order to operationalise the ideas in the previous section, we assume a dialogue-based character interaction
network model designed to investigate questions of vocal and relational (dis)empowerment in Hollywood
narratives (see Jones 2018). Unlike existing approaches to character networks which tend to deploy automatic
extraction methods based on textual proximity (discussed in Section 2), the manual approach proposed here
constructs ties based on direct interaction between characters, as we cannot be sure from scene co-presence
or speech adjacency that interaction between characters actually occurred, and thus we cannot learn anything
about the pattern, frequency, direction or sequence of the interactions between characters in the narrative.
We take the units of analysis in our representation to be the vocal interactions between characters, an approach
which allows us to measure the amount of vocal activity of characters, as well as the interpersonal structures
that emerge among characters as a result of these patterns of interaction.
Thus, for a given film, a network can be constructed by recording each line of dialogue between characters.
A line of dialogue here is defined as a single continuous stream of speech; a line ends when the dialogue pauses
to allow for a response, or the scene or topic changes notably. To be included in the network, a character must
both speak and be named, and only intelligible lines of dialogue spoken by and to named speaking characters
are recorded. The ties in such a network are directed in that each line of dialogue has a distinct sender and
receiver(s). This is important as there are valuable distinctions in speaking and being spoken to.
These interactions can be represented as a static, aggregated network graph, with the frequency of
interaction between characters being retained as a tie value or weight, allowing for simple visualisation of the
network. However, for analytical purposes the basic data structure is the time-ordered and disaggregated list
of interactions, the “raw materials” from which the static graph-based representation may be constructed
(Amati et al. 2019). Thus, although this manual network extraction method is labour-intensive in comparison
with computational approaches, it results in rich directed character interaction data which can serve as
something of a gold standard when considering what other approaches are able to capture. In the conclusions,
we discuss the ways in which other approaches based on automatic network extraction can satisfy the principles
set forth here.
An additional distinction to make in establishing this data representation relates to the extent to which the
dynamic network has concurrent relations or not. We can distinguish between concurrency across actors and
within actors. For the cases considered by Moody (2002) and Kontoleon et al. (2013), ties may be concurrent
across actors in the sense that there may be overlap between ties between distinct pairs, for example two
distinct pairs may be in sexual relationships or in concurrent phone calls. An example where actions are not
concurrent across actors is passing the ball in a football match. From a narrative perspective, concurrency across
actors, while in principle possible, does not make narrative sense. Concurrency within actors has two separate
dimensions that we could call sender exclusivity and receiver exclusivity. The call network of Kontoleon et al.
(2013) has both sender and receiver exclusivity in the sense that it is assumed that an actor can only be in a
phone conversation with one person at a time. In Moody’s (2002) network there is neither sender nor receiver
exclusivity, something which is deemed crucial for disease transmission. The dialogue-based character
interaction network representation we describe here assumes sender exclusivity but not necessarily receiver
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
7
exclusivity. These narrative considerations taken together suggest a measure with a different aim than to
summarise the temporal-topological structure as in Holme and Saramäki (2012) and Kontoleon et al. (2015).
Based on this data representation, we propose the cumulative measure  which represents the score of
node at time . The narrative activity referred to by the second principle specified in Section 3 can be
approximated using the character interaction data outlined above such that vocal interactions are the basis of
our understanding of a character’s relative importance within the narrative. However, in order to distinguish
between speaking and being spoken-to, we define two closely-related versions of the measure: the speaking
measure 
, which tells us the speaking score  of node at time and bases relative character
importance on speaking activity; and the spoken-to measure 
, which tells us the spoken-to score  of node
at time and bases relative character importance on reception of dialogue. This allows for consideration that
the narrative importance conveyed through speaking activity may be different from the narrative importance
conveyed by being spoken to.
Definition of the speaking measure
Defining the measure at each time ensures satisfaction of the first principle, which dictates that the
measure should be calculated at a level of temporal disaggregation so that character scores are specific to each
measured point of time. Using the time-ordered interaction data, time is equivalent to a line of dialogue, with
lines being temporally ordered in a sequence of positive integers, such that the first line of dialogue takes place
at , the second line at , and so on.
The second principle suggests that scores should increase upon narrative activity which, in the case of the
dialogue network data, is a vocal interaction. The measure should therefore be cumulative, increasing
monotonically when characters speak, and the third principle suggests that the scale of this increase should
depend on the character(s) being spoken to. We can satisfy each of the three principles above by following a
similar approach to that taken in Everett et al. (2018). These authors present a centrality-like measure (based
on a dynamic two-mode network representation called the bi-dynamic line graph) in which nodes accumulate
points through network activity, and the amount of points gained by a node in each temporally-situated activity
is dependent in part on the points held by the other nodes also participating in that activity.
Following a similar logic, we propose letting speaking character ’s score increase by a fraction of the score
of character being spoken to at time . This fraction can be defined using a weighting parameter , such that


 
 
(1)
Thus, the amount that ’s score increases depends on the score of character , ensuring that the measure is
relational, as well as on the value of , allowing us to specify how much we want the relational aspect to
contribute to our sense of ’s importance.
The measure has a flavour of how eigenvector centrality is defined on static graphs. Holme and Saramäki
(2012) develop a dynamic node-level measure that respects the time-ordering of interactions explicitly to follow
the logic of eigenvector centrality. However, their measure does not take into account the directionality of
interactions and, because of its relation to eigenvector centrality, it is not non-decreasing, and thus does not
satisfy the second principle above as some interactions can lower one’s centrality score. This property could
have interesting implications in other contexts where researchers may be interested in investigating how talking
to the ‘wrong’ people could have adverse effects on your position in the dialogue network. However, this is not
appropriate for narrative character networks given our argument that narrative activity always increases the
amount of character space taken up by a character.
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
8
The measure as specified above would be sufficient for a case where a line of dialogue has only one recipient.
However, as we do not require receiver exclusivity, the measure may be modified to account for multiple
recipients. Simply adding a fraction of each spoken-to character ’s score to ’s score would mean that lines
with multiple recipients have a larger effect on ’s score than lines with only one recipient. However, lines
addressing multiple people are not inherently more narratively important than lines addressing one person, so
we propose instead multiplying by the average score of the spoken-to characters. Thus, let  be a variable
that can take either the value 1 or 0 to indicate whether a character is spoken to by character at time .
Then the total number of characters spoken to by speaking character is given by 
, where is the
total number of nodes in the network. The sum of all scores for spoken-to characters is given by 
 
.
The speaking measure can then be more generally defined as





 
(2)
which tells us that character ’s relative importance at line is equal to whatever ’s importance was at line
plus a fraction of the importance of the character(s) to whom they are speaking
4
. For characters not
speaking at line , (2) will simply imply that they retain their score from as  will be equal to zero for
these characters. Thus, so long as characters scores at time (the starting scores before a line has been
spoken) take positive nonzero values, the measure is monotonically cumulative, increasing only at moments of
narrative activity as expressed through character interactions. There are a number of options for how to specify
scores at , but giving all characters a score of 1 is a convenient choice for two reasons. Firstly, it allows us to
assume that at the beginning of the narrative, no character has demanded any more attention than any other
and thus has not occupied any more character space than any other. Secondly, the absolute values given by the
measure are of less interest than the relative values such as those given by 




(which will be the
same for any value of 
 shared by all characters, see Appendix A.1), so taking the first positive integer is as
good a starting point as any.
Definition of the spoken-to measure
A similar measure can be defined in which importance is based on being spoken to rather than speaking.
Defining the spoken-to measure is slightly simpler than the speaking measure as there is only ever one speaking
character per line (by sender exclusivity). The spoken-to measure should let spoken-to character ’s score
increase at line by a fraction of the score of speaking character , with this fraction being given by weighting
parameter . In this case, let  be a variable taking the value 1 if node is being spoken to by at line .
Then, for every node , the spoken-to measure can be defined as 

 
 
, which tells
us that character ’s relative importance at line is equal to whatever ’s importance was at line plus a
fraction of the importance of character speaking to at line . Characters’ scores will only increase when they
are spoken to and will do so monotonically; characters not being spoken to retain their scores from .
Formally, it should be noted that both the spoken-to measure and the speaking measure share the property
that they are not invariant under permutations of sequence. This is crucial as it allows the measure to reflect
4
The specification of 
 ensures that the equation cannot possibly entail division by zero for non-speaking
actors.
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
9
the sequence of events which, as we have already noted, is at the heart of narrative questions relating to how
the story is told. This property also distinguishes the measure from standard approaches to measuring node
centrality in sequences, such as looking at the cumulative weighted degree of nodes after each interaction.
Consider, for example, two sequences: (1) AB, BA, AB, and (2) AB, AB, BA. Table 1 calculates the
spoken-to measure for both sequences, assuming that A begins each sequence with score and B begins each
sequence with score . If we cumulatively calculate weighted in-degree for (1) and (2), A will end each
sequence with a score of 1, and B will end each sequence with a score of 2. However, Table 1 shows that both
nodes end with different scores in sequences (1) and (2) using the measure developed here. Thus, our sense of
each character’s importance is dependent on how the action unfolds.
(1)
(2)
A
B
A
B









Table 1. Spoken-to scores for sequences (1) and (2).
Normalisation and properties
Both spoken to and speaking measures can be normalised by the total score across actors as outlined above.
Here we consider some properties of the measures which generalise to all dynamic character interaction
networks, in order to illustrate how the measure works in cases where narrative focus is distributed differently.
Given the dynamic structure of the narrative network, it is not trivial to derive properties of the measures. The
measures can be related to the achievable maximum and minimum scores for a number of stylised dynamic
networks (details are provided in Appendix A.2). Although mainstream film narratives are typically more complex
than the kinds of networks in the stylised graphs, exploring them helps us to understand how a key aspect of the
measure, the “feedback” introduced by , works to either concentrate or diffuse narrative attention in the
character system depending on how dialogue is distributed.
For example, we can consider two stylised dynamic graphs that have minimal narrative focus in terms of
being spoken to. One would be a “game of telephone” chain of dialogue, where each actor being spoken to then
speaks to someone who has not yet been spoken to. If the length of this chain is less than , then the th
actor in the chain will have a score of
 . In other words, the character’s importance will only depend on
where in the timeline they appear. Another example of minimal narrative focus would be where only one
character speaks and other characters are only spoken to once, for acts of speaking, . For this star-
like dynamic graph, actors would have a score of if they are spoken to, and otherwise. In the game of
telephone there is more heterogeneity in scores as the narrative importance accumulates, but in the star-like
network, the repeated speaking acts mean that the narrative importance does not accumulate as the importance
gained by being spoken to is never passed on. In both of these cases, narrative focus is minimal because our
sense of an actor’s importance is never reinforced by an addressee responding to their speech, or by another
speaker speaking to the same addressee.
On the other hand, we can consider two stylised dyadic dynamic networks which produce maximal narrative
focus: one in which only one actor speaks and the other in which the actors take turns addressing each other.
These can be thought of as graphs that maximise spoken-to scores as no act of speaking is wasted on addressing
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
10
someone who has not been spoken to before. For the first scenario, the score of the actor being spoken to
increases linearly but for the second, the act of reinforcing each other’s narrative importance means that the
scores increase in a curvilinear way and, after a certain number of lines of dialogue, the scores of both actors
overtakes that of the spoken-to actor in the first scenario. The maximal score after lines of dialogue can thus
be thought of as an achievable maximum spoken-to score.
5. Application: women in popular film
In this section we provide an example of how the measure can be applied to real questions about narrative
texts using the example of gendered representation in film. In particular, we focus on the issue of the vocal and
relational disempowerment of female characters in popular Hollywood cinema.
Relational and vocal disempowerment
Feminist film theory has long theorised the way that a masculine perspective is privileged in Hollywood
through the “male gaze”, understood as the way that narrative cinema produces a masculine viewing position
which renders male characters as active narrative forces and female characters as passive and defined primarily
by their “to-be-looked-at-ness” (Mulvey 1975). While these ideas are based in the visual aspects of cinematic
representation, we can see how the visual-narrative paradigm of the male gaze produces effects which also play
out through dialogue. For example, feminist film scholars have discussed how the telling of stories from a male
point of view has led to a silencing of women’s voices, and a representational system in which women exist
primarily through their relations to male protagonists (Haskell 1987; Kaplan 1983). This point has been
popularised through the simple device known as the Bechdel test (based on a comic strip by Alison Bechdel),
which asks whether a film has two female characters who speak to each other about something other than a
man (Bechdel 1986, 22). Though originally intended as a joke, this intentionally low bar has proved too difficult
to clear for an alarming number of Hollywood films. While the literature on women and film offers a theoretical
basis for understanding why narratives might not give women the opportunity to interact with each other, there
has been a conspicuous lack of empirical investigations of the gendered distribution of dialogue in contemporary
Hollywood cinema, limiting our understanding of what Jennifer O’Meara calls the “vocal and verbal
(dis)empowerment” of women on-screen (O’Meara 2016).
Most available empirical evidence on vocal representation comes from a number of content analyses
published annually which look at mainstream films released that year and offer a quantitative report on the
representation therein. From these yearly reports, we know that over the last decade or so (the period for which
data is available), the proportion of speaking characters that are female in top-grossing Hollywood films is around
30% (Lauzen 2018; Smith et al. 2018). The consistency of these findings is remarkable: in the Smith et al. (2018)
study, the prevalence of female speaking characters was never lower than 28.1% and never higher than 32.8% in
a given year across their 11-year sample, while in the Lauzen (2018) study, the annual prevalence of women
among speaking characters was never less than 30% and never more than 34%. This consistency suggests that the
under-representation of women in speaking roles is part of the formula of the mainstream Hollywood screenplay.
However, these figures cannot tell us very much about how vocal disempowerment actually operates in
Hollywood narratives. For example, knowing that 30% of the speaking characters in a given film are female does
not tell us whether 30% of the lines in the film were spoken by females, nor does it tell us to whom those lines
were spoken (Jones 2018).
This latter point leads to the second aspect of the disempowerment of women in Hollywood narratives that
we focus on here: relational disempowerment. By foregrounding female homosocial interaction, the Bechdel
test insists on a relational understanding of the narrative effects of gendered representation in film narratives.
In doing so, it chimes with scholarship on post-feminism and backlash discourses in popular media culture. This
work has identified a trend in Hollywood towards “taking into account” feminism by representing strong female
characters, while simultaneously individualising and depoliticising their narratives by isolating those female
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
11
characters among men rather than offering narrative opportunities for collective empowerment (Faludi 1992;
McRobbie 2004; Sutherland and Feltey 2016).
However, much of the scholarship on women in film, especially in relation to active female heroes, has been
geared towards identifying and theorising the strong female character at the level of individual characterisation.
This is particularly the case in the literature on women in action cinema, where there has been a wealth of
scholarship fleshing out and analysing the characteristics of what has been variously termed the “action babe”,
“action chick” or “action heroine” (e.g. Brown 2011; Inness 2004; O’Day 2004; Schubart 2007). However, by
focusing primarily on the characteristics of the individual female hero in discussions of gendered representation
in action cinema, these studies overlook the ways in which such ostensibly strong female characters can be
disempowered through their relational positioning within the overall narrative and lack of independence from
men (Purse 2011). Moreover, such an approach risks losing sight of what E. Ann Kaplan refers to as “female
positionings,” and “the larger structuring of the narrative and on the placement of the woman within that
narrative” (Kaplan 1983, 2). Thus, there may be a lot to gain from developing an approach which re-embeds
female characters into the context of the broader character system of the narrative. A network-based approach
offers a way to situate the discussion of these female characters within the broader relational character system
in which they play out their role in the story.
Elsewhere Jones (2018) has argued that character interaction networks offer an illuminating model for
investigating both the vocal and relational elements of female representation identified here. In particular, the
approach illustrates the limitations of knowing only the prevalence of women among speaking characters, as the
proportion of dialogue spoken by women can differ significantly from this simple character distribution; it can
highlight how the gendered distribution of dialogue can enable or constrain female characters from accessing a
collective mode of empowerment in narratives; and it is able to identify important distinctions between speaking
and being spoken-to. Some aspects of this relational disempowerment, such as the marginalisation of female-
female relationships, can be further explored in more detail using dyadic approaches such as the relational event
model (Butts 2008). However, node-level measures such as the one developed in this paper can shine further
light on the problem by helping us to operationalise Kaplan’s notion of “female positionings” and consider where
the strong female character stands in terms of the relative importance of the characters in the overall narrative,
as defined by their relational activity.
The Force Awakens
To explore these questions about the gendered distribution of character interactions in film narratives, we
use the 2015 film Star Wars: The Force Awakens (henceforth simply The Force Awakens) as an example. The
Force Awakens serves as an interesting case study for a couple of reasons. For one, the film is the highest grossing
film of all time in the United States and the fourth highest grossing film of all time worldwide (Box Office Mojo
2019). This not only points to the immense popularity and cultural reach of the film, but also places the film
clearly within the context of top-grossing or “blockbuster” films addressed by existing Hollywood content
analyses. Furthermore, The Force Awakens sparked discussion about a more inclusive and diverse direction for
representation in the hugely popular and culturally impactful Star Wars franchise through the film’s inclusion of
female and non-white characters in central active roles. Much of this discussion centred around the prominence
of the character Rey (Daisy Ridley) as a key protagonist in the film. For example, following the film’s release,
Rey was credited with “carrying the heavily-mantled weight of the new series” (Kermode 2015); it was
proclaimed that “Rey is clearly the main character and our destined-hero for this trilogy,” that "Star Wars has a
female lead,” and that “the main male character is her sidekick” (Cox 2015); there were claims that, in the
film, “the male-centric universe of the original Star Wars gives way to a woman warrior [Rey] and a female
version of Yoda [Maz Kanata]” (Roddy 2015), and that “the plot of The Force Awakens, in fact, revolves around
[Rey’s abilities]” (Garber 2015). This not only renders it an interesting example for exploring node positions
through the lens of vocal and relational (dis)empowerment, but also provides a useful case for examining what
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
12
looking at characters’ positions within the overall narrative character system can tell us that focusing only on
the characterisation of the individual hero (Rey) cannot.
A brief summary of the plot of the film will help contextualise and aid understanding of the discussion of the
positions of the characters therein. The Force Awakens is the seventh feature film in the Star Wars franchise
and centres around a conflict between the tyrannical First Order and the Resistance. The film follows Poe
Dameron (Oscar Isaac), a Resistance fighter pilot, and a turncoat First Order Stormtrooper named Finn (John
Boyega) as they try to deliver a droid named BB-8 to the Resistance leaders as it contains information on the
whereabouts of Luke Skywalker (Mark Hammill). Finn and Dameron crash and are separated on the planet Jakku,
where a scavenger named Rey discovers BB-8 and agrees to help keep it safe. Finn encounters and joins Rey as
the First Order arrive and the two escape in the Millennium Falcon. Han Solo (Harrison Ford), the ship’s previous
owner, finds the ship and boards it, wishing to reclaim his property. Rey and Finn explain their situation to Han,
who agrees to help them deliver the droid to the Resistance, which is led by Han’s estranged lover Leia Organa
(Carrie Fisher). In the process of delivering BB-8 to the Resistance, Rey is captured by the First Order and
delivered to Kylo Ren (Adam Driver), one of the First Order’s chief commanders and the runaway son of Han and
Leia. While interrogating Rey, Ren discovers that she, like Ren, has control of an energy-like power known as
the Force. The Resistance fighters free Rey from the First Order base, destroy its main weapon and uncover the
map to Skywalker’s location.
In the dialogue data for The Force Awakens, there are 688 lines of dialogue distributed between 31 named
speaking characters. 22 percent of named speaking characters in the film are female (well below the industry
benchmark of around one-in-three), while 28.1 percent of the lines are spoken by female characters, who
account for 26.5 percent of the recipients of lines of dialogue.
Discussion
Figures 1 and 2 plot the speaking and spoken-to measures, respectively, using the value of 0.01. Lower
values allow for smoother trajectories for the characters but make the measure less sensitive, which makes it
harder for characters to establish their centrality later in the film. A second consequence of this insensitivity is
that the score of characters who establish a high centrality earlier in the film but do not continue to contribute
vocally to the film thereafter will decline at a much slower rate, and may end up with higher scores than
characters who contributed more in the latter stages of the film. Using higher values on the other hand, has
the opposite problem, being very sensitive to rises and falls in character centrality at each interaction, an effect
that is compounded later in the film as the scores of characters are generally higher. Thus, the value of can
be thought of as how many times a character would need to interact with another character to overtake their
score. For a value of 0.1 on the spoken-to measure, for example, character i would receive all of j’s centrality
after being spoken to ten times by j without reply. Thus, higher values allow characters to “steal the show”
at later stages of the film, as the number of interactions required to overtake other characters is lower. Further
illustration and discussion of some of these aspects of is provided in Appendix A.3. Setting at 0.01 here
offers a balance between these sensitivity issues. In lay terms, using this value of 0.01 for the speaking measure
means that each time a character speaks, their score becomes what it was prior to speaking plus one percent of
the average score of the characters to whom they speak. For the spoken-to measure, it means that each time a
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
13
character is spoken to, their score becomes what it was prior to being spoken to plus one percent of the score
of the character speaking to them.
The horizontal axis in Figures 1 and 2 indicates the sequence of lines, with all characters beginning with an
equal score. Key characters are labelled in the graph based on whether their normalised score at  (the end
of the film) is greater than their normalised score at (the beginning of the film). In each case scores have
Figure 2. Normalised speaking measure scores in The Force Awakens (λ = 0.01).
Figure 1. Normalised spoken-to measure scores in The Force Awakens (λ = 0.01).
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
14
been normalised to give relative scores by dividing the scores by the sum of all characters’ scores such that





. These relative scores effectively convey the share of total narrative importance, allowing
scores to both increase and decrease. This operationalises the idea of the narrative as a distribution of finite
attention and neglect between characters; for the normalised speaking scores, for example, when one character
speaks their share of the total narrative importance increases, while each non-speaking character’s share of the
total narrative importance declines slightly.
The first thing to note from Figures 1 and 2 is that Finn spends the majority of the film (including,
importantly, the final act) with the largest share of the scores on both the speaking and spoken-to measures.
This is significant in its own right as Finn is a person of colour (in a historically very white franchise) and, although
the focus here is on gender, this illustrates the broader appeal of being able to characterise characters’ dynamic
narrative positions for scholars of social representation in fictional texts. Moreover, while it is quite clear from
the graphs that Rey is central to the plot of the film, it is also clear that from the perspective of vocal activity
this centrality is secondary to Finn and, in the latter part of the film, Han. This is not to downplay the significance
of her character’s contribution to the narrative, but it should temper some of the claims made in the critical
reception to the film about her role. If Rey is the main character in The Force Awakens, then we might question
why Rey spends so little time holding the largest share of the scores on either of the measures. Moreover, when
looking across scores for all the characters in the film, we can certainly call into question the idea that the
male-centricity of the Star Wars universe has “given way,” given how few female characters separate themselves
from the pack throughout the narrative. The narrative is still driven vocally by men, for the most part.
There are also several things to note regarding the dynamic nature of the measure. We can see that all
characters begin at the same point on the vertical axis, due to starting with an equal score. However, as lines
are spoken, characters begin to delineate their narrative activity through their verbal interactions. As the
narrative evolves, different characters become more and less central. For example, Finn and Rey occupy a
prominent role in the narrative throughout the film, though Rey’s activity declines somewhat in the third act
due to her being imprisoned during this period. Han Solo emerges after the first act and becomes increasingly
central to the plot thereafter, while Leia rises in importance in the third act as she orchestrates the Resistance’s
fightback against the First Order. Kylo Ren remains fairly central throughout the film, even as other characters
dominate the narrative activity, though Ren’s centrality rises towards the end of the film as he comes face to
face with several of the film’s other central characters for the first time. The measure thus allows us to say
something about the relative narrative positions of these characters within the context of the dynamics of the
narrative.
Consider, for example, the position of Maz Kanata in Figure 1. Over the course of a few key scenes in the
second act of the film, Maz (Lupita Nyong’o) becomes very important to the plot, and her share of the scores
increases significantly during this period. During these scenes, Han, Finn and Rey are dependent on Maz to help
them contact the Resistance, and Maz is thus pivotal to the advancement of the narrative. Not only is she vocally
active during this part of the film, but her vocal activity is with the most central characters in the film up to
that point, thus rapidly increasing her relative importance to the narrative. However, her importance is defined
temporally; she is important during those moments of the film, but she is not present at all in the final act of
the film, and thus her share of the narrative centrality declines again. Likewise, we can see from Figures 1 and
2 that Poe (alongside Finn) is very central to the narrative in the first act of the film, as the two escape the First
Order together, but that the narrative focuses more on Rey and Finn (and later Han) after this point. It would
be impossible to adequately characterise these temporally-situated narrative positions using measures based on
a static network representation.
These dynamic properties bring other advantages when relating this measure to more interpretive
understandings of characters’ positions within the narrative. Scholarship on the role of women in action cinema,
for example, has discussed the ways in which women are often ejected from narratives (for example, killed or
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
15
kidnapped) in order to motivate a male hero’s narrative actualisation (Purse 2011; Tasker 1993). In cases where
this occurs, the scores of those characters who have ceased to be active narrative contributors will no longer
increase, and thus their share of the character space will decline, reflecting this narrative ejection. Indeed,
when Rey is captured and imprisoned in The Force Awakens, we see her share of the scores decrease as her role
becomes temporarily more passive while others are motivated to act. Moreover, as the (non-normalised)
measure is monotonically non-decreasing, interactions towards the end of the narrative will tend to be given
more weight than interactions nearer the beginning. In substantive terms, this makes sense as we are more likely
to have a sense of which characters are important after we have spent more time watching the narrative unfold
(Bal 2017; Frermann et al. 2018). Thus, our sense of the importance of a character entering the narrative towards
the end is informed by our accumulated understanding of the relative importance of the characters they interact
with, though we have less information about this earlier in the film.
Finally, there are interesting differences between the speaking scores and spoken-to scores. Han Solo and
Kylo Ren hold a greater share of the narrative importance on the speaking measure than they do on the spoken-
to measure. This could imply simply that these characters speak more than they are spoken to in the film.
However, looking at the number of lines spoken by and spoken to the characters reveals that Han speaks 124
lines and is spoken to 134 times, while Kylo Ren speaks 53 lines and is spoken to 48 times. From this we can infer
that it is not only how much Han speaks that gives him higher speaking scores, but rather that his lines tend to
also be delivered to more important characters (as defined by the measure). The measure suggests that when
Han speaks, he tends to speak to other characters who speak a lot, and the characters he speaks to tend to be
more important than the characters that speak to him. On the other hand, Rey’s share of the narrative
importance is greater when the measure is based on being spoken to than when it is based on speaking. Again,
this suggests not only that Rey is spoken to a lot (Rey speaks 132 lines and is spoken to 160 times, both more
than Han), but that the characters speaking to her tend to be more important than the characters she speaks
to.
These differences between Han and Rey suggest two different kinds of importance Han’s tendency to speak
to more important characters implies a more authoritative and teacherly role, while Rey’s tendency to be spoken
to by more important characters implies a more apprentice-like role. On the face of it, this makes sense in the
context of the narrative, as Han is a much more senior and experienced character in the fictional universe while
Rey is much more “green”. However, a gendered reading of this discrepancy might problematise the relationship
here between gender, seniority and vocal activity. Hollywood is known to allow older males to continue to
feature prominently in narratives, while older women are underrepresented and negatively associated with
seniority (Bazzini et al. 1997; Lauzen and Dozier 2005). This is likely to produce scenarios such as that between
Han and Rey, where an older and more senior male figure is featured alongside a younger female figure.
However, The Force Awakens does also feature Leia Organa, a senior female figure who, like Han, returns to
the series from the original trilogy of films and holds an important role in the plot. Leia is the general of the
Resistance and the mother of the film’s antagonist Kylo Ren. One might assume that this would grant her
narrative centrality comparable to that of Han, who is Kylo’s father and not a senior Resistance figure. However,
Figures 1 and 2 show that, despite her role, Leia’s vocal activity pales in comparison with the likes of Han, Finn
and Rey. This chimes with scholarship on the way that postfeminist media narratives merely “take into account”
feminism (McRobbie 2004) as, unlike Han, Leia’s leadership comes primarily from what we know of her position
in the Resistance, rather than from us actually seeing her leading the protagonists. In this regard, we might
question how much has changed since the original instalment of the saga in 1977. The directed nature of the
measure thus allows for a deeper understanding of the ways in which the characters interact with each other
over the course of the narrative.
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
16
6. Conclusions
In this paper we have aimed to develop a more integrated conceptual-methodological approach to the node-
level analysis of character networks based on fictional texts. Thus, the paper contributes on two fronts: (1) a
conceptual refinement of the character networks approach to analysing fictional texts, and (2) the development
of a dynamic node-level measure for analysing the positions of characters within such texts. The conceptual
refinement is found in the departure from the existing approach of applying off-the-shelf network analytical
tools based on static network representations to narratives and instead pursuing a more bespoke approach to
analysing narrative texts as dynamic networks. By doing so, we can relate the concept of centrality in the
character network to a notion of narrative importance more in keeping with key ideas from the work on
narratology and narrative comprehension. The measure presented in this paper is intended as a simple example
of how such an approach might yield more useful, accurate and appropriate results for meaningful analysis of
how characters are positioned in narratives. Through an application to The Force Awakens, we have shown that
the dynamic node-level measure presented here can aid our understanding of the gendered dynamics in film
texts in ways that existing measures cannot. Moreover, it allows us to describe the characters’ evolving narrative
positions within the context of the temporal dynamics of the narrative; it gives us a relational way of tracing
character activity over the course of a narrative; and it can reveal relational differences in speaking and being
spoken to.
It should be noted that the insistence on a dynamic network representation in this paper does not commit
one to a labour-intensive manually coded network extraction method such as the one assumed here. All that is
required for the principles set forth in this paper to be satisfied is that the basic structure from which the
character network is constructed is a sequential list of relational events, and the question of how to represent
the data is separate from the question of whether this process should be manual or computational. The limitation
we identify with existing studies is that they have configured network extraction tools which move directly from
the text to a static representation, usually either by increasing the cell values in an adjacency matrix for each
interaction or by increasing the weight in an edgelist. Neither the adjacency matrix nor the weighted edgelist
contain information about the temporal sequence of the events. However, those same extraction tools could
just as easily store the interaction data in an event list format, with the same basic data structure as the one
used in this paper. This event list can then be used to generate static weighted edgelists and adjacency matrices
if desired, but if the event list step is skipped, it cannot be reverse-engineered later from the static
representations.
For example, a screenplay parsing tool could add a row to an event list for each identified speech act,
recording an event ID (incremented for each speech act, capturing the sequence), the scene ID (incremented
after each scene boundary), the speaker ID (based on named entity recognition), and the “recipients” (which
would not be direct recipients, but could be those characters who also spoke within the same scene). Then, for
example, the dynamic speaking centrality measure developed here could be applied directly to this data
structure without the need for modification. The spoken-to measure would no longer be appropriate, given that
the tool can only accurately identify the speaker but not the recipients. However, for the speaking measure, we
would still gain a sense of the vocal narrative activity of each character, with interactions weighted by the
characters involved in the narrative at the time of the interaction, illustrating how attention is distributed
among characters through the narrative. In this way, existing computational approaches can be brought much
more in line with the dynamic approach which we have argued for in this paper. Thus, we suggest that one of
the implications of this paper for character networks research is that the computational approach can be an
effective tool for analysing how stories are told, with some minor adjustments.
Moreover, the utility of the approach is not limited to films and can add to existing character network studies
of literary texts such as novels and plays. Whatever the media, the measure can help to enhance readings of
how characters move through narratives by way of their activity in the character system, telling us much more
than can be gleaned from measures such as degree or betweenness in a static or time-windowed network
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
17
representation. This is important as cultural texts engage in the reproduction of social meaning, and we need
approaches that can accurately measure the important identificatory power relations at play in these texts. The
stronger the conceptual links between narrative texts and the ways we represent them in data are developed,
the more useful the character network approach becomes for illuminating these power relations.
The measure developed here is offered as a starting point for further thinking about how to measure the
relationships between characters and narratives, and there are a number of ways in which the measure might
be further developed. For one, dialogue is a crucial dimension of how film narratives are played out, but it is
only one dimension. It would be useful to explore the extent to which the picture of the dynamic character
system that emerges from dialogue-based network data would acquiesce with that derived from more visually-
based data such as screen-sharing patterns. Moreover, the measure might be refined so that the content or type
of an interaction is taken into account. It may be reasonable to assume that certain kinds of interaction carry
more narrative weight. For example, talking about certain topics considered pivotal to the plot might be
considered a greater signifier of the narrative importance of the characters. Alternatively, the measure might
be refined so that a character can increase their score slightly if they are not directly involved in an interaction
but they are the subject of that interaction. As currently defined, the measure assumes that a character’s
relative share of the narrative attention decreases when they are not involved in an interaction, but if characters
A and B talk to each other about C, then some narrative attention is also being paid to C. Such modifications
would require deeper consideration, but the measure developed here is presented as a platform on which more
complex properties such as these might be developed.
A. Appendix
A.1. Scale invariance
The relative measure is invariant to multiplication of initial values , when  for all .
For spoken to, this is immediately obvious through observing that 
 implies 
, given
through direct calculation




 
 
and 
. Consequently 

, for all  and it does not matter what initial
value is used. The same relation holds trivially for speaking.
A.2. Some minimal and maximal scores
We consider first two dyadic schematic dynamic graphs. To simplify the exposition, in the remainder we
consider only the spoken to scores and dynamic graphs with both sender and receiver exclusivity. Consider two
actors  speaking to each other, in which case we can define the sequence of speech in terms of a binary
sequence defined such that  and , and assume .
Dyadic Asymmetric Speech (DAS): If for , we get the dynamic graph for 
of Figure A1(a). For , the scores at the th round are  and . Thus if actor 2 speaks
the first lines, then the score of actor 1 increases linearly.
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
18
Dyadic Turn-taking (DTT): If , the actors are taking turns speaking to each other. Assuming
2 speaks odd rounds, we get the dynamic graph in Figure A.1.(b)
Figure A.1. The dynamic graph of DAS (a) and DTT (b) for 4 acts of speech.
To derive the scores for DTT let , in which case

(4)
where 
 
 
for equal to 1 or 0 according to whether is odd or even, respectively. From (4) it also follows that



(5)
Due to the recursion (4), the elements of must be polynomials in .
Lemma 1: Defining  for and , the scores for DTT at the th line of dialogue, are given by


(6)
and



(7)
with coefficients  that satisfy the recursion
 
(8)
Proof: For =1,2, it is trivial to check that (6), (7), and (8) holds. Assume that (6) holds for , the
firstly





and by applying (5)


Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
19


 







Theorem 1: The coefficients that follow (8) are given by


(9)
where is the ceiling function.
Proof (David Schoch, personal communication, 2019): The equality (9) holds trivially for =0,1,2. By direct
calculation using Lemma 1,

.
The coefficients  form a Pascal-like triangle, a flipped form of which was derived by Gould (1965). The
triangle represented as a lower-triangular matrix, has as its first column 1s, second column repetitions of
counting numbers, and third column repetitions of triangular numbers.
Analysing , provides some insight into how controls the strength of feedback in accumulative
narrative importance. Define the row-sums

Corollary 1: For , 
where is the th Fibonacci number.
The proof follows easily from noting that , and adding row-sums  and  as in Gould
(1965).
Further insight is provided by comparing the highest score at for DAS and DTT. In the following we will only
consider the highest scores, i.e.  and , respectively. Firstly, the scores are equal for .
The score  for DAS is linear but the maximal score for DTT is a convex function of . Clearly, if ,
the scores for both remain 1. To investigate the conditions for , define
.
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
20
Remark: exists for
Proof: A very blunt lower bound is obtained from only using the first half of the coefficients


 

Consequently, for an that satisfies


where we can majorize the left-hand side using a known result






and thus satisfies

Of course, the first part of the inequality can be sharpened considerably, for example
 


For , , and Figure 1 provides as a function of .
Figure A.2. as a function of .
Single Speaker Speech (SSS): For , assume that only one actor speaks and other actors are only
spoke to once. Without loss of generality assume that the speaking actor is , and speaking acts are ,
, …, , as in Figure A.3.(a). This yields scores of  , for , and 
, for 
0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8
050 100 150 200 250
l
rc
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
21
Game of Telephone (GOT): Figure A.3.(b); This type of graph has minimal narrative focus in that dialogue
is simply passed on, not centring on any of the characters. Without loss of generality assume that the sequence
is , , …, , …, , in which case


for .
Game of Telephone with Coord (GTC): Figure A.3.(c). Assume that at time, speech is directed back to
, where . The score for is clearly 
 from GOT, and



Figure A.3.: Distributed speech dynamic graphs SSS (a), GOT (b), and GTC (c).
Comparing SSS, GOT, and GTC with the two dyadic dynamic graphs DAS and DTT it becomes obvious that a
dyadic graph will maximise the narrative measure. We offer the following without proof:
Theorem 2: Let actors be labelled so that , for , and let 
, , and 
, then

The interpretation is that dyadic interactions maximise the narrative measure and that interactions involving
3 or more actors dilute the concentration of the narrative focus. Consequently,  can be considered a
maximum if and 
 if .
A.3. Choice of
All parametric centrality measures (e.g. Bonacich and Lloyd 2001; Bonacich and Lu 2012; Katz 1953; Opsahl
et al. 2010; Taylor et al. 2017) offer the choice of how to set parameter values. The goal for the present measure
is simplicity, with the effect of the parameter on output values being both clear and sensible. Like other
parametric measures such as those cited above, different values emphasise different aspects of the
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
22
phenomenon, and there is no obvious or best value to choose. Thus, we provide in this section some more
discussion of , explaining what the use of the value of 0.01 in our illustrations in Section 5 implies.
Figure A.5. Speaking and spoken to measures against value of λ (on log scale).
0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5
0.00 0.05 0.10 0.15 0.20 0.25 0.30
l = 0.01 to l = 0.2
Ci ,688
out
Ci ,688
in
Poe Dameron
Kylo Ren
Finn
Rey
Han Solo
Leia Organa
Figure A.4. Changes in measures as λ is increased.
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
23
The parameter regulates the amount of feedback in the measure (as seen, for example, in comparison of
DTT and DAS). For The Force Awakens, increasing amplifies dialogue with highly central actors, especially in
later stages of the narrative. To illustrate this effect, Figure A.4 maps the trajectory of the measures when
increasing from  to .
The final scores for the characters in The Force Awakens are provided as a function of in Figure A.5. Figure
A.5 shows that the range of output values for the measure remains fairly stable for changes in values at the
lower end of the scale. The picture of relative character importance painted by the measure does not begin to
change drastically until begins to reach values of around 0.03-0.04 (the area between the rightmost two
reference lines in Figure A.5). At this point, increasing causes instability in the narrative, as illustrated by
crossing lines. Thus, for values beyond this point, the scores become dominated by those characters involved
in interactions at the conclusion of the film. This illustrates the “show-stealing” quality of higher values
discussed in Section 5, most clearly seen in Kylo Ren’s rapid rise in relative centrality at higher values, and
Han Solo’s corresponding decline. When identifying a useful value for the illustrations in Section 5, the range
of values was narrowed to the area where the output values first begin to spread out in Figure A.5 (the area
between 0.005 and 0.02) in order to preserve the importance of earlier narrative events, while still allowing for
events to gain importance as the narrative progresses.
Character trajectories were then plotted using a selection of values in this range, as shown in Figure A.6.
These plots were used to more closely consider the sensitivity of the measure in light of the goals of simplicity
Figure A.6. Examining the effect of values between 0.005 and 0.02 on character trajectories.
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
24
and clarity. As discussed in Section 5, the value of 0.01 was chosen as a middle-ground between the
insensitivity of 0.005 and the sensitivity of 0.02. Moreover, the trajectories mapped using the value of 0.01
appear to correspond with a sensible reading of how the narrative unfolds. This is not to say that 0.01 will be a
useful value in all cases; for example, narratives with vastly fewer or more events may require more fine-tuning
to ensure correspondence of output values to the narrative. However, for the present purposes, 0.01 was
selected as a useful illustrative value.
To validate the results for The Force Awakens against a more extensive corpus, we calculate the proposed
measures for each of the first 18 films of the Marvel Cinematic Universe (MCU) from Iron Man (2008) to Black
Panther (2018), each coded using exactly the same method as was used for The Force Awakens. This then gives
us 19 different sets of character scores for each measure. Figures A.7a and A.7b provide a comparison of the
final scores given by the proposed measures in these 19 films against two existing centrality indices for three
different values (0.01, 0.05, and 0.1). Figure A.7a compares the proposed measure with the simple indegree
and outdegree of each character. Figure A.7b compares the proposed measure with the total number of lines
spoken and received. For all measures, scores have been normalised by dividing a character’s score by the sum
of the scores for all characters in that film. We again see the effect of higher values on the rank order of
character importance: for values of 0.05 and 0.1, Kylo Ren is able to steal the show by overtaking Rey in
importance despite speaking drastically fewer lines (Figure A.7b).
For each individual film we also calculate the Spearman rank correlation of the final normalised scores with
the normalised outdegree/indegree and normalised number of lines spoken/received for a range of values.
These correlations are plotted in Figure A.8. Figure A.8 shows that the final rank order of character importance
given by the proposed measures is highly correlated with that given by the number of lines spoken/received,
but that the association is weaker for higher values. The same cannot be said for the rank correlation between
Figure A.7a. Speaking and spoken to measures against outdegree and indegree for

and 0.1. Black
points indicate characters from The Force Awakens, blue points indicate characters from 18 MCU films.
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
25
the proposed measures and indegree/outdegree, where the association is weaker across each value. This
illustrates that degree offers a picture of relative character importance that often corresponds only spuriously
Figure A.7b. Speaking and spoken to measures against number of lines spoken and received for 
and 0.1. Black points indicate characters from The Force Awakens, blue points indicate characters from 18 MCU
films.
Figure A.8. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients for proposed measures against lines spoken/received and
outdegree/indegree for five values of .
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
26
to the extent to which the narrative is told through those characters.
Finally, a summary measure across all is provided by area under the curve for the scores in Figure A.5.
which may be obtained through numerically integrating the scores 
. The total area under curve for
each actor is provided in Figure A.9.
References
Abell, Peter. 2004. “Narrative Explanation: An Alternative to Variable-Centred Explanation?” Annual Review of
Sociology 30: 287-310.
Abbott, H. Porter. 2008. The Cambridge Introduction to Narrative. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Agarwal, Apoorv, Augusto Corvalan, Jacob Jensen and Owen Rambow. 2012. “Social Network Analysis of Alice
in Wonderland.” Workshop on Computational Linguistics for Literature, 88-96. Montréal, Canada, June 8
2012.
Agarwal, Apoorv, Sriramkumar Balasubramanian, Jiehan Zheng and Sarthak Dash. 2014. “Parsing Screenplays
for Extracting Social Networks from Movies.” Proceedings of the 3rd Workshop on Computational Linguistics
for Literature (CLfL), 50-58. Gothenburg, Sweden, April 27 2014.
Amati, Viviana, Alessandro Lomi and Daniele Mascia. 2019. “Some days are better than others: Examining
time-specific variation in the structuring of interorganizational relations.” Social Networks 57: 18-33.
Bal, Mieke. 2017. Narratology: Introduction to the Theory of Narrative. 4th ed. Toronto: University of Toronto
Press.
0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5
0.00 0.05 0.10 0.15 0.20 0.25 0.30
ó
õ0
1C(l)d l
out
in
Poe Dameron
Kylo Ren
Finn
Rey
Han Solo
Leia Organa
Figure A.9. Areas under the curves for speaking and spoken-to
measures.
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
27
Bazzini, Doris G., William D. McIntosh, Stephen M. Smith, Sabrina Cook and Caleigh Harris. 1997. “The Aging
Woman in Popular Film: Underrepresented, Unattractive, Unfriendly, and Unintelligent.” Sex Roles 36 (7/8):
521-543.
Bearman, Peter S. and Katherine Stovel. 2000. “Becoming a Nazi: A model for narrative networks.Poetics 27:
69-90.
Bechdel, Alison. 1986. Dykes to Watch Out For. Ithaca: Firebrand Books.
Bonacich, Phillip and Paulette Lloyd. 2001. “Eigenvector-like measures of centrality for asymmetric relations.”
Social Networks 23: 191-201.
Bonacich, Phillip and Philip Lu. 2012. Introduction to Mathematical Sociology. Princeton: Princeton University
Press.
Bordwell, David. 1985. Narration in the Fiction Film. London: Routledge.
Brandes, Ulrich, Garry Robins, Ann McRanie and Stanley Wasserman. 2013. “Editorial: What is network
science?” Network Science 1 (1): 1-15.
Branigan, Edward. 1992. Narrative Comprehension and Film. London: Routledge.
Broccatelli, Chiara, Martin Everett and Johan Koskinen. 2016. “Temporal dynamics in covert networks.”
Methodological Innovations 9: 1-14.
Chatman, Seymour. 1980. “What Novels Can Do That Films Can’t (And Vice Versa).” Critical Inquiry 7 (1): 121-
140.
Cox, Susan. 2015. “Star Wars: The Force Awakens provides the kick-ass female lead we’ve been waiting for.”
Feminist Current, Dec, 27.
Elson, David K., Nicholas Dames and Kathleen R. McKeown. 2010. “Extracting Social Networks from Literary
Fiction”. ACL '10 Proceedings of the 48th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics:
138-147.
Everett, Martin G., Chiara Broccatelli, Stephen P. Borgatti and Johan Koskinen. 2018. “Measuring knowledge
and experience in two mode temporal networks.” Social Networks 55: 63-73.
Faludi, Susan. 1992. Backlash: The Undeclared War Against Women. London: Vintage.
Falzon, Lucia, Eric Quintane, John Dunn and Garry Robins. 2018. “Embedding time in positions: Temporal
measures of centrality for social network analysis.” Social Networks 54: 168-178.
Fischer, Frank, Mathias Göbel, Dario Kampkaspar, Christopher Kittel and Peer Trilcke. 2017. “Network
Dynamics, Plot Analysis: Approaching the Progressive Structuration of Literary Texts.” Digital Humanities 2017
(Montréal, 811 August 2017). Book of Abstracts. Montréal: McGill University.
Frermann, Lea, Shay B. Cohen and Mirella Lapata. 2018. “Whodunnit? Crime Drama as a Case for Natural
Language Understanding.” Transactions of the Association for Computational Linguistics 6: 1-15.
Garber, Megan. 2015. “Star Wars: The Feminism Awakens.” The Atlantic, Dec, 19.
Genette, Gérard. 1980. Narrative Discourse: An Essay in Method. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.
Gil, Sebastian, Laney Kuenzel, and Caroline Suen. 2011. “Extraction and analysis of character interaction
networks from plays and movies.” Technical report, Stanford University.
Gould, Henry W. 1965. “A Variant of Pascal's Triangle.” Fibonacci Quarterly 3 (4): 257-271.
Grayson, Siobhán, Karen Wade, Gerardine Meaney and Derek Greene. 2016. “The Sense and Sensibility of
Different Sliding Windows in Constructing Co-occurrence Networks from Literature.” In Computational History
and Data-Driven Humanities. Springer.
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
28
Haskell, Molly. 1987. From Reverence to Rape, 2nd edn. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Holme, Petter and Jari Saramäki. 2012. “Temporal networks.” Physics Reports 519: 97-125.
Inness, Sherrie A. 2004. Action Chicks: New Images of Tough Women in Popular Culture. New York: Palgrave
Macmillan.
Jayannavar, Prashant Arun, Melody Ju, Apoorv Agarwal and Owen Rambow. 2015. “Validating Literary Theories
Using Automatic Network Analysis.” Proceedings of NAACL-HLT Fourth Workshop on Computational Linguistics
for Literature: 3241. Denver, Colorado, June 4, 2015.
Jones, Pete. 2018. “Diana in the World of Men: a character network approach to analysing gendered vocal
representation in Wonder Woman.” Feminist Media Studies, DOI: 10.1080/14680777.2018.1510846.
Kagan, Dima, Thomas Chesney and Michael Fire. 2019. “Using Data Science to Understand the Film Industry’s
Gender Gap.” arXiv:1903.06469.
Kaplan, E. Ann. 1983. Women and Film: Both Sides of the Camera. New York and London: Methuen.
Katz, Leo. 1953. “A new status index derived from sociometric analysis.” Psychometrika 18 (1): 39-43.
Kermode, Mark. 2015. “Star Wars: The Force Awakens review consider me conquered.” The Guardian, Dec,
20.
Kontoleon, Nectarios, Lucia Falzon and Philippa Pattison. 2013. “Algebraic structures for dynamic networks.”
Journal of Mathematical Psychology 57 (6): 310-319.
Kydros, Dimitrios, Panagiotis Notopolous and Georgios Exarchos. 2015. “Homer’s Iliad – A Social Network
Analytic Approach.” International Journal of Humanities and Arts Computing 9 (1): 115-132.
Lauzen, Martha. 2018. “It’s a Man’s (Celluloid) World: Portrayals of Female Characters in the Top 100 Films of
2017.” Center for the Study of Women in Television & Film (San Diego State University). Accessed February
12, 2019. https://womenintvfilm.sdsu.edu/wp-
content/uploads/2018/03/2017_Its_a_Mans_Celluloid_World_Report_3.pdf.
Lauzen, Martha M. and David M. Dozier. 2005. “Maintaining the Double Standard: Portrayals of Age and Gender
in Popular Films.” Sex Roles 52 (7/8): 437-446.
Lv, Jinna, Bin Wu, Lili Zhou and Han Wang. 2018. “StoryRoleNet: Social Network Construction of Role
Relationship in Video.” IEEE Access, DOI: 10.1109/ACCESS.2018.2832087.
Masías, Víctor Hugo, Paula Baldwin, Sigifredo Laengle, Augusto Vargas and Fernando A. Crespo. 2017.
“Exploring the prominence of Romeo and Juliet’s characters using weighted centrality measures.” Digital
Scholarship in the Humanities 32 (4): 837-858.
McRobbie, Angela. 2004. “Post-feminism and popular culture.” Feminist Media Studies 4 (3): 255-264.
Min, Semi and Juyong Park. 2016. “Mapping Out Narrative Structures and Dynamics Using Networks and Textual
Information.” arXiv:1604.03029.
Moody, James. 2002. “The Importance of Relationship Timing for Diffusion.” Social Forces 81 (1): 25-56.
Moody, James, Daniel McFarland and Skye Bender-deMoll. 2005. “Dynamic Network Visualization.” American
Journal of Sociology 110 (4): 1206-1241.
Moretti, Franco. 2011. “Network Theory, Plot Analysis.” New Left Review 68: 80-102.
Mourchid, Youssef, Benjamin Renoust, Hocine Cherifi and Mohammed El Hassouni. 2018. “Multilevel Network
Model of Movie Script.” arXiv:1812.05718v1.
Mulvey, Laura. 1975. “Visual Pleasure and Narrative Cinema.” Screen 16 (3): 6-18.
Accepted author manuscript https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2020.03.003 Jones, Quinn and Koskinen
29
Niehaus, James and R. Michael Young. 2014. “Cognitive models of discourse comprehension for narrative
generation.” Literary and Linguistic Computing 29 (4): 561-582.
O’Day, Marc. 2004. “Beauty in Motion: Gender, Spectacle and Action Babe Cinema.” In Action and Adventure
Cinema, edited by Yvonne Tasker. Abingdon: Routledge.
O’Meara, Jennifer. 2016. “What “The Bechdel Test” doesn’t tell us: examining women’s verbal and vocal
(dis)empowerment in cinema.” Feminist Media Studies, DOI: 10.1080/14680777.2016.1234239.
Opsahl, Tore, Filip Agneessens, John Skvoretz. 2010. “Node centrality in weighted networks: Generalizing
degree and shortest paths.” Social Networks 32 (3): 245-251.
Padgett, John F. 2018. “Faulkner’s Assembly of Memories into History: Narrative Networks in Multiple Times.”
American Journal of Sociology 124 (2): 406-478.
Prado, Sandra D., Silvio R. Dahmen, Ana L. C. Bazzan, Pádraig Mac Carron and Ralph Kenna. 2016. “Temporal
Network Analysis of Literary Texts.” arXiv:1602.07275.
Purse, Lisa. 2011. Contemporary Action Cinema. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
Ricœur, Paul. 1980. “Narrative Time.” Critical Inquiry 7 (1): 169-190.
Roddy, Michael. 2015. “Review – New Star Wars: The Force Awakens lets women take the lead.” Reuters, Dec,
16.
Ruegg, Claire and James Jaehoon Lee. 2019. “Epic social networks and Eve’s centrality in Milton’s Paradise
Lost.” Digital Scholarship in the Humanities. DOI: 10.1093/llc/fqz001.
Sack, Graham Alexander. 2014. “Character Networks for Narrative Generation: Structural Balance Theory and
the Emergence of Proto-Narratives.” In Complexity and the Human Experience: Modeling Complexity in the
Humanities and Social Sciences, edited by Paul A. Youngman and Mirsad Hadzikadic. Boca Raton: CRC Press.
Schoch, David. 2019. Personal communication.
Schubart, Rikke. 2007. Super Bitches and Action Babes: The Female Hero in Popular Cinema, 1970-2006.
Jefferson, NC: McFarland & Co.
Smith, Stacy L., Marc Choueiti, Katherine Pieper, Ariana Case and Angel Choi. 2018. “Inequality in 1,100
Popular Films: Examining Portrayals of Gender, Race/ Ethnicity, LGBT, and Disability from 2007 to 2017.”
Annenberg Inclusion Initiative Initiative. Accessed February 12, 2019.
http://assets.uscannenberg.org/docs/inequality-in-1100-popular-films.pdf.
Smith, Tammy. 2007. “Narrative boundaries and the dynamics of ethnic conflict and conciliation.” Poetics 35:
22-46.
Sutherland, Jean-Anne and Kathryn M. Feltey. 2016. “Here’s looking at her: an intersectional analysis of
women, power and feminism in film.” Journal of Gender Studies, DOI: 10.1080/09589236.2016.1152956.
Tasker, Yvonne. 1993. Spectacular Bodies: Gender, genre and the action cinema. London: Routledge.
Taylor, Dane, Sean A. Myers, Aaron Clauset, Mason A. Porter and Peter J. Mucha. 2017. “Eigenvector-Based
Centrality Measures for Temporal Networks.” Multiscale Modeling & Simulation 15 (1): 537-574.
Waumans, Michaël C., Thibaut Nicodème, and Hugues Bersini. 2015. “Topology Analysis of Social Networks
Extracted from Literature.” PLoS ONE 10 (6), DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0126470.
Weng, Chung-Yi, Wei-Ta Chu, and Ja-Ling Wu. 2009. “RoleNet: Movie Analysis from the Perspective of Social
Networks.” IEEE Transactions on Multimedia 11 (2): 256271.
Woloch, Alex. 2003. The One vs. the Many: Minor Characters and the Space of the Protagonist in the Novel.
Princeton: Princeton University Press.
... Similarly, Bal (2017) suggests that the more a character does something (such as speaking), the more attention they receive. Verbal dialogue can be used to construct the network (Jones, 2020;Jones et al., 2020). Character network analysis has been used in both film (Jones, 2020;Jones et al., 2020;Park et al., 2012;Weng et al., 2009) and television series (Bost et al., 2018;Tan et al., 2017) and provides the methods to successfully investigate the use of language in television as recommended by Bednarek (2018). ...
... Verbal dialogue can be used to construct the network (Jones, 2020;Jones et al., 2020). Character network analysis has been used in both film (Jones, 2020;Jones et al., 2020;Park et al., 2012;Weng et al., 2009) and television series (Bost et al., 2018;Tan et al., 2017) and provides the methods to successfully investigate the use of language in television as recommended by Bednarek (2018). ...
... The weight refers to how many lines of dialogue were spoken by a nurse or doctor. Centrality measures were used to interpret the results (see Jones et al., 2020;Park et al., 2012). Degree centrality is the total number of links coming into and out of a node. ...
Article
Full-text available
Aim The aim of this study was to describe how nurses' dialogue compares with that of doctors in the Australian reality television program Emergency and to explain how this dialogue and the use of narration and direct‐to‐camera monologues contribute to the portrayal of nurses. The final aim was to outline how these findings can inform the actions of nurses, nursing organizations and writers and journalists. Design A descriptive study conducted in 2021 and 2022. Methods Character network analysis was used to describe the verbal interaction of nurses and doctors and identify major, minor and background characters. Narration and direct‐to‐camera monologues were analysed through frequency counts. Cultivation theory was used to interpret the results. Results Fifty‐four characters were identified, with 19 of these being nurses. Doctors spoke 87.9% of the dialogue. Most of the dialogue (43%) was monologue by doctors directly to the camera. All major characters were doctors, and only one nurse was a minor character. The mean number of mentions by the narrator of a nurse per episode was 4, and 30 for a doctor. Conclusion The portrayal of nurses in Emergency is inaccurate and the production methods are used to privilege the role of doctors. This finding comes after decades of research showing inaccurate images of nursing in television. Drawing on recent Australian inquiries and the challenges experienced by mental health and aged care nursing, several approaches for collaborative action to improve these images are suggested. Impact This is the first study of the portrayal of nursing in reality television. These results suggest that much effort is still required even in contemporary reality television to accurately reflect the work and contribution of nurses. It is the shared responsibility of individual nurses, nursing organizations and writers and journalists to accurately portray nurses in the media. No patient or public contribution This network analysis of a postproduction television program did not allow patient or public involvement in the design or analysis.
... Their insights underscored the pronounced roles of these characters in shaping film storylines. Similarly, both Kagan et al. (2020) and Jones et al. (2020) employed these analytical tools to interpret character centrality within film narratives, further exemplifying the versatility of network analysis tools across varying contexts. Also, Lee (2015) examined the structure and flow of knowledge within entrepreneurial networks in the creative domain. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study quantitatively investigates the collaborative framework and competitive landscape of Thailand’s evolving music industry, driven by technological progress and changing consumer preferences. By examining data obtained from Thailand’s Department of Intellectual Property, specifically 138,868 songs, it explores the complex network of relationships among music creators, artists, and various rights-holders, including those associated with recording, music, melodies, and lyrics. Utilizing social network analysis, this research uncovers a power law distribution in these networks, reflecting a scale-free market configuration. This characteristic is marked by a few dominant players exercising considerable market influence, contrasted with numerous less-interconnected participants. This investigation notes regular patterns of collaboration between artists and different rights-holders. Furthermore, the network of music creators displays small-world properties, with short collaborative distances fostering efficient information exchange and creative synergy. Crucially, this study identifies key influential players instrumental in directing the industry’s major trends, highlighting their role in market concentration. These significant findings will provide critical evidence for informing future policy development aimed at improving efficiency and equity in the digital content industries.
... Total influence is not dominated by apparent factors. This is unlike, for instance, in Jones et al. (36), where a time-dependent speaking measure is proposed to assess the positions of characters in networks of movie dialogs. Transferring that approach to the AFL, we would determine coaches' influence from the temporal network of coaches "speaking" to future coaches they are currently coaching. ...
Article
Full-text available
We study the lineage network of coaches in the Australian Football League (AFL) using a novel process of influence propagation through temporal social networks. Coaching and being coached are considered major opportunities for learning, and the vast majority of AFL coaches are former AFL players. We, therefore, establish influence via two antagonistic components: as players, future coaches are influenced by their coaches, and later liberate themselves from these influences while being coaches themselves. Influence thus propagates through time-dependent player–coach relationships, and we obtain a ranking of coaches by their aggregated influence on others. In addition to being based on an explicit process, we argue that the ranking has face validity, because it indeed favors highly reputed coaches, and is not determined by temporal or activity indicators such as the starting year of a coaching career, its length, or the number of future coaches coached.
... Determining the type of the oncological network under consideration boils down to the problem of differences between centralized and decentralized interorganizational networks [17][18][19][20][21][22][23][24][25]. For each voivodeship coordinator, there is a central unit coordinating the activities of a centralized subnet of entities. ...
Article
Full-text available
The global pandemic triggered by the SARS-CoV-2 virus has caused marked changes in the economic landscape in essentially every branch of the economy. The pandemic has disordered lives across all countries in the world and also affected the public sector in interesting ways. Our empirical research, of which selected elements are presented in this paper, was conducted under pandemic conditions. This paper aims to identify the relationships between selected distinguishing features of the oncological interorganizational network (exchange, engagement, reciprocity) and determine their effectiveness under the conditions of the economic crisis caused by the COVID-19 pandemic. A side thread, which concludes the article, is the introduction of the category “economic virus” into management terminology; i.e., a set of factors causing economic crises with a microbiological genesis. Of particular importance are considerations regarding the uncertainty of the length and depth of the health crisis-related economic effects in financial markets and corporate decision making.
... A recently introduced framework for the scalable examination of character representations adopts computational tools from network science. In this line of research, narratives are typically computationally parsed, abstracted, and parsimoniously encoded in a mathematical object called a graph (Bollobás, 2001), where the set of parts (network nodes) frequently represent a story's characters and connections (network edges) between characters denote some quality or strength of their (social) relation (e.g., Hopp et al., 2020;Jones et al., 2020;Kagan et al., 2020;Skowron et al., 2016). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
In the Hollywood film industry, racial minorities remain underrepresented. Characters from racially underrepresented groups receive less screen time, fewer central story positions, and frequently inherit plotlines, motivations, and actions that are primarily driven by White characters. Currently, there are no clearly defined, standardized, and scalable metrics for taking stock of racial minorities’ cinematographic representation. In this paper, we combine methodological tools from computer vision and network science to develop a content analytic framework for identifying visual and structural racial biases in film productions. We apply our approach on a set of 89 popular, full-length movies, demonstrating that this method provides a scalable examination of racial inclusion in film production and predicts movie performance. We integrate our method into larger theoretical discussions on audiences’ perception of racial minorities and illuminate future research trajectories towards the computational assessment of racial biases in audiovisual narratives.
Article
Full-text available
The relationship between on-screen and off-screen inequality in film industries and the relative impact of these on movie attendance is widely discussed but not necessarily empirically demonstrated. This article examines the binary gender composition of film project teams and the gendered representation of film characters as factors for cinema attendance. We collected a unique dataset (N=1285) of all films released during the pre-pandemic decade (2008-2019) in Russia – at that time the largest European cinema market. A marked-up subset of 243 films was used to calculate a novel version of the Bechdel-Wallace test that accounts for the proportion of all non-stereotypical dialogues in the film narration, as opposed to the classical binary test. Our test proves very informative, revealing a strikingly high proportion of dialogues with stereotypical portrayals of women even among the films that pass the Bechdel-Wallace binary threshold. We also undertook a social network analysis (SNA) of the characters’ communications. This analysis demonstrate that women predominantly occupy a peripheral position in film plots. Both stereotyping and marginalization of women are positively related to the proportion of men in the film crew, especially in the role of screenwriter. Simultaneously, having more men in key positions is also correlated with access to larger budgets and better distribution, thus effectively impeding films with stronger women characters from wider audiences. These audiences, however, show no prejudice towards films with such characters: after 2015, films featuring central women protagonists have the same level of attendance as movies without them. Although Russia exemplifies a large non-Western cinema market, the trends we identify, particularly the “gatekeeping” effect of male filmmakers, is notably in line with those observed in Western democracies.
Article
Full-text available
A character network is a graph extracted from a narrative in which vertices represent characters and edges correspond to interactions between them. A number of narrative-related problems can be addressed automatically through the analysis of character networks, such as summarization, classification, or role detection. Character networks are particularly relevant when considering works of fiction (e.g., novels, plays, movies, TV series), as their exploitation allows developing information retrieval and recommendation systems. However, works of fiction possess specific properties that make these tasks harder. This survey aims at presenting and organizing the scientific literature related to the extraction of character networks from works of fiction, as well as their analysis. We first describe the extraction process in a generic way and explain how its constituting steps are implemented in practice, depending on the medium of the narrative, the goal of the network analysis, and other factors. We then review the descriptive tools used to characterize character networks, with a focus on the way they are interpreted in this context. We illustrate the relevance of character networks by also providing a review of applications derived from their analysis. Finally, we identify the limitations of the existing approaches and the most promising perspectives.
Article
Full-text available
Data science can offer answers to a wide range of social science questions. Here we turn attention to the portrayal of women in movies, an industry that has a significant influence on society, impacting such aspects of life as self-esteem and career choice. To this end, we fused data from the online movie database IMDb with a dataset of movie dialogue subtitles to create the largest available corpus of movie social networks (15,540 networks). Analyzing this data, we investigated gender bias in on-screen female characters over the past century. We find a trend of improvement in all aspects of women‘s roles in movies, including a constant rise in the centrality of female characters. There has also been an increase in the number of movies that pass the well-known Bechdel test, a popular—albeit flawed—measure of women in fiction. Here we propose a new and better alternative to this test for evaluating female roles in movies. Our study introduces fresh data, an open-code framework, and novel techniques that present new opportunities in the research and analysis of movies.
Article
Full-text available
The automatic extraction of social relationship among individuals from massive quantities of video data is an important aspect of information extraction. However, most existing studies have focused on rough information, which result in inaccurate social network of role relationship. In this paper, the StoryRoleNet model is proposed for constructing an accurate and integral network representing the relationships among roles. First, to avoid the redundancy calculation of the relationships on the segmentation points of neighboring story units, we measure the weights of relationships by a weighted-Gaussian method in each story unit. More importantly, a new story segmentation method for long video is proposed by analyzing hierarchical features of the video. Then, we combine relationship networks constructed from the video and subtitle text. Some missed relationships can be complemented by this way. At last, the final network is analyzed to discover communities and important roles. Comprehensive evaluations were conducted using three movies and one television drama. The results demonstrate that the proposed method outperforms state-of-the-art methods in terms of the F1F_{1} accuracy measure and the normalized mutual information value.
Article
In this article, we analyze the social networks of Paradise Lost at multiple scales of analysis: from the whole poem, down to the level of the individual book and character, and also through quantitative analysis of social network analysis metrics, to understand from multiple angles how network structure can inflect the plot and the effects of relationships between characters. In the case of Eve, our multiscaled analysis reveals her complex role binding together her social world in ways not previously considered. Only through examining the network do we see that Milton has placed her at the very center of the human universe, providing the primary connection between the reader and the world of God. This example shows promise that our multiscaled network analysis can come to important conclusions about the role of gender in a text, and the method can be expanded outward to come to conclusions about other social identities that likewise remain largely overlooked in current implementations of digital humanities scholarship.
Article
In Absalom, Absalom! William Faulkner develops a processual model for how multivocal narrative history and time can emerge from conversation among heterogeneous subjective voices, both living and dead. Four social processes of memory assembly are involved: individual memories, changing perspectives, symmetry, and resonance. Iterated stories about characters in the past construct layered identities for narrators in the present by synchronizing them into multiple time registers: phenomenological time, episodic time, narrative time, epistemological time, projective time, and historical time. Different selves within the same person, Faulkner implies, emerge from these six ways of remembering, which brains and conversations throw up.
Article
The generative mechanisms controlling change in interorganizational relations are typically assumed to be time-independent, i.e., operate homogeneously and synchronously over time. In this paper we consider some of the implications of violating this assumption. We adopt and extend statistical models for relational events to reveal time-specific variations in mechanisms underlying interorganizational relations observed within a small community of health care organizations. We find that aggregate estimates of parameters associated with mechanisms of theoretical interest mask fine-grained temporal variation in relational events sequences. We discuss the implications of this result for studies of interorganizational relations-and social networks more generally.
Article
Two mode social network data consisting of actors attending events is a common type of social network data. For these kinds of data it is also common to have additional information about the timing or sequence of the events. We call data of this type two-mode temporal data. We explore the idea that actors attending events gain information from the event in two ways. Firstly the event itself may provide information or training; secondly, as co-attendees interact, they may pass on skills or information they have gleaned from other events. We propose a method of measuring these gains and demonstrate its usefulness using the classic Southern Women Data and a covert network dataset.
Article
As a big-budget action film both directed by and starring a woman, Wonder Woman is unusual given the well-documented under-representation of women in Hollywood both in front of and behind the camera. Strong claims have been made about Wonder Woman in the critical reception, with Diana, the film’s hero, being hailed as an empowered, independent leader. While there is a lot of literature on the visual aspects of gendered representation, there is a lack of research focusing on gender disparities in film dialogue despite a history of feminist theoretical work on the suppression of the female voice in Hollywood cinema. In this article, I present an approach which allows for the empirical analysis of gendered dialogue in film. I use the methods of social network analysis to develop a character networks approach to analysing vocal representation in Wonder Woman and compare the results with two other recent films: Thor (2011), and The Hunger Games (2012). I argue that the findings show the ways in which Diana’s narrative power is contained within her relationship with her male sidekick and call for a reconsideration of what it means for a film to be “female-led.”
Article
Digital data enable researchers to obtain fine-grained temporal information about social interactions. However, positional measures used in social network analysis (e.g., degree centrality, reachability, betweenness) are not well suited to these time-stamped interaction data because they ignore sequence and time of interactions. While new temporal measures have been developed, they consider time and sequence separately. Building on formal algebra, we propose three temporal equivalents to positional network measures that incorporate time and sequence. We demonstrate how these temporal equivalents can be applied to an empirical context and compare the results with their static counterparts. We show that, compared to their temporal counterparts, static measures applied to interaction networks obscure meaningful differences in the way in which individuals accumulate alters over time, conceal potential disconnections in the network by overestimating reachability, and bias the distribution of betweenness centrality, which can affect the identification of key individuals in the network. https://authors.elsevier.com/c/1WgCW_8dLKymGm
Book
Xena, Buffy, Lara Croft. WWF, The Sopranos, Witchblade, La Femme Nikita. The women of pop culture are center stage and as tough as ever. Action Chicks is a groundbreaking collection highlighting the heroines we’ve grown to worship. What can they tell us about women in the Twent-first-century? What can they tell us about how popular culture depicts women? Do the characters escape traditional gender role expectations? Or do they adhere to sexual, racial, ethnic, and class stereotypes? The essays in Action Chicks provide fans with a new look at their favourite icons and their relationship to the popular media machine. A fascinating collection that’s bound to stir up some excitement.