ArticlePDF Available

Body Size and Bite Force of Stray and Feral Cats—Are Bigger or Older Cats Taking the Largest or More Difficult-to-Handle Prey?

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

As carnivorans rely heavily on their head and jaws for prey capture and handling, skull morphology and bite force can therefore reflect their ability to take larger or more difficult-to-handle prey. For 568 feral and stray cats (Felis catus), we recorded their demographics (sex and age), source location (feral or stray) and morphological measures (body mass, body condition); we estimated potential bite force from skull measurements for n = 268 of these cats, and quantified diet composition from stomach contents for n = 358. We compared skull measurements to estimate their bite force and determine how it varied with sex, age, body mass, body condition. Body mass had the strongest influence of bite force. In our sample, males were 36.2% heavier and had 20.0% greater estimated bite force (206.2 ± 44.7 Newtons, n = 168) than females (171.9 ± 29.3 Newtons, n = 120). However, cat age was the strongest predictor of the size of prey that they had taken, with older cats taking larger prey. The predictive power of this relationship was poor though (r2 < 0.038, p < 0.003), because even small cats ate large prey and some of the largest cats ate small prey, such as invertebrates. Cats are opportunistic, generalist carnivores taking a broad range of prey. Their ability to handle larger prey increases as the cats grow, increasing their jaw strength, and improving their hunting skills, but even the smallest cats in our sample had tackled and consumed large and potentially ‘dangerous’ prey that would likely have put up a defence.
Content may be subject to copyright.
animals
Article
Body Size and Bite Force of Stray and Feral
Cats—Are Bigger or Older Cats Taking the Largest
or More Dicult-to-Handle Prey?
Patricia A. Fleming * , Heather M. Crawford, Clare H. Auckland and Michael C. Calver
Environmental and Conservation Sciences, Murdoch University, Perth, WA 6150, Australia;
crawfh01@gmail.com (H.M.C.); c.auckland@murdoch.edu.au (C.H.A.); M.Calver@murdoch.edu.au (M.C.C.)
*Correspondence: t.fleming@murdoch.edu.au; Tel.: +61-8-9360-6577
Received: 3 February 2020; Accepted: 30 March 2020; Published: 17 April 2020


Simple Summary:
Predation by cats (Felis catus) threatens Australian wildlife. As they rely on their
jaws to hold and subdue prey, their body size, skull shape and bite force can reflect an individual’s
prey handling ability. Prey less than 100 g are the usual prey of F. catus but they have frequently
been recorded to take larger prey, and previous studies have suggested that large male cats represent
a disproportionate risk to threatened and translocated native wildlife populations. We tested whether
a cat’s sex, age, body mass, body condition, and bite force determined the size of the prey they
took (prey body mass) especially for those prey that might be ‘dangerous’ or dicult to handle
(our subjective assessment of whether animals would be capable of fighting back and would therefore
require skill to subdue). Large male cats do indeed represent the greatest risk in that they have greater
body mass and bite force that would allow them to handle a greater range of prey. However even
small cats were active hunters, and many had taken large or dangerous prey species. The strongest
predictor of prey size was the age of the cat, with older cats taking the largest prey.
Abstract:
As carnivorans rely heavily on their head and jaws for prey capture and handling,
skull morphology and bite force can therefore reflect their ability to take larger or more
dicult-to-handle prey. For 568 feral and stray cats (Felis catus), we recorded their demographics
(sex and age), source location (feral or stray) and morphological measures (body mass, body condition);
we estimated potential bite force from skull measurements for n=268 of these cats, and quantified
diet composition from stomach contents for n=358. We compared skull measurements to estimate
their bite force and determine how it varied with sex, age, body mass, body condition. Body mass had
the strongest influence of bite force. In our sample, males were 36.2% heavier and had 20.0% greater
estimated bite force (206.2
±
44.7 Newtons, n=168) than females (171.9
±
29.3 Newtons, n=120).
However, cat age was the strongest predictor of the size of prey that they had taken, with older cats
taking larger prey. The predictive power of this relationship was poor though (r
2
<0.038, p<0.003),
because even small cats ate large prey and some of the largest cats ate small prey, such as invertebrates.
Cats are opportunistic, generalist carnivores taking a broad range of prey. Their ability to handle
larger prey increases as the cats grow, increasing their jaw strength, and improving their hunting
skills, but even the smallest cats in our sample had tackled and consumed large and potentially
‘dangerous’ prey that would likely have put up a defence.
Keywords: Australia; body condition; diet; Felis catus; feral; predation; prey; stray; wildlife; urban
1. Introduction
In Australia, predation of native fauna by invasive carnivores, including the feral domestic
cat (Felis catus), is recognised as a ‘Key Threatening Process’ under the Australian Commonwealth
Animals 2020,10, 707; doi:10.3390/ani10040707 www.mdpi.com/journal/animals
Animals 2020,10, 707 2 of 23
Environment Protection and Biodiversity Act 1999 (EPBC Act). Since its introduction to Australia
200 years ago, the domestic cat has become established across the entire continent [
1
]. Recent estimates
indicate that there are 1.4–5.6 million feral cats in natural environments, and another 0.7 million stray
(‘semi-feral’) cats in highly modified environments such as urban areas, refuse dumps and intensive
farms [
1
]. It has been argued that feral cats are the single largest threat to biodiversity regardless of
ecological context, with 142 native species and subspecies (40 mammals, 40 birds and 21 reptiles)
listed as threatened by feral cats [
2
]. For example, feral cat predation has been implicated in the
extinction of at least 22 mammal species in Australia [
3
]. Marsupials weighing 35 g–5.5 kg (termed the
‘critical weight range’, CWR) from low rainfall areas have been particularly vulnerable to population
decline and extinction [
4
6
]; animals within the CWR are ideally-sized prey for introduced eutherian
predators, including the feral cat. Predation by feral cats has also been implicated in the failure of
several reintroduction programs on mammals, especially those involving species of macropods less
than 2.0 kg, dasyurids and peramelids (e.g., Dickman [
7
]), and feral cat abundance is the best predictor
of decline for native conilurine rodents [
8
]. While there is no doubt that other factors such as habitat
clearing, increasing salinity/aridity, altered fire regimes and other introduced pest and predator species
all threaten Australia’s native fauna [
9
], feral cat predation has had the most rapid, dramatic and
demonstrable impact on species’ survival [
10
]. Success in reducing the harmful impact of feral cats
requires detailed understanding of their ecology across a broad spectrum of climatic and environmental
conditions [
11
]. An important aspect that needs greater understanding is what factors determine the
diet of feral cats?
There is substantial sexual dimorphism in cats [
12
]. In Australia, male feral cats generally weigh
3.4–7.3 kg and females 2.5–5.0 kg [
13
]. It is possible that such dierences in body size may influence
prey hunted by dierent sexes, as larger prey are likely to be more ‘challenging’ to hunt or subdue
and may require more musculature and skeletal strength for prey restraint. For example, bobcats
(Felis rufus) show similar body size dimorphism ratio (males average 8.4 kg and females 6.2 kg [
14
]),
and Litvaitis, Clark and Hunt [
15
] found that male bobcats generally consumed more white-tailed deer
(Odocoileus virginianus) than female bobcats because males were able to hunt larger prey and defend
carcasses from other predators, whereas females preferred smaller snowshoe hares (Lepus americanus).
While F. catus are generally described as having an opportunistic diet [
16
], there are indications of sex
dierences in predation behaviour: a review of the profiles of known individual cats responsible for
significant impacts on Australian wildlife protection or translocation programs across 20 studies [
17
]
indicated that large male cats
3.5 kg were disproportionally responsible for predation events on a
range of birds and mammals. Understanding how dierences in body size relate to diet would therefore
help in understanding influences on diet of feral cats.
Prey handling ability could also aect the range of prey taken by feral cats. Carnivorans rely
on their head and jaws for prey acquisition and ingestion of food, and their skull morphology and
bite force can therefore reflect their feeding ecology [
18
,
19
]. The hunting behaviour of domestic cats
has been thoroughly studied and sequences for specific prey types compared (e.g., rat vs. mouse vs.
bird [
20
]). Typically, the domestic cat ambushes or stalks and pounces on prey, using its forelimbs and
claws to pin it to the ground. The spinal cord of prey is then severed when cats place their mouth
over the neck and crush their canine teeth between the cervical vertebrae. To deliver the fatal neck bite,
cats must orientate their short-muzzled face and eyes close to the prey’s head, risking injury from the
prey’s defensive biting and kicking. In response to this defense, cats have developed a ‘play’ hunting
sequence in which the cat repeatedly paws and swats the prey [
21
], regularly picking it up in its jaws
and dropping it to the ground again until it tires [
22
]. Bite force can therefore reflect an important aspect
of prey handling ability in cats. Bite force can be estimated from dry skulls using lever models [
23
,
24
]
allowing analysis of ontogenetic changes in prey handling or direct comparison with diet for the same
individuals [
25
]. Understanding sex dierences in bite force, and how bite force develops as cats age
and grow, could therefore reflect ontogenetic and sex dierences in diet separation.
Animals 2020,10, 707 3 of 23
In this study, we examined estimated bite forces (as a proxy for prey handling capacity) and
the stomach contents for domestic cats (Felis catus). To include a broad a range of cat and potential
prey our analyses, we included cats collected from rural environments where they were independent
of food subsidies from people (which we define as ‘feral’ cats) as well as a smaller sample of those
trapped in and around urban areas (which we define as ‘stray’ cats). We examined the relationship
between estimated bite force and demographics for individual cats to test the predictions that bite force
is greater for males, as well as for larger or older cats. We included sampling location in our analyses,
which allowed us to test whether there were dierences in estimated bite force for rural feral cats vs.
urban stray cats. We then tested whether diet composition or prey size (body mass) was influenced by
the cat’s bite force, sex, body mass, age or sampling location.
2. Methods
This project was carried out with permission of the Murdoch University Animal Ethics Committee
(W2266/09) and adhered to the Animal Welfare Act 2002.
2.1. Specimens
A total of 567 culled animals were collected over a period of eight years across almost 100 rural
and urban locations across southwest Western Australia (Shires or Suburbs listed in Table S1). Animals
were sourced from licenced animal controllers contracted to various Cities/Shires or natural resource
management groups who trapped and euthanised (pentobarbitone overdose or shot) animals that
were identified as a threat to endangered native species or that had been reported by the public
or local government authority due to nuisance behaviour, welfare concerns, or abandonment [
26
].
We categorised animals by source location according to the type of anthropogenic development at their
immediate point of capture:
a.
Rural: feral cats were trapped and then shot as part of conservation management activities
focused around habitats of threatened species (i.e., bushland reserves) or shot while free-roaming
on private pastoral properties. These cats live and bre beyond the periphery of dense human
settlements and survive independently (i.e., were not fed by people). We did not include animals
that had been trapped from around rural refuse sites, as these cats were subsidised to a large
extent by human refuse [26].
b.
Urban: stray cats were identified as unowned (‘semi-feral’) cats trapped in urban and peri-urban
residential areas, the premises of private and commercial businesses, private small-holdings
and fragmented bush reserves within and on the immediate periphery of the metropolitan area
(Perth, Western Australia). Enforced from 2013, the Western Australia Cat Act 2011 requires that,
by the age of 6 months, all pet cats must be: (1) desexed, (2) microchipped with sub-dermal ID tag,
(3) registered with a local municipal council and (4) wearing a collar with ID and registration
tags. Any cat not identifiable as an owned pet trapped within an urban/peri-urban area because
of nuisance or welfare concerns, and/or surrendered to a shelter, is therefore considered a stray
cat. These stray cats are generally rehomed but may be euthanised if their temperament is not
suitable for rehoming. Our stray cats were neither pets nor suitable for rehoming.
Animal carcasses were stored frozen until they could be analysed in the laboratory. Once thawed,
we recorded the sex and body mass (m
b
;
±
0.01 kg) of individuals. The head length, head-body length
and pes length (metacarpal and forepaw of left hindlimb) were measured with a dressmakers’ tape
(
±
0.5 cm). A body condition index (BCI)—reflecting how much heavier-than-average each cat was,
accounting for its sex and body size—was calculated for each individual animal as the residual of
Log-m
b
against body size indicators (head length and head-body length), with sex included as a factor
(residual analysis in Statistica 8.0; Statsoft Inc., Tulsa, OK, USA); variability in pes length proved to be
too unreliable for inclusion in the BCI calculation.
Animals 2020,10, 707 4 of 23
2.2. Diet Analysis
Gastrointestinal tracts were dissected out of carcasses for diet analysis. We analysed stomach
contents of n=358 individual cats (excluding animals with empty stomachs). Description of the
urban stray cat diet has been published separately [
26
] but prey composition was reanalysed for
comparison to bite force in the present study. Consumed animals were classified as fresh or carrion
using tissue friability, smell and/or the presence/absence of maggots; data for animal carrion were not
included in the diet composition analysis or analyses of determinants of prey mass because carrion
would not pose the same mechanical challenges as live prey capture and handling. Refuse items were
classified as sources of food that would have been taken from anthropogenic sources and included food
scraps, (e.g., sandwich meats and vegetables, pieces of fruit), paper, plastic, aluminium foil, packing
Styrofoam, glass shards, synthetic fibres etc. Trap bait and plant material that was likely to be either
deliberately ingested (i.e., green grass) or incidentally ingested during consumption of food othe
ground (i.e., small volumes of twigs, leaves, bark) were also identified and excluded from diet analyses
for the purpose of this study. For reptiles, we identified prey items down to species where possible,
using field guides and expert opinion (see Acknowledgements). We identified mammals and birds to
species where possible using either fur [27] or feathers, identifiable body parts and expert opinion.
We used unpublished trapping data or field guides and other literature (e.g., [
28
,
29
]) to determine
average body mass of each identified prey species (‘prey size’). Where a cat had consumed a range of
species (e.g., one individual had six reptile species present), these data were entered as separate data
points, but where a single prey category was evident (e.g., the stomach of one cat contained 30 house
mice Mus musculus), this was only counted as a single prey size data point. All invertebrates represented
one data point (i.e., one prey category) regardless of taxonomic order and are therefore a conservative
underestimate of the diversity of invertebrate prey. As well as prey size, we also distinguished
potentially ‘dangerous’ prey species—a subjective category based on the authors’ experience of species
that could potentially deliver a damaging bite or kick while handled.
2.3. Bite Force
Heads were removed from carcasses and soft tissue was removed from skulls by maceration
at 30–40
C. Cats were then aged using tooth eruption patterns, incremental lines of their canine
cementum [
30
], and closure of cranial sutures [Fleming, Crawford, Auckland, Calver, unpublished
data]. As shooting was a main form of euthanasia and because the skulls of juvenile cats are less
robust than adult skulls (where cranial sutures have fused), a smaller number of skulls (n=268) were
available for analysis of bite force compared with diet and demographics. While we made every eort
possible to obtain useable data from juvenile cats, they are nevertheless under-represented in our bite
force dataset.
For skull analysis, digital photographs of each skull were taken in lateral, ventral and dorsal
views (Figure 1), and measurements (mm) were made using ImageJ software following Damasceno,
Hingst-Zaher and Astua [
31
]. Bite force (in Newtons; N) was calculated using the lever model method
according to published formulae [2325,32].
Estimated bite force was calculated as the estimated force of the temporalis and
masseter/pterygoideus muscle groups (derived from their cross-sectional areas) relative to their
centroid distances from the temporomandibular joint (TMJ) and the distance from the TMJ to the
canines (the estimated bite force output), which corresponds to the moment or out-lever arm (c).
These values were multiplied by two to estimate the estimated bite force (F; N) generated by muscle
groups on both sides of the skull:
Estimated bite force F(N)=2×(dm ×m×σ)+(dt ×t×σ)
c(1)
where σis the isometric muscle stress value of 0.3 N/mm2[32].
Animals 2020,10, 707 5 of 23
As the dry skull method has been shown to underestimate bite force, this absolute value was
corrected (Fcorr) [32] to give the corrected estimated bite force:
Fcorr =10(0.859×logF×0.559)(2)
Animals 2020, 10, x 5 of 24
As the dry skull method has been shown to underestimate bite force, this absolute value was
corrected (Fcorr) [32] to give the corrected estimated bite force:
𝐹
 = 10 (.    .) (2)
Figure 1. (a) Right lateral, (b) ventral and (c) dorsal view of a cat (Felis catus) skull. Distances recorded
for estimation of bite force: (a) SH skull height, dt the distance between the centroid of the temporalis
and Temporo-Mandibular Joint (TMJ) and c distance between the canine and TMJ; (b) ZAW
zygomatic arch width, SL ventral skull length, dm distance between the centroid of the masseter
muscle and the TMJ, and the cross-sectional areas of the masseter muscle/pterygoideus muscle (m);
and, (c) the cross-sectional area of the temporalis muscle (t).
2.4. Statistical Analyses
We compared male and female cat body mass, head-body length and age using Mann–Whitney
U Test [33] as the data did not conform to a Gaussian distribution and were not homoscedastic
(Levene’s Test). We employed generalised linear modelling (GLM) using the package lme4 in R [34]
to compare estimated bite force (Log-transformed N) for each cat (dependent variable with a
Gaussian link function) against (1) source location and (2) sex as dummy variables (coded 0 or 1),
with the continuous predictor variables (3) body mass (Log mb; kg), (4) body condition (BCI values)
and (5) cat age (Log months). The continuous independent factors were mean-standardised and then
we included all combinations of the five independent factors and their first order interaction terms.
Figure 1.
(
a
) Right lateral, (
b
) ventral and (
c
) dorsal view of a cat (Felis catus) skull. Distances recorded
for estimation of bite force: (
a
)
SH
skull height,
dt
the distance between the centroid of the temporalis
and Temporo-Mandibular Joint (TMJ) and
c
distance between the canine and TMJ; (
b
)
ZAW
zygomatic
arch width,
SL
ventral skull length,
dm
distance between the centroid of the masseter muscle and
the TMJ, and the cross-sectional areas of the masseter muscle/pterygoideus muscle (
m
); and, (
c
) the
cross-sectional area of the temporalis muscle (t).
2.4. Statistical Analyses
We compared male and female cat body mass, head-body length and age using Mann–Whitney
U Test [
33
] as the data did not conform to a Gaussian distribution and were not homoscedastic
(Levene’s Test). We employed generalised linear modelling (GLM) using the package lme4 in R [
34
] to
compare estimated bite force (Log-transformed N) for each cat (dependent variable with a Gaussian
link function) against (1) source location and (2) sex as dummy variables (coded 0 or 1), with the
Animals 2020,10, 707 6 of 23
continuous predictor variables (3) body mass (Log m
b
; kg), (4) body condition (BCI values) and (5) cat
age (Log months). The continuous independent factors were mean-standardised and then we included
all combinations of the five independent factors and their first order interaction terms. An Information
Theoretic approach using Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC) was used to identify correlations
between bite force and combinations of the five independent variables. We used the drop1 function in
R to compare the global model against variations of the model, excluding each parameter, to support
whether each factor was a substantial contribution to model-fit. Akaike model weights (w
i
) were
calculated for each model in the model-set [
35
], which were used to weight the standardised beta
values (calculated assuming a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1 for each variable) for each variable
across the top models (Σβ·wi) [36].
We used the same modelling approach to compare prey mass (Log m
b
; g) against: (1) source
location and (2) sex as dummy variables (coded 0 or 1), with (3) estimated bite force (Log N), (4) cat
body mass (Log m
b
; kg), (5) body condition (BCI values) and (6) cat age (Log months) as continuous
predictor variables.
Dierences in diet composition were analysed by Permutational Multivariate Analysis of Variance
(PERMANOVA) (adonis package) as part of non-metric Multidimensional Scaling (MDS) using the
package vegan in R [
37
]. The PERMANOVA [
38
] included sex and location as the categorical predictor
variables, and cat age (months), body mass (kg) and estimated bite force (N) as covariates. This was
followed by similarity percentage (SIMPER) analyses. Diet items were grouped as all medium native
species (
500 g), or small native species (<500 g), house mice, black rats Rattus rattus, European rabbits
Oryctolagus cuniculus, birds, reptiles, frogs/fish, invertebrates, grains/animal feed and human refuse.
Trap bait, animal carrion and incidental food items (e.g., plant material) were not included in this
statistical analysis.
Values are presented as means ±1 SD unless stated otherwise.
3. Results
3.1. Demographics
Male cats were 36.2% heavier and 9.2% longer (head-body length) with heads that were 8.9% longer
than females, but females in our sample were marginally older than males (Table 1a). Even pregnant
females were marginally lighter than the average male body mass. Although rural cats sampled were
marginally lighter and shorter than urban cats, only the head lengths were significantly dierent; there
was no significant eect of sample location on the body mass or head-body length between source
locations (Table 1b). There was also no significant dierence in the ages of cats from these two source
locations (Table 1b).
Table 1.
Demographics and significance testing for stray and feral cats (Felis catus) presented by (a) sex
and (b) source location in southwest Western Australia.
(a) Sex Males Females Mann–Whitney U test
Body mass 3.50 ±1.34 kg, n=308, max 6.7 kg 2.57 ±0.89 kg, n=260, max 4.9 kg Zadj =8.72, p<0.001
Pregnant 3.36 ±0.51 kg, n=24
Non-pregnant 2.49 ±0.88 kg, n=236
Head-body length 551 ±84 mm, n=299 504 ±74 mm, n=247 Zadj =7.36, p<0.001
Head length 112 ±14 mm, n=288 103 ±13 mm, n=235 Zadj =7.01, p<0.001
Age 2.42 ±2.05 years, n=154 3.21 ±2.88 years, n=105 Zadj =1.98, p=0.047
(b) Location Rural (feral cats) Urban (stray cats) Mann–Whitney U test
Body mass 3.05 ±1.26 kg, n=419, max 6.7 kg 3.14 ±1.20 kg, n=148, max 6.0 kg Zadj =0.56, p=0.575
Head-body length 527 ±83 mm, n=398 538 ±82 mm, n=148 Zadj =1.13, p=0.257
Head length 106 ±15 mm, n=375 110 ±12 mm, n=148 Zadj =2.60, p=0.009
Age 2.87 ±2.56 years, n=143 2.58 ±2.30 years, n=116 Zadj =1.11, p=0.266
Animals 2020,10, 707 7 of 23
3.2. Bite Force
We obtained a complete dataset of bite force as well as age and body measurements for 108 female
cats (49.1% feral cats collected from rural locations and 50.9% stray cats from urban locations) and
160 male cats (61.9% feral cats from rural locations and 38.1% stray cats from urban locations).
Four models adequately described the variation in bite force in this dataset (likelihood of being the
best model of those in our model-set: M1 w
i
=36.4%, M2 w
i
=32.4%, M3 w
i
=16.9%, M4 w
i
=14.3%;
Table 2). These models included all predictor variables except for the BCI factor; cat body condition
therefore did not contribute to describing the dierences in bite force.
Table 2.
Summary of generalised linear modelling testing for factors influencing bite force in stray and
feral cats (Felis catus) in southwest Western Australia.
Model df AIC Delta AIC wi
1
BF ~ Cat mb* Sex +Location * Age
8
828.69 0.00 0.364
2
BF ~ Cat mb* Sex +Location * Age +Location * Sex
9
828.45 0.23 0.324
3 BF ~ Cat m
b
* Sex +Mass * Location +Location * Age 9
827.15 1.54 0.169
4
BF ~ Cat mb* Sex +Location * Age +Sex * Age
9
826.81 1.88 0.143
BF bite force was log-transformed (N); Cat body mass (m
b
) was log-transformed (kg); Age was
log-transformed (months).
The strongest single factor influencing estimated bite force was body mass (model-weighted
standardised beta
Σβ·
w
i
=0.379); heavier cats were capable of generating greater mechanical force
than lighter cats (Figure 2a,d). Males had 20.0% greater estimated bite force (206.2
±
44.7 N, n=168)
than females (171.9
±
29.3 N, n=120) (Figure 2c;
Σβ·
w
i
=0.202). The interaction between sex and body
mass was also included in all the top models: males showed steeper increase in estimated bite force
with body mass compared with females (Figure 2b;
Σβ·
w
i
=0.374). Small males had estimated bite
forces less than that of females of equivalent mass, while large males had greater estimated bite forces
than females of equivalent mass (Figure 2a).
Age also influenced estimated bite force (Figure 2b,e;
Σβ·
w
i
=0.094), with older cats having
greater estimated bite force than younger animals. Although our sample of stray cats had marginally
larger heads, stray cats had smaller estimated bite forces than feral cats. There was also an interaction
between source location and animal age (Figure 2e,f); while adults showed little age and bite force
dierence between rural and urban source locations, young feral cats had greater estimated bite force
than young stray cats. A similar relationship was present between location and body mass (Figure 2d),
with lighter feral cats having greater estimated bite force than lighter stray cats.
3.3. Diet Composition
Only 27 of the 352 cats analysed for diet (7.7%) had consumed carrion (20 sheep Ovis aries,
three pig Sus scrofa, one European rabbit, and three unknown species samples with maggots), all other
fauna was freshly consumed or classified as refuse if identified as processed for human consumption
(e.g., processed ham).
There was no significant eect of sex (PERMANOVA pseudo-F
1,128
=0.34, p=0.961), estimated
bite force (pseudo-F
1,128
=1.19, p=0.290), body mass (pseudo-F
1,128
=0.96, p=0.429) or age
(pseudo-F
1,128
=1.07, p=0.360) on diet composition using 11 broad categories (Table 3). However,
diet was significantly dierent between locations (PERMANOVA pseudo-F
1,128
=9.23, p<0.001).
Similarity percentage (SIMPER) analysis indicated that the main contributors to the dierences
between rural (feral) and urban (stray) cat diets were the greater amount of refuse present in urban
cat diets (SIMPER 35.6% of the dierence), and the greater proportion of mice (SIMPER 15.9%),
birds (SIMPER 14.7%) and invertebrates (SIMPER 12.0%) in rural cat diets (Table 3). The remaining
diet items contributed <10% to the SIMPER analysis (Table 3).
Animals 2020,10, 707 8 of 23
Figure 2.
Estimated bite force (note Log-transformed axis) of cats (Felis catus) from southwest Western
Australia shown by (
a
) sex (left-hand panel; males =red triangles n=160, females =grey circles n=108)
or (
b
) source location (right-hand panel; rural =green circles and urban =blue triangles). Estimated
bite force shown against age (c,d) and cat body mass (e,f).
Table 3.
Similarity percentage (SIMPER) analysis comparing the diets of n=256 rural (feral) and
n=102 urban (stray) cats (Felis catus) collected across southwest Western Australia.
Diet Category
Contribution to
Dierence in Diet
(SIMPER %)
Rural =Feral Cat
Mean ±1 SD
Percentage Volume
Urban =Stray Cat
Mean ±1 SD
Percentage Volume
Refuse 35.6 9.25 ±25.72 18.83 ±30.02
House mouse Mus musculus 15.9 28.56 ±39.57 1.49 ±8.10
Birds 14.7 10.03 ±25.15 5.32 ±19.38
Invertebrates 12.0 4.32 ±14.25 1.24 ±4.54
European rabbit Oryctolagus cuniculus
5.7 4.91 ±18.77 0.49 ±4.95
Reptiles 5.2 2.85 ±11.28 0.01 ±0.10
Black rat Rattus rattus 4.7 2.36 ±13.82 3.97 ±17.20
Frogs/Fish 2.4 1.03 ±9.53 0.15 ±1.49
Total medium native mammal 500 g 2.1 0.97 ±9.08 0.90 ±8.91
Grains/Animal feed 1.8 2.76 ±13.38 0.00 ±0.00
Total small native mammal <500 g <0.01 0.07 ±1.09 0.00 ±0.00
Medium-sized native mammals included black-footed rock wallaby Petrogale lateralis, common brushtail possum
Trichosurus vulpecula hypoleucus and quenda Isoodon fusciventer. Small native mammals included western
bush rat Rattus fuscipes fuscipes, fat-tailed dunnart Sminthopsis crassicaudata and the western pygmy possum
Cercartetus concinnus.
Animals 2020,10, 707 9 of 23
3.4. Prey Size (Average Body Mass for Each Prey Category)
We analysed prey size for a total of 611 invertebrate and vertebrate prey items collected from
n=294 cats that had fauna present, with an average of 1.72
±
1.07 (range 1–6) prey categories analysed
per individual cat (full list of prey taken by cats given in Table 4). House mice (average m
b
19 g) were
the most common prey item recorded, being present in 167 cats, while 107 cats had invertebrates
(each estimated as m
b
0.5 g mass) present in their stomachs. The heaviest prey was black-footed rock
wallaby (Petrogale lateralis, adult m
b
averages 4.05 kg)—body parts including the hindfeet of a juvenile
were identified.
Table 4.
List of prey species identified from stomach content analysis of n=294 feral and stray cats
(Felis catus) from southwest Western Australia that had fauna present in their stomachs. The number
of cats eating each prey item does not equal the total number of species eaten as many cats ate >1
species. Except for invertebrates (which were all assigned an arbitrary mass value of 0.5 g, regardless
of whether or not we could identify them), analysis of prey mass excluded prey items that could not
be identified suciently to ascribe a mass to the item. The average body mass for each prey species
(carried out for n=611 prey items) represents an approximation of adult size from various sources in
the literature. Prey were distinguished as potentially ‘dangerous’ prey species—a subjective category
based on the authors’ experience of those species that could potentially deliver a reasonable bite or kick
while being handled.
Prey Species Species Name ‘Dangerous’ Avg. mb(g) Eaten by No. Cats
Invertebrates
(all orders/class/clade grouped) N 0.5 107
Cockroaches Blattodea
Beetles, weevils Coleoptera
Wasps, fly larvae Hymenoptera
Moths, moth larvae Lepidoptera
Dragonflies Odonata
Grasshoppers, crickets, mole-crickets Orthoptera
Scorpions Scorpiones
Spiders Araneae
Earthworms Haplotaxida
Centipedes Chilopoda clade
Snails Gastropoda class
Fish
Unknown fish sp. - N - 6
Amphibian
Kunapalari (wheatbelt) frog ENeobatrachus kunapalari N 16 1
Western banjo frog ELimnodynastes dorsalis N 19 1
Reptile
South-western clawless gecko ECrenadactylus ocellatus N 1 2
Fine-faced gecko Diplodactylus pulcher N 4 2
Tree dtella Gehyra variegata N 4 6
Bynoe’s gecko Heteronotia binoei N 2 1
Leopard skink Ctenotus patherinus N 8 3
Unknown skink sp. Ctenotus sp. N 7 3
King’s skink EEgernia kingii N 288 1
Southwestern earless skink Hemiergis initialis N 1 1
Unknown skink sp. Morethia sp. N 1 1
Bobtail skink ETiliqua rugosa rugosa N 512 1
Unknown skink sp. Family: Scincidae N 1 2
Western heath dragon Ctenophorus adelaidensis N 2 1
Ornate crevice dragon Ctenophorus ornatus N 2 1
Western netted dragon Ctenophorus reticulatus N 2 1
Western bearded dragon EPogona minor minor N 31 5
Thorny devil Moloch horridus N 38 1
Gould’s monitor Varanus gouldii Y 350 1
Black-headed monitor Varanus tristis Y 200 1
Unknown monitor sp. Varanus sp. Y - 3
Southern blindsnake Anilios australis N 4.5 ? 1
Dark-spined blindsnake Anilios bicolor N 4.5 1
Pale-headed blindsnake EAnilios hamatus N 20 1
Unknown blindsnake sp. Anilios sp. N 20 2
Animals 2020,10, 707 10 of 23
Table 4. Cont.
Prey Species Species Name ‘Dangerous’ Avg. mb(g) Eaten by No. Cats
Southern shovel-nosed snake Brachyurophis semifasciata N 100 1
Bardick snake Echiopsis curta Y 400 1
Western crowned snake EElapognathus coronatus Y 40 1
Gould’s hooded snake EParasuta gouldii Y 200 1
Western brown snake Pseudonaja mengdeni Y 700 1
Unknown snake sp. Family: Elapidae Y - 2
Unknown reptile sp. - - - 1
Bird
Domestic chicken *Gallus domesticus N 1000 8
Laughing dove * Streptopelia senegalensis N 82 1
Spotted dove Streptopelia chinensis N 132 1
Unknown dove sp. Family: Columbidae N 155 2
Brush bronzewing Phaps elegans N 200 2
Australian wood duck Chenonetta jubata N 450 3
Stubble quail Coturnix pectoralis N 115 3
Unknown button-quail sp. Turnix sp. N 110 1
Galah Eolophus roseicapillus Y 300 7
Rainbow lorikeet * Trichoglossus haematodus Y 116 1
Purple-crowned lorikeet Glossopsitta porphyrocephala Y 44 1
Western rosella EPlatycercus icterotis Y 61 3
Red-capped parrot EPurpureicephalus spurius Y 116 1
Australian ringneck parrot Barnardius zonarius Y 137 33
Elegant parrot Neophema elegans Y 43 1
Unknown parrot sp. Order: Psittaciformes Y 149 1
Unknown non-passerine sp. - N - 2
Rufous tree creeper Climacteris rufa N 31 1
Unknown fairy wren sp. Malurus sp. N 10 1
New Holland honeyeater Phylidonyris novaehollandiae N 20 2
White-cheeked honeyeater Phylidonyris niger N 20 1
Unknown honeyeater sp. Phylodonyris sp. N 20 1
Yellow-throated miner Manorina flavigula N 55 1
Rufous whistler Pachycephala rufiventris N 25 1
Australian raven Corvus coronoides N 650 1
Willie wagtail Rhipidura leucophrys N 20 1
Grey fantail Rhipidura albiscapa N 9 1
Magpie-lark Grallina cyanoleuca N 83 1
Unknown small passerine sp. - N - 3
Unknown bird sp. - - - 22
Mammal
European rabbit * Oryctolagus cuniculus Y 1800 34
House mouse * Mus musculus N 19 167
Black rat * Rattus rattus Y 218 21
Western bush rat ERattus fuscipes fuscipes Y 76 1
Fat-tailed dunnart Sminthopsis crassicaudata N 15 1
Quenda (bandicoot) E,CS Isoodon fusciventer N 755 1
Western pygmy possum ECercartetus concinnus N 45 1
Common brushtail possum ETrichosurus vulpecula hypoleucus Y 2850 10
Black-footed rock wallaby E,CS Petrogale lateralis Y 4050 1
Unknown mammal sp. - - - 1
Total number of cats eating vertebrates 256
Grand total cats eating invertebrates and vertebrates 294
* Introduced species in WA.
E
Endemic WA species.
CS
Species is of conservation significance? Body mass estimated
based on that for congeneric species.
ł
Species that were not included in the analysis of prey mass predictors as they
could represent scavenging of carrion.
For 307 prey items, we had complete body measurements, age and estimated bite force for the cat
that had consumed them. There was no single best model to describe prey body mass data. Thirteen
models had a
AIC <2; none of these 13 models had particularly strong predictive power although
all models returned a significant coecient of determination (r
2
<0.038, p<0.003). Cat age was
the strongest factor, included in 10 of 13 of these top models (
Σβ·
w
i
=0.101, Figure 3c,d). Bite force
was represented in seven of 13 models (
Σβ·
w
i
=0.066, Figure 3e,f) and cat mass was present in six
of 13 (
Σβ·
w
i
=0.044, Figure 3g,h). Sex (4/13 models,
Σβ·
w
i
=0.029), source location (3/13 models,
Σβ·wi=0.012
) and body condition (1/13 models,
Σβ·
w
i
=
0.002) each had low explanatory power
to describe prey body mass. Prey species taken by feral (average prey m
b
279
±
655 g, n=545) and
Animals 2020,10, 707 11 of 23
stray (190
±
543 g, n=94) cats were not statistically significantly dierent in terms of their body mass
(Mann–Whitney U test Z=1.34, p=0.179). There was also no dierence in the average prey mass
taken by male (284
±
641 g, n=360) and female (247
±
646 g, n=280) cats (Mann–Whitney U test
Z=1.52, p=0.129).
Animals 2020, 10, x 12 of 24
Figure 3. Prey body mass (average body mass for an adult of that species) consumed by cats (Felis
catus) from southwest Western Australia shown by (a) sex (left-hand panel; males = red triangles and
females = grey circles) or (b) source location (right-hand panel; rural = green circles and urban = blue
triangles). Prey body mass shown against age (c,d), estimated bite force (e,f) and cat body mass (g,h).
Note Log-transformed axes. Due to overlapping data, symbol sizes represent relative numbers of prey
items. Diet was determined for some small cats that we could not determine bite force for (and
Prey body mass (g)
Figure 3.
Prey body mass (average body mass for an adult of that species) consumed by cats (Felis catus)
from southwest Western Australia shown by (
a
) sex (left-hand panel; males =red triangles and
females =grey circles) or (
b
) source location (right-hand panel; rural =green circles and urban =blue
triangles). Prey body mass shown against age (
c
,
d
), estimated bite force (
e
,
f
) and cat body mass (
g
,
h
).
Note Log-transformed axes. Due to overlapping data, symbol sizes represent relative numbers of prey
items. Diet was determined for some small cats that we could not determine bite force for (and therefore
Animals 2020,10, 707 12 of 23
the x axis has a lower minimum value compared with Figure 2). The most common prey items are
indicated in (
g
): European rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus 1.8 kg), black rats (Rattus rattus 220 g), house
mice (Mus musculus 19 g), and invertebrates (all grouped as 0.5 g). Prey that might be perceived
as requiring greater hunting skills (‘dangerous’ prey) are indicated with a black cross and the black
regression lines in left-hand panels.
Black rats, a number of parrots, venomous snakes and varanids (‘monitor’), as well as the European
rabbit, brushtail possum and the black-footed rock wallaby, were subjectively identified as potentially
‘dangerous’ prey that could be considered more dicult for cats to handle (Table 4). There were no
significant relationships for the presence of ‘dangerous’ prey items by cat age (see the black lines on
Figure 3c, r
2
=0.033, p=0.064), by estimated bite force (Figure 3e, r
2
=0.003, p=0.639), or by cat body
mass (Figure 3g, r2=0.013, p=0.171).
4. Discussion
Review of previous studies has suggested that large male cats represent a disproportionate risk to
threatened and translocated native wildlife populations in Australia [
17
]. To test this observation tested
whether a cat’s sex, age, body mass, body condition, and bite force reflected the size of the prey they had
consumed. Males were 36.2% heavier and had 20.0% greater bite force than females. Males therefore
attain greater body mass and bite forces that would allow them to handle a greater range of prey.
However, the strongest predictor of prey size was the age of the cat, with older cats taking the largest
prey on average. Older cats had greater estimated bite force and would also have an experience
advantage over younger animals, as an individual’s hunting skills would improve with repeated
exposure to a broadening range of prey types [
39
]. None of the factors that we tested were predictors
of the body mass of ‘dangerous’ prey items that had been consumed.
4.1. Bite Force Anatomy
Felids vary markedly in body size, with skull size and structure strongly correlated with bite force
and maximum prey size taken [
40
,
41
]. Bite force is therefore a useful proxy for prey handling ability
in these carnivorans, revealing dierences in potential diet. Cat jaw mechanics follow the general
carnivore pattern of having a high coronoid process, hinge-like jaw condyle to improve mechanical
leverage of the temporalis muscle, and a tooth row at the same level of this condyle [
40
]. The masseter
and pterygoideus muscles play a crucial role in adduction and lateral stabilisation of the jaw during
subduing and handling of prey. Among felids, species of the domestic cat lineage show a broader
face, and the ratio of their zygomatic arches breadth to skull length is significantly larger than in most
other Felidae species [
40
]. Furthermore, F. catus possess a relatively broad skull with wide braincase
and robust cheek bones—typically associated with the allocation of strong masseters [
40
]. Felids have
a short rostrum and, consequently, a short jaw out-force moment arm—such adaptation typifies a skull
with large jaw muscles that facilitate mechanical closure and substantial grip, such as is required for
subduing moving prey. This robust anatomy contrasts with skulls that have a longer snout that would
enable higher velocities at the canine, a characteristic of predators specialising in relatively small, more
agile prey and those that use a pounce-pursuit or ambush hunting style [25,42].
Body mass was the strongest single factor influencing estimated bite force in our sample of F. catus.
The body mass values that we recorded (overall average of 3.08
±
1.24 kg for our sample of n=568)
are within national ranges (e.g., 1.50–7.30 kg [
43
45
]). Our maximum body masses (males 6.7 kg,
females 4.9 kg) were smaller than extremes documented by other studies (e.g., [
43
45
]), and anecdotal
records for large feral cats in Australia [
46
], which, if they can be verified, are alarming. For example,
in Gippsland, Victoria, a hunter claimed to have shot a giant cat with head-body measurement of 1.6 m
plus a 0.6 m tail [
47
]. If feral cats are increasing in size over generations, then the predatory impact
of such giant cats on native fauna may also increase. Taking this argument a step further, Dickman,
Legge and Woinarski [
48
] have identified the risk of importing felid genetics into Australia that could
Animals 2020,10, 707 13 of 23
contribute to larger feral cats. Overseas, Felis catus has been deliberately cross-bred with other felid
species under captive conditions to produce hybrid ‘designer breed’ cats with characteristics that are
deemed to be desirable by the domestic pet trade [
48
]. For example, ‘savannah cats’ are a cross between
Felis catus and the African serval Leptailurus serval. Servals are extremely agile [
49
] and ecient hunters
(49% of hunting attempts yield prey), and while their diet generally comprises prey items <200 g,
they can also take medium-sized mammals, birds and reptiles [50]. Savannah cats have inherited the
serval’s long legs and ability to jump several metres in a single bound [
49
]. Weighing 2–4 times the
average mass of F. catus (males can weigh 8.0–11.0 kg; females are slightly smaller [
48
]), savannah cats
could therefore pose a threat to a greater range of Australian native species. In recognition of this
threat, the Australian government banned the importation of the savannah cat in 2008 [48].
Sexual dimorphism in skulls is common in felids [
40
]. The cat data we collected conformed
with this pattern, with male cats being 36.2% heavier than females (males 3.50
±
1.34 kg, females
2.57
±
0.89 kg) and having 20.0% greater bite force. While small males had estimated bite forces
somewhat less than that of females of equivalent mass, males developed disproportionately greater
estimated bite force as they grew, such that heavier males had greater estimated bite forces than
females of equivalent mass. Males should therefore have a greater range of prey sizes available to
them, although we note that there were no sex dierences in overall diet composition.
Estimated bite force increased with age in F. catus. An increase in bite force measures with age has
similarly been reported in a range of other species (e.g., [
25
,
51
,
52
]). Older F. catus should therefore
have greater prey handling ability, independent of their skills and experience. Anecdotally, we had
one large male cat (#362, 4.24 kg, BCI 18.0% heavier than predicted from his body size) that had no
teeth at all—they had all broken o. Closure of cranial sutures indicated that he would have been at
least 6 years of age [Fleming et al. unpublished data], but was likely older than this (without teeth we
could not age the animal using incremental cementum lines). The cat had 10 house mice and domestic
chicken in its stomach and so, despite lacking teeth, it was clearly still a successful predator.
We also recorded dierences in bite force according to their source location, with feral (rural) cats
having greater estimated bite forces compared with stray (urban) cats, while there was no dierence
in body mass by source location. The dierence was most marked for younger and smaller cats,
where feral cats had greater estimated bite force than their stray counterparts. Handling live prey
would influence the development of bite force, and dierences in diet composition (see next section:
greater proportion of live prey present in the diets of feral cats) are therefore likely to be driving these
dierences in bite force.
Cat body condition did not contribute to the dierences in estimated bite force, suggesting that
even nutritionally-stressed cats with lower body condition are capable of delivering a similar estimated
bite force as relatively heavier individuals. Bite force is estimated from the skulls, looking at the area of
muscle attachment. This is ‘retrospective’ in the sense that it represents past condition—the cat had to
have had the muscles at some time to need that area of support. Therefore, current condition is not
the key predictor of bite force—past condition, rather, or perhaps condition in adolescence or young
adulthood, is likely to be more important.
4.2. Diet Composition
Cats in our sample had consumed a broad range of prey, including invertebrates, birds, reptiles
and small and medium-sized mammals. Despite the dierences in bite force and body mass that
we recorded, the anatomy of cats in our sample did not appear to influence their diet composition.
The only factor that aected diet composition was the source location of these cats.
The greatest dierence in diet of feral (rural) and stray (urban) cats was due to the abundance of
live prey for feral cats (house mice, rabbits, reptiles, and invertebrates) compared with human refuse
for stray cats. These findings likely reflect dierences in the foods available in each environment.
For example, an average of 18.8%
±
30.0% of stomach contents volume for our stray cat sample
was made up of refuse—mostly human food scraps and plastic, paper and foil (more detail has
Animals 2020,10, 707 14 of 23
been published elsewhere [
26
]). Other studies similarly report various forms of ‘food scraps’ in cat
diets (e.g., [
53
56
]). These data suggest that cats living close to human habitation modify their diet,
possibly in relation to either prey availability or preference for easily obtained scavenged refuse
(e.g., old sandwiches, meat trays) that requires less jaw strength to access and ingest.
While both stray and rural cats consumed human refuse, very few ingested animal carrion
(7.7%, total n=27 ate sheep, pig, rabbit or unknown carrion, 9 peri-urban cats, 18 rural; carrion data
were not included in our diet analyses). Other authors have also reported that carrion is rarely taken
by feral cats (e.g., [
57
]). Carrion was readily taken by red foxes sourced from the same agricultural
environment as our rural cats [
25
], so was clearly available. The cat’s propensity to hunt live prey even
when carrion is available may therefore threaten native fauna still surviving in agricultural areas.
There was no significant dierence (p=0.179) in prey body mass taken by feral (279
±
655 g,
n=545) or stray (190
±
543 g, n=94) cats in this study. Diet data in the present study may be limited
by prey availability at our sample locations; we note that we had also removed carrion from our
analyses. By contrast with our findings, Bateman and Fleming [
16
] analysed data presented by Pearre
and Maass [
58
] and found that cats sampled from sites close to human habitation (farms, suburban
and urban studies) take significantly smaller prey (23.2
±
8.3 g; n=16 studies) than cats in rural areas
(72.6
±
92.1 g, n=28 studies). The broader prey base available across dierent studies at dierent
locations (and perhaps also the inclusion of carrion) may be a better test of this location dierence in
prey size than for our present study.
In terms of Australian mammal prey, introduced rabbits and house mice are predominant in diet
of cats from semi-arid and some arid habitats, while marsupials are predominant in diet of cats from
temperate forest, urban and suburban habitats (reviewed by [
7
,
11
]). Cats are noted rodent specialists
(e.g., Dickman and Newsome [
21
]) and rodents were the most common prey taken by cats in our
sample. Just over half the cats in our study (56.8% of those that had consumed fauna) had consumed
house mice prior to trapping, with rural cats taking the largest numbers (maximum 30 mice in one
cat’s stomach). By contrast, only 7.1% of cats had consumed black rats. In comparison to mice, rats are
large and aggressive and take skill to dispatch [
59
] (matching our definition of ‘dangerous’ prey for
the present study). For example, two studies on the interactions between rats and cats cohabiting in
alleyways in Baltimore, USA, found that stray cats preyed on juvenile but not adult rats and would
scavenge refuse despite presence of rats [60,61].
Where these introduced animals have become well established [
62
], European rabbits are
common prey in the diet of cats across many parts of Australia (e.g., [
21
,
53
,
63
,
64
], reviewed by
Doherty et al. [
11
]). There is some suggestion that larger male cats are able to take larger rabbits
(e.g., Hart [
65
], but see Catling [
64
] who found no dierence in the representation of rabbit in the diet
of ‘immature’ females <2.2 kg, males <3.5 kg and ‘mature’ cats). Anecdotal data suggest that cats in
some ‘rabbit-infested’ inland regions may be larger than those in rabbit-free areas in northern and
eastern Australia (P. Wagner, pers. comm. cited by Dickman [
7
]), perhaps indicating selection for larger
body size, or greater longevity due to access to prey [
7
]. Although not common across our southwest
Western Australian study sites, rabbits were evident in low abundance in the stomachs of cats we
sampled: averaging 3%
±
15% by stomach contents volume, or 57%
±
33% of stomach contents volume
for the 34 cats that had eaten rabbit. Prey availability clearly plays an important role in diet for feral
and semi-feral cats, as has also been noted for pet cats across Australia (see short overview in Grayson
and Calver [66]), which may account for variation in prevalence of rabbits in the diet of cats [11].
Feral cats include a substantial number of birds in their diet, with an estimated toll of 272 million
Australian birds per year (95% confidence interval: 169–508 million) [
67
]. Birds were common in the
diets of the cats we studied with several cats consuming >1 bird species or individual (one cat ate four
bird species, three cats ate three species, and one cat ate 11 individuals of two species). In reviewing the
traits of birds predated by cats, Woinarski et al. [
68
] reported increased cat predation risk for birds that
nest or forage on the ground. Our data support this finding, with evidence of cats having consumed at
least seven parrot species, many of which are ground-foraging species. Notably, 11.2% of cats had
Animals 2020,10, 707 15 of 23
feathers or body parts of Australian ringneck parrots present in their stomachs. These parrots are
gregarious and flighty so hunting them on the ground takes stealth. Other ground-foraging species
were also common, including pigeon, button- and stubble-quail species, with one cat having consumed
10 baby stubble-quails (in addition to a small passerine and two reptiles).
Cat predation has been recorded for about one-quarter of described Australian reptile species [
69
].
A broad range of reptile species were similarly recorded as prey in both rural and urban cats in the
present study. Cats had consumed at least 23 reptile species, including monitors, dragons, skinks,
geckos, snakes and ground burrowing blindsnakes. Some of these species would be dangerous to
handle. For example, monitor lizards (e.g., Varanus gouldii, measuring up to 1.4 m long and weighing
up to 6.0 kg) grow to be large and aggressive; they are fast runners with strong legs and sharp claws,
and use their whip-like tail as a defensive weapon. In our study, three of the seven cats that had
eaten monitor lizards were aged just 5–10 months and weighed 1.4–4.0 kg. Six cats in our study
(weight range 2.3–4.8 kg) had eaten venomous snakes, with the youngest cat only 10 months of age.
McGregor et al. [
70
] video recorded rural cats skilfully catching a venomous adult western brown
snake (Pseudonaja mengdeni,
1.2–2 m in length) and immediately crushing its head to subdue the
‘dangerous end’. Cats are therefore clearly a threat to even venomous snakes. Our data also suggests
that even young cats could threaten venomous and non-venomous reptiles.
We recorded the consumption of frogs by only two cats. This finding conforms with studies that
have found frogs to be an uncommon prey item, except for island cat populations [
71
]. Both cats were
from rural locations. One had a single western banjo frog in its stomach while the other had 30 juvenile
endemic Kunapalari frogs of identical size that were probably hunted at a single breeding location
(e.g., farm dam). Such large numbers of prey suggest an opportunistic hunting strategy that could
impact local populations of frogs.
Invertebrates are normally viewed as a minor supplementary food source for cats (e.g., [
53
,
57
])
except for on islands or in arid/semi-arid areas where invertebrates are readily available and where
other prey taxa may be limited or seasonal [
71
,
72
]. However, we recorded substantial amounts of
invertebrates in the diets of both stray and feral cats. Up to 51 g of invertebrates (actual measurements
of the mass of invertebrates recorded from the stomach) were eaten by 31% of cats (body mass
range 0.5–5.8 kg), of both sexes and across locations, with grasshoppers, beetles and centipedes most
commonly ingested. Grasshoppers and centipedes were similarly the most frequent invertebrates
recorded from feral cat stomachs from Eastern Musgrave Ranges in arid South Australia [
44
], although
they rarely made up the majority of stomach contents. Invertebrates can be highly nutritious, especially
gravid females that have large body mass and lipid concentrations in the developing eggs [
73
].
Additionally, although cats can survive without access to free-standing water [
74
], it is likely that
invertebrates are an important source of water, fat and protein for cats when other prey is scarce
(similar to red foxes [
75
77
]). This may explain why, regardless of bite force, 36.4% of cats had consumed
invertebrates. The importance of invertebrates as prey likely varies with location and season, in keeping
with the understanding of cats as opportunistic predators.
4.3. Prey Size
Prey body mass was positively correlated with cat age as well as their bite force and body
mass. These relationships were not strong, however, with models each explaining less than 4% of the
variability in prey body mass. This is due to the wide range of prey sizes taken by all cats. Even small
cats had large prey items in their stomach contents; for example, two of the cats that predated European
rabbit (
1.8 kg) weighed 1.4 and 1.5 kg respectively. Furthermore, large cats had taken many small
prey, with 20% of cats weighing more than 5.0 kg having consumed invertebrates, our smallest prey
size category (0.5 g).
Felidae species
10 kg can take prey larger than themselves, while species <10 kg tend to take
prey that are smaller than themselves [
40
]. The average mass of prey taken by cats in our study was
379
±
740 g for 434 prey items (excluding invertebrates). This is around 12.4 % of the average body
Animals 2020,10, 707 16 of 23
mass for our sample of cats and therefore similar to predicted prey range based on their body mass
(e.g., 13% [
78
], 11% [
79
]). Paltridge et al. [
57
] similarly report common prey in the range of 10–350 g.
Animals within the critical weight range (35 g–5.5 kg) are ideal weight range for cat prey [
5
,
80
].
Such small prey species would be unlikely to pose a challenge to the cat in terms of the bite force
required to handle the prey.
Larger prey have also been recorded in the diets of feral cats in Australia [
80
], albeit less
frequently [
7
,
11
,
80
]. For example, Dickman [
7
] records bird prey up to 3.5 kg on islands as exceptions.
Compared with smaller prey, medium-sized marsupials are likely to pose a greater hunting challenge
because their body size represents a reasonable match to an average cat. We recorded the remains
of a juvenile black-footed rock wallaby in the stomach of one cat trapped from around a protected
rock wallaby population. The female cat that had eaten the wallaby weighed 3.5 kg, a near match for
a juvenile wallaby. In addition to their body size, marsupials such as rock wallabies (Petrogale spp.)
also escape predators using propulsive hopping locomotion and are capable of delivering injurious
kicks. Despite their size and challenges with handling them, however, there is mounting evidence that
even medium-sized marsupials are taken by feral cats as prey (Table 5). For sexually dimorphic prey
species (where females are smaller), females and juveniles are more vulnerable to cat predation due to
their smaller body size [
81
], while juveniles are also predator-naïve and females with pouch-young are
more encumbered [
82
]. Even if it is relatively infrequent, predation by feral cats therefore represents
a significant conservation issue for populations of medium-sized threatened species.
Table 5.
Evidence of feral cats (Felis catus) killing medium-sized Australian mammals (1 kg or larger);
all these species are of conservation significance. For reference, the body mass of cats in our studies
averaged 3.08
±
1.24 kg, n=568. Unless given by specific publications (e.g., Page et al. [
83
]), body
mass measures of prey species are from Van Dyck and Strahan [28].
Prey Species Average Adult Body
Mass (by Sex) Evidence Adult/Juvenile Reference
Tammar wallaby
(Macropus eugenii)6.0 kg Extirpation from islands J and A Dickman [7]
Tasmanian pademelon
(Thylogale billardierii)
M 7.0 kg (3.8–12.0 kg)
F 3.9 kg (2.4–10.0 kg) Camera trap A
(F 4 kg) Fancourt [81]
Allied rock-wallaby
(Petrogale assimilis)
M 4.7 kg
F 4.3 kg Direct observation, carcasses * J and A
(4.0 kg) Spencer [84]
Black-footed rock wallaby
(Petrogale lateralis)
M 4.5 kg (4.1–5.0 kg)
F 3.5 kg (3.1–3.8 kg)
Stomach contents J This study
Stomach contents and direct
observation of cat feeding at a
freshly-killed adult
J and A Read et al. [44]
Stomach contents ? Paltridge et al. [57]
Bridled nailtail wallaby
(Onychogalea fraenata)
M 6.0 kg (5.0–8.0 kg)
F 4.5 kg (4.0–6.0 kg)
Stomach contents J (1.5 kg)
Horsup and Evans [
85
]
Predation ł J (3.0 kg) Fisher, Blomberg and
Hoyle [86]
Spectacled hare-wallaby
(Lagorchestes conspicillatus)Extirpation from islands J and A Dickman [7]
Rufous hare-wallaby
(Lagorchestes hirsutus)
M 1.6 kg (1.2–1.8 kg)
F 1.7 kg (0.8–2.0 kg)
Stomach contents
Predation ł
?
J and A Gibson et al. [82]
Predation ł A Hardman et al. [87]
Stomach contents ? Paltridge et al. [57]
Banded hare-wallaby
(Lagostrophus fasciatus)1.6 kg (1.0–2.3 kg) Predation ł A Hardman et al. [87]
Brush-tailed
bettong/woylie
(Bettongia penicillata)
M 1.27 kg (0.98–1.85 kg)
F 1.40 kg (0.75–1.50 kg)
Predation ł
Loss oislands AMarlow et al. [88]
Dickman [7]
Burrowing bettong/boodie
(Bettongia lesueur)Loss oislands Dickman [7]
* Carcasses showing characteristic evidence of having been eaten by a cat. łPredation of radio-collared animals.
? No way to identify whether the animals were taken were juvenile or adult from analysis of hair in stomach contents.
Cats appear to learn how to take large prey, with individuals becoming specialised to particular
prey [
21
], and therefore the incidence of specific larger prey species being taken can increase in a cat’s
Animals 2020,10, 707 17 of 23
diet over time. Hardman, Moro and Calver [
87
] present evidence showing that once one translocated
rufous hare-wallaby (Lagorchestes hirsutus) was killed, predation continued until each population was
eliminated; in many cases this extirpation was within a matter of days, suggesting that individual
cats quickly became specialised as hunters of these mammals. Similarly, Gibson et al. [
82
] report
that trapping of specific individual cats (no mention of their body mass) resulted in the cessation
of predation on rufous hare-wallabies. Our data supports these field observations, with a positive
correlation between prey body mass and cat age that could support learned skills in prey capture as
cats age. Feral cat trapping programmes are therefore essential to preserving even medium-sized
marsupial populations.
4.4. Other Considerations—Bite Force and Fighting/Territorial Mating Behaviour
Increases in ‘bite performance’ [
89
], which presumably includes bite force, improves territorial
defence and therefore mating possibilities in lizards [
89
91
], rodents [
92
] and lemurs [
51
]. Cats are
territorial and will fight other cats using claws and biting [
93
,
94
], and cats with greater bite forces could
therefore be advantaged in physical confrontations. Male cats also restrain females during copulation
by biting the back of the neck [
95
], taking advantage of immobility induced by the ‘scrureflex’ [
96
]
and protects the male from retaliatory aggression from the female. Bite force in cats may not be solely
a product of prey selection, therefore, with sexual selection also possibly playing a role.
4.5. Limitations of This Study
There are a few caveats to interpreting diet analyses based on stomach contents of these cats.
First, the diet for our animals represents a snapshot in time—our diet data are only a measure of
what each individual cat had eaten in the previous
12–24 h and would also be influenced by prey
availability and if cats were trapped in cages for a period. There is reasonable evidence to suggest that
individual cats become specialised on particular prey [
17
,
21
,
97
,
98
], however, which may lend weight
to the interpretation of diet from a single sample as reasonably reflective of what that cat might eat on
an ‘average day’.
Second, some prey items identified from hair or body parts could be juveniles, and therefore
the species’ average adult body mass values that we compared our data against would need to be
considered with caution. The black-footed rock wallaby consumed by a cat was identified as juvenile
(although adult rock wallabies are also predated by feral cats [
44
]). Identifying the size of prey from
stomach contents can be informative. For example, Catling [
64
] identified that small rabbits (estimated
to be <50 days old) comprised 73% of the diet of both mature and immature cats, but adult rabbits
comprised only 8% of their diet.
Third, identifying whether prey are taken live should always be considered. Although we retrieved
whole or parts of animals, we also identified prey species from hair or feathers. Arguably, some of
these could represent prey that had escaped capture (e.g., birds). Many authors report that carrion is
rarely consumed by feral cats (e.g., [
57
,
80
]), although this may depend on alternative food availability.
This avoidance of carrion is behind the diculty reported in baiting feral cats and the extensive
work undertaken in developing poison baits that are attractive, especially at times of high food
availability [
99
101
]. We similarly found that carrion (defined as consumption of part of a decomposing
carcass, with or without fly maggots, as opposed to scraps of discarded human foods classified as
refuse) was uncommon—only 27 cats had consumed sheep, pig or rabbit carrion (<8% of the 352 cats
analysed for diet). As we are confident that we separated carrion from kills, we know that the prey
were hunted and killed as live animals.
Finally, female cats bring back prey for their young which means that small cats would have access
to larger prey than they could capture themselves. From 4 weeks post-partum, mothers bring their
kittens a range of live and dead prey so that they can ‘play’ with dierent foods and develop hunting
sequences (e.g., pouncing, grappling; species-specific hunting sequences needed to kill rats vs. bird
etc. [
102
,
103
]). Kittens are generally weaned from maternal care and start hunting independently by
Animals 2020,10, 707 18 of 23
the age of 3–4 months [
39
,
97
], but weaning can occur earlier in resource-poor environments where the
lactation period may be reduced [103]. Our sample therefore included very few cats that would have
still been dependent on their mothers for food. The prey detected in the four kittens aged <4 months
in our study included a single mouse, and invertebrates in three kittens, and likely reflects this
maternal provision (or opportunistic consumption). However, older cats were likely to be capturing
their own live prey. Sixteen cats were under 6 months of age (estimated to be either 4 or 5 months
and possibly still weaning). While most had ingested invertebrates, four of these had consumed
vertebrates: a blindsnake, a black-headed monitor, a brushtail possum, and a domestic chicken—two
of which we subjectively classified as ‘dangerous’ prey. Our sample also included 155 juvenile cats
aged between 6 and 10 months of age which should all have been hunting independently. Trap bait or
human refuse was present in the stomach of 81 of these cats (52.2%) and probably reflects opportunistic
consumption of available ‘free’ foods. The remaining 74 cats consumed a wide range of prey (37.8% of
cats 5–10 months ate 122 vertebrates; 21.6% ate invertebrates), with up to seven and eight vertebrates
in two stomachs each respectively. Notably, many of the prey species taken by these young cats were
large and/or dangerous. For example, one cat ate a highly venomous western brown snake, three cats
ate monitor lizards, six ate parrots, five consumed black rats, and three consumed brushtail possums.
Such dangerous prey require considerable skill to capture and subdue without injury to the cat, so their
consumption illustrates just how quickly cats become adept hunters following weaning.
5. Conclusions
The availability of prey is presumably the strongest determinant of stray and feral cat diets, which
will opportunistically feed on common or most easily captured prey (reviewed by [
11
]). Various studies
similarly indicate that learned behaviour influences prey taken by cats (e.g., [
21
,
87
,
97
]). Our data adds
to this list of factors influencing cat diet by revealing that the range of prey accessible to cats is also
influenced by their anatomy. Large male cats do indeed represent the greatest predation risk for native
wildlife species in that their body mass and bite force would allow them to access a greater range of
prey species. The threat posed by smaller cats is not to be ignored, however, as even small cats in our
study had been active hunters, and many had taken some surprisingly large prey species.
The strongest predictor of prey size in our study was the age of the cat, with older cats taking larger
prey on average. We note that age is also the strongest factor correlated with relative body condition
for a sample of n=79 male stray cats (there was no obvious relationship for n=77 females [
26
]).
Other evidence of increased hunting success with age comes from cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus [
104
]) and
spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta [
105
]). Success in hunting with age has also been documented in raptors
(e.g., northern goshawk Accipiter gentilis), where older birds have improved hunting success, increased
provisioning, and therefore greater breeding success [
106
]. While male cats do not provision their young
or their mates, greater hunting success would nevertheless influence their own fitness, while greater
hunting success in older females would benefit themselves as well as their young. Two of the oldest feral
cats in our sample we estimated to be
14 and
16 years of age (both were females; notably one was
lactating and the other pregnant). One had consumed livestock feed (demonstrating opportunism in
diet) while the other had both bird and reptile remains in its stomach. Another old male (unknown age)
was demonstrably a successful mouse hunter, despite lacking all his teeth. Age gives a combination of
size and experience that both facilitate successful hunting of the greatest range of prey sizes and taxa.
Consequently, age and body mass together describe a cat that poses the most substantial threat to
native species.
Supplementary Materials:
The following are available online at http://www.mdpi.com/2076-2615/10/4/707/s1,
Table S1. Locations where 567 feral and stray cats (Felis catus) were sourced across southwest Western Australia
between 2010 and 2018.
Author Contributions:
Conceptualization, P.A.F.; Data curation, P.A.F., H.M.C. and C.H.A.; Formal analysis, P.A.F.
and H.M.C.; Investigation, H.M.C. and C.H.A.; Methodology, C.H.A. and M.C.C.; Supervision, P.A.F. and M.C.C.;
Animals 2020,10, 707 19 of 23
Writing—original draft, P.A.F.; Writing—review and editing, P.A.F., H.M.C. and M.C.C. All authors have read and
agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: This research received no external funding.
Acknowledgments:
Specimens donated by private or government animal controllers. Thanks to Peter Adams,
Bill Bateman, Zsa Zsa Wong, Diana Nottle, and Stephen Callahan for contributions to skinning, skull maceration
and specimen processing, to Joe Porter, Jack Eastwood and Tegan Douglas for prey species identification, and to
Kate Bryant for allowing access to prey species body mass data based on 7 years of trapping at Gnangara.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
1.
Legge, S.; Murphy, B.P.; McGregor, H.; Woinarski, J.C.Z.; Augusteyn, J.; Ballard, G.; Baseler, M.; Buckmaster, T.;
Dickman, C.R.; Doherty, T. Enumerating a continental-scale threat: How many feral cats are in Australia?
Biol. Conserv. 2017,206, 293–303. [CrossRef]
2.
Commonwealth of Australia. Background Document for the Threat Abatement Plan for Predation by Feral Cats;
Department of the Environment, Australian Government: Canberra, Australia, 2015.
3.
Woinarski, J.C.Z.; Burbidge, A.A.; Harrison, P.L. Ongoing unraveling of a continental fauna: Decline and
extinction of Australian mammals since European settlement. PNAS 2015,112, 4531–4540. [CrossRef]
4.
Burbidge, A.A.; McKenzie, N.L. Patterns in the modern decline of Western Australia’s vertebrate fauna:
Causes and conservation implications. Biol. Conserv. 1989,50, 143–198. [CrossRef]
5.
Radford, J.Q.; Woinarski, J.C.Z.; Legge, S.; Baseler, M.; Bentley, J.; Burbidge, A.A.; Bode, M.; Copley, P.;
Dexter, N.; Dickman, C.R.; et al. Degrees of population-level susceptibility of Australian terrestrial non-volant
mammal species to predation by the introduced red fox (Vulpes vulpes) and feral cat (Felis catus). J. Wildl. Res.
2018,45, 645–657. [CrossRef]
6.
Johnson, C.N.; Isaac, J.L. Body mass and extinction risk in Australian marsupials: The ‘Critical Weight Range’
revisited. Austral Ecol. 2009,34, 35–40. [CrossRef]
7.
Dickman, C.R. Overview of the Impacts of Feral Cats on Australian Native Fauna; 0642213798; Australian Nature
Conservation Agency: Canberra, Australia, 1996.
8.
Smith, A.P.; Quin, D.G. Patterns and causes of extinction and decline in Australian conilurine rodents.
Biol. Conserv. 1996,77, 243–267. [CrossRef]
9.
EPBC. Species Profile and Threats Database: Listed Key Threatening Processes; Australian Government Department
of the Environment and Energy: Canberra, Australia, 2019.
10.
Woinarski, J.C.Z.; Burbidge, A.A.; Harrison, P.L. The Action Plan for Australian Mammals 2012;
CSIRO Publishing: Collingwood, Victoria, 2014.
11.
Doherty, T.S.; Davis, R.A.; Etten, E.J.B.; Algar, D.; Collier, N.; Dickman, C.R.; Edwards, G.; Masters, P.;
Palmer, R.; Robinson, S. A continental-scale analysis of feral cat diet in Australia. J. Biogeogr.
2015
,42, 964–975.
[CrossRef]
12.
Pitakarnnop, T.; Buddhachat, K.; Euppayo, T.; Kriangwanich, W.; Nganvongpanit, K. Feline (Felis catus)
skull and pelvic morphology and morphometry: Gender-related dierence? Anat. Histol. Embryol.
2017
,46,
294–303. [CrossRef]
13.
Denny, E. Cat Felis catus. In The Mammals of Australia, 3rd ed.; Van Dyck, S., Strahan, R., Eds.; Reed New
Holland: Sydney, Australia, 2008; pp. 742–744.
14.
Litvaitis, J.A.; Stevens, C.L.; Mautz, W.W. Age, sex, and weight of bobcats in relation to winter diet.
J. Wildl. Manag. 1984,48, 632–635. [CrossRef]
15. Litvaitis, J.A.; Clark, A.G.; Hunt, J.H. Prey selection and fat deposits of bobcats (Felis rufus) during autumn
and winter in Maine. J. Mammal. 1986,67, 389–392. [CrossRef]
16.
Bateman, P.W.; Fleming, P.A. Big city life: Carnivores in urban environments. J. Zool. Lond.
2012
,287, 1–23.
[CrossRef]
17.
Moseby, K.E.; Peacock, D.E.; Read, J.L. Catastrophic cat predation: A call for predator profiling in wildlife
protection programs. Biol. Conserv. 2015,191, 331–340. [CrossRef]
18.
Christiansen, P.; Wroe, S. Bite forces and evolutionary adaptations to feeding ecology in carnivores. Ecology
2007,88, 347–358. [CrossRef]
Animals 2020,10, 707 20 of 23
19.
Therrien, F. Feeding behaviour and bite force of sabretoothed predators. Zool. J. Linn. Soc.
2005
,145, 393–426.
[CrossRef]
20.
Baerends-van Roon, J.M.; Baerends, G.P. The Morphogenesis of the Behaviour of the Domestic Cat,
with a Special Emphasis on the Development of Prey-Catching; North-Holland Publishing Company: Amsterdam,
The Netherlands, 1979; p. 72.
21.
Dickman, C.R.; Newsome, T.M. Individual hunting behaviour and prey specialisation in the house cat
Felis catus: Implications for conservation and management. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.
2015
,173, 76–87. [CrossRef]
22. Tabor, R. Cat Behaviour; The Bath Press for David and Charles: Devon, UK, 1997.
23. Kiltie, R.A. Size ratios among sympatric neotropical cats. Oecologia 1984,61, 411–416. [CrossRef]
24.
Thomason, J.J. Cranial strength in relation to estimated biting forces in some mammals. Can. J. Zool.
1991
,69,
2326–2333. [CrossRef]
25.
Forbes-Harper, J.L.; Crawford, H.M.; Dundas, S.J.; Warburton, N.M.; Adams, P.J.; Bateman, P.W.; Calver, M.C.;
Fleming, P.A. Diet and bite force in red foxes: Ontogenetic and sex dierences in an invasive carnivore.
J. Zool. 2017,303, 54–63. [CrossRef]
26.
Crawford, H.M.; Calver, M.C.; Fleming, P.A. Subsidised by junk foods: Factors influencing body condition in
stray cats (Felis catus). J. Urban Ecol. 2020, in press. [CrossRef]
27.
Brunner, H.; Triggs, B. Hair ID: An Interactive Tool for Identifying Australian Mammalian Hair; Ecobyte Pty Ltd.,
Ed.; CSIRO Publishing: Collingwood, Australia, 2002.
28. Van Dyck, S.; Strahan, R. The Mammals of Australia, 3rd ed.; Reed New Holland: Sydney, Australia, 2008.
29.
WA Museum. Checklist of the Terrestrial Vertebrate Fauna of Western Australia; Western Australian Museum:
Perth, Australia, 2019.
30.
Grue, H.; Jensen, B. Review of the formation of incremental lines in tooth cementum of terrestrial mammals.
Dan. Rev. Game Biol. 1979,11, 1–48.
31.
Damasceno, E.M.; Hingst-Zaher, E.; Astua, D. Bite force and encephalization in the Canidae (Mammalia:
Carnivora). J. Zool. Lond. 2013,290, 246–254. [CrossRef]
32. Sakamoto, M.; Lloyd, G.T.; Benton, M.J. Phylogenetically structured variance in felid bite force: The role of
phylogeny in the evolution of biting performance. J. Evol. Biol. 2010,23, 463–478. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
33. Statsoft Inc. Statistica (Data Analysis Software System); Version 8.0; Statsoft Inc.: Tulsa, OK, USA, 2007.
34.
R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing:
Vienna, Austria, 2010.
35.
Burnham, K.P.; Anderson, D.R. Model Selection and Multimodal Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic
Approach, 2nd ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2002.
36.
Grueber, C.E.; Nakagawa, S.; Laws, R.J.; Jamieson, I.G. Multimodel inference in ecology and evolution:
Challenges and solutions. J. Evol. Biol. 2011,24, 699–711. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
37.
Oksanen, J.; Blanchet, F.G.; Kindt, R.; Legendre, P.; Minchin, P.R.; O’hara, R.; Simpson, G.L.; Solymos, P.;
Stevens, M.H.H.; Wagner, H. Package ‘Vegan’. Community Ecology Package, R package version 2; R Foundation
for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2013; Volume 2, pp. 1–295.
38.
R Development Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for
Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2017.
39.
Bradshaw, J.W.S.; Casey, R.A.; Brown, S.L. Hunting and Predation. In The Behaviour of the Domestic Cat,
2nd ed.; CABI Publishers: Oxfordshire, UK, 2012; pp. 128–141.
40.
Sicuro, F.L.; Oliveira, L.F.B. Skull morphology and functionality of extant Felidae (Mammalia: Carnivora):
A phylogenetic and evolutionary perspective. Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 2011,161, 414–462. [CrossRef]
41.
Piras, P.; Maiorino, L.; Teresi, L.; Meloro, C.; Lucci, F.; Kotsakis, T.; Raia, P. Bite of the cats: Relationships
between functional integration and mechanical performance as revealed by mandible geometry. Syst. Biol.
2013,62, 878–900. [CrossRef]
42.
Attard, M.R.G.; Chamoli, U.; Ferrara, T.L.; Rogers, T.L.; Wroe, S. Skull mechanics and implications for feeding
behaviour in a large marsupial carnivore guild: The thylacine, Tasmanian devil and spotted-tailed quoll.
J. Zool. 2011,285, 292–300. [CrossRef]
43.
Read, J.; Bowen, Z. Population dynamics, diet and aspects of the biology of feral cats and foxes in arid South
Australia. Wildl. Res. 2001,28, 195–203. [CrossRef]
44.
Read, J.L.; Dagg, E.; Moseby, K.E. Prey selectivity by feral cats at central Australian rock-wallaby colonies.
Aust. Mammal. 2019,41, 132–141. [CrossRef]
Animals 2020,10, 707 21 of 23
45.
Jones, E.; Coman, B.J. Ecology of the feral cat, Felis catus (L.), in south-eastern Australia II.* Reproduction.
Wildl. Res. 1982,9, 111–119. [CrossRef]
46.
Naish, D. Williams and Lang’s Australian Big Cats: Do Pumas, Giant Feral Cats and Mystery Marsupials
Stalk the Australian Outback? Available online: https://blogs.scientificamerican.com/tetrapod-zoology/
williams-and-langs-australian- big-cats/(accessed on 15 January 2020).
47.
Williams, M.; Lang, R. Australian Big Cats: An Unnatural History of Panthers; Strange Nation: Hazelbrook,
Australia, 2010; p. 437. ISBN 978-0-646-53007-9.
48.
Dickman, C.R.; Legge, S.M.; Woinarski, J.C.Z. Assessing risks to wildlife from free-roaming hybrid cats:
The proposed introduction of pet Savannah cats to Australia as a case study. Animals
2019
,9, 795. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
49.
Markula, A.; Hannan-Jones, M.; Csurhes, S. Serval Hybrids; Hybrids of Leptailurus serval (serval) and Felis catus
(Domestic cat), Including the ‘Savannah cat’; Department of Agriculture and Fisheries, Biosecurity Queensland,
Queensland Government: Brisbane, Australia, 2016.
50.
Geertsema, A.A. Aspects of the ecology of the serval Leptailurus serval in the Ngorongoro Crater, Tanzania.
Neth. J. Zool. 1985,35, 527–610. [CrossRef]
51.
Chazeau, C.; Marchal, J.; Hackert, R.; Perret, M.; Herrel, A. Proximate determinants of bite force capacity in
the mouse lemur. J. Zool. Lond. 2013,290, 42–48. [CrossRef]
52.
Binder, W.J.; Van Valkenburgh, B. Development of bite strength and feeding behaviour in juvenile spotted
hyenas (Crocuta crocuta). J. Zool. 2000,252, 273–283. [CrossRef]
53.
Risbey, D.A.; Calver, M.C.; Short, J. The impact of cats and foxes on the small vertebrate fauna of Heirisson
Prong, Western Australia. I. Exploring potential impact using diet analysis. Wildl. Res. 1999,26, 621–630.
54.
Izawa, M.; Doi, T.; Ono, Y. Grouping patterns of feral cats (Felis catus) living on a small island in Japan.
Jpn. J. Ecol. 1982,32, 373–382. [CrossRef]
55.
Brickner-Braun, I.; Geen, E.; Yom-Tov, Y. The domestic cat as a predator of Israeli wildlife. Isr. J. Ecol. Evol.
2007,53, 129–142. [CrossRef]
56.
Campos, C.B.; Esteves, C.F.; Ferraz, K.M.P.M.B.; Crawshaw, P.G.; Verdade, L.M. Diet of free-ranging cats and
dogs in a suburban and rural environment, south-eastern Brazil. J. Zool. Proc. Zool. Soc. Lond.
2007
,273,
14–20. [CrossRef]
57.
Paltridge, R.; Gibson, D.; Edwards, G. Diet of the feral cat (Felis catus) in central Australia. Wildl. Res.
1997
,
24, 67–76. [CrossRef]
58.
Pearre, S.; Maass, R. Trends in the prey size-based trophic niches of feral and house cats Felis catus L.
Mammal Rev. 1998,28, 125–139. [CrossRef]
59.
Leyhausen, P. Cat Behaviour: The Predatory and Social Behaviour of Domestic and Wild Cats; Garland STPM Press:
New York, NY, USA, 1979.
60.
Childs, J.E. Size-dependent predation on rats (Rattus norvegicus) by house cats (Felis catus) in an urban setting.
J. Mammal. 1986,67, 196–199. [CrossRef]
61.
Glass, G.E.; Gardner-Santana, L.C.; Holt, R.D.; Chen, J.; Shields, T.M.; Roy, M.; Schachterle, S.; Klein, S.L.
Trophic garnishes: Cat–rat interactions in an urban environment. PLoS ONE 2009,4, e5794. [CrossRef]
62.
Williams, C.K.; Parer, I.; Coman, B.J.; Burley, J.; Braysher, M.L. Managing Vertebrate Pests: Rabbits; Bureau
of Resource Sciences/CSIRO Division of Wildlife and Ecology, Australian Government Publishing Service:
Canberra, Australia, 1995.
63.
Jones, E.; Coman, B.J. Ecology of the feral cat, Felis catus (L.), in south-eastern Australia I. Diet. Wildl. Res.
1981,8, 537–547. [CrossRef]
64.
Catling, P.C. Similarities and contrasts in the diets of foxes, Vulpes vulpes, and cats, Felis catus, relative to
fluctuating prey populations and drought. Aust. Wildl. Res. 1988,15, 307–317. [CrossRef]
65.
Hart, S. The Diet of Foxes (Vulpes vulpes) and Feral cats (Felis catus) in the Flinders Ranges National Park,
South Australia. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Adelaide, Adelaide, Australia, 1994.
66.
Grayson, J.; Calver, M.C. Regulation of domestic cat ownership to protect urban wildlife: A justification
based on the precautionary principle. In Urban Wildlife: More Than Meets the Eye; Lunney, D., Burgin, S., Eds.;
Royal Zoological Society of New South Wales: Mosman, Australia, 2004; pp. 169–178.
67.
Woinarski, J.C.Z.; Murphy, B.P.; Legge, S.M.; Garnett, S.T.; Lawes, M.J.; Comer, S.; Dickman, C.R.; Doherty, T.S.;
Edwards, G.; Nankivell, A. How many birds are killed by cats in Australia? Biol. Conserv.
2017
,214, 76–87.
[CrossRef]
Animals 2020,10, 707 22 of 23
68.
Woinarski, J.C.Z.; Woolley, L.A.; Garnett, S.T.; Legge, S.M.; Murphy, B.P.; Lawes, M.J.; Comer, S.; Dickman, C.R.;
Doherty, T.S.; Edwards, G. Compilation and traits of Australian bird species killed by cats. Biol. Conserv.
2017,216, 1–9. [CrossRef]
69.
Woinarski, J.C.Z.; Murphy, B.P.; Palmer, R.; Legge, S.M.; Dickman, C.R.; Doherty, T.S.; Edwards, G.;
Nankivell, A.; Read, J.L.; Stokeld, D. How many reptiles are killed by cats in Australia? Wildl. Res.
2018
,45,
247–266. [CrossRef]
70.
McGregor, H.; Legge, S.; Jones, M.E.; Johnson, C.N. Feral cats are better killers in open habitats, revealed by
animal-borne video. PLoS ONE 2015,10, e0133915. [CrossRef]
71.
Bonnaud, E.; Medina, F.M.; Vidal, E.; Nogales, M.; Tershy, B.; Zavaleta, E.; Donlan, C.J.; Keitt, B.; Le Corre, M.;
Horwath, S.V. The diet of feral cats on islands: A review and a call for more studies. Biol. Invasions
2011
,13,
581–603. [CrossRef]
72.
Woinarski, J.C.Z.; South, S.L.; Drummond, P.; Johnston, G.R.; Nankivell, A. The diet of the feral cat (Felis catus),
red fox (Vulpes vulpes) and dog (Canis familiaris) over a three-year period at Witchelina Reserve, in arid South
Australia. Aust. Mammal. 2018,40, 204–213. [CrossRef]
73.
Brooks, S.J.; Calver, M.C.; Dickman, C.R.; Meathrel, C.E.; Bradley, J.S. Does intraspecific variation in the
energy value of a prey species to its predators matter in studies of ecological energetics? A case study using
insectivorous vertebrates. Ecoscience 1996,3, 247–251.
74.
National Research Council. Nutrient Requirements of Cats. In Nutrient Requirements of Domestic Animals
Revised Edition; National Academy Press: Washington DC, USA, 1986.
75.
Dell’Arte, G.L.; Leonardi, G. The feeding choice of the Red Fox (Vulpes vulpes) in a semi-arid fragmented
landscape of North Africa in relation to water and energy contents of prey. Afr. J. Ecol. 2009,47, 729–736.
76.
Downer, R.G.H.; Matthews, J.R. Patterns of lipid distribution and utilisation in insects. Am. Zool.
1976
,16,
733–745.
77. Kouˇrimská, L.; Adámková, A. Nutritional and sensory quality of edible insects. NFS J. 2016,4, 22–26.
78. Peters, R.H. The Ecological Implications of Body Size; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1983.
79.
V
é
zina, A.F. Empirical relationships between predator and prey size in terrestrial vertebrates. Oecologia
1985
,
67, 555–565.
80.
Woolley, L.A.; Geyle, H.M.; Murphy, B.P.; Legge, S.M.; Palmer, R.; Dickman, C.R.; Augusteyn, J.; Comer, S.;
Doherty, T.S.; Eager, C.; et al. Introduced cats Felis catus eating a continental fauna: Inventory and traits of
Australian mammal species killed. Mammal Rev. 2019,49, 354–368.
81.
Fancourt, B.A. Making a killing: Photographic evidence of predation of a Tasmanian pademelon
(Thylogale billardierii) by a feral cat (Felis catus). Aust. Mammal. 2015,37, 120–124.
82.
Gibson, D.F.; Lundie-Jenkins, G.; Langford, D.G.; Cole, J.R.; Clarke, D.E.; Johnson, K.A. Predation by feral cats,
Felis catus, on the rufous hare-wallaby, Lagorchestes hirsutus, in the Tanami Desert. Aust. Mammal. 1994,17.
83.
Page, K.D.; Ruykys, L.; Miller, D.W.; Adams, P.J.; Bateman, P.W.; Fleming, P.A. Influences of behaviour and
physiology on body mass gain in the woylie (Bettongia penicillata ogilbyi) post-translocation. Wildl. Res.
2019
,
46, 429–443.
84. Spencer, P. Evidence of predation by a feral cat, Felis catus (Carnivora: Felidae) on an isolated rock-wallaby
colony in tropical Queensland. Aust. Mammal. 1991,14, 143–144.
85.
Horsup, A.; Evans, M. Predation by feral cats, Felis catus, on an endangered marsupial, the bridled nailtail
wallaby, Onychogalea fraenata.Aust. Mammal. 1993,16, 85–86.
86.
Fisher, D.O.; Blomberg, S.P.; Hoyle, S.D. Mechanisms of drought-induced population decline in an endangered
wallaby. Biol. Conserv. 2001,102, 107–115. [CrossRef]
87.
Hardman, B.; Moro, D.; Calver, M.C. Direct evidence implicates feral cat predation as the primary cause of
failure of a mammal reintroduction programme. Ecol. Manag. Restor. 2016,17, 152–158. [CrossRef]
88.
Marlow, N.J.; Thomas, N.D.; Williams, A.A.E.; Macmahon, B.; Lawson, J.; Hitchen, Y.; Angus, J.; Berry, O.
Cats (Felis catus) are more abundant and are the dominant predator of woylies (Bettongia penicillata) after
sustained fox (Vulpes vulpes) control. Aust. J. Zool. 2015,63, 18–27. [CrossRef]
89.
Gomes, V.; Carretero, M.A.; Kaliontzopoulou, A. Run for your life, but bite for your rights? How interactions
between natural and sexual selection shape functional morphology across habitats. Sci. Nat.
2018
,105, 9.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
90.
Husak, J.F. Does survival depend on how fast you can run or how fast you do run? Funct. Ecol.
2006
,20,
1080–1086. [CrossRef]
Animals 2020,10, 707 23 of 23
91.
Lappin, A.K.; Hamilton, P.S.; Sullivan, B.K. Bite-performance and head shape in a sexually dimorphic
crevice-dwelling lizard, the common chuckwalla [Sauromalus ater (=obesus)]. Biol. J. Linn. Soc.
2006
,88,
215–222. [CrossRef]
92.
Becerra, F.; Echeverr
í
a, A.I.; Marcos, A.; Casinos, A.; Vassallo, A.I. Sexual selection in a polygynous rodent
(Ctenomys talarum): An analysis of fighting capacity. Zoology 2012,115, 405–410. [CrossRef]
93.
Calver, M.C.; Adams, G.; Clark, W.; Pollock, K.H. Assessing the safety of collars used to attach predation
deterrent devices and ID tags to pet cats. Anim. Welf. 2013,22, 95–105. [CrossRef]
94.
Finkler, H.; Gunther, I.; Terkel, J. Behavioral dierences between urban feeding groups of neutered and
sexually intact free-roaming cats following a trap-neuter-return procedure. J. Am. Vet. Med Assoc.
2011
,238,
1141–1149. [CrossRef]
95.
Seidensticker, J.; Lumpkin, S. Cats in Question: The Smithsonian Answer Book; Smithsonian: Washington, DC,
USA, 2004.
96.
Bradshaw, J.W.S.; Casey, R.A.; Brown, S.L. Behavioural Development. In The Behaviour of the Domestic Cat,
2nd ed.; CABI Publishers: Oxfordshire, UK, 2012; pp. 63–90.
97.
Fitzgerald, B.M.; Turner, D.C. Hunting behaviour of domestic cats and their impact on prey populations.
In The Domestic Cat: The Biology of its Behaviour; Turner, D.C., Bateson, P., Eds.; Cambridge University Press:
Cambridge, UK, 2000; pp. 151–176.
98.
Ancillotto, L.; Serangeli, M.T.; Russo, D. Curiosity killed the bat: Domestic cats as bat predators.
Mamm. Biol.-Zeitschrift Säugetierkunde 2013,78, 369–373. [CrossRef]
99.
Risbey, D.A.; Calver, M.C.; Short, J. Control of feral cats for nature conservation. I. Field tests of four baiting
methods. Wildl. Res. 1997,24, 319–326. [CrossRef]
100.
Moseby, K.E.; Hill, B.M. The use of poison baits to control feral cats and red foxes in arid South Australia I.
Aerial baiting trials. Wildl. Res. 2011,38, 338–349. [CrossRef]
101.
Doherty, T.S.; Algar, D. Response of feral cats to a track-based baiting programme using Eradicat
®
baits.
Ecol. Manag. Restor. 2015,16, 124–130. [CrossRef]
102.
Caro, T.M. Eects of the mother, object play, and adult experience on predation in cats. Behav. Neural Biol.
1980,29, 29–51. [CrossRef]
103.
Deag, J.M.; Manning, A.; Lawrence, C.E. Factors influencing the mother-kitten relationship. In The Domestic
Cat: The Biology of its Behaviour, 2nd ed.; Turner, D.C., Bateson, P., Eds.; Cambridge University Press:
Cambridge, UK, 2000; pp. 23–45.
104.
Hilborn, A.; Pettorelli, N.; Orme, C.D.L.; Durant, S.M. Stalk and chase: How hunt stages aect hunting
success in Serengeti cheetah. Anim. Behav. 2012,84, 701–706. [CrossRef]
105.
Holekamp, K.E.; Smale, L.; Berg, R.; Cooper, S.M. Hunting rates and hunting success in the spotted hyena
(Crocuta crocuta). J. Zool. 1997,242, 1–15. [CrossRef]
106.
Rutz, C.; Whittingham, M.J.; Newton, I. Age-dependent diet choice in an avian top predator. Proc. R. Soc. B
Biol. Sci. 2006,273, 579–586. [CrossRef]
©
2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
... African wildcats would prefer pilfering eggs and chickens at night when poultry security by farmers is usually low. As male wild cats are often bigger than their female counterparts, they prey on relatively large mammals and birds [30], mainly targeting large healthy, productive hens in free-ranging or unprotected poultry facilities [31]. ...
... Male cats prefer larger birds to smaller ones [30] Time of the day Categorical Morning, afternoon, night-time Predators prefer attacking prey at particular time of the day The majority (74.3%, n = 74) of respondents reported that poultry predation in Chiawa GMA usually occurred in the night-time (Fig. 3). In a 3-year period (2020-2022), a total annual number of chickens lost by the 101 poultry farmers in the night-time was 1, 430 ± 0.1 SE, being 14.1 ± 0.1 per poultry farmer. ...
Article
Full-text available
African wildcats (Felis lybica) cause considerable poultry losses to poultry farmers in African rural areas through predation, and contribute to human food insecurity, loss of income and livelihood, and social well-being. Unable to manage poultry farmer-wildcat conflicts, the affected poultry farmers reduce their support to wildlife conservation and resort to retaliatory killings of African wildcats and other wildlife, mainly by secretly using poisons and traps. Such a human-carnivore conflict becomes protracted due to poor anti-predator responses implemented by the poultry farmers and wildlife management authorities. In this study, we used 101 semi-structured questionnaires to evaluate poultry predation by African wildcats occurring adjacent (approximately between 7 and 68 km) to Zambia’s Lower Zambezi National Park in Chiawa Game Management Area by focusing on local poultry farmer knowledge, attitudes and practices. The study’s Generalized Linear Model showed that season (i.e., hot-wet season), time (i.e., night) and countermeasures (i.e., absence of wire fences) were the primary predictors of poultry predation by African wildcats, while low tree cover and education attained by the poultry farmers were the secondary factors. The rate of chicken loss per year/poultry farmer was 19.1 ± 1.3 SE. We suggest strengthening farmer-based countermeasures, such as small-mesh fencing to protect poultry from African wildcats, and sensitisation programmes by wildlife managers for poultry farmer transformation towards supporting conservation alongside conservation planning in these human-dominated-wildlife landscapes for improved farmer-African wildcat co-existence, particularly in the hot-wet season and night-time when the poultry losses from attacks by African wildcats spike.
... Further, we hypothesised that cats survive better in more productive landscapes: cat home-ranges are larger in landscapes with lower productivity (Bengsen et al. 2015;Nottingham et al. 2022). We hypothesised that larger cats survive better, as they can hunt a greater range of prey sizes ; although see Fleming et al. 2020). Similarly, we hypothesised that larger male cats may move further due to their polygynous mating system and feral cats moving longer distances in landscapes with low to moderate level of resources (Bengsen et al. 2015;McGregor et al. 2015;Mirmovitch 1995;Say and Pontier 2004). ...
... A growing body of evidence also shows that the individual traits of feral cats, particularly body mass, can influence their hunting behaviour and prey size (Dickman and Newsome 2015;Kutt 2012;Moseby et al. 2020). Note, however that Fleming et al. (2020) found that while older cats might pose a greater risk to large and 'dangerous' prey, small cats were also capable of hunting big prey. Nonetheless, their greater hunting experience and ability to handle larger prey may enhance the survival rate of heavier cats. ...
Article
Full-text available
Feral cats (Felis catus) pose a significant global threat to biodiversity, primarily through predation, disease and competition. A key gap in parameterizing models for improving management decisions for feral cat control relates to factors that drive feral cat survival and movement in the wild. Our study objective was to conduct the first continental-scale analysis of survival rates and displacement distances for feral cats. We collated data on 528 feral cats from telemetry studies in naturally-vegetated landscapes across Australia. Using Cox-proportional hazards models, we investigated the effects of sex, presence of larger predators (dingoes, Canis familiaris and introduced foxes, Vulpes vulpes), presence of introduced prey (rabbits, Oryctolagus cuniculus), body mass, landscape productivity and feral cat density on feral cat survival. We also analysed the effects of sex, body mass and landscape productivity on feral cat displacement using linear mixed model analysis. Feral cat survival was positively associated with presence of dingoes and increasing body mass, whereas there was no clear association between feral cat survival and sex, presence of rabbits, or cat density. Presence of foxes had a strong negative effect on feral cat survival, but the hazard ratio was associated with considerable uncertainty. Net displacement of male feral cats was nearly two times further than that of females, and the proportion of feral cats making long-distance movements was greater in landscapes with low productivity. Increasing body mass of feral cats was positively related to net displacement, with heavier cats moving further. Analysis of metadata from telemetry studies can provide valuable insights into wildlife survival rates and movement behaviour. Our findings will help inform the development of effective management strategies and improve feral cat management for biodiversity conservation.
... While behaviour, diet, and neutered status can all affect the weight of domesticated cats (Nguyen et al., 2004), the default clinical expectation for a pet cat is to weigh approximately 4 kg, and between 3.5 and 4.5 kg, this being consistent across most domesticated breeds in good health. Feral cats are typically slightly smaller than pet cats and weigh between 2.8 and 4.0 kg on average (Fleming et al., 2020;Scott et al., 2002). Due to our approximation of mean mass being similar to the 3.75 kg value used for the mass of an individual cat by Greenspoon et al. (2023) in their global assessment, we define the global mean mass of a cat as 3.75 ± 1.0 kg in this study for consistency between studies. ...
Article
Full-text available
Domesticated livestock and their waste streams are considered a significant source of nitrogen (N) and phosphorus (P) pollution at the global scale; however, the waste generated (excreta) by domesticated cats and dogs, whose global numbers are estimated at 700 million and 900 million, respectively, is not included in any global inventories or models of N and P pollution sources. Based on parameters derived from a variety of literature sources, this study estimates the total global N and P excretion from domestic cats and dogs to be 4.32 (1.27–7.38) Tg N yr⁻¹ and 0.76 (0.31–1.21) Tg P yr⁻¹ which are equivalent to 3.3 (1.0–5.7)% of N and 3.3 (1.3–5.3)% of P waste produced by livestock at a global level. These estimates are in line with the combined mass of the animals (the total mass of cats and dogs is equivalent to 3.6% of the total mass of domesticated mammalian livestock). While there is a severe under reporting of waste streams for cat and dog waste deposition in literature, we infer from our estimates that global emissions of N2O and NH3 from cat and dog waste are in the region of 43 (13–74) Gg N2O–N yr⁻¹ and 864 ± 654 Gg NH3–N, representing an unreported contribution that may exceed 17.7% of the carbon footprint associated with global pet food production (in the form of N2O emissions).
... However, cats are ambush predators that mostly move slow and careful, and they often use elevated vantage points. 37 Their anatomy is highly specialized for climbing, jumping, and even for avoiding the fatal consequences of falling from considerable heights. 38 As they do not have functional collarbones, they show remarkable adaptation for squeezing through narrow gaps. ...
Article
Full-text available
Various animal species can make a priori decisions about the passability of openings, based on their own size knowledge. So far no one has tested the ability for self-representation in cats. We hypothesized that cats may rely on their size awareness when they have to negotiate small openings. Companion cats (N = 30) were tested with incrementally decreasing sized openings, which were either the same height, or the same width. Cats approached and entered even the narrowest openings, but they slowed down before reaching, and while passing through the shortest ones. Because of their specific anatomical features and cautious locomotory strategy, cats readily opt for the trial-and-error method to negotiate narrow apertures, but they seemingly rely on their body-size representing capacity in the case of uncomfortably short openings. Ecologically valid methodologies can provide answers in the future as to whether cats would rely on their body awareness in other challenging spatial tasks.
... Alternatively, the release of mesopredators might not be uniform across leopard cat demographics resulting in older/larger individuals persisting longer. In a parallel case involving domestic cats in Australia anecdotal evidence suggested these traits permit predation of "dangerous prey" including brushtailed possum (Trichosurus vulpecula), black-headed monitor (Varanus tristis), and domestic chicken (Gallus gallus domesticus) [48]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Amongst the unintended consequences of anthropogenic landscape conversion is declining apex predator abundance linked to loss of forest integrity, which can potentially re-order trophic networks. One such re-ordering, known as mesopredator release, occurs when medium-sized predators, also called mesopredators, rapidly increase in abundance following the decline in apex predator abundance, consequently reducing the abundance of mesopredator prey, notably including terrestrial avifauna. We examine the cascading impacts of declining Sunda clouded leopard abundance, itself consequent upon a reduction in forest integrity, on the mesopredator community of Sabah, Malaysia, to determine whether the phenomenon of mesopredator release is manifest and specifically whether it impacts the terrestrial avifauna community of pheasants and pittas. To explore this trophic interaction, we used a piecewise structural equation model to compare changes in the relative abundance of organisms. Our results suggest that loss of forest integrity may have broad impacts on the community and trigger mesopredator release, the two acting additively in their impact on already vulnerable species of terrestrial avifauna: a result not previously documented in tropical systems and rarely detected even on a global scale. The limiting effect that the Sunda clouded leopard has on the Sunda leopard cat could illuminate the mechanism whereby mesopredator release impacts this system. Both Bulwer's pheasant and pittas appear to be significantly impacted by the increase in Sunda leopard cats, while the great argus pheasant shows similar compelling, although not statistically significant, declines as Sunda leopard cats increase. The inverse relationship between Sunda clouded leopards and Sunda leopard cats suggests that if a mesopredator release exists it could have downstream consequences for some terrestrial avifauna. These results suggest the under-studied interface between mammalian carnivores and avifauna, or more broadly species interactions in general, could offer important conservation tool for holistic ecosystem conservation efforts.
... This may occur because juvenile rabbits are less abundant in late summer, because rabbits breed from late winter to early summer in the study area (Bowen and Read 1998). Small juvenile rabbits are likely to be easier for smaller nonbreeding female cats and/or younger inexperienced cats to hunt, because smaller cats may be less effective at catching larger prey (Read and Bowen 2001;Fleming et al. 2020). This is supported by Moseby et al. (2021), who found a significant interaction between bodyweight and temperature on rabbit consumption by feral cats, with smaller cats feeding on rabbits only in the cooler months whereas larger cats fed on rabbits regardless of temperature. ...
Article
Full-text available
Context Animal-borne devices can affect animal survival, reproduction, and behaviour through both the addition of weight and bulk and the direct effects of initial and subsequent capture. Researchers commonly employ a general rule of thumb that weight of the device must be less than 5% of bodyweight for terrestrial animals; however, this threshold has little empirical basis. Aims We evaluated the effects of environmental variables, repeated capture, and weight of animal-borne devices on bodyweight in free-ranging feral cats. Methods We recaptured feral cats at varying frequencies, wearing GPS and/or VHF collars that ranged from 0.29% to 4.88% of bodyweight, and recorded change in cat weight over time. Key results Collar weight as a percentage of bodyweight was not a significant predictor of feral cat weight change. Rather, change in bodyweight was best described by a negative relationship with an increasing temperature and number of captures, and a positive relationship with time since collar attachment. Conclusions Capture had a significant influence on feral cat weight but collar weights up to 5% of bodyweight did not significantly contribute to weight loss. However, the absence of control cats without collars hindered definitive conclusions on the effect of collar weight on cat weight change. Implications Researchers should space capture and handling events more than 30 days apart to reduce effects of weight loss from capture and handling. Researchers should also consider increasing collar weight and reducing frequency of capture (where collars are less than 5% of bodyweight), particularly if cat bodyweight is a parameter of interest.
... 27 The weights required to create a 200-N bite force using the pulley setup was 35 kg, which resulted in a bite force of 150 to 250 N, depending on skull anatomy. In cats, the target bite force was 63 N for one side, representing one-third of a maximal bite force of 190 N. 28 The hanging weights used to achieve 63 N were 15.5 kg, which resulted in a bite force of 60 to 70 N for one side of the mandible among cat skulls. For mouth opening, the force of the digastricus muscle is 60% of that of the masseter muscle, the masseter muscle accounting for about 35% of the total bite force. ...
Article
Full-text available
OBJECTIVE To evaluate the kinematics and stability of the temporomandibular joint (TMJ) of cats and dogs with and without a TMJ replacement (TMJR) prosthesis under simulated bite forces and mouth opening. ANIMALS Sixteen cadaver skulls from domestic cats (n = 8) and medium- to large-breed dogs (n = 8). METHODS Intact TMJs were tested. Following condylectomy and coronoidectomy, the skulls were fitted with a TMJR prosthesis unilaterally and retested. Prosthesis was similarly implanted in the contralateral TMJ in 4 cats and 4 dogs before retesting. Left and right bite motions were evaluated before bite contact to peak bite force (200 N in dogs, 63 N in cats). Mouth opening motion was recorded. Mandibular displacement under load was evaluated in 3 orthogonal planes. Maximal displacement was compared between TMJR groups and native TMJ. Prosthesis-bone motion of the temporal and mandibular components was evaluated during simulated bites and mouth opening. RESULTS TMJR resulted in joint motion not demonstrably different from the native TMJ, with the ability to fully open and close the mouth and with minimal laterotrusion. The TMJR prosthesis demonstrated similar stability after unilateral and bilateral replacement during bite force and with an open mouth. Mean implant-bone motion during bite simulations for the temporal and mandibular TMJR components was ≤ 60 µm in cats and ≤ 30 µm in dogs. CLINICAL RELEVANCE A novel TMJR can be implanted and allows normal jaw motion. Joint stability is maintained after TMJR implantation in the TMJ of dogs and cats TMJ that is devoid of muscular support.
... Secondly, domestic cat management should also include elements that help reduce the potential impact of domestic cats on biodiversity. As an opportunistic predator, domestic cats prey not only on synanthropic, potentially invasive species, but also on a variety of other small wild animals, both vertebrates and invertebrates (Loss et al., 2013;Parsons et al., 2018;Fleming et al., 2020;Loss et al., 2022), and thus have some impact on wildlife and its diversity. Indeed, domestic cats have been shown to be individually specialised predators, with some cats showing stable prey preferences for particular taxa, such that not all individuals are equally "problematic" (Dickman and Newsome, 2015;Moseby et al., 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Among domestic animals, the domestic cat (Felis catus) probably has the highest proportion of free-ranging individuals in the population and as such is an important component (i.e. predator) in ecosystems. Due to their potential impact on wildlife, the management of domestic cats is a hot topic today. However, there are few studies on the chronobiology and activity of domestic cats in relation to natural cycles. Therefore, the aim of our research was to investigate their circadian, lunar and seasonal rhythms. We monitored the activity of free-ranging cats in a selected corridor within an approximately 800 m2 patch of shrubs and trees in an agricultural landscape that is regularly used by wildlife in the surrounding area. Monitoring was carried out for two consecutive years using a trail camera. Data collection was based on video analysis of all recorded video clips (n = 2081 from 732 camera trap nights). A total of at least 15 individuals were identified in the area. Cats were most frequently observed in spring and summer (a total of 70% of all observations). Cats with prey were mostly observed in summer (∼56%) and never in winter. Circadian activity of domestic cats was nocturnal/crepuscular (p < 0.0001) with two peaks of activity, one in the late evening (∼21.00 h) and another in the early morning (∼5.00 h). The appearance of the cats with the prey corresponded to the general circadian activity. Slight shifts in activity due to day length (sunset/sunrise) were observed when comparing circadian rhythms in relation to seasons, especially in winter when there was a pronounced peak in activity in the early evening. For cat activity in relation to the lunar cycle, we found a tendency for higher nocturnal activity around new moon (p = 0.065), with this pattern being significant in spring (p < 0.05). However, the appearance of cats with prey was not related to the lunar cycle. Understanding the circadian, lunar and seasonal activity rhythms of domestic cats is an important prerequisite for developing an optimal cat management strategy that takes into account both welfare aspects (especially natural behaviour) and minimising the impact of domestic cats on wildlife. _____**Full text temporarily available (50 days - until 20 January 2024) at the following address: https://authors.elsevier.com/a/1i0B9cF2OojKg
Book
Full-text available
Antik kökenleri tam olarak bilmesek de kediyle olan etkileşimimizin en az 9 500 yıl önce başladığı düşünülmektedir. Bu uzun süreçte ne yazık ki onu olması gerektiği kadar tanıyabilmiş değiliz. En önemlisi de bunca yıla rağmen kedi bizim için hala bir çok yönüyle gizemini koruyan öngörülemez bir hayvandır. Bunca yıla rağmen onunla iletişim kurmakta hala zorlanıyoruz. Kendi isteğiyle evcilleşmeye evet diyen ve her geçen gün evlerimizde daha çok yer edinen onuna ilgilenmemize ancak kendi isteği olduğunda izin veren bu canlının insanlık tarihinde bıraktığı önemli izler vardır. Tarihsel süreçte kimi zaman şeytanla özdeşleştirilen kimi zaman da tanrısal nitelikler atfedilen kediyi, ve onun özelliklerini, ihtiyaçlarını bilimsel anlamda araştırmaya çok geç başladık. Türkçe kaynaklar üzerinde yapılacak bir çalışmada çiftlik hayvanları ile ilgili onca bilimsel makale ve kitap varken kedilerle ilgili bilimsel makale ve kitapların yok denecek kadar az olduğu hemen görülecektir. İnternet üzerinden yapılacak araştırmalarda kedilerle ilgili yetiştirme ve beslemeye dair bilgilerin popüler ev hayvanı sitelerinde verilen oldukça yüzeysel ve dar kapsamlı bilgileri kapsadığı, akademik çalışmaların ise çok birkaç makaleden ibaret olduğu görülmektedir. Bu kitabın yazılma amacı, kedilerin beslenmesine dair geniş kapsamlı bilimsel bilginin Türkçe bir kitapta toplanmasıdır. Kitabın incelenmesinde de görüleceği gibi kitap, kedilerin tüm yaşam süreçleri içerisinde beslenmelerine ilişkin kapsamlı bilgi sunmaktadır. Kitap hem hesaplamalar ve hem de tablolarla okuyucularına kedi beslemeye ait veriler sunarken aynı zamanda beslemeye bağlık hastalıklar, kedilerin evciltme süreci, kedi ırkları, kedilerin kişilik yapıları, kedilerin çiftleşme davranışı, kedilerin iletişim araçları ile kedilerin çevre ve barınma istekleri ile kedilerde stres kaynakları üzerinde de okuyucuya farklı başlıklar altında bilimsel kitap ve makalelerden önemli bilgiler sunmaktadır. Kitap, akademik literatüre katkı yapmayı amaçlayarak yazılmış olsa da gerek ziraat fakültelerinin zootekni bölümü öğrencileri gerekse de veteriner fakültesi öğrencileri için de önemli bir bilgi kaynağı olacak niteliktedir.
Article
Full-text available
Defining species within the Canidae family is challenging due to morphological convergence, behavioral plasticity, traditional taxonomic limitations, and possible hybridisation. This challenge is timely with the recent population and range expansion of the golden jackal ( Canis aureus ). Exploring their morphological data and sexual dimorphism is essential for identifying factors driving their success in new habitats. The proven hybridization of golden jackals with dogs and wolves may affect species description, population dynamics, and genetic diversity, impacting conservation strategies. This study, for the first time, conducts a morphometric analysis of golden jackals in Somogy County, Hungary, to prove sexual size dimorphism (SSD) in body and skull and sexual shape dimorphism (SShD) in skull across juvenile and adult age groups. 719 golden jackals (362 females and 357 males) were collected between January 2021 and January 2023. Descriptive statistics revealed significant SSD in body and skull measurements among both age groups, with males generally larger than females, particularly in body mass (11.72% in juveniles and 13.37% in adults). Most skull dimensions differed significantly between sexes and age groups, except for foramen magnum height, foramen magnum width, and postorbital breadth among juveniles and foramen magnum height and postorbital breadth among adults. We used principal component analyses (PCA) on raw dimension data and the log shape ratio method to extract shape information. Linear discriminant analysis (LDA) explored skull SShD between sexes. Notably, our study achieved over 71% accuracy in sex classification, illustrating the clear presence of SShD of the skull in golden jackals across both age groups. Our study provides a comprehensive database of golden jackals in the overpopulated Hungarian habitat, which will be helpful for further research on ecology, behavior, and conservation management.
Article
Full-text available
Domestic cats (Felis catus) are one of the most widely distributed and successful carnivores globally. While cats are popular pets, many unowned, ‘stray’ cats live freely in anthropogenic environments at high densities where they make use of anthropogenic resources. These stray cats present a management challenge due to concerns about wildlife predation, pathogen transmission, public nuisance and threats to cat welfare (e.g. vehicle collisions). In Australia, there are few studies of strays compared with pet cats or feral cats (free-roaming cats in rural areas that are independent of resources provided by humans). To contribute original data about stray cat biology, the carcasses of 188 euthanised stray cats were collected from Perth, Western Australia. Cats were assessed for general health, age, reproduction, diet and gastrointestinal parasite biomass. The influence of cat demographics, collection location, season, parasite biomass, diet and history of supplemental feeding by people were tested against body condition. Overall, strays were physically healthy and reproductive, with few life-threatening injuries or macroscopic evidence of disease; however, helminths were extremely common (95% of cats) and pose a threat. Nearly 40% of strays consumed wildlife, including two species of endemic marsupial. Alarmingly, 57.5% of strays were scavenging vast amounts of refuse, including life-threatening items in volumes that blocked their gastrointestinal tracts. These findings illustrate that strays need to be removed from anthropogenic environments for their own health and welfare and to prevent continued breeding. Targeted control programmes should prioritise removal of cats from areas where refuse is common and where valued native fauna exist.
Article
Full-text available
Hybrid cats—created by crossing different species within the family Felidae—are popular pets, but they could potentially threaten native species if they escape and establish free-roaming populations. To forestall this possibility, the Australian government imposed a specific ban on importation of the savannah cat, a hybrid created by crossing the domestic cat Felis catus and serval Leptailurus serval, in 2008. We develop a decision–framework that identifies those species of non-volant native mammals in Australia that would likely have been susceptible to predation by savannah cats if importation and establishment had occurred. We assumed that savannah cats would hunt ecologically similar prey to those that are depredated by both the domestic cat and the serval, and categorised native mammals as having different levels of susceptibility to predation by savannah cats based on their size, habitat range, and behaviour. Using this framework, we assessed savannah cats as likely to add at least 28 extant native mammal species to the 168 that are known already to be susceptible to predation by the domestic cat, posing a risk to 91% of Australia’s extant non-volant terrestrial mammal species (n = 216) and to 93% of threatened mammal species. The framework could be generalised to assess risks from any other hybrid taxa.
Article
Full-text available
Context Temperament can affect an individual’s fitness and survival if it also influences behaviours associated with predator avoidance, interactions with conspecifics, refuge selection and/or foraging. Furthermore, temperament can determine an individual’s response to novel stimuli and environmental challenges, such as those experienced through translocation. Increasing our understanding of the effect of temperament on post-translocation fitness is thus necessary for improving translocation outcomes. Aims The aim was to test whether differences in an individual’s behaviour or physiology could help predict body mass changes post-translocation in the woylie (brush-tailed bettong, Bettongia penicillata ogilbyi). In the absence of predation (due to release into a predator-free exclosure), body mass was used as a proxy for an individual’s success in securing resources in the new habitat, and therefore fitness. Methods Forty woylies were translocated from two predator-free exclosures to a larger exclosure, all in Western Australia. Behavioural and physiological measures were recorded during trapping, processing, holding, and release, and again at re-capture ~100 days post-release. Key results Translocated woylies generally increased in body mass post-translocation. This suggests that, in the absence of predation, the selected candidates were able to cope with the stress of translocation and possessed the behavioural plasticity to successfully find resources and adapt to a novel environment. The strongest predictors of body mass gain were sex, heart rate lability and escape behaviour when released (a convoluted escape path). Conclusions There was no significant difference in body mass between males and females pre-translocation but females showed greater mass gain post-translocation than did males, which could reflect greater investment in reproduction (all females had pouch young). Heart rate lability and escape behaviour are likely to reflect reactivity or fearfulness, a significant temperament trait in the context of translocation success. Implications Behavioural measures that can be easily incorporated into the translocation process – without increasing stress or affecting welfare of individuals – may hold promise for predicting the fate of translocated animals.
Article
Full-text available
Context: Over the last 230 years, the Australian terrestrial mammal fauna has suffered a very high rate of decline and extinction relative to other continents. Predation by the introduced red fox (Vulpes vulpes) and feral cat (Felis catus) is implicated in many of these extinctions, and in the ongoing decline of many extant species. Aims: To assess the degree to which Australian terrestrial non-volant mammal species are susceptible at the population level to predation by the red fox and feral cat, and to allocate each species to a category of predator susceptibility. Methods: We collated the available evidence and complemented this with expert opinion to categorise each Australian terrestrial non-volant mammal species (extinct and extant) into one of four classes of population-level susceptibility to introduced predators (i.e. 'extreme', 'high', 'low' or 'not susceptible'). We then compared predator susceptibility with conservation status, body size and extent of arboreality; and assessed changes in the occurrence of species in different predator-susceptibility categories between 1788 and 2017. Key results: Of 246 Australian terrestrial non-volant mammal species (including extinct species), we conclude that 37 species are (or were) extremely predator-susceptible; 52 species are highly predator-susceptible; 112 species are of low susceptibility; and 42 species are not susceptible to predators. Confidence in assigning species to predator-susceptibility categories was strongest for extant threatened mammal species and for extremely predator-susceptible species. Extinct and threatened mammal species are more likely to be predator-susceptible than Least Concern species; arboreal species are less predator-susceptible than ground-dwelling species; and medium-sized species (35 g-3.5 kg) are more predator-susceptible than smaller or larger species. Conclusions: The effective control of foxes and cats over large areas is likely to assist the population-level recovery of ∼63 species-the number of extant species with extreme or high predator susceptibility-which represents ∼29% of the extant Australian terrestrial non-volant mammal fauna. Implications: Categorisation of predator susceptibility is an important tool for conservation management, because the persistence of species with extreme susceptibility will require intensive management (e.g. predator-proof exclosures or predator-free islands), whereas species of lower predator susceptibility can be managed through effective landscape-level suppression of introduced predators.
Chapter
The second edition of this book contains 12 chapters that discuss the domestication and general biology (anatomy and physiology), senses, behaviour mechanisms, behavioural development, communication, feeding behaviour, hunting and predatory behaviours, social behaviour, relationship with humans, welfare, abnormal/unwanted behaviour and the physiological and pathological causes of behavioural changes. The book contains some tables, black and white illustrations, a list of references for further reading and an index. It will be of use to those with an interest in cat behaviour.
Chapter
The second edition of this book contains 12 chapters that discuss the domestication and general biology (anatomy and physiology), senses, behaviour mechanisms, behavioural development, communication, feeding behaviour, hunting and predatory behaviours, social behaviour, relationship with humans, welfare, abnormal/unwanted behaviour and the physiological and pathological causes of behavioural changes. The book contains some tables, black and white illustrations, a list of references for further reading and an index. It will be of use to those with an interest in cat behaviour.
Article
Mammals comprise the bulk of the diet of free‐ranging domestic cats Felis catus (defined as including outdoor pet cats, strays, and feral cats) in most parts of their global range. In Australia, predation by introduced feral cats has been implicated in the extinction of many mammal species, and in the ongoing decline of many extant species. Here, we collate a wide range of records of predation by cats (including feral and pet cats) on Australian mammals and model traits of extant, terrestrial, native mammal species associated with the relative likelihood of cat predation. We explicitly seek to overcome biases in such a continental‐scale compilation by excluding possible carrion records for larger species and accounting for differences in the distribution and abundance of potential prey species, as well as study effort, throughout each species’ range. For non‐volant species, the relative likelihood of predation by cats was greatest for species in an intermediate weight range (peaking at ca. 400 g), in lower rainfall areas and not dwelling in rocky habitats. Previous studies have shown the greatest rates of decline and extinction in Australian mammals to be associated with these traits. As such, we provide the first continental‐scale link between mammal decline and cat predation through quantitative analysis. Our compilation of cat predation records for most extant, terrestrial, native mammal species (151 species, or 52% of the Australian species’ complement) is substantially greater than previously reported (88 species) and includes 50 species listed as threatened by the IUCN or under Australian legislation (57% of Australia's 87 threatened terrestrial mammal species). We identify the Australian mammal species most likely to be threatened by predation by cats (mulgaras Dasycercus spp., kowari Dasyuroides byrnei , many smaller dasyurids and medium‐sized to large rodents, among others) and hence most likely to benefit from enhanced mitigation of cat impacts, such as translocations to predator‐free islands, the establishment of predator‐proof fenced exclosures, and broad‐scale cat poison baiting.
Book
The Action Plan for Australian Mammals 2012 is the first review to assess the conservation status of all Australian mammals. It complements The Action Plan for Australian Birds 2010 (Garnett et al. 2011, CSIRO Publishing), and although the number of Australian mammal taxa is marginally fewer than for birds, the proportion of endemic, extinct and threatened mammal taxa is far greater. These authoritative reviews represent an important foundation for understanding the current status, fate and future of the nature of Australia. This book considers all species and subspecies of Australian mammals, including those of external territories and territorial seas. For all the mammal taxa (about 300 species and subspecies) considered Extinct, Threatened, Near Threatened or Data Deficient, the size and trend of their population is presented along with information on geographic range and trend, and relevant biological and ecological data. The book also presents the current conservation status of each taxon under Australian legislation, what additional information is needed for managers, and the required management actions. Recovery plans, where they exist, are evaluated. The voluntary participation of more than 200 mammal experts has ensured that the conservation status and information are as accurate as possible, and allowed considerable unpublished data to be included. All accounts include maps based on the latest data from Australian state and territory agencies, from published scientific literature and other sources. The Action Plan concludes that 29 Australian mammal species have become extinct and 63 species are threatened and require urgent conservation action. However, it also shows that, where guided by sound knowledge, management capability and resourcing, and longer-term commitment, there have been some notable conservation success stories, and the conservation status of some species has greatly improved over the past few decades. The Action Plan for Australian Mammals 2012 makes a major contribution to the conservation of a wonderful legacy that is a significant part of Australia’s heritage. For such a legacy to endure, our society must be more aware of and empathetic with our distinctively Australian environment, and particularly its marvellous mammal fauna; relevant information must be readily accessible; environmental policy and law must be based on sound evidence; those with responsibility for environmental management must be aware of what priority actions they should take; the urgency for action (and consequences of inaction) must be clear; and the opportunity for hope and success must be recognised. It is in this spirit that this account is offered. Winner of a 2015 Whitley Awards Certificate of Commendation for Zoological Resource.