ArticlePDF Available

Impact Protection Potential of Mammalian Hair: Testing the Pugilism Hypothesis for the Evolution of Human Facial Hair

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Because facial hair is one of the most sexually dimorphic features of humans (Homo sapiens) and is often perceived as an indicator of masculinity and social dominance, human facial hair has been suggested to play a role in male contest competition. Some authors have proposed that the beard may function similar to the long hair of a lion’s mane, serving to protect vital areas like the throat and jaw from lethal attacks. This is consistent with the observation that the mandible, which is superficially covered by the beard, is one of the most commonly fractured facial bones in interpersonal violence. We hypothesized that beards protect the skin and bones of the face when human males fight by absorbing and dispersing the energy of a blunt impact. We tested this hypothesis by measuring impact force and energy absorbed by a fiber epoxy composite, which served as a bone analog, when it was covered with skin that had thick hair (referred to here as “furred”) versus skin with no hair (referred to here as “sheared” and “plucked”). We covered the epoxy composite with segments of skin dissected from domestic sheep (Ovis aries), and used a drop weight impact tester affixed with a load cell to collect force versus time data. Tissue samples were prepared in three conditions: furred (n = 20), plucked (n = 20), and sheared (n = 20). We found that fully furred samples were capable of absorbing more energy than plucked and sheared samples. For example, peak force was 16% greater and total energy absorbed was 37% greater in the furred compared to the plucked samples. These differences were due in part to a longer time frame of force delivery in the furred samples. These data support the hypothesis that human beards protect vulnerable regions of the facial skeleton from damaging strikes.
Content may be subject to copyright.
A Journal of the Society
for Integrative and
Comparative Biology
Organismal
Integrative
Biology
academic.oup.com/icb
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/iob/article-abstract/2/1/obaa005/5799080 by guest on 16 April 2020
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Impact Protection Potential of Mammalian Hair: Testing the
Pugilism Hypothesis for the Evolution of Human Facial Hair
E. A. Beseris,
*
S. E. Naleway,
and D. R. Carrier
1,*
*Department of Biology, University of Utah, 257 S. 1400 E, Salt Lake City, UT 84112, USA;
Department of Mechanical
Engineering, University of Utah, 100 S. 1495 E, Salt Lake City, UT 84112, USA
1
E-mail: carrier@biology.utah.edu
Synopsis Because facial hair is one of the most sexually dimorphic features of humans (Homo sapiens) and is often
perceived as an indicator of masculinity and social dominance, human facial hair has been suggested to play a role in
male contest competition. Some authors have proposed that the beard may function similar to the long hair of a lion’s
mane, serving to protect vital areas like the throat and jaw from lethal attacks. This is consistent with the observation
that the mandible, which is superficially covered by the beard, is one of the most commonly fractured facial bones in
interpersonal violence. We hypothesized that beards protect the skin and bones of the face when human males fight by
absorbing and dispersing the energy of a blunt impact. We tested this hypothesis by measuring impact force and energy
absorbed by a fiber epoxy composite, which served as a bone analog, when it was covered with skin that had thick hair
(referred to here as “furred”) versus skin with no hair (referred to here as “sheared” and “plucked”). We covered the
epoxy composite with segments of skin dissected from domestic sheep (Ovis aries), and used a drop weight impact tester
affixed with a load cell to collect force versus time data. Tissue samples were prepared in three conditions: furred
(n¼20), plucked (n¼20), and sheared (n¼20). We found that fully furred samples were capable of absorbing more
energy than plucked and sheared samples. For example, peak force was 16% greater and total energy absorbed was 37%
greater in the furred compared to the plucked samples. These differences were due in part to a longer time frame of force
delivery in the furred samples. These data support the hypothesis that human beards protect vulnerable regions of the
facial skeleton from damaging strikes.
Introduction
As is the case in other species of great apes, human
males perpetrate the vast majority of violence and
most of these acts of aggression are directed at other
males (Adams 1983;Chagnon 1988;Daly and Wilson
1988;Keeley 1996;Wrangham and Peterson 1996;
Walker 2001;Puts 2010;Ellis et al. 2013;Hill et al.
2016). When human males fight hand-to-hand, the
face is usually the primary target (Carrier and
Morgan 2015). Consequently, it is not surprising
that human males suffer substantially more injuries
to the face from interpersonal violence than do
females. Epidemiology studies of interpersonal vio-
lence indicate that males suffer 68–92% more inju-
ries to the face from fights than do females
(Shepherd et al. 1990;Bostrom 1997;Brink et al.
1998;Simsek et al. 2007;Czerwinski et al. 2008;
Lee 2009;Allareddy et al. 2011;Suh and Kim 2012).
Because sexual dimorphism is often greatest in
those phenotypes that enhance a male’s capacity to
dominate other males (Clutton-Brock and Harvey
2009;Parker 1983;Andersson 1994), it is not sur-
prising that the facial bones which suffer the highest
rates of fracture from interpersonal violence are the
parts of the skull that exhibit the greatest sexual di-
morphism in both modern humans and early hom-
inins (i.e., bipedal apes; Carrier and Morgan 2015).
From the perspective of sexual selection, it is reason-
able to suspect that these dimorphic facial features
emerged as a result of male–male contest competi-
tion, and act to protect the face against damaging
strikes (Puts 2010;Stirrat et al. 2012;Carrier and
Morgan 2015;Puts et al. 2015;Puts 2016).
Consistent with this suggestion is the observations
that masculine facial structure is correlated with
greater upper body strength (Fink et al. 2007;Sell
ßThe Author(s) 2020. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Society for Integrative and Comparative Biology.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Integrative Organismal Biology
Integrative Organismal Biology, pp. 1–12
doi:10.1093/iob/obaa005 A Journal of the Society for Integrative and Comparative Biology
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/iob/article-abstract/2/1/obaa005/5799080 by guest on 16 April 2020
et al. 2009;Windhager et al. 2011), aggressive behav-
ior (Carr
e and McCormick 2008;Carr
e et al. 2009,
2010;T
rebicky et al. 2013), social dominance
(Muller and Mazur 1997;Geniole et al. 2015), and
reproductive success (Mazur et al. 1994).
Another trait that exhibits pronounced sexual di-
morphism in humans is facial hair (Darwin 1871;
Dixson et al. 2018). Among our closest relatives,
the African apes (chimps, bonobos, and gorillas),
facial hair is equally prominent in males and females
(Darwin 1871). Relative to the African apes, human
females have significantly reduced facial hair,
whereas at puberty human males develop continu-
ously growing hair that covers the front of the upper
jaw (mustache) and the anterior neck and lower jaw
(beard; Darwin 1871;Dixson et al. 2005;Dixson
et al. 2017). As is true for masculine skeletal features,
men with full beards are reportedly perceived as be-
ing more masculine, socially dominant, and behav-
iorally aggressive in comparison to clean-shaven men
(Neave and Shields 2008;Dixson and Vasey 2012;
Dixson and Brooks 2013;Saxton et al. 2016;
Sherlock et al. 2017;T
rebicky et al. 2019). In addi-
tion, human male facial hair has been shown to pos-
itively impact mating success in highly competitive
environments (Barber 2001;Dixson et al., 2017).
Some authors suggest these relationships are due
to facial hair enhancing the size and appearance of
the sexually dimorphic regions of the face—most
notably the mandible and maxilla (Guthrie 1970;
Muscarella and Cunningham 1996;Neave and
Shields 2008;Dixson et al. 2017,2018). Others
have proposed that the beard actually serves to pro-
tect the throat and jaw during fighting (Blanchard
2010). In this context, the mane of male lions offers
an intriguing possible analogy. Like human beards,
lion manes are specific to males. The very thick hair
of a lion’s mane could provide protection from an
attacker’s teeth or it might make the head, neck, and
chest more difficult for an attacker to grab and hold
with the claws of his forelimbs so that it could de-
liver a damaging bite with his jaws. Indeed, Darwin
(1871) suggested that manes of male lions, Canadian
lynx, baboons, sea lions, bison, and elk provide phys-
ical protection in male–male fights. (In contrast,
when considering humans, Darwin speculated that
the beard evolved as an “ornament” favored by
females.) More recently, Blanchard (2010) has ar-
gued that the manes of lions may “mitigate” the
danger of fights among pride males by making the
existence of multi-male and female groups possible
facilitat protection of prides against take-overs and
infanticide by nomadic males. In contrast, West et al.
(2006) compared patterns of injury, mane
development, and adult mane morphology in
African lions and found no evidence that the mane
conferred effective protection against wounding.
However, they also argue that their results suggest
that “the general mane area is not a target, but hint
that attackers avoid the mane, or that the mane
protects this area from attack.” Thus, the extent to
which the mane of lions is protective remains
unresolved.
The suggestion that human beards may provide
protection in a fight is consistent with the observa-
tions that (1) the mandible, which is superficially
covered by the beard, is one of the most commonly
fractured facial bones in interpersonal violence
(Shepherd et al. 1990;Bostrom 1997;Lee 2009;
Hojjat et al. 2016) and (2) a fractured mandible,
prior to modern surgical methods, likely represented
a relatively grave facial injury. Based on these obser-
vations, we hypothesized that human facial hair pro-
vides physical protection from strikes that would
cause blunt trauma. Specifically, we predicted that
thick facial hair reduces the amount of force that
underlying tissues experience from a strike due to
absorption and dispersal of energy of the strike.
Methods
Human bone tissue was modeled using a short fiber
epoxy composite bone analog (manufactured by
Pacific Research Laboratories, Inc., Vashon, WA),
which has material properties similar to human cor-
tical bone (Cuppone et al. 2004;Chong et al. 2007).
Because it was not practical to obtain fully bearded
skin samples from human cadavers, and loose hu-
man hair was anticipated to not distribute the force
of impact the way in situ hair may, we used skin
samples from domestic sheep (Ovis aries) purchased
from a local slaughterhouse. Sheep fleece is not a
perfect analogy for the hair of human beards. The
follicles of sheep fleece average one fourth the diam-
eter of human beard hair (18 lm versus 75 lm;
Bosman 1934;Floyd et al. 2018) and are much
more densely packed (6000 follicles per cm
2
versus
70 follicles per cm
2
;Bosman 1934;Maurer et al.
2016). This represents a five-fold greater cross-
sectional area of hair follicles for fleece than beards.
However, the follicles of full human beards are often
more than five-fold longer than the follicles of the
sheep fleece samples that we tested (3.30 61.04 cm,
mean and standard deviation [SD]). Consequently,
the volume of follicles in our fleece samples did ap-
proximate the volume of full beards which is un-
likely to be true for the pelts of most other species.
2E. A. Beseris et al.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/iob/article-abstract/2/1/obaa005/5799080 by guest on 16 April 2020
The bone analog was cut into small rectangles
with dimensions 60 mm 65 mm 3 mm and cov-
ered by sheepskin. Skin samples were cut to the same
dimensions as the fiberglass and were soaked in a
saline solution (0.9% NaCl) for at least one hour
prior to testing to ensure the skin had the same
water content as living tissue. Hydration level has
been shown to have significant effects on the mate-
rial properties of organic matter, and therefore must
be standardized for all samples (Lee et al. 2011;Trim
et al. 2011). Care was taken to keep the hair of the
samples dry. The hair of the sheepskin samples was
prepared in three separate conditions: sheared,
plucked, and furred. Sheared samples were trimmed
with manual sheep shears to 0.5 cm in length.
Sheared samples were included to test whether the
presence of hair roots in the skin influenced the
results. Plucked samples had all hair fibers removed,
including the roots. Furred samples were not manip-
ulated in any way, and had an approximate hair
length of 8 cm. Of note, these three conditions result
in different total volumes and masses of hair and
were chosen to best represent states that would occur
in human males (i.e., full beard, trimmed beard, and
hairless).
All data were obtained by using a drop-weight
impact test on an Instron Dynatup 8250 drop weight
impact tester (Instron Corporation, Norwood, MA;
Fig. 1). All tests were performed in accordance with
ASTM Standard D5420 (ASTM Standard D5420-16
2016). This test involves dropping a blunt striker
(diameter 3 cm, mass ¼4.70 kg), from a known
height toward a material sample mounted on an an-
vil. The anvil had a 55 mm 50 mm hole to allow
free suspension of the sample and to avoid effects of
the contact between the anvil and sample that could
alter the results. The Instron Dynatup Impulse data
acquisition system (Instron Corporation, Norwood,
MA) takes measurements from a 200 kN load cell to
generate a graph of load (kN) versus time (ms). A
velocity detector was also used to measure the in-
stantaneous velocity (m/s) of the striker head at the
time of impact.
Prior to obtaining data to compare across the
three conditions, a standard drop height was deter-
mined. Starting from 5 cm above the anvil, the
striker head was dropped onto a furred sample. If
the sample showed signs of failure, the striker head
was lowered an additional centimeter. If the sample
did not show signs of failure, the striker head was
raised an additional centimeter. Failure was defined
as the point at which the fiberglass sample shows any
cracks, fractures, holes, or dislodged shards. The
striker head mass was not changed during the entire
duration of testing. This process was repeated for 20
samples, following the approach of the ASTM
Standard D5420 (ASTM Standard D5420-16 2016).
From these data, the mean failure height was calcu-
lated by using Equation (1):
h¼h0þdhA
N60:5Þ
, where
h¼mean failure height (mm)
dh¼increment of height (mm)
N¼total number of failures or non-failures,
whichever is smaller
h0¼lowest height at which failure or non-failure
occurred (mm)
A¼P
k
i¼0
ini, where i¼integer. ni¼number of
events occurring at hi, and hi¼h0þidh
Using this approach, the mean failure height was
determined to be 7.4 cm (Supplementary Table S1),
and the drop tower height was set to this height for
the entire series of experimental tests. Twenty sam-
ples for each condition (shaved, plucked, and furred)
were tested. Using the resultant load data (kN) and
the mass of the striker (4.7 kg), the acceleration of
the striker head (m/s
2
) was determined using
Newton’s Second Law (F¼ma). The resultant
acceleration dataset was integrated across the impact
time frame to yield the instantaneous velocity (m/s)
Fig. 1 Photograph of the experimental setup using an Instron
Dynatup 8250 drop weight impact tester.
Impact protection from mammalian hair 3
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/iob/article-abstract/2/1/obaa005/5799080 by guest on 16 April 2020
for each time frame, and subsequently, the kinetic
energy (J). The energy absorbed by the sample (J)
was calculated from the amount of kinetic energy
lost by the striker head from the start of impact to
the end of impact. From these data, the peak force in
Newtons (PF), peak energy in Joules (PE), time to
peak force in milliseconds (TPF), and time to peak
energy in milliseconds (TPE) were recorded for each
test.
A series of two-sample, single-tailed, unequal var-
iance t-tests were used to determine statistical signif-
icance between raw PF, PE, TPF, and TPE data. We
assumed the results were significantly different when
the P-value was <0.05. Percent difference was also
calculated for each of the four metrics between con-
ditions, along with mean and SD for each condition.
All data calculations, statistical analyses, and graphs
were performed using Microsoft Excel (Microsoft
Corporation, Redmond, WA).
Statement on human and animal rights
This research did not involve human or animal
subjects.
Results
The furred samples provided greater protection
against impact than did the plucked or sheared sam-
ples (Table 1). Under the condition of the study in
which the loading was set so that 50% of the
furred samples would fail on impact, all of the
plucked samples, 95% of the sheared samples, and
45% of the furred samples failed.
Example recordings of force and energy absorbed
for impact tests of the furred, plucked, and sheared
skin samples are shown in Fig. 2. As can be seen in
these traces, the average peak force was significantly
lower, energy absorbed was higher, and the time to
peak force and peak energy absorbed was substan-
tially greater in the furred samples than in the
sheared and plucked samples (Fig. 3 and Table 2).
The greatest differences between the furred and
plucked or sheared samples were observed in times
to reach peak force and peak energy absorption
(Fig. 3 and Table 2). The sheared and plucked sam-
ples were loaded more rapidly by impact and more
often than not experienced loads that exceeded their
breaking strength. This suggests that the greatest ad-
vantage offered by the hair is that it distributes the
force of impact over a longer time frame.
The higher variation observed in the furred sam-
ples is largely due to differences in the samples that
failed versus those that did not. Samples that did not
fail (see Fig. 2) had PF, PE, TPF, and TPE values
similar to the mean values for the furred condition
(PF ¼0.67 kN, PE ¼2.46 J, TPF ¼8.24 ms,
TPE ¼10.29 ms). In contrast, samples that did fail
had much higher PF values (0.82 kN) and lower PE
(1.91 J), TPF (5.26 ms), and TPE (5.39 ms) in com-
parison to the mean. The furred condition had a
nearly equal amount of failures and non-failures
(frequency of failure ¼0.45), whereas the plucked
Table 1 Frequency of failure for each condition
Frequency of failure
Furred 0.45
Plucked 1
Sheared 0.95
Fig. 2 Representative graphs of impact force (black line) and
energy (gray line) versus time for (A) a furred sample, (B)a
sheared sample, and (C) a plucked sample.
4E. A. Beseris et al.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/iob/article-abstract/2/1/obaa005/5799080 by guest on 16 April 2020
and sheared conditions had nearly all failures (fre-
quency of failure ¼1 and 0.95, respectively).
Discussion
Our results show that on average the furred samples
absorbed nearly 30% more energy than the sheared
and plucked samples. Furred samples experienced
lower peak impact forces and were loaded more
slowly. These factors contributed to a reduced rate
of furred sample failure as compared to sheared and
plucked samples. Thus, the results of this study
indicate that hair is indeed capable of significantly
reducing the force of impact from a blunt strike and
absorbing energy, thereby reducing the incidence of
failure. If the same is true for human facial hair, then
having a full beard may help protect vulnerable
regions of the facial skeleton from damaging strikes,
such as the jaw. Presumably, full beards also reduce
injury, laceration, and contusion, to the skin and
muscle of the face. Although not tested in this study,
it is also likely that the hair of beards helps deflect an
oblique blow by reducing friction between the face
and the object striking it. These protective functions
of beards may provide an advantage in male contest
competition, and therefore be selectively favored.
This may also explain why facial hair is associated
with high masculinity, social dominance, and behav-
ioral aggressiveness, as it may function as a true in-
dicator of level of invulnerablity to facial injury
(Neave and Shields 2008;Dixson and Vasey 2012;
Dixson and Brooks 2013;Saxton et al. 2016;
Sherlock et al. 2017).
No measures were significantly different between
the plucked versus sheared conditions, except for
TPE (P¼0.049). We anticipated this result as the
presence or absence of hair roots in the skin was
expected to have little influence on impact
protection.
Among the significant differences between sample
conditions, the time to peak force and time to peak
energy are likely the most salient. Furred samples
absorbed the impact more slowly than the sheared
and plucked samples. We suspect that this is a result
of individual hair fibers taking up part of the load as
the striker head descended toward the skin, slowing
the striker head as it passed by. By loading the hair
fibers in addition to the skin and bone, the force of
impact may also be distributed over a larger surface
area. This is a similar mechanism to how a Kevlar
fiber vest distributes the force of an incoming bullet
(Cheeseman and Bogetti 2003). Regardless, absorp-
tion of energy by the fur must explain why furred
samples were able to absorb 37% more energy than
sheared and plucked ones.
Our results appear to conflict with a recent study
that demonstrated beards do not provide a perfor-
mance advantage in mixed martial arts (MMA)
fights as measured by number wins by knock-out
and decision (Dixson et al. 2018). This carefully con-
trolled and compelling study, compared rates of win-
ning in 600 fights involving 395 fighters, found no
evidence of a performance advantage provided by
facial hair, and concluded that “beards represent dis-
honest signals of formidability that may serve to cur-
tail the escalation of intra-sexual conflict through
Fig. 3 Box and whisker plots showing median, first and third
quartiles, and minimum and maximum values of (A) peak force
(kN), (B) peak energy (J), (C) time to peak force (ms), and (D)
time to peak energy (ms) for each of the three conditions.
Impact protection from mammalian hair 5
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/iob/article-abstract/2/1/obaa005/5799080 by guest on 16 April 2020
intimidation rather than providing advantages in di-
rect combat.” It is sensible to test the protective ef-
fect of beards in MMA fighters because epidemiology
indicates that the most common injuries in MMA
fights are facial lacerations, fractures, and concus-
sions (Lystad et al. 2014;Jensen et al. 2017).
Although this is not the result we would have pre-
dicted based on our observation that thick hair
reduces peak impact force and energy applied to
the structure beneath the hair, the metric used in
their study “number of wins by knockout or techni-
cal knockout” is not a direct measure of the rate of
those injuries that may be reduced by full beards.
Our results provide no evidence that beards provide
protection against being knockedout, rather our
results are presumed to be most relevant to skin
lacerations and facial bone fractures. Finally, as
Dixon and collaborators note, their finding that
beards do not provide a performance advantage
may be more relevant to professional fighters than
non-professionals.
While our data are consistent with the hypothesis
that hair can protect bone and skin from the dam-
aging effects of a blunt strike, it should be noted that
this may not be true in every case. Human facial hair
has great variation across populations—individuals
from Middle Eastern and Northern European ances-
try are capable of growing thick, bushy beards,
whereas people of East Asian and American Indian
heritage have relatively little facial hair. The sheep-
skin used in our study was extremely thick and
wooly, and is probably only a good model for a
very full and long human beard. To our knowledge,
no quantitative data exist on how coarseness, den-
sity, and thickness of human facial hair varies across
populations. Future research should incorporate
these measures to determine which types of facial
hair may provide the best protection against impact.
It is unknown why human populations vary in
their developed facial hair. In groups in which thick
facial hair is not present in males, other selective
forces may have acted against facial hair. These
groups may have lower rates of contest competition
between males, thereby negating the advantage of a
beard or they may need to maximize bare skin
surface area for efficient thermoregulation in hot
environments. The fact that facial hair is sexually
dimorphic in humans, with females lacking beards
and mustaches, strongly suggests that there are real
disadvantages to having thick facial hair. If there
were no tangible disadvantages, selection for facial
hair in males would have resulted in beards in
both sexes (Lande 1980).
Additional studies are needed to ascertain the
mechanism by which hair dampens the effects of
impact. We theorized that the hair fibers absorbed
energy from the impactor head as it passed by and
by spreading the force over a larger area. This is
supported by the furred samples having a longer av-
erage time to reach peak force and absorbing more
energy. This could be further substantiated by using
highspeed video to see exactly what the hair fibers
are doing upon impact. This could also be accom-
plished by creating a model of the hair fibers and
running simulations.
The results of this study are consistent with the
suggestion that the sexually dimorphic facial hair of
humans may have evolved in response to selection
on male–male fighting performance. Similarly, al-
though not tested here, our results also support the
suggestion that the mane of male lion’s provides
some level of protection from injury when males
fight (Darwin 1871;West et al. 2006;Blanchard
2010) due to the capacity of hair to slow and expand
the area of energy transfer. As mentioned in the
Introduction, male beards are one of the most sex-
ually dimorphic features of human anatomy (Darwin
1871;Dixson et al. 2018). Men with full beards are
perceived as being more masculine, socially domi-
nant, and behaviorally aggressive in comparison to
clean-shaven men (Neave and Shields 2008;Dixson
and Vasey 2012;Dixson and Brooks 2013;Saxton
et al. 2016;Sherlock et al. 2017;T
rebicky et al.
2019). Additionally, facial hair has been shown to
positively impact mating success in highly competi-
tive environments (Barber 2001;Dixson et al. 2017).
These observations are all consistent with the hy-
pothesis that beards evolved to enhance fighting per-
formance by providing protection to vulnerable
aspects of the face. Indeed, aspects of the anatomy
Table 2 Mean, SD, percent difference, and P-values for furred (F), plucked (P), and sheared (S) conditions
Furred mean6SD Plucked mean6SD Sheared mean6SD F 3P %diff. (P)F3S %diff. (P)P3S %diff. (P)
PF (kN) 0.68 60.16 0.79 60.10 0.77 60.09 15.60 (0.004) 12.79 (0.014) 2.82 (0.23)
PE (J) 2.46 60.43 1.70 60.34 1.80 60.43 36.77 (<0.001) 31.24 (<0.001) 5.69 (0.211)
TPF (ms) 8.24 62.40 4.38 61.29 5.10 61.40 61.17 (<0.001) 47.10 (<0.001) 15.16 (0.049)
TPE (ms) 10.30 64.54 4.57 61.28 5.36 61.70 77.04 (<0.001) 63.14 (<0.001) 15.83 (0.054)
6E. A. Beseris et al.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/iob/article-abstract/2/1/obaa005/5799080 by guest on 16 April 2020
of the human facial skeleton, and sexual dimorphism
in facial shape, have been suggested to have evolved
as a result of male–male contest competition, and act
to protect the face against damaging strikes (Puts
2010;Stirrat et al. 2012;Carrier and Morgan 2015;
Puts et al. 2015).
More broadly, the results of this study add to a
growing body of evidence suggesting that specializa-
tion for male fighting has played a significant role in
the evolution of the musculoskeletal system of
humans. For example, the short limbs (Carrier
2007), plantigrade foot posture (Carrier and
Cunningham 2017), and bipedal posture of our ear-
liest hominins ancestors (Carrier 2011), and the
force–velocity tuning (Carrier et al. 2011) and size
(Carrier et al. 2015) of the muscles of the human leg
may also be associated with improved fighting per-
formance. Of direct relevance to this study is the
suggestion that the proportions of the human hand
(Morgan and Carrier 2013;Horns et al. 2014), and
human sexual dimorphism in both strength of the
muscles of the arm (Morris et al. 2020) and facial
shape (Carrier and Morgan 2015) are, at some level,
a product of selection on performance during fight-
ing with fists. Many of these anatomical traits dis-
tinguish hominins from the other great apes and all
of them are associated with performance improve-
ments in other non-fighting behaviors. Nevertheless,
the fact that the appearance of hominins in the fossil
record coincides with the appearance of a suite of
anatomical traits that have been demonstrated to
improve performance in behaviors important to hu-
man fighting suggests that specialization for physical
aggression may have played an early and persistent
role in the evolution of our lineage.
Funding
This work was funded by National Science
Foundation grant IOS-0817782 (to D.R.C.).
Supplementary data
Supplementary data available at IOB online.
Data archiving
Data will be archived on Dryad once the manuscript
is accepted for publication.
References
Adams DB. 1983. Why there are so few women warriors.
Behav Sci Res 18:196–212.
Allareddy V, Allareddy V, Nalliah RP. 2011. Epidemiology of
facial fracture injuries. J Oral Maxillofac Surg 69:2613–8.
Andersson M. 1994. Sexual Selection. Princeton (NJ):
Princeton University Press.
ASTM Standard D5420-16 2016. Standard test method for
impact resistance of flat, rigid plastic specimen by means
of a striker impacted by a falling weight (Gardner impact).
West Conshohocken (PA): ASTM International.
Barber N. 2001. Mustache fashion covaries with a good mar-
riage market for women. J Nonverbal Behav 25:261–72.
Blanchard DC. 2010. Of lion manes and human beards: some
unusual effects of the interaction between aggression and
sociality. Front Behav Neurosci 3:45.
Bosman V. 1934. The determination of fleece density in the
Merino sheep. Onderstepoort J Vet Sci Anim Ind 3:217–20.
Bostrom L. 1997. Injury panorama and medical consequences
for 1158 persons assaulted in the central part of Stockholm,
Sweden. Arch Orthop Trauma Surg 116:315–20.
Brink O, Vesterby A, Jensen J. 1998. Patterns of injuries due
to interpersonal violence. Injury 29:705–9.
Carr
e JM, McCormick CM. 2008. In your face: facial metrics
predict aggressive behaviour in the laboratory and in var-
sity and professional hockey players. Proc Biol Sci
275:2651–6.
Carr
eJM, McCormick CM, Mondloch CJ. 2009. Facial struc-
ture is a reliable cue of aggressive behavior. Psychol Sci
20:1194–8.
Carre JM, Morrissey MD, Mondloch CJ, McCormick CM.
2010. Estimating aggression from emotionally neutral faces:
which facial cues are diagnostic?. Perception 39:356–77.
Carrier DR. 2007. The short legs of great apes: evidence for
aggressive behavior in australopiths. Evolution 61:596–605.
Carrier DR. 2011. The advantage of standing up to fight and
the evolution of habitual bipedalism in hominins. PLoS
One 6:e19630.
Carrier DR, Anders C, Schilling N. 2011. The musculoskeletal
system of humans is not tuned to maximize the economy
of locomotion. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:18631–6.
Carrier DR, Cunningham C. 2017. The effect of foot posture
on capacity to apply free moments to the ground: impli-
cations for fighting performance in great apes. Biol Open
6:269–77.
Carrier DR, Morgan MH. 2015. Protective buttressing of the
hominin face. Biol Rev 90:330–46.
Carrier DR, Schilling N, Anders C. 2015. Muscle activation
during maximal effort tasks: evidence of the selective forces
that shaped the musculoskeletal system of humans. Biol
Open 4:1635–42.
Chagnon NA. 1988. Life histories, blood revenge, and warfare
in a tribal population. Science 239:985–92.
Cheeseman BA, Bogetti TA. 2003. Ballistic impact into fabric
and compliant composite laminates. Compos Struct
61:161–73.
Chong AM, Miller F, Buxton M, Friis EA. 2007. Fracture
toughness and fatigue crack propagation rate of short fiber
reinforced epoxy composites for analogue cortical bone. J
Biomech Eng 129:487–93.
Clutton-Brock TH, Harvey PH. 2009. Primate ecology and
social organization. J Zool 183:1–39.
Cuppone M, Seedhom B, Berry E, Ostell AE. 2004. The
Longitudinal Young’s Modulus of Cortical Bone in the
Midshaft of Human Femur and its Correlation with CT
Scanning Data. Calcif Tissue Int 74:302.
Impact protection from mammalian hair 7
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/iob/article-abstract/2/1/obaa005/5799080 by guest on 16 April 2020
Czerwinski M, Parker WL, Chehade A, Williams HB. 2008.
Identification of mandibular fracture epidemiology in
Canada: enhancing injury prevention and patient evalua-
tion. Can J Plast Surg 16:36–40.
Daly M, Wilson M. 1988. Homicide. New York: Aldine De
Gruyter.
Darwin C. 1871. The Descent of Man. New York: Prometheus
Books.
Dixson BJ, Brooks RC. 2013. The role of facial hair in wom-
en’s perceptions of men’s attractiveness, health, masculinity
and parenting abilities. Evol Hum Behav 34:236–41.
Dixson A, Dixson B, Anderson M. 2005. Sexual selection and
the evolution of visually conspicuous sexually dimorphic
traits in male monkeys, apes, and human beings. Annu
Rev Sex Res 16:1–19.
Dixson BJW, Lee AJ, Sherlock JM, Talamas SN. 2017. Beneath
the beard: do facial morphometrics influence the strength
of judgments of men’s beardedness?. Evol Hum Behav
38:164–74.
Dixson BJW, Rantala MJ, Melo EF, Brooks RC. 2017. Beards
and the big city: displays of masculinity may be amplified
under crowded conditions. Evol Hum Behav 38:259–64.
Dixson JW, Sherlock JM, Cornwell WK, Kasumovic MM.
2018. Contest competition and men’s facial hair: beards
may not provide advantages in combat. Evol Hum Behav
39:147–53.
Dixson BJ, Vasey PL. 2012. Beards augment perceptions of
men’s age, social status, and aggressiveness, but not attrac-
tiveness. Behav Ecol 23:481–90.
Ellis L, Hershberger S, Field E, Wersinger S, Pellis S, Geary D,
Palmer C, Hoyenga K, Hetsroni A, Karadi K. 2013. Sex
differences: summarizing more than a century of scientific
research. New York: Psychology Press.
Fink B, Neave N, Seydel H. 2007. Male facial appearance
signals physical strength to women. Am J Hum Biol
19:82–7.
Floyd EL, Henry JB, Johnson DL. 2018. Influence of facial
hair length, coarseness, and areal density on seal leakage of
a tight-fitting half-face respirator. J Occup Environ Hyg
15:334–40.
Geniole SN, Denson TF, Dixson BJ, Carr
e JM, McCormick
CM. 2015. Evidence from metaanalyses of the facial width-
to-height ratio as an evolved cue of threat. PLoS One
10:e0132726.
Guthrie R. 1970. Evolution of human threat display organs.
Evol Biol 4:257–302.
Hill AK, Bailey DH, Puts DA. 2016. Gorillas in our midst?
Human sexual dimorphism and contest competition in
men. In: Tibayrenc M, Ayala FJ, editors. On human nature:
biology, psychology, ethics, politics, and religion.
Amsterdam: Elsevier Science. p. 235–49.
Hojjat H, Svider PF, Lin HS, Folbe AJ, Shkoukani MA, Eloy
JA, Zuliani G. 2016. Adding injury to insult: a national
analysis of combat sport–related facial injury. Ann Otol
Rhinol Laryngol 125:652–9.
Horns JJ, Jung R, Carrier DR. 2014. Testing the protective
buttressing hypothesis of hominin hand proportions. Integr
Comp Biol 54:E289–E289.
Jensen AR, Maciel RC, Petrigliano FA, Rodriguez JP, Brooks
AG. 2017. Injuries sustained by the mixed martial arts ath-
lete. Sports Health 9:64–9.
Keeley LH. 1996. War before civilization. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Lande R. 1980. Sexual dimorphism, sexual selection, and ad-
aptation in polygenic characters. Evolution 34:292–305.
Lee K. 2009. Interpersonal violence and facial fractures. J Oral
Maxillofac Surg 67:1878–83.
Lee S, Novitskaya EE, Reynante B, Vasquez J, Urbaniak R,
Takahashi T, Woolley E, Tombolato L, Chen P-Y,
McKittrick J, et al. 2011. Impact testing of structural bio-
logical materials. Mater Sci Eng C Mater Biol Appl
31:730–9.
Lystad RP, Gregory K, Wilson J. 2014. The epidemiology of
injuries in mixed martial arts: a systematic review and
meta-analysis. Orthop J Sports Med
2:232596711351849.2325967113518492.
Maurer M, Rietzler M, Burghardt R, Siebenhaar F. 2016. The
male beard hair and facial skin–challenges for shaving. Int J
Cosmet Sci 38:3–9.
Mazur A, Halpern C, Udry JR. 1994. Dominant looking male
teenagers copulate earlier. Ethol Sociobiol 15:87–94.
Morgan MH, Carrier DR. 2013. Protective buttressing of the
human fist and the evolution of hominin hands. J Exp Biol
216:236–44.
Morris JS, Link J, Martin JC, Carrier DR. 2020. Sexual di-
morphism in human arm power and force: implications for
sexual selection on fighting ability. J Exp Biol 223:1–7.
Muller U, Mazur A. 1997. Facial dominance in Homo sapiens
as honest signaling of male quality. Behav Ecol 8:569–79.
Muscarella F, Cunningham MR. 1996. The evolutionary sig-
nificance and social perception of male pattern baldness
and facial hair. Ethol Sociobiol 17:99–117.
Neave N, Shields K. 2008. The effects of facial hair manipu-
lation on female perceptions of attractiveness, masculinity,
and dominance in male faces. Pers Individ Dif 45:373–7.
Parker GA. 1983. Arms races in evolution – an ESS to the
opponent independent costs game. J Theor Biol
101:619–48.
Puts D. 2016. Human sexual selection. Curr Opin Psychol
7:28–32.
Puts DA. 2010. Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual
selection in humans. Evol Hum Behav 31:157–75.
Puts DA, Bailey DH and Reno PL. 2015. Contest competition
in men. In: Buss DM, editor. The handbook of evolution-
ary psychology. Wiley & Sons.
Saxton TK, Mackey LL, McCarty K, Neave N. 2016. A lover
or a fighter? Opposing sexual selection pressures on men’s
vocal pitch and facial hair. Behav Ecol 27:512–9.
Sell A, Cosmides L, Tooby J, Sznycer D, von Rueden C,
Gurven M. 2009. Human adaptations for the visual assess-
ment of strength and fighting ability from the body and
face. Proc Biol Sci 276:575–84.
Shepherd J, Shapland M, Pearce N, Scully C. 1990. Pattern,
severity and aetiology of injuries in victims of assault. J R
Soc Med 83:75–8.
Sherlock JM, Tegg B, Sulikowski D, Dixson BJ. 2017. Facial
masculinity and beardedness determine men’s explicit, but
not their implicit, responses to male dominance. Adapt
Human Behav Physiol 3:14–29.
Simsek S, Simsek B, Abubaker AO, Laskin DM. 2007. A com-
parative study of mandibular fractures in the United States
and Turkey. Int J Oral Maxillofac Surg 36:395–7.
8E. A. Beseris et al.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/iob/article-abstract/2/1/obaa005/5799080 by guest on 16 April 2020
Stirrat M, Stulp G, Pollet TV. 2012. Male facial width is as-
sociated with death by contact violence: narrow-faced
males are more likely to die from contact violence. Evol
Hum Behav 33:551–6.
Suh YH, Kim YJ. 2012. Statistical analysis of factors associated
with facial bone fractures. Arch Craniofac Surg 13:36–40.
T
rebicky V, Havl
ı
cek J, Roberts SC, Little AC, Kleisner K.
2013. Perceived aggressiveness predicts fighting perfor-
mance in mixed-martial-arts fighters. Psychol Sci
24:1664–72.
T
rebickyV, Fialov
a J, Stella D, Coufalov
a K, Pavelka R,
Kleisner K, Kuba R,
St
erbov
a Z, Havl
ı
cek J. 2019.
Predictors of fighting ability inferences based on faces.
Front Psychol 9:2740.
Trim MW, Horstemeyer MF, Rhee H, El Kadiri H, Williams
LN, Liao J, Walters KB, McKittrick J, Park S. 2011. The
effects of water and microstructure on the mechanical
properties of bighorn sheep (Ovis Canadensis) horn kera-
tin. Acta Biomater 7:1228–40.
Walker PL. 2001. A bioarcheological perspective on the his-
tory of violence. Annu Rev Anthropol 30:573–96.
West PM, MacCormick H, Hopcraft G, Whitman K, Ericson
M, Hordinsky M, Packer C. 2006. Wounding, mortality
and mane morphology in African lions, Panthera leo.
Anim Behav 71:609–19.
Windhager S, Schaefer K, Fink B. 2011. Geometric morpho-
metrics of male facial shape in relation to physical strength
and perceived attractiveness, dominance, and masculinity.
Am J Hum Biol 23:805–14.
Wrangham RW, Peterson DE. 1996. Demonic males: apes and
the origins of human violence. Boston (MA): Houghton
Mifflin Harcourt.
Impact protection from mammalian hair 9
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/iob/article-abstract/2/1/obaa005/5799080 by guest on 16 April 2020
... Beards are a notably derived (e.g., compared to chimpanzees, where the area around the mouth is one of the least hairy regions), and highly dimorphic feature of human anatomy (Darwin, 1871). Facial hair is morphologically distinct from scalp hair in ways that may allow it to deflect and/or absorb blows to the face (Beseris et al., 2020). Men perceive potential competitors with full beards as more dominant (Puts, 2010;Dixson and Vasey, 2012;Dixson et al., 2017a). ...
Article
Full-text available
Human sexual dimorphism has been widely misunderstood. A large literature has underestimated the effect of differences in body composition and the role of male contest competition for mates. It is often assumed that sexually dimorphic traits reflect a history of sexual selection, but natural selection frequently builds different phenotypes in males and females. The relatively small sex difference in stature (∼7%) and its decrease during human evolution have been widely presumed to indicate decreased male contest competition for mates. However, females likely increased in stature relative to males in order to successfully deliver large-brained neonates through a bipedally-adapted pelvis. Despite the relatively small differences in stature and body mass (∼16%), there are marked sex differences in body composition. Across multiple samples from groups with different nutrition, males typically have 36% more lean body mass, 65% more muscle mass, and 72% more arm muscle than women, yielding parallel sex differences in strength. These sex differences in muscle and strength are comparable to those seen in primates where sexual selection, arising from aggressive male mating competition, has produced high levels of dimorphism. Body fat percentage shows a reverse pattern, with females having ∼1.6 times more than males and depositing that fat in different body regions than males. We argue that these sex differences in adipose arise mainly from natural selection on women to accumulate neurodevelopmental resources.
... Even today, the lions' mane and its defensive function attract the attention of many scholars (Blanchard, 2010). However, West et al. (2006) found no evidence of the mane's effectiveness as a protection against injury (but see Beseris et al., 2020). Thus, we suggest that the visual cue of beardedness is related to other people's perceptions of the beard owner, but not related to the actual physical or psychological traits of the bearded individual. ...
Article
Full-text available
The male beard is one of the most visually salient and sexually dimorphic traits and a hypothesized potential marker of other traits, such as dominance, masculinity, social status, and self-confidence. However, as men can easily alter their facial hair, beards may provide unreliable information about the beard owner’s characteristics. Here, we examined whether beards are honest signals of biological (testosterone levels) and psychological (self-reported dominance) traits. Young (M = 21.29, SD = 1.54) and healthy men (N = 97) participated in the study. Their beards were measured directly (using digital calipers) and by self-report. Participants provided saliva samples before and after acute exercise (to assess their testosterone and cortisol levels) and reported their dominance on a 5-item scale. The results showed that beard length (directly measured and self-reported) was not related to testosterone levels or dominance; thus, no evidence was found to support the hypothesis that beards are honest (or dishonest) signals of the beard owners’ testosterone levels and dominance.
... Within the last decade, a series of papers have been published investigating the hypothesis that selection pressures related to fistfighting had a significant impact on the morphology of modern male Homo sapiens, and in some cases even on earlier hominin species (Carrier & Morgan 2015). Under this paradigm, some traits alleged to have been influenced by sexual selection to improve fighting performance include the structure of the hand and fist (Morgan & Carrier 2013), upper limb length (Caton & Lewis, 2021), dentition and facial skeleton (Carrier & Morgan 2015), and beards (Beseris et al. 2020). Research in this area has relied on experimental analogues (Morgan & Carrier 2013;Berseris et al. 2020), mixed-martial arts (MMA) fight outcome data (Caton & Lewis, 2021), and hospitalization data from contemporary state societies (Carrier & Morgan 2015) to make their inferences, and thus may not be reflective of our evolutionary history in important ways (Tinbergen 1963). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
It has been hypothesized that key aspects of human male upper limb and facial morphology evolved through selective pressures related to fistfighting. Based on the primatological, archaeological, and ethnographic evidence, I argue these proposals are misguided. An important trend during recent hominin evolution was a decline in upper body strength and facial robusticity, coinciding in part with the rise of complex tools and weaponry. Consistent with this, dueling with weapons is a more a salient form of male-male conflict and conflict management than fistfighting across contemporary hunter-gatherer societies. Among foragers in the Standard-Cross-Cultural Sample (SCCS), fistfighting is comparatively rare, while wrestling is widespread, and dueling with weapons falls in between. I emphasize that hypotheses regarding human evolutionary history should be evaluated carefully against the cross-species, cross-cultural, and historical evidence.
... These findings are also supported by experimental studies suggesting that beards communicate masculinity, dominance and aggressiveness to other men in static ) and dynamic stimuli (Craig et al. 2019;Dixson and Vasey 2012). The lack of association between men's beardedness and fighting ability reported in previous research (Dixson et al. 2018b) and its possible role in protecting the jaw from strikes (Beseris et al. 2020) highlights that facial hair may operate as a badge of age, masculinity and status (Dixson et al. 2005;Grueter et al. 2015) as in males of many species of nonhuman primates (Petersen and Higham 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives To test whether cross-cultural variation in men’s facial hair conforms to patterns predicted by processes of inter-sexual and intra-sexual selection.Methods Data were taken from the PEW Research Center’s World’s Muslims’ project that collected information from 14,032 men from 25 countries. An Independent Factor Analysis was used to analyse how suites of demographic factors predict men’s beardedness.ResultsAnalyses replicated those from past research using the PEW data, showing that beardedness was more frequent under prevailing conditions of lower health and higher economic disparity.Conclusions These findings contribute to evidence that men’s decision to augment their masculinity via full beardedness occurs under conditions characterised by stronger inter-sexual and intra-sexual selection.
Article
Woran forschen Menschen abseits von Chemie, und was lässt sich mit ihren Ergebnissen anfangen? Wenn schon auf den ersten Blick nichts, dann doch vielleicht auf den zweiten. Und einen genauen Blick wirft das Ig‐Nobelpreis‐Komitee. Unter den Geehrten: Forschung zu Katzen, Kinoluft, Bärten und Orgasmen.
Article
Full-text available
Facial perception plays a key role in various social interactions, including formidability assessments. People make relatively accurate inferences about men's physical strength, aggressiveness, and success in physical confrontations based on facial cues. The physical factors related to the perception of fighting ability and their relative contribution have not been investigated yet, since most existing studies employed only a limited number of threat potential measures or proxies. In the present study, we collected data from Czech Mixed Martial Arts (MMA) fighters regarding their fighting success and physical performance in order to test physical predictors of perceived fighting ability made on the basis of high-fidelity facial photographs. We have also explored the relationship between perceived and actual fighting ability. We created standardized 360° photographs of 44 MMA fighters which were assessed on their perceived fighting ability by 94 raters (46 males). Further, we obtained data regarding their physical characteristics (e.g., age, height, body composition) and performance (MMA score, isometric strength, anaerobic performance, lung capacity). In contrast to previous studies, we did not find any significant links between the actual and the perceived fighting ability. The results of a multiple regression analysis have, however, shown that heavier fighters and those with higher anaerobic performance were judged as more successful. Our results suggest that certain physical performance-related characteristics are mirrored in individuals' faces but assessments of fighting success based on facial cues are not congruent with actual fighting performance. © 2019 Třebický, Fialová, Stella, Coufalová, Pavelka, Kleisner, Kuba, Štěrbová and Havlíček.
Article
Full-text available
Background. OSHA regulations state that an employer shall not permit tight-fitting respirators to be worn by employees who have facial hair that comes between the skin and facepiece seal. Studies have shown that facial hair in the face seal zone can increase penetration and decrease the fit factor (FF), though the relationship between the amount and characteristics of facial hair and the increase in penetration is not well quantified. This work examines the influence of facial hair length, areal density, and coarseness on FF for one model of half-face elastomeric negative-pressure air purifying respirator. Approach. Quantitative fit tests (QNFT) were performed on 19 subjects with beards initially 0.500 inch long and subsequently trimmed to 0.250, 0.125, and 0.063 inch, then after a razor shave. Three fit tests were performed at each of the 5 lengths, for 285 total tests. The average diameter and areal density of cheek and chin hair were measured. Penetration was modeled as a function of hair length category, beard areal density, and hair coarseness. Results. FF decreased with beard length, especially beyond 0.125 inch. However, passing FF scores were achieved on all tests by all subjects at the smooth shave and 0.063 inch conditions, and 98% of tests were passed at 0.125 inch; 7 subjects passed all tests at all conditions. Chin and cheek areal densities were significantly different and were only weakly correlated. Beard hair diameters were normally distributed across subjects (mean 76 µm, standard deviation 7.4 µm). Beard length and areal density, but not coarseness, were statistically significant predictors of fit using an arcsine transformed penetration model. FF decreased with increasing beard length, especially beyond 0.125 inch, though FF with a “stubble” beard did not differ significantly from a smooth shave. FF also decreased with increasing areal beard hair density. Conclusion. Beard length and areal density negatively influence FF. However, tight-fitting half-face negative-pressure respirator fit tests can achieve adequate fit factor scores even with substantial facial hair in the face seal area.
Article
Full-text available
In contrast to most other primates, great apes have feet in which the heel supports body weight during standing, walking and running. One possible advantage of this plantigrade foot posture is that it may enhance fighting performance by increasing the ability to apply free moments (i.e. force couples) to the ground. We tested this possibility by measuring performance of human subjects when performing from plantigrade and digitigrade (standing on the ball of the foot and toes) postures. We found that plantigrade posture substantially increased the capacity to apply free moments to the ground and to perform a variety of behaviors that are likely to be important to fighting performance in great apes. As predicted, performance in maximal effort lateral striking and pushing was strongly correlated with free moment magnitude. All else being equal, these results suggest species that can adopt plantigrade posture will be able to apply larger free moments to the ground than species restricted to digitigrade or unguligrade foot posture. Additionally, these results are consistent with the suggestion that selection for physical competition may have been one of the factors that led to the evolution of the derived plantigrade foot posture of great apes.
Chapter
Full-text available
The literature on human sexual selection has historically focused on the role of female mate choice, but cumulating experimental, correlational, and cross-cultural evidence suggests that male contest competition may have been more influential in shaping men's phenotypes. Cross-species comparison has shown similarities between humans and our closest extant phylogenetic relatives, the Great Apes, in male–male aggression, and archeological evidence also indicates a great antiquity for male–male violence. Compared to women, men possess substantially greater muscle mass, strength, cranial robusticity, physical aggression, pain tolerance, risk-taking, weapons use, and participation in coalitional aggression. Men also exhibit displays of physical prowess and acuity to the formidability of male conspecifics, as well as possessing a suite of traits, such as facial hair and low vocal pitch, that increase perceptions of dominance. These traits are consistent with having been shaped by contest competition over mates: they are sexually dimorphic, appear at sexual maturity, and predict success in male contests as well as success in mating and reproduction. While alternative explanations for some of these sexually dimorphic traits are possible, contest competition among males throughout human evolutionary history is the most parsimonious.
Article
Sexual dimorphism often arises from selection on specific musculoskeletal traits that improve male fighting performance. In humans, one common form of fighting includes using the fists as weapons. Here, we test the hypothesis that selection on male fighting performance has led to the evolution of sexual dimorphism in the musculoskeletal system that powers striking with a fist. We compared male and female arm cranking power output, using it as a proxy for the power production component of striking with a fist. Using backward arm cranking as an unselected control, our results indicate the presence of pronounced male-biased sexual dimorphism in muscle performance for protracting the arm to propel the fist forward. We also compared overhead pulling force between males and females, to test the alternative hypothesis that sexual dimorphism in the upper body of humans is a result of selection on male overhead throwing ability. We found weaker support for this hypothesis, with less pronounced sexual dimorphism in overhead arm pulling force. The results of this study add to a set of recently identified characters indicating that sexual selection on male aggressive performance has played a role in the evolution of the human musculoskeletal system and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in hominins.
Article
In contemporary human societies, where direct male-male competition is reduced compared to ancestral societies, sporting competitions remain an avenue for status acquisition via intra-sexual competition. Beards are the most visually salient and sexually dimorphic of men's secondary sexual traits and play a strong role in communicating masculinity, dominance and aggressiveness intra-sexually. Hypotheses have been advanced that beards provide advantages in intra-sexual combat, as protective organs and honest signals of fighting ability. Here we provide the first test of these hypotheses using data from professional mixed martial arts fighters competing in the Ultimate Fighting Championship. We explored whether secondary sexual traits (height, weight, beardedness), fighting stance (southpaw, orthodox), arm reach and past contest experiences impact on contest outcomes. If beards function as protective organs, bearded fighters should succumb to fewer knock-outs, and hence protection to injuries to the jaw, fewer abrasions and lacerations to the face and brain damage than clean-shaven fighters. Alternatively, if beards signal fighting ability then bearded fighters should win more fights. We found no evidence that beardedness was associated with fewer losses by knock-out or greater fighter ability. While fighters with longer reaches won more fights, neither stance nor past experience influenced fight outcomes. We suggest that beards represent dishonest signals of formidability that may serve to curtail the escalation of intra-sexual conflict through intimidation rather than providing advantages in direct combat.
Chapter
In the past 20 years, there has been a resurgence of archaeological interest in prehistoric and ancient warfare. Whether warfare is seen as a cause or an effect of features of and changes in the archaeological, ethnohistorical, or ancient historical record, it is back “in play.” This change was the result archaeologists working in Europe and the New World who were confronted by the warfare obvious in records in their areas of research. They then argued in the most widely read and stringently refereed publication venues, citing unequivocal evidence and using clear logic, that prehistoric and ethnohistoric warfare did occur and needs attention. One area that is still neglected by most archaeologists are the rare periods of relative peace—periods during which evidences of both war and homicide are rare. If recent archaeologists have shown limited interest in peace, they have at least now recognized that prehistoric war is not an oxymoron. While we vehemently disagree about the causes of war and when it began, archaeologists now recognize that ancient and prehistoric warfare did exist and that it is a topic that we can and should investigate, and most are gratified by the attention given to this subject by a new generation of scholars.
Article
Facial hair is a prominent secondary sexual trait, particularly given the importance of the face in interpersonal communication. Bizarrely by animal standards, men expend considerable effort every day trimming, waxing or shaving this androgen-dependent trait. Why some men shave this cue of masculinity off, and why women's preferences for facial hair vary so dramatically, remains largely unresolved. Using a large cross-cultural sample, we explore city- and nation-level variation in preferences for beards and in facial hair grooming patterns to test how economic and demographic conditions alter frequency-dependence in preferences for beardedness. We found that women's preferences for beards were strongest in countries with lower average incomes. Beards were most common in cities with larger populations, in countries where women express stronger preferences for facial hair and life expectancy was higher. Frequencies of non-beard facial hair styles (e.g. moustaches, goatees) were most common in large cities, but were unrelated to any demographic factors. Our results suggest a role for female choice in shaping large-scale patterns of facial hair grooming and highlight that under crowded conditions with high anonymity, displays of masculinity may be amplified.