ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Efficiently communicating information on vaccination is crucial to maintaining a high level of immunization coverage, but it implies finding the right content for the right audience. Provaccination individuals, who represent the majority of the population, and who have been neglected in the literature, could play an important role relaying provaccination messages through informal discussions, if only these messages are (a) found plausible, (b) remembered, and (c) shared. We conducted 7 experiments on 2,761 provaccination online participants (United States and United Kingdom), testing whether the valence of a statement (positive or negative) and its rhetorical orientation (pro- or antivaccine) affected these 3 steps. Participants deemed more plausible, were more willing to transmit (and actually transmitted more), but did not remember positively framed statements better. Provaccination rhetorical orientation had little or no effect. Overall, the framing effects observed were dramatic: one framing made participants very eager to transmit a statement, while another made them reluctant to transmit it at all. The framing effects also influenced vaccination attitudes, with participants exposed to positively framed statements reporting more positive attitudes toward vaccination. Since messages have to be framed one way or the other, the framing effects demonstrated here should be considered when designing public health messages. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
Content may be subject to copyright.
Framing Messages for Vaccination Supporters
Sacha Altay and Hugo Mercier
Jean Nicod Institute, Paris, France
Efficiently communicating information on vaccination is crucial to maintaining a high level of immu-
nization coverage, but it implies finding the right content for the right audience. Provaccination
individuals, who represent the majority of the population, and who have been neglected in the literature,
could play an important role relaying provaccination messages through informal discussions, if only these
messages are (a) found plausible, (b) remembered, and (c) shared. We conducted 7 experiments on 2,761
provaccination online participants (United States and United Kingdom), testing whether the valence of
a statement (positive or negative) and its rhetorical orientation (pro- or antivaccine) affected these 3 steps.
Participants deemed more plausible, were more willing to transmit (and actually transmitted more), but
did not remember positively framed statements better. Provaccination rhetorical orientation had little or
no effect. Overall, the framing effects observed were dramatic: one framing made participants very eager
to transmit a statement, while another made them reluctant to transmit it at all. The framing effects also
influenced vaccination attitudes, with participants exposed to positively framed statements reporting
more positive attitudes toward vaccination. Since messages have to be framed one way or the other, the
framing effects demonstrated here should be considered when designing public health messages.
Public Significance Statement
Subtle changes in the way information about vaccination is presented can affect how people perceive
it. Participants found more plausible, and were much more likely to share, positively framed
statements about vaccination than negatively framed statements. These framing effects should be
taken into account when designing public health messages.
Keywords: vaccination, communication, framing, transmission, plausibility
Despite the clear scientific consensus on the safety and benefits
of vaccines, a significant part of the population remains vaccine
hesitant (i.e., holding doubts and concerns about the benefits and
safety of vaccination; Larson, Jarrett, Eckersberger, Smith, &
Paterson, 2014; MacDonald & the SAGE Working Group on
Vaccine Hesitancy, 2015; Yaqub, Castle-Clarke, Sevdalis, & Cha-
taway, 2014). Most efforts dedicated to increasing vaccine accep-
tance have aimed at vaccine-hesitant individuals, examining
whether provaccination messages attenuate their misgivings (e.g.,
Chanel, Luchini, Massoni, & Vergnaud, 2011; Horne, Powell,
Hummel, & Holyoak, 2015). Typically, these messages are meant
to be part of health campaigns, or to be delivered by medical
professionals. However, information about vaccination also
spreads through other means, with significant portions of the
public relying, in part, on informal sources such as friends, col-
leagues, and family (Freed, Clark, Butchart, Singer, & Davis,
2011). In the area of public opinion more generally, the role of
peers to relay messages from the media or other official sources is
well known (e.g., Katz & Lazarsfeld, 1955). In science commu-
nication, a recent study has shown that the more people know
about the scientific consensus on global warming, the more likely
they are to discuss it with their peers, which leads to a positive
“pro-climate social feedback loop” (Goldberg, van der Linden,
Maibach, & Leiserowitz, 2019, p. 14804). In the case of vaccina-
tion, peers might play an equally important role of information
transfer—for instance, relatives’ opinions on vaccines are a strong
predictor of vaccination attitudes (Brunson, 2013; Opel & Mar-
cuse, 2013) and interpersonal communication is commonly used to
address health problems (Knapp & Daly, 2002).
In most countries, the majority of the population supports vac-
cination at least to the extent that they and their children get
vaccinated (Kennedy, Basket, & Sheedy, 2011). We could there-
fore expect this majority to effectively help spread provaccination
messages, a strategy that has been successfully implemented by
Schoeppe et al. (2017) in small communities. Unfortunately, most
XSacha Altay and XHugo Mercier, Department of Cognitive Studies,
Jean Nicod Institute, Paris, France.
Financial support for this study was provided entirely by the grant
ANR-17-EURE-0017 FrontCog and ANR-10-IDEX-0001-02 PSL. The
funding agreement ensured the authors’ independence in designing the
study, interpreting the data, writing, and publishing the report. Sacha
Altay’s PhD thesis is funded by the Direction Générale de l’Armement
(DGA). We are grateful to Aurélien Allard for statistical advice. We would
also like to thank Camille Williams and Brent Strickland for valuable
feedback and suggestions.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Sacha
Altay, Departement d’étude cognitive, Institut Jean-Nicod, 29 rue d’Ulm,
75005 Paris, France. E-mail: sacha.altay@gmail.com
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied
© 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 2, No. 999, 000
ISSN: 1076-898X http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xap0000271
1
provaccination members of the public are only willing or able to
offer relatively weak arguments to defend their opinions, relying
on personal anecdotes rather than proper evidence (Faasse, Chat-
man, & Martin, 2016; Fadda, Allam, & Schulz, 2015). Provacci-
nation individuals are also less likely to engage in online commu-
nication (McKeever, McKeever, Holton, & Li, 2016), leading to an
overrepresentation of antivaccination comments online (Feinberg
et al., 2015; Lei et al., 2015; Pereira et al., 2013). This could reflect
the fact that most vaccinees passively accept vaccination, and often
lack information about its scientific backing and benefits (Das &
Das, 2003; Nichter, 1995, p. 617; Yaqub et al., 2014; for a more
detailed classification of attitudes toward vaccination, see Stree-
fland, Chowdhury, & Ramos-Jimenez, 1999). Moreover, studies
have shown that even vaccinees often have concerns regarding the
safety of vaccination (e.g., Casiday, Cresswell, Wilson, & Panter-
Brick, 2006).
Many efforts at convincing staunchly antivaccine individuals
have failed (Dubé, Gagnon, MacDonald, N. E., & the SAGE
Working Group on Vaccine Hesitancy, 2015; Sadaf, Richards,
Glanz, Salmon, & Omer, 2013) or have even backfired (e.g.,
Nyhan, Reifler, Richey, & Freed, 2014). These failures may reflect
the lack of trust antivaccine individuals place in the medical
establishment (Salmon et al., 2005; Yaqub et al., 2014). As a
result, members of the public may be in a good position to
convince vaccine-hesitant individuals (at least fence-sitters; Leask,
2011), if only they could muster convincing arguments. Thus,
improving the argumentative arsenal of provaccination individuals
could indirectly help convince vaccine hesitant individuals—and it
might further assuage the remaining concerns of provaccine indi-
viduals.
So far, the effectiveness of provaccination arguments has been
measured in terms of persuasiveness. However, if arguments are to
be further spread by provaccine individuals, these individuals
should not only accept the arguments, but they should also be able
to memorize them and be willing to transmit them to others.
Although the field of cultural evolution has long paid attention to
memory and willingness to transmit in theoretical (Boyer, 2018;
Morin, 2015) and empirical work (for a recent review, see Miton
& Charbonneau, 2018; for applications to vaccination, see Jimé-
nez, Mesoudi, & Tehrani, 2020; Jiménez, Stubbersfield, & Teh-
rani, 2018; Miton & Mercier, 2015), only one of these factors
(memorization) has been examined in the case of vaccination
(Pluviano, Watt, & Della Sala, 2017).
Studies on information transmission suggest that seemingly
minor changes in the presentation of a piece of information can
influence its likelihood of being accepted, remembered, and fur-
ther transmitted. Framing is the best studied of these superficial
changes: logically equivalent descriptions can lead to very differ-
ent decisions when they are framed differently (Kahneman &
Tversky, 1979; Tversky & Kahneman, 1981). One of the most
robust framing effects is valence framing, where negatively framed
information is judged as truer (Fessler, Pisor, & Navarrete, 2014),
is better memorized (Bebbington, MacLeod, Ellison, & Fay,
2017), and is transmitted more often (Heath, 1996) than positively
framed information (a glass half empty is a negative frame while
a glass half full would be a positive frame). However, there are
relevant exceptions to this dominance of negative frames. When
people are asked whether to approve medical treatments, empha-
sizing survival rates, by contrast with mortality rates, leads to more
positive attitudes toward the treatment (Levin, Schnittjer, & Thee,
1988; Marteau, 1989; Wilson, Kaplan, & Schneiderman, 1987).
This preference for positive frames is also found in cases such as
rating basketball players (Levin, 1987), judging the quality of
ground beef (Levin & Gaeth, 1988), support for condom use
(Linville, Fischer, & Fischhoff, 1993), or evaluating industry proj-
ect teams (Dunegan, 1993, 1995). Anecdotally, a survey on vac-
cine communication found that positive messages were rated as
more convincing and more believable than negative messages
(Association of State and Territorial Health Officials, 2009), but
the conclusions we can draw from this study are limited since the
information available in the two frames was not equivalent.
Framing is unavoidable: a message has to be framed one way or
the other. This is relevant for common provaccination messages:
those regarding side effects and those regarding medical consen-
sus. Since overestimating the dangerousness of vaccine side effects
appears to be a major reason for vaccine refusal (Betsch & Wicker,
2012; Omer, Salmon, Orenstein, deHart, & Halsey, 2009), provac-
cination campaigns often stress the safety of vaccines (Dittmann,
2001; van der Linden, Clarke, & Maibach, 2015; World Health
Organization, 2016). Such messages can be framed positively (e.g.,
999 out of 1,000 people do not have any severe side effects after being
injected with a vaccine) or negatively (e.g., 1 individual out of 1,000
has severe side effects after being injected with a vaccine). For
example, the World Health Organization opted for a negative frame
when tweeting “Severe side effects are extremely rare” (https://twitter
.com/WHO/status/1192071297230147584?s!20). Since knowledge
that the medical consensus on the efficacy and safety of vaccination
is positively correlated with favorable beliefs on vaccination (Dixon
&Clarke,2013),messagesaboutmedicalconsensushavealsobeen
used. These messages (e.g., 90% of medical scientists agree that
vaccines are safe; van der Linden et al., 2015) have managed to
reduce belief in the autism-vaccine link and to increase public support
for vaccination. Here as well, the message has to be framed either
positively (90% of medical scientists agree that vaccines are safe) or
negatively (10% of medical scientists do not agree that vaccines are
safe). This type of simple framing has been classified as attribute
framing, and it is considered “the most straightforward test of the
influence of positive and negative framing” (for a typology of framing
effects, see Levin, Schneider, & Gaeth, 1998, p. 158).
Even though frames are logically equivalent, it has been sug-
gested that they leak information, such as information about the
speaker’s beliefs (McKenzie, 2004; McKenzie & Nelson, 2003;
Sher & McKenzie, 2006). Specifically, framing valence influences
perceived rhetorical orientation—that is, what conclusion the in-
formation appears to support. A speaker who uses the positive
frame might be perceived as being provaccine, while one who uses
the negative frame might be perceived as being antivaccine. As a
result, people who want to be perceived as either pro- or antivac-
cine may show a preference for either one of the frames, by being
more eager to transmit messages that appear to reflect their posi-
tion (Chung & Darke, 2006; Hennig-Thurau, Gwinner, Walsh, &
Gremler, 2004; Sundaram, Mitra, & Webster, 1998). Rhetorical
orientation could even affect the perceived plausibility of the
messages. Studies in political science have shown that survey
respondents often provide answers on factual questions as a way of
expressing their political opinions (Bullock, Gerber, Hill, & Hu-
ber, 2015). Similarly, the plausibility ratings of statements related
to vaccination could be used by participants to communicate their
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
2ALTAY AND MERCIER
attitudes toward vaccination, with statements having a provaccine
rhetorical orientation being deemed more plausible by provaccine
individuals, and vice versa.
The goal of the study was to test whether framing influences the
steps necessary for a statement to be used by a provaccine indi-
vidual in informal discussions: (a) evaluation of message plausi-
bility (Experiments 2 and 5), (b) memorization of the message
(Experiment 3), (c) willingness to transmit the message further
(Experiments 4 and 6), and finally (d) the propensity to use the
statements to convince someone who refuses to vaccinate (Exper-
iment 7). Experiment 1 aimed at establishing that our predictions
about the direction of rhetorical orientation of the experimental
statements (in favor or against vaccination) were accurate. Exper-
iments 5 and 6 were replications of Experiments 2 and 4 using a
different sample. In each experiment, we compared statements that
varied in valence (positively vs. negatively framed) and in rhetor-
ical orientation (whether they are associated with a pro- or an
antivaccine position). However, statements with a rhetorical ori-
entation in favor of vaccination also had a positive valence (“90%
of medical scientists think that vaccines are safe” and “999 out of
1,000 don’t have any severe side effects after being injected with
a vaccine”). To disentangle the effects of valence framing and
rhetorical orientation, we added a third variant, that was negatively
framed but whose rhetorical orientation was likely to be perceived
as provaccine because it began with the word only (e.g., “Only 1
individual out of 1,000 has severe side effects after being injected
with a vaccine”). We hypothesized that (a) only would convey an
evaluative judgment indicating that the given frequency is consid-
ered to be low, and (b) that this evaluative judgment would be
more likely attributed to a vaccination supporter than to someone
holding negative views on vaccination.
The literature on the pragmatics of framing effects (McKenzie,
2004; McKenzie & Nelson, 2003; Sher & McKenzie, 2006) leads
us to expect that the choice of frame made by a person will
influence what ideological position others attribute to them:
Hypothesis 1: Positively framed statements with a rhetorical
orientation in favor of vaccination will be more strongly
associated with provaccination individuals than statements
with a negative valence and a rhetorical orientation against
vaccination.
Hypothesis 2: Negatively framed statements with a rhetorical
orientation in favor of vaccination (i.e., the statements with
only) will be more strongly associated with provaccination
individuals than statements with a negative valence and a
rhetorical orientation against vaccination.
The literature on the negativity bias (e.g., Baumeister, Brat-
slavsky, Finkenauer, & Vohs, 2001) leads us to expect that:
Hypothesis 3: Negatively framed statements will be judged
truer than positively framed statements.
Hypothesis 4: Negatively framed statements will be better
memorized than positively framed statements.
Hypothesis 5: Negatively framed statements will be more
transmitted than positively framed statements.
The literature on attribute framing (e.g., Levin et al., 1998) leads
us to expect that:
Hypothesis 6: Positively framed statements will be judged
truer than negatively framed statements.
Hypothesis 7: Exposition to positively framed statements will
create more positive attitudes toward vaccination than expo-
sition to negatively framed statements.
The literature in political science (e.g., Bullock et al., 2015)
leads us to expect the rhetorical orientation to have the following
effects:
Hypothesis 8: Statements with a rhetorical orientation in favor
of vaccination will be judged truer than statements with a
rhetorical orientation against vaccination.
Hypothesis 9: Statements with a rhetorical orientation in favor
of vaccination will be more shared than statements with a
rhetorical orientation against vaccination.
Some of these predictions are conflicting (e.g., H
3
and H
6
),
others are unique (e.g., H
7
). The present investigation aims at
putting these theories to the test in the case of information about
vaccines.
Experiment 1
In this first experiment we measured the perceived ideological
position of individuals sharing the statements. We predicted that
positively framed statements (with a rhetorical orientation in favor
of vaccination) would be attributed to provaccination individuals
(H
1
), and that the addition of the word only at the beginning of
negatively framed statements would modify the statements’ per-
ceived rhetorical orientation from antivaccination to provaccina-
tion (H
2
).
Participants
For Experiments 1 to 4, participants residing in the United States
were recruited through Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk). Sam-
ples recruited on MTurk have proven suitable for similar experi-
ments (Paolacci & Chandler, 2014). A power analysis for a chi-
square test with an estimated effect size of w!0.5 (we expected
a large effect size since information leakage in attribute framing
can have drastic effects; Sher & McKenzie, 2008), an "level of
5%; and a power of 80%, suggested that we needed 52 participants
per condition. Since we have six conditions, we needed a mini-
mum of 312 participants. Because we did not know how many
participants we would need to exclude and considering that we
could have overestimated the expected effect size, we recruited
480 participants, who were paid $0.30. Eleven participants failed
to complete the survey, and we removed 50 participants catego-
rized as being against vaccination, leaving 419 participants (184
women, M
age
!35.03, SD !11.05; Public Support Index (from
1!negative attitudes to 7 !positive attitudes): M!6.25, SD !
0.83). Approval from the ethics committee was obtained for all
experiments (CER-Paris Descartes; No. 2019-03-MERCIER; all
data presented in this article are available at https://osf.io/ksa5b).
Design and Procedure
The overall design and procedures were similar for all experi-
ments. Participants signed a consent form, took part in the exper-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
3
FRAMING MESSAGES FOR VACCINATION SUPPORTERS
iment itself, then filled in a Public Support Index questionnaire
developed by van der Linden et al. (2015) to assess respondents’
position on vaccination. The index is composed of seven state-
ments supporting vaccination that participants have to rate on a
scale from 1 (Strongly disagree)to7(Strongly agree). Participants
who mostly agreed with these statements (i.e., average score
higher than 4) were considered to be in favor of vaccination, others
against vaccination. Finally, in a debriefing, participants were
provided with accurate information about vaccination, and then
filled in demographic questions.
In Experiment 1, for the experimental task participants were
provided with one of the six statements from Table 1, and asked to
answer the question “Would this person [the person who uttered
the statement] more likely to be: in favor of vaccination or against
vaccination?”
Results and Discussion
Table 1 provides the percentage of participants who thought the
individual uttering each statement was more likely to be pro- or
antivaccination.
As predicted by H
1
, positively framed statements with a rhetor-
ical orientation in favor of vaccination were more likely to be
attributed to a provaccination individual than negatively framed
statements with a rhetorical orientation against vaccination (#
2
(1,
N!285) !91.13, p$.001, %!1.37).
As predicted by H
2
, negatively framed statements with a rhe-
torical orientation in favor of vaccination were more likely to be
attributed to a provaccination individual than negatively framed
statements with a rhetorical orientation against vaccination #
2
(1,
N!276) !98.84, p$.001, %!1.49.
Statements perceived as having a provaccination rhetorical ori-
entation were more likely to be attributed to a provaccination
individual than statements perceived as having an antivaccination
rhetorical orientation #
2
(1, N!419) !119.84, p$.001, %!
1.23.
Experiment 2
In the second experiment we measured how valence framing
and rhetorical orientation influence the perceived plausibility of
the statements. The literature on the negativity bias predicts that
negatively framed statements will be judged truer than positively
framed statements (H
3
), while the literature on attribute framing
leads us to expect the opposite (H
6
). The literature on political
science leads us to expect that statements with a rhetorical orien-
tation in favor of vaccination will be judged truer than statements
with a rhetorical orientation against vaccination (H
8
).
Participants
A power analysis for an analysis of variance (ANOVA) with an
estimated effect size of f!0.325 (the literature led us to expect
an effect size between medium and large, see, Fessler et al., 2014), an
"-level of 5%, and a power of 80% suggested that we needed a
minimum of 77 participants per condition. Since we have six condi-
tions, we needed a minimum of 462 participants. Considering that we
would need to exclude participants, we recruited 480 participants who
Table 1
The Messages Used in All the Experiments, Along With Whether They Are Framed Positively or Negatively, Their Presumed
Rhetorical Orientation, and the Results of All the Experiments (Experiment 1 [U.S.]: Perception as Provaccine; Experiments 2 [U.S.]
and 5 [UK]: Plausibility; Experiment 3 [U.S.]: Recall Rate; Experiments 4 [U.S.] and 6 [UK]: Willingness to Transmit)
Rhetorical
orientation Perception as
provaccine
Plausibility (1–7) Willingness to transmit
(1–5)
Statement Frame U.S. UK Recall rate U.S. UK
Side effects
999 people out of 1,000 don’t
have any severe side effects
after being injected with a
vaccine.
&In favor of
vaccination
99% (N!73) M!4.87
SD !1.95
N!71
M!5.09
SD !1.71
N!65
69.64% (N!56) M!3.82
SD !1.11
N!72
M!3.75
SD !1.07
N!68
Only 1 individual out of 1,000
has some severe side effects
after being injected with a
vaccine.
'In favor of
vaccination
91% (N!67) M!4.88
SD !1.55
N!69
M!4.23
SD !1.56
N!65
80.00% (N!55) M!2.83
SD !1.16
N!70
M!3.02
SD !1.20
N!68
1 individual out of 1,000 has
some severe side effects after
being injected with a vaccine.
'Against
vaccination
46% (N!71) M!3.92
SD !1.77
N!66
M!3.94
SD !1.67
N!68
83.64% (N!110) M!2.56
SD !1.22
N!63
M!2.90
SD !1.30
N!71
Medical consensus
90% of medical scientists think
that vaccines are safe.
&In favor of
vaccination
97% (N!72) M!5.97
SD !1.37
N!69
M!5.86
SD !1.11
N!71
78.00% (N!50) M!3.87
SD !1.01
N!77
M!3.52
SD !1.03
N!63
Only 10% of medical scientists
don’t think that vaccines are
safe.
'In favor of
vaccination
90% (N!67) M!4.10
SD !1.98
N!67
M!3.79
SD !1.82
N!65
73.33% (N!60) M!2.62
SD !1.31
N!63
M!2.66
SD !1.38
N!70
10% of medical scientists don’t
think that vaccines are safe.
'Against
vaccination
42% (N!69) M!3.11
SD !1.95
N!66
M!3.54
SD !1.84
N!69
81.08% (N!111) M!2.03
SD !1.14
N!67
M!2.34
SD !1.05
N!65
Note. U.S. !United States; UK !United Kingdom.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
4ALTAY AND MERCIER
were paid $0.30. Five participants failed to complete the survey, and
we removed 67 participants categorized as being against vaccination,
leaving 408 participants (223 women, M
age
!36.19, SD !11.29;
score on the Public Support Index: M!6.17, SD !0.84).
Design and Procedure
Overall design and procedure were identical to Experiment 1.
For the experimental task, each participant had to read one of the
statements from Table 1 and answer “How confident are you that
the statement is TRUE or FALSE?” on a 7-point Likert scale
ranging from 1 (I’m absolutely certain this statement is FALSE) to
7(I’m absolutely certain this statement is TRUE). The question is
identical to the one used by Fessler et al. (2014).
Results and Discussion
Plausibility scores of each statement are presented in Table 1. A
three-way ANOVA was conducted to compare the main effects of
rhetorical orientation (pro- or antivaccine), valence (positive or
negative), and type of statement (side effects or consensus) on the
plausibility ratings. There was a small effect of valence F(1,
404) !17.22, p$.001, (
2
!0.04, with participants rating
positive statements (M!5.41, SD !1.77) as more plausible than
negative statements (M!4.01, SD !1.92). The effect of rhetor-
ical orientation was small F(1, 404) !19.36, p$.001, (
2
!0.04,
with participants rating provaccination statements (M!4.96,
SD !1.85) as more plausible than antivaccination statements
(M!3.52, SD !1.90). The type of statement had no influence on
plausibility ratings F(1, 404) !0.66, p!.42, (
2
!0.001.
Contrary to the predictions of the negativity bias hypothesis
(H
3
), but in accordance with the literature on attribute framing
(H
6
), positively framed statements were found to be more plausible
than negatively framed ones. We also found that statements with a
rhetorical orientation in favor of vaccination were judged truer
than statements with a rhetorical orientation against vaccination,
supporting H
8
.
Experiment 3
In the third experiment we tested whether valence framing and
rhetorical orientation of the statements would affect memorization.
The literature on the negativity bias led us to expect that negatively
framed statements would be better memorized than positively
framed statements (H
4
).
Participants
A power analysis for logistic regression with an estimated odds
ratio of 2 (corresponding to an effect size between small and
medium (Chen, Cohen, & Chen, 2010), as found in a previous
experiment, see Bebbington et al., 2017), an "-level of 5%, and a
power of 80% suggested that we needed a minimum of 88 partic-
ipants per condition. Since we have three conditions (across the 6
dialogues), we needed a minimum of 264 participants. Considering
that we could have overestimated the expected effect size, we
recruited 280 participants who were paid $0.40. Four participants
failed to complete the survey, and we removed 33 participants
categorized as being against vaccination based on their answers to
the Public Support Index. We also removed 22 participants whose
answers in the free-recall task bore no relationship with the para-
graph to be memorized, leaving 221 participants (99 women,
M
age
!35.37, SD !10.81). The sample size is smaller compared
to the other experiments but the number of data points is similar,
since each participant had to read two statements instead of one.
Due to constraints on the organization of the dialogue to be
memorized, we could not counterbalance the three factors per-
fectly, so that negatively framed statements with a rhetorical
orientation against vaccination (1/1000 and 10%) received more
responses.
Materials
The statements were integrated in small dialogues, such as the
following:
Denis: Can you come to the game on Monday?
Raymond: I cannot, I have to take my kid to the pediatrician,
he’s getting his vaccines.
Denis: Do you know that [1 individual out 1,000 has
severe side effects after being injected with a
vaccine?]
Raymond: Maybe, but [90% of medical scientists think that
vaccines are safe.]
A statement on vaccines side effects was always associated with
a statement on the medical consensus with a different frame. In
half of the dialogues a statement on vaccine side effects was
presented first (as in the example) and in the other half statements
on the medical consensus was presented first. Six different dia-
logues were used (1/1,000 and 90%; 90% and 1/1,000; 10% and
999/1,000; 999/1,000 and 10%; only 1/1,000 and 10%; only 10%
and 1/1,000).
Design and Procedure
The overall design and procedure were similar to those of
Experiments 1 and 2. The experimental task consisted in asking
participants to read the dialogue and, on the next page, write what
they remembered of it. Such free-recall tasks are frequently used in
the literature on cultural evolution (e.g., Eriksson & Coultas,
2014).
Results and Discussion
Each statement was attributed a score of one when the partici-
pant correctly recalled it (including the correct statistic) and a
score of zero otherwise. The memorization scores of each state-
ment are presented in Table 1.
We conducted a logistic regression with participants as a ran-
dom effect to compare the main effects of valence, rhetorical
orientation, and statement type on the memorization score and
found no significant effects (valence (positive compared to nega-
tive): Z!0.002, SE !0.30, p!.99; rhetorical orientation (in
favor of vaccination compared to against vaccination): Z!'1.05,
SE !0.29, p!.29; type of statement (side effects compared to
scientific consensus): Z!0.09, SE !0.23, p!.93). The hypoth-
esis that negatively framed statements would be better remem-
bered was not supported (H
4
).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
5
FRAMING MESSAGES FOR VACCINATION SUPPORTERS
Experiment 4
In the fourth experiment we measured the effect of valence
framing and rhetorical orientation on the willingness of partici-
pants to share the statements. The negativity bias hypothesis led us
to expect that participants would be more willing to share nega-
tively framed statements than positively framed statements (H
5
).
The literature in political science led us to expect that participants
would be more willing to share statements with a rhetorical ori-
entation in favor of vaccination (H
9
).
Participants
A power analysis for ANOVA with an estimated effect size of
f!0.325 (based on previous findings we expected to find an effect
size between medium and large; Heath, 1996), an "-level of 5%,
and a power of 80% suggested that we needed a minimum of 77
participants per condition. Since we have six conditions, we
needed a minimum of 462 participants. Considering we would
need to exclude participants, we recruited 480 participants who
were paid $0.30. Six participants failed to complete the survey, and
we removed 62 participants categorized as being against vaccina-
tion, leaving 412 participants (195 women, M
age
!36.69, SD !
11.99; Public Support Index: M!6.14, SD !0.86).
Design and Procedure
The overall design and procedure were similar to those of
Experiments 1 and 2. The experimental task consisted in asking
participants to read one of the statements and to answer “How
likely would you be to pass along this statement to other people”
on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (very unlikely to pass
along)to5(very likely to pass along). This task is commonly used
in the literature on cultural evolution (e.g., see van Leeuwen,
Parren, Miton, & Boyer, 2018), and has proven to be correlated
(.44) with actual sharing patterns (Mosleh, Pennycook, & Rand,
2020).
Results and Discussion
The willingness to transmit ratings of each statement are pre-
sented in Table 1. A three-way ANOVA was conducted to com-
pare the main effects of valence, rhetorical orientation, and state-
ment type on the willingness to transmit rating. There was a
medium effect of valence F(1, 408) !63.62, p$.001, (
2
!0.13,
with participants more likely to transmit positively framed state-
ments (M!3.85, SD !1.05) than negatively framed statements
(M!2.51, SD !1.24). The effect of the rhetorical orientation was
small F(1, 408) !9.17, p!.003, (
2
!0.02 with participants
more willing to transmit provaccine statements (M!3.33, SD !
1.27) than antivaccine statements (M!2.30, SD !1.21). The type
of statement had a very small influence on willingness to transmit
rating F(1, 408) !4.77, p!.03, (
2
!0.007, with participants
being slightly more willing to transmit statements on vaccine side
effects (M!3.08, SD !1.28) than on the consensus (M!2.90,
SD !1.39).
Again, our results go against the predictions of the negativity
bias hypothesis (H
6
), with people being more willing to trans-
mit positively framed statements. Participants were also more
willing to transmit statements whose rhetorical orientation they
supported (H
9
).
Experiment 5
In the fifth experiment we replicated Experiment 2 on the
perceived plausibility of the statements with a different population
sample: in the United Kingdom rather than the United States (for
a review of the differences and commonalities between the two
countries, see Wheelock, Thomson, & Sevdalis, 2013). Despite the
differences between the two populations in attitudes toward vac-
cination, we expected to observe the same patterns.
Participants
Four hundred twenty-five participants were recruited through
Prolific Academic and paid £0.20. We removed 22 participants
categorized as being against vaccination, leaving 403 participants
(274 women, M
age
!35.89, SD !11.69; score on the Public
Support Index: M!6.19, SD !0.78).
Results and Discussion
The plausibility ratings of each statement are presented in Table
1. A three-way ANOVA was conducted to compare the main
effects of the three independent variables on plausibility ratings.
There was a medium effect of valence, F(1, 399) !53.50,
p$.001, (
2
!0.11 with participants rating positive statements
(M!5.49, SD !1.48) as more plausible than negative state-
ments (M!3.87, SD !1.74). The effects of rhetorical orien-
tation, F(1, 399) !1.78, p!.18, (
2
!0.003 and of type of
statement, F(1, 399) !0.001, p!.97, (
2
$.001 were not
significant. Experiment 5 thus replicates the effects of valence,
but not of rhetorical orientation. These results again support H
6
in opposition to H
3
, but they do not support H
8
.
The aggregated results of Experiments 2 and 5 are displayed in
Figure 1.
Experiment 6
The sixth experiment is an exact replication of Experiment 4
with a different population sample: crowdsourcing participants
recruited in the United Kingdom (through Prolific Academic)
rather than the United States.
Participants
Four hundred twenty-six participants were recruited and paid
£0.20. We removed 21 participants categorized as being against
vaccination, leaving 405 participants (290 M
age
!36.88, SD !
12.80; Public Support Index: M!6.09, SD !0.78).
Results and Discussion
Willingness to transmit ratings attributed to each statement are
presented in Table 1. A three-way ANOVA was conducted to
compare the main effects of the three independent variables on the
willingness to transmit ratings. There was a medium effect of
valence, F(1, 401) !30.50, p$.001, (
2
!0.07, with participants
being more willing to transmit positively framed statements (M!
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
6ALTAY AND MERCIER
3.64, SD !1.05) than negatively framed statements (M!2.73,
SD !1.26). The effect of rhetorical orientation was not signifi-
cant, F(1, 401) !2.2, p!.14, (
2
!0.004. The effect of type of
statement was small F(1, 401) !10.64, p!.001, (
2
!0.02, with
participants being more willing to transmit statements on vaccine
side effects (M!3.22, SD !1.25) than on the consensus (M!
2.83, SD !1.27).
Like Experiment 5, Experiment 6 replicates the effects of va-
lence, but not of rhetorical orientation. In both experiments, par-
ticipants were slightly more likely to share statements on vaccine
side effects than on consensus. These results again contradict
against H
5
, but they also do not support H
9
.
The aggregated results of Experiments 4 and 6 are displayed in
Figure 2.
Cross-Experiments Analysis
Finally, we assessed the influence of being exposed to the
statements on vaccine attitudes by merging the data of all the
experiments except Experiments 2 and 7 in which participants
were exposed to multiple statements. We did not exclude partici-
pants based on their attitudes toward vaccination since it was the
main variable of interest in this analysis. The total sample was
composed of 2,273 participants (1,274 women, M
age
!36.15,
Figure 1. Aggregated results of Experiments 2 and 5 (N!811): estimated plausibility (y-axis) as a function
of the type of statements (x-axis). The boxes represent the middle 50% of the scores; the lines that divide the
boxes are the median; upper and lower whiskers represent scores outside the middle 50% (except outliers); and
diamonds represent the means.
!
p$.05.
!!
p$.01.
!!!
p$.001.
Figure 2. Aggregated results of Experiments 4 and 6 (N!817): willingness to transmit (y-axis) as a function
of the type of statements (x-axis). The boxes represent the middle 50% of scores for the group; the lines that
divide the boxes are the median; upper and lower whiskers represent scores outside the middle 50% (except
outliers); and diamonds represent the means. ns !not significant.
!!
p$.01.
!!!
p$.001.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
7
FRAMING MESSAGES FOR VACCINATION SUPPORTERS
SD !11.81; initial score on the Public Support Index: M!5.84,
SD !1.31).
We found that participants exposed to statements with a positive
valence reported more positive attitudes toward vaccination (M!
5.94, SD !1.22) than participants exposed to statements with a
negative valence (M!5.79, SD !1.35; Welch’s t(1655.4) !2.70,
p!.007, d!0.12; Figure 3). Participants exposed to statements with
apositiverhetoricalorientationwerenotmorelikelytoreportpositive
attitudes toward vaccination (M!5.85, SD !1.33) than participants
exposed to statements with a negative rhetorical orientation (M!
5.83, SD !1.27; Welch’s t(1578.4) !.28, p!.78).
These results are consistent with participants saying that they
find positively framed statements more plausible, and they are
important from a practical standpoint, given that participants re-
ported a greater willingness to share positively framed statements
(i.e., the most persuasive statements). These results also offer
evidence in favor of H
7
.
Experiment 7
In this last experiment, we measured the propensity of partici-
pants to use our statements in order to convince someone who
refuses to vaccinate, a few minutes after reading them. We com-
pared the arguments provided by participants who had read some
of our statements to the arguments of participants who had not read
the statements (control condition). This experiment offers a more
ecological setting than the memorization task of Experiment 2.
Participants
We increased the sample size compared to Experiment 2 to
make sure we would be sufficiently powered. Five hundred forty-
eight participants were recruited through Prolific Academic and
paid £0.20. We removed 24 participants categorized as being
against vaccination and two participants who failed at the attention
check, leaving 522 participants (358 women, M
age
!36.59, SD !
12.29; Public Support Index: M!6.52, SD !0.78).
Materials
We excluded statements with only because we wanted to focus
on the statements’ valence, which appeared to play a more impor-
tant effect on willingness to transmit. The statements were inte-
grated in vignettes such as the following:
Imagine the following situation:
You’re listening to a program on the radio in which doctors and
scientists are interviewed about vaccination. At some point, you hear:
“90 % of medical scientists think that vaccines are safe.”
A few minutes later you hear:
“1 individual out of 1,000 has some severe side effects after being
injected with a vaccine.”
A statement on vaccine side effects was always associated with
a statement on the medical consensus with a different frame. In
half of the dialogues the statement on vaccine side effects was
presented first (as in the example) and in the other half the
statement on the medical consensus was presented first.
Design and Procedure
The overall design and procedure were similar to those of
Experiment 2, with the exceptions that we introduced a control
group receiving no arguments, and we added a distraction task
lasting approximately four minutes. Half of the participants read
two statements on vaccination as described above; the other half
did not. The experimental task consisted in asking participant to
convince someone to vaccinate. Participants were presented with
the following text:
Imagine the following situation:
You are chatting with a friend of yours, and at some point she tells
you: “Vaccines are not safe, I won’t vaccinate myself or my kids.”
Concerned for your friend and her children, you try to convince her
that her opinion is misguided by answering:
[Free text entry]
Coding
We coded participants’ answer in the following way:
Side effects. The participant mentioned that very few people
suffer from side effects or that most people do not suffer from any
side effects.
Scientific consensus. The participant mentioned that most
medical scientists (or doctors) are in favor of vaccination or think
it is safe, or that very few medical scientists (or doctors) are not in
favor of vaccination or think it is unsafe.
Vaccines safety. The participant mentioned the fact that vac-
cines are safe, but did not mention scientific consensus or the side
effects.
Figure 3. Influence of valence framing on vaccine attitudes across Ex-
periments 1, 3, 4, 5, and 6 (N!2,273): participants’ attitudes toward
vaccination (y-axis) as a function of the valence of the statements’ partic-
ipants have been exposed to (x-axis). The boxes represent the middle 50%
of scores for the group; the lines that divide the boxes are the median;
upper and lower whiskers represent scores outside the middle 50% (except
outliers); and diamonds represent the means.
!!
p$.01.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
8ALTAY AND MERCIER
Herd immunity. The participants mentioned that vaccination
is not only useful for individuals but for society at large or others.
Diseases are riskier than vaccines. The participant argued
that diseases that vaccinate protect against are more dangerous
than the vaccines themselves.
Scientific evidence. The participant mentioned scientific ev-
idence in favor of vaccination such as scientific articles or exper-
iments.
Vaccines offer individual protection. The participant men-
tioned the protective nature of vaccines for individuals.
Disease are dangerous. The participant mentioned that some
diseases vaccines protect against are dangerous.
Not vaccinating is dangerous. The participant mentioned
that not vaccinating is dangerous (without explaining why).
Anecdotal evidence. The participant used anecdotal evidence
relying on firsthand experience to support vaccination.
No argument. What the participant wrote could not fit into
any of the categories above because it was not an argument.
Twenty percent of responses (104/522) were recoded by an
independent coder blind to our hypotheses. Coders agreed on
92.31% of the observations ()!0.85, SE $.01; 95% CI [0.83,
0.87]), the strength of agreement is considered to be “almost
perfect” (Landis & Koch, 1977).
Results and Discussion
All of the arguments used by participants can be found in Table 2.
In comparison to the control condition, participants in the ex-
perimental conditions were more likely to use arguments that
relied on the low rate of side effects (58 vs. 1, Z!0.21, p$.001)
or the scientific consensus on vaccination (22 vs. 2, Z!0.08, p$
.001).
In the experimental conditions, participants who received the
side effects statement positively framed were not more likely to
use it as an argument that participants who received it negatively
framed (31 vs. 27, Pearson’s chi-square test with Yates’ continuity
correction: #
2
(1, N!58) !.14, p!.70). Most participants
reported the side effects framed negatively (43 vs. 15, chi-square
goodness of fit test: #
2
(1, N!58) !13.52, p$.001, %!0.48),
but most of them added only (27/43) or other rhetorical devices
(8/43) to modify the rhetorical orientation of the statement.
Participants who received the consensus statement positively
framed were more likely to use it as an argument than participants
who received it negatively framed (19 vs. 3, Pearson’s chi-square
test with Yates’ continuity correction: #
2
(1, N!22) !10.91, p$
.001, %!0.70). All the mentions of the scientific consensus were
positively framed (22/22), as the three participants who had been
presented with the consensus negatively framed reframed it posi-
tively. When the participants made use of the statements that had
been provided—whether on the scientific consensus or the side
effects—nearly all gave them a rhetorical orientation in favor of
vaccination (76/80).
In the experimental conditions, participants who mentioned at
least one of the statements used on average more arguments (M!
2.36, SD !0.96) than participants who did not (M!1.35, SD !
0.61; Welch’s t(521) !7.85, p$.001, d!1.36), suggesting that
these statements reinforced their argumentative arsenal.
These results suggest that it is important to frame the consensus
statement positively because it increases the rate at which it is then
used by readers in their arguments. However, it is not clear that it
would be beneficial to positively frame the statement on the side
effects. Framing these statements negatively while maintaining a
provaccination rhetorical frame (e.g., with only, which many par-
ticipants spontaneously did) could minimize the effort required for
participants to appropriate the argument.
General Discussion
Our studies tested the effects of framing on the estimated
plausibility, memorization, and willingness to transmit of provac-
cination statements among provaccination individuals in the
United States and United Kingdom. The statements had two con-
tents, relating either to vaccine side effects or to the medical
consensus on vaccination. As established by Experiment 1, the
statements’ valence could be positive or negative, and the state-
ments could be perceived as strongly provaccination or as being
ambiguous between being pro- and antivaccination.
Contrary to previous studies (Bebbington et al., 2017; Fessler et
al., 2014), negatively framed statements, compared to positively
framed statements, were not better memorized (Experiment 3),
were deemed less plausible (Experiments 2 and 5), and were less
appealing to transmit (Experiments 4 and 6). These effects in
favor of positively framed statements ranged from small to
large. The size of these effects and the conflicting direction of
our results with the literature justified our decision to replicate
Experiments 2 and 4.
The effects of rhetorical orientation were broadly as expected.
Rhetorical orientation had no effect on memory, but participants
deemed more plausible (Experiment 2), and were more willing to
transmit (Experiment 4), statements whose rhetorical orientation
better fitted their own views, compared to statements that didn’t.
While these effects ranged from small to medium for participants
residing in the United States, there was no significant effect of
rhetorical orientation on plausibility or willingness of transmission
with the sample from the United Kingdom. It is unclear what drove
this potential difference between the United States and United
Kingdom populations. Nonetheless, this discrepancy does not af-
fect the practical conclusions that can be drawn from this study,
since the same statements were found to be more plausible and
more transmissible in the two populations.
Table 2
Types of Arguments Used by Participants in Experiment 7 as a
Function of the Condition
Arguments
Experimental
conditions
(N!264)
Control
condition
(N!258)
Side effects 58 1
Scientific consensus 22 2
Vaccines are safe 62 83
Herd immunity 45 60
Diseases are riskier than vaccines 66 41
Scientific evidence support vaccination 41 40
Vaccines offer individual protection 21 56
Diseases are dangerous 15 25
Not vaccinating is dangerous 27 31
Anecdotal evidence 8 8
No arguments 48 41
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
9
FRAMING MESSAGES FOR VACCINATION SUPPORTERS
Visual inspection of the results on plausibility and willingness to
transmit in Figures 1 and 2 reveal the striking effects of framing.
Comparing the most favorable frame (positive with provaccination
rhetorical orientation) to the most negative frame (negative with
ambiguous rhetorical orientation), we move from statements
deemed very plausible to statements deemed barely plausible, and
from statements participants are keen to transmit to statements
participants would rather not transmit.
These strong framing effects have at least two significant prac-
tical consequences. First, positively valenced statements prompted
participants to adopt a more positive attitude toward vaccination.
Second, in the case of statements about the scientific consensus,
positively valenced statements were more likely to be used by
participants to argue against someone who opposes vaccination.
Moreover, framing effects were also clearly perceptible in the way
participants framed the arguments they chose to use, with nearly
all arguments being framed with a provaccination rhetorical ori-
entation.
In relation with the literature on framing, our results confirm
that the choice of frame induces information leakage (McKenzie,
2004; McKenzie & Nelson, 2003; Sher & McKenzie, 2006, 2008),
as frames informed readers about the attitudes of the speakers
toward vaccination. Moreover, our results join a growing body of
research in questioning the generality of the negativity bias (for a
literature review, see Levin et al., 1998; for more recent findings,
see Berriche & Altay, 2019; Stubbersfield, Dean, Sheikh, Laland,
& Cross, 2019; van Leeuwen et al., 2018). It is thus important to
test, on a case-by-case basis, the effects of positive or negative
framing, and to acknowledge that framing could differently affect
memorization, plausibility evaluations, willingness to share, and
actual sharing behaviors. We thus encourage future research to
combine different measures and not to presume that framing will
have the same effect on multiple measures.
Some limitations of our study should be pointed out. First, we
obviously do not claim that our manipulation will be sufficient to
overcome vaccine hesitancy on its own—indeed, it is not even its
direct goal. However, we suggest that seemingly superficial fea-
tures of vaccine communication might have a substantial impact
on how some provaccine individuals process the communication,
making it more or less likely that they transmit it further. In
particular, some frames—in the case at hand, positive frames—
appear to make provaccine participants more likely to accept and
transmit provaccine messages, which are intrinsically desirable
outcomes. e.g., information campaigns often frame information
about vaccines side effects negatively, albeit with a provaccination
rhetorical orientation, as in the example above: “Severe side ef-
fects are extremely rare.” A positive frame (e.g., “The overwhelm-
ing majority of people do not have any side effects”) might be
more easily further transmitted by provaccination audiences.
Most research on vaccination-related messaging has focused on
its direct effects on vaccine-hesitant individuals, largely disregard-
ing vaccination supporters (Vanderslott, 2019). Yet, in most coun-
tries, the majority of the population is provaccine, suggesting that
these individuals could play a significant role in propagating
provaccination messages through informal communication. The
present study reveals that messages aimed at reinforcing the argu-
mentative tools of provaccination individuals must consider the
way the messages are framed. Finally, our research highlights the
importance of conducting experiments on the specific messages
we wish to provide to the population, rather than extrapolating
purely from past results, since the major framing effects we ob-
served went in the opposite direction from what was expected from
some of the existing literature.
References
Association of State and Territorial Health Officials. (2009). Communicating
effectively about vaccines: New communication resources for health offi-
cials. Retrieved from http://www.astho.org/Programs/Immunization/
Communicating-Effectively-About-Vaccines--Summary-of-a-Survey-of-U-
S--Parents-and-Guardians/
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001).
Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323
Bebbington, K., MacLeod, C., Ellison, T. M., & Fay, N. (2017). The sky
is falling: Evidence of a negativity bias in the social transmission of
information. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38, 92–101. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.07.004
Berriche, M., & Altay, S. (2019). “Sister, I love you!” Phatic posts are
more successful on Facebook than health misinformation. Retrieved
from https://psyarxiv.com/nj2sr/
Betsch, C., & Wicker, S. (2012). E-health use, vaccination knowledge and
perception of own risk: Drivers of vaccination uptake in medical stu-
dents. Vaccine, 30, 1143–1148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2011
.12.021
Boyer, P. (2018). Minds make societies: How cognition explains the world
humans create. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Brunson, E. K. (2013). The impact of social networks on parents’ vacci-
nation decisions. Pediatrics, 131, e1397–e1404.
Bullock, J. G., Gerber, A. S., Hill, S. J., & Huber, G. A. (2015). Partisan
bias in factual beliefs about politics. Quarterly Journal of Political
Science, 10, 519–578.
Casiday, R., Cresswell, T., Wilson, D., & Panter-Brick, C. (2006). A
survey of U. K. parental attitudes to the MMR vaccine and trust in
medical authority. Vaccine, 24, 177–184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.vaccine.2005.07.063
Chanel, O., Luchini, S., Massoni, S., & Vergnaud, J.-C. (2011). Impact of
information on intentions to vaccinate in a potential epidemic: Swine-
origin Influenza A (H1N1). Social Science & Medicine, 72, 142–148.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.11.018
Chen, H., Cohen, P., & Chen, S. (2010). How big is a big odds ratio?
Interpreting the magnitudes of odds ratios in epidemiological studies.
Communications in Statistics Simulation and Computation, 39, 860
864. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03610911003650383
Chung, C. M., & Darke, P. R. (2006). The consumer as advocate: Self-
relevance, culture, and word-of-mouth. Marketing Letters, 17, 269–279.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11002-006-8426-7
Das, J., & Das, S. (2003). Trust, learning, and vaccination: A case study of
a North Indian village. Social Science & Medicine, 57, 97–112. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00302-7
Dittmann, S. (2001). Vaccine safety: Risk communication—A global per-
spective. Vaccine, 19, 2446–2456. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0264-
410X(00)00470-9
Dixon, G. N., & Clarke, C. E. (2013). Heightening uncertainty around
certain science: Media coverage, false balance, and the autism-vaccine
controversy. Science Communication, 35, 358–382. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1177/1075547012458290
Dubé, E., Gagnon, D., MacDonald, N. E., & the SAGE Working Group on
Vaccine Hesitancy. (2015). Strategies intended to address vaccine hes-
itancy: Review of published reviews. Vaccine, 33, 4191–4203. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2015.04.041
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
10 ALTAY AND MERCIER
Dunegan, K. J. (1993). Framing, cognitive modes, and image theory:
Toward an understanding of a glass half full. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 78, 491–503. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.78.3.491
Dunegan, K. J. (1995). Image theory: Testing the role of image compati-
bility in progress decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Deci-
sion Processes, 62, 7986. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1995.1033
Eriksson, K., & Coultas, J. C. (2014). Corpses, maggots, poodles and rats:
Emotional selection operating in three phases of cultural transmission of
urban legends. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 14, 1–26. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1163/15685373-12342107
Faasse, K., Chatman, C. J., & Martin, L. R. (2016). A comparison of
language use in pro- and anti-vaccination comments in response to a
high profile Facebook post. Vaccine, 34, 5808–5814. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.vaccine.2016.09.029
Fadda, M., Allam, A., & Schulz, P. J. (2015). Arguments and sources on
Italian online forums on childhood vaccinations: Results of a content
analysis. Vaccine, 33, 7152–7159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine
.2015.11.007
Feinberg, Y., Pereira, J. A., Quach, S., Kwong, J. C., Crowcroft, N. S.,
Wilson, S. E., . . . the Public Health Agency of Canada/Canadian
Institutes of Health Research Influenza Research Network (PCIRN)
Program Delivery and Evaluation Group. (2015). Understanding public
perceptions of the HPV vaccination based on online comments to
Canadian news articles. PLoS ONE, 10, e0129587. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1371/journal.pone.0129587
Fessler, D. M., Pisor, A. C., & Navarrete, C. D. (2014). Negatively-biased
credulity and the cultural evolution of beliefs. PLoS ONE, 9, e95167.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0095167
Freed, G. L., Clark, S. J., Butchart, A. T., Singer, D. C., & Davis, M. M.
(2011). Sources and perceived credibility of vaccine-safety information
for parents. Pediatrics, 127, S107–S112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds
.2010-1722P
Goldberg, M. H., van der Linden, S., Maibach, E., & Leiserowitz, A.
(2019). Discussing global warming leads to greater acceptance of cli-
mate science. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
United States of America, 116, 14804–14805. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/
pnas.1906589116
Heath, C. (1996). Do people prefer to pass along good or bad news?
Valence and relevance of news as predictors of transmission propensity.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 68, 79–94.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1996.0091
Hennig-Thurau, T., Gwinner, K. P., Walsh, G., & Gremler, D. D. (2004).
Electronic word-of-mouth via consumer-opinion platforms: What moti-
vates consumers to articulate themselves on the internet? Journal of
Interactive Marketing, 18, 38–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dir.10073
Horne, Z., Powell, D., Hummel, J. E., & Holyoak, K. J. (2015). Countering
antivaccination attitudes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci-
ences of the United States of America, 112, 10321–10324. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1073/pnas.1504019112
Jiménez, Á. V., Mesoudi, A., & Tehrani, J. (2020). No evidence that
omission and congruity biases affect the perception and recall of
vaccine-related information. PLoS ONE, 15(3), e0228898. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228898
Jiménez, Á. V., Stubbersfield, J. M., & Tehrani, J. J. (2018). An experi-
mental investigation into the transmission of antivax attitudes using a
fictional health controversy. Social Science & Medicine, 215, 23–27.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.08.032
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of
decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–292. http://dx.doi.org/10
.2307/1914185
Katz, E., & Lazarsfeld, P. F. (1955). Personal influence: The part played
by people in the flow of mass communications. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
Kennedy, A., Basket, M., & Sheedy, K. (2011). Vaccine attitudes, con-
cerns, and information sources reported by parents of young children:
Results from the 2009 HealthStyles survey. Pediatrics, 127, S92–S99.
Knapp, M. L., & Daly, J. A. (2002). Handbook of interpersonal commu-
nication. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer
agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159–174.
Larson, H. J., Jarrett, C., Eckersberger, E., Smith, D. M., & Paterson, P.
(2014). Understanding vaccine hesitancy around vaccines and vaccina-
tion from a global perspective: A systematic review of published liter-
ature, 2007–2012. Vaccine, 32, 2150–2159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.vaccine.2014.01.081
Leask, J. (2011). Target the fence-sitters. Nature, 473, 443–445. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1038/473443a
Lei, Y., Pereira, J. A., Quach, S., Bettinger, J. A., Kwong, J. C., Corace, K.,
. . . the Public Health Agency of Canada/Canadian Institutes of Health
Research Influenza Research Network Program Delivery and Evaluation
Group. (2015). Examining perceptions about mandatory influenza vac-
cination of healthcare workers through online comments on news stories.
PLoS ONE, 10, e0129993. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone
.0129993
Levin, I. P. (1987, May). Associative effects of information framing on
human judgments. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Mid-
western Psychological Association, Chicago, IL.
Levin, I. P., & Gaeth, G. J. (1988). Framing of attribute information before
and after consuming the product. Journal of Consumer Research, 15,
374–378.
Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All frames are not
created equal: A typology and critical analysis of framing effects.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76, 149–188.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1998.2804
Levin, I. P., Schnittjer, S. K., & Thee, S. L. (1988). Information framing
effects in social and personal decisions. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 24, 520–529. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(88)
90050-9
Linville, P. W., Fischer, G. W., & Fischhoff, B. (1993). AIDS risk percep-
tions and decision biases. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
MacDonald, N. E., & the SAGE Working Group on Vaccine Hesitancy.
(2015). Vaccine hesitancy: Definition, scope and determinants. Vaccine,
33, 4161–4164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2015.04.036
Marteau, T. M. (1989). Framing of information: Its influence upon deci-
sions of doctors and patients. British Journal of Social Psychology, 28,
89–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1989.tb00849.x
McKeever, B. W., McKeever, R., Holton, A. E., & Li, J.-Y. (2016). Silent
majority: Childhood vaccinations and antecedents to communicative
action. Mass Communication & Society, 19, 476498. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1080/15205436.2016.1148172
McKenzie, C. R. M. (2004). Framing effects in inference tasks—And why
they are normatively defensible. Memory & Cognition, 32, 874885.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03196866
McKenzie, C. R. M., & Nelson, J. D. (2003). What a speaker’s choice of
frame reveals: Reference points, frame selection, and framing effects.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 596602. http://dx.doi.org/10
.3758/BF03196520
Miton, H., & Charbonneau, M. (2018). Cumulative culture in the labora-
tory: Methodological and theoretical challenges. Proceedings of the
Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285, 20180677.
Miton, H., & Mercier, H. (2015). Cognitive obstacles to pro-vaccination
beliefs. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 633–636. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.tics.2015.08.007
Morin, O. (2015). How traditions live and die. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
11
FRAMING MESSAGES FOR VACCINATION SUPPORTERS
Mosleh, M., Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. (2020). Self-reported willingness
to share political news articles in online surveys correlates with actual
sharing on Twitter. PLoS ONE, 15, e0228882.
Nichter, M. (1995). Vaccinations in the Third World: A consideration of
community demand. Social Science & Medicine, 41, 617–632. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/0277-9536(95)00034-5
Nyhan, B., Reifler, J., Richey, S., & Freed, G. L. (2014). Effective
messages in vaccine promotion: A randomized trial. Pediatrics, 133,
e835–e842. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-2365
Omer, S. B., Salmon, D. A., Orenstein, W. A., deHart, M. P., & Halsey, N.
(2009). Vaccine refusal, mandatory immunization, and the risks of
vaccine-preventable diseases. The New England Journal of Medicine,
360, 1981–1988. http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMsa0806477
Opel, D. J., & Marcuse, E. K. (2013). Window or mirror: Social networks’
role in immunization decisions. Pediatrics, 131, e1619e1620. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-0531
Paolacci, G., & Chandler, J. (2014). Inside the Turk. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 23, 184–188. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0963721414531598
Pereira, J. A., Quach, S., Dao, H. H., Kwong, J. C., Deeks, S. L., Crow-
croft, N. S., . . . the Public Health Agency of Canada/Canadian Institutes
of Health Research Influenza Research Network (PCIRN) Program
Delivery, Evaluation Group. (2013). Contagious comments: What was
the online buzz about the 2011 Quebec measles outbreak? PLoS ONE, 8,
e64072. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064072
Pluviano, S., Watt, C., & Della Sala, S. (2017). Misinformation lingers in
memory: Failure of three pro-vaccination strategies. PLoS ONE, 12,
e0181640. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0181640
Sadaf, A., Richards, J. L., Glanz, J., Salmon, D. A., & Omer, S. B. (2013).
A systematic review of interventions for reducing parental vaccine
refusal and vaccine hesitancy. Vaccine, 31, 4293–4304. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2013.07.013
Salmon, D. A., Moulton, L. H., Omer, S. B., DeHart, M. P., Stokley, S., &
Halsey, N. A. (2005). Factors associated with refusal of childhood
vaccines among parents of school-aged children: A case-control study.
Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 159, 470476. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.159.5.470
Schoeppe, J., Cheadle, A., Melton, M., Faubion, T., Miller, C., Matthys, J.,
& Hsu, C. (2017). The immunity community: A community engagement
strategy for reducing vaccine hesitancy. Health Promotion Practice, 18,
654661. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1524839917697303
Sher, S., & McKenzie, C. R. (2006). Information leakage from logically
equivalent frames. Cognition, 101, 467–494. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.cognition.2005.11.001
Sher, S., & McKenzie, C. R. M. (2008). Framing effects and rationality. In
N. Chater & M. Oaksford (Eds.), The probabilistic mind: Prospects for
Bayesian cognitive science (pp. 79–96). Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Press.
Streefland, P., Chowdhury, A. M. R., & Ramos-Jimenez, P. (1999). Pat-
terns of vaccination acceptance. Social Science & Medicine, 49, 1705–
1716. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(99)00239-7
Stubbersfield, J. M., Dean, L. G., Sheikh, S., Laland, K. N., & Cross, C. P.
(2019). Social transmission favours the ‘morally good’ over the ‘merely
arousing’. Palgrave Communications, 5, 70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1057/
s41599-019-0269-y
Sundaram, D.S., Mitra, K., & Webster, C. (1998). Word-of-mouth com-
munications: A motivational analysis. Advances in Consumer Research,
25, 527–531.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the
psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/
science.7455683
van der Linden, S. L., Clarke, C. E., & Maibach, E. W. (2015). Highlight-
ing consensus among medical scientists increases public support for
vaccines: Evidence from a randomized experiment. BMC Public Health,
15, 1207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12889-015-2541-4
Vanderslott, S. (2019). Exploring the meaning of pro-vaccine activism
across two countries. Social Science & Medicine, 222, 5966.
van Leeuwen, F., Parren, N., Miton, H., & Boyer, P. (2018). Individual
choose-to-transmit decisions reveal little preference for transmitting
negative or high-arousal content. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 18,
124–153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/15685373-12340018
Wheelock, A., Thomson, A., & Sevdalis, N. (2013). Social and psycho-
logical factors underlying adult vaccination behavior: Lessons from
seasonal influenza vaccination in the U.S. and the U. K. Expert Review
of Vaccines, 12, 893–901. http://dx.doi.org/10.1586/14760584.2013
.814841
Wilson, D. K., Kaplan, R. M., & Schneiderman, L. J. (1987). Framing of
decisions and selections of alternatives in health care. Social Behaviour,
2, 51–59.
World Health Organization. (2016). Vaccine safety communication: Guide
for immunization programme managers and national regulatory author-
ities. Manila, Philippines: WHO Regional Office for the Western Pa-
cific.
Yaqub, O., Castle-Clarke, S., Sevdalis, N., & Chataway, J. (2014). Atti-
tudes to vaccination: A critical review. Social Science & Medicine, 112,
1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.04.018
Received December 4, 2019
Revision received February 14, 2020
Accepted February 22, 2020 !
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
12 ALTAY AND MERCIER
... Compared to the low flu vaccination rates described above, uptake of the COVID-19 vaccine in Austria at the time of this study (March, 2021, one year into the pandemic) was far higher: by the end of May 2021, just under half of those aged 16 and older had received at least their first dose (Pollak et al., 2021) 1 . Reported willingness to be vaccinated rose continually from 32% in October and December of 2020 (before a vaccine was available) to 68% in May of 2021 (this percentage includes those who had already received at least one dose). ...
... This last factor was reflected in the way intention to be vaccinated differed widely depending on the specific vaccine and its perceived risk of side effects: in March 2021, the percentage of people who stated they would probably turn down the chance to be vaccinated was 17% for the BionTech/Pfizer vaccine but 49% for AstraZeneca . These figures tally with research that finds concerns about and overestimation of vaccine side effects to be a major reason for vaccine hesitancy ( 1 The first COVID-19 cases in Austria were officially registered in late February of 2020. The World Health Organization officially declared a pandemic on March 11, 2020, and Austria went into the first of several lockdowns on March 16, 2020. ...
... In the work focusing on the promotion of vaccine uptake, the aims of most such measures can be put into two broad categories: influencing intention (i.e., persuading or reassuring people that they want and intend to be vaccinated) and facilitating action (i.e., getting people to actually follow through on their intentions) ( Public messaging and framing information are another tool in influencing vaccination intentions. In general, negatively framed information is judged to be truer than positively framed in-formation (Fessler et al., 2014), but in the case of medical treatments, positive framing (e.g., giving survival rather than mortality rates) leads to a more positive attitude towards these treatments (Altay & Mercier, 2020). However, the effect of positive/gain framing on prevention behaviors appear to be far weaker for vaccines than e.g. for cancer prevention, smoking cessation or safer sex (Gallagher & Updegraff, 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Many people are hesitant or unwilling to take advantage of potentially life-saving vaccines; this is true even in a global pandemic and even when vaccines are readily available and/or free. Apart from general unwillingness to be vaccinated, fear of side effects is a frequently named reason for vaccine hesitation. This survey-based study, carried out online in Austria in March of 2021, examines the efficacy of two different approaches – framing and nudging – in promoting a fictional COVID-19 vaccine program. While nudging had no visible effects, either positive or negative, framing was associated with more positive feelings toward the campaign and a greater intention to be vaccinated, though only among those already pre-disposed to get the vaccine. Governments and healthcare organizations are eager to find low-cost measures to increase vaccination rates for COVID-19 and other diseases. This study’s results suggest that addressing and positively framing the high likelihood of problem-free vaccinations should form an integral part of public messages, but that this approach may be ineffective among those who are strongly hesitant or unwilling. AcknowledgementSupported by funds from the province of Tyrol.
... This news framing can be sometimes misleading and some of it may be false (Hua & Shaw, 2020). There are cases where media frequently abound with health-related misinformation, including misleading information regarding vaccination (Altay & Mercier, 2020). Gislason (2013) states that the utilisation of a specific news frame during a health crisis has substantial implications for public comprehension and responses to the outbreak. ...
... Vaccination message framing can impact readers' vaccination attitudes; a positive frame can lead to a positive attitude (Altay & Mercier, 2020). It is crucial to crafting vaccine messages carefully, considering factors that influence message acceptance (Palm et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
The widespread dissemination of misinformation or the ‘infodemic’ surrounding COVID-19 poses a significant challenge as the virus continues to spread globally, compounded by the reluctance of many individuals to accept ‘COVID-19 vaccines’. While existing research has predominantly explored how the media framed the disease, its psychological impact, and the presence of disinformation in its messaging, limited attention has been given to the portrayal of ‘COVID-19 vaccines’ in the media and their reception among the general population. This study employs a corpus-based methodology to examine the framing of ‘COVID-19 vaccines’ within a specific English Malaysian newspaper corpus. The data for analysis was gathered through the compilation of news reports from The Star, one of the nation’s primary English newspapers. The analysis showed that the vaccine was positively framed and correlated with keywords like ‘special’, ‘effective’, and ‘important’, as well as with keywords indicating its importance to one’s health and the masses. However, two negatively connoted keywords were also used to describe the act of immunisation. This study’s findings can encourage informed decision-making on COVID-19 vaccination by dispelling unwarranted fears. Positive news coverage can boost confidence and prevent panic while mitigating negative emotions and risk perceptions. These findings can help media practitioners better frame vaccine-related issues in their writings and understand how the public perceives them.
... Rothman & Salovey 1997;Gallagher & Updegraff 2012) that motivates a serious philosophical discussion of VACCINATION and similar epistemic nudges. Recently, Altay and Mercier (2020) conducted a series of experiments on the influence of the positive and negative framing of vaccinations, in terms of side effects and medical consensus. ...
... Again, when information about vaccinations is put in a particular frame by an authority (VACCINATION), the frame "leaks" an implicit recommendation from that authority. In fact, Altay and Mercier (2020) take their study of vaccination framing, which we mentioned earlier, to be coherent with the information leakage hypothesis. There is nothing irrational about being influenced by recommendations when people need to judge the safety of vaccinations. ...
Article
Full-text available
Libertarian paternalism is a weak form of paternalism that recommends nudges rather than bans, restrictions, or other strong interventions. Nudges influence people’s choice by modifying contextual factors (the “choice architecture”). This paper explores the possibility of an epistemic analogue of libertarian paternalism. What I call “epistemic libertarian paternalism” is a weak form of epistemic paternalism that recommends “epistemic nudges” rather than stronger paternalistic interventions. Epistemic nudges influence people’s beliefs and judgments by modifying contextual factors (the “epistemic choice architecture”). The main aim of this paper is to defend epistemic libertarian paternalism from the “irrationality problem”, which I take to be the most urgent problem for epistemic libertarian paternalism; given how epistemic nudges work (i.e. they typically co-opt psychological biases), nudged beliefs are irrational. In response to the irrationality problem, I admit that nudged beliefs are often (not always, though) irrational, but insist that there are conditions in which epistemic nudging can be justifiable nonetheless. I will propose two conditions that are jointly sufficient for justifiable epistemic nudging: “Veridicality Condition” (which says that nudged beliefs are more likely to be true than non-nudged beliefs) and “Not-More-Irrationality Condition” (which says that nudged beliefs are not more likely to be irrational than non-nudged beliefs).
... 11 Participants also judged positively framed statements about vaccine risks more plausible, they were more willing to share those and had a more positive attitude toward vaccines than participants who read negatively framed statements. 16 Equivalence framing can use percentages and complementary outcomes as above or be more subtle by focusing on the same outcome but using positive or negative verbal probabilities (e.g., ''a chance'' v. ''unlikely''). Verbal probability framing is especially interesting because health professionals use words more often than numbers to communicate risks to their patients. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background How health workers frame their communication about vaccines’ probability of adverse side effects could play an important role in people’s intentions to be vaccinated (e.g., positive frame: side effects are unlikely v. negative frame: there is a chance of side effects). Based on the pragmatic account of framing as implicit advice, we expected that participants would report greater vaccination intentions when a trustworthy physician framed the risks positively (v. negatively), but we expected this effect would be reduced or reversed when the physician was untrustworthy. Design In 4 online experiments ( n = 191, snowball sampling and n = 453, 451, and 464 UK residents via Prolific; M age ≈ 34 y, 70% women, 84% White British), we manipulated the trustworthiness of a physician and how they framed the risk of adverse side effects in a scenario (i.e., a chance v. unlikely adverse side effects). Participants reported their vaccination intention, their level of distrust in health care systems, and COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs. Results Physicians who were trustworthy (v. untrustworthy) consistently led to an increase in vaccination intention, but the way they described adverse side effects mattered too. A positive framing of the risks given by a trustworthy physician consistently led to increased vaccination intention relative to a negative framing, but framing had no effect or the opposite effect when given by an untrustworthy physician. The exception to this trend occurred in unvaccinated individuals in experiment 3, following serious concerns about one of the COVID vaccines. In that study, unvaccinated participants responded more favorably to the negative framing of the trustworthy physician. Conclusions Trusted sources should use positive framing to foster vaccination acceptance. However, in a situation of heightened fears, a negative framing—attracting more attention to the risks—might be more effective. Highlights How health workers frame their communication about a vaccine’s probability of adverse side effects plays an important role in people’s intentions to be vaccinated. In 4 experiments, we manipulated the trustworthiness of a physician and how the physician framed the risk of adverse side effects of a COVID vaccine. Positive framing given by a trustworthy physician promoted vaccination intention but had null effect or did backfire when given by an untrustworthy physician. The effect occurred over and above participants’ attitude toward the health care system, risk perceptions, and beliefs in COVID misinformation.
... The role of the media in conveying the stress-causing information that the sideeffects of the vaccine were dangerous for the body perhaps did not always have a direct impact on people, but the information received did enter into their memories and influence their evaluation of the urgency to pay for the vaccine. This was in line with the findings of a study conducted by Altay and Mercier (2020) concerning the effects of framing, that small changes in the provision of information were capable of influencing the recall and attitudes of people regarding vaccination. ...
Article
Full-text available
During the pandemic, the Indonesian government has striven to implement steps to prevent and control the impacts of COVID-19, one of which is through a free program of vaccinations, from the first to the third dose (booster), in order to raise herd immunity. Although these were free, the take-up of the booster vaccination has been slow. This study was aimed at exploring individual preparedness to pay for COVID-19 vaccination, if the government was no longer to pay for these vaccines. The study was conducted using a quantitative approach, employing thematic analysis techniques. The participants comprised 27 people, with their ages ranging from 18-83 years. The results of the study found there were two principle themes (or factors) influencing the preparedness to pay for vaccinations, those of: (1) drive; and (2) deterrent. The drive factor comprised the aspects which supported the public in being prepared to receive, and pay for, vaccinations; these being the obligations to adhere to the regulations of government and institutions, to receive social assistance (welfare), and to ensure personal protection. The deterrent factor comprised the aspects which made the public reluctant to pay for the vaccinations, amongst others being the framing of information about the side-effects of the vaccine, the non-halal (religiously acceptable) composition of the vaccine, and the feeling of satisfaction after having received two doses. Amongst all of these factors, the obligatory nature of vaccination was the aspect most influencing the preparedness to pay. This study provides a picture of why the public may be prepared to pay for the vaccine, the factors which may elevate and depress their preparedness to pay, and thoughts about the costs of the vaccine, which may be the benchmarks in planning policies related to health behavior. The plans of the government to make COVID-19 vaccination subject to payment require assessment of the cost of the vaccine, because this will influence the number of members of the public who are prepared to pay to receive vaccination. This may certainly influence the level of public immunity to COVID-19, in the long term. Selama pandemi, pemerintah Indonesia berupaya melakukan pencegahan dan penanganan dampak COVID-19, salah satunya melalui program vaksinasi gratis dari dosis pertama hingga dosis ketiga (booster) untuk meningkatkan herd immunity. Meskipun gratis, laju vaksinasi booster lambat meningkat. Studi ini bertujuan untuk mengeksplorasi kesediaan individu untuk membayar vaksin COVID-19 jika pemerintah tidak lagi membiayai vaksinasi COVID-19. Studi dilakukan dengan pendekatan kualitatif dengan teknik analisis tematik. Partisipan terdiri dari 27 orang dengan rentang usia antara 18-83 tahun. Hasil studi ini menemukan dua tema utama yang memengaruhi kesediaan membayar vaksin: (1) faktor pendorong (drive); dan (2) faktor penghalang (deterrent). Faktor pendorong (drive) terdiri dari aspek yang mendorong masyarakat untuk bersedia mendapat dan membayar vaksin, yaitu kewajiban untuk mengikuti aturan pemerintah dan institusi, mendapatkan bantuan sosial, dan untuk perlindungan diri. Faktor penghalang (deterrent) merupakan aspek yang membuat masyarakat enggan membayar vaksin, antara lain framing informasi efek samping vaksin, komposisi vaksin yang tidak halal, dan merasa puas dengan dua dosis. Di antara seluruh faktor tersebut, kewajiban vaksinasi adalah aspek yang paling memengaruhi kesediaan membayar. Studi ini memberikan gambaran mengapa masyarakat bersedia membayar untuk vaksin, faktor yang dapat meningkatkan dan menurunkan kesediaan mereka untuk membayar vaksin, dan perkiraan harga vaksin COVID-19 yang dapat menjadi tolok ukur dalam merancang kebijakan terkait perilaku kesehatan. Rencana pemerintah untuk menjadikan vaksinasi COVID-19 sebagai vaksinasi berbayar membutuhkan asesmen mengenai harga vaksin karena akan memengaruhi seberapa banyak masyarakat yang bersedia untuk mengeluarkan biaya sendiri demi mendapatkan vaksinasi. Hal ini tentu dapat memengaruhi tingkat kekebalan masyarakat melawan COVID-19 dalam jangka panjang.
... It is still unclear in the literature if promoting vaccination through narratives has more impact when using negative (e.g., emphasis on the risk of vaccine-preventable diseases) or positive (e.g., emphasis on the safety, efficacy and usefulness of vaccines) framing. Some studies found that negative information is more convincing while others concluded that positive messages were more credible [36,39,40]. Findings of our qualitative study indicated that parents want to be informed about the benefits and risks about vaccines and found that messages focused only on benefits of vaccination were less credible and trustworthy [41,42]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Negative information about vaccines that spreads online may contribute to parents’ vaccine hesitancy or refusal. Studies have shown that false claims about vaccines that use emotive personal narratives are more likely to be shared and engaged with on social media than factual evidence-based public health messages. The aim of this study was to explore parents’ views regarding the use of positive narratives to promote childhood vaccination. Methods We identified three ∼4-minute video narratives from social media that counter frequent parental concerns about childhood vaccination: parents and informed decision-making (online misinformation about vaccines); a paediatrician’s clinical experience with vaccine-preventable diseases (prevention of still existing diseases); and a mother’s experience with vaccine-preventable disease (risks of the disease). Focus group discussions were held with parents of children aged 0 to 5 years to assess their views on these three narratives and their general opinion on the use of narratives as a vaccine promotion intervention. Results Four focus groups discussions were virtually held with 15 parents in December 2021. In general, parents trusted both health care provider’s and parent’s narratives, but participants identified more with stories having a parent as the main character. Both narratives featuring personal stories with vaccine-preventable diseases were preferred by parents, while the story about informed decision-making was perceived as less influential. Parents expressed the need for reliable and nuanced information about vaccines and diseases and felt that a short video format featuring a story was an efficient vaccine promotion intervention. However, many mentioned that they generally are not watching such videos while navigating the Web. Conclusion While vaccine-critical stories are widely shared online, evidence on how best public health could counter these messages remains scarce. The use of narratives to promote vaccination was well-perceived by parents. Future studies are needed to assess reach and impact of such an intervention.
... DeGolia (2019) discovered that in order to acquire political and public support for environmental management, communicating within a loss framework was more effective than communicating within a gain framework [21]. Meanwhile, participants' opinions regarding vaccination were more favorable when the topic was framed in a positive format, according to a study by Altay (2020) [22]. Furthermore, punitive framing was shown to diminish information sharing willingness and affective commitment while enhancing effort-related commitment, according to the findings of Fehrenbacher (2019) [23]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Information security issues have triggered both academic and practical circles to think about operation management and the sustainable development of information systems. Based on the theory of framing effect, this study constructs a theoretical model of the presentation framework of security notice information on users’ compliance willingness and empirically tests the proposed research hypotheses using a combination of behavioral experiments and questionnaires to analyze the mechanism of the information presentation framework on compliance willingness. The results show that (1) the information presentation framework has a significant effect on users’ decision to comply, but it varies according to specific frameworks. While the attribute and risk frameworks have a significant effect on users’ decision to comply, the goal framework does not have a significant effect on users’ decision to comply. (2) The security notice situation moderates the relationship between the security notice information presentation frame and users’ compliance willingness, but this varies according to the specific situation of the specific framework. The security notice situation moderates the relationship between the attribute framework, the risk framework, and users’ compliance willingness but not the relationship between the goal framework and users’ compliance willingness. (3) Information security cognition has a moderating effect on the relationship between the security notice presentation framework and users’ compliance willingness, but it varies by the specific frameworks. Information security cognition moderates the relationship between attribute frames, risk frames, and users’ compliance willingness but not the relationship between goal frames and users’ compliance willingness.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose – From a public health perspective, vaccination programmes significantly add to long-term, safe co- existence. However, because there is no social consensus about their benefits and risks, the promotion of vaccinations is difficult. Based on Kim and Grunig’s situational theory of problem solving (STOPS), including communicative action in problem solving (CAPS), this paper both proposes a model for identifying the involvement of mothers of young children in communication regarding vaccination and advocates for a novel approach to STOPS and CAPS data analyses. Design/methodology/approach – The methodological design develops empirical analyses of the data yielded by the STOPS model. Two approaches to determining associations between situational- motivational variables and communicative-action variables in random-sample survey data obtained in Slovenia in 2016 (N 5 1704) are implemented – i.e. visual methods and multivariate agglomerative clustering algorithm. Findings – The STOPS model has been confirmed and both data-analyses approaches have shown potential by clearly demonstrating associations and patterns in the data. Based on these findings, we conclude that they have the potential to be the norm in analysing STOPS models. Research limitations/implications – Limitations of the study, which are still to be overcome, involve drawing on one sample in one country and testing only one set of indicators. Practical implications – From an academic point of view, confirmation of both the model and the analytical power of the pragmatic data-analyses methods significantly add to communication studies. From practical and social points of view, relationships among attitudes and communication behaviour, as outlined in the exposed segments of the public, enable the improvement of every step in strategic-communication planning and implementation.
Article
Full-text available
As a major concern shared by parents globally, COVID-19 vaccine safety is typically being messaged to the public in a negative frame in many countries. However, whether the COVID-19 vaccine safety framing have an effect on parents when vaccinating their children is unclear. Here we implement an online survey with a convenience sample of 3,861 parents living in mainland China, all over 18 years old and with at least one child under 18. The parents were randomly assigned to receive information about COVID-19 vaccine safety in either a negative frame (incidence of side effects) or a positive frame (the inverse incidence of side effects), to compare parental reactions to a range of questions about communication, risk perception, trust, involvement and behavioral intention. We found that parents were more likely to regard vaccine safety as relevant to policy support and as a higher priority for government when receiving positively framed information (p = 0.002). For some specific subgroups, parents in positive framing group showed lower risk perception and higher trust (p<0.05). This suggests that positive framing of COVID-19 vaccine safety messages show more effective performance than negative framing in terms of involvement, as well as trust and risk perception in specific subgroups, which may lead to a reflection on whether to adjust the current widespread use of negative framing. Our findings inform how governments and health care workers strategically choose the framing design of COVID-19 vaccine safety information, and have important implications for promoting COVID-19 vaccination in children in the future.
Article
One of the most pervasive findings in attribute framing research is the valence consistent shift; that is, positively valenced frames (e.g., 95% natural ingredients) are preferred over semantically equivalent but negatively valenced frames (e.g., 5% artificial ingredients). Despite the robustness of this finding, it has primarily been observed in judgments of prospective or hypothetical consumption. When valenced frames are presented during or immediately prior to an actual consumption experience, evidence for the valence consistent shift is weaker and less conclusive. In the present research, we propose and show that individuals' susceptibility to a valenced frame encountered around the time of a related consumption experience depends on whether they focus primarily on their cognitions or their emotions during the experience. Specifically, five experiments provide evidence that the valence consistent shift is attenuated in visual, auditory, and (simulated) gustatory consumption contexts when individuals are prompted to rely more on affective (vs. cognitive) inputs. Implications for both theory and practice are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Despite the spectacular success of vaccines in preventing infectious diseases, fears about their safety and other anti-vaccination claims are widespread. To better understand how such fears and claims persist and spread, we must understand how they are perceived and recalled. One influence on the perception and recall of vaccination-related information might be universal cognitive biases acting against vaccination. An omission bias describes the tendency to perceive as worse, and recall better, bad outcomes resulting from commissions (e.g. vaccine side effects) compared to the same bad outcomes resulting from omissions (e.g. symptoms of vaccine preventable diseases). Another factor influencing the perception and recall of vaccination-related information might be people’s attitudes towards vaccines. A confirmation bias would mean that pre-existing pro-vaccination attitudes positively predict perceptions of severity and recall of symptoms of vaccine preventable diseases and negatively predict perceptions of severity and recall of vaccine side effects. To test for these hypothesized biases, 202 female participants aged 18–60 (M = 38.15, SD = 10.37) completed an online experiment with a between-subjects experimental design. Participants imagined that they had a 1-year old child who suffered from either vaccine side effects (Commission Condition) or symptoms of a vaccine-preventable disease (Omission Condition). They then rated a list of symptoms/side effects for their perceived severity on a 7-point Likert scale. Finally, they completed a surprise recall test in which they recalled the symptoms/side effects previously rated. An additional scale was used to measure their attitudes towards vaccines. Contrary to the hypotheses, perceptions of severity and the recall of symptoms/side effects were not associated with experimental condition, failing to support the omission bias, nor did they interact with attitudes towards vaccines, failing to support the confirmation bias. This cast doubt on the possibility that the spread of anti-vaccination claims can be explained by these particular universal cognitive biases.
Article
Full-text available
There is an increasing imperative for psychologists and other behavioral scientists to understand how people behave on social media. However, it is often very difficult to execute experimental research on actual social media platforms, or to link survey responses to online behavior in order to perform correlational analyses. Thus, there is a natural desire to use self-reported behavioral intentions in standard survey studies to gain insight into online behavior. But are such hypothetical responses hopelessly disconnected from actual sharing decisions? Or are online survey samples via sources such as Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) so different from the average social media user that the survey responses of one group give little insight into the on-platform behavior of the other? Here we investigate these issues by examining 67 pieces of political news content. We evaluate whether there is a meaningful relationship between (i) the level of sharing (tweets and retweets) of a given piece of content on Twitter, and (ii) the extent to which individuals (total N = 993) in online surveys on MTurk reported being willing to share that same piece of content. We found that the same news headlines that were more likely to be hypothetically shared on MTurk were also shared more frequently by Twitter users, r = .44. For example, across the observed range of MTurk sharing fractions, a 20 percentage point increase in the fraction of MTurk participants who reported being willing to share a news headline on social media was associated with 10x as many actual shares on Twitter. We also found that the correlation between sharing and various features of the headline was similar using both MTurk and Twitter data. These findings suggest that self-reported sharing intentions collected in online surveys are likely to provide some meaningful insight into what content would actually be shared on social media.
Article
Full-text available
Climate change is an urgent global issue, with demands for personal , collective, and governmental action. Although a large body of research has investigated the influence of communication on public engagement with climate change, few studies have investigated the role of interpersonal discussion. Here we use panel data with 2 time points to investigate the role of climate conversations in shaping beliefs and feelings about global warming. We find evidence of reciprocal causality. That is, discussing global warming with friends and family leads people to learn influential facts, such as the scientific consensus that human-caused global warming is happening. In turn, stronger perceptions of scientific agreement increase beliefs that climate change is happening and human-caused, as well as worry about climate change. When assessing the reverse causal direction, we find that knowing the scientific consensus further leads to increases in global warming discussion. These findings suggest that climate conversations with friends and family enter people into a proclimate social feedback loop.
Article
Full-text available
Moral stories are pervasive in human culture, forming the basis of religious texts, folklore, and newspaper articles. We used a linear transmission chain procedure to test three competing hypotheses: (1) that moral content in general is preferentially transmitted between individuals compared to non-moral content; (2) that negativity bias leads specifically to morally bad content being preferentially transmitted; and (3) that a bias towards pro-social information leads specifically to morally good content being preferentially transmitted. While we found no support for a bias for moral content in general, we did find that morally good content was transmitted with greater fidelity than neutral or morally bad content, with ratings of morally good content but not morally bad content predicting transmission. Moral content, therefore, appears to be particularly culturally potent when it describes the ‘virtuous’ rather than the ‘sinful’. A second study repeated the first but also tested the influence of physiological arousal on transmission by measuring the electrodermal activity of participants. This study also found that morally good content was transmitted with greater fidelity than neutral or morally bad content and that physiological arousal had a negative effect on transmission with more arousing material being less faithfully transmitted. These results suggest that the communication of content relating to moral virtue might serve to avoid negative impression formation and promote social bonding, and that this might partially explain the ubiquity of moral content in human culture.
Article
Full-text available
While vaccine-critical activism has been widely documented and discussed, comparatively little has been said about the concerted response of pro-vaccine activists defending the majority view. This paper explores two case studies of pro-vaccine activism in Australia and the United States (US). It shows how pro-vaccine views and behaviours can take varying forms due to different aims and methods of engagement – oppositional counteractivities in favour of vaccination in Australia, and issue-based advocacy as part of a political alliance in the US. The focus in Australia comes from a pro-science stance and includes 'skeptics’ against pseudoscience directly opposing vaccine-critical groups. In the US, the focus takes the form of an issue-specific campaign that has arisen from existing pro-vaccine parent blogs and discussion groups pushing for policy change rather than public confrontation. These case studies exemplify how pro-vaccine activism can take varying forms of either reinforcing the mainstream view or countering digression from it. Drawing on qualitative research, this paper aims to examine the types of practices and strategies employed by activists to voice their support of vaccination, and discusses the means, messages, and motivations of pro-vaccine activism. It ends with an argument for why a study – of public support for in addition to studying public opposition to vaccination – can help to better understand vaccination views and behaviours. These findings have wider implications for the study of counter-activism and the polarisation of civil society groups.
Article
Full-text available
In the last decade, cultural transmission experiments (transmission chains, replacement, closed groups and seeded groups) have become important experimental tools in investigating cultural evolution. However, these methods face important challenges, especially regarding the operationalization of theoretical claims. In this review, we focus on the study of cumulative cultural evolution, the process by which traditions are gradually modified and, for technological traditions in particular, improved upon over time. We identify several mismatches between theoretical definitions of cumulative culture and their implementation in cultural transmission experiments. We argue that observed performance increase can be the result of participants learning faster in a group context rather than effectively leading to a cumulative effect. We also show that in laboratory experiments, participants are asked to complete quite simple tasks, which can undermine the evidential value of the diagnostic criterion traditionally used for cumulative culture (i.e. that cumulative culture is a process that produces solutions that no single individual could have invented on their own). We show that the use of unidimensional metrics of cumulativeness drastically curtail the variation that may be observed, which raises specific issues in the interpretation of the experimental evidence. We suggest several solutions to these mismatches (learning times, task complexity and variation) and develop the use of design spaces in experimentally investigating old and new questions about cumulative culture.
Article
Full-text available
People’s inability to update their memories in light of corrective information may have important public health consequences, as in the case of vaccination choice. In the present study, we compare three potentially effective strategies in vaccine promotion: one contrasting myths vs. facts, one employing fact and icon boxes, and one showing images of non-vaccinated sick children. Beliefs in the autism/vaccines link and in vaccines side effects, along with intention to vaccinate a future child, were evaluated both immediately after the correction intervention and after a 7-day delay to reveal possible backfire effects. Results show that existing strategies to correct vaccine misinformation are ineffective and often backfire, resulting in the unintended opposite effect, reinforcing ill-founded beliefs about vaccination and reducing intentions to vaccinate. The implications for research on vaccines misinformation and recommendations for progress are discussed.
Article
Research on social transmission suggests that people preferentially transmit information about threats and social interactions. Such biases might be driven by the arousal that is experienced as part of the emotional response triggered by information about threats or social relationships. The current studies tested whether preferences for transmitting threat-relevant information are consistent with a functional motive to recruit social support. USA residents were recruited for six online studies. Studies 1a and 1B showed that participants more often chose to transmit positive, low-arousal vignettes (rather than negative, high-arousal vignettes involving threats and social interactions). Studies 2A and 2B showed higher intentions to transmit emotional vignettes (triggering disgust, fear, anger, or sadness) to friends (rather than to strangers or disliked acquaintances). Study 4 showed a preference for transmitting stories that participants had modified and were therefore novel and unique. Studies 2A and 3 (but not Studies 2B and 4) suggest that motivations for seeking social support might influence transmission preferences. Overall, the findings are not easily accounted for by any of the major theories of social transmission. We discuss limitations of the current studies and directions for further research.
Article
Rationale: Although vaccines are an invaluable weapon in combatting diseases, they are often surrounded by controversy. Vaccine controversies usually arise with the claims of some parents or doctors who link vaccines to harmful outcomes. These controversies often negatively affect vaccination coverage. Objectives: This experiment simulated a vaccine controversy to understand which content features of vaccination-related information are well transmitted and how this transmission affects vaccine intention. Method: All participants (N = 64) read two conflicting views (pro- and anti-) about a fictional vaccine ('dipherpox vaccine'). These conflicting views were held by a parent and a doctor, whose views varied across conditions. This information was transmitted along linear chains of four participants who recalled it and the product of their recall was passed to the next participant within their chain. They also responded whether they would vaccinate or not. Results: The experience-based view held by the parent was better transmitted than the medical-based view held by the doctor, while the pro-vaccine and anti-vaccine views were similarly transmitted. Despite all the participants having neutral or positive attitudes towards vaccines in general, 39.1% of them decided not to vaccinate. Nevertheless, vaccination attitude was the strongest predictor of vaccination intention. The less positive participants' attitudes were towards vaccines in general, the less likely they were to vaccinate against dipherpox after exposure to the controversy. Conclusion: The results suggest that vaccination campaigns may be made more effective by including personal experiences of the negative consequences of non-vaccination.