Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Preprint:Pleasenotethatthisarticlehasnotcompletedpeerreview.
Anobservationalstudyofrecessqualityandphysical
activityinurbanprimaryschools
CURRENTSTATUS:UNDERREVIEW
WilliamVincentMassey
OregonStateUniversity
william.massey@oregonstate.eduCorrespondingAuthor
ORCiD:https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4002-3720
MeganBStellino
UniversityofNorthernColorado
JohnGeldhof
OregonStateUniversity
DOI:
10.21203/rs.2.24385/v1
SUBJECTAREAS
HealthPolicy HealthEconomics&OutcomesResearch
KEYWORDS
Schoolhealth,Adultengagement,Play,Obesity
2
Abstract
Background
Todate,thereisscantliteraturethatexaminestherecesscontextconcurrentwith,butseparate
from,levelsofphysicalactivity.Theprimarypurposeofthecurrentstudywastoexaminehowrecess
qualityimpactedphysicalactivitylevels,andhowthiswasmoderatedbygender.Asecondary
purposewastoexamineifdifferencesinchildren’sengagementinactivitiesoccurredbetweenrecess
sessionsscoredaslow-orhigh-quality.
Methods
Thiswasanobservationalstudyofchildrenat13urbanelementaryschoolsintheU.S.Acrossthe13
schools,datawerecollectedat55recesssessions,with3,419child-levelobservations(n=1,696
boys;n=1,723girls).PhysicalactivitydatawerecollectedusingFitbitaccelerometers,recessquality
datawerecollectedusingtheGreatRecessFramework–ObservationalTool(GRF-OT),recess
engagementdatawerecollectedusingtheObservationofPlaygroundPlay(OPP),andbasic
psychologicalneedsatisfaction(BPNS)datawerecollectedusingamodifiedversionoftheBPNSfor
recessphysicalactivitysurvey.PrimaryanalyseswereconductedusingHierarchicalLinearModeling
(HLM)withchildrennestedwithinrecesssessions.
Results
Gendermoderatedtherelationshipbetweenadultengagementandmoderate-to-vigorousphysical
activity(MVPA)(b=.012;95%CI.001,.024),studentbehaviorandMVPA(b=-.014;95%CI-.021,
-.007),andstudentbehaviorsandlightphysicalactivity(b=.009,95%CI.003,.015).Bothboysand
girlsengagedinmoreplayduringrecesssessionsscoredashighqualityontheGRF-OT.Children
reportedhigherlevelsofbasicpsychologicalneedsatisfactionatrecessessessionsscoredashigh
qualityontheGRF-OT.
Conclusions
Resultsofthecurrentstudyshowedthatthequalityoftherecessenvironment,andtheinteractions
ofbothadultsandstudentsinthatenvironment,needtobetakenintoconsiderationinfutureschool-
basedrecessstudies.
3
Background
Lowlevelsofphysicalactivity(PA)remainaproblemthatcontributetothehighobesityratesseenin
children.Increasingly,schoolshavebecomeafocusforphysicalactivityinterventionsamongst
researchers(1)particularlygiventheamountoftimechildrenspendinthisenvironment.
Unfortunately,childrenintheUnitedStates(U.S.)continuetolackmeaningfulopportunitiesfor
physicalactivityatschool,despiteresearchthatshowstimespentengaginginphysicalactivity
makespositivecontributionstoacademics(2).AsidefromPE,school-basedrecessisalsoaprime
opportunityforphysicalactivitywithintheschoolday.Specifically,recesshasbeenshowntoaccount
for42%ofchildren’sopportunitiestobephysicallyactiveinschool(3),andupto44%ofstepcounts
duringtheschoolday(4).Despitethis,datafromthe2012–2013academicyearintheUnitedStates
(U.S.)suggestthat60%ofschooldistrictshavenoformalpolicyregardingdailyrecess.Ofschool-
districtsthathavearecesspolicy,only22%requiredailyrecessforelementaryschoolstudents,with
lessthanhalfoftheserequiringatleast20minutesofrecessperday(5).Whilerecentlegislative
effortshavebeenmadetopromoterecessatthestatelevel,onlysevenoutof50U.S.statesrequire
dailyrecessforchildrenduringtheschoolday(6).
Perhapsmoreconcerningisthetrendsdescribedabovedisproportionallyaffectchildrenfrom
disadvantagedbackgrounds.Datashowthatchildrenwhogotolarge,urbanschools;schoolswitha
highminoritypopulation;andschoolswithlow-incomelevelsaretheleastlikelytogetaccessto
recess,andoftenreporttheshortestamountoftimededicatedtorecess(7,8).Itisplausiblethat
environmentalfactorsaffectchildren’saccesstophysicalactivityopportunitiesinurbanandlow-
incomeschoolsystems.Notably,bullyingandaggressivebehaviorhavebeenreportedonthe
playgroundaturbanelementaryschools(9,10),whichcouldresultinfeweropportunitiesforstudents.
Furthermore,accesstospaceandequipmentarethoughttobecentraltorecessfacilitation(11),
whichcouldbelackingatlow-income,and/orurbanschools.Forexample,asystematicreview
conductedbyErwinetal.(4)suggeststhataddingmoreplaygroundequipmentandprovidinga
structuredrecessyieldsthelargesteffectonphysicalactivityduringrecess.Yet,budgetaryrestraints
couldlimitthepurchaseofequipmentinlow-incomeschooldistricts.Thus,thereisaneedtoconsider
4
bothaccesstorecess,andthequalityoftherecessenvironmentforpromotingphysicalactivity
throughouttheschoolday,particularlyinlow-incomeandurbanenvironments.
Asidefromaddressingdisparitiestophysicalactivityopportunitiessuchasrecess,aneedalsoexists
toexaminebarriersto,andfacilitatorsfor,physicalactivitywhenchildrendohaveaccessto
discretionarytimeduringtheschoolday.Onecommonareaoffocushasbeenondifferencesbetween
activitylevelsforboysandgirls,giventhatdatahasconsistentlyshownthatgirlsarelessactive
duringrecessperiods(12–14).Thesedatasuggestthatsocialdeterminantsmightplayarolein
children’sbehaviorontheplayground.Inexaminingbarrierstophysicalactivity,Pawlowskiand
colleagues(15,16)reportedseveralgenderedissuesthatmaylimitphysicalactivityduringrecess.
Forexample,elementaryschoolgirlsreportedwantingtoplaysports(i.e.,ballgames),butthose
wereusuallydominatedbytheboysontheplayground(15).Moreover,activitiesatrecesscanbe
labeledalonggenderlines,withgirlsbeingexpectedtoconformtomoresedentaryactivities(16).
Whilegirlsseemmoreinterestedincrossinggenderbordersatrecess(17),boysinthePawlowskiet
al.study(16)dominatedthelargerplaygroundspaces(i.e.,footballpitch)oftenexcludinggirlsduring
thegame.Thus,itappearsthatthereisaneedforbothvarietyofgamesandplayspaces,aswellas
inclusivebehaviorontheplayground,inanefforttomoreeffectivelypromotephysicalactivityat
recess.Indeed,researchershavereportedgirlsengageinsimilarlevelsofmoderate-to-vigorous
physicalactivity(MVPA)asboyswhenplayingteamsports(14)andthatprovidinganactivityofthe
weekinterventioncanyieldgainsinphysicalactivity(18).
Anotherimportantconsiderationforexaminingbehavioratrecessishowperceptionsofphysicaland
emotionalsafetymightimpactphysicalactivitybehaviorduringrecess.Previousresearchwithchild
participantshassuggestedthatconflictisaregularpartoftheplaygroundexperience(19,20).
Similarly,behavioralobservationshavesuggestedthatbullyingregularlytakesplaceduringrecess
periods(10).Intermsofphysicalactivityduringrecess,childrenhavespecificallydiscussedconflict
asabarriertoplayforboysandgirlsalike.Moreover,childrenhavereportedthatastrongerpresence
byadultsatrecessmighthelptofacilitatehigherlevelsofplay,astherewouldbeamoreneutral
mediatortoweighinongames(16).Thisnotionwassupportedbyarecentstudythatshowedadult
5
engagementandsupervisionasasignificantpredictorofplayforboysandgirlsduringrecess(21).
Additionalworkhasshownthatthepresenceofrecessinterventionschangesplaypatterns,
specificallyreducingnon-engagementandincreasingengagementinmoretraditionalgamessuchas
four-square,hopscotch,anduseoflooseequipment(22)
Inconsideringthepotentialbarrierstophysicalactivityatrecess,multipleinteractingfactorsareat
play.Accesstoresources,schoolpolicies,thephysicalenvironment,varioussocialdeterminants,and
studentbehaviorsallplayvariousrolesinfacilitating,orimpeding,physicalactivityatrecess.
Understandingoptimalstandardsforfacilitatingphysicalactivityduringrecessiscurrentlyneeded,
especiallyforthoseinurbanand/orlow-incomeschoolswhomayhavelimitedopportunities.Todate,
thereisscantliteraturethatexaminestherecesscontextconcurrentwith,butseparatefrom,levels
ofphysicalactivity.Recently,Masseyandcolleagues(23)developedanobservationaltooltomeasure
environmentalandsocialdeterminantsofelementaryschoolrecess.Specifically,theGreatRecess
Framework–ObservationalTool(GRF-OT)measuresthesafetyandstructureoftheplayground,adult
engagementandsupervision,aswellasstudentbehaviors.GiventheoverlapbetweentheGRF-OT
andresearchingexaminingpotentialbarriersandfacilitatorstorecessphysicalactivity,thereisa
needtoexaminehowtheabove-mentionedcontextualfactors,asmeasuredbytheGRF-OT,impact
physicalactivitylevels.Assuch,theprimarypurposeofthecurrentstudywastoexaminehowrecess
qualityimpactedphysicalactivitylevels,andhowthiswasmoderatedbygender.Asecondary
purposewastoexamineifdifferencesinchildren’sengagementinactivitiesoccurredbetweenrecess
sessionsscoredaslow-orhigh-quality.
Methods
InstitutionalReviewBoardapprovalwasobtainedpriortothestartofanystudyprocedures
(ConcordiaUniversityWisconsinID:932380-3;926512-1).Additionally,allprotocolsandprocedures
wereapprovedbytheresearchboardattheschooldistrictlevel,aswellastheprincipalateach
school.Inanefforttoensurewecouldincludeamaximumnumberofchildrenontheplayground,a
passiveconsentprotocolwasfollowed.Consentformsweresenthometoeachstudentandparents
andstudentsweregiventheopportunitytooptoutofthecurrentstudy.Inaccordancewithschool
6
districtpolicyonpassiveconsentprotocols,personallyidentifyinginformationwasnotcollectedon
participants.
Participants
Participantsincludedchildrenat13urbanelementaryschoolsintheU.S.Acrossthe13schools,
accelerometerdatawerecollectedat55recesssessions,with3,419child-levelobservations(n =
1,696boys;n = 1,723girls).Observationsofengagementinrecessactivitieswerecollectedat61
recesssessionswith4,528child-levelobservations(n = 2243boys;n = 2285girls).Thenumberof
childrenwithineachrecesssessionrangedfrom12to117withanaverageof62.16(SD = 26.34)
childrenperrecess.Allschoolsenrolledinthisstudyservedchildreninlow-incomeareas,with
publiclyavailabledatashowingthat78.8%ofthestudentpopulationiseconomicallydisadvantaged
(12-out-of-13schools > 50%ofeconomicallydisadvantagedstudents;range = 22.5%− 99.6%).Ofthe
13schools,10wereinthepublic-schoolsystem,withthreeofthese10schoolsbeinglanguage
immersionschools.Twelveofthe13schoolswereexclusivelyelementaryschools(i.e.,grades1–5),
andoneschoolalsoservedmiddleschoolstudents(i.e.,grades4–8).Enrollmentateachschool
rangedfrom253studentsto690students,withanaverageof436studentsperschool.
Measures
Physicalactivity.TheFitbitFlex™isawristworntriaxialaccelerometerthatusesproprietary
algorithmstoestimatestepscountsandtimespentinvariousactivitylevels.TheFitbitFlexprovides
themostsimplisticuserdisplayofallFitbitproducts,withonlyLEDlightstorepresentprogress
towardsdailygoals(thedefaultsettingis10,000steps;2000stepsperdotshown).Thiswasthought
tobeadvantageous,asparticipantswouldnotbeabletodirectlymonitortheirstepcountsofphysical
activitylevelsduringrecess.TheFitbitFlexcanbysynchedwirelesslytoasmartphoneortablet,and
providesinformationonstepscountsandtimespendinvariousactivitylevels(i.e.,sedentary,light,
moderate,vigorous).Forthepurposesofthecurrentstudy,theresearchteamcreatedanonymous
accountsforeachdevicethatcouldonlybeaccessedbytheresearchteam.Eachaccountwas
assignedtoeitheramaleorfemaleuser,withnationalaveragesforheightandweightbeingusedfor
userdemographicinformation.Fitbitswereplacedonstudentsintheirclassroom,orinthelunch
7
room,approximately30minutespriortothestartofrecess.Dataassessorsrecordedtheexactstart
andstoptimesofrecesssothatdatacouldbeextractedtomatchthetimestamp.Datawerehoused
byathird-partyvendor(FitabaseLLC,SanDiego,California)andprocessedusing60secondepochs
withinthenotedtimestamp,themostsensitivesettingavailableforthisdevice.Inchild-based
studies,bothwaist-worn(24)andwristworn(25)Fitbitdevices(FitbitOneandFitbitCharge,
respectively)havebeenshowntohaveconsistentlevelsofstepcountswithActigraph
accelerometers,yetmayover-estimateabsolutenumberofsteps,aswellastimespentinMVPA.
Additionalresearchinyoungadultpopulationshasshownmoderatevaliditybetweenthewrist-worn
FitbitFlexandthewrist-wornActigraphGT3X + infree-livingconditions(26),yettheFitbitflex
showedhigherlevelsofvariability,andwasmorelikelytounder-estimatedactivitylevels.
Recessquality.TheGreatRecessFramework–ObservationalTool(GRF-OT)wasusedasameasureof
recessqualityinthecurrentstudy.TheGRF-OTrepresentsfourdomainsofrecessthatincludesafety
andstructureoftheplayground,adultsupervisionandengagement,studentbehaviors,and
transitionstoandfromtherecessspace(23).Inthecurrentstudydatawascollectedonthreeofthe
foursub-scales(transitionswereexcluded,astheyaccountforthetimesimmediatelybeforeand
afterrecessandthefocusofthecurrentstudywasPAduringrecess).Itemsarescoredona4-point
scalebyaliveobserverwhowaspresentatrecess(4 = highestquality;1 = lowestquality).Thesafety
andstructuresub-scaleexaminesthephysicalenvironmentandaccesstoequipment;theadult
engagementandsupervisionsub-scaleexaminesthenumberofadultspresent,theirproximityto
students,andwhetherornottheyengagewithstudentsontheplayground;andthestudent
behaviorssub-scalesexaminesstudentengagement,initiationofplay,conflict,andconflict
resolution.EachitemanditsassociatedscoringprocedurecanbefoundinMasseyetal.2018(20).A
completescoringmanualwithdetailedinstructionsisavailableatwww.greatrecessframework.org.
Datainthecurrentstudysuggestacceptablelevelsofinternalconsistentforthesafetyandstructure
sub-scale(α = .806),adultengagementandsupervisionsub-scale(α = .736),andstudentbehavior
sub-scale(α = .788).Previousresearchhasshownsupportfortheinter-raterreliabilityandfactorial
validityoftheGRF-OT(23).
8
Engagementinrecessactivities.Thedifferenttypesofactivitieschildrenengagedinduringrecess
weremeasuredusingtheObservationsofPlaygroundPlay(OPP)(22).TheOPPallowsobservesto
codeengagementin32commonrecessactivitiesacrosseightdifferentplaydomains.Observersare
alsoabletowriteinobservedbehaviorswithineachoftheeightdomainstoensureallrecess
activitiesarecaptured.Previousresearchhasbeenreportedonthereliabilityofthisassessmenttool
(22).
Basicpsychologicalneedssatisfaction(BPNS).Asub-sampleoffourthandfifthgradestudents(n =
820)completedamodifiedversionofthebasicpsychologicalneedsatisfactionscale(27).Theoriginal
21-itemquestionnairedesignedtoassessindividualperceptionsofautonomy(7items),competence
(6items),andrelatedness(8items)needsatisfactionatworkwasmodifiedtospecificallyexamine
children’sneedsatisfactionduringrecessandreducedtotwo-itemsperscaletoensure
comprehensionforayoungerpopulation.Additionally,twoitemsrelatedtophysicalandemotional
safetyatrecesswereaddedtothescale.Allresponsescorrespondedtoa5-pointLikertscale(5 =
highneedsatisfaction)onitemssuchas“IfeellikeIcansaymyideasaboutwhatIwanttodoat
recess”(autonomy),“KidstellmeIamgoodatthingsIdoatrecess”(competence),“Ireallylikethe
kidsIplaywithatrecess”(relatedness),and“IamsafewhenIamplayingatrecess”(safety).
Procedures
Withtheexceptionofoneschoolthatconductedconcurrentindoorandoutdoorrecessperiods,all
recessdatacollectionperiodswereconductedoutside.DatacollectiontookplacebetweenFebruary
andMayinalargecityintheMidwesternregionoftheUnitedStates.Recessperiodsrangedfrom12
minutesto40minutesinlength(M = 21.12minutes;SD = 5.83minutes)andprimarilyincluded
traditionallunchrecessperiods.Schoolsmaintainedvariableschedules,withsomeschoolssending
groupsofstudentsoutsideallatonce,whileothersrotatedthesessionswithdifferentchildrenand
differentsupervisors(e.g.,onlyfirstthroughthirdgradersatrecessone,followedbyonlyfourthand
fifthgradersatrecesstwo).
Outcomeassessorsarrivedtotheschoolapproximately60minutesbeforethescheduledrecess
sessiontoensurestudentswereproperlyfittedwithactivitymonitoringdevices.Eachdatacollection
9
periodcontainedfourstudyteammembers.Twomembersoftheteamwereassignedtoensure
complianceintermsofproperlywearingtheactivitymonitoringdevices.Theothertwoteam
memberscollectedobservationaldatathroughouttheentirerecesssession.DatausingtheGRF-OT
werecollectedbythePIoratrainedgraduatestudent.Inallcases,therecessenvironmentwas
completelyvisibletotheoutcomeassessor,andtheoutcomeassessormovedthroughoutthe
playgroundinadiscreetmannerinanefforttoobservepatternsofinteractionandbehavior.Final
scoringofeachitemwascompletedimmediatelyaftertherecesssessionandtookintoaccountthe
aggregatepatternsofbehaviorthroughoutthedurationoftherecesssession.DatausingtheOPP
werecollectedatfive-minuteintervalsacrosseachrecessperiod.OPPdatawerethenaveragedto
createacompositelevelofstudentengagementindifferentactivitiesforeachrecesssession.
DataAnalysis
Priortodataanalysis,FitbitFlexdatawerescreenedanddevicesregistering0stepcountsinthe
recesstimerecordingwereeliminatedfromthedataset.Timerecordingsofthebeginningandending
ofeachrecesssessionwerekepttoallowforspecificityindataextractionwhenexaminingrecess-
basedphysicalactivity.Furthermore,Fitbitnumberswereloggedandtrackedforeachrecesssession
toensurewhichdeviceswereinuseforeachsession,andwhichdeviceswerereturnedattheendof
eachrecesssession.Giventhevaryingtimesandnumberofstudentsacrossrecesssessions,we
convertedphysicalactivitydatatothepercentoftimespentinMVPAorlightphysicalactivity(LPA)
duringrecessandusedthesepercentagesasthedependentvariableinprimaryanalyses.
PrimaryanalyseswereconductedusingHierarchicalLinearModeling(HLM)withchildrennested
withinrecesssessions.Interceptsfreelyvariedacrossrecesssessions,whileallprimarypredictors
wereenteredasfixedeffects.Anunconditionalnestedmodelwasfirsttestedtoexaminepossible
recess-leveleffectsforalldependentvariables(i.e.,physicalactivitylevels).Next,modelswerefitted
inwhichrecessqualityscoreindicators(i.e.,adultengagementandsupervision,studentbehaviors,
safetyandstructure)wereenteredaspredictorsofphysicalactivitylevelswhilecontrollingforschool
asafixedeffect.Moderationwasalsotestedbyexaminingtheinteractionbetweengender(alevel
onepredictor)andrecessqualityindicators(aleveltwopredictor)onlevelsofphysicalactivityduring
10
recess.Significantinteractionswereprobedtoexaminethesimpleslopesandinterceptsasafunction
ofgenderusingtheformuladepictedbelowandasdescribedbyPreacherandcolleagues(28),where
ý00representstheintercept,ý01,ý10andý11aretheregressioncoefficients,xrepresentsthefocal
predictor,andwrepresentsthemoderatorvariable.
yij=ý00+ý10x1+ý01w1+ý11w1x1
Finally,trendsacrossrecessqualitywereexaminedrelativetohigh-andlow-qualityrecesssessions
(i.e.,oneSDaboveandoneSDbelowthesamplemean).Ingeneral,high-qualityrecesssessionswere
characterizedbysafephysicalenvironments(e.g.,lackofhazardousmaterials),abroadrangeof
equipmentandactivitiestoengageinplay,prosocialstudentbehaviors(e.g.,initiatinggames,
positivecommunication,lackofphysicalviolence)andpresentandengagedadults.Incontrast,low-
qualityrecesssessionswereoftencharacterizedasunsafeenvironments(e.g.,glassandhazardous
debris),limitedornoequipmenttouseforgameplay,verbalandphysicalconflicts,anddisengaged
adults.AggregateprofilesofrecesssessionsintheupperandlowerquartileforGRF-OTscoreswere
createdtoexaminedifferentialpatternsinthegamesandactivitiesinwhichchildrenparticipatein,
andthepsychologicalneedsatisfactionofchildrenduringrecess.
Results
DescriptivestatisticswerecalculatedforallvariablesunderstudyandcanbefoundinTable1.
ResultsofthenullmodelscanbefoundinTable2(MVPA)andTable3(LPA).
11
Table1
Descriptivestatistics.
Variable Mean SD Range(possiblerange)
TotalRecessQualityScore 41.15 7.44 19–54(14–56)
SafetyandStructureof
Environment 14.96 3.49 6–20(5–20)
AdultEngagementand
Supervision 10.61 2.49 5–16(4–16)
StudentBehaviors 15.58 3.37 5–20(5–20)
PhysicalActivity
Percentoftimespentin
MVPA 50.54% 35.96% 0-100%
Percentoftimespentin
LPA 36.42% 29.20% 0-100%
PsychologicalNeed
Satisfaction
34.34 4.74 12–40(10–40)
Autonomy 7.94 1.91 2–10(2–10)
Competence 6.77 1.84 2–10(2–10)
Relatedness 8.23 1.68 2–10(2–10)
Safety 8.41 1.67 2–10(2–10)
EngagementinRecess
Activities(percentof
students)
Equipment 0% 1% 0–8%
Organizedactivities 30% 24% 0–92%
Anti-socialbehavior 1% 3% 0–22%
Non-engagedinplay 30% 22% 0–84%
Nature 2% 6% 0–34%
Running/chasinggames 17% 15% 0–76%
Traditionalplayground
games 16% 14% 0–68%
Roughandtumbleplay 2% 7% 0–52%
Table2
UnconditionalnestedmodelforpercentoftimespentinMVPAduringrecess
Estimationofcovarianceparameters
Parameter Estimate s.e.WaldZ p-value 95%CIof
the
estimate
ICC
Residual .122 .003 40.99 < .001 0.116,
0.128
InterceptVariance
(RecessSession) .009 .002 3.97 < .001 .005,.014 0.066
Table3
UnconditionalnestedmodelforpercentoftimespentinLPAduringrecess
Estimationofcovarianceparameters
Parameter Estimate s.e.WaldZ p-value 95%CIof
the
estimate
ICC
Residual .082 .002 41.00 < .001 .078,.086
InterceptVariance
(RecessSession) .003 .001 3.59 < .001 .002,.006 0.041
AnexaminationofpredictorsofthepercentoftimespentinMVPAatrecessshowedgenderasthe
onlysignificantpredictorinthecurrentstudy(p = .001).However,moderationanalysesrevealedthat
gendermoderatedtherelationshipbetweenadultengagementandMVPA(p = .046),andstudent
12
behaviorandMVPA(p = < .001).ResultscanbefoundinTable4.Simpleslopesanalysesindicated
thatgenderwasnotasignificantpredictorofpercentoftimespentinMVPAatlow(M-1SD;b =
− .116;p = .107),moderate(M;b = − .085;p = .287)orhigh(M + 1SD;b = − .055,p = .542)levelsof
adultengagementandsupervision.However,ascanbeseeninFig.1,thedifferencebetweenboys’
andgirls’percentoftimeinMVPAwasminimizedashigherlevelsofadultengagementand
supervisionwereobserved.Inexaminingstudentbehaviors,simpleslopesanalysesrevealedthat
genderwasasignificantpredictorofpercenttimeinMVPAatlow(M-1SD;b = − .381;p = < .001),
moderate(M;b = − .429;p = < .001)andhigh(M + 1SD;b = − .477,p = < .001)levelsofprosocial
studentbehavior.AscanbeseeninFig.2,boys’percentoftimeinMVPAwashigheratrecess
sessionsinwhichmoreprosocialstudentbehaviorswereobserved;whereasgirls’percentoftimein
MVPAwasloweratrecesssessionsinwhichmoreprosocialstudentbehaviorswereobserved.
Table4
EstimatesofeffectsonpercentofMVPAduringrecess
Estimationofcovarianceparameters
Parameter Estimate s.e.WaldZ p-value 95%CIoftheestimate
Residual .110 .003 40.99 < .001 .105,.115
Intercept
Variance
(recess)
.009 .002 4.12 < .001 .006,.014
Estimationoffixedeffects
Parameter Estimate s.e.Ttest
statistic
p-value 95%CIoftheestimate
Intercept .641 .093 6.87 < .001 .454,.827
Gender − .215 .065 -3.32 .001 − .341,− .088
School − .002 .005 − .365 .717 − .012,.008
Safetyand
Structureof
Environment
− .002 .006 − .380 .705 − .014,.010
Adult
Engagement
and
Supervision
− .003 .008 − .354 .724 − .018,.013
Student
behaviors .003 .006 .519 .605 − .009,.015
Gender*
Safetyand
Structureof
Environment
.006 .004 1.32 .186 − .003,.014
Gender*
Adult
Engagement
and
Supervision
.012 .006 1.99 .046 .001,.024
Gender*
Student
behaviors
− .014 .004 -3.725 < .001 − .021,− .007
InexaminingLPA,genderwasonceagaintheonlysignificantpredictorofLPA(p = .004).Genderalso
moderatedtherelationshipbetweenstudentbehaviorsandLPA(p = .005).Resultscanbefoundin
13
Table5.Simpleslopesanalysesrevealedthatgenderwasasignificantpredictorofpercenttimein
LPAatlow(M-1SD;b = .256;p = < .001),moderate(M;b = .286;p = < .001)andhigh(M + 1SD;b
= .317,p = < .001)levelsofprosocialstudentbehavior.Specifically,girlsrecordedhigherlevelsof
LPAduringrecesssessionswithhighlevelsofprosocialstudentbehavior;whereasboysrecorded
lowerlevelsofLPAduringrecesssessionswithhighlevelsofprosocialstudentbehavior(seeFig.3).
Table5
EstimatesofeffectsonpercentofLPAduringrecess
Estimationofcovarianceparameters
Parameter Estimate s.e.WaldZ p-value 95%CIoftheestimate
Residual .074 .002 41.00 < .001 .071,.078
Intercept
Variance
(recess)
.003 .001 3.60 < .001 .002,.006
Estimationoffixedeffects
Parameter Estimate s.e.Ttest
statistic
p-value 95%CIoftheestimate
Intercept .201 .063 3.21 .002 .076,.326
Gender .152 .053 2.86 .004 .048,.257
School .004 .003 1.18 .241 − .003,.010
Safetyand
Structureof
Environment
.004 .005 .885 .379 − .004,.012
Adult
Engagement
and
Supervision
.008 .005 1.52 .133 − .003,.019
Student
behaviors − .006 .004 -1.41 .163 − .014,.002
Gender*
Safetyand
Structureof
Environment
− .002 004 − .679 .497 − .009,.005
Gender*
Adult
Engagement
and
Supervision
− .008 .005 -1.50 .133 − .017,.002
Gender*
Student
behaviors
.009 .003 2.83 .005 .003,.015
EngagementInRecessActivities
Followinganalysesofphysicalactivityatrecess,patternsofplaywerecomparedforrecesssessions
atleastonestandarddeviationabovethemeanrecessqualityscoreandatleastonestandard
deviationbelowthemeanrecessqualityscore(n = 952boys;n = 952girls).AscanbeseeninFig.4,
thelargestdifferenceswereseeninnon-engagementinplay(e.g.,talkingwithfriends,watching
others),with61percentofgirlsatlow-qualityrecesssessionsnon-engagedinplayascomparedto22
percentofgirlsathighqualityrecesssessions.Girlsathighqualityrecesssessionsalsoparticipated
inmoreorganizedgamesthangirlsatlow-qualityrecesssessions(e.g.,dance,kickball,soccer;23%
14
vs.9%),andmoretraditionalplaygroundactivities(e.g.,four-square,jumpropes;21%vs.9%).As
seeninFig.5,asimilarpatternwasobservedforboysasitrelatedtonon-engagement(10%athigh
qualityrecesssessionsvs.36%atlowqualityrecesses),participationinorganizedactivities(52%at
highqualityrecesssessionsvs.37%atlowqualityrecesses),andparticipationintraditional
playgroundactivities(15%athighqualityrecesssessionsvs.5%atlowqualityrecesses).
Finally,basicpsychologicalneedsatisfactionscoreswerecomparedforthoseattendinghigh-quality
recesssessions(i.e.,atleastoneSDabovethemean)andthoseattendinglow-qualityrecesssessions
(i.e.,atleastoneSDbelowthemean).Resultsindicatethatchildrenatahigh-qualityrecesssession
reporthigherlevelsofbasicpsychologicalneedsatisfactionatrecessthanchildrenatalow-quality
recesssession(t(324) = 2.65;p = .004).Anexaminationoftheindividualsub-scalesrevealedno
groupdifferencesincompetenceorrelatedness,howeverthoseatahigh-qualityrecessreported
higherlevelsofperceivedautonomyatrecesst(333) = 1.79;p = .037)andhigherlevelsofperceived
safetyatrecess(t(306) = 4.52;p < .001).
Discussion
Theprimarypurposeofthecurrentstudywastoexaminehowcontextualfeaturesoftheplayground
impactedphysicalactivitylevels,andhowthiswasmoderatedbygender.Asecondarypurposewas
toexaminepatternsofengagementatrecesstobetterunderstandhowthequalityofthe
environmentshapesphysicallyactivebehaviorduringthisdiscretionarytimeperiod.Resultsofthe
currentstudyshowedthatthequalityoftherecessenvironment,andtheinteractionsofbothadults
andstudentsinthatenvironment,needtobetakenintoconsiderationinfutureschool-basedrecess
studies.InexaminingpatternsofMVPA,resultsshowedthathigherlevelsofadultengagementand
supervisionreducedthedisparitybetweenboys’andgirls’physicalactivitylevelsatrecess.This
findingissupportedbypreviousresearchthatshowshigherlevelsofadultengagementpredicthigher
levelsofstudentengagementatrecess(21),thatchildrenreportteacherengagementasapositive
influenceonrecess(29),andthatteachersupportisafacilitatorofactivityatrecess(30).Moreover,
givenpreviousreportsofsocialbarriersgirlsfaceinbeingphysicallyactiveatrecess(e.g.,boys
dominatingequipmentandspace;15,16),itislikelythatengagedadultsontheplaygroundhelpto
15
facilitateequityinaccesstoplaygroundresourcesduringrecess.
Anexaminationofphysicalactivitylevelsatvariouslevelsofprosocialstudentbehaviorsshowedthat
boysparticipatedinhigherlevelsofMVPAduringrecesssessionswithhighpro-socialbehaviors.For
boys,atahigh-qualityrecesssession,higherlevelsoforganizedgames(i.e.,soccer,basketball,
football)likelyaccountedforhigherlevelsofMVPA.Notably,whenprosocialbehaviorswerehigh(i.e.,
limitedfighting,limitedarguments,highlevelsofgameinitiation,highlevelsofconflictresolution)
gameswentonuninterrupted,allowingboystofurtherengageinMVPAduringrecess.Incontrast,
girlsparticipatedinhigherlevelsofMVPAduringrecesssessionswithlowpro-socialbehaviors.While
contrarytoexpectations,thefindingsrelatedtogirlsincreasedratesofMVPAatrecesssessionswith
lowpro-socialbehaviorscanpartiallybeexplainedbyactivitypatternsatrecess.Popularactivitiesfor
girlsathighqualityrecesssessionsincludedtraditionalplaygroundgames(e.g.,4-square,hopscotch)
aswellasorganizedactivities(e.g.,dance),whicharegenerallyalignedwithLPA,asopposedto
MVPA.AsshowninFig.3,LPAwashigherforgirlsatrecesssessionswithhighlevelsofpro-social
behavior.Thus,whenprosocialbehaviorswerehigh,gamesandactivitiesinwhichgirlswere
participatinginhadfewerinterruptions.However,atlow-qualityrecesssessions,orrecesssessions
characterizedashavinglowlevelsofprosocialbehavior(i.e.,higherratesoffightingandarguing,low
levelsofconflictresolution),itispossiblethatotherwiseunengagedgirlsengagedinintermittent
boutsofMVPA.Thesedataareconsistentwithpreviousdataofboysandgirlsplaypatternsatrecess
(15,16,21,22),yetalsounderscoretheimportanceofexaminingthecontextinwhichphysicalactivity
occurs.Notably,whileincreaselevelsofPAremainsagoalofmanyresearchers,datafromthe
currentstudyunderscorestheneedtounderstandthequalityofthePAenvironmentasitrelatesto
children’sholisticdevelopment.
Previousresearchhasalsoexaminedpsychologicalneedsatisfactionandmotivationforphysical
activityatrecess.Notably,StellinoandSinclair(27)reportedthatpsychologicalneedsatisfactionwas
predictiveofphysicalactivitymotivationatrecess,andthatautonomywaspredictiveofphysical
activityduringrecess.Resultsofthecurrentstudysupportthesedata,andfurthersuggestthata
high-qualityrecessenvironmentcanhelpfacilitatebasicpsychologicalneedsatisfaction.Notably,
16
childrenathigh-qualityrecesssessionsreportedhigheroverallpsychologicalneedsatisfaction,higher
autonomyforrecessphysicalactivity,andhigherlevelsofperceivedsafetythanthoseatlow-quality
recesssessions.Thesefindingssuggestthatstructuringtherecessenvironmenttoincludeavariety
ofplayopportunitiesandtrainingadultstobeactivelyengagedinrecessareautonomysupportive,as
opposedtocontrollingstrategies.Whilerecessremainsoneoffewdiscretionarytimeperiodsduring
theschoolday,datasuggeststhatimplementingevidence-basedpracticesduringrecesscanhelpto
enhancethisdiscretionarytimeandsatisfystudent’sbasicpsychologicalneedsofautonomy,
competence,andrelatedness.
StrengthsAndLimitations
Takentogether,themajorstrengthsofthisstudyincludeamultifacetedexaminationofrecesswithin
anunder-researchedpopulation–urbanelementaryschoolstudentsinlowincomeschools.Further,
theresultsofthisstudyshedimportantlightonfuturerecessresearch.Notably,theequivocal
findingsofvariousinterventionsonphysicalactivitypromotionandsocialbehaviors(31–33)warranta
morenuancedunderstandingofrecess,particularlyinunder-resourcedcommunities.Thecurrent
studysuggeststheneedformulti-facetedinterventionsthatconcurrentlyfocusonincreasedaccess
torecess,increasedaccesstoequipmentandplayspaces,positiveandencouragingadultbehavior,
andpro-socialstudentbehavior.Indeed,perhapstheshort-comingsinpreviousinterventionstudies
relytosingularordual-purposeinterventions,ratherthanafocusonmultipleinterventionstaking
placesimultaneously.
Themajorlimitationsinthecurrentstudyarethereducedsamplesizeduetoanalysistakingplaceat
thegrouplevel,thecross-sectionalnatureofthedatacollection,andlimitationswiththeuseofFitbits
tocaptureobjectivephysicalactivityinchildpopulations.Becausechildrenengageinmore
intermittentboutsofPA,particularlyduringrecess,shortermeasurementintervalsarethoughtto
providemoreaccurateestimatesoftimespentinvariousPAintensities.Inastudyexamining
differencesinMVPAforchildrenduringphysicaleducationclasses,McClainandcolleagues(34)
compareddirectobservationandaccelerometryatvariousepochlengths.Resultsshowedthat
estimatesofMVPAwerelowerwhenusingaccelerometry,ascomparedtodirectobservation
17
regardlessofepochlength.However,longerepochlengthswereassociatedwithlowerMVPAcounts.
Similarly,morerecentresearchhasshownlowerestimatesofMVPAinchildrenwhenusinga60
secondepochasopposedtoa15-or5-secondmeasurementperiod(35).Moreover,Bandaand
colleagues(35)reportedthatincreasedepochlengthmayover-estimateLPAinchildrenasincreased
epochtimewasassociatewithsedentarybehaviorbeingreclassifiedasLPA.Giventhis,itispossible
thatMVPAwasunder-estimatedbasedonthedurationandintensityofvariousactivitiesengagedin
duringrecess;whereasLPAmayhavebeenover-estimated.However,concurrentobservationaldata
ofengagementinrecessactivitiessupportstheoverallpatternofresults;specifically,higherlevelsof
engagementinactivitiesassociatedwithincreasedPAduringhigh-asopposedtolow-qualityrecess
sessions.
Conclusions
Recessremainsacriticalopportunityforchildrentobephysicalactiveduringtheschoolday.Results
ofthecurrentstudysuggestthatincreasingadultengagementandfacilitatinghigherlevelsofpro-
socialbehaviorareimportanttonotonlyphysicalactivitypromotionatrecess,butalsochildren’s
psychologicalneedsatisfaction.
Abbreviations
PA
PhysicalActivity
US
UnitedStates
PE
PhysicalEducation
MVPA
Moderate-to-VigorousPhysicalActivity
GRF-OT
GreatRecessFramework–ObservationalTool
LPA
LightPhysicalActivity
OPP
ObservationsofPlaygroundPlay
BPNS
18
BasicPsychologicalNeedSatisfaction
HLM
HierarchicalLinearModeling
SD
StandardDeviation
Declarations
EthicsApproval.EthicsapprovalwasprovidedbytheinstitutionalreviewboardatConcordia
UniversityWisconsin(ID:932380-3;926512-1).Theneedforconsentwaswaivedasnoindividual
leveldatawerecollected.ThePI(WVM)waspreviouslyfacultyatConcordiaUniversityWisconsin.
ConsentforPublication.Notapplicable.
AvailabilityofDataandMaterials.Thedatasetsusedand/oranalysedduringthecurrentstudy
areavailablefromthecorrespondingauthoronreasonablerequest.
CompetingInterests.Theauthorsdeclarethattheyhavenocompetinginterests
Funding.FundingforthisstudywasprovidedbyPlayworksEducationEnergized.Thefundingagency
wasnotinvolvedindatacollection,analysis,orinterpretation.
AuthorContributions.WVMdesignedthestudy,trainedalldataassessors,assistedwithdata
collectionanalyzedthedata,interpretedthedata,anddraftedthemanuscript.MBSassistedwith
studydesign,interpretation,andwritingofthemanuscript.JGassistedindataanalysis,data
interpretation,andeditingofthemanuscript.Allauthorshavegiventheirfinalapprovalforthework
tobepublishedandhaveagreedtotakeaccountabilityforallaspectsofthework.
Acknowledgments.NotApplicable.
References
1. D o bb i n s M , H us s o n H , D eC o r by K , La R o cc a R L. S c ho o l - b as e d ph y s i c al a c t iv i t y
programsforpromo t i n g p h y s ic a l a c ti v i t y a n d f i t n e ss i n ch i l d r e n a n d a d o le s c e n ts
aged6to18.Cochra n e D a t a b as e Sy s t R ev . 2 01 3 F e b 28 ; ( 2 ): C D 0 07 6 51 .
2. R a sb e r ry C N , L e e SM , R ob i n L, L a r is B A , R u s s el l L A , C oy l e K K, e t a l. T h e a s s o ci a t i o n
betweenschool-based p h y s i c a l ac t i v i t y , i n c l u d in g p h ys i c a l e d u c at i o n , an d a ca d e mi c
performance:asyste m a t i c r e v ie w o f t h e li t e r a t u re . P r e v M e d. 2 01 1 Ju n ; 5 2 S u p pl
19
1:S10-20.
3. R o be r t Wo o d J o h n s on F o un d a ti o n . R ec e s s r u l e s : W h y t h e u nd e r va l u e d p l a y ti m e m ay
beAmerica’sbestinve s t m e n t f o r h e a lt h y k id s a n d h e a lt h y s c ho o l s r e p o rt [ I n t e rn e t ] .
2007.Availablefrom:
http://www.rwjf.org/co n t e n t / d a m /f a r m/ r e po r t s / re p o r ts / 2 0 07 / r w jf 1 8 06 0
4. E r wi n H , A b e l M , B ei g h l e A , N o la n d M P, W or l e y B , R i g gs R . T h e Co n t r i b ut i o n of R e ce s s
toChildren’sSchool-Day P h y s i c a l A ct i v i t y . J P h y s Ac t H ea l t h . 20 1 2 M a r 1 ; 9 (3 ) : 4 4 2– 8 .
5. U S C e n te r s f o r D i s e as e C on t r o l. S t r a te g i e s f o r s u pp o r ti n g r e ce s s in e l e m en t a ry
schools,updateforthe 2 0 1 2 – 13 s c ho o l y ea r [ I n te r n e t] . A v a il a b l e fr o m :
https://www.cdc.gov/h e a l t h y s c h oo l s / n pa o / p df / L W P_ R ec e s s _B r i e f_ 2 0 12 _ 1 3. p d f.
6. S h af e r M, W hi t e h o us e E . S t a t e p o l i c i e s o n ph y s i c al a c t iv i t y i n s ch o o l s [ I n t e r ne t ] .
CouncilofStateGovernm e n t s ; 2 01 8 [ c i t e d 20 2 0 J a n 20 ] . A va i l a b le f r o m:
http://knowledgecenter . c s g . o r g /k c / s y st e m /f i l e s / C R _a c t iv i t y _ s c ho o l . p df
7. B a rr o s RM , Si l v e r E J, S t e i n R E . Sc h o o l r e c e ss a n d g r o up c l a ss r o o m b eh a v io r .
Pediatrics[Internet].200 9 [ c i t e d 2 01 9 J u n 2 5] ; 1 2 3( 2 ) :4 3 1 –6 . A va i l a b le f r o m :
http://pediatrics.aappub l i c a t i o n s . o rg / c g i / do i / 1 0 .1 5 4 2/ p e ds . 2 00 7 - 28 2 5
8. M on n a t S M , L o u n s be r y M A F , M c K en z i e TL , C h an d l e r R F . As s o c ia t i o n s b e t w ee n
demographiccharac t e r i s t i c s a n d p h y si c a l a ct i v i t y p ra c t i c es i n N ev a d a s c ho o l s . Pr e v
Med[Internet].2017F e b [ c i t e d 2 0 2 0 J a n 2 0 ] ; 9 5: S 4 –9 . A va i l a b le f r o m:
https://linkinghub.elsevier . c o m / r e t r ie v e / p ii / S 0 0 91 7 43 5 1 63 0 23 6 5
9. A n de r s o n- B u t ch e r D , N e ws o m e W S, N a y S . S oc i a l Sk i l l s I n t er v e n ti o n d ur i n g
ElementarySchoolRe c e s s : A V i s ua l A n al y s i s . C h i l d S ch . 2 00 3 ;2 5 ( 3 ): 1 3 5– 4 6 .
10. M a s se y W V, S t e ll i n o M B, H ol l i d a y M, G o db e r se n T , R o d ia R , K uc h e r G , e t a l . T h e
impactofamulti-comp o n e n t p h ys i c a l a c t i v i t y p r o g ra m me i n l o w - i n co m e e l e m en t a ry
schools.HealthEducJ.2 0 1 7 ; 7 6 (5 ) : 5 17 – 53 0 .
20
11. H u b e rt y J L , B e e t s M W , B e i g hl e A , W e l k G . E n vi r o n m en t a l m o di f i c a t i o ns t o in c r e a se
physicalactivityduringre c e s s : p r e li m i n a ry f i n d in g s f ro m r e a d y f o r r ec e s s. J P h y s A c t
Health.2011Sep;8S u p p l 2 : S 24 9 -2 5 6 .
12. V i c i a n a J , M ay o r g a- V e g a D , M a r t ín e z - B a en a A. M o de r a te - t o - V ig o r o u s P h y si c a l A ct i v i t y
LevelsinPhysicalEduca t i o n , S c h o ol R e c es s , a nd A ft e r - S c ho o l T im e : In f l u e n ce o f
Gender,Age,andWe i g h t S t a t u s. J P h y s A c t He a l t h . 2 0 1 6; 1 3( 1 0 ): 1 1 1 7– 2 3 .
13. S a i n t - M au r ic e P F , W e l k G J , S il v a P , S ia h p u sh M , H u b er t y J . A s s e ss i n g c hi l d r e n’ s
physicalactivitybehavior s a t r e c e ss : a mu l t i - m et h o d a p p ro a c h. P e d ia t r E xe r c Sc i .
2011Nov;23(4):585–99.
14. B a q u et G , R i d g e rs N D , B l a e s A , A uc o u tu r i e r J , V a n Pr a a g h E , Be r t h oi n S . Ob j e c ti v e l y
assessedrecessphys i c a l a c t i v i t y in g i r l s an d b oy s f ro m hi g h a nd l o w s o c io e c o n om i c
backgrounds.BMCP u b l i c H e a lt h . 2 0 14 F eb 2 1; 1 4 :1 9 2 .
15. P a w lo w s k i C S , T jø r n h ø j- T h o m se n T, S c h ip p e r ij n J , T r o el s e n J . B a r ri e r s f or r e c es s
physicalactivity:agende r s p e c i f i c q u a l i t a ti v e f o cu s g ro u p e x p l o ra t i o n . B M C P u b li c
Health.2014Jun23;1 4 ( 1 ) : 6 3 9.
16. P a w lo w s k i C S , E rg l e r C, T j ø r nh ø j - T h om s en T , Sc h i p pe r i j n J , T r o e ls e n J . “ Li k e a S o c c er
CampforBoys”:AQu a l i t a t i v e E x pl o r a ti o n o f G e n d er e d A c t i v i t y P a t t e rn s i n Ch i l d r e n’ s
Self-OrganizedPlaydur i n g S c h o o l R e c e ss . E ur P h y s E d uc R e v. 2 0 15 A ug ; 2 1 (3 ) : 2 75 –
91.
17. B o y l e D E , M ar s ha l l N L , R o b es o n WW . G e n d er a t P l a y : F ou r t h -G r a d e G i r l s a n d Bo y s on
thePlayground.AmB e h a v S c i . 2 0 0 3 J u n 1; 4 6 (1 0 ) : 1 32 6 –4 5 .
18. S t e l l i n o M B , S i n c l a ir C D , Pa r t r i d ge J A , K i n g KM . D if f e r e nc e s in c h i l d r en ’ s r ec e s s
physicalactivity:recessa c t i v i t y o f t h e w e ek i n t e rv e n ti o n . J S ch H ea l t h . 20 1 0
Sep;80(9):436–44.
19. R i d g e rs N D , S t r a tt o n G , M c Ke n z i e T L . R el i a b i l i t y an d v al i d i t y of t h e S y s t e m f o r
21
ObservingChildren’sAc t i v i t y a n d R el a t i o ns h i p s d u r i n g P l a y ( S O C AR P ) . J P h y s A c t
Health.2010Jan;7(1) : 1 7 – 2 5 .
20. P a w lo w s k i C S , S ch i p p er i j n J , T jø r n h ø j- T h o m se n T , T r o el s e n J . G i v i n g c h i l d re n a v o i c e :
Exploringqualitativeper s p e c t i v e s on f a ct o r s i n fl u e n ci n g r e ce s s p h y s ic a l a c ti v i t y . E u r
PhysEducRev.2018 F e b 1 ; 2 4( 1 ) :3 9 – 5 5.
21. M a s se y W V, S t e ll i n o M B, F r as e r M. I n d iv i d u a l a n d en v i r o nm e nt a l c o rr e l a t es o f sc h o o l-
basedrecessengage m e n t . P re v M ed R ep . 2 01 8 S e p 1; 1 1 :2 4 7 –5 3 .
22. M a s se y W V, K u B , S t el l i n o M B. O bs e r v at i o n s o f p l a yg r o un d p la y d u ri n g e le m e nt a r y
schoolrecess.BMCRe s N o t e s [ In t e r n e t] . 2 0 18 O ct 2 3 [ c i t e d 2 0 1 9 A u g 2 0 ]; 1 1 .
Availablefrom:https://w w w . n c b i. n l m . ni h . g o v/ p m c/ a r t i c le s / P M C6 1 99 6 97 /
23. M a s se y W V, S t e ll i n o M B, M ul l e n S P, C l a as s e n J , W i l k is o n M . D e v el o p m en t of t h e g r e a t
recessframework– o b s e r v a ti o n a l t o ol t o m ea s ur e c on t e x tu a l a n d b e ha v i o ra l
componentsofeleme n t a r y s c h oo l r e ce s s . B M C P u b l i c H e a lt h . 2 0 18 M ar 2 2; 1 8 (1 ) : 3 94 .
24. H a m ar i L , K u l l b e rg T , Ru o h on e n J, H e i n on e n OJ , D í a z- R o d rí g u e z N , L i l i u s J , e t a l.
Physicalactivityamong c h i l d r e n : o bj e c t i v e m e as u r em e n ts u s i n g F i t b it O n e ® a n d
ActiGraph.BMCResN o t e s [ I n t e r ne t ] . 2 01 7 A p r 2 0 [ c i t ed 2 0 19 A ug 2 0] ; 1 0 . A v a il a b l e
from:https://www.ncb i . n l m . n i h . go v / p mc / a rt i c l e s / PM C 5 39 7 82 8 /
25. V o s s C, G a rd n e r R F , D ea n P H , H ar r i s K C . V a li d i t y o f C o m me rc i a l A c ti v i t y T r ac k e rs i n
ChildrenWithCongenital H e a r t D i s ea s e . C a n J C a r d io l . 2 0 17 ; 3 3( 6 ) : 79 9 – 80 5 .
26. S u s h am e s A , E dw a r ds A , T h o m ps o n F , M cD e r mo t t R, G e be l K . Va l i d i t y a n d Re l i a b il i t y
ofFitbitFlexforStepCou n t , M o d e ra t e to V i g o ro u s Ph y s i c al A c t i v it y a n d A c t i v it y
EnergyExpenditure.Plo S O n e . 2 01 6 ;1 1 ( 9) : e 0 16 1 2 24 .
27. S t e l l i n o M B , S i n c l a ir C D . Ps y c h ol o g i c a l P r e d ic t o r s o f C h il d r e n ’ s R e c es s P hy s i c a l
ActivityMotivationandBe h a v i o r . R es Q E x e r c S p o rt . 2 0 13 J u n 1 ; 8 4( 2 ) : 16 7 – 76 .
28. P r e a ch e r K J, C u r r an P J , B a u e r D J . C om p ut a t i o na l T o ol s f o r P r o b in g I n t e ra c t i o ns i n
22
MultipleLinearRegress io n , M u l t i l e v el M o de l i n g , a n d L a t e n t C u r v e A n a ly s i s . J E d u c
BehavStat.2006De c 1 ; 3 1 ( 4 ): 4 3 7– 4 8 .
29. P a r r i s h A - M , Y e a tm a n H , I v e rs o n D , R u ss e l l K . U s i n g i n t e r vi e w s a n d pe e r pa i r s to
betterunderstandhow s c h o o l e nv i r o n me n ts a f f ec t y o un g c hi l d r e n ’s p l a yg r o u nd
physicalactivitylevels:aq u a l i t a t i v e s tu d y . H e a lt h E d u c R e s . 2 0 1 2 A p r; 2 7 (2 ) : 2 6 9– 8 0.
30. S t a n l e y R M , B o s ho f f K , D o l l m an J. V o i c e s i n t h e p l a y gr o u n d: A q ua l i t a ti v e e x pl o r a ti o n
ofthebarriersandfac i l i t a t o r s o f l u nc h t i m e p l a y. J S ci M e d S p o rt . 2 0 12 J a n
1;15(1):44–51.
31. B u n d y A , E ng e l e n L , W yv e r S , T r an t e r P , R a ge n J , B a u m an A, e t a l. S y d ne y P la y g r o un d
Project:ACluster-Rando m i z e d T r i a l t o I n c re a s e P h y s ic a l A c ti v i t y , P la y , a nd S o ci a l
Skills.JSchHealth.2017 O c t 1 ; 8 7( 1 0 ): 7 5 1– 9 .
32. M a y fi e l d C A , C h i l d S , W ea v e r R G , Z a r r e tt N , B ee t s M W , M o o re J B . Ef f e c ti v e n e ss o f a
PlaygroundIntervention f o r A n t i s o ci a l , P r os o c i a l, a n d P h y s ic a l A c ti v i t y B e ha v i o rs . J
SchHealth.2017Ma y 1 ; 8 7 ( 5 ): 3 3 8– 4 5 .
33. F a r m er V L, W i l l i a ms S M, M an n JI , S c h o fi e l d G , M c P he e J C , T a y lo r R W . T h e e f f e ct o f
increasingriskandcha l l e n g e i n t h e s c h oo l p l a yg r o un d o n p h y si c a l a ct i v i t y a nd
weightinchildren:aclus t e r r a n d o mi s e d c o n tr o l l e d t r i a l ( P L AY ) . I n t J O b e s 2 0 05 .
2017;41(5):793–800.
34. M c c l a in J J , A b ra h a m T L , B r u ss e a u T A , T ud o r - Lo c k e C . E p oc h L en g t h a n d
AccelerometerOutputs i n C h i l d r e n : C o m pa r is o n t o D i r e c t O b s er v a t i o n. M ed S c i S p o r ts
Exerc[Internet].2008 D e c [ c i t e d 2 0 2 0 J a n 2 1 ] ; 40 ( 1 2) : 2 0 80 – 7. A v a il a b l e fr o m :
https://insights.ovid.com/ c r o s s r e f ? an = 00 0 0 57 6 8- 2 0 08 1 20 0 0- 0 0 0 10
35. B a n d a J A , Ha y d el K F , D a v i l a T , D e sa i M , B r y so n S , H a s ke l l W L, e t a l. E f f e ct s o f
VaryingEpochLengths , W e a r T im e A lg o r i t h ms , a nd A ct i v i t y C ut - P o i n ts o n E st i m a te s
ofChildSedentaryBeh a v i o r a n d P h y s ic a l A c ti v i t y f r o m A cc e l e r om e te r D a ta .
23
PappalardoF,editor.P L O S O N E [ I n t e r ne t ] . 2 01 6 M a r 3 [ c i t e d 2 0 20 J a n
21];11(3):e015053 4 . A v a i l a b le f r o m:
https://dx.plos.org/10.1 3 7 1 / j o u rn a l . p on e . 0 15 0 53 4
Figures
Figure1
GenderxAdultengagementandsupervisioninteractionforpercentageoftimeinMVPA
duringrecess.
24
Figure2
GenderxStudentbehaviorinteractionforpercentageoftimeinMVPAduringrecess.
Figure3
GenderxStudentbehaviorinteractionforpercentageoftimeinlightphysicalactivityduring
recess.
25
Figure4
Differencesingirls'engagementinplayathigh-versuslow-qualityrecesssessions
Figure5
Differencesinboys’engagementinplayathigh-versuslow-qualityrecesssessions
SupplementaryFiles
Thisisalistofsupplementaryfilesassociatedwiththispreprint.Clicktodownload.