PreprintPDF Available

Changes in risk perception and protective behavior during the first week of the COVID-19 pandemic in the United States

Authors:
Preprints and early-stage research may not have been peer reviewed yet.

Abstract and Figures

By mid-March 2020, the COVID-19 pandemic spread to over 100 countries and all 50 states in the US. Government efforts to minimize the spread of disease emphasized behavioral interventions, including raising awareness of the disease and encouraging protective behaviors such as social distancing and hand washing, and seeking medical attention if experiencing symptoms. However, it is unclear to what extent individuals are aware of the risks associated with the disease, how they are altering their behavior, factors which could influence the spread of the virus to vulnerable populations. We characterized risk perception and engagement in preventative measures in 1591 United States based individuals over the first week of the pandemic (March 11th-16th 2020) and examined the extent to which protective behaviors are predicted by individuals’ perception of risk. Over 5 days, subjects demonstrated growing awareness of the risk posed by the virus, and largely reported engaging in protective behaviors with increasing frequency. However, they underestimated their personal risk of infection relative to the average person in the country. We found that engagement in social distancing and handwashing was most strongly predicted by the perceived likelihood of personally being infected, rather than likelihood of transmission or severity of potential transmitted infections. However, substantial variability emerged among individuals, and using data-driven methods we found a subgroup of subjects who are largely disengaged, unaware, and not practicing protective behaviors. Our results have implications for our understanding of how risk perception and protective behaviors can facilitate early interventions during large-scale pandemics.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Changes in risk perception and protective behavior during the first week
of the COVID-19 pandemic in the United States
Toby Wise1,2,3, Tomislav Zbozinek1, Giorgia Michelini4, Cindy C Hagan1 & Dean Mobbs1,5
1Division of Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA
2Max Planck UCL Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing Research, University College London, London, UK
3Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, University College London, London, UK
4Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA
5Computational Neural Systems Program, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA
Abstract
By mid-March 2020, the COVID-19 pandemic spread to over 100 countries and all 50 states in the
US. Government efforts to minimize the spread of disease emphasized behavioral interventions,
including raising awareness of the disease and encouraging protective behaviors such as social
distancing and hand washing, and seeking medical attention if experiencing symptoms.
However, it is unclear to what extent individuals are aware of the risks associated with the
disease, how they are altering their behavior, factors which could influence the spread of the
virus to vulnerable populations. We characterized risk perception and engagement in
preventative measures in 1591 United States based individuals over the first week of the
pandemic (March 11th-16th 2020) and examined the extent to which protective behaviors are
predicted by individuals’ perception of risk. Over 5 days, subjects demonstrated growing
awareness of the risk posed by the virus, and largely reported engaging in protective behaviors
with increasing frequency. However, they underestimated their personal risk of infection relative
to the average person in the country. We found that engagement in social distancing and
handwashing was most strongly predicted by the perceived likelihood of personally being
infected, rather than likelihood of transmission or severity of potential transmitted infections.
However, substantial variability emerged among individuals, and using data-driven methods we
found a subgroup of subjects who are largely disengaged, unaware, and not practicing protective
behaviors. Our results have implications for our understanding of how risk perception and
protective behaviors can facilitate early interventions during large-scale pandemics.
Introduction
The genesis of the novel coronavirus epidemic (spread of COVID-19 disease) has been tied to the Hubei
province of China and rapidly progressed to the level of a global pandemic, with multiple countries
across the globe reporting exponentially increasing numbers of cases (1). The first case in the US was
reported in January 14 2020 (2), followed by government interventions in travel restrictions. On March
11, however, COVID-19 officially become an global pandemic (3) and the introduction of a series of
governmental decisions to restrict social and economic behavior began. By March 17, all 50 states
reported at least one person with the virus (2). Like most developed countries, a major focus of the US
has been minimizing transmission of the virus in order to flatten the epidemic peak and lessen the
impact on healthcare services (4,5), enabling the most severe cases to be treated successfully and reduce
overall mortality. The success of these measures is particularly critical in the case of COVID-19 due to
its high transmissibility, even in the absence of symptoms (6,7), severity (4), and mortality rate, particular
among older individuals (5). However, these protective measures rely largely on rapid changes in
population behavior, which are dependent on individuals’ ability to perceive risks associated with the
virus and adapt their behavior accordingly (8).
Given the importance of human psychological and behavioral factors in managing pandemics, it is
crucial to assess psychological and behavioral responses to the situation and determine how perceived
risk is linked to engagement in protective behaviors (9). There is limited evidence on reactions to prior
pandemics in the early stages when preventative measures are most necessary (9). While some studies
have emphasized the role of risk perception, predominantly the personal effects of the disease (in terms
of likelihood and severity if infection for the individual), on preventative behaviors, these often take
place either in anticipation of an outbreak or long after its emergence (9). In addition, lab-based research
has suggested that increased perceived effects of disease spread on others may increase engagement
in social distancing (10). The few studies that have surveyed individuals during the early stages of a
pandemic have also suggested that perceived personal risk of infection and health effects are linked to
engagement in protective behaviors (11). However, it is also well established that individuals typically
tend to underestimate their likelihood of experiencing adverse life events (such as cancer) relative to
the average person, an effect known as optimism bias (12). Together, it is apparent that perceived risk
is likely to affect individuals behavior during a pandemic, but that individuals are often poor at
perceiving risk. However, it is unknown how perceived risk relates to protective behaviors in the early
stages of a pandemic on the scale of COVID-19. Additionally, we are unaware of any data of this kind
for the current COVID-19 pandemic; given COVID-19’s ongoing rampant nature, this data may have
global value to the medical community, government leaders, and society more broadly.
Figure 1. Timeline of events early in the United States COVID-19 pandemic. Days of current study data acquisition shown in gray.
News events in green are most relevant for United States. COVID-19 data acquired from European Centre for Disease Prevention
and Control. Major news events retrieved from National Broadcasting Company (NBC) News, Columbia Broadcasting System (CBS)
News, and Cable News Network (CNN).
Results
We conducted an online survey of 1591 individuals in the USA during the early stages of the country’s
outbreak in March 2020, asking about their perceptions of risk and behavioral responses to the
pandemic (see Figure 2 for demographic information). Subjects were recruited through Prolific (13)
between 3/11/20, the day when the WHO declared COVID-19 a pandemic, and 3/16/20. The study was
approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at California Institute of Technology. We focused on
how perceived risk from the virus and propensity to engage in protective behaviors developed as the
pandemic progressed. We also sought to quantify the extent to which engagement in protective
behaviors was dependent on perceived risk. 375 subjects of the 495 who participated on the first day
were followed up after 5 days to provide a picture of within-person changes in perceptions and
behaviors. We also replicated cross-sectional results in a subset of our data to ensure robustness.





 





 










































 




























































        









       

      

                

        




























         

   



Figure 2. Demographics of survey respondents. The red dotted line on the lower panel represents the median age (30 years).
Perceptions of risk from COVID-19
While at the time of submission it remains unclear exactly how widespread the pandemic will be, current
estimates suggest that up to 80% of the population may contract the disease (4). We sought to
characterize perceptions of infection likelihood and severity, for both the study participants themselves
and others. All responses were recorded on a visual analogue scale coded between 0 and 100. We also
examined changes in behavior over time by sampling independent groups of subjects over five days
and retesting subjects who participated on the first day after a five-day period. Despite being a short
period of time, multiple significant political events occurred during this time period, including travel
bans and restrictions on public gatherings (Figure 1).
Figure 3. Distributions of responses to items regarding risk perception (n=1591). All responses were recorded on a visual analogue
scale ranging from 0 to 100. Bar plots indicate mean responses to these items over the two timepoints where a subgroup of
subjects was retested (n=375).
As shown in Figure 3, subjects assessed their risk of being infected as relatively high (mean = 43.06, SD
= 26.62). Additionally, they reported perceiving the disease as being a threat to their health (mean =
44.70, SD=26.93). They also indicated that they would be personally affected economically, such as
through loss of work (mean = 45.68, SD = 34.35), and that they would be affected by the global
economic consequences, such as through economic recession and effects on healthcare provision
(mean = 64.38, SD = 24.02), although responses to this question were not unimodally distributed..
Subjects were also aware of the potential for contagion, indicating that if they became infected, they
would be likely to pass it to someone else (mean = 66.18, SD = 27.39, Figure 3). As with perceptions of
infection likelihood, subjects believed that if they did infect another person, they would be worse
affected than themselves, both in terms of health and of economic effects (mean difference = 14.82, SD
difference = 26.67). Linear regression indicated that the difference between perceived effects on another
person and reported personal health risk was partially dependent on age (t(1550) = -8.33, p < .001),
suggesting that this may be explained, in part, by the relatively young age of the participants (median
age=30 years) and knowledge of the worse health effects in older individuals. However, the intercept in
this model remained positive and significant (β=29.68, p < .001), indicating the presence of such a bias
even after accounting for age.
Perceived likelihood of infection however differed according to who participants were rating (F(3, 4737)
= 579.00, p < .001, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.27), with participants rating the average person in the US to have the highest
risk of infection, but themselves to have the lowest risk, in line with work on optimism bias (12) (Figure
4C). Perceived likelihood of infection differed across samples tested on different days, demonstrating a
higher rate over time (F(6, 1579) = 6.48, p < .001, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.024, Figure 4C). An increase in perceived
likelihood was found within-subjects in a subsample followed up after 5 days (F(1, 374) = 69.19, p <
.001, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.16, Figure 4D). There was an interaction between time and subject of rating (F(3, 1122) =
7.56, p < .001, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.02), representing the greatest changes in risk perception for the self, however this
was weak and likely influenced by ceiling effects.
Figure 4. Changes in protective behaviors and risk perception over time. A) Reported likelihood of attending events with a given
number of other people in separate samples tested on 5 days in the early stages of the outbreak in the United States. B) Reported
likelihood of attending events of different sizes in a subset of subjects followed up 5 days after initially completing the survey. C)
Perceived likelihood of becoming infected for participants themselves and average people at different geographic scales in
separate samples tested over 5 days. D) Perceived likelihoods of infection in a subset of subjects followed up after 5 days.
Engagement in protective behaviors
We next assessed the extent to which subjects reported engaging in protective behaviors, such as social
distancing and hand washing, in addition to superficially helpful behaviors such as buying more food
and water. On average, subjects indicated that they were engaging in such behaviors more than usual,
although response distributions included peaks at the extremes (Figure 5). Five out of six protective
behaviours had a peak for not engaging in the protective behaviour more than normal, and three out
of six had a peak for engaging in the protective behaviour more than normal. In particular, subjects
reported washing their hands more than normal (median = 77, IQR = 38) and staying home more than
normal (median = 62, IQR = 69), representing high engagement with sanitization and social distancing
measures. In subjects who completed the survey a second time point 5 days after first completion
(3/11/2020), responses had changed for both hand-washing (Wilcoxon W(375) = 25027.5, p < .001) and
social distancing (W(375) = 12269, p < .001) to reflect increased engagement in these behaviors.
We also asked people how likely they would be to attend events with varying numbers of people (10 to
1000) to assess how they were adapting their behavior according to transmission risk. As expected, we
observed a main effect of group size (F(4, 6316) = 1311.68, p < .001, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.45, Figure 4A), whereby
individuals were less likely to attend an event with more people. We also saw markedly lower likelihood
ratings over time in separate samples collected across multiple days (F(6, 1579) = 22.84, p < .001, 𝜂𝑝
2 =
0.08, Figure 4A). Congruently, a decrease over time emerged in our within-subject analysis (F(1, 374) =
279.02, p < .001, 𝜂𝑝
2 = 0.43, Figure 4B), providing evidence that individuals reported dramatically
changing their intended behavior within the space of only a few days. Notably, this occurred before and
 
 
after the CDC’s recommendation of avoiding gatherings of 50+ people (3/15/20) and on the same day
of President Trump’s announcement to avoid gatherings of 10+ people (3/16/20).
Figure 5. Distributions of responses to items regarding behavior (n=1591). All responses were recorded on a visual analogue scale
ranging from 0 to 100. Bar plots indicate mean responses to these items over the two timepoints where a subgroup of subjects
was retested (n=375).
Influence of risk perception on protective behaviors
We next investigated the extent to which risk perception was predictive of engagement in protective
behaviors. We used multiple linear regression to assess the extent to which of our 10 items assessing
risk perception (shown in Figure 6) were associated with engagement in two primary protective
behaviors, hand washing and social distancing (assessed through asking subjects whether they were
staying home more than normal), controlling for age. We performed this analysis in a subset consisting
of 75% of participants and repeated it in the remaining 25% to ensure reproducibility of our results.
Results reported here are from the larger dataset but were consistent across both subsets (Figure 6). All
data were scaled to zero mean and unit variance prior to analysis to allow comparability of regression
coefficients.
The clearest effect common to both behaviors was a significant effect of perceived likelihood of
personally becoming infected (hand washing β = 0.17, p < .001, social distancing β = 0.20, p < .001,
Figure 6), while perceived severity of illness was not a significant predictor (hand washing β = -0.03, p =
.37, social distancing β = 0.002, p = .95, Figure 6). Perceived impact from global consequences (e.g.
economic recession, healthcare overcapacity) also significantly predicted engagement in both behaviors
to a lesser extent (β=0.08, p = .01, social distancing β=0.14, p < .001, Figure 6). Notably, likelihood of
passing the virus on to others and perceived negative effects for another individual who contracted the
virus did not significantly predict behavior (Figure 6). Age did not have a significant effect in either
dataset.
Figure 6. Results of linear regression predicting engagement in hand washing and social distancing (represented by responses to
an item regarding staying home) from measures of risk perception, with validation in a subsample of 25% of subjects. A represents
the discovery dataset and B represents results from the validation dataset.
Identification of subgroups demonstrating low engagement in protective behavior
The distributions shown in Figure 5 clearly indicate that the pattern of responses to questions on
protective behaviors was not Gaussian, and was not consistently unimodal, suggesting that there are
likely to be subgroups of individuals responding to the outbreak in qualitatively different ways. To
explore this further, we used a Bayesian Guassian mixture model (GMM) to decompose the distribution
of responses to four primary questions (avoiding social interaction, hand-washing, staying home, and
travelling less) into latent components. The Bayesian GMM approach assigns weights to these
components and we rejected any with a weight below 0.01 as these had a negligible contribution to the
model, leaving 16 components as the final solution (Figure 7B). Based on the mean response scores of
the components, two components (components 4 and 6) were characterised by high and very low
reported engagement with the four protective behaviours respectively (Figure 7A). Others indicated that
there were clusters of individuals selectively engaging in certain protective behaviours but not others
(components 3, 10, and 16 for example).
The model allowed us to assign a probability of each subject being described by each component, which
we used to select individuals most likely to belong to the low or high engagement cluster. Having
labelled individuals according to their behaviour, we then assessed Z-scored responses to other items
to examine how these individuals compared to the group average in terms of percevied risk, information
seeking, and personal effects of the pandemic (Figure 7C). This revealed a broad pattern of below
average perceived risk for both themselves (mean Z = -0.68) and others (mean Z = -0.38), perceived
likelihood of transmission (mean Z = -0.28), low engagement with information sources (mean Z = -0.89),
and low perceived personal effects (mean Z = -0.66), while the opposite pattern was observed in the
high engagement group. Significant differences from the group average are shown in Figure 3C.
Together, this indicates that there exists a subgroup in the population who are generally disengaged in
terms of information seeking, feel unaffected by the situation, and perceive the risk of COVID-19 as
being low for themselves and others, and who do not engage in protective behaviours.
Figure 6. Results of Bayesian Gaussian Mixture Model (GMM) decomposing response distributions for protective behavior items
into clusters. A) Mean scores for each component in the GMM model on the four items used to generate clusters. B) Weights of
retained components. Four components were rejected due to having negligible weights (< .01). C) Z-scored responses on other
questionnaire items for the low engagement and high engagement clusters, demonstrating how they compare to the average
individual. Asterisks represent significant differences from the group average (one sample t-test on the Z scores versus zero, FDR
corrected for 18 comparisons).
Discussion
Understanding how psychological factors influence behavior in severe, global pandemics such as that
COVID-19 is key to facilitating disease minimization strategies. Our analyses indicate that although most
individuals are aware of the risk caused by the pandemic to some extent, they typically underestimate
their personal risk relative to that of others, an example of optimism bias (12). In turn, higher perceived
personal risk predicts engagement in protective behaviors such as hand washing and social distancing,
as shown in studies of prior pandemics (9). Notably, we identified and characterized a non-negligible
subset of subjects reporting little to no engagement in protective behaviors, who rated overall likelihood
of infection as low and reported being generally disengaged in information seeking and being
personally unaffected. Overall, the presence a subgroup is concerning given the threat posed by COVID-
19 and the beneficial effects of widespread behavioural changes.
One explanation for our results is the optimism bias (12). This bias is associated with the belief that we
are less likely to acquire a disease than others, and has been shown across a variety of diseases including
lung cancer (14). Indeed, those who show the optimism bias are less likely to be vaccinated against
disease (15). Recent evidence suggests that this may also be the case for COVID-19 and could result in
a failure to engage in behaviors that contribute to the spread this highly contagious disease. Our results
extend on these findings by showing that behavior changes over the first week of the COVID-19
pandemic such that as individuals perceive an increase in personal risk they increasingly engage in risk-
prevention behaviors. Notably, we observed rapid increases in risk perception over a 5-day period,
indicating that public health messages spread through government and the media can be effective in
raising awareness of the risk. This effect was strongest for perceptions of subjects’ own risk, diminishing
the optimism bias. The speed at which perceptions changed is such that this could have a meaningful
effect in terms of reducing disease transmission.
Our results point to candidate targets for intervention in public information campaigns during
pandemics on this scale. Clear communication of risk could aid the development of accurate risk
perception, in turn facilitating engagement in protective behaviors. It would be particularly important
to target the subset of individuals who remain disengaged and are not themselves seeking information
on the pandemic. This suggests the need to expand outreach methods to individuals who do not seek
information themselves (e.g., emergency alerts on phones). Furthermore, such disengagement should
be considered in epidemiological models used to forecast the effects of behavioral interventions on
disease spread. Additionally, education on the beneficial effects of such behaviors for others may
improve engagement, particularly in those at low perceived personal risk; it is possible that links
between protective behavior and perceived personal risk minimization are merely easier to appreciate.
There are limitations to our work that should be considered. First, the median age (30 years) of our
sample is relatively young. However, many of our results do not appear to be highly dependent on age,
for example age was not a significant predictor of hand-washing or social distancing. In addition, young
people are typically the primary target of efforts to encourage social distancing, having on average
larger social networks (16) and therefore a higher likelihood of engaging in social contact. This is
particularly important in the context of COVID-19, where there is evidence that the spread of the virus
has been facilitated by the movement of young people with limited to no symptoms (5,6). Second, our
data only reflects views of those in the United States and may not be applicable to other cultures. It will
be important to characterize psychological and behavioral responses across the globe during
pandemics in order to recommend and implement the most optimal strategies for effecting behavioural
change, which often are culturally specific.
Adaptation of behavior will be fundamental to the management of a pandemic on the scale of COVID-
19. Our results provide insights into key psychological and behavioral states during a crucial time in the
developing situation.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by US National Institute of Mental Health grant 2P50MH094258 and a Chen
Institute Award (P2026052) (support to D.M.). TW is supported by a Wellcome Trust Sir Henry
Wellcome Fellowship. TZ is supported by the National Science Foundation (#1911441).
References
1. World Health Organisation. Coronavirus [Internet]. 2020 [cited 2020 Mar 17]. Available from:
https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019
2. CDC. Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) in the U.S. [Internet]. Centers for Disease Control
and Prevention. 2020 [cited 2020 Mar 19]. Available from:
https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/cases-updates/cases-in-us.html
3. WHO Director-General’s opening remarks at the media briefing on COVID-19 - 11 March 2020
[Internet]. [cited 2020 Mar 19]. Available from: https://www.who.int/dg/speeches/detail/who-
director-general-s-opening-remarks-at-the-media-briefing-on-covid-19---11-march-2020
4. COVID-19 reports | Faculty of Medicine | Imperial College London [Internet]. [cited 2020 Mar
17]. Available from: https://www.imperial.ac.uk/mrc-global-infectious-disease-analysis/news--
wuhan-coronavirus/
5. Lai C-C, Shih T-P, Ko W-C, Tang H-J, Hsueh P-R. Severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus
2 (SARS-CoV-2) and coronavirus disease-2019 (COVID-19): The epidemic and the challenges.
International Journal of Antimicrobial Agents. 2020 Mar 1;55(3):105924.
6. Bai Y, Yao L, Wei T, Tian F, Jin D-Y, Chen L, et al. Presumed Asymptomatic Carrier Transmission
of COVID-19. JAMA [Internet]. 2020 Feb 21 [cited 2020 Mar 17]; Available from:
https://jamanetwork.com/journals/jama/fullarticle/2762028
7. Li R, Pei S, Chen B, Song Y, Zhang T, Yang W, et al. Substantial undocumented infection
facilitates the rapid dissemination of novel coronavirus (SARS-CoV2). Science [Internet]. 2020
Mar 16 [cited 2020 Mar 17]; Available from:
https://science.sciencemag.org/content/early/2020/03/13/science.abb3221
8. Xu J, Peng Z. People at Risk of Influenza Pandemics: The Evolution of Perception and Behavior.
PLoS ONE. 2015;10(12):e0144868.
9. Bish A, Michie S. Demographic and attitudinal determinants of protective behaviours during a
pandemic: A review. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2010;15(4):797824.
10. Kappes A, Nussberger A-M, Faber NS, Kahane G, Savulescu J, Crockett MJ. Uncertainty about
the impact of social decisions increases prosocial behaviour. Nat Hum Behav. 2018
Aug;2(8):57380.
11. Liao Q, Wu P, Lam WWT, Cowling BJ, Fielding R. Trajectories of public psycho-behavioural
responses relating to influenza A(H7N9) over the winter of 2014-15 in Hong Kong. Psychology
& Health. 2019 Feb 1;34(2):16280.
12. Sharot T. The optimism bias. Current Biology. 2011 Dec 6;21(23):R9415.
13. Palan S, Schitter C. Prolific.acA subject pool for online experiments. Journal of Behavioral and
Experimental Finance. 2018 Mar 1;17:227.
14. Brnstrm R, Brandberg Y. Health Risk Perception, Optimistic Bias, and Personal Satisfaction
[Internet]. 2010 [cited 2020 Mar 19]. Available from:
https://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/png/ajhb/2010/00000034/00000002/art00007
15. Brewer NT, Chapman GB, Gibbons FX, Gerrard M, McCaul KD, Weinstein ND. Meta-analysis of
the relationship between risk perception and health behavior: The example of vaccination.
Health Psychology. 2007;26(2):13645.
16. Wrzus C, Hänel M, Wagner J, Neyer FJ. Social network changes and life events across the life
span: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2013;139(1):5380.
... This might be related to a different perception of risk ), a complex phenomenon that includes both the perceived likelihood of getting sick (personal vulnerability) and the perceived harmfulness for one's health (disease severity) Sheeran and Abraham, 1996). The perceived risk has been positively associated with people's adherence to precautionary measures during previous respiratory infectious outbreaks and also COVID-19 pandemic (Niepel et al., 2020;Wise et al., 2020;. Conversely, individuals who perceive a low risk might not sufficiently engage in preventive behaviors, jeopardizing their own and others' health; for instance, unrealistic optimism about the likelihood of getting sick with COVID-19 in comparison to peers has been reported (Dolinski et al., 2020;Monzani et al., 2021). ...
... The keystone is a perception of risk that matches the real threat and that promotes an optimal engagement in preventive actions: indeed, risk perception can be modulated. Intense exposure to disease-related information through the media influenced the perception of risk for other respiratory infectious diseases (Barennes et al., 2010;Han et al., 2014;, whereas the government's health communications have been effective in raising awareness about the risk for COVID-19 (Wise et al., 2020). The understanding of the factors that shape risk perception is, thus, fundamental because it might help to identify those targets more in need of a risk re-appraisal and requiring an extra communicative effort. ...
... Fear is a basic and fundamental emotion for survival, which is presented as a response to a specific and imminent perceived threat (Schimmenti et al., 2020;Starcevic et al., 2020). Studies indicate that feeling at risk of being infected allows for greater engagement in certain health prevention behaviors, such as hand washing and maintaining social distancing during the early stages of a pandemic (Wise et al., 2020). Inversely, the absence of fear can be detrimental, generating a decrease in hygiene behaviors and leading to ignoring measures aimed at mitigating the spread of the disease (Taylor, 2019). ...
... The second narrative (group 2) described the declaration as having effects on both respondents' daily lives and their perceptions of COVID-19. Furthermore, as observed by Wise, Zbozinek, Michelini, Hagan, and Mobbs (2020), the risk perception model identifies that the perceived risk of oneself getting infected as being one of the most important determinant of behavior modification. Thus, it is crucial to determine if the state of emergency changed the perceptions of respondents toward COVID-19, and if so, how perceptions changed. ...
... Given such varied implications, this study suggests that the declaration of a state of emergency was a significant COVID-19 disease control measure despite the fact that many viewed this measure as being implemented too late. Reflecting on the findings of Wise et al. (2020) that suggest that the perception of risk was one of the triggers for people responding to and adopting behavior modification measures, even though many respondents adopted preventative measures, it is difficult to contribute these changes to the declaration. Indeed, although the declaration resulted in a heightened sense of awareness concerning the pandemic situation in Japan, only a small number of respondents (2 to 3) explicitly mentioned that the declaration caused them to perceive COVID-19 as a threat. ...
... As Wise et al. (2020) suggest, the perception that one is personally at risk of being infected with COVID-19 motivates people to adopt behavior modification measures. To examine this further, we asked respondents to first identify whether they had witnessed any positive COVID-19 cases around them and whether they felt that they were likely to contract COVID-19, and if so, why. ...
Article
This study examines social measures meant to stop the spread of COVID-19 in Japan, where COVID-19 control relies heavily on the voluntary compliance of citizens. We examine how the government's campaign for controlling COVID-19 is perceived by people, and how these measures influenced people’s attitudes and behavior. This paper specifically discusses how people’s perceptions regarding COVID-19 control measures temper orthodox understandings of behavior modification, such as perceived threats, and how it influences their attitude and behavior. Qualitative data collected from an online open-ended questionnaire was cross-sectionally analyzed. This study argues that high risk perception is actually just one of many triggers of behavior modification. That is, people responsive to risk perception had to match actual hazards to perceived real risk. Furthermore, popular sources of information on COVID-19, like news and social media, were found to substantially influence people’s behavior. In particular, whether campaigns were easy to understand, whether there were societal expectations for people to follow behavior modifications, and whether people thought measures were effective at preventing COVID-19 infection had a strong impact on the adoption of behavior modification. These findings thus fill in lacuna in existing scholarship concerning the rationality behind people’s decision to voluntary adopt behavior modification measures.
... Recent work on the COVID-19 pandemic has also pointed to perceived risk as an important factor for explaining compliance with infection control measures. A survey of Americans during the early phase of the pandemic (March [11][12][13][14][15][16] found that proclivity to follow infection control measures was predicted by the perceived severity of contracting COVID-19 [14]. The association between perceived risk and knowledge of COVID-19, and the adoption of protective measures has been found in a number of countries [e.g. in China; 15; in Saudi Arabia; 16; Egypt and Nigeria; 17; and in Germany; 18]. ...
... Recent work on the COVID-19 pandemic has also pointed to perceived risk as an important factor for explaining compliance with infection control measures. A survey of Americans during the early phase of the pandemic (March [11][12][13][14][15][16] found that proclivity to follow infection control measures was predicted by the perceived severity of contracting COVID-19 [14]. The association between perceived risk and knowledge of COVID-19, and the adoption of protective measures has been found in a number of countries [e.g. in China; 15; in Saudi Arabia; 16; Egypt and Nigeria; 17; and in Germany; 18]. ...
... This is in line with previous research as it is typically assumed that perceiving a situation as threatening will motivate people to take precautions, while seeing a situation as safe can induce indifference towards protective behavior [8]. It also supports previous research indicating associations between perceived risk and compliance from the H1N1 pandemic [11,12] and similar findings from correlational data in the COVID-19 pandemic [14,19]. Similar to Bish and Michie (2010), we also found that female participants indicated higher intentions to comply than men, and that older (60 or above) participants were slightly more certain of complying than younger participants. ...
Article
Full-text available
To limit an infectious outbreak, the public must be informed about the infection risk and be motivated to comply with infection control measures. Perceiving a situation as threatening and seeing benefits to complying may be necessary to motivate for compliance. The current study used a preregistered survey experiment with a 2-by-2 between-subject design to investigate if emphasizing high infection risk and appealing to societal benefits impacted intention to comply with infection control measures. The results from a representative Norwegian sample (N = 2533) show that describing a high (as opposed to low) personal risk scenario had a small main effect on compliance. Further, appealing to public (as opposed to self-interested) benefits also had a small main effect. There was no interaction between risk scenario and motivational emphasis. The results suggest that to maximize compliance, information about disease outbreak should emphasize the individual risk of contracting the disease, and could also underline the public value of limiting infection spread. These findings can inform health authorities about the motives underlying compliance with infection control measures during an infectious disease outbreak.
... At the individual level, these interventions included requiring people to engage in prosocial action, including behaviours that may inconvenience individuals, but protect the collective, such as movement restriction, social distancing, hygiene practices, and contact tracing (Orom et al. 2021). In practice, how well these preventive methods and behaviours can be pursued is influenced by people's knowledge, risk perceptions and understanding of the disease Wise et al. 2020;Zhang et al. 2020), as well as by people's anxiety levels, historical exposure to contagious diseases, social and cultural worldviews of individualism, and government responses and trust (Dryhurst et al. 2020;Nino et al. 2021). These cognitive outcomes of COVID-19, however, may vary by sociodemographic factors, such as gender, age, income, education, ability to access to COVID-19-related information and health literacy, and race and ethnicity (Geldsetzer 2020; Reiter and Katz 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Aim Asians are the second largest and fastest growing non-European population in New Zealand but are under-researched in terms of their COVID-19 pandemic response. The paper aims to illustrates Asians’ risk perceptions and knowledge of COVID-19, and self-protection practices to avoid infection and prevent community transmission. Subject and methods An online survey was used to collect data and received 402 valid responses. Data analyses included: 1) a descriptive analysis by using Chi-square tests and a Kruskal-Wallis rank sum tests to explore associations between responses and the four demographic variables (i.e. age, gender, country of origin/ethnicity, and region); and 2) a correlation analysis between different survey objectives. Results The descriptive analysis of the survey found that while ethnicity (within the Asian category) was the most influential variable that resulted in varying responses to many questions, gender and age were other two important variables in influencing the answering patterns. The correlation analysis found a positive correlation between the perceived ‘dangerousness’ of COVID-19 and respondents’ overall compliance behaviour to New Zealand authorities’ recommendations to prevent spread of COVID-19. Conclusion The majority of the respondents provided correct answers to the questions about the vulnerable populations, symptoms, asymptomatic transmission and potential sequelae of COVID-19; however, their understanding of the availability of a cure for, and the incubation period of COVID-19 was not consistent with the official information. The research also found that the higher perceived dangerousness of COVID-19, the better compliance to self-protection practices among the surveyed population.
... The older people, females, educated people, and one having high anxiety or trust in government policies are more likely to modify their 10 behavior accordingly. It is yet not clear however, that how much knowledge people have regarding COVID-19 risks, how they are modifying their lifestyle during this pandemic, and factors that determine 11 spread and control of disease. Pakistan being a developing country is facing its own challenges like poverty, illiteracy, religious beliefs, and limited medical resources. ...
... Como resultado del fenómeno social que trajo esta pandemia, se evidenciaron cambios en diferentes áreas vitales de la población, entre ellas se encuentran: el desarrollo económico (Abel & Gietel-Basten, 2020;Bashir, Ma, & Shahzad, 2020;Noy, Doan, Ferrarini, & Park, 2020), salud mental (Auerbach et al., 2018;Cao et al., 2020;Huang & Zhao, 2020;Husky, Kovess-Masfety, & Swendsen, 2020;Kutana & Lau, 2020), conductas de prevención de salud (Ahmad, Iram, & Jabeen, 2020;Berg & Lin, 2020;Melki et al., 2020;Prasetyo, Castillo, Salonga, Sia, & Seneta, 2020;Wise, Zbozinek, Michelini, Hagan, & mobbs, 2020), educación (Allen, Rowan, & Singh, 2020;Baglioni et al., 2020;Marinoni, Van't Land, & Jensen, 2020;Thevannoor, 2020;Usak, Masalimova, Cherdymova, & Shaidullina, 2020) y conductas diarias Liu X et al., 2020;Muto et al., 2020) . Cada una de estas áreas es investigada desde el inicio de este fenómeno ya que es necesario entender los cambios que se han dado y obtener información suficiente para poder enfrentarnos a ellos. ...
Article
Full-text available
El desarrollo de la pandemia sanitaria por COVID-19 ha resultado en un amplio fenómeno social y de salud integral. A partir de esto, alrededor del mundo se ha visto la necesidad de estudiar las diferentes áreas de afectación, protocolos de intervención, y, por las características del fenómeno, también de prevención masiva del COVID-19. En Bolivia, hasta el momento, no se cuenta con información sobre las conductas de prevención que fueron adoptadas por la población. En este estudio se describe el cumplimiento de conductas de prevención sanitaria en 561 personas pertenecientes a la comunidad de la Universidad Privada de Santa Cruz de la Sierra (UPSA) durante los primeros meses de aislamiento sanitario rígido en la ciudad de Santa Cruz de la Sierra. La muestra estudiada expresa alta frecuencia en el cumplimiento de conductas de prevención, entre las que se encuentran conductas de evitación de contacto y conductas de limpieza e higiene. También se expresan altos niveles de auto-eficacia percibida para su uso, y alta intención de mantención una vez finalizada la restricción sanitaria. Estas conductas son motivadas principalmente por el interés en el cuidado personal y por responsabilidad social. Se encontró una relación directa pero leve entre la cantidad de conductas realizadas y la cantidad de horas con las que las personas se informan sobre el COVID-19.
... The above finding is in line with Wise et al. (2020), who found in their study, that feeling personally at risk of infection predicted a greater propensity to engage in hand washing and social distancing behaviours in the early stages of the pandemic. The finding also agrees with Li et al. (2020)'s study, which found that subjectively judged self-control attenuated the association between perceived Covid-19 severity and worse mental health. ...
Article
This study investigated the relationship between Covid-19 anxiety and compliance with preventive protocol among healthcare workers in Delta Central Senatorial District. Four hypotheses guided the study. The correlational research design was employed in the study. The population for the study comprised 1,296 healthcare workers in Delta Central Senatorial District. A sample size of 278 health workers was drawn from health facilities through Proportionate Stratified and convenience sampling techniques. The instrument used for data collection was a questionnaire. The instrument was assessed for face, content, and construct validities by experts, while the Cronbach alpha reliability coefficient was used to check for the reliability of the items, and it yielded a coefficient of 0.89 and 0.71 for Covid-19 Anxiety Rating Scale and Compliance with Preventive Protocols Rating Scale respectively. The hypotheses were tested with regression statistics and Fisher-z statistics at a 0.05 level of significance. The findings of the study revealed that there is a significant relationship between health workers with low Covid-19 anxiety and compliance with preventive protocols; that there is a significant relationship between health workers with moderate Covid-19 anxiety and compliance with preventive protocols; and that there is a significant relationship between health workers with high Covid-19 anxiety and compliance with preventive protocols. The finding, however, revealed that there is no significant moderating impact of gender in the relationship between Covid-19 anxiety and compliance with preventive protocols. The study recommended amongst others, that health workers with a low level of Covid-19 anxiety should be reminded and sensitised that the fact that they have a low level of anxiety should not deter them from complying with Covid-19 preventive protocols. Article visualizations: </p
... com/ us-news/ 2021/ may/ 31/ us-gun-sales-rise-pande mic). Some research supports this interpretation, with data suggesting that more substantial changes in COVID-19 risk perception may have occurred prior to the beginning of our study 39,40 . Another possibility is that objective indices of COVID-19 threat, like number of cases and deaths, are not the only contributing factor for subjective threat perception. ...
Article
Full-text available
Prior laboratory research has suggested that humans may become more prosocial in stressful or threatening situations, but it is unknown whether the link between prosociality and defense generalizes to real-life. Here, we examined the association between defensive responses to a real-world threat (the COVID-19 pandemic) and everyday altruism. Four independent samples of 150 (N = 600) US residents were recruited online at 4 different timepoints, and self-report measures of perceived COVID-19 threat, defensive emotions (e.g., stress and anxiety), and everyday altruism were collected. Our operationalization of defensive emotions was inspired by the threat imminence framework, an ecological model of how humans and animals respond to varying levels of threat. We found that perceived COVID-19 threat was associated with higher levels of everyday altruism (assessed by the Self-report Altruism scale). Importantly, there was a robust association between experiencing acute anxiety and high physiological arousal during the pandemic (responses typically characteristic of higher perceived threat imminence), and propensity to engage in everyday altruism. Non-significant or negative associations were found with less acute defensive responses like stress. These findings support a real-life relation between defensive and altruistic motivation in humans, which may be modulated by perceived threat imminence.
Article
The new coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) has become a complex issue around the world. As the disease advancing and death rates are continuously increasing, governments are trying to control the situation by implementing different response policies. In order to implement appropriate policies, we need to consider the behavior of the people. Risk perception (RP) is a critical component in many health behavior change theories studies. People's RP can shape their behavior. This research presents a system dynamics (SD) model of the COVID-19 outbreak considering RP. The proposed model considers effective factors on RP, including different media types, awareness, and public acceptable death rate. In addition, the simplifying assumption of permanent immunity due to infection has been eliminated, and reinfection is considered; thus, different waves of the pandemic have been simulated. Using the presented model, the trend of advancing and death rates due to the COVID-19 pandemic in Iran can be predicted. Some policies are proposed for pandemic management. Policies are categorized as the capacity of hospitals, preventive behaviors, and accepted death rate. The results show that the proposed policies are effective. In this case, reducing the accepted death rate was the most effective policy to manage the pandemics. About 20% reduction in the accepted death rate causes about 23% reduction in cumulative death and delays at epidemic peak. The mean daily error in predicting the death rate is less than 10%.
Article
Full-text available
Undetected cases The virus causing coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) has now become pandemic. How has it managed to spread from China to all around the world within 3 to 4 months? Li et al. used multiple sources to infer the proportion of early infections that went undetected and their contribution to virus spread. The researchers combined data from Tencent, one of the world's largest social media and technology companies, with a networked dynamic metapopulation model and Bayesian inference to analyze early spread within China. They estimate that ∼86% of cases were undocumented before travel restrictions were put in place. Before travel restriction and personal isolation were implemented, the transmission rate of undocumented infections was a little more than half that of the known cases. However, because of their greater numbers, undocumented infections were the source for ∼80% of the documented cases. Immediately after travel restrictions were imposed, ∼65% of cases were documented. These findings help to explain the lightning-fast spread of this virus around the world. Science , this issue p. 489
Article
Full-text available
Uncertainty about how our choices will affect others infuses social life. Past research suggests uncertainty has a negative effect on prosocial behaviour1–12 by enabling people to adopt self-serving narratives about their actions1,13. We show that uncertainty does not always promote selfishness. We introduce a distinction between two types of uncertainty that have opposite effects on prosocial behaviour. Previous work focused on outcome uncertainty (uncertainty about whether or not a decision will lead to a particular outcome). However, as soon as people’s decisions might have negative consequences for others, there is also impact uncertainty (uncertainty about how others’ well-being will be impacted by the negative outcome). Consistent with past research1–12, we found decreased prosocial behaviour under outcome uncertainty. In contrast, prosocial behaviour was increased under impact uncertainty in incentivized economic decisions and hypothetical decisions about infectious disease threats. Perceptions of social norms paralleled the behavioural effects. The effect of impact uncertainty on prosocial behaviour did not depend on the individuation of others or the mere mention of harm, and was stronger when impact uncertainty was made more salient. Our findings offer insights into communicating uncertainty, especially in contexts where prosocial behaviour is paramount, such as responding to infectious disease threats.
Article
Full-text available
The number of online experiments conducted with subjects recruited via online platforms has grown considerably in the recent past. While one commercial crowdworking platform - Amazon's Mechanical Turk - basically has established and since dominated this field, new alternatives offer services explicitly targeted at researchers. In this article, we present www.prolific.ac and lay out its suitability for recruiting subjects for social and economic science experiments. After briefly discussing key advantages and challenges of online experiments relative to lab experiments, we trace the platform's historical development, present its features, and contrast them with requirements for different types of social and economic experiments.
Article
Full-text available
Influenza pandemics can severely impact human health and society. Understanding public perception and behavior toward influenza pandemics is important for minimizing the effects of such events. Public perception and behavior are expected to change over the course of an influenza pandemic, but this idea has received little attention in previous studies. Our study aimed to understand the dynamics of public perception and behavior over the course of the 2009 H1N1 influenza pandemic. Three consecutive cross-sectional surveys were administered among Beijing residents with random-digit dialing techniques in March 2008 and August and November 2009. Effective samples of 507, 508 and 1006 respondents were interviewed in each of the three surveys, respectively. The mean scores of risk perception were low to moderate across the three surveys. The perceived risk of infection of self was significantly lower than that of the community, revealing an optimistic bias. Longitudinally, the perceived risk of contracting H1N1 increased, whereas the perceived risk of being unable to obtain medicine and medical care once influenza permeated the community first increased and then decreased. Responsive actions toward influenza varied. Most respondents took actions that required little extra effort, such as ventilating rooms; these actions did not change over time. Comparatively, a smaller number of respondents took actions for coping with influenza, such as vaccination; however, these actions were taken by an increasing number of respondents over time. The association between risk perception and behavior was unstable. Positive, insignificant, and negative associations were obtained in the three surveys. In conclusion, the evolving patterns of risk perception and responsive behavior over the course of an influenza pandemic are sensitive to how risk and behavior are defined and scoped.
Article
Full-text available
For researchers and practitioners interested in social relationships, the question remains as to how large social networks typically are, and how their size and composition change across adulthood. On the basis of predictions of socioemotional selectivity theory and social convoy theory, we conducted a meta-analysis on age-related social network changes and the effects of life events on social networks using 277 studies with 177,635 participants from adolescence to old age. Cross-sectional as well as longitudinal studies consistently showed that (a) the global social network increased up until young adulthood and then decreased steadily, (b) both the personal network and the friendship network decreased throughout adulthood, (c) the family network was stable in size from adolescence to old age, and (d) other networks with coworkers or neighbors were important only in specific age ranges. Studies focusing on life events that occur at specific ages, such as transition to parenthood, job entry, or widowhood, demonstrated network changes similar to such age-related network changes. Moderator analyses detected that the type of network assessment affected the reported size of global, personal, and family networks. Period effects on network sizes occurred for personal and friendship networks, which have decreased in size over the last 35 years. Together the findings are consistent with the view that a portion of normative, age-related social network changes are due to normative, age-related life events. We discuss how these patterns of normative social network development inform research in social, evolutionary, cultural, and personality psychology. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Risk perceptions are central to many health behavior theories. However, the relationship between risk perceptions and behavior, muddied by instances of inappropriate assessment and analysis, often looks weak. A meta-analysis of eligible studies assessing the bivariate association between adult vaccination and perceived likelihood, susceptibility, or severity was conducted. Thirty-four studies met inclusion criteria (N = 15,988). Risk likelihood (pooled r = .26), susceptibility (pooled r = .24), and severity (pooled r = .16) significantly predicted vaccination behavior. The risk perception-behavior relationship was larger for studies that were prospective, had higher quality risk measures, or had unskewed risk or behavior measures. The consistent relationships between risk perceptions and behavior, larger than suggested by prior meta-analyses, suggest that risk perceptions are rightly placed as core concepts in theories of health behavior.
Article
Emergence of severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2, previously provisionally named 2019 novel coronavirus or 2019-nCoV) disease (COVID-19) in China at the end of 2019, has caused a large global outbreak and a major public health issue. As of February 11, 2020, data from the WHO has shown that more than 43,000 confirmed cases have been identified in 28 countries/regions, with more than 99% of the cases being detected in China. On January 30, 2020, WHO has declared COVID-19 as the sixth public health emergency of international concern. The SARS-CoV-2 is closely related to two bat-derived severe acute respiratory syndrome-like coronaviruses, bat-SL-CoVZC45 and bat-SL-CoVZXC21. It is spread by human-to-human transmission via droplets or direct contact, and infection has been estimated to have mean incubation period of 6.4 days and a basic reproduction number of 2.24-3.58. Among the patients with pneumonia caused by the SARS-CoV-2 (novel coronavirus pneumonia or Wuhan pneumonia), fever was the most common symptom, followed by cough. Bilateral lung involvement with ground glass opacity was the most common finding from computerized tomography images of the chest. Although the one case of SARS-CoV-2 pneumonia in the United States responding well to remdesivir, which is now undergoing a clinical trial in China. Currently, controlling infection to prevent the spread of the SARS-CoV-2 is the primary intervention being used. However, public health authorities should keep monitoring the situation closely, as the more we can learn about this novel virus and its associated outbreak, the better we can respond.
Article
Purpose: A new strain of H1N1 influenza, also known as swine flu was confirmed in the UK in May 2009 and has spread to over 100 countries around the world causing the World Health Organization to declare a global flu pandemic. The primary objectives of this review are to identify the key demographic and attitudinal determinants of three types of protective behaviour during a pandemic: preventive, avoidant, and management of illness behaviours, in order to describe conceptual frameworks in which to better understand these behaviours and to inform future communications and interventions in the current outbreak of swine flu and subsequent influenza pandemics. Methods: Web of Science and PubMed databases were searched for references to papers on severe acute respiratory syndrome, avian influenza/flu, H5N1, swine influenza/flu, H1N1, and pandemics. Forward searching of the identified references was also carried out. In addition, references were gleaned from an expert panel of the Behaviour and Communications sub-group of the UK Scientific Pandemic Influenza Advisory Group. Papers were included if they reported associations between demographic factors, attitudes, and a behavioural measure (reported, intended, or actual behaviour). Results: Twenty-six papers were identified that met the study inclusion criteria. The studies were of variable quality and most lacked an explicit theoretical framework. Most were cross-sectional in design and therefore not predictive over time. The research shows that there are demographic differences in behaviour: being older, female and more educated, or non-White, is associated with a higher chance of adopting the behaviours. There is evidence that greater levels of perceived susceptibility to and perceived severity of the diseases and greater belief in the effectiveness of recommended behaviours to protect against the disease are important predictors of behaviour. There is also evidence that greater levels of state anxiety and greater trust in authorities are associated with behaviour. Conclusions: The findings from this review can be broadly explained by theories of health behaviour. However, theoretically driven prospective studies are required to further clarify the relationship between demographic factors, attitudes, and behaviour. The findings suggest that intervention studies and communication strategies should focus on particular demographic groups and on raising levels of perceived threat of the pandemic disease and belief in the effectiveness of measures designed to protect against it.
Trajectories of public psycho-behavioural responses relating to influenza A(H7N9) over the winter of 2014-15 in Hong Kong
  • Q Liao
  • P Wu
  • Wwt Lam
  • B J Cowling
  • R Fielding
Liao Q, Wu P, Lam WWT, Cowling BJ, Fielding R. Trajectories of public psycho-behavioural responses relating to influenza A(H7N9) over the winter of 2014-15 in Hong Kong. Psychology & Health. 2019 Feb 1;34(2):162-80.