ArticlePDF Available

Examining the Mutual Relations Between Language and Mathematics: A Meta-Analysis

American Psychological Association
Psychological Bulletin
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study presents a meta-analysis of the relation between language and mathematics. A moderate relation between language and mathematics was found in 344 studies with 393 independent samples and more than 360,000 participants, r = .42, 95% CI [.40, .44]. Moderation and partial correlation analyses revealed the following: (a) More complicated language and mathematics skills are associated with stronger relations between language and mathematics; after partialling out working memory and intelligence, rapid automatized naming showed the strongest relation to numerical knowledge; (b) The relation between language and mathematics was stronger among native language speakers than among second-language learners, but this difference was not found after partialling out working memory and intelligence; (c) Working memory and intelligence together explained over 50% of the variance in the relation between language and mathematics and explained more variance in such relations involving complex mathematics skills; (d) Language and mathematics predicted the development of one another even after controlling for initial performance. These findings suggest that we may use language as a medium to communicate, represent, and retrieve mathematics knowledge as well as to facilitate working memory and reasoning during mathematics performance and learning. With development, the use of language to retrieve mathematics knowledge may be more important for foundational mathematics skills, which in turn further strengthens linguistic thought processes for performing more advanced mathematics tasks. Such use of language may boost the mutual effects of cognition and mathematics across development.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Psychological Bulletin
Examining the Mutual Relations Between Language and
Mathematics: A Meta-Analysis
Peng Peng, Xin Lin, Zehra Emine Ünal, Kejin Lee, Jessica Namkung, Jason Chow, and Adam Sales
Online First Publication, April 16, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bul0000231
CITATION
Peng, P., Lin, X., Ünal, Z. E., Lee, K., Namkung, J., Chow, J., & Sales, A. (2020, April 16). Examining
the Mutual Relations Between Language and Mathematics: A Meta-Analysis. Psychological
Bulletin. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bul0000231
Examining the Mutual Relations Between Language and Mathematics:
A Meta-Analysis
Peng Peng and Xin Lin
The University of Texas at Austin Zehra Emine U
¨nal
University of Missouri
Kejin Lee and Jessica Namkung
University of Nebraska–Lincoln Jason Chow
Virginia Commonwealth University
Adam Sales
The University of Texas at Austin
This study presents a meta-analysis of the relation between language and mathematics. A moderate
relation between language and mathematics was found in 344 studies with 393 independent samples and
more than 360,000 participants, r.42, 95% CI [.40, .44]. Moderation and partial correlation analyses
revealed the following: (a) more complicated language and mathematics skills were associated with
stronger relations between language and mathematics; after partialing out working memory and intelli-
gence, rapid automatized naming showed the strongest relation to numerical knowledge; (b) the relation
between language and mathematics was stronger among native language speakers than among second-
language learners, but this difference was not found after partialing out working memory and intelli-
gence; (c) working memory and intelligence together explained over 50% of the variance in the relation
between language and mathematics and explained more variance in such relations involving complex
mathematics skills; (d) language and mathematics predicted the development of one another even after
controlling for initial performance. These findings suggest that we may use language as a medium to
communicate, represent, and retrieve mathematics knowledge as well as to facilitate working memory
and reasoning during mathematics performance and learning. With development, the use of language to
retrieve mathematics knowledge may be more important for foundational mathematics skills, which in
turn further strengthens linguistic thought processes for performing more advanced mathematics tasks.
Such use of language may boost the mutual effects of cognition and mathematics across development.
Public Significance Statement
We use language as a medium to communicate, represent, and retrieve mathematics knowledge, as
when we learn mathematics vocabulary, follow classroom instruction, and understand mathematics
texts. Meanwhile, we also use language to facilitate the processes of memory and reasoning during
mathematics performance and learning. With development, the use of language to retrieve mathe-
matics knowledge may become more important for foundational mathematics skills, which frees up
cognitive resources for more advanced mathematics performance; such use of language may boost
the mutual effects between cognition and mathematics during development.
Keywords: language, mathematics, mutualism, medium, development
Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bul0000231.supp
XPeng Peng and Xin Lin, Department of Special Education, College of
Education, The University of Texas at Austin; Zehra Emine U
¨nal, Depart-
ment of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri; XKejin Lee,
The Nebraska Center for Research on Children, Youth, Families and
Schools, University of Nebraska–Lincoln; Jessica Namkung, Department
of Special Education and Communication Disorders, University of
Nebraska–Lincoln; Jason Chow, School of Education, Virginia Common-
wealth University; Adam Sales, College of Education, The University of
Texas at Austin.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Peng Peng or
Xin Lin, Department of Special Education, College of Education, The Uni-
versity of Texas at Austin, SZB 408D, 1912 Speedway STOP D5300, Austin,
TX 78712. E-mail: kevpp2004@hotmail.com or lxjy1105@hotmail.com
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Psychological Bulletin
© 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 2, No. 999, 000
ISSN: 0033-2909 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bul0000231
1
Language is a fundamental skill with which we express our
ideas, feelings, and queries to the world around us. It includes, but
is not limited to, phonological processing (manipulation and re-
trieval of sounds), vocabulary/morphology (the meaning of words),
and oral comprehension (making sense of oral discourse; see, e.g.,
Barre, Morgan, Doyle, & Anderson, 2011; Catts, Adlof, & Ellis
Weismer, 2006; Elbro, 1996; Language and Reading Research
Consortium, 2015; Wagner, Torgesen, Rashotte, & Pearson, 1999;
Zwaan & Radvansky, 1998). Language is also a critical skill for
academic performance, which includes reading and mathematics
(Chow & Wehby, 2018; Hoover & Gough, 1990; Kleemans,
Segers, & Verhoeven, 2011; LeFevre et al., 2010). The relation
between language and reading is well evidenced, such that word
decoding heavily involves phonological processing especially
early on, whereas reading comprehension is greatly influenced by
vocabulary and oral comprehension across one’s development
(Francis, Kulesz, & Benoit, 2018; Hjetland et al., 2019; Hoover &
Gough, 1990).
In contrast, findings on the relation between language and
mathematics remain mixed. Some have reported strong correla-
tions (rs.70; e.g., Merz et al., 2015; Pind, Gunnarsdóttir, &
Jóhannesson, 2003), and others have found weak to nonsignificant
correlations (rs.20; e.g., Mellard, Woods, & McJunkin, 2015;
Mestre, 1981). It is necessary to gain better insight into the relation
between language and mathematics, and the factors that influence
this relation. Such knowledge is important not only for theorists of
language and mathematical cognition, but also for educators so
that they can make informed decisions about how to use language
and to what degrees language should be emphasized in mathemat-
ics instruction.
To our knowledge, three reviews have specifically investigated
the relation between language and mathematics (Chow & Jacobs,
2016; de Araujo, Roberts, Willey, & Zahner, 2018; Koponen,
Georgiou, Salmi, Leskinen, & Aro, 2017). Chow and Jacobs
(2016) reviewed the relation between oral language and fraction
outcomes among school-age students. Based on three studies con-
ducted in the U.S., the authors suggested that oral language plays
a meaningful role in fraction performance, and that cognition may
partially explain the relation between oral language and fraction
performance. Koponen, Georgiou, Salmi, Leskinen, and Aro
(2017) investigated the relation between mathematics and rapid
automatized naming (RAN; one component of phonological pro-
cessing that mainly taps verbal retrieval abilities). Across 38
studies, Koponen et al. (2017) found a significant correlation
between RAN and mathematics, r.37; 95% CI [.33, .42]. RAN
was more strongly correlated with calculations (e.g., solving stan-
dard arithmetic problems) than with general mathematics perfor-
mance. Among calculation tasks, RAN was more strongly corre-
lated with tasks that tap automatic fact retrieval (single-digit
calculations and mathematics fluency tasks). These findings sug-
gest that the relation between RAN and mathematics can be partly
explained by the retrieval of mathematics representations from
long-term memory (Georgiou, Wei, Inoue, & Deng, in press;
Koponen et al., 2017).
In the third review, de Araujo, Roberts, Willey, and Zahner
(2018) synthesized qualitative literature, with a focus on how
English language learners learn mathematics in a multilinguistic
educational setting. The authors suggest that language is a primary
medium for English language learners’ acquisition of mathematics
knowledge. That is, curricula incorporating a language component
and multimodal instruction such as visual presentations (to com-
pensate for limited English language proficiency) can facilitate
mathematics learning among English language learners. They also
suggested that it is important for English language learners to draw
on their first language in mathematics learning, because it is easier
to engage in mathematical reasoning in one’s first language than in
one’s second language (e.g., de Araujo et al., 2018; Martin &
Fuchs, 2019).
Taken together, the findings of these reviews shed some light on
the relation between language and mathematics. However, it re-
mains unclear whether different types of language skills might be
related to different types of mathematics skills in different ways—
whether these relations are influenced by task characteristics (e.g.,
measurement format) and sample characteristics (e.g., second-
language learning status), whether these relations may differ dur-
ing development, and how these relations may be influenced by
important cognitive skills such as working memory and intelli-
gence. The present study aims to contribute to the literature by
addressing these gaps.
The Language Function Hypothesis in Mathematics
This meta-analysis is guided by a general theoretical frame-
work: the function hypothesis of language, which suggests that
language serves many functions in our lives such as exchanging/
delivering messages/information, expressing our feelings and atti-
tudes, and informing our thoughts (Bruner, 1966; Fetzer & Tiede-
mann, 2018; Vygotsky, 1986). In these ways, language can lead to
the development of other skills such as mathematics. Among all
language functions, two that are fundamental to mathematics de-
velopment are language as a medium of exchange and language as
a means of thought (Bruner, 1966; Fetzer & Tiedemann, 2018).
Medium Function Hypothesis
The medium function hypothesis of language in mathematics
posits that we use language as a tool for communicating mathe-
matics knowledge with others, and building and retrieving repre-
sentations of mathematics knowledge from long-term memory,
implying a causal relation between language and subsequent math-
ematics development (Bruner, 1966; Dehaene, 1992; Dehaene &
Cohen, 1995; Fetzer & Tiedemann, 2018; Gersten et al., 2009;
LeFevre et al., 2010). More specifically, both general language and
mathematics-specific language (i.e., mathematics vocabulary such
as hexagon, dividend, and numerator, and mathematical expres-
sions such as the interior angle of a triangle) are used to commu-
nicate mathematical information in school settings (Gersten et al.,
2009; National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, 2006) and to
construct mathematical meanings in general, particularly when
paired with symbolic or other visual information (Chow & Wehby,
2019; O’Halloran, 2005).
Different types of language are important for constructing math-
ematical representations. In the pathways to mathematics model,
LeFevre et al. (2010) proposed and demonstrated that both pho-
nological processing and vocabulary serve as important mediums
to form visual and verbal representations of the meaning of num-
bers in early childhood. In the triple-code model of number pro-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
2PENG ET AL.
cessing, Dehaene and colleagues (Dehaene, 1992; Dehaene &
Cohen, 1995) proposed that numerical tasks are supported by three
types of mental codes: the visual Arabic number code, the verbal
word code, and the analogue magnitude representation code. The
verbal word code is especially important at the beginning of formal
mathematics learning, linking the visual Arabic number code and
the analogue magnitude representation code (LeFevre et al., 2010).
Spelke (2017) proposed that children use language to learn to pair
number words with items in a set through the process of counting
to develop their abilities to represent exact numbers of unlimited
magnitude. Hecht, Torgesen, Wagner, and Rashotte (2001) sug-
gested that the quality of the phonological representations is im-
portant for arithmetic performance, such that children need to
transform numbers and operators into a speech-based code and use
phonological-based strategies such as counting to solve problems
(e.g., De Smedt et al., 2010).
Language also facilitates knowledge retrieval from long-term
memory during mathematics performance. It has been suggested
that arithmetic facts are stored in a language format (Spelke &
Tsivkin, 2001), and mathematics has specific vocabulary/expres-
sions that are also stored in a language format (O’Halloran, 2005).
Over the course of development, as students cumulatively build
their mathematics knowledge, the direct retrieval of arithmetic
facts and other mathematics knowledge (e.g., mathematics vocab-
ulary/expressions) from long-term memory can reduce cognitive
load (i.e., working memory demands) as they perform increasingly
difficult mathematics tasks (Koponen et al., 2017; Peng & Lin,
2019). In addition, many mathematics tasks consist of word prob-
lems, which also require general knowledge. The direct retrieval of
non-mathematics-specific knowledge from long-term memory can
facilitate the comprehension of word problems and reduce cogni-
tive load in solving them (Fuchs, Fuchs, Compton, Hamlett, &
Wang, 2015; Peng & Lin, 2019).
Thinking Function Hypothesis
In contrast to the medium function hypothesis, the thinking
function hypothesis of language for mathematics acknowledges
language as a medium for performing/learning mathematics, but
emphasizes that the use of language involves cognition (Daneman
& Merikle, 1996; Lombrozo, 2006; Peng, Wang, & Namkung,
2018; Vygotsky, 1986). That is, we use language to think about
more abstract mathematical concepts and relations between them,
as when we construct a schema and an equation in solving word
problems. We do this even for seemingly nonverbal mathematics
tasks, such as reasoning about orientation, locations, and patterns
in performing geometrical tasks (Bruner, 1966; Carruthers, 2002;
Fetzer & Tiedemann, 2018; Fuchs et al., 2015; Ingram, Andrews,
& Pitt, 2016; Sfard, 2008; Sidney, Hattikudur, & Alibali, 2015).
Even when we use language as a medium to share/learn mathe-
matics ideas with/from others or to construct mathematical mean-
ings, this use of language in turn affects our ideas about how to
make sense of mathematics problems (Fetzer & Tiedemann, 2018).
In comparison with the medium function hypothesis, which
implies that better language leads to better mathematics skills, the
thinking function hypothesis emphasizes that a third variable,
high-level cognition, underlies how we use language for mathe-
matics. Among many cognitive skills, working memory and intel-
ligence are human cognition’s core components (Ackerman, Beier,
& Boyle, 2005) that underlies the relation between language and
mathematics (e.g., as a third variable/confounder; see, e.g., Dane-
man & Merikle, 1996; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1990; Montgom-
ery, 2001; Peng, Wang, Wang, & Lin, 2019). The investigation of
whether working memory, intelligence, or both fully or partially
explain the relations between language and mathematics provides
not only relatively direct evidence on the thinking function of
language for mathematics, but also some indirect evidence on the
medium function of language for mathematics (i.e., the residual
variance on the relation between language and mathematics that is
not explained by working memory, intelligence, or both).
Working Memory
Working memory is the ability to simultaneously process and
store information (Baddeley, 1986). It is assumed to influence both
language and mathematics. Various language models agree that
language processing and development rely on working memory’s
limited capacity (Fedorenko, 2014; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1993;
Just & Carpenter, 1992). The ability to actively maintain and
integrate linguistic materials in working memory determines a
person’s success in learning, comprehending, and producing lan-
guage (Acheson & MacDonald, 2009; Baddeley, Gathercole, &
Papagno, 1998; Daneman & Carpenter, 1980; Ellis Weismer,
Evans, & Hesketh, 1999). Moreover, research on individuals with
specific language impairment (those with normal IQ but delayed
language development in the absence of any obvious cause) high-
lights that a deficit in working memory, across verbal and nonver-
bal domains, is one of the most distinctive markers of this group,
even when nonverbal cognitive skills are controlled for (Ellis
Weismer et al., 1999; Henry, Messer, & Nash, 2012; Im-Bolter,
Johnson, & Pascual-Leone, 2006; Marton, 2008; Marton &
Schwartz, 2003; Montgomery, Magimairaj, & Finney, 2010).
In mathematics, working memory is related to mathematics
performance both concurrently and prospectively (Bull & Lee,
2014; Geary, Nicholas, Li, & Sun, 2017; Lee & Bull, 2016). Many
mathematics tasks require individuals to actively maintain multiple
conceptions of mathematical expressions and intermediate num-
bers while solving problems, switching between strategies as ap-
propriate (e.g., Fuchs et al., 2005; Geary & Widaman, 1992; Peng,
Namkung, Barnes, & Sun, 2016; Raghubar, Barnes, & Hecht,
2010; Swanson & Beebe-Frankenberger, 2004). Especially for
more advanced mathematics tasks such as solving word problems,
working memory makes a unique contribution even after control-
ling for foundational mathematics skills (e.g., arithmetic; Swanson
& Sachse-Lee, 2001).
The effects of working memory on mathematics are not limited
to numerical processing (numerical working memory). Converging
evidence from behavioral and neuroimaging studies suggests that
verbal working memory is especially important for numerical
development (Menon, 2016; Raghubar et al., 2010) and for math-
ematics performance involving comprehension (e.g., word prob-
lems; Fuchs et al., 2015, 2018). Visuospatial working memory is
especially important for enhancing the proficiency of different
types of mathematics skills over time (Menon, 2016; Szucs,
Devine, Soltesz, Nobes, & Gabriel, 2014). Not surprisingly, work-
ing memory deficits across domains are often found to be the most
distinctive markers for children with various subtypes of mathematics
difficulties (Geary, 1993, 2004; Peng, Wang, & Namkung, 2018).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
3
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
In addition to the conventional model of working memory that
emphasizes simultaneous information processing and storage, the
long-term working memory model provides further explanations
for the relation between language and mathematics (Baddeley,
2000; Ericsson & Kintsch, 1995; Peng et al., 2018). In this model,
working memory is an important part of executive function (Bad-
deley, 1986; Miyake & Friedman, 2012; Miyake et al., 2000),
which closely collaborates with long-term memory in academic
learning/performance (Peng et al., 2018). Long-term memory sup-
plements working memory such that working memory, as an
“executive commander,” helps one evaluate and integrate incom-
ing information through the retrieval of relevant information from
long-term memory (Kirschner, Sweller, & Clark, 2006; Miller-
Cotto & Byrnes, 2019). Given that many mathematics tasks rely
heavily on mathematics fact retrieval (Koponen et al., 2017),
working memory can influence the relation between language and
mathematics, even if tasks tap retrieval skills primarily (Friso-van
den Bos, van der Ven, Kroesbergen, & van Luit, 2013; Peng et al.,
2016).
Working memory, language, and mathematics also seem to
influence each other mutually. Research shows that the relation
between working memory and mathematics performance can be
partially explained by language (Peng & Lin, 2019). The effects of
language-centered mathematics instruction partly depend on chil-
dren’s working memory, such that children with very low working
memory benefit more from structured instruction that emphasizes
conceptual understanding than do those with relatively high work-
ing memory, potentially due to a reduced cognitive load (Fuchs et
al., 2016, 2014; Sweller, 1994). Mathematics difficulties among
children with language disorders can be partially explained by
their working memory abilities (Cross, Joanisse, & Archibald,
2019; Fyfe, Matz, Hunt, & Alibali, 2019). Furthermore, the rela-
tion between mathematics development and working memory
growth is influenced by language proficiency, especially among
second-language learners who perform mathematics in their sec-
ond language (Swanson, Kong, & Petcu, 2018). Taken together,
these findings further suggest that working memory may be a key
factor in explaining the relation between language and mathemat-
ics.
Intelligence
Intelligence refers to the reasoning capacity to solve novel
complex problems by means of mental operations, such as drawing
inferences, forming concepts, classifying, identifying relations,
finding solutions, and so forth (Cattell, 1963; Newton & McGrew,
2010). Obviously, such reasoning is heavily involved in both
language and mathematics (Lombrozo, 2006; Peng et al., 2019;
Wechsler, 2008). In this review, reasoning and intelligence refer to
the same construct and are used interchangeably. Although some
researchers consider intelligence to be strongly related to working
memory (Kane, Hambrick, & Conway, 2005), in the present meta-
analysis, we consider intelligence to be a relatively independent
cognitive skill of working memory (Ackerman et al., 2005) and
examine its role in the relation between language and mathematics.
Across different languages, there are rules about sound combi-
nations in phonological processing. Phonological tasks or decod-
ing tasks that involve phonological processing often require the
analysis and manipulation of sound structures, which involve
intelligence especially in early stages of reading development and
among children with intellectual/learning disabilities (Levy, 2011;
Peng et al., 2019; Tiu, Thompson, & Lewis, 2003). The acquisition
and use of vocabulary often involve reasoning. For example, when
they read books, children often learn new vocabulary by using
context clues and inference-making skills to decipher the clues
(Cain, Oakhill, & Lemmon, 2004). Direct vocabulary instruction
in schools often emphasizes the analysis of word roots and affixes
(suffixes and prefixes) and the use of categorization rules (e.g.,
synonyms and antonyms; National Reading Panel, 2000). In ad-
dition, the use of vocabulary in comprehending language often
draws on inference-making skills. It is suggested that not just
knowledge of individual word meanings but also rich semantic
networks with robust connections between the meanings of words
associated by topic are important for easy, accurate inference-
making during comprehension (Ahmed et al., 2016; Currie & Cain,
2015). Oral comprehension is structurally complicated, and accu-
rately comprehending orally presented sentences/passages requires
careful analyses and consistency checks. The involvement of rea-
soning in oral comprehension increases as sentences/passages be-
come longer and more complicated (Evans & Stanovich, 2013;
Mata, Schubert, & Ferreira, 2014).
The relation between mathematics and intelligence is well doc-
umented (Ackerman & Lohman, 2003; Blair, Gamson, Thorne, &
Baker, 2005; Deary, Strand, Smith, & Fernandes, 2007; Geary,
2011; Peng et al., 2019). For foundational mathematics skills (e.g.,
numerical knowledge and calculations), reasoning is involved in
the mastery of numerical symbols, their relations and applications
in the number system, and the rules and principles for calculations
(Fuchs et al., 2006; Östergren & Träff, 2013). Word problems,
fractions, and algebra are advanced mathematics skills that build
on foundational skills. Reasoning is highly involved in developing
conceptual and procedural understanding of these advanced skills.
For instance, we use reasoning to make sense of the magnitudes of
fractions based on our understanding of the magnitudes of whole
numbers (Stein, Grover, & Henningsen, 1996). In addition, math-
ematics tasks are often presented via word problems (Common
Core State Standards Initiative, 2010), and reasoning is often
required to construct problem models or schema to formalize
conceptual relations among quantities and guide the application of
strategies for solutions (e.g., Fuchs et al., 2015; Jordan et al., 2013;
Kaufmann & Schmalstieg, 2003; Namkung & Fuchs, 2016; Re-
uhkala, 2001).
The role of intelligence in the relation between language and
mathematics can also be understood from the perspective of school
instruction. In mathematics curricula and instruction across cul-
tures, students are often encouraged to “explain” the process of
solving mathematics problems to their peers and teachers (Ingram,
Andrews, & Pitt, 2019). This is commonly approached through
verbal explanation or by explaining an accurate solution in a
written form (e.g., in explaining how to solve a problem, con-
structing viable arguments, and critiquing the reasoning of others).
Such explanations and demonstrations require reasoning and log-
ical inferences (Horne, Muradoglu, & Cimpian, 2019). That is,
when children produce explanations and make causal attributions,
they need to make logical inferences (Griffiths & Tenenbaum,
2009), and the extent to which children provide favorite answers in
response to certain features (e.g., to achieve simplicity) requires
explanatory reasoning (Wilson & Keil, 1998).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
4PENG ET AL.
Factors in the Language Function Framework
for Mathematics
Although the medium and thinking function hypotheses can
help explain how language is related to mathematics from a
theoretical perspective, these function-based hypotheses have
never been systematically investigated in mathematics learning
and performance, partly because the medium and thinking func-
tions of language are often considered “entangled” with each other
in the use of language (Carruthers, 2002). In the present meta-
analysis, we investigated several factors that may provide direct
and indirect evidence on the role of the medium and the thinking
functions of language in the relation between language and math-
ematics, respectively. These factors map directly onto this study’s
hypothesized moderators of the relation between language and
mathematics. We describe these factors as follows.
Types of Language/Mathematics
Different types of language skills may bear the medium function
to varying degrees in relation to different types of mathematics
tasks. For example, phonological processing (phonological aware-
ness and RAN) is often suggested to be heavily involved in
building numerical representations and retrieving mathematics
facts in long-term memory for numerical and calculation tasks
(e.g., Koponen et al., 2017; LeFevre et al., 2010; Singer, Strasser,
& Cuadro, 2019). By contrast, vocabulary and oral comprehension
are used in communicating mathematics knowledge in general
(Carey, 2009; Carruthers, 2002; Donlan, Cowan, Newton, &
Lloyd, 2007; Peng & Lin, 2019; Praet, Titeca, Ceulemans, &
Desoete, 2013; Singer et al., 2019). For example, oral comprehen-
sion may be generally important for the uptake of mathematics
instruction via receptive processes (e.g., syntax) that facilitate
comprehension of verbally delivered instruction (e.g., Chow &
Ekholm, 2019). Vocabulary and oral comprehension also play an
important role in developing/performing more advanced mathe-
matics skills such as solving word problems (Fuchs et al., 2015).
This may be because solving word problems differs from other
forms of mathematics competence in that it requires considerable
skills in reading comprehension, tapping vocabulary and oral com-
prehension to a greater extent (e.g., Cummins, Kintsch, Reusser, &
Weimer, 1988; Kintsch & Greeno, 1985).
Similarly, different types of language skills may bear the think-
ing function to varying degrees in relation to different types of
mathematics skills that also involve cognition to varying degrees.
According to the intrinsic cognitive load theory, an inherent level
of difficulty is associated with specific tasks (Chandler & Sweller,
1991; Sweller, 1994). Tasks with multiple interactive steps and
sequential thinking are assumed to be more difficult than tasks
involving fewer noninteractive steps. Thus, in comparison with the
relations between foundational language skills (e.g., phonological
processing and RAN) and foundational mathematics skills (e.g.,
numerical knowledge and calculations), the relations between
more complicated and semantically oriented language skills (e.g.,
vocabulary and oral language comprehension) and more compli-
cated mathematic skills such as solving word problems and frac-
tions may require more working memory and intelligence (Born-
stein, Hahn, Putnick, & Suwalsky, 2014; Chow & Jacobs, 2016;
Fuchs et al., 2015; Geary, 2004; Kintsch & Greeno, 1985; Lervåg,
Hulme, & Melby-Lervåg, 2018; Namkung & Fuchs, 2016).
Age (Development)
Mathematics development is accumulative, and the contribution
of cognitive skills and foundational mathematics skills to mathe-
matics performance varies with development. For example, lon-
gitudinal studies suggest that the importance of foundational
mathematics skills (numerical knowledge and calculations) in
subsequent mathematics achievement increases across elementary
and secondary grades, whereas cognitive skills are constantly
important for mathematic performance over time; cognitive skills
are more important than foundational mathematics skills for math-
ematics achievement only in early grades (Geary et al., 2017; Lee
& Bull, 2016). These findings likely reflect the increasingly im-
portant role of direct retrieval of mathematics knowledge from
long-term memory in partly reducing cognitive load on later math-
ematics performance (Geary et al., 2017; Peng & Lin, 2019).
Given such developmental features of mathematics, it is
reasonable to expect the relations between different language
skills and mathematics skills to vary with age under different
function hypotheses of language. For example, under the me-
dium function hypothesis of language, it is reasonable that the
relation between language and mathematics might not vary with
age, except with respect to foundational mathematics skills.
That is, numerical knowledge and calculations may be more
strongly related to phonological processing, especially RAN,
because performing these foundational tasks taps more
retrieval-based processes as children become more proficient
and automatic through development.
In contrast, under the thinking function hypothesis of language,
it is reasonable that the relations between different language skills
and different mathematics skills might vary with age. The relations
between phonological processing/vocabulary (relatively founda-
tional language skills) and various mathematics skills may de-
crease with age. The relation between oral comprehension and
mathematics, especially advanced mathematics skills such as word
problems, fractions, and algebra that require oral comprehension,
may stay stable or increase with age. Correspondingly, age may
not influence the impact of working memory and intelligence on
the relation between phonological processing and mathematics,
whereas working memory and intelligence may account for more
variance in the relation between oral comprehension and mathe-
matics with age.
Mutualism
Examining developmental relations may also help in further
testing of the thinking function of language in mathematics. Recent
research and reviews suggest that the relation between high-level
cognitive skills and academic performance is reciprocal, such that
both working memory/reasoning and academic skills influence
each other mutually during development (e.g., Kievit et al., 2017;
McArdle, Hamagami, Meredith, & Bradway, 2000; Peng et al.,
2019; Peng et al., 2018; Rindermann, Flores-Mendoza, & Mansur-
Alves, 2010; Van Der Maas et al., 2006). One rationale for the
impact of academic skills on cognition is that learning and per-
forming academic tasks often involve a constant use of working
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
5
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
memory and intelligence, which to some extent may serve as a
“long-term intervention” for these cognitive skills (Peng & Kievit,
2020; Peng et al., 2019; Peng et al., 2018). Moreover, the accu-
mulation of academic knowledge in long-term memory can further
strengthen the efficiency of using cognitive skills associated with
academic performance (Peng et al., 2018). Thus, if language
primarily serves the thinking function in mathematics, based on
mutualism, we would expect that language and mathematics pre-
dict each other from a longitudinal perspective.
Language Measurement
How language is measured may influence the evaluation of
cognitive load’s involvement in language skills. Language can be
measured in terms of reception or expression (Language and
Reading Research Consortium, 2015), respectively tapping one’s
ability to understand words, sentences, and passages or one’s
ability to put information into words and sentences grammatically
(Gleason & Ratner, 2005). Working memory is generally consid-
ered to show stronger relations with language expression or pro-
duction than with language reception because more information
(especially syntactic and grammatical knowledge) is simultane-
ously activated, processed, and stored in language production
(Acheson & MacDonald, 2009). This is especially so among
children, such that children with better working memory can
produce language that is more grammatically complex, contains a
richer array of words, and includes longer utterances than can
children with poorer working memory (Adams & Gathercole,
1995).
Second-Language Learning Status
Second-language learning status (using a second language to
learn/perform mathematics vs. using one’s first language to learn/
perform mathematics) may influence the medium function of
language in mathematics. Many studies have investigated how
English-language learners (those for whom English is a second
language) learn and perform mathematics in an English-speaking
environment (de Araujo et al., 2018). This research has adopted
various sociocultural models that emphasize the medium effect of
the English language on learning/performing mathematics in
English-language learners. It suggests that learning mathematics
requires the expression of mathematics in mathematics-specific
vocabulary and syntax (Pimm, 1987) and that English-language
learners need to be able to use English efficiently to acquire and
use mathematics-specific language in learning and performing
mathematics (Schleppegrell, 2007). English-language learners
who import meanings of mathematics vocabulary from their first
language to English often interpret them differently than intended
(Kazima, 2007). In addition, English-language learners can use
English to better communicate in mathematics-specific language
during mathematics instruction (e.g., Moschkovich, 2002;
Schleppegrell, 2007). All this evidence, taken together, suggests
that the medium function of language in mathematics may be
particularly important among second-language learners.
Research Questions
To summarize, this meta-analysis addresses four major ques-
tions.
Question 1: Is there a significant relation between language
and mathematics, and if so, what is the magnitude of this
relation?
Question 2: Does the relation between language and mathe-
matics vary as a function of types of language skills (i.e.,
phonological processing, vocabulary, oral comprehension,
comprehensive language ability), types of mathematics (i.e.,
numerical knowledge, calculations, word problems, fractions,
algebra, geometry, comprehensive mathematics skills),
second-language learning status (using a second language to
learn/perform mathematics vs. using the first language to
learn/perform mathematics), language measurement type (re-
ceptive vs. expressive), and age?
Question 3: What is the role of working memory and intelli-
gence in the relation between language and mathematics?
Specifically, is there a relation between language and mathe-
matics after controlling for working memory, intelligence, or
both? If so, how much variance can working memory, intel-
ligence, and both explain? Is the partial correlation between
language and mathematics, controlling for working memory
and intelligence, influenced by types of language skills, types
of mathematics, second-language learning status, and age?
Question 4: What is the longitudinal relation between lan-
guage and mathematics? Specifically, does language predict
mathematics after controlling for initial mathematics skills,
and does mathematics predict language after controlling for
initial language skills? What are the predictive powers of
different types of early language/mathematics on later
mathematics/language?
Method
Literature Search
Articles for this meta-analysis were identified in three ways.
First, the Communication & Mass Media Complete, Education
Source, ERIC, PsycINFO, and Medline databases were searched
for literature from the earliest possible start date until October
2018. Titles, abstracts, and keywords were searched using the
following terms: (language OR vocabulary OR linguistics OR
“listening comprehension” OR phonolog
OR morpholog
OR
syntax OR semantics OR OR “rapid naming” OR “rapid automat
naming” OR RAN) AND (math
OR arithmetic OR “number
sense” OR numerical OR calcul
OR computation OR “word
problem
” OR algebra OR geometry OR calculus OR fraction).
The asterisk enables the inclusion of different forms of search
terms (e.g., calcul
can include calculation and calculating). Sec-
ond, we searched unpublished literature through the Dissertation
and Masters Abstract indexes in ProQuest and the Cochrane Da-
tabase of Systematic Reviews. Third, we searched in previous
relevant reviews and also contacted researchers to request corre-
lation tables not provided in their reported studies. The initial
search yielded 22,828 studies. Three authors of this study then
reviewed all studies by titles and abstracts. After excluding 682
duplicate and 19,990 irrelevant articles, the remaining 2,156 were
closely reviewed using specific criteria (see Figure 1 for a flow
diagram of the inclusion of studies).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
6PENG ET AL.
First, studies had to include at least one quantitative task mea-
suring language and at least one quantitative task measuring math-
ematics. Language measures refer to the tasks that tap three lan-
guage skills: phonological processing including phonological
awareness (speech sound manipulation and short-term memory at
the word, onset-rime, syllable, and phonemic levels) and RAN
(phonological retrieval); vocabulary (expressive and receptive vo-
cabulary tasks; morphological and grammatical tasks targeting
only the vocabulary/word level); and oral comprehension (mor-
phological and grammatical tasks involving sentences, syntactic
judgment tasks, sentences/passages listening comprehension/re-
tell). Because we did not want to confound language skills with
writing/reading skills, we did not include language measures that
required substantial writing and reading (e.g., spelling, writing
down sentences/passages to answer oral comprehension ques-
tions). We also did not include measures of mathematics language
(e.g., mathematics vocabulary), because such measures are often
specific to a certain domain of mathematics (e.g., “pentagon” for
geometry; “dividend” for calculations) and are likely to be
confounded with general language and mathematics skills; this
might bias the relation between language and mathematics. For
this study, mathematics measures refer to the tasks that tap one
of the following: numerical knowledge (e.g., counting, subitiz-
ing, number comparison), calculations, solving of word prob-
lems, fractions, algebra, and geometry (see Table 1 for defini-
tions of different language and mathematics skills and examples
of tasks). Second, studies had to report at least one correlation
(r) between any measure of language and any measure of
mathematics.
Coding
Studies were coded according to their characteristics, samples,
and tasks used to measure language and mathematics. Among the
coded variables, we included some as covariates that were con-
trolled in our moderation analyses: publication type (peer-
reviewed vs. other), language type (alphabetic vs. nonalphabetic),
sample status (typically developing vs. atypically developing), and
socioeconomic status (SES; coded as low or middle, based on two
resources from the original studies: direct report of family SES
from the study and relevant SES information such as parental
education level, years of education, income level, and free-reduced
lunch status; studies that did not specify SES were coded as
general/unspecified SES). The moderator variables included types
of language skills (phonological awareness, RAN, vocabulary, oral
comprehension, and comprehensive language skills), second-
language learning status (using one’s second language vs. using
one’s first language to learn/perform mathematics), language mea-
surement type (expressive vs. receptive), types of mathematics
skills (numerical knowledge, calculations, word problems, frac-
tions, geometry, algebra, and comprehensive mathematics skills),
and age. We also coded working memory and intelligence (if
provided by the included studies) and the materials used to mea-
sure those cognitive skills (verbal, nonverbal, mixed verbal and
nonverbal). For measures of intelligence, we included only non-
verbal reasoning tasks or composite reasoning tasks. We excluded
reasoning tasks that were based only on verbal/numerical materials
that are often confounded with vocabulary/mathematics skills. In
addition to these variables, we also coded the number of partici-
pants for each correlation, which was needed to weight each effect
size, so that correlations obtained from larger samples were given
more weight in the analysis than were those from smaller samples.
The important features of individual studies are provided in online
supplemental materials. The variables were discussed until a con-
sensus was reached between the first and second authors. Then,
using the same coding system, the second author and a trained
research assistant (with a master’s degree in education) each
independently coded all included studies. The interrater reliability
between the two coders was .87.99 for all variables of interest.
Articles are excluding duplicates
(n=22,145)
Search
Initial
Screening
Eligibility
Included
Records identified through database search and previous
reviews
(n=22,828)
Articles based on titles and abstracts
(n=2,115)
Duplicate articles
removed
(n=683)
Titles and abstracts were
not related to the research
topic
(n=19,990)
Articles included in the current meta-analysis
(n=344)
Full-text articles excluded
(n=1,811)
1472: No objective language
measures
312: No objective
mathematics measures
27: No correlation data
Figure 1. Flow diagram for the inclusion of studies for the current review.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
7
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
Any disagreements were resolved by consulting the original article
or by discussion.
Missing Data
Not all studies provided sufficient information on the vari-
ables of interest for the present study. In case of insufficient
information, authors were contacted to obtain missing informa-
tion. However, if missing data could not be retrieved, especially
when data were missing for moderator variables, the studies
were excluded from the moderator analyses for which data were
missing but were included in all moderator analyses for which
data were provided.
Analytic Strategies
The effect size index used for all outcome measures is Pearson’s
r, for the correlation between language and mathematics. We
considered all eligible effect sizes in each study. That is, studies
could contribute multiple effect sizes as long as the sample for
each effect size was independent. For studies that reported multiple
effect sizes from the same sample, we accounted for the statistical
dependencies by using the random effects robust standard error
estimation technique developed by Hedges, Tipton, and Johnson
(2010). This analysis allowed for the clustered data (i.e., effect
sizes nested within samples) by correcting studies’ standard errors
to take into account correlations between effect sizes from the
same sample. The robust standard error technique requires that an
estimate of the mean correlation () between all pairs of effect
sizes within a cluster be estimated for calculating the between-
study sampling variance estimate,
2
. In all analyses, we estimated
2
with ␳⫽.80; sensitivity analyses showed that the findings were
robust across different reasonable estimates of .
Analyses were based on Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, and Roth-
stein’s (2005) recommendations: We converted correlation coef-
Table 1
Description of Codes and Examples of Response Categories for Types Language and Mathematics Tasks
Types of skills Definition Examples of response categories
Types of language skills
Phonological processing Phonological Awareness: Tasks that tap the ability
to identify, manipulate, and temporally
memorize units of oral language parts (words,
syllables, onsets and rimes, and phonemes).
Identify the rhyme of words; identify initial sounds or
final sounds in words; identify medial sounds in
words; segment words into their component
syllable/sound; delete/add sounds from/to words;
Sound blending; Pseudoword repetition
Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN): Phonological
codes retrieval efficiency
Name letters/digits/colors/objects rapidly
Vocabulary Tasks that require individuals to point to a picture
corresponding to a word or explain what a word
means
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Wechsler
Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence-Vocabulary,
Nelson Reading Skills Test-Vocabulary, Word
production fluency (e.g., say words that start with
letter B); Extended Range Vocabulary Test
Oral comprehension Tasks that require individuals to comprehend a
passage in an oral format (listening
comprehension)
Passage listening comprehension; Oral syntax
judgement; WAIS-Information/Comprehension;
Sentence repetition; Retell
Types of mathematics skills
Numerical knowledge Questions that tap numerosity (i.e., cardinality) as
well as the relation between numbers (i.e.,
ordinality), counting words, and Arabic digits
(i.e., symbolic knowledge)
Counting; seriation; classification of numbers; number
comparison; compare pairs of piles of objects;
quantity estimation; number line; number
identification/naming; Early Numeracy Test; place
value; transcoding from Arabic to verbal numerals
Calculations Single-digit or multidigit addition, subtraction,
multiplication, and division
Addition (e.g., 2 1;2060 ); subtraction
(e.g., 6 4;2015 ); division (e.g., 6/2 ;
20/10 ); multiplication (e.g., 2 4;20
12 ); WJ-Math Fluency; CBM-Calculation;
WRAT-4-Math; WIAT-Arithmetic
Word problems Questions that involves the ability to understand
the problem narrative, focus on relevant and
ignore irrelevant information, construct a
number sentence, and solve for the missing
number to find the answer
WISC-Word Problem; Arithmetic Word Problems
(e.g., John had nine pennies. He spent three
pennies at the store. How many pennies did he
have left?); key-math problem solving
Fractions Questions that tap the understanding of the part-
whole relation, measurement interpretation of
fractions, and math problems that involve
fractional quantities
Fractions calculations (e.g., ¼ ½); fractions
comparisons (e.g., ¼ ___ ½); NAEP-Fraction;
symbol-picture correspondence; calculations and
word problem-solving involving fractions;
fractional estimate
Geometry Questions that involves the properties and
relations of points, lines, surfaces, solids, and
higher dimensional analogs.
The MT advanced geometry task; the intuitive
geometry task.
Algebra Problems that can be solved by prelearned symbol
manipulation algorithms that are taught in many
algebra curricula
Algebra problem solving (e.g., if x 23, then
x5); algebra judgement (e.g., 3y 220;
y2)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
8PENG ET AL.
ficients to Fisher’s Zscale, and all analyses were performed using
transformed values. The results, such as the summary effect and its
confidence interval, were then converted back to correlation coef-
ficients for presentation. Also, because we hypothesized that this
body of research reports a distribution of correlation coefficients
with significant between-studies variance, as opposed to a group of
studies that attempts to estimate one true correlation, a random-
effects model was appropriate for the present study (Lipsey &
Wilson, 2001). Weighted random-effects metaregression models
using Hedges et al.’s (2010) corrections were run with
ROBUMETA in Stata (Hedberg, 2014) to summarize correlation
coefficients and to examine potential moderators.
We first estimated only the overall weighted mean correlation
between language and mathematics. Then, subgroup analyses were
used to examine the relation between language and mathematics
for each subgroup of each moderator. Metaregression analyses
were used to examine whether types of language skills, types of
mathematics skills, second-language status, language measure-
ment type, and age moderated the relation between language and
mathematics. For the moderation analysis, all moderators were
entered into the model simultaneously, with publication type, SES,
language type, and sample status as covariates in the model as
well. For categorical moderators, we created dummy coded vari-
ables to examine comparisons among categories (Cohen, Cohen,
West, & Aiken, 2013). Because there were multiple subgroups for
some moderators (e.g., types of language/mathematics), we ap-
plied the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure to those categorical mod-
erators in the metaregression models to reduce Type I errors.
To examine the influence of working memory and intelligence
on the relation between language and mathematics, we calculated
correlations between mathematics and language, partialing out
working memory, intelligence, and both. The calculation of the
partial correlation was based on the correlation matrices retrieved
from the original studies (e.g., if a study provided two measures of
mathematics, two measures of language, and two measures of
working memory, we would calculate all possible eight correla-
tions between [the two] mathematics measures and [the two]
language measures, partialing out [the two] working memory
measures). To calculate the correlation between language and
mathematics partialing out both working memory and intelligence,
we used the cor2pcor function from the corpcor package in R. We
then synthesized these partial correlations to indicate whether the
relation between language and mathematics was influenced by
working memory, intelligence, or both. We also calculated the
proportion of variance in the relation between language and math-
ematics that was explained by working memory, intelligence, or
both, based on the formula: 1 (r
partial
/r) (Huang, Sivaganesan,
Succop, & Goodman, 2004; Preacher & Kelley, 2011), where
r
partial
represents the partial correlation between language and
mathematics, partialing out working memory, intelligence, or both,
and rrepresents the correlation between language and mathemat-
ics.
To examine the relation between language and mathematics
longitudinally, we calculated (a) the correlations between mathe-
matics measured at Time 1 and language measured at Time 2 (a
later time point), partialing out their relations with language mea-
sured at Times 1; and (b) the correlations between language
measured at Time 1 and mathematics measured at Time 2, partial-
ing out their relations with mathematics measured at Times 1. The
partial correlation was based on the correlation matrices retrieved
from the original studies. We then synthesized these partial cor-
relations to indicate whether mathematics measured earlier pre-
dicted language later or vice versa. For all partial correlation
synthesis, we accounted for the statistical dependencies of multiple
partial correlations from one study using the random effects robust
standard error estimation technique developed by Hedges et al.
(2010) as mentioned earlier.
Publication bias (the problem of selective publication, in which
the decision to publish a study is influenced by its results) was
examined using the method of Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider, and
Minder (1997) and funnel plots (we aggregated all effect sizes for
each independent sample, as there are currently no funnel plots that
handle dependency with ROBUMETA). We did not find signifi-
cant publication bias based on Egger et al.’s (1997) statistics (i.e.,
the standard errors of correlations did not significantly predict
correlations among studies with ROBUMETA in Stata) for the
correlations between language and calculations/fractions/algebra,
or correlations between language and mathematics partialing out
intelligence/working memory, or the longitudinal correlations for
earlier language predicting later mathematics, ps.05. However,
we found that Egger et al.’s (1997) test was significant for the
correlation between language and overall mathematics/numerical
knowledge/word problems/geometry, the relations between lan-
guage and mathematics partialing out working memory, and the
longitudinal correlations for earlier mathematics predicting later
language. Further examination of the funnel plots of the correla-
tions with Egger et al.’s (1997) test did not reveal any obvious
asymmetrical patterns, except that the funnel plot for the correla-
tion between language and geometry indicated that there were not
many studies with big sample sizes (probably due to the fact that
there were only 10 effect sizes available). That said, we considered
both published and unpublished literature for the present meta-
analysis, we included a large number of studies and effect sizes,
and we were thus not very concerned about publication bias,
particularly because there is some evidence that correlational
meta-analyses are less susceptible to bias due to the publication
process (Chow & Ekholm, 2018). In addition, we controlled for
publication type (peer-reviewed vs. other types of publication) in
all metaregression analyses.
Results
Based on our inclusion criteria, 344 studies (including 54 non-
peer reviewed articles) involving 393 independent samples,
368,268 participants, and 2,831 correlations between language and
mathematics were included for the final analyses.
Question 1: Is There a Significant Relation Between
Language and Mathematics?
The overall magnitude of the relation between language and
mathematics was r.42, 95% CI [.40, .44]. As Table 2 shows, the
average correlation between different language skills and different
mathematics types was moderate and significant, rs.29.51,
ps.05.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
9
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
Question 2: What Variables Moderate the Relation
Between Language and Mathematics?
Types of language skills. As Table 3 shows, after controlling
for covariates (alphabetic language vs. nonalphabetic language,
publication type, SES, sample status) and other moderators (types
of mathematics skills, language measurement type, second-
language status, and age), comprehensive language skills were
more strongly related to mathematics than were phonological
awareness and RAN; vocabulary and oral comprehension were
more strongly related to mathematics than was RAN; no signifi-
cant differences were found in other comparisons.
For the relation between language and numerical knowledge
after controlling for covariates and other moderators, only types
of language skills emerged as a significant moderator, such that
comprehensive language skills showed a stronger relation to
numerical knowledge than did phonological awareness, RAN,
and vocabulary (see Table 4); for the relation between language
and calculations after controlling for covariates and other mod-
erators, comprehensive language skills showed a stronger rela-
tion to calculations than did phonological awareness (see Table
5); for the relation between language and word problems after
controlling for covariates and other moderators, vocabulary,
oral comprehension, and comprehensive language skills all
showed stronger relations to word problems than did RAN (see
Table 6).
Mathematics types. As Table 3 shows, after controlling for
covariates and other moderators, word problems showed stronger
relations with language than did numerical knowledge, calcula-
tions, fractions, and algebra; comprehensive mathematics skills
were more strongly related to language than were numerical
knowledge, calculations, and algebra; no significant differences
were found in other comparisons.
Age. As Table 3 shows, after controlling for covariates and
other moderators, age did not significantly moderate the relation
between language and mathematics. However, for the relation
between language and calculations, after controlling for covariates
and other moderators, the relation between language and calcula-
tions decreased with age.
We also a series of moderation analysis to examine whether age
moderated the relations between phonological awareness/RAN/
oral comprehension and numerical knowledge/calculations/word
problems, with/without controlling for working memory and in-
telligence. The results showed that after controlling for covariates
and other moderators, without controlling for working memory
and intelligence, only age significantly moderated the relations
between phonological awareness and numerical knowledge/calcu-
lations such that these relations decreased with age. After control-
ling for working memory and intelligence, age did not emerge as
a significant moderator. Due to insufficient data points (less than
10), we did not examine whether age moderated the relations
between phonological awareness/RAN/oral comprehension and
other mathematics skills.
Language measurement type. As Table 2 shows, the aver-
age correlation between mathematics and receptive language
was r.44, 95% CI [.41, .47]; the average correlation between
mathematics and expressive language was r.39, 95% CI [.36,
.41], and the average correlation between mathematics and
receptive-expressive language was r.50, 95% CI [.43, .57].
As Table 3 shows, after controlling for covariates and other
moderators, there were no significant differences among recep-
tive language, expressive language, and receptive-expressive
language.
Second-language learning status. As Table 2 shows, the
average correlation between language and mathematics among
second-language learners was r.34, 95% CI [.28, .39]; the
average correlation between language and mathematics among
native language speakers was r.43, 95% CI [.41, .45], and the
average correlation between language and mathematics among
samples mixed with second-language learners and native speakers
was r.40, 95% CI [.35, .43]. As Table 3 shows, after controlling
for covariates and other moderators, the relation between language
and mathematics was stronger among native language speakers
and among samples mixed with second-language learners and
native language speakers than among second-language learners.
There was no difference between native language speakers and
Table 2
Correlations Between Language and Mathematics
Categories of covariates
and moderators
Language and mathematics
kr95% CI of r
2
Main average correlation 2066 .42 [.40, .44] .04
Publication type
1. Peer-reviewed 1650 .41 [.39, .43] .05
2. Other types 416 .46 [.41, .51] .03
Sample status
1. Typically developing 1381 .44 [.42, .46] .03
2. Atypically developing 685 .36 [.33, .40] .05
SES
1. Below middle class 482 .41 [.37, .44] .04
2. Middle class or above 515 .43 [.39, .47] .05
3. General/unspecified 1069 .42 [.39, .45] .03
Language type
1. Alphabetic 1985 .43 [.40, .44] .04
2. Nonalphabetic 81 .40 [.35, .45] .03
Language measurement type
1. Receptive 391 .44 [.41, .47] .02
2. Expressive 1513 .39 [.36, .41] .05
3. Receptive Expressive 110 .50 [.43, .57] .06
Second-language learning status
1. Native language speakers 1713 .43 [.41, .45] .04
2. Second-language learners 96 .34 [.28, .39] .02
3. Mixed sample 257 .40 [.35, .43] .03
Types of language skills
1. Phonological processing 835 .35 [.31, .38] .04
1a. Phonological awareness 476 .35 [.31, .39] .05
1b. Rapid automatized naming 359 .31 [.25, .35] .03
2. Vocabulary 830 .42 [.41, .45] .03
3. Oral comprehension 262 .43 [.39, .47] .04
4. Comprehensive language 139 .51 [.44, .57] .09
Types of mathematics skills
1. Numerical knowledge 790 .35 [.33, .39] .05
2. Calculations 608 .35 [.33, .38] .01
3. Word problems 309 .48 [.45, .51] .01
4. Fractions 13 .29 [.14, .43] .02
5. Geometry 10 .39 [.22, .53] .03
6. Algebra 32 .32 [.15, .36] .02
7. Comprehensive mathematics 304 .49 [.45, .52] .02
Note.knumber of effect sizes; CI confidence interval; SES
socioeconomic status;
2
between-study sampling variance.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
10 PENG ET AL.
Table 3
Moderation Analysis on the Relation Between Language and Mathematics
Measure Beta SE t 95% CI pvalue
Publication type
Peer-Reviewed versus Other Types .06 .03 2.08 [.12, .003] .04
Sample status
Typically Developing versus Atypically Developing .08 .02 3.18 [.03, .13] .002
SES
Low SES versus General/Unspecified .02 .03 .68 [.07, .03] .49
Middle SES versus General/Unspecified .002 .03 .11 [.05, .06] .92
Middle SES versus Low SES .02 .03 .69 [.04, .08] .49
Language type
Alphabetic versus Nonalphabetic .04 .03 1.18 [.02, .09] .24
Language measurement type
Receptive versus Expressive .01 .02 .45 [.04 .06] .65
Receptive versus Receptive Expressive .05 .05 .92 [.14, .05] .36
Expressive versus Receptive Expressive .06 .05 1.19 [.15, .04] .24
Age .003 .002 1.50 [.01, .001] .13
Second-language learning status
Second-Language Learners versus Mixed .15 .05 2.96 [.26, .05] .003
Native Language Speakers versus Second-Language Learners .17 .05 3.29 [.06, .26] .001
Native Language Speaker versus Mixed .01 .03 .31 [.04, .06] .76
Types of language skills
Phonological Awareness versus Rapid Automatized Naming .04 .04 .98 [.04, .11] .33
Phonological Awareness versus Vocabulary .06 .03 1.92 [.11, .001] .06
Phonological Awareness versus Oral Comprehension .06 .03 1.80 [.12, .005] .07
Phonological Awareness versus Comprehensive Language .16 .05 2.91 [.26, .05] .004
Rapid Automatized Naming versus Vocabulary .09 .04 2.55 [.16, .02] .01
Rapid Automatized Naming versus Oral Comprehension .09 .04 2.50 [.17, .02] .01
Rapid Automatized Naming versus Comprehensive Language .19 .06 3.44 [30, .08] .001
Vocabulary versus Oral Comprehension .00 .03 .12 [.05, .05] .91
Vocabulary versus Comprehensive Language .10 .05 2.04 [.20, .003] .04
Oral Comprehension versus Comprehensive Language .10 .05 1.94 [.20, .002] .05
Types of mathematics skills
Calculations versus Numerical Knowledge .02 .03 .74 [.07, .03] .46
Word Problems versus Numerical Knowledge .13 .03 4.76 [.08, .18] <.001
Fractions versus Numerical Knowledge .03 .06 .55 [.16, .09] .58
Geometry versus Numerical Knowledge .002 .09 .02 [.17, .18] .98
Algebra versus Numerical Knowledge .14 .06 2.29 [.26, .02] .02
Comprehensive Mathematics versus Numerical Knowledge .11 .03 3.57 [.05, .17] <.001
Word Problems versus Calculations .15 .03 5.89 [.10, .20] <.001
Fractions versus Calculations .02 .06 .26 [.13, .10] .80
Geometry versus Calculations .02 .09 .24 [.15, .19] .81
Algebra versus Calculations .12 .06 2.08 [.24, .007] .04
Comprehensive Mathematics versus Calculations .13 .03 4.49 [.07, .19] <.001
Fractions versus Word Problems .16 .06 2.65 [.28, .04] .01
Geometry versus Word Problems .13 .09 1.44 [.30, .05] .15
Algebra versus Word Problems .27 .06 4.44 [.39, .15] <.001
Comprehensive Mathematics versus Word Problems .02 .03 .60 [.08, .04] .55
Geometry versus Fractions .04 .10 .35 [.17, .24] .73
Algebra versus Fractions .10 .08 1.26 [.26, .06] .21
Comprehensive Mathematics versus Fractions .15 .06 2.26 [.02, .27] .03
Algebra versus Geometry .14 .08 1.75 [.31, .02] .08
Comprehensive Mathematics versus Geometry .11 .09 1.22 [.07, .28] .22
Comprehensive Mathematics versus Algebra .25 .06 4.29 [.14, .37] <.001
Note. All covariates and moderators were entered in one model. Several models were run for thorough subgroup comparisons among moderators with
more than two categories. For the convenience of presentation, subgroup comparisons within categorical moderators are all listed in the model. CI
confidence interval; SES socioeconomic status. The second group in each group comparison variable is the reference group (e.g., in alphabetic versus
nonalphabetic, nonalphabetic is the reference group in the dummy coding of language type). Bolded number means significant after applying Benjamini-
Hochberg procedure. There are 2,006 correlations and 331 independent samples. Between-study sampling variance (
2
) is .02.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
11
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
mixed samples with second-language learners and native language
speakers.
Question 3: What Is the Role of Working Memory
and Intelligence in the Relation Between Language
and Mathematics?
Controlling for working memory. There were 101 studies,
with 111 independent samples and 2,111 correlations between
language and mathematics partialing out working memory. The
results showed that based on these studies, the correlation between
language and mathematics was r.37, 95% CI [.35, .41], the
correlation between language and working memory was r.29,
95% CI [.26, .32], and the correlation between working memory
and mathematics was r.32, 95% CI [.30, .35]. After partialing
out working memory, the correlation between language and math-
ematics was r.31, 95% CI [.28, .34]. After partialing out
working memory and controlling for types of working memory
tasks (controlling for the medium effects), the correlation between
language and mathematics was r.34, 95% CI [.29, .37]. Taken
together, working memory explained about 8%16% of the vari-
ance in the relation between language and mathematics.
Numerical knowledge. There were 47 studies, with 49 inde-
pendent samples and 837 correlations between language and nu-
merical knowledge partialing out working memory. The results
showed that based on these studies, the correlation between lan-
guage and numerical knowledge was r.32, 95% CI [.28, .35],
the correlation between language and working memory was r
.27, 95% CI [.24, .30], and the correlation between working
memory and numerical knowledge was r.28, 95% CI [.25, .31].
After partialing out working memory, the correlation between
language and numerical knowledge was r.26, 95% CI [.24, .30].
After partialing out working memory and controlling for types of
working memory tasks (controlling for the medium effects), the
correlation between language and numerical knowledge was r
.28, 95% CI [.25, .33]. Taken together, working memory explained
about 13%19% of the variance in the relation between language
and numerical knowledge.
Calculations. There were 64 studies, with 71 independent
samples and 706 correlations between language and calculations
partialing out working memory. The results showed that based on
these studies, the correlation between language and calculations
was r.33, 95% CI [.29, .36], the correlation between language
and working memory was r.29, 95% CI [.25, .32], and the
correlation between working memory and calculations was r
.30, 95% CI [.27, .33]. After partialing out working memory, the
correlation between language and calculations was r.26, 95%
Table 4
Moderation Analysis on the Relation Between Language and Numerical Knowledge
Measure Beta SE t 95% CI pvalue
Publication type
Peer-reviewed versus Other Types .06 .06 .97 [.18, .06] .34
Sample status
Typically Developing versus Atypically Developing .10 .04 2.53 [.02, .18] .01
SES
Low SES versus General/Unspecified .03 .04 .73 [.05, .11] .47
Middle SES versus General/Unspecified .04 .04 1.05 [.13, .04] .30
Middle SES versus Low SES .07 .05 1.60 [.16, .02] .11
Language type
Alphabetic versus Nonalphabetic .07 .04 1.47 [.02, .15] .15
Language measurement type
Receptive versus Expressive .06 .04 1.59 [.02, .14] .12
Receptive versus Receptive Expressive .19 .13 1.43 [.07, .45] .16
Expressive versus Receptive Expressive .13 .13 .95 [.14, .39] .35
Age 0.00 .01 .03 [.02, .02] .98
Second-language learning status
Second-Language Learners versus Mixed .14 .11 1.22 [.37, .09] .22
Native Language Speakers versus Second-Language Learners .17 .11 1.49 [.06, .39] .14
Native Language Speaker versus Mixed .03 .04 .67 [.05, .11] .50
Types of language skills
Rapid Automatized Naming versus Phonological Awareness .05 .04 1.33 [.13, .03] .19
Vocabulary versus Phonological Awareness .003 .04 .09 [.07, .08] .93
Oral Comprehension versus Phonological Awareness .05 .05 .96 [.05, .16] .34
Comprehensive Language versus Phonological Awareness .23 .11 2.02 [.01, .46] .046
Vocabulary versus Rapid Automatized Naming .06 .04 1.41 [.02, .14] .16
Oral Comprehension versus Rapid Automatized Naming .11 .06 1.85 [.01, .22] .07
Comprehensive Language versus Rapid Automatized Naming .29 .11 2.55 [.06, .51] .01
Oral Comprehension versus Vocabulary .05 .05 .95 [.05, .15] .34
Comprehensive Language versus Vocabulary .23 .11 2.10 [.01, .45] .04
Comprehensive Language versus Oral Comprehension .18 .12 1.56 [.05, .41] .12
Note. All covariates and moderators were entered in one model. Several models were run for thorough subgroup comparisons among moderators with
more than two categories. For the convenience of presentation, subgroup comparisons within categorical moderators are all listed in the model. CI
confidence interval. The second group in each group comparison variable is the reference group (e.g., in alphabetic versus nonalphabetic, nonalphabetic
is the reference group in the dummy coding of language type). Bolded number means significant after applying Benjamini-Hochberg procedure. There are
781 correlations and 113 independent samples. Between-study sampling variance (
2
) is .03.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
12 PENG ET AL.
CI [.23, .29]. After partialing out working memory and controlling
for materials of working memory (controlling for the medium
effects), the correlation between language and calculations was
r.31, 95% CI [.26, .35]. Taken together, working memory
explained about 6%21% of the variance in the relation between
language and calculations.
Word problems. There were 44 studies, with 46 independent
samples and 225 correlations between language and word prob-
lems partialing out working memory. Results showed that based on
these studies, the correlation between language and word problems
was r.45, 95% CI [.42, .49], the correlation between language
and working memory was r.33, 95% CI [.28, .36], and the
correlation between working memory and word problems was r
.38, 95% CI [.35, .41]. After partialing out working memory, the
correlation between language and word problems was r.38,
95% CI [.35, .41]. After partialing out working memory and
controlling for types of working memory tasks (controlling for the
medium effects), the correlation between language and word prob-
lems was r.43, 95% CI [.37, .49]. Taken together, working
memory explained about 4%16% of the variance in the relation
between language and word problems.
Fractions. There were eight studies, with eight independent
samples and 40 correlations between language and fractions par-
tialing out working memory. The results showed that based on
these studies, the correlation between language and fractions was
r.31, 95% CI [.23, .38], the correlation between language and
working memory was r.23, 95% CI [.15, .30], and the corre-
lation between working memory and fractions was r.24, 95%
CI [.17, .31]. After partialing out working memory, the correlation
between language and fractions was r.26, 95% CI [.20, .35].
After partialing out working memory and controlling for types of
working memory tasks (controlling for the medium effects), the
correlation between language and fractions was r.31, 95% CI
[.19, .42]. Taken together, working memory explained about
0%16% of the variance in the relation between language and
fractions.
Geometry. There were four studies, with five independent
samples and 67 correlations between language and geometry par-
tialing out working memory. The results showed that based on
these studies, the correlation between language and geometry was
r.37, 95% CI [.13, .57], the correlation between language and
working memory was r.28, 95% CI [.13, .42], and the corre-
lation between working memory and geometry was r.35, 95%
CI [.18, .51]. After partialing out working memory, the correlation
between language and geometry was r.29, 95% CI [.03, .51].
Table 5
Moderation Analysis on the Relation Between Language and Calculations
Measure Beta SE t 95% CI pvalue
Publication type
Peer-reviewed versus Other Types .06 .03 1.69 [.12, .01] .09
Sample status
Typically Developing versus Atypically Developing .05 .03 1.86 [.003, .11] .07
SES
Low SES versus General/Unspecified .06 .03 1.97 [.12, 0] .05
Middle SES versus General/Unspecified .03 .05 .53 [.08, .13] .59
Middle SES versus low SES .09 .05 1.64 [.02, .19] .10
Language type
Alphabetic versus Nonalphabetic .06 .06 1.15 [.17, .05] .25
Language Measurement Type
Receptive versus Expressive .03 .04 .88 [.04, .10] .38
Receptive versus Receptive Expressive .02 .07 .30 [.16, .12] .77
Expressive versus Receptive Expressive .05 .06 .83 [.18, .07] .41
Age .004 .002 2.59 [.01, .001] .01
Second-Language Learning Status
Second-Language Learners versus Mixed .01 .04 .24 [.08, .10] .81
Native Language Speakers versus Second-Language Learners .01 .04 .19 [.07, .08] .85
Native Language Speaker versus Mixed .02 .03 .53 [.05, .09] .60
Types of language skills
Rapid Automatized Naming versus Phonological Awareness .08 .04 1.82 [.01, .17] .07
Vocabulary versus Phonological Awareness .08 .04 1.78 [.01, .16] .08
Oral Comprehension versus Phonological Awareness .07 .04 1.73 [.01, .16] .09
Comprehensive Language versus Phonological Awareness .15 .06 2.30 [.02, .28] .02
Vocabulary versus Rapid Automatized Naming .003 .04 .06 [.09, .09] .96
Oral Comprehension versus Rapid Automatized Naming .01 .04 .20 [.09, .07] .84
Comprehensive Language versus Rapid Automatized Naming .07 .06 1.07 [.06, .19] .29
Oral Comprehension versus Vocabulary .01 .03 .20 [.06, .05] .84
Comprehensive Language versus Vocabulary .07 .06 1.24 [.04, .18] .22
Comprehensive Language versus Oral Comprehension .08 .06 1.37 [.03, .19] .17
Note. All covariates and moderators were entered in one model. Several models were run for thorough subgroup comparisons among moderators with
more than two categories. For the convenience of presentation, subgroup comparisons within categorical moderators are all listed in the model. CI
confidence interval. The second group in each group comparison variable is the reference group (e.g., in alphabetic versus nonalphabetic, nonalphabetic
is the reference group in the dummy coding of language type). Bolded number means significant after applying Benjamini-Hochberg procedure. There are
596 correlations and 160 independent samples. Between-study sampling variance (
2
) is .01.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
13
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
After partialing out working memory and controlling for types of
working memory tasks (controlling for the medium effects), the
correlation between language and geometry was r.31, 95% CI
[.14, .65]. Taken together, working memory explained all the
variance in the relation between language and geometry.
Algebra. There were three studies, with independent samples
and 53 correlations between language and algebra partialing out
working memory. Results showed that based on these studies, the
correlation between language and algebra was r.40, 95% CI
[.29, .49], the correlation between language and working memory
was r.28, 95% CI [.09, .45], and the correlation between
working memory and algebra was r.34, 95% CI [.21, .45]. After
partialing out working memory, the correlation between language
and algebra was r.34, 95% CI [.16, .49]. After partialing out
working memory and controlling for types of working memory
tasks (controlling for the medium effects), the correlation between
language and algebra was r.39, 95% CI [.28, .47]. Taken
together, working memory explained 3%15% of the variance in
the relation between language and algebra.
Controlling for intelligence. There were 128 studies, with
141 independent samples and 1,550 correlations between language
and mathematics partialing out intelligence. Results showed that
based on these studies, the correlation between language and
mathematics was r.39, 95% CI [.36, .42], the correlation
between language and intelligence was r.35, 95% CI [.32, .37],
and the correlation between intelligence and mathematics was r
.36, 95% CI [.34, .39]. After partialing out intelligence, the cor-
relation between language and mathematics was r.30, 95% CI
[.27, .32]. After partialing out intelligence and controlling for types
of intelligence tasks (controlling for the medium effects), the
correlation between language and mathematics was r.31, 95%
CI [.28, .33]. Taken together, intelligence explained about
21%23% of the variance in the relation between language and
mathematics.
Numerical knowledge. There were 46 studies, with 49 inde-
pendent samples and 432 correlations between language and nu-
merical knowledge partialing out intelligence. The results showed
that based on these studies, the correlation between language and
numerical knowledge was r.35, 95% CI [.30, .38], the corre-
lation between language and intelligence was r.35, 95% CI
[.29, 40], and the correlation between intelligence and numerical
knowledge was r.32, 95% CI [.28, .36]. After partialing out
intelligence, the correlation between language and numerical
knowledge was r.25, 95% CI [.23, .29]. After partialing out
intelligence controlling for types of intelligence tasks (controlling
for the medium effects), the correlation between language and
Table 6
Moderation Analysis on the Relation Between Language and Word Problems
Measure Beta SE t 95% CI pvalue
Publication type
Peer-reviewed versus Other Types .05 .05 1.03 [.15, .05] .30
Sample status
Typically Developing versus Atypically Developing .04 .04 .96 [.04, .12] .34
SES
Low SES versus General/Unspecified .01 .04 .26 [.07, .09] .79
Middle SES versus General/Unspecified .04 .05 .75 [.06, .13] .45
Middle SES versus Low SES .02 .05 .48 [.08, .12] .63
Language type
Alphabetic versus Nonalphabetic .16 .08 2.12 [.01, .31] .04
Language measurement type
Receptive versus Expressive .05 .05 1.21 [.03, .14] .23
Receptive versus Receptive Expressive .01 .07 .20 [.13, .15] .84
Expressive versus Receptive Expressive .04 .07 .57 [.18, .10] .57
Age .002 .002 .63 [.01, .003] .53
Second-language learning status
Second-Language Learners versus Mixed .19 .11 1.79 [.41, .02] .08
Native Language Speakers versus Second-Language Learners .24 .10 2.31 [.03, .45] .02
Native Language Speaker versus Mixed .05 .04 1.17 [.03, .13] .25
Types of language skills
Rapid Automatized Naming versus Phonological Awareness .21 .06 3.67 [.32, .10] <.001
Vocabulary versus Phonological Awareness .07 .06 1.18 [.05, .18] .24
Oral Comprehension versus Phonological Awareness .06 .06 .98 [.06, .17] .33
Comprehensive Language versus Phonological Awareness .08 .08 .96 [.08, .24] .34
Vocabulary versus Rapid Automatized Naming .27 .05 5.89 [.18, .37] <.001
Oral Comprehension versus Rapid Automatized Naming .27 .05 5.42 [.17, .36] <.001
Comprehensive Language versus Rapid Automatized Naming .29 .07 4.20 [.15, .42] <.001
Oral Comprehension versus Vocabulary .01 .04 .24 [.08, .06] .81
Comprehensive Language versus Vocabulary .01 .07 .17 [.13, .15] .87
Comprehensive Language versus Oral Comprehension .02 .07 .30 [.11, .15] .77
Note. All covariates and moderators were entered in one model. Several models were run for thorough subgroup comparisons among moderators with
more than two categories. For the convenience of presentation, subgroup comparisons within categorical moderators are all listed in the model. CI
confidence interval. The second group in each group comparison variable is the reference group (e.g., in alphabetic versus nonalphabetic, nonalphabetic
is the reference group in the dummy coding of language type). Bolded number means significant after applying Benjamini-Hochberg procedure. There are
302 correlations and 112 independent samples. Between-study sampling variance (
2
) is .01.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
14 PENG ET AL.
numerical knowledge was r.26, 95% CI [.23, .30]. Taken
together, intelligence explained about 26%29% of the variance
in the relation between language and numerical knowledge.
Calculations. There were 82 studies, with 94 independent
samples and 615 correlations between language and calculations
partialing out intelligence. Results showed that based on these
studies, the correlation between language and calculations was r
.35, 95% CI [.31, .39], the correlation between language and
intelligence was r.34, 95% CI [.31, .37], and the correlation
between intelligence and calculations was r.35, 95% CI [.31,
.38]. After partialing out intelligence, the correlation between
language and calculations was r.26, 95% CI [.24, .29]. After
partialing out intelligence and controlling for types of intelligence
tasks (controlling for the medium effects), the correlation between
language and calculations was r.27, 95% CI [.24, .30]. Taken
together, intelligence explained about 23%26% of the variance
in the relation between language and calculations.
Word problems. There were 49 studies, with 53 independent
samples and 305 correlations between language and word prob-
lems partialing out intelligence. The results showed that based on
these studies, the correlation between language and word problems
was r.45, 95% CI [.41, .49], the correlation between language
and intelligence was r.36, 95% CI [.31, .41], and the correlation
between intelligence and word problems was r.41, 95% CI [.37,
.45]. After partialing out intelligence, the correlation between
language and word problems was r.35, 95% CI [.32, .40]. After
partialing out intelligence controlling for types of intelligence
tasks (controlling for the medium effects), the correlation between
language and word problems was r.36, 95% CI [.33, .40].
Taken together, intelligence explained about 20%22% of the
variance in the relation between language and word problems.
Fractions. There were nine studies, with nine independent
samples and 33 correlations between language and fractions par-
tialing out intelligence. The results showed that based on these
studies, the correlation between language and fractions was r
.36, 95% CI [.28, .43], the correlation between language and
intelligence was r.38, 95% CI [.28, .47], and the correlation
between intelligence and fractions was r.39, 95% CI [.30, .47].
After partialing out intelligence, the correlation between language
and fractions was r.25, 95% CI [.18, .30]. After partialing out
intelligence and controlling for types of intelligence tasks (con-
trolling for the medium effects), the correlation between language
and fractions was r.25, 95% CI [.18, .31]. Taken together,
intelligence explained about 29% of the variance in the relation
between language and fractions.
Geometry. There were six studies, with seven independent
samples and 16 correlations between language and geometry par-
tialing out intelligence. Results showed that based on these studies,
the correlation between language and geometry was r.34, 95%
CI [.15, .49], the correlation between language and intelligence
was r.35, 95% CI [.18, .51], and the correlation between
intelligence and geometry was r.31, 95% CI [.23, .39]. After
partialing out intelligence, the correlation between language and
geometry was r.25, 95% CI [.10, .38]. Because we acquired
only nonverbal intelligence for the relation between language and
geometry, we did not run the partial correlation controlling for
types of intelligence tasks. Taken together, intelligence explained
about 26% of the variance in the relation between language and
geometry.
Algebra. There were six studies, with six independent sam-
ples and 13 correlations between language and algebra partialing
out intelligence. The results showed that based on these studies,
the correlation between language and algebra was r.24, 95% CI
[.07, .39], the correlation between language and intelligence was
r.32, 95% CI [.20, .43], and the correlation between intelligence
and algebra was r.34, 95% CI [.25, .41]. After partialing out
intelligence, the correlation between language and algebra was r
.15, 95% CI [.03, .32]. Because we acquired only nonverbal
intelligence for the relation between language and algebra, we did
not run the partial correlation controlling for types of intelligence
tasks. Taken together, intelligence explained 38% of the relation
between language and algebra based on the data we acquired.
Controlling for intelligence and working memory. There
were 59 studies, with 66 independent samples and 1,469 correla-
tions between language and mathematics partialing out intelli-
gence and working memory. Results based on this pool of studies
showed that the correlation between language and mathematics
was r.37, 95% CI [.34, .41]. After partialing out intelligence
and working memory, the correlation between language and math-
ematics was r.17, 95% CI [.15, .20]. After partialing out
intelligence and working memory and controlling for types of
intelligence and working memory tasks (controlling for the me-
dium effects), the correlation between language and mathematics
was r.22, 95% CI [.17, .26]. Taken together, intelligence and
working memory explained about 41%54% of the variance in
the relation between language and mathematics.
Numerical knowledge. There were 22 studies, with 23 inde-
pendent samples and 517 correlations between language and nu-
merical knowledge partialing out intelligence and working mem-
ory. Results based on this pool of studies showed that the
correlation between language and numerical knowledge was r
.30, 95% CI [.24, .35]. After partialing out intelligence and work-
ing memory, the correlation between language and numerical
knowledge was r.22, 95% CI [.19, .25]. After partialing out
intelligence and working memory and controlling for types of
intelligence and working memory tasks (controlling for the me-
dium effects), the correlation between language and numerical
knowledge was r.27, 95% CI [.22, .33]. Taken together,
intelligence and working memory explained about 10%30% of
the variance in the relation between language and numerical
knowledge.
Calculations. There were 41 studies, with 47 independent
samples and 571 correlations between language and calculations
partialing out intelligence and working memory. Results based on
this pool of studies showed that the correlation between language
and calculations was r.33, 95% CI [.27, .37]. After partialing
out intelligence and working memory, the correlation between
language and calculations was r.19, 95% CI [.16, .22]. After
partialing out intelligence and working memory and controlling for
types of intelligence and working memory tasks (controlling for
the medium effects), the correlation between language and calcu-
lations was r.22, 95% CI [.16, .27]. Taken together, intelligence
and working memory explained about 33%42% of the variance
in the relation between language and calculations.
Word problems. There were 27 studies, with 29 independent
samples and 231 correlations between language and word prob-
lems partialing out intelligence and working memory. Results
based on this pool of studies showed that the correlation between
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
15
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
language and word problems was r.46, 95% CI [.41, .51]. After
partialing out intelligence and working memory, the correlation
between language and word problems was r.15, 95% CI [.12,
.18]. After partialing out intelligence and working memory and
controlling for types of intelligence and working memory tasks
(controlling for the medium effects), the correlation between lan-
guage and word problems was r.24, 95% CI [.18, .29]. Taken
together, intelligence and working memory explained about
48%67% of the variance in the relation between language and
word problems.
Fractions. There were five studies, with five independent
samples and 21 correlations between language and fractions
partialing out intelligence and working memory. Results based
on the pool of studies showed that the correlation between
language and fractions was r.32, 95% CI [.21, .43]. After
partialing out intelligence and working memory, the correlation
between language and fractions was r.18, 95% CI [.12, .23].
Because we did not have sufficient effect sizes for nonverbal
intelligence, we did not run the partial correlation analysis
controlling for types of intelligence and working memory tasks.
Taken together, intelligence and working memory explained
about 50% of the variance in the relation between language and
fractions.
Geometry. There were three studies, with three indepen-
dent samples and 19 correlations between language and geom-
etry partialing out intelligence and working memory. Results
based on this pool of studies showed that the correlation be-
tween language and geometry was r.32, 95% CI [.31, .75].
Because the relation between language and geometry based on
this pool of studies was not significant, we did not run the
partial correlation analysis controlling for intelligence and
working memory.
Algebra. There were two studies, with two independent
samples and five correlations between language and algebra
partialing out intelligence and working memory. Results based
on this pool of studies showed that the correlation between
language and algebra was r.40, 95% CI [.39, .84]. Because
the relation between language and algebra based on this pool of
studies was not significant, we did not run the partial correla-
tion analysis controlling for intelligence and working memory.
Moderation analyses based on partial correlations. Next,
we ran moderation analyses for partial relations between lan-
guage and mathematics controlling for intelligence and working
memory (see Table 7). After controlling for covariates, we did
not find any significant moderators. With respect to the relation
between language and numerical knowledge controlling for
intelligence and working memory (see Table 8), after control-
ling for covariates, only types of language skills emerged as a
significant moderator such that RAN showed a stronger relation
to numerical knowledge than did comprehensive language
skills, p.01. With respect to the relation between language
and calculations controlling for intelligence and working mem-
ory (see Table 9), after controlling for covariates, we did not
find any significant moderators. With respect to the relation
between language and word problems controlling for intelli-
gence and working memory (see Table 10), after controlling for
covariates, we did not find any significant moderators. Because
we did not have sufficient studies (n5), we did not run
moderation analyses for the partial correlations between lan-
guage and fractions/geometry/algebra.
Question 4: What Is the Longitudinal Relation
Between Language and Mathematics?
Last, we examined whether language and mathematics were
significantly correlated from a longitudinal perspective. We first
investigated whether language predicted later mathematics perfor-
mance partialing out initial mathematics performance. There were
61 studies, with 63 independent samples and 884 correlations, with
the majority of studies focusing on children before age 14 (around
314 years old) and the majority of prediction time intervals
spanning within 2 years. Results showed that language signifi-
cantly predicted later mathematics performance partialing out ini-
tial mathematics performance, r.20, 95% CI [.17, .23]. We then
ran moderation analyses to investigate the predictive power of
different types of language skills for later mathematics perfor-
mance and whether time interval (the length of time between
Times 1 and 2) affected the relation. Results showed that after
controlling for covariates, types of mathematics skills at Times 1
and 2, types of language measures, second-language status, age,
and time interval (the length of time between Times 1 and 2), we
did not find types of language skills as a significant moderator,
ps.05.
Next, we investigated whether mathematics performance at
Time 1 predicted later language partialing out initial language
performance. There were 26 studies, with 27 independent samples
and 178 correlations, with the majority of studies focusing on
children before age 14 (around 314 years old) and the majority
of prediction time intervals spanning within 2 years. Results
showed that mathematics performance at Time 1 significantly
predicted later language performance partialing out initial lan-
guage performance, r.22, 95% CI [.17, .27]. We then ran
moderation analyses to investigate the predictive power of differ-
ent types of mathematics for later language skills and whether time
intervals affected the relation. The results showed that after con-
trolling for covariates, types of language skills at Times 1 and 2,
types of language measures at Times 1 and 2, second-language
status, age, and time interval, we did not find the type of mathe-
matics skills as a significant moderator, ps.05.
Summary of Results
Taken together, these results indicate that the relation between
language and mathematics was significant and moderate and was
influenced by types of language skills, types of mathematics skills,
second-language learning status, and age. Specifically, after con-
trolling covariates and including all moderators simultaneously in
the metaregression models, we found the following:
1. Comprehensive language skills showed the strongest re-
lations to mathematics in general, numerical knowledge,
calculations, and word problems, whereas phonological
processing including phonological awareness and RAN
showed the weakest relations with mathematics in gen-
eral, numerical knowledge, calculations, and word prob-
lems. After partialing out both working memory and
intelligence, only RAN seemed to show the strongest
relation to numerical knowledge.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
16 PENG ET AL.
2. The relations between language and mathematics perfor-
mance in general were stronger among native language
speakers than among second-language learners. After
partialing out working memory and intelligence, there
were no differences between native language speakers
and second-language learners on the relation between
language and mathematics.
3. Advanced and more comprehensive mathematics skills,
such as solving word problems showed stronger relations
Table 7
Moderation Analysis on the Relation Between Language and Mathematics Controlling for Working Memory and Reasoning
Measure Beta SE t 95% CI pvalue
Publication type
Peer-Reviewed versus Other Types .11 .07 1.49 [.03, .26] .14
Sample status
Typically Developing versus Atypically Developing .01 .03 .38 [.05, .08] .70
SES
Low SES versus General/Unspecified .07 .04 1.81 [.15, .01] .08
Middle SES versus General/Unspecified .04 .05 .73 [.14, .07] .47
Middle SES versus low SES .03 .05 .64 [.07, .13] .53
Language type
Alphabetic versus Nonalphabetic
Language measurement type
Receptive versus Expressive .004 .04 .11 [.07, .08] .91
Receptive versus Receptive Expressive .05 .06 .71 [.08, .18] .48
Expressive versus Receptive Expressive .04 .05 .83 [.06, .14] .41
Age .0002 .001 .22 [.002, .001] .83
Second-language learning status
Second-Language Learners versus Mixed .003 .04 .08 [.07, .08] .94
Native Language Speakers versus Second-Language Learners .06 .05 1.33 [.15, .03] .19
Native Language Speaker versus Mixed .06 .04 1.56 [.13, .02] .13
Types of language skills
Rapid Automatized Naming versus Phonological Awareness .02 .05 .44 [.12, .08] .79
Vocabulary versus Phonological Awareness .02 .04 .47 [.06, .10] .64
Oral Comprehension versus Phonological Awareness .002 .04 .05 [.07, .07] .98
Comprehensive Language versus Phonological Awareness .08 .05 2.00 [.0004, .16] .05
Vocabulary versus Rapid Automatized Naming .04 .05 .78 [.06, .14] .43
Oral Comprehension versus Rapid Automatized Naming .02 .05 .42 [.08, .12] .68
Comprehensive Language versus Rapid Automatized Naming .10 .05 2.08 [.003, .20] .04
Oral Comprehension versus Vocabulary .02 .03 .75 [.07, .03] .45
Comprehensive Language versus Vocabulary .06 .04 1.71 [.01, .13] .09
Comprehensive Language versus Oral Comprehension .08 .04 2.19 [.01, .15] .03
Types of mathematics skills
Calculations versus Numerical Knowledge .02 .03 .54 [.08, .04] .59
Word Problems versus Numerical Knowledge .05 .03 1.53 [.12, .02] .13
Fractions versus Numerical Knowledge .03 .04 .95 [.11, .04] .35
Geometry versus Numerical Knowledge .02 .04 .44 [.07, .11] .66
Algebra versus Numerical Knowledge .03 .04 .77 [.10, .05] .45
Comprehensive Mathematics versus Numerical Knowledge .001 .06 .01 [.12, .12] .99
Word problems versus Calculations .04 .03 1.21 [.10, .02] .23
Fractions versus Calculations .02 .03 .51 [.09, .05] .61
Geometry versus Calculations .04 .05 .72 [.06, .14] .48
Algebra versus Calculations .01 .04 .32 [.09, .07] .75
Comprehensive Mathematics versus Calculations .02 .06 .25 [.11, .14] .81
Fractions versus Word Problems .02 .04 .47 [.06, .10] .64
Geometry versus Word Problems .07 .05 1.37 [.03, .18] .18
Algebra versus Word Problems .02 .04 .57 [.06, .11] .57
Comprehensive Mathematics versus Word Problems .05 .06 .80 [.08, .18] .43
Geometry versus Fractions .05 .06 .87 [.07, .18] .39
Algebra versus Fractions .01 .05 .11 [.09, .10] .91
Comprehensive Mathematics versus Fractions .03 .07 .49 [.10, .17] .63
Algebra versus Geometry .05 .06 .86 [.16, .06] .39
Comprehensive Mathematics versus Geometry .02 .07 .29 [.16, .12] .77
Comprehensive Mathematics versus Algebra .03 .07 .39 [.11, .17] .70
Note. All covariates and moderators were entered in one model. Several models were run for thorough subgroup comparisons among moderators with
more than two categories. For the convenience of presentation, subgroup comparisons within categorical moderators are all listed in the model. CI
confidence interval. The second group in each group comparison variable is the reference group (e.g., in alphabetic versus nonalphabetic, nonalphabetic
is the reference group in the dummy coding of language type). — no data for analysis. There are 1,457 correlations and 66 independent samples.
Between-study sampling variance (
2
) is .01.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
17
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
with language than did relatively foundational mathemat-
ics skills, such as numerical knowledge and calculations.
However, the types of mathematics skills did not affect
the relation between language and mathematics after
partialing out working memory and intelligence.
4. Age moderated the relations between language in general/
phonological awareness/RAN and numerical knowledge/
calculations such that these relations decreased with age.
However, these age effects were not detected when work-
ing memory and intelligence were partialed out.
5. Working memory alone explained 8%16% of the vari-
ance in the relation between language and mathematics in
general and 0%21% of the variance in the relation
between language and different types of mathematics
skills; intelligence alone explained 21%–23% of the vari-
ance in the relation between language and mathematics in
general and 20%38% of the variance in the relation
between language and different types of mathematics;
working memory and intelligence together (in one
model) can explain 49%54% of the variance in the
relation between language and mathematics in general
and 27%67% of the variance in the relation between
language and different types of mathematics.
6. Language significantly predicted later mathematics when
controlling for initial mathematics. Mathematics perfor-
mance significantly predicted later language when con-
trolling for initial language.
Discussion
In the present meta-analysis, we found that language was mod-
erately related to mathematics. More importantly, our moderation
and partial correlation analyses provided direct and indirect evi-
dence on the role of the medium and the thinking functions of
language in the relation between language and mathematics, re-
spectively. In the following sections, we discuss these findings in
detail.
The Medium Function of Language in Mathematics
Types of language/mathematics skills and development.
Based on the medium function hypothesis of language and the
accumulative nature of mathematics learning, we hypothesized
Table 8
Moderation Analysis on the Relation Between Language and Numerical Knowledge Controlling for Working Memory and Reasoning
Measure Beta SE t 95% CI pvalue
Publication type
Peer-reviewed versus Other Types .29 .17 1.71 [.09, .68] .12
Sample status
Typically Developing versus Atypically Developing .03 .06 .58 [.09, .16] .58
SES
Low SES versus General/Unspecified .05 .04 1.11 [.14, .05] .29
Middle SES versus General/Unspecified .06 .08 .81 [.11, .23] .44
Middle SES versus low SES .11 .08 1.32 [.07, .29] .22
Language type
Alphabetic versus Nonalphabetic
Language measurement type
Receptive versus Expressive .003 .06 .06 [.13, .13] .95
Receptive versus Receptive Expressive .05 .11 .48 [.19, .29] .64
Expressive versus Receptive Expressive .05 .07 .73 [.10, .20] .49
Age .01 .02 .42 [.05, .03] .68
Second-language learning status
Second-Language Learners versus Mixed
Native Language Speakers versus Second-Language Learners
Native Language Speaker versus Mixed .12 .11 1.14 [.36, .12] .28
Types of language skills
Rapid Automatized Naming versus Phonological Awareness .10 .05 1.83 [.02, .22] .10
Vocabulary versus Phonological Awareness .05 .07 .73 [.20, .10] .48
Oral Comprehension versus Phonological Awareness .05 .09 .55 [.24, .15] .60
Comprehensive Language versus Phonological Awareness .10 .07 1.38 [.26, .06] .20
Vocabulary versus Rapid Automatized Naming .15 .06 2.34 [.29, .01] .04
Oral Comprehension versus Rapid Automatized Naming .14 .08 1.80 [.32, .03] .10
Comprehensive Language versus Rapid Automatized Naming .20 .04 4.64 [.29, .10] .001
Oral Comprehension versus Vocabulary .002 .06 .04 [.12, .13] .97
Comprehensive Language versus Vocabulary .05 .07 .76 [.20, .10] .46
Comprehensive Language versus Oral Comprehension .05 .08 .63 [.24, .13] .54
Note. All covariates and moderators were entered in one model. Several models were run for thorough subgroup comparisons among moderators with
more than two categories. For the convenience of presentation, subgroup comparisons within categorical moderators are all listed in the model. CI
confidence interval. The second group in each group comparison variable is the reference group (e.g., in alphabetic versus nonalphabetic, nonalphabetic
is the reference group in the dummy coding of language type). — no data for analysis. Bolded number means significant after applying Benjamini-
Hochberg procedure. There are 517 correlations and 23 independent samples. Between-study sampling variance (
2
) is .01.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
18 PENG ET AL.
that different types of language skills might be differentially re-
lated to different types of mathematics skills. Foundational lan-
guage skills (e.g., phonological processing) might be important
mediums for foundational mathematics skills (e.g., numerical
knowledge and calculations), and the importance of language
retrieval (e.g., RAN) for these foundational mathematics skills
would also increase with age. In contrast, more semantically
oriented language skills (e.g., vocabulary and oral comprehension)
are more important mediums for word-problem solving (Fuchs et
al., 2015, 2018). We indeed found different types of mathematics
related to phonological processing, vocabulary, and oral compre-
hension, although comprehensive language skills (tapping two or
more skills out of phonological processing [phonological aware-
ness and RAN], vocabulary, and oral comprehension) showed the
strongest relations to most mathematics tasks. Compared with
RAN, vocabulary and oral comprehension showed stronger rela-
tions with word problems. However, we also found that working
memory and intelligence explained more variance (48%67%) in
the relation between language and word problems than in the
relation between language and foundational mathematics skills
(numerical knowledge and calculations, 27%42%). These find-
ings suggest that the stronger relation between vocabulary/oral
comprehension and word problems may reflect the important
medium function of semantically oriented language skills for word
problems, but can also partly reflect that these semantically ori-
ented language skills bear a bigger thinking function for word
problems. That is, word problems draw heavily on making infer-
ences in the use of vocabulary and oral comprehension about the
relations among quantities beyond simply using vocabulary and
oral comprehension to decode text.
We did not find phonological processing to show stronger
relations to numerical knowledge/calculations in comparison with
vocabulary/oral comprehension. The relation between phonologi-
cal processing and numerical knowledge/calculations also did not
increase with age. Actually, we found that age exerted a negative
impact on the relations between phonological awareness/RAN and
numerical knowledge/calculations. This might be because both
RAN and numerical knowledge/calculations have less variance
among older students. However, after partialing out intelligence
and working memory, we did not find that age affected these
relations, and RAN seemed to show the strongest relation with
numerical knowledge. These findings suggest that the thinking
function of language could potentially mask the importance of
foundational language skills as important mediums for founda-
tional mathematics skills. That is, when cognitive processes are
controlled for, performing numerical tasks indeed relies more
Table 9
Moderation Analysis on the Relation Between Language and Calculations Controlling for Working Memory and Reasoning
Measure Beta SE t 95% CI pvalue
Publication type
Peer-Reviewed versus Other Types .18 .10 1.85 [.02, .37] .07
Sample status
Typically Developing versus Atypically Developing .01 .04 .24 [.06, .08] .81
SES
Low SES versus General/Unspecified .09 .05 1.92 [.19, .005] .06
Middle SES versus General/Unspecified .07 .06 1.15 [.20, .05] .26
Middle SES versus Low SES .02 .05 .44 [.07, .12] .66
Language type
Alphabetic versus Nonalphabetic
Language measurement type
Receptive versus Expressive .01 .05 .12 [.11, .10] .91
Receptive versus Receptive Expressive .07 .08 .85 [.09, .22] .40
Expressive versus Receptive Expressive .07 .04 1.99 [.002, .15] .06
Age .002 .007 .31 [.01, .02] .76
Second-language learning status
Second-Language Learners versus Mixed .0003 .06 .01 [.12, .12] .99
Native Language Speakers versus Second-Language Learners .03 .06 .50 [.15, .09] .62
Native Language Speaker versus Mixed .03 .04 .79 [.10, .05] .43
Types of language skills
Rapid Automatized Naming versus Phonological Awareness .01 .05 .12 [.11, .10] .91
Vocabulary versus Phonological Awareness .06 .05 1.07 [.05, .17] .29
Oral Comprehension versus Phonological Awareness .03 .05 .64 [.07, .14] .53
Comprehensive Language versus Phonological Awareness .06 .05 1.33 [.03, .16] .19
Vocabulary versus Rapid Automatized Naming .06 .05 1.22 [.04, .17] .23
Oral Comprehension versus Rapid Automatized Naming .04 .05 .78 [.06, .14] .44
Comprehensive Language versus Rapid Automatized Naming .07 .04 1.55 [.02, .16] .13
Oral Comprehension versus Vocabulary .03 .03 .77 [.09, .04] .45
Comprehensive Language versus Vocabulary .01 .04 .14 [.07, .08] .89
Comprehensive Language versus Oral Comprehension .03 .04 .86 [.04, .10] .40
Note. All covariates and moderators were entered in one model. Several models were run for thorough subgroup comparisons among moderators with
more than 2 categories. For the convenience of presentation, subgroup comparisons within categorical moderators are all listed in the model. CI
confidence interval. The second group in each group comparison variable is the reference group (e.g., in Alphabetic versus Nonalphabetic, Nonalphabetic
is the reference group in the dummy coding of language type). — no data for analysis. There are 571 correlations and 47 independent samples.
Between-study sampling variance (
2
) is .01.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
19
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
heavily on the retrieval of mathematics facts based on language
compared with other mathematics skills. This finding is also in
line with Koponen et al.’s (2017) finding that RAN is a strong
predictor of the relation between language and calculations. Al-
though we did not find the salient retrieval role of language in
calculations as Koponen et al. (2017) did (partly due to the fact that
those researchers placed a great emphasis on relatively simple
calculations such as fact retrieval tasks and calculation fluency
tasks), their findings, together with ours, suggest that the retrieval
mechanism underlying RAN makes RAN an important variable in
explaining the variance of numerical knowledge and calculations.
The importance of using language as a medium to retrieve
mathematics knowledge for foundational mathematics also re-
ceives support from intervention and cross-cultural comparison
studies. Specifically, several intervention studies with both typi-
cally and atypically developing children have highlighted that the
involvement of language in numerical knowledge and calculations
may be most effective when language is used as a medium to
facilitate the consolidation and the retrieval of mathematics facts
(Geary, 1994). For example, Morgan et al. (2015) found that for
first graders with mathematics difficulties, instruction focused on
discussion/explanation for early numeracy and calculations was
not as effective as teacher-directed activities that focused on ex-
plicit and direct number knowledge instruction and practice. Rittle-
Johnson (2006) found that using language for self-explanation in
calculations did not produce more sophisticated procedures or
understanding, but tended to place more cognitive load on per-
forming calculation tasks, which sometimes led to incorrect pro-
cedures. Moreover, research on cross-cultural comparisons re-
vealed that Chinese children and adults often outperformed their
English-speaking counterparts in solving calculation problems
(LeFevre & Liu, 1997). The advantage in calculations among
Chinese individuals, especially in early development, has partly
been attributed to mathematics instruction in China, which puts a
strong emphasis on memorizing mathematics facts (e.g., rote mem-
ory of a multiplication table) and relies heavily on the retrieval of
mathematics facts in performing calculations (Geary, Bow-
Thomas, Liu, & Siegler, 1996; Liu, Ding, Xu, & Wang, 2017).
Second-language learning status. Research on mathematics
learning among second-language learners often emphasizes the
medium effects of the second language on acquiring mathematics-
specific terminology and expressions as well as on students’ com-
munication quality in mathematics classrooms (Moschkovich,
2002; Pimm, 1987; Schleppegrell, 2007). Thus, we hypothesized
that the relation between language and mathematics might be
stronger among second-language learners. However, our findings
Table 10
Moderation Analysis on the Relation Between Language and Word Problems Controlling for Working Memory and Reasoning
Measure Beta SE t 95% CI pvalue
Publication type
Peer-reviewed versus Other Types .06 .07 .79 [.09, .21] .44
Sample status
Typically Developing versus Atypically Developing .02 .04 .46 [.07, .10] .65
SES
Low SES versus General/Unspecified .004 .04 .08 [.09, .10] .94
Middle SES versus General/Unspecified .01 .07 .21 [.15, .13] .84
Middle SES versus Low SES .02 .05 .35 [.12, .09] .73
Language type
Alphabetic versus Nonalphabetic
Language measurement type
Receptive versus Expressive .05 .04 1.34 [.03, .14] .20
Receptive versus Receptive Expressive .03 .05 .52 [.13, .08] .61
Expressive versus Receptive Expressive .08 .06 1.26 [.21, .05] .23
Age .001 .001 1.46 [.002, .0004] .16
Second-language learning status
Second-Language Learners versus Mixed
Native Language Speakers versus Second-Language Learners
Native Language Speaker versus Mixed .03 .03 1.00 [.10, .04] .33
Types of language skills
Rapid Automatized Naming versus Phonological Awareness .002 .05 .04 [.12, .11] .97
Vocabulary versus Phonological Awareness .01 .07 .10 [.15, .14] .93
Oral Comprehension versus Phonological Awareness .02 .06 .34 [.15, .11] .74
Comprehensive Language versus Phonological Awareness .02 .06 .34 [.11, .16] .74
Vocabulary versus Rapid Automatized Naming .004 .05 .09 [.11, .10] .93
Oral Comprehension versus Rapid Automatized Naming .02 .04 .47 [.10, .07] .65
Comprehensive Language versus Rapid Automatized Naming .02 .05 .51 [.07, .12] .61
Oral Comprehension versus Vocabulary .01 .03 .51 [.07, .04] .62
Comprehensive Language versus Vocabulary .03 .03 .98 [.03, .09] .34
Comprehensive Language versus Oral Comprehension .04 .03 1.43 [.02, .10] .17
Note. All covariates and moderators were entered in one model. Several models were run for thorough subgroup comparisons among moderators with
more than two categories. For the convenience of presentation, subgroup comparisons within categorical moderators are all listed in the model. CI
confidence interval. The second group in each group comparison variable is the reference group (e.g., in alphabetic versus nonalphabetic, nonalphabetic
is the reference group in the dummy coding of language type). — no data for analysis. There are 219 correlations and 29 independent samples.
Between-study sampling variance (
2
) is .01.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
20 PENG ET AL.
indicated that the relation between language and mathematics was
weaker among second-language learners than among native lan-
guage speakers. When partialing out working memory and intel-
ligence, we did not find a significant difference in the relation
between language and mathematics between native language
speakers and second-language learners. These findings, taken to-
gether, suggest that language may play an equally important me-
dium role in mathematics performance for both native language
speakers and second-language learners, but that the thinking func-
tion of language for mathematics may be more obvious for native
language speakers than for second-language learners.
One plausible explanation is that inadequate proficiency in the
second language among second-language learners may create a
bottleneck in their use of the second language to think in mathe-
matics tasks. This is also in line with some research that suggests
that teaching mathematics to second-language learners in their first
language can make it easier for them to engage in mathematical
reasoning (e.g., de Araujo et al., 2018; Martin & Fuchs, 2019).
Another explanation is that it may be more cognitively efficient for
second-language learners to rely on their first language to perform
mathematics. That is, using only the first language may reduce the
cognitive switching cost between the first language and the second
language during mathematics performance. This cognitive switch-
ing cost may be especially important for solving difficult mathe-
matics tasks, and using the first language can save more cognitive
resources for mathematics cognition (Clarkson, 2007; Rubinstein-
Ávila, Sox, Kaplan, & McGraw, 2015).
Partial correlations. One important piece of indirect evi-
dence on the medium function of language in mathematics comes
from the partial correlation analysis. We found that working mem-
ory and intelligence can each independently explain up to 25% of
the variance in the relation between language and mathematics,
and when combined together can explain up to about 50% of the
variance in the relation between language and mathematics. These
findings suggest that core cognition, although important as re-
flected in large variances (we discuss the importance of the think-
ing function of language in mathematics later), cannot explain all
variance in the relation between language and mathematics, sup-
porting the medium function of language in mathematics.
The Thinking Function of Language in Mathematics
Partial correlations. We begin by discussing partial correla-
tions in the context of the thinking function hypothesis, because
the findings from the partial correlations between language and
mathematics, controlling for working memory, intelligence, or
both, provided the most direct evidence for the thinking function of
language for mathematics. Specifically, working memory alone
explained 8%16% of the variance in the relation between lan-
guage and mathematics in general, and 0%21% of the variance
in the relations between language and different types of mathe-
matics skills. Interestingly, working memory did not seem to
explain more variance in the relations between language and
relatively complex mathematics skills (e.g., word problems, frac-
tions, and algebra) than in those between language and relatively
simple mathematics skills (e.g., numerical knowledge and calcu-
lations). This finding is line with the previous meta-analysis on the
relation between mathematics and working memory suggesting
that working memory was related to both advanced and founda-
tional mathematics skills to a similar degree, even when control-
ling for age (Peng et al., 2016).
Intelligence alone explained 21%23% of the variance in the
relation between language and mathematics in general and
20%38% of the variance in the relations between language and
different types of mathematics. Thus, in contrast to working mem-
ory, intelligence seemed to be more important in the relation
between language and mathematics. This may be partly because
intelligence tasks often tap working memory (Ackerman et al.,
2005), and intelligence exerts greater influence on language and
mathematics than working memory does (Peng et al., 2016; Peng
et al., 2018). Similar to working memory, intelligence did not seem
to explain more variance in the relations involving relatively
complex mathematics skills (e.g., word problems, fractions, and
algebra) than in those involving relatively simple mathematics
skills (e.g., numerical knowledge and calculations).
However, when both working memory and intelligence were
combined in a model, they explained 49%54% of the variance in
the relation between language and mathematics in general and
27%67% of the variance in the relations between language and
different types of mathematics. These findings, taken together,
support the hypothesis that working memory and intelligence are
the core cognitive components reflecting the thinking function of
language for mathematics. However, working memory and intel-
ligence did not explain all variance in the relation between lan-
guage and mathematics. Thus, language is important in and of
itself, potentially as a medium as discussed above, for mathemat-
ics. For example, research has suggested that, controlling for
cognition, language skills such as general vocabulary, mathematics
vocabulary, and language comprehension are demonstrated as
important parts of many important mathematics skills such as
calculation (e.g., Peng & Lin, 2019), solving word problems (e.g.,
Fuchs et al., 2018), and solving problems with fractions (Jordan et
al., 2013).
Types of language/mathematics skills and development.
We found that relatively complex language skills (e.g., compre-
hensive language and oral comprehension) showed the strongest
relations with mathematics performance in general, numerical
knowledge, calculations, and word problems, whereas relatively
simple language skills (e.g., phonological awareness and RAN)
showed the weakest relations with mathematics in general, numer-
ical knowledge, calculations, and word problems. Relatively com-
plex mathematic skills (e.g., solving word problems, comprehen-
sive mathematics skills) showed stronger relations with language
than did other types of mathematics skills. Working memory and
intelligence together seemed to explain more variance in the rela-
tions involving relatively more complex mathematics skills (e.g.,
solving word problems and fractions; e.g., about 48%67%) than
in the relations involving relatively simple mathematics skills (e.g.,
numerical knowledge and calculations) and language (e.g., about
27%42%). In addition, the relations between phonological
awareness/RAN and numerical knowledge/calculations decreased
with age, but age did not affect the relations between relatively
complex language skills (e.g., oral comprehension) and relatively
complex mathematics skills (e.g., word problems).
These findings, taken together, are in line with the thinking func-
tion hypothesis of language and the accumulative nature of mathe-
matics learning. That is, more complex mathematics skills may in-
volve more working memory, intelligence, and more complex and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
21
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
semantically oriented language skills (e.g., language comprehension)
than do relatively simple mathematics skills. The thinking function of
language is relatively stable for these complex language and mathe-
matics skills across development, but the importance of the thinking
function of language decreases for relatively simple mathematics
skills across development, partly due to the increasing role of math-
ematics knowledge retrieval in performing simple mathematics tasks
(e.g., Geary et al., 2017; Lee & Bull, 2016).
Language measurement. Based on the thinking function hy-
pothesis of language, expressive language tasks would show stronger
relations to mathematics than would receptive language tasks. How-
ever, our findings indicate that expressive and receptive language
show comparable relations to mathematics, which is also in line with
previous research showing that expressive language is not necessarily
more working memory demanding than receptive language when
controlling for age and sample status (Adams & Gathercole, 1995;
Peng et al., 2018). This finding may suggest that the expressive/
receptive format of language should be considered a characteristic of
the medium function of language. Measuring language in either an
expressive format or a receptive format would not alter the degrees of
cognitive involvement in those measures. That said, our finding is
based on unselected samples. An expressive language measure may
be more cognitively demanding than a receptive language measure for
individuals with disabilities (e.g., language impairment or cognitive
deficits).
Mutualism. Based on the theory of mutualism, high-level cog-
nition (i.e., working memory and intelligence) and academic skills
mutually influence each other during development (Kievit et al.,
2017; McArdle et al., 2000; Peng et al., 2019; Peng et al., 2018;
Rindermann et al., 2010; Van Der Maas et al., 2006). This is because
working memory and intelligence are invested in the learning of
academic skills, and the learning of academic skills that involve
working memory and intelligence may serve as a “long-term inter-
vention” for those cognitive skills (e.g., Peng et al., 2019; Van Der
Maas et al., 2006). Our synthesis of the findings from longitudinal
studies suggests that language and mathematics significantly pre-
dicted each other, even when initial performance was controlled for.
This finding indirectly supports mutualism and provides further evi-
dence on the importance of the thinking function of language for
mathematics from a developmental perspective.
Limitations
We note several limitations in interpreting our findings. First, other
functions of language may play an indirect but still non-negligible role
in the relation between language and mathematics. For example, an
increasing number of studies of children with emotional and behav-
ioral disorders emphasize the social-emotional function of language
for behavioral management and academic performance. Often, we use
language to make meaning of sensations from our body and world in
a given context so that we can better regulate our emotions and
behaviors (Lindquist, MacCormack, & Shablack, 2015). Further, we
need language in order to navigate complex educational and social
systems, systems that often involve dyadic linguistic interactions
(Chow, 2018; Chow & Ekholm, 2019). Research on children with
emotional and behavioral disorders has shown that these children
often demonstrate language deficits, which may be one major reason
for their behavioral problems (Chow & Wehby, 2019; Hollo, Wehby,
& Oliver, 2014). Moreover, interventions that focus on language or
communication skills for children with language delays can signifi-
cantly reduce behavioral problems (Curtis, Kaiser, Estabrook, & Rob-
erts, 2019). Because emotional regulation and behavior management
are foundational for academic performance at school (Hinshaw, 1992;
Kremer, Flower, Huang, & Vaughn, 2016) as well as essential for
high-quality language-learning interactions (Chow & Ekholm, 2019),
the relation between language and mathematics may be partly ex-
plained by the social-emotional function of language (language ¡
emotion/behavior ¡mathematics learning/performance).
Second, in this study we considered only working memory and
intelligence as reflecting the thinking function of language, because
they are important for both language and mathematics (Ackerman et
al., 2005; Daneman & Merikle, 1996; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1990;
Peng et al., 2019). That said, other cognitive skills such as executive
functions and processing speed are also important for language and
mathematics (Bull & Johnston, 1997; Bull & Lee, 2014; Miller, Kail,
Leonard, & Tomblin, 2001; Weiland & Yoshikawa, 2013). Working
memory and intelligence overlap largely with (arguably contain)
executive functions (e.g., Baddeley, 1992; Rey-Mermet, Gade, Souza,
von Bastian, & Oberauer, 2019) and processing speed (e.g., process-
ing speed may share overlapping variance with RAN in predicting
mathematics ability; Georgiou, Tziraki, Manolitsis, & Fella, 2013),
yet including executive functions and processing speed (or other
important cognitive skills) may increase the importance of the think-
ing function of language for mathematics.
Third, to increase the generalizability of our findings, we included
heterogeneous samples (i.e., typically and nontypically developing
individuals). Although we controlled for sample types in our analyses,
we could not conduct further analyses within the atypical sample. This
is because the atypically developing group was quite heterogeneous,
including different developmental or acquired disorders such as learn-
ing disabilities, Alzheimer’s disease, hearing impairment, brain inju-
ries, and the like, and the sample sizes for those subgroups were very
small. Future studies should further examine whether different disor-
ders differentially influence the relations between language and math-
ematics. On a related note, there is research on patients with aphasia
(we did not have any aphasia sample in this study) in which some of
the patients could still perform nonlinguistic intelligence tasks and
mathematics tasks that involved relatively simple numerical knowl-
edge and calculations (Fedorenko & Varley, 2016). These findings
suggest that mathematics facts/knowledge in long-term memory may
not necessarily be stored or processed in a language format and thus
that the medium/thinking function of language may not explain all of
the retrieval and reasoning processes during mathematics tasks. Fu-
ture studies should investigate this issue further.
Fourth, we defined the types of language skills as phonological
processing (phonological awareness and RAN), vocabulary, and oral
comprehension. This categorization of language skills, although com-
monly used in studying the relations between language and academic
skills and emphasized in language assessment and instruction at
school (Chow & Wehby, 2018; Fuchs et al., 2015; LeFevre et al.,
2010; Peng & Lin, 2019; Purpura & Napoli, 2015), may not fully
capture the effects of more nuanced skills from the perspective of
linguistics (e.g., grammar on both the word and sentence level; mor-
phological skills tapping vocabulary measured in an oral comprehen-
sion format). Relatedly, we are also aware of the research/debate on
the dimensionality/construct of language, which suggests that differ-
ent language skills may reflect different constructs depending on
development. For example, some suggests receptive language, ex-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
22 PENG ET AL.
pressive language, vocabulary, oral comprehension, and grammar
may be best captured as one construct across development (Bornstein
et al., 2014; Lervåg et al., 2018), while some suggests grammar,
vocabulary, and discourse may become gradually distinct with devel-
opment (Language and Reading Research Consortium, 2015). Thus,
future studies on the relation between language and mathematics
should consider the dimensionality of language and the interaction
between the dimensionality of language and development.
Last but not least, because of our small sample size, we were unable
to run analyses for some categories of moderators, and some of the
moderation analyses we conducted may have been underpowered. For
example, we did not have a sufficient number of studies to run
moderation analyses on the relation between language and geometry,
algebra, or fractions or moderation analyses on whether age moder-
ates the relations between phonological awareness/RAN/oral compre-
hension and fractions/geometry/algebra with/without controlling for
working memory and intelligence. We had only a small number of
effect sizes for the moderation analyses on the longitudinal studies,
and the results of those moderation analyses may be more exploratory
in nature and warrant further investigation.
Implications for Theory
With these limitations in mind, this is the first meta-analysis that
systematically investigated the relation between language and math-
ematics and the mechanisms underlying this relation. The findings
have important implications for our understanding of theories of
language and mathematics cognition. First, although the medium and
the thinking functions of language have long been discussed and
debated in the theory of language/cognition (Bruner, 1966; Car-
ruthers, 2002; Fetzer & Tiedemann, 2018; Vygotsky, 1986), little
research has disentangled the contributions of these functions to
academic performance. The present meta-analysis provides direct and
indirect empirical evidence to show that language bears both medium
and thinking functions in its relation to mathematics.
Second, the importance of these two functions of language varies
for different types of mathematics and across development. The
medium function of language is consistently important for all types of
mathematics, especially for the foundational mathematics skills (nu-
merical knowledge and simple calculations) that rely heavily on the
retrieval of mathematics knowledge based on language with devel-
opment. The thinking function of language is important for all types
of mathematics but is particularly important for advanced mathemat-
ics that heavily involves high-level cognition. However, the impor-
tance of the thinking function of language for foundational mathe-
matics seems to reduce over development, as individuals become
more efficient in retrieving mathematics knowledge through lan-
guage. Thus, the medium and the thinking functions of language may
interact with each other in learning/performing mathematics. That is,
the fluent use of language as a medium in mathematics performance,
especially for foundational mathematics skills, can possibly free up
cognitive resources for the use of the thinking function of language in
learning/performing advanced mathematics skills. This interaction
effect between the medium and the thinking functions of language
may become more important for mathematics with the accumulation
of mathematics knowledge.
Third, our findings further supplement the mutualism theory of
cognition. Previous research has suggested that core cognition (e.g.,
working memory and intelligence) and academic skills mutually
influence each other’s development. Our findings suggest that such
mutualistic effects between cognition and academic skills (e.g., math-
ematics) may be partly driven by the use of language in performing/
learning academic skills.
Considering all these findings, we tentatively propose a develop-
mental function hypothesis of language for mathematics (see Figure
2). Based on this hypothesis, children use language as a medium to
communicate, represent, and retrieve mathematics knowledge and
also as a thinking tool to facilitate working memory and reasoning
processes during mathematics performance and learning. The me-
dium function of language may become more important for funda-
mental mathematics skills with development, which further strength-
ens the role of the thinking function of language in advanced
mathematics. Such use of language may bolster the reciprocal effects
between cognition and mathematics during development.
Implications for Practice
Our findings also have implications for mathematics instruction.
First, mathematics instruction may need to place different emphases
on the medium and the thinking functions of language for different
types of mathematics skills during development. For foundational
mathematics skills, although language should be used to help children
gain conceptual knowledge, it is critical for children to use language
primarily as a medium (e.g., for retrieval) in foundational tasks after
they grasp conceptual knowledge. This is also in line with recent
research showing that early mathematics classroom instruction em-
phasizing discussion (the thinking function of language) is not as
effective as explicit instruction and practice, especially for children
with mathematics difficulties who lack foundational numerical
knowledge (Morgan et al., 2015). For advanced mathematics skills,
language should be used as an important tool to understand concep-
tual knowledge first before procedural knowledge/practice. This sug-
gestion is especially important for children with mathematics diffi-
culties and cognitive weaknesses, and it is supported by recent
findings that children with mathematics difficulties and very weak
working memory learn fractions better with conceptual activities
(using language to think) than with fluency activities (using language
as a medium; Fuchs et al., 2014).
On a related note, despite researchers advocating against the use of
ineffective “keyword” strategies, such strategies are commonly used
in teaching word-problem solving. Keyword strategies rely on iden-
tifying certain key words related to a specific mathematical operation
(e.g., total,sum,altogether,in all denote addition; Xin, Jitendra, &
Deatline-Buchman, 2005). Such a strategy focuses narrowly on the
meaning of vocabulary (the medium function of language) rather than
engaging students in high levels of understanding, thinking, and
reasoning (the thinking function of language). Given our finding that
word problems draw heavily on the thinking function, the instruc-
tional focus should be on facilitating students’ reasoning and infer-
ence making in solving word problems. For example, word-problem
solving strategies, such as schema-based instruction, in which stu-
dents build a schema for an underlying structure of a word problem,
and cognitive strategy instruction, in which students engage in meta-
cognitive processes of solving word problems, have been found to be
effective (e.g., Montague, 1992; Pennequin, Sorel, Nanty, & Fontaine,
2010; Powell & Fuchs, 2018; Xin et al., 2005).
Second, language proficiency at school entry varies enormously
(e.g., Norbury et al., 2016), and some children need extensive lan-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
23
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
guage support (Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018). Indeed, language
intervention has gained increasing attention for children with mathe-
matics difficulties. Research has demonstrated that oral comprehen-
sion is malleable and can be taught to improve mathematics. For
example, Fuchs, Geary, Fuchs, Compton, and Hamlett (2016) found
that embedding supported self-explaining in a fractions intervention
significantly improved children’s accuracy and quality of explana-
tions for comparing fractions, in comparison with the same fractions
intervention but without a language component. This effect of expla-
nation is especially obvious for those with relatively low working
memory at the beginning of the intervention. In another study, Fuchs
et al. (2020) found that teaching children mathematics vocabulary and
integrating mathematics vocabulary instruction with text comprehen-
sion instruction for word problems can help at-risk children better
understand the word-problem schema and improve their word-
problem solving accuracy in comparison with instruction focused
solely on calculations. Likewise, Purpura, Napoli, Wehrspann, and
Gold (2017) have demonstrated that dialogic intervention focused on
quantitative and spatial mathematical language can improve pre-
schoolers’ mathematical language as well as early numeracy skills.
Third, it may be important for second-language learners to learn
mathematics in their second language. On the one hand, trying to
fluently use the second language in mathematics classrooms is nec-
essary for second-language learners to better engage in mathematics
classroom instruction and discussion (de Araujo et al., 2018). On the
other hand, learning numerical facts in a second language can form a
stronger representation of the numerical facts in the second language
than in the first language (Spelke & Tsivkin, 2001). These findings,
together with our finding that the second language constantly serves
as an important medium for mathematics performance among second-
language learners, suggest that for second-language learners, it is
important to master mathematics skills in their second language,
especially for the foundational skills at the beginning of formal
mathematics education. Because the medium function of language
may also facilitate the thinking function of language for mathematics
during development, second-language learners may gradually become
more efficient in using the second language as a thinking tool in
mathematical contexts. Using the second language to learn mathe-
matics with appropriate support may be more beneficial for second-
language learners in the long run.
Last, we have based our proposed developmental function hypoth-
esis of language for mathematics and the above-mentioned implica-
tions for practice on the correlational data from our meta-analysis.
Future experimental/intervention studies should further investigate
Figure 2. Developmental function hypothesis of language for mathematics. A solid arrow means the impact is
relatively stable with development. A dotted arrow with a “” means the impact increases with development.
A dotted arrow with a “” means the impact decreases with development. See the online article for the color
version of this figure.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
24 PENG ET AL.
whether training foundational language skills (e.g., phonological pro-
cessing) can increase foundational mathematics performance, whether
training higher order language skills (e.g., vocabulary and language
comprehension) can transfer to foundational mathematics as well as
advanced mathematics skills, and whether the language training ef-
fects on mathematics, if any, vary with students’ language skills,
mathematics knowledge, and cognitive abilities.
Conclusion
In summary, in this meta-analysis, we have investigated the relation
(and mechanism of this relation) between language and mathematics.
Our main findings provide some new and updated information as
follows: (a) language is significantly and moderately associated with
mathematics, and this relation is stronger for relatively complex
language and mathematics skills; (b) working memory and intelli-
gence can each explain up to 25% of the variance in the relation
between language and mathematics, and, when combined, they can
explain over 50% of the variance in this relation; (c) the retrieval of
language plays an especially important role in numerical knowledge
even when controlling for working memory and intelligence; (d) the
medium function of language for mathematics is equally important for
both native language speakers and second-language learners, but the
thinking function of language for mathematics is more important
among first-language speakers; (e) language and mathematics predict
each other in their development, suggesting a reciprocal relation
between the two.
References
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the
meta-analysis.
Abrahamson, C. A. (1994). Reading disabilities and the third R: The
relationship between reading and arithmetic performance in reading
disabled children (Doctoral dissertation). Springfield, MA: American
International College.
Abreu-Lima, I. M., Leal, T. B., Cadima, J., & Gamelas, A. M. (2013).
Predicting child outcomes from preschool quality in Portugal. European
Journal of Psychology of Education, 28, 399420. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1007/s10212-012-0120-y
Acheson, D. J., & MacDonald, M. C. (2009). Verbal working memory and
language production: Common approaches to the serial ordering of
verbal information. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 5068. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/a0014411
Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., & Boyle, M. O. (2005). Working memory
and intelligence: The same or different constructs? Psychological Bul-
letin, 131, 3060. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.30
Ackerman, P. T., Holloway, C. A., Youngdahl, P. L., & Dykman, R. A.
(2001). The double-deficit theory of reading disability does not fit all.
Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 16, 152–160. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/0938-8982.00016
Ackerman, P. L., & Lohman, D. F. (2003). Education and g. In H. Nyborg
(Ed.), The scientific study of general intelligence (pp. 275–292). http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-008043793-4/50052-0
Adams, A. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Phonological working memory
and speech production in preschool children. Journal of Speech and
Hearing Research, 38, 403–414. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3802
.403
Ahmed, Y., Francis, D. J., York, M., Fletcher, J. M., Barnes, M., & Kulesz,
P. (2016). Validation of the direct and inferential mediation (DIME)
model of reading comprehension in grades 7 through 12. Contemporary
Educational Psychology, 44, 6882. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.cedpsych.2016.02.002
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Adams, A. M., Willis, C., Eaglen, R.,
& Lamont, E. (2005). Working memory and phonological awareness as
predictors of progress towards early learning goals at school entry.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 417–426. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1348/026151005X26804
Andersson, U. (2007). The contribution of working memory to children’s
mathematical word problem solving. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21,
1201–1216.
Andersson, U., & Lyxell, B. (2007). Working memory deficit in children
with mathematical difficulties: A general or specific deficit? Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 197–228. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.jecp.2006.10.001
Anvari, S. H., Trainor, L. J., Woodside, J., & Levy, B. A. (2002).
Relations among musical skills, phonological processing, and early
reading ability in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 83, 111–130. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0965(02)
00124-8
Aram, D. M., & Ekelman, B. L. (1988). Scholastic aptitude and achieve-
ment among children with unilateral brain lesions. Neuropsychologia,
26, 903–916. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(88)90058-9
Ardila, A., Galeano, L. M., & Rosselli, M. (1998). Toward a model of
neuropsychological activity. Neuropsychology Review, 8, 171–190.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1021618218943
Argulewicz, E. N., Bingenheimer, L. T., & Anderson, C. C. (1983).
Concurrent validity of the PPVT-R for Anglo-American and Mexican-
American students. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 1, 163–
167. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/073428298300100207
Arndt, D., Sahr, K., Opfermann, M., Leutner, D., & Fritz, A. (2013). Core
knowledge and working memory as prerequisites of early school arith-
metic. South African Journal of Childhood Education, 3, 1–20. http://
dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajce.v3i1.29
Arnold, D. H., Kupersmidt, J. B., Voegler-Lee, M. E., & Marshall, N.
(2012). The association between preschool children’s social functioning
and their emergent academic skills. Early Childhood Research Quar-
terly, 27, 376–386. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.12.009
Arvedson, P. J. (1999). Numerical cognition in preschool children who
have specific language impairment (Doctoral dissertation). University of
California, Los Angeles, CA.
Austin, A. M. B., Blevins-Knabe, B., & Lokteff, M. (2013). Early math-
ematics and phonological awareness in two child care settings. Early
Child Development and Care, 183, 1197–1214. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1080/03004430.2012.716832
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory, reading and dyslexia. Advances in
Psychology, 34, 141–152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0166-4115(08)
61202-9
Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556–559. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1126/science.1736359
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working
memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2
Baddeley, A., Gardner, J. M., & Grantham-McGregor, S. (1995). Cross-
cultural cognition: Developing tests for developing countries. Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 9, S173–S195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp
.2350090711
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop
as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.105.1.158
Barnes, M. A., Raghubar, K. P., English, L., Williams, J. M., Taylor, H.,
& Landry, S. (2014). Longitudinal mediators of achievement in mathe-
matics and reading in typical and atypical development. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 119, 1–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.jecp.2013.09.006
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
25
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
Barnes, M. A., Stubbs, A., Raghubar, K. P., Agostino, A., Taylor, H.,
Landry, S.,...Smith-Chant, B. (2011). Mathematical skills in 3- and
5-year-olds with spina bifida and their typically developing peers: A
longitudinal approach. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Society, 17, 431–444. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000233
Barre, N., Morgan, A., Doyle, L. W., & Anderson, P. J. (2011). Language
abilities in children who were very preterm and/or very low birth weight:
A meta-analysis. The Journal of Pediatrics, 158, 766–774.e1. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2010.10.032
Bat-Haee, M. A., Mehryar, A. H., & Sabharwal, V. (1972). The correla-
tion between Piaget’s conservation of quantity tasks and three measures
of intelligence in a select group of children in Iran. The Journal of
Psychology, 80, 197–201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1972
.9924796
Beck, F. W., Black, F. L., & Doles, J. (1985). The concurrent validity of
the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-revised relative to the comprehen-
sive tests of basic skills. Educational and Psychological Measurement,
45, 705–710. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001316448504500333
Becker, D. R., Miao, A., Duncan, R., & McClelland, M. M. (2014).
Behavioral self-regulation and executive function both predict visuomo-
tor skills and early academic achievement. Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 29, 411–424. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.04.014
Bell, N. L., Rucker, M., Finch, A. J., Jr., & Alexander, J. (2002).
Concurrent validity of the Slosson full-range intelligence test: Compar-
ison with the Wechsler intelligence scale for children–third ed. and the
Woodcock Johnson tests of achievement–revised. Psychology in the
Schools, 39, 31–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pits.10002
Billings, L. R. (2015). The Peabody picture vocabulary test-revised: Adult
alternate from reliability and comparisons with the Wechsler Adult
Intelligence Scale-revised and Wide Range Achievement Test-revised
(Doctoral dissertation). The University of San Francisco, San Francisco,
CA.
Bjork, I. M., & Bowyer-Crane, C. (2013). Cognitive skills used to solve
mathematical word problems and numerical operations: A study of 6-to
7-year-old children. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 28,
1345–1360. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10212-012-0169-7
Blair, C., Gamson, D., Thorne, S., & Baker, D. (2005). Rising mean IQ:
Cognitive demand of mathematics education for young children, popu-
lation exposure to formal schooling, and the neurobiology of the pre-
frontal cortex. Intelligence, 33, 93–106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell
.2004.07.008
Bloland, R. N. (1998). A comparison of the relative effectiveness of a
measure of Piagetian cognitive development and a set of conventional
prognostic measures in the prediction of the future success of ninth- and
tenth-grade students in algebra (Doctoral dissertation). University of
Southern California, Los Angeles, CA.
Blums, A., Belsky, J., Grimm, K., & Chen, Z. (2017). Building links
between early socioeconomic status, cognitive ability, and math and
science achievement. Journal of Cognition and Development, 18, 16
40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2016.1228652
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2005).
Comprehensive meta-analysis (Version 2). Englewood, NJ: Biostat.
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C. S., Putnick, D. L., & Suwalsky, J. T. (2014).
Stability of core language skill from early childhood to adolescence: A
latent variable approach. Child Development, 85, 1346–1356. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12192
Bowey, J. A. (1995). Socioeconomic status differences in preschool
phonological sensitivity and first-grade reading achievement. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 87, 476487. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
0663.87.3.476
Bracken, B. A., Prasse, D. P., & Breen, M. J. (1984). Concurrent validity
of the Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery with regular and
learning-disabled students. Journal of School Psychology, 22, 185–192.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-4405(84)90038-4
Breen, M. I. (1983). A correlational analysis between the PPVT-R and
Woodcock-Johnson achievement cluster scores for nonreferred regular
education and learning disabled students. Psychology in the Schools, 20,
295–297. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1520-6807(198307)20:3295::
AID-PITS23102003073.0.CO;2-7
Brock, H. (1982). Factor structure of intellectual and achievement mea-
sures for learning disabled children. Psychology in the Schools, 19,
297–304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1520-6807(198207)19:3297::
AID-PITS23101903063.0.CO;2-R
Brown, H. S. (1950). Differential prediction by the ACE. The Journal of
Educational Research, 44, 116–121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
00220671.1950.10881846
Bruner, J. S. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction (Vol. 59). Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Bull, R., & Johnston, R. S. (1997). Children’s arithmetical difficulties:
Contributions from processing speed, item identification, and short-term
memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 65, 1–24. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1996.2358
Bull, R., & Lee, K. (2014). Executive functioning and mathematics
achievement. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 3641. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/cdep.12059
Bulotsky-Shearer, R. J., Bell, E. R., Carter, T. M., & Dietrich, S. L.
(2014). Peer play interactions and learning for low-income preschool
children: The moderating role of classroom quality. Early Education and
Development, 25, 815–840. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2014
.864214
Burchinal, M., Field, S., López, M. L., Howes, C., & Pianta, R. (2012).
Instruction in Spanish in pre-kindergarten classrooms and child out-
comes for English language learners. Early Childhood Research Quar-
terly, 27, 188–197. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.11.003
Burchinal, M., Vernon-Feagans, L., Vitiello, V., & Greenberg, M. (2014).
Thresholds in the association between child care quality and child
outcomes in rural preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quar-
terly, 29, 41–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.09.004
Bustamante, A. S., White, L. J., & Greenfield, D. B. (2017). Approaches
to learning and school readiness in Head Start: Applications to preschool
science. Learning and Individual Differences, 56, 112–118. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.10.012
Cadima, J., Leal, T., & Burchinal, M. (2010). The quality of teacher-
student interactions: Associations with first graders’ academic and be-
havioral outcomes. Journal of School Psychology, 48, 457–482. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2010.09.001
Cai, D., Li, Q. W., & Deng, C. P. (2013). Cognitive processing charac-
teristics of 6th to 8th grade Chinese students with mathematics learning
disability: Relationships among working memory, PASS processes, and
processing speed. Learning and Individual Differences, 27, 120–127.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.07.008
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Lemmon, K. (2004). Individual differences in the
inference of word meanings from context: The influence of reading
comprehension, vocabulary knowledge, and memory capacity. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 96, 671–681. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0022-0663.96.4.671
Callens, M., Tops, W., Stevens, M., & Brysbaert, M. (2014). An explor-
atory factor analysis of the cognitive functioning of first-year bachelor
students with dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 64, 91–119. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1007/s11881-013-0088-6
Cantrell, J. A. (1992). A comparison of cognitive ability measures in the
prediction of achievement in a referred population of college students
(Doctoral dissertation). The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio.
Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. New York, NY: Oxford Univer-
sity Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195367638.001
.0001
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
26 PENG ET AL.
Carruthers, P. (2002). The cognitive functions of language. Behavioral and
Brain Sciences, 25, 657–674. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525
X02000122
Carvajal, H., Kixmiller, J., Knapp, M., Vitt, J., & Weaver, K. A. (1991).
The use of the PPST and intelligence tests in teacher education pro-
grams. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 29, 192–194. http://dx.doi
.org/10.3758/BF03342675
Casey, B. M., Pezaris, E., Fineman, B., Pollock, A., Demers, L., &
Dearing, E. (2015). A longitudinal analysis of early spatial skills com-
pared to arithmetic and verbal skills as predictors of fifth-grade girls’
math reasoning. Learning and Individual Differences, 40, 90–100.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2015.03.028
Castles, A., Rastle, K., & Nation, K. (2018). Ending the reading wars:
Reading acquisition from novice to expert. Psychological Science in the
Public Interest, 19, 5–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1529100618772271
Cathcart, W. G. (1974). The correlation of selected nonmathematical
measures with mathematics achievement. Journal for Research in Math-
ematics Education, 5, 47–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/748721
Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A
critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 1–22. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0046743
Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2006). Language deficits
in poor comprehenders: A case for the simple view of reading. Journal
of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 278–293. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/023)
Cavanaugh, A. L. (1992). Computers, self-efficacy, and primary children
(Doctoral dissertation). The University of Connecticut, Mansfield, CN.
Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1991). Cognitive load theory and the format of
instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 8, 293–332. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1207/s1532690xci0804_2
Chansa-Kabali, T. (2017). Home literacy activities: Accounting for dif-
ferences in early grade literacy outcomes in low-income families in
Zambia. South African Journal of Childhood Education, 7, 1–9. http://
dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajce.v7i1.523
Cheng, D., Yan, X., Gao, Z., Xu, K., & Chen, Q. (2017). attention
contributes to arithmetic Deficits in new-Onset childhood absence epi-
lepsy. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8, 166. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt
.2017.00166
Chow, J. C. (2018). Comorbid language and behavior problems: Develop-
ment, frameworks, and intervention. School Psychology Quarterly, 33,
356–360. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/spq0000270
Chow, J. C., & Ekholm, E. (2018). Do published studies yield larger effect
sizes than unpublished studies in education and special education? A
meta-review. Educational Psychology Review, 30, 727–744.
Chow, J. C., & Ekholm, E. (2019). Language domains differentially predict
mathematics performance in young children. Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 46, 179–186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.02.011
Chow, J. C., & Jacobs, M. (2016). The role of language in fraction
performance: A synthesis of literature. Learning and Individual Differ-
ences, 47, 252–257. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2015.12.017
Chow, J. C., & Wehby, J. H. (2018). Associations between language and
problem behavior: A systematic review and correlational meta-analysis.
Educational Psychology Review, 30, 61–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s10648-016-9385-z
Chow, J. C., & Wehby, J. H. (2019). Effects of symbolic and nonsymbolic
equal-sign intervention in second-grade classrooms. The Elementary
School Journal, 119, 677–702. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/703086
Christian, K., Morrison, F. J., & Bryant, F. B. (1998). Predicting kinder-
garten academic skills: Interactions among child care, maternal educa-
tion, and family literacy environments. Early Childhood Research Quar-
terly, 13, 501–521. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2006(99)80054-4
Chung, K. K., Liu, H., McBride, C., Wong, A. M. Y., & Lo, J. C. (2017).
How socioeconomic status, executive functioning and verbal interac-
tions contribute to early academic achievement in Chinese children.
Educational Psychology, 37, 402–420. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
01443410.2016.1179264
Cirino, P. T. (2011). The interrelationships of mathematical precursors in
kindergarten. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 713–733.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.11.004
Cirino, P. T., Child, A. E., & Macdonald, K. (2018). Longitudinal pre-
dictors of the overlap between reading and math skills. Contemporary
Educational Psychology, 54, 99–111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.cedpsych.2018.06.002
Cirino, P. T., Tolar, T. D., Fuchs, L. S., & Huston-Warren, E. (2016).
Cognitive and numerosity predictors of mathematical skills in middle
school. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 145, 95–119. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.12.010
Clark, C. A., Sheffield, T. D., Wiebe, S. A., & Espy, K. A. (2013).
Longitudinal associations between executive control and developing
mathematical competence in preschool boys and girls. Child Develop-
ment, 84, 662–677. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01854.x
Clarke, B., Shanley, L., Kosty, D., Baker, S. K., Cary, M. S., Fien, H., &
Smolkowski, K. (2018). Investigating the incremental validity of cog-
nitive variables in early mathematics screening. School Psychology
Quarterly, 33, 264–271. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/spq0000214
Clarkson, P. C. (2007). Australian Vietnamese students learning mathe-
matics: High ability bilinguals and their use of their languages. Educa-
tional Studies in Mathematics, 64, 191–215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s10649-006-4696-5
Coates, D. L., & Lewis, M. (1984). Early mother-infant interaction and
infant cognitive status as predictors of school performance and cognitive
behavior in six-year-olds. Child Development, 55, 1219–1230. http://dx
.doi.org/10.2307/1129991
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2013). Applied multiple
regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York,
NY: Routledge. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780203774441
Coladarci, T., & McIntire, G. (1988). Gender, urbanicity, and ability!
Reading, 50, 24–72.
Collins, M. A., & Laski, E. V. (2019). Digging deeper: Shared deep
structures of early literacy and mathematics involve symbolic mapping
and relational reasoning. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 46, 201–
212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.02.008
Common Core State Standards Initiative. (2010). National Governors
Association, Washington, DC.
Cornu, V., Schiltz, C., Martin, R., & Hornung, C. (2018). Visuo-spatial
abilities are key for young children’s verbal number skills. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 604620. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.006
Cowan, R., Donlan, C., Shepherd, D. L., Cole-Fletcher, R., Saxton, M., &
Hurry, J. (2011). Basic calculation proficiency and mathematics achieve-
ment in elementary school children. Journal of Educational Psychology,
103, 786803. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024556
Cowan, R., & Powell, D. (2014). The contributions of domain-general and
numerical factors to third-grade arithmetic skills and mathematical
learning disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 214–229.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034097
Critten, V., Campbell, E., Farran, E., & Messer, D. (2018). Visual per-
ception, visual-spatial cognition and mathematics: Associations and
predictions in children with cerebral palsy. Research in Developmental
Disabilities, 80, 180–191. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2018.06.007
Cross, A. M., Joanisse, M. F., & Archibald, L. M. D. (2019). Mathematical
abilities in children with developmental language disorder. Language,
Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 50, 150–163. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1044/2018_LSHSS-18-0041
Cummins, D. D., Kintsch, W., Reusser, K., & Weimer, R. (1988). The role
of understanding in solving word problems. Cognitive Psychology, 20,
405–438. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(88)90011-4
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
27
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
Cupples, L., Ching, T. Y., Crowe, K., Day, J., & Seeto, M. (2014).
Predictors of early reading skill in 5-year-old children with hearing loss
who use spoken language. Reading Research Quarterly, 49, 85–104.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/rrq.60
Currie, N. K., & Cain, K. (2015). Children’s inference generation: The role
of vocabulary and working memory. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 137, 57–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.03.005
Curtis, K. (1992). An investigation of the predictive utility of the Satz
Battery for the early identification of learning disabilities (Master dis-
sertation). University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
Curtis, P. R., Kaiser, A. P., Estabrook, R., & Roberts, M. Y. (2019). The
longitudinal effects of early language intervention on children’s problem
behaviors. Child Development, 90, 576–592. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
cdev.12942
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working
memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
19, 450466. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6
Daneman, M., & Merikle, P. M. (1996). Working memory and language
comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3,
422–433. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03214546
Davis, D. J. N. (1995). The relationship between reading and attitudes
and mathematical verbal problem-solving ability of introductory alge-
bra college students (Doctoral dissertation). University of Houston,
Houston, Texas.
Day, S. L. (2012). Examining the relations between self-regulation and
academic achievement in third grade students (Doctoral dissertation).
Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL.
de Araujo, Z., Roberts, S. A., Willey, C., & Zahner, W. (2018). English
learners in K–12 mathematics education: A review of the literature.
Review of Educational Research, 88, 879–919. http://dx.doi.org/10
.3102/0034654318798093
Deary, I. J., Strand, S., Smith, P., & Fernandes, C. (2007). Intelligence and
educational achievement. Intelligence, 35, 13–21. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.intell.2006.02.001
de Bilde, J., Boonen, T., Speybroeck, S., De Fraine, B., Van Damme, J.,
& Goos, M. (2015). Experiential education in kindergarten: Associations
with school adjustment and the moderating role of initial achievement.
The Elementary School Journal, 116, 173–197. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1086/683802
Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44, 1–42.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(92)90049-N
Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an anatomical and functional
model of number processing. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 83–120.
Deloche, G., Hannequin, D., Carlomagno, S., Agniel, A., Dordain, M., &
Pasquier, F. (1995). Calculation and number processing in mild Alzhei-
mer’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,
17, 634639. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01688639508405151
Demetriou, A., Merrell, C., & Tymms, P. (2017). Mapping and predicting
literacy and reasoning skills from early to later primary school. Learning
and Individual Differences, 54, 217–225. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.lindif.2017.01.023
De Smedt, B., & Boets, B. (2010). Phonological processing and arithmetic
fact retrieval: Evidence from developmental dyslexia. Neuropsycholo-
gia, 48, 3973–3981. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010
.10.018
De Smedt, B., Taylor, J., Archibald, L., & Ansari, D. (2010). How is
phonological processing related to individual differences in children’s
arithmetic skills? Developmental Science, 13, 508–520. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00897.x
Dinehart, L., & Manfra, L. (2013). Associations between low-income
children’s fine motor skills in preschool and academic performance in
second grade. Early Education and Development, 24, 138–161. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2011.636729
Donker, M., Kroesbergen, E., Slot, E., Van Viersen, S., & De Bree, E.
(2016). Alphanumeric and non-alphanumeric Rapid Automatized Nam-
ing in children with reading and/or spelling difficulties and mathematical
difficulties. Learning and Individual Differences, 47, 8087. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2015.12.011
Donlan, C., Cowan, R., Newton, E. J., & Lloyd, D. (2007). The role of
language in mathematical development: Evidence from children with
specific language impairments. Cognition, 103, 23–33. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.cognition.2006.02.007
Downer, J. T., & Pianta, R. C. (2006). Academic and cognitive function-
ing in first grade: Associations with earlier home and child care predic-
tors and with concurrent home and classroom experiences. School Psy-
chology Review, 35, 11–31.
Duncan, R. J., McClelland, M. M., & Acock, A. C. (2017). Relations
between executive function, behavioral regulation, and achievement:
Moderation by family income. Journal of Applied Developmental Psy-
chology, 49, 21–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2017.01.004
Dunn, D. W., Johnson, C. S., Perkins, S. M., Fastenau, P. S., Byars, A. W.,
deGrauw, T. J., & Austin, J. K. (2010). Academic problems in children
with seizures: Relationships with neuropsychological functioning and
family variables during the 3 years after onset. Epilepsy & Behavior, 19,
455–461. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2010.08.023
Dupere, V., Leventhal, T., Crosnoe, R., & Dion, E. (2010). Understanding
the positive role of neighborhood socioeconomic advantage in achieve-
ment: The contribution of the home, child care, and school environ-
ments. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1227–1244. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/a0020211
Durand, M., Hulme, C., Larkin, R., & Snowling, M. (2005). The cognitive
foundations of reading and arithmetic skills in 7- to 10-year-olds. Jour-
nal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91, 113–136. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.jecp.2005.01.003
Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in
meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical
Journal, 315, 629634. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629
Einarsdóttir, J. T., Björnsdóttir, A., & Símonardóttir, I. (2016). The
predictive value of preschool language assessments on academic
achievement: A 10-year longitudinal study of Icelandic children. Amer-
ican Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 25, 67–79. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1044/2015_AJSLP-14-0184
Elbro, C. (1996). Early linguistic abilities and reading development: A
review and a hypothesis. Reading and Writing, 8, 453–485. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1007/BF00577023
Ellis Weismer, S., Evans, J., & Hesketh, L. J. (1999). An examination of
verbal working memory capacity in children with specific language
impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42,
1249–1260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4205.1249
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory.
Psychological Review, 102, 211–245. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-
295X.102.2.211
Espy, K. A., McDiarmid, M. M., Cwik, M. F., Stalets, M. M., Hamby, A.,
& Senn, T. E. (2004). The contribution of executive functions to emer-
gent mathematic skills in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsy-
chology, 26, 465–486. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2601_6
Evans, J. S. B., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher
cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Sci-
ence, 8, 223–241. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685
Faux, D. K. (1985). The relationship of spatial and memory factors to
reading and arithmetic learning disabilities in children (Doctoral disser-
tation). McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada.
Fazio, B. B. (1999). Arithmetic calculation, short-term memory, and
language performance in children with specific language impairment: A
5-year follow-up. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
42, 420431. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4202.420
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
28 PENG ET AL.
Fedorenko, E. (2014). The role of domain-general cognitive control in
language comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 335. http://dx.doi
.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00335
Fedorenko, E., & Varley, R. (2016). Language and thought are not the
same thing: Evidence from neuroimaging and neurological patients.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1369, 132–153. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13046
Ferrari, M., & Palladino, P. (2012). A longitudinal study of English as
foreign language learning: L1 predictors in Italian students. Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 26, 616625. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.2839
Fetzer, M., & Tiedemann, K. (2018). The interplay of language and objects
in the process of abstracting. In J. Moschkovich, D. Wagner, A. Bose, J.
Rodrigues, & M. Schütte (Eds.), Language and communication in math-
ematics education, international perspectives (pp. 91–103). Dordrecht,
the Netherlands: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-75055-
2_8
Fitzpatrick, C., McKinnon, R. D., Blair, C. B., & Willoughby, M. T.
(2014). Do preschool executive function skills explain the school read-
iness gap between advantaged and disadvantaged children? Learning
and Instruction, 30, 25–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013
.11.003
Foley, T. E. (2003). Community college students’ understanding of frac-
tions: An investigation of the cognitive and metacognitive characteristics
of students verbalizations while problem solving (Doctoral dissertation).
University of Connecticut, Mansfield, CT.
Foreman-Murray, L., & Fuchs, L. S. (2019). Quality of explanation as an
indicator of fraction magnitude understanding. Journal of Learning Disabil-
ities, 52, 181–191. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022219418775120
Forsyth, S. R., & Powell, S. R. (2017). Differences in the mathematics-
vocabulary knowledge of fifth-grade students with and without learning
difficulties. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 32, 231–245.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ldrp.12144
Foster, M. E., Anthony, J. L., Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. H. (2015).
Processes in the development of mathematics in kindergarten children
from Title 1 schools. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140,
56–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.004
Francis, D. J., Kulesz, P. A., & Benoit, J. S. (2018). Extending the simple
view of reading to account for variation within readers and across texts:
The complete view of reading (CVRi). Remedial and Special Education,
39, 274–288. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0741932518772904
Freeman, H. (2018). Developmental predictors of early numeracy out-
comes (Master dissertation). University of the Sciences in Philadelphia,
Philadelphia, PA.
Friso-Van Den Bos, I., Van Der Ven, S. H., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Van
Luit, J. E. (2013). Working memory and mathematics in primary school
children: A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 10, 2944.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2013.05.003
Frost, B. P. (1968). Anxiety and educational achievement. The British
Journal of Educational Psychology, 38, 293–301. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/j.2044-8279.1968.tb02022.x
Fuchs, L. S., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Hollenbeck, K. N., Craddock,
C. F., & Hamlett, C. L. (2008). Dynamic assessment of algebraic
learning in predicting third graders’ development of mathematical prob-
lem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 829850. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012657
Fuchs, L. S., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Paulsen, K., Bryant, J. D., &
Hamlett, C. L. (2005). The prevention, identification, and cognitive
determinants of math difficulty. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97,
493–513. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.97.3.493
Fuchs, L. S., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Powell, S. R., Schumacher, R. F.,
Hamlett, C. L.,...Vukovic, R. K. (2012). Contributions of domain-
general cognitive resources and different forms of arithmetic develop-
ment to pre-algebraic knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 48, 1315–
1326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027475
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Compton, D. L., Hamlett, C. L., & Wang, A. Y.
(2015). Is word-problem solving a form of text comprehension? Scien-
tific Studies of Reading, 19, 204–223. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
10888438.2015.1005745
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Compton, D. L., Powell, S. R., Seethaler, P. M.,
Capizzi, A. M.,...Fletcher, J. M. (2006). The cognitive correlates of
third-grade skill in arithmetic, algorithmic computation, and arithmetic
word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 2943. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.29
Fuchs, L. S., Seethaler, P. M., Sterba, S. K., Craddock, C., Fuchs, D.,
Compton, D. L.,...Changas, P. (2020). Closing the word-problem
achievement gap in first grade: Schema-based word-problem interven-
tion with embedded language comprehension instruction. Journal of
Educational Psychology. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/edu0000467
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hamlett, C. L., Lambert, W., Stuebing, K., &
Fletcher, J. M. (2008). Problem solving and computational skill: Are
they shared or distinct aspects of mathematical cognition? Journal of
Educational Psychology, 100, 3047. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
0663.100.1.30
Fuchs, L. S., Geary, D. C., Fuchs, D., Compton, D. L., & Hamlett, C. L.
(2016). Pathways to third-grade calculation versus word-reading com-
petence: Are they more alike or different? Child Development, 87,
558–567. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12474
Fuchs, L. S., Gilbert, J. K., Fuchs, D., Seethaler, P. M., & Martin, B. N.
(2018). Text comprehension and oral language as predictors of word-
problem solving: Insights into word-problem solving as a form of text
comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22, 152–166. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1398259
Fuchs, L. S., Gilbert, J. K., Powell, S. R., Cirino, P. T., Fuchs, D., Hamlett,
C.L.,...Tolar, T. D. (2016). The role of cognitive processes,
foundational math skill, and calculation accuracy and fluency in word-
problem solving versus prealgebraic knowledge. Developmental Psy-
chology, 52, 2085–2098. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000227
Fuchs, L. S., Schumacher, R. F., Long, J., Namkung, J., Hamlett, C. L.,
Cirino, P. T.,...Changas, P. (2013). Improving at-risk learners’
understanding of fractions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105,
683–700. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032446
Fuchs, L. S., Schumacher, R. F., Sterba, S. K., Long, J., Namkung, J.,
Malone, A.,...Changas, P. (2014). Does working memory moderate the
effects of fraction intervention? An aptitude–treatment interaction. Jour-
nal of Educational Psychology, 106, 499–514. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/a0034341
Fuhs, M. W., & McNeil, N. M. (2013). ANS acuity and mathematics
ability in preschoolers from low-income homes: Contributions of inhib-
itory control. Developmental Science, 16, 136–148. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/desc.12013
Fuhs, M. W., Nesbitt, K. T., Farran, D. C., & Dong, N. (2014). Longitu-
dinal associations between executive functioning and academic skills
across content areas. Developmental Psychology, 50, 1698–1709. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036633
Fuller, R., Nopoulos, P., Arndt, S., O’Leary, D., Ho, B. C., & Andreasen,
N. C. (2002). Longitudinal assessment of premorbid cognitive function-
ing in patients with schizophrenia through examination of standardized
scholastic test performance. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 159,
1183–1189. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.159.7.1183
Fyfe, E. R., Matz, L. E., Hunt, K. M., & Alibali, M. W. (2019). Mathe-
matical thinking in children with developmental language disorder: The
roles of pattern skills and verbal working memory. Journal of Commu-
nication Disorders, 77, 17–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2018
.11.001
Gardner-Neblett, N., DeCoster, J., & Hamre, B. K. (2014). Linking
preschool language and sustained attention with adolescent achievement
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
29
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
through classroom self-reliance. Journal of Applied Developmental Psy-
chology, 35, 457–467. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2014.09.003
Garrett, H. E. (1930). A study of the CAVD intelligence examination. The
Journal of Educational Research, 21, 103–108. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1080/00220671.1930.10880021
Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Willis, C., & Adams, A. M. (2006).
Working memory in children with reading disabilities. Journal of Ex-
perimental Child Psychology, 93, 265–281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.jecp.2005.08.003
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits
in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of
Memory and Language, 29, 336–360. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0749-
596X(90)90004-J
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Phonological working mem-
ory: A critical building block for reading development and vocabulary
acquisition? European Journal of Psychology of Education, 8, 259–272.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF03174081
Gathercole, S. E., Tiffany, C., Briscoe, J., & Thorn, A. (2005). Develop-
mental consequences of poor phonological short-term memory function
in childhood: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 46, 598611. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004
.00379.x
Geary, D. C. (1993). Mathematical disabilities: Cognitive, neuropsycho-
logical, and genetic components. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 345–362.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.2.345
Geary, D. C. (1994). Children’s mathematical development: Research and
practical applications (1st ed.). Washington, DC: American Psycholog-
ical Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10163-000
Geary, D. C. (2004). Mathematics and learning disabilities. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 37, 4–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221940
40370010201
Geary, D. C. (2011). Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in
mathematics: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology,
47, 1539–1552. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025510
Geary, D. C., Bow-Thomas, C. C., Liu, F., & Siegler, R. S. (1996).
Development of arithmetical competencies in Chinese and American
children: Influence of age, language, and schooling. Child Development,
67, 2022–2044. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131607
Geary, D. C., Nicholas, A., Li, Y., & Sun, J. (2017). Developmental change
in the influence of domain-general abilities and domain-specific knowl-
edge on mathematics achievement: An eight-year longitudinal study.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 109, 680693. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/edu0000159
Geary, D. C., & Widaman, K. F. (1992). Numerical cognition: On the
convergence of componential and psychometric models. Intelligence,
16, 47–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(92)90025-M
Georgiou, G. K., Tziraki, N., Manolitsis, G., & Fella, A. (2013). Is rapid
automatized naming related to reading and mathematics for the same
reason(s)? A follow-up study from kindergarten to Grade 1. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 481–496. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.jecp.2013.01.004
Georgiou, G. K., Wei, W., Inoue, T., & Deng, C. (in press). Are the
Relations of RAN with Reading and Mathematics Accuracy and Flu-
ency. Journal of Educational Psychology.
Gersten, R., Chard, D. J., Jayanthi, M., Baker, S. K., Morphy, P., & Flojo,
J. (2009). Mathematics instruction for students with learning disabilities:
A meta-analysis of instructional components. Review of Educational
Research, 79, 1202–1242. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654309334431
Ghaleb, M. S. (1992). Performance and solution strategies of Arabic-
speaking second-graders in simple addition and subtraction word prob-
lems and relations of performance to their degree of bilingualism
(Doctoral dissertation). University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI.
Gilligan, K. A., Flouri, E., & Farran, E. K. (2017). The contribution of
spatial ability to mathematics achievement in middle childhood. Journal
of Experimental Child Psychology, 163, 107–125. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.jecp.2017.04.016
Gleason, J. B., & Ratner, N. B. (2005). The development of language. 6th.
Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Gold, D. P., Andres, D., Etezadi, J., Arbuckle, T., Schwartzman, A., &
Chaikelson, J. (1995). Structural equation model of intellectual change
and continuity and predictors of intelligence in older men. Psychology
and Aging, 10, 294–303. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.10.2.294
Goldman, R. D., Hudson, D., & Daharsh, B. J. (1973). Self-estimated task
persistence as a nonlinear predictor of college success. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 65, 216–221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
h0034989
Goodwin, J. W., III. (2017). Arithmetical computation and associated
neuropsychological capabilities in children, adolescents, and young
adults with nonsyndromic orofacial clefts (Doctoral dissertation). The
University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA.
Gray, S. A., & Reeve, R. A. (2016). Number-specific and general cogni-
tive markers of preschoolers’ math ability profiles. Journal of Experi-
mental Child Psychology, 147, 1–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp
.2016.02.004
Green, C. T., Bunge, S. A., Briones Chiongbian, V., Barrow, M., & Ferrer,
E. (2017). Fluid reasoning predicts future mathematical performance
among children and adolescents. Journal of Experimental Child Psy-
chology, 157, 125–143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.12.005
Gremillion, M. L., & Martel, M. M. (2012). Semantic language as a
mechanism explaining the association between ADHD symptoms and
reading and mathematics underachievement. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology, 40, 1339–1349. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-
9650-7
Griffin, E. A., & Morrison, F. J. (1997). The unique contribution of home
literacy environment to differences in early literacy skills. Early Child
Development and Care, 127, 233–243. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
0300443971270119
Griffiths, T. L., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2009). Theory-based causal induc-
tion. Psychological Review, 116, 661–716. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0017201
Gubbels, J., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2018). How children’s intel-
lectual profiles relate to their cognitive, socio-emotional, and academic
functioning. High Ability Studies, 29, 149–168. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1080/13598139.2018.1507902
Guo, Y., Piasta, S. B., & Bowles, R. P. (2015). Exploring preschool
children’s science content knowledge. Early Education and Develop-
ment, 26, 125–146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2015.968240
Guthmiller, M. L. (2010). Identifying factors predicting the math compu-
tation and math reasoning performance levels of Latino elementary
school students (Doctoral dissertation). The University of Utah, Salt
Lake City, Utah.
Häidkind, P., Kikas, E., Henno, H., & Peets, T. (2011). Controlled
drawing observation for assessing a child’s readiness for school and
predicting academic achievement at the end of the first grade. Scandi-
navian Journal of Educational Research, 55, 61–78. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1080/00313831.2011.539854
Han, K. S. (2003). Domain-specificity of creativity in young children:
How quantitative and qualitative data support it. The Journal of Creative
Behavior, 37, 117–142. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.2003
.tb00829.x
Hansen, N. M. (2015). Development of fraction knowledge: A longitudinal
study from third through sixth grade (Doctoral dissertation). University
of Delaware, Newark, DE.
Harvey, H. A., & Miller, G. E. (2017). Executive function skills, early
mathematics, and vocabulary in head start preschool children. Early
Education and Development, 28, 290–307. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
10409289.2016.1218728
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
30 PENG ET AL.
Hauser-Cram, P., Durand, T. M., & Warfield, M. E. (2007). Early feelings
about school and later academic outcomes of children with special needs
living in poverty. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 22, 161–172.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2007.02.001
Hawes, Z., Moss, J., Caswell, B., Naqvi, S., & MacKinnon, S. (2017).
Enhancing children’s spatial and numerical skills through a dynamic
spatial approach to early geometry instruction: Effects of a 32-week
intervention. Cognition and Instruction, 35, 236–264. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1080/07370008.2017.1323902
Hecht, S. A., Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2001).
The relations between phonological processing abilities and emerging
individual differences in mathematical computation skills: A longitudi-
nal study from second to fifth grades. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 79, 192–227. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2000.2586
Hedberg, E. C. (2014). ROBUMETA: Stata module to perform robust
variance estimation in meta-regression with dependent effect size esti-
mates. Statistical Software Components. Retrieved from https://ideas
.repec.org/c/boc/bocode/s457219.html
Hedges, L. V., Tipton, E., & Johnson, M. C. (2010). Robust variance
estimation in meta-regression with dependent effect size estimates. Re-
search Synthesis Methods, 1, 3965. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.5
Heikkilä, R., Torppa, M., Aro, M., Närhi, V., & Ahonen, T. (2016).
Double-deficit hypothesis in a clinical sample: Extension beyond read-
ing. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 49, 546–560. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1177/0022219415572895
Henry, L. A., Messer, D. J., & Nash, G. (2012). Executive functioning in
children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychol-
ogy and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 53, 37–45. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02430.x
Hershberger, K. A. (1997). The relationship between conceptually similar
subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised and the Career
Ability Placement Survey with a vocational rehabilitation sample (Doc-
toral dissertation). University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH.)
Heyns, B., & Hilton, T. L. (1982). The cognitive tests for high school and
beyond: An assessment. Sociology of Education, 55, 89–102. http://dx
.doi.org/10.2307/2112290
Hill, M. A. (1981). Variables predicting student achievement on state
mandated competency tests: grant joint union high school district (Doc-
toral dissertation). The University of San Francisco, San Francisco, OH.
Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Academic underachievement, attention deficits, and
aggression: Comorbidity and implications for intervention. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 893–903. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/0022-006X.60.6.893
Hjetland, H. N., Lervåg, A., Lyster, S. A. H., Hagtvet, B. E., Hulme, C., &
Melby-Lervåg, M. (2019). Pathways to reading comprehension: A lon-
gitudinal study from 4 to 9 years of age. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 111, 751–763. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000321
Hollo, A., Wehby, J. H., & Oliver, R. M. (2014). Unidentified language
deficits in children with emotional and behavioral disorders: A meta-
analysis. Exceptional Children, 80, 169–186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
001440291408000203
Hoover, W., & Gough, P. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and
Writing, 2, 127–160. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00401799
Hornburg, C. B., Schmitt, S. A., & Purpura, D. J. (2018). Relations
between preschoolers’ mathematical language understanding and spe-
cific numeracy skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 176,
84–100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.07.005
Horne, Z., Muradoglu, M., & Cimpian, A. (2019). Explanation as a
cognitive process. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23, 187–199. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.12.004
Hornung, C., Martin, R., & Fayol, M. (2017). General and specific
contributions of RAN to reading and arithmetic fluency in first graders:
A longitudinal latent variable approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 8,
1746. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01746
Hornung, C., Schiltz, C., Brunner, M., & Martin, R. (2014). Predicting
first-grade mathematics achievement: The contributions of domain-
general cognitive abilities, nonverbal number sense, and early number
competence. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 272. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/
fpsyg.2014.00272
Hotulainen, R., Mononen, R., & Aunio, P. (2016). Thinking skills inter-
vention for low-achieving first graders. European Journal of Special
Needs Education, 31, 360–375. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08856257
.2016.1141541
Hoy, C. A. (1982). An investigation of certain components of division with
learning disabled and normal six-grade boys (Doctoral dissertation).
Northwestern University, Evanston, IL.
Hu, B. Y., Zhou, Y., Chen, L., Fan, X., & Winsler, A. (2017). Preschool
expenditures and Chinese children’s academic performance: The medi-
ating effect of teacher-child interaction quality. Early Childhood Re-
search Quarterly, 41, 37–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.05
.002
Huang, B., Sivaganesan, S., Succop, P., & Goodman, E. (2004). Statistical
assessment of mediational effects for logistic mediational models. Sta-
tistics in Medicine, 23, 2713–2728. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/sim.1847
Huang, Q., Zhang, X., Liu, Y., Yang, W., & Song, Z. (2017). The
contribution of parent-child numeracy activities to young Chinese chil-
dren’s mathematical ability. The British Journal of Educational Psychol-
ogy, 87, 328–344. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12152
Hur, E., Buettner, C. K., & Jeon, L. (2015). The association between
teachers’ child-centered beliefs and children’s academic achievement:
The indirect effect of children’s behavioral self-regulation. Child Youth
Care Forum, 44, 309–325. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10566-014-
9283-9
Hutton, J. B., & Davenport, M. A. (1985). The WISC-R as a predictor of
Woodcock-Johnson achievement cluster scores for learning-disabled
students. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 410414. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1002/1097-4679(198505)41:3410::AID-JCLP22704103183
.0.CO;2-K
Hyde, J. S., Geiringer, E. R., & Yen, W. M. (1975). On the empirical
relation between spatial ability and sex differences in other aspects of
cognitive performance. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 10, 289–309.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr1003_3
Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2006). Processing limita-
tions in children with specific language impairment: The role of exec-
utive function. Child Development, 77, 1822–1841. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00976.x
Ingram, J., Andrews, N., & Pitt, A. (2016). Patterns of interaction that
encourage student explanations in mathematics lessons. Proceedings of
the British Society for Research into Learning Mathematics, 36, 37–41.
Ingram, J., Andrews, N., & Pitt, A. (2019). When students offer explana-
tions without the teacher explicitly asking them to. Educational Studies
in Mathematics, 101(1), 51–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10649-018-
9873-9
Jap, B. A. J., Borleffs, E., & Maassen, B. A. M. (2017). Towards
identifying dyslexia in Standard Indonesian: The development of a
reading assessment battery. Reading and Writing, 30, 1729–1751. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9748-y
Jensen, A. R., & Reynolds, C. R. (1983). Sex differences on the WISC-R.
Personality and Individual Differences, 4, 223–226. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/0191-8869(83)90029-6
Jerauld, J. (2014). Predicting early academic achievement: an investiga-
tion of the contribution of executive function (Doctoral dissertation). The
University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ.
Jerman, O. Y. (2007). Does growth in working memory predict growth in
crystallized intelligence in children with reading disabilities? (Doctoral
dissertation). University of California, Riverside, CA.
Jordan, J. A., Wylie, J., & Mulhern, G. (2015). Mathematics and reading
difficulty subtypes: Minor phonological influences on mathematics for
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
31
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
5–7-years-old. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 221. http://dx.doi.org/10
.3389/fpsyg.2015.00221
Jordan, N. C., Hansen, N., Fuchs, L. S., Siegler, R. S., Gersten, R., &
Micklos, D. (2013). Developmental predictors of fraction concepts and
procedures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 45–58.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.02.001
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehen-
sion: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review,
99, 122–149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.122
Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., & Conway, A. R. A. (2005). Working
memory capacity and fluid intelligence are strongly related constructs:
Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle (2005). Psychological Bulle-
tin, 131, 66–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.66
Kang, C. (1990). The nature and development of a cognitive screening
battery for academically gifted second and third grade students (Doc-
toral dissertation). University of Wisconsin-Madison, Wisconsin, WI.
Kaufmann, H., & Schmalstieg, D. (2003). Mathematics and geometry
education with collaborative augmented reality. Computers & Graphics,
27, 339–345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0097-8493(03)00028-1
Kazima, M. (2007). Malawian students’ meanings for probability vocab-
ulary. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 64, 169–189. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1007/s10649-006-9032-6
Kempert, S., Saalbach, H., & Hardy, I. (2011). Cognitive benefits and
costs of bilingualism in elementary school students: The case of math-
ematical word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 547–
561. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023619
Kievit, R. A., Lindenberger, U., Goodyer, I. M., Jones, P. B., Fonagy, P.,
Bullmore, E. T.,...theNeuroscience in Psychiatry Network. (2017).
Mutualistic coupling between vocabulary and reasoning supports cog-
nitive development during late adolescence and early adulthood. Psy-
chological Science, 28, 1419–1431. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0956797617710785
Kikas, E., & Jõgi, A. L. (2016). Assessment of learning strategies:
Self-report questionnaire or learning task. European Journal of Psychol-
ogy of Education, 31, 579–593. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10212-015-
0276-3
Kikas, E., Soodla, P., & Mägi, K. (2018). Teacher judgments of student
reading and math skills: Associations with child-and classroom-related
factors. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 62, 783–797.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00313831.2017.1307271
Kim, Y. (1994). The relationship between the performance on Piagetian
tasks in kindergarten and arithmetic achievement in first-grade in Korea
(Doctoral dissertation). Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ.
King, C. M. (2009). Predictive validity of a kindergarten screening tool
(Doctoral dissertation). Fairleigh Dickinson University, Teaneck, NJ.
Kintsch, W., & Greeno, J. G. (1985). Understanding and solving word
arithmetic problems. Psychological Review, 92, 109–129. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/0033-295X.92.1.109
Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance
during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of construc-
tivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teach-
ing. Educational Psychologist, 41, 75–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/
s15326985ep4102_1
Kleczkowski, T. A. (1990). The interrelationships of emotional and
cognitive attributes among junior high students (Doctoral dissertation).
Northern Illinois University, DeKalb, IL.
Kleemans, T., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2011). Cognitive and linguistic
precursors to numeracy in kindergarten: Evidence from first and second
language learners. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 555–561.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.07.008
Kleemans, T., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2012). Naming speed as a
clinical marker in predicting basic calculation skills in children with
specific language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
33, 882–889. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.12.007
Kleemans, T., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2013). Relations between
home numeracy experiences and basic calculation skills of children with
and without specific language impairment. Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 28, 415–423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.10.004
Kleemans, T., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2014). Cognitive and linguis-
tic predictors of basic arithmetic skills: Evidence from first-language and
second-language learners. International Journal of Disability Develop-
ment and Education, 61, 306–316. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1034912X
.2014.934017
Kleemans, T., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2018). Individual differences
in basic arithmetic skills in children with and without developmental
language disorder: Role of home numeracy experiences. Early Child-
hood Research Quarterly, 43, 62–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq
.2018.01.005
Koponen, T., Georgiou, G., Salmi, P., Leskinen, M., & Aro, M. (2017). A
meta-analysis of the relation between RAN and mathematics. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 109, 977–992. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
edu0000182
Koponen, T., Mononen, R., Räsänen, P., & Ahonen, T. (2006). Basic
numeracy in children with specific language impairment: Heterogeneity
and connections to language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Research, 49, 58–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/005)
Krajewski, K., & Schneider, W. (2009). Exploring the impact of phono-
logical awareness, visual-spatial working memory, and preschool
quantity-number competencies on mathematics achievement in elemen-
tary school: Findings from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 516–531. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.jecp.2009.03.009
Kremer, K. P., Flower, A., Huang, J., & Vaughn, M. G. (2016). Behavior
problems and children’s academic achievement: A test of growth-curve
models with gender and racial differences. Children and Youth Services
Review, 67, 95–104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2016.06.003
Kuusinen, J., & Leskinen, E. (1988). Latent structure analysis of longi-
tudinal data on relations between intellectual abilities and school
achievement. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 23, 103–118. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr2301_6
Kvaraceus, W. C., & Lanigan, M. A. (1948). Pupil performance on the
Iowa every-pupil tests of basic skills administered at half-year intervals
in the junior high school. Educational and Psychological Measurement,
8, 91–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001316444800800109
Kyttälä, M., Aunio, P., Lepola, J., & Hautamäki, J. (2014). The role of the
working memory and language skills in the prediction of word problem
solving in 4-to 7-year-old children. Educational Psychology, 34, 674
696. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2013.814192
Lago, R. M. (2007). Examining the psychometrics of number sense among
kindergarten students (Doctoral dissertation). Penn State University,
State College, PA.
Language and Reading Research Consortium. (2015). The dimensionality
of language ability in young children. Child Development, 86, 1948
1965. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12450
Larocque, L. (1999). Preadolescent self-concept and self-concept/
academic achievement relations: Investigating multidimensional and
hierarchical structures within and across gender (Doctoral dissertation).
University of Ottawa, Ottawa, ON, Canada.
Leather, C. V., & Henry, L. A. (1994). Working memory span and
phonological awareness tasks as predictors of early reading ability.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 88–111. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1006/jecp.1994.1027
Lee, K., & Bull, R. (2016). Developmental changes in working memory,
updating, and math achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology,
108, 869882. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000090
LeFevre, J.-A., Fast, L., Skwarchuk, S.-L., Smith-Chant, B. L., Bisanz, J.,
Kamawar, D., & Penner-Wilger, M. (2010). Pathways to mathematics:
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
32 PENG ET AL.
Longitudinal predictors of performance. Child Development, 81, 1753–
1767. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01508.x
LeFevre, J. A., & Liu, J. (1997). The role of experience in numerical skill:
Multiplication performance in adults from Canada and China. Mathe-
matical Cognition, 3, 31–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546799
7387470
LeFevre, J. A., Skwarchuk, S. L., Smith-Chant, B. L., Fast, L., Kamawar,
D., & Bisanz, J. (2009). Home numeracy experiences and children’s
math performance in the early school years. Canadian Journal of Be-
havioural Science, 41, 55–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014532
Lepola, J., Niemi, P., Kuikka, M., & Hannula, M. M. (2005). Cognitive-
linguistic skills and motivation as longitudinal predictors of reading and
arithmetic achievement: A follow-up study from kindergarten to grade 2.
International Journal of Educational Research, 43, 250–271. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2006.06.005
Lervåg, A., Hulme, C., & Melby-Lervåg, M. (2018). Unpicking the devel-
opmental relationship between oral language skills and reading compre-
hension: It’s simple, but complex. Child Development, 89, 1821–1838.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12861
Levy, Y. (2011). IQ predicts word decoding skills in populations with
intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32,
2267–2277. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.07.043
Li, K., Zhang, P., Hu, B. Y., Burchinal, M. R., Fan, X., & Qin, J. (2019).
Testing the ‘thresholds’ of preschool education quality on child out-
comes in China. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 47, 445–456.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.08.003
Li, Y. H., & Tompkins, L. J. (2004). Examining the construct validity for
the multiple-content testing programs. International Journal of Testing,
4, 217–238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327574ijt0403_2
Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2013). Is approximate
number precision a stable predictor of math ability? Learning and
Individual Differences, 25, 126–133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif
.2013.02.001
Libertus, M. E., Odic, D., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2016). The
precision of mapping between number words and the approximate
number system predicts children’s formal math abilities. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 150, 207–226. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.jecp.2016.06.003
Lin, G. C. (2018). The roles of language and executive function on early
mathematics among emergent bilinguals (Doctoral dissertation). Univer-
sity of California, Irvine, CA.
Lindgren, S. D. (1978). Finger localization and the prediction of reading
disability. Cortex, 14, 87–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-
9452(78)80011-2
Lindquist, K. A., MacCormack, J. K., & Shablack, H. (2015). The role of
language in emotion: Predictions from psychological constructionism.
Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 444. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015
.00444
Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand
Oaks, CA: SAGE publications, Inc.
Liu, R. D., Ding, Y., Xu, L., & Wang, J. (2017). Involvement of working
memory in mental multiplication in Chinese elementary students. The
Journal of Educational Research, 110, 380–390. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1080/00220671.2015.1103689
Liu, Y., Lin, D., & Zhang, X. (2016). Morphological awareness longitu-
dinally predicts counting ability in Chinese kindergarteners. Learning
and Individual Differences, 47, 215–221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.lindif.2016.01.007
Lockl, K., Ebert, S., & Weinert, S. (2017). Predicting school achievement
from early theory of mind: Differential effects on achievement tests and
teacher ratings. Learning and Individual Differences, 53, 93–102. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.11.007
Lombrozo, T. (2006). The structure and function of explanations. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 10, 464470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006
.08.004
Long, I., Malone, S. A., Tolan, A., Burgoyne, K., Heron-Delaney, M.,
Witteveen, K., & Hulme, C. (2016). The cognitive foundations of early
arithmetic skills: It is counting and number judgment, but not finger
gnosis, that count. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 152,
327–334. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.08.005
Lopes-Silva, J. B., Moura, R., Júlio-Costa, A., Wood, G., Salles, J. F., &
Haase, V. G. (2016). What is specific and what is shared between
numbers and words? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 22. http://dx.doi.org/
10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00022
Maclean, M. (1987). Rhymes, nursery rhymes, and reading in early
childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 255–281.
Magee, P., & Newcomer, P. (1978). The relationship between oral lan-
guage skills and academic achievement of learning disabled children.
Learning Disability Quarterly, 1, 63–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/
1510965
Majumder, S. (2004). Factors in mathematical word problem solving: The
role of inhibition (Doctoral dissertation). York University, Toronto, ON,
Canada.
Malone, A. S., Loehr, A. M., & Fuchs, L. S. (2017). The role of domain-
general cognitive abilities and decimal labels in at-risk fourth-grade
students’ decimal magnitude understanding. Learning and Individual
Differences, 58, 90–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.05.007
Männamaa, M., Kikas, E., Peets, K., & Palu, A. (2012). Cognitive
correlates of math skills in third-grade students. Educational Psychol-
ogy, 32, 21–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2011.621713
Martin, B. N., & Fuchs, L. S. (2019). The mathematical performance of
at-risk first graders as a function of limited English proficiency status.
Learning Disability Quarterly, 42, 244–251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0731948719827489
Martin, R. B., Cirino, P. T., Sharp, C., & Barnes, M. (2014). Number and
counting skills in kindergarten as predictors of grade 1 mathematical
skills. Learning and Individual Differences, 34, 12–23. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.lindif.2014.05.006
Marton, K. (2008). Visuo-spatial processing and executive functions in
children with specific language impairment. International Journal of
Language & Communication Disorders, 43, 181–200. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1080/16066350701340719
Marton, K., & Schwartz, R. G. (2003). Working memory capacity and
language processes in children with specific language impairment. Jour-
nal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1138–1153. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2003/089)
Mata, A., Schubert, A. L., & Ferreira, M. B. (2014). The role of
language comprehension in reasoning: How “good-enough” repre-
sentations induce biases. Cognition, 133, 457–463. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.cognition.2014.07.011
Matthews, J. S., Ponitz, C. C., & Morrison, F. J. (2009). Early gender
differences in self-regulation and academic achievement. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 101, 689–704. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0014240
Mayo-Booker, T. E. (1995). Racial and global self-concept as they relate
to academic achievement in African Americans (Doctoral dissertation).
Fairleigh Dickinson University, Teaneck, NJ.
McArdle, J. J., Hamagami, F., Meredith, W., & Bradway, K. P. (2000).
Modeling the dynamic hypotheses of Gf–Gc theory using longitudinal
life-span data. Learning and Individual Differences, 12, 53–79. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1041-6080(00)00036-4
McClelland, M. M., Acock, A. C., Piccinin, A., Rhea, S. A., & Stallings,
M. C. (2013). Relations between preschool attention span-persistence
and age 25 educational outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
28, 314–324. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.07.008
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
33
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Connor, C. M., Farris, C. L., Jewkes,
A. M., & Morrison, F. J. (2007). Links between behavioral regulation
and preschoolers’ literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental
Psychology, 43, 947–959. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.947
McDevitt, T. M., Hess, R. D., Kashiwagi, K., Dickson, W. P., Miyake, N.,
& Azuma, H. (1987). Referential communication accuracy of mother-
child pairs and children’s later scholastic achievement: A follow-up
study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 171–186.
McEntire, M. E. (1981). Relationships between the language proficiency
of adolescents and their mathematics performance (Doctoral disserta-
tion). The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX.
McShane, D., & Plus, J. M. (1988). The relationship of intellectual and
psycholinguistic abilities to the achievement gains of American Indian
children. Canadian Journal of Native Education, 15, 6686.
Mellard, D. F., Woods, K. L., & McJunkin, L. (2015). Literacy compo-
nents model for at-risk young adults enrolled in career and technical
education. Journal of Research in Reading, 38, 249–271. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12014
Menon, V. (2016). Working memory in children’s math learning and its
disruption in dyscalculia. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 10,
125–132. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.05.014
Merz, E. C., Zucker, T. A., Landry, S. H., Williams, J. M., Assel, M.,
Taylor, H. B.,...theSchool Readiness Research Consortium. (2015).
Parenting predictors of cognitive skills and emotion knowledge in so-
cioeconomically disadvantaged preschoolers. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 132, 14–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.11
.010
Mestre, J. P. (1981). Predicting academic achievement among bilingual
Hispanic college technical students. Educational and Psychological
Measurement, 41, 1255–1264. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001316448
104100433
Meuwissen, A. S., & Carlson, S. M. (2018). The role of father parenting
in children’s school readiness: A longitudinal follow-up. Journal of
Family Psychology, 32, 588–598. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
fam0000418
Michael, R. (2003). Children’s cognitive skill development in Britain and
the United States. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27,
396408. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01650250344000000
Michalczyk, K., Krajewski, K., Preler, A. L., & Hasselhorn, M. (2013).
The relationships between quantity-number competencies, working
memory, and phonological awareness in 5- and 6-year-olds. British
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31, 408424. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1111/bjdp.12016
Miles, D. D. (1991). Algebra vocabulary: Domain-specific language as a
predictor of algebra skills (Doctoral dissertation). The University of
Connecticut, Mansfield, CN.
Miller, C. A., Kail, R., Leonard, L. B., & Tomblin, J. B. (2001). Speed of
processing in children with specific language impairment. Journal of
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 416433. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/034)
Miller, K. F., Smith, C. M., Zhu, J., & Zhang, H. (1995). Preschool origins
of cross-national differences in mathematical competence: The role of
number-naming systems. Psychological Science, 6, 5660. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00305.x
Miller, M. R., Müller, U., Giesbrecht, G. F., Carpendale, J. I., & Kerns,
K. A. (2013). The contribution of executive function and social under-
standing to preschoolers’ letter and math skills. Cognitive Development,
28, 331–349. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.10.005
Miller-Cotto, D., & Byrnes, J. P. (2019). What’s the best way to charac-
terize the relationship between working memory and achievement?: An
initial examination of competing theories. Journal of Educational Psy-
chology. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
edu0000395
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of
individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8–14. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1177/0963721411429458
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A.,
& Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions
and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable
analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/
cogp.1999.0734
Mokrova, I. L., O’Brien, M., Calkins, S. D., Leerkes, E. M., & Marco-
vitch, S. (2013). The role of persistence at preschool age in academic
skills at kindergarten. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 28,
1495–1503. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10212-013-0177-2
Moll, K., Snowling, M. J., Göbel, S. M., & Hulme, C. (2015). Early
language and executive skills predict variations in number and arithme-
tic skills in children at family-risk of dyslexia and typically developing
controls. Learning and Instruction, 38, 53–62. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.03.004
Montague, M. (1992). The effects of cognitive and metacognitive strategy
instruction on the mathematical problem solving of middle school stu-
dents with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25,
230–248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221949202500404
Montgomery, J. W., Magimairaj, B. M., & Finney, M. C. (2010). Working
memory and specific language impairment: An update on the relation
and perspectives on assessment and treatment. American Journal of
Speech-Language Pathology, 19, 78–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/
1058-0360(2009/09-0028)
Montgomery, W. (2001). Creating culturally responsive, inclusive class-
rooms. Teaching Exceptional Children, 33, 4–9. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1177/004005990103300401
Morgan, J. E., Loo, S. K., & Lee, S. S. (2018). Neurocognitive functioning
mediates the prospective association of birth weight with youth ADHD
symptoms. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 47,
727–736. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2016.1183498
Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., Hillemeier, M. M., Mattison, R., Maczuga, S.,
Li, H., & Cook, M. (2015). Minorities are disproportionately underrep-
resented in special education: Longitudinal evidence across five disabil-
ity conditions. Educational Researcher, 44, 278–292. http://dx.doi.org/
10.3102/0013189X15591157
Moschkovich, J. (2002). A situated and sociocultural perspective on bilin-
gual mathematics learners. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 4,
189–212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327833MTL04023_5
Mulder, H., Verhagen, J., Van der Ven, S. H. G., Slot, P. L., & Leseman,
P. P. M. (2017). Early executive function at age two predicts emergent
mathematics and literacy at age five. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1706.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01706
Mullins, J. E. (1999). A comparison of performance of students referred
for gifted evaluation on the WISC-III and Binet IV (Doctoral disserta-
tion). West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV.
Munford, P. R. (1978). A comparison of the WISC and WISC-R on black
child psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34, 938
942. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(197810)34:4938::AID-
JCLP22703404233.0.CO;2-A
Naglieri, J. A., & Pfeiffer, S. I. (1981). Correlations among scores on
WISC-R and Woodcock-Johnson Achievement Tests for learning dis-
abled children. Psychological Reports, 49, 913–914. http://dx.doi.org/10
.2466/pr0.1981.49.3.913
Namkung, J. M., & Fuchs, L. S. (2016). Cognitive predictors of calcula-
tions and number line estimation with whole numbers and fractions
among at-risk students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108, 214
228. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000055
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2006). Curriculum focal
points for prekindergarten through grade 8 mathematics: A quest for
coherence. Reston, VA: Author.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
34 PENG ET AL.
National Reading Panel. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel:
Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scien-
tific research literature on reading and its implications for reading
instruction: Reports of the subgroups. Bethesda, MD: National Institute
of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health.
Navarro, J. I., Aguilar, M., Alcalde, C., Ruiz, G., Marchena, E., &
Menacho, I. (2011). Inhibitory processes, working memory, phonolog-
ical awareness, naming speed, and early arithmetic achievement. The
Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14, 580–588. http://dx.doi.org/10.5209/
rev_SJOP.2011.v14.n2.6
Navarro, M. G., Braham, E. J., & Libertus, M. E. (2018). Intergenerational
associations of the approximate number system in toddlers and their
parents. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 36, 521–539.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12234
Nesbitt, K. T., Farran, D. C., & Fuhs, M. W. (2015). Executive function
skills and academic achievement gains in prekindergarten: Contributions
of learning-related behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 51, 865–878.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000021
Neuman, G. A., Bolin, A. U., & Briggs, T. E. (2000). Identifying general
factors of intelligence: A confirmatory factor analysis of the ball aptitude
battery. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 60, 697–712.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00131640021970853
Newman, S. D. (2016). Does finger sense predict addition performance?
Cognitive Processing, 17, 139–146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10339-
016-0756-7
Newton, J. H., & McGrew, K. S. (2010). Introduction to the special issue:
Current research in Cattell–Horn–Carroll–based assessment. Psychol-
ogy in the Schools, 47, 621–634. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pits.20495
Niklas, F., & Schneider, W. (2013). Casting the die before the die is cast:
The importance of the home numeracy environment for preschool chil-
dren. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 29, 327–345.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10212-013-0201-6
Norbury, C. F., Gooch, D., Wray, C., Baird, G., Charman, T., Simonoff, E.,
. . . Pickles, A. (2016). The impact of nonverbal ability on prevalence
and clinical presentation of language disorder: Evidence from a popu-
lation study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied
Disciplines, 57, 1247–1257. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12573
Novak, P. A., Tsushima, W. T., & Tsushima, M. M. (1991). Predictive
validity of two short-forms of the WPPSI: A 3-year follow-up study.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47, 698–702. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
1097-4679(199109)47:5698::AID-JCLP22704705113.0.CO;2-Q
O’Halloran, K. L. (2005). Mathematical discourse: Language, symbolism
and visual images. New York, NY: Continuum.
Olexa, L. W. (2001). Language ability scores at kindergarten entrance as
a predictor of later academic performance (Doctoral dissertation). Wal-
den University, Minneapolis, MN.
O’Neill, D. K., Pearce, M. J., & Pick, J. L. (2004). Preschool children’s
narratives and performance on the Peabody Individualized Achievement
Test–Revised: Evidence of a relation between early narrative and later
mathematical ability. First Language, 24, 149–183. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1177/0142723704043529
O’Shaughnessy, T. E. (1997). A comparison of two reading interventions
for children with or at-risk for reading disabilities (Doctoral disserta-
tion). University of California, Riverside, CA.
O’Shea, L. M. (2014). Predictive validity of reading and mathematics
curriculum-based measures on mathematics performance at third grade
(Doctoral dissertation). University of Oregon, Eugene, OR.
Östergren, R., & Träff, U. (2013). Early number knowledge and cognitive
ability affect early arithmetic ability. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 115, 405–421. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.03.007
Pace, A., Alper, R., Burchinal, M. R., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek,
K. (2019). 2018). Measuring success: Within and cross-domain predic-
tors of academic and social trajectories in elementary school. Early
Childhood Research Quarterly, 46, 112–125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.ecresq.2018.04.001
Pagani, L. S., Fitzpatrick, C., Archambault, I., & Janosz, M. (2010).
School readiness and later achievement: A French Canadian replication
and extension. Developmental Psychology, 46, 984–994. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/a0018881
Palmer, S. (2000). Development of phonological recoding and literacy
acquisition: A four-year cross-sequential study. British Journal of De-
velopmental Psychology, 18, 533–555. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/
026151000165841
Panizo, M. I. (1991). English language proficiency as a moderator vari-
able in the prediction of academic achievement of Hispanic students
(Doctoral dissertation). University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL.
Parlato, V., Lopez, O., Panisset, M., Iavarone, A., Grafman, J., & Boller,
F. (1992). Mental calculation in mild Alzheimer’s-disease: A pilot-
study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 7, 599602. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1002/gps.930070810
Passolunghi, M. C., & Lanfranchi, S. (2012). Domain-specific and
domain-general precursors of mathematical achievement: A longitudinal
study from kindergarten to first grade. The British Journal of Educa-
tional Psychology, 82, 42–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279
.2011.02039.x
Passolunghi, M. C., Lanfranchi, S., Altoè, G., & Sollazzo, N. (2015).
Early numerical abilities and cognitive skills in kindergarten children.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 135, 25–42. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.02.001
Passolunghi, M. C., Vercelloni, B., & Schadee, H. (2007). The precursors
of mathematics learning: Working memory, phonological ability and
numerical competence. Cognitive Development, 22, 165–184. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.09.001
Pauly, H., Linkersdörfer, J., Lindberg, S., Woerner, W., Hasselhorn, M.,
& Lonnemann, J. (2011). Domain-specific rapid automatized naming
deficits in children at risk for learning disabilities. Journal of Neurolin-
guistics, 24, 602–610. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2011.02
.002
Pawk, M. L. (2003). A quantitative analysis of full-day kindergarten in
rural elementary schools: A measure of academic and social outcomes
for children at-risk in the arm strong school district (Doctoral disserta-
tion). Duquesne University, Pittsburgh, PA.
Peng, P., & Kievit, R. (2020). The development of academic achievement
and cognitive abilities: A bidirectional perspective. Child Development
Perspectives, 14, 15–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12352
Peng, P., & Lin, X. (2019). The relation between mathematics vocabulary
and mathematics performance among fourth graders. Learning and
Individual Differences, 69, 11–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif
.2018.11.006
Peng, P., Namkung, J., Barnes, M., & Sun, C. (2016). A meta-analysis of
mathematics and working memory: Moderating effects of working
memory domain, type of mathematics skill, and sample characteristics.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 108, 455–473. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/edu0000079
Peng, P., Namkung, J. M., Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., Patton, S., Yen, L.,...
Hamlett, C. (2016). A longitudinal study on predictors of early calcula-
tion development among young children at risk for learning difficulties.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 152, 221–241. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.07.017
Peng, P., Wang, C., & Namkung, J. (2018). Understanding the cognition
related to mathematics difficulties: A meta-analysis on the cognitive
deficit profiles and the bottleneck theory. Review of Educational Re-
search, 88, 434476. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654317753350
Peng, P., Wang, T., Wang, C., & Lin, X. (2019). A meta-analysis on the
relation between fluid intelligence and reading/mathematics: Effects of
tasks, age, and social economics status. Psychological Bulletin, 145,
189–236. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bul0000182
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
35
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
Pennequin, V., Sorel, O., Nanty, I., & Fontaine, R. (2010). Metacognition
and low achievement in mathematics: The effect of training in the use of
metacognitive skills to solve mathematical word problems. Thinking &
Reasoning, 16, 198–220. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2010
.509052
Phillips, C. J., & Marvelly, A. (1984). Basic educational skills of Asian
children at the end of infant schooling. Educational Research, 26,
3645. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0013188840260107
Pimm, D. (1987). Speaking mathematically: Communication in mathemat-
ics classrooms. London, UK: Routledge & K. Paul. http://dx.doi.org/10
.4324/9781315278858
Pina, V., Fuentes, L. J., Castillo, A., & Diamantopoulou, S. (2014).
Disentangling the effects of working memory, language, parental edu-
cation, and non-verbal intelligence on children’s mathematical abilities.
Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 415. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014
.00415
Pind, J., Gunnarsdóttir, E. K., & Jóhannesson, H. S. (2003). Raven’s
standard progressive matrices: New school age norms and a study of the
test’s validity. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 375–386.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00058-2
Post, P. C. (1999). The usefulness of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for
Children-Third Edition in the diagnosis of attentional disorders in a
special education population at the secondary level (Doctoral disserta-
tion). Adler School of Professional Psychology, Chicago, IL.
Powell, S. R., Cirino, P. T., & Malone, A. S. (2017). Child-level predic-
tors of responsiveness to evidence-based mathematics intervention. Ex-
ceptional Children, 83, 359–377. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0014402917690728
Powell, S. R., Driver, M. K., Roberts, G., & Fall, A. (2017). An analysis
of the mathematics vocabulary knowledge of third- and fifth-grade
students: Connections to general vocabulary and mathematics computa-
tion. Learning and Individual Differences, 57, 22–32. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.lindif.2017.05.011
Powell, S. R., & Fuchs, L. S. (2018). Effective word-problem instruction:
Using schemas to facilitate mathematical reasoning. Teaching Excep-
tional Children, 51, 31–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/004005991
8777250
Praet, M., Titeca, D., Ceulemans, A., & Desoete, A. (2013). Language in
the prediction of arithmetics in kindergarten and grade 1. Learning and
Individual Differences, 27, 90–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif
.2013.07.003
Preacher, K. J., & Kelley, K. (2011). Effect size measures for mediation
models: Quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psy-
chological Methods, 16, 93–115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022658
Purpura, D. J., Hume, L. E., Sims, D. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2011). Early
literacy and early numeracy: The value of including early literacy skills
in the prediction of numeracy development. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 110, 647–658. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011
.07.004
Purpura, D. J., & Logan, J. A. R. (2015). The nonlinear relations of the
approximate number system and mathematical language to early math-
ematics development. Developmental Psychology, 51, 1717–1724.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000055
Purpura, D. J., Logan, J. A. R., Hassinger-Das, B., & Napoli, A. R. (2017).
Why do early mathematics skills predict later reading? The role of
mathematical language. Developmental Psychology, 53, 1633–1642.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000375
Purpura, D. J., & Napoli, A. R. (2015). Early numeracy and literacy:
Untangling the relation between specific components. Mathematical
Thinking and Learning, 17, 197–218. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
10986065.2015.1016817
Purpura, D. J., Napoli, A. R., Wehrspann, E. A., & Gold, Z. S. (2017).
Causal connections between mathematical language and mathematical
knowledge: A dialogic reading intervention. Journal of Research on
Educational Effectiveness, 10, 116–137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
19345747.2016.1204639
Purpura, D. J., & Reid, E. E. (2016). Mathematics and language: Individ-
ual and group differences in mathematical language skills in young
children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 259–268. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.12.020
Purpura, D. J., Schmitt, S. A., & Ganley, C. M. (2017). Foundations of
mathematics and literacy: The role of executive functioning components.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 153, 15–34. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.08.010
Raghubar, K. P., Barnes, M. A., & Hecht, S. A. (2010). Working memory
and mathematics: A review of developmental, individual difference, and
cognitive approaches. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 110
122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2009.10.005
Reid, G. A. (2000). Examining standardized and psychometric potential
assessment and acculturation of Jamaican immigrant children (Doctoral
dissertation). Howard University, Washington, DC.
Ren, L., Zhang, X., Yang, W., & Song, Z. (2018). Relations among
parenting, child behavioral regulation and early competencies: A study
on Chinese preschoolers. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 27,
639652. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10826-017-0898-y
Reuhkala, M. (2001). Mathematical skills in ninth-graders: Relationship
with visuo-spatial abilities and working memory. Educational Psychol-
ogy, 21, 387–399. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01443410120090786
Rey-Mermet, A., Gade, M., Souza, A. S., von Bastian, C. C., & Oberauer,
K. (2019). Is executive control related to working memory capacity and
fluid intelligence? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
148(8), 1335–1372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000593
Reynolds, C. R. (1983). Regression analyses of race and sex bias in seven
preschool tests. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 1, 169–178.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/073428298300100208
Reynolds, E., Vernon-Feagans, L., Bratsch-Hines, M., & Baker, C. E.
(2019). Mothers’ and fathers’ language input from 6 to 36 months in
rural two-parent-families: Relations to children’s kindergarten achieve-
ment. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 47, 385–395. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.09.002
Rhemtulla, M., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2011). Correlated longitudinal
changes across linguistic, achievement, and psychomotor domains in
early childhood: Evidence for a global dimension of development.
Developmental Science, 14, 1245–1254. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j
.1467-7687.2011.01071.x
Rindermann, H., Flores-Mendoza, C., & Mansur-Alves, M. (2010). Recip-
rocal effects between fluid and crystallized intelligence and their depen-
dence on parents’ socioeconomic status and education. Learning and
Individual Differences, 20, 544–548. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif
.2010.07.002
Rios, J. A. (2011). Psychometric evaluation of the listening sentence span
task: A working memory measure for English language learners (Doc-
toral dissertation). University of California, Riverside, CA.
Rispens, J. E., McBride-Chang, C., & Reitsma, P. (2008). Morphological
awareness and early and advanced word recognition and spelling in
Dutch. Reading and Writing, 21, 587–607. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s11145-007-9077-7
Rittle-Johnson, B. (2006). Promoting transfer: Effects of self-explanation
and direct instruction. Child Development, 77, 1–15. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00852.x
Roberts, J. E., Burchinal, M. R., & Zeisel, S. A. (2002). Otitis media in
early childhood in relation to children’s school-age language and aca-
demic skills. Pediatrics, 110, 696–706. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds
.110.4.696
Roditi, B. N. (1988). Automaticity, cognitive flexibility, and mathematics:
A longitudinal study of children with and without learning disabilities
(Doctoral dissertation). Tufts University Middlesex, MA.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
36 PENG ET AL.
Rohde, T. E., & Thompson, L. A. (2007). Predicting academic achieve-
ment with cognitive ability. Intelligence, 35, 83–92. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.intell.2006.05.004
Romano, E., Babchishin, L., Pagani, L. S., & Kohen, D. (2010). School
readiness and later achievement: Replication and extension using a
nationwide Canadian survey. Developmental Psychology, 46, 995–1007.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018880
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Arvizu, L., Kretzmer, T., Standish, V., & Lieber-
mann, J. (1998). Arithmetical abilities in Alzheimer disease. The Inter-
national Journal of Neuroscience, 96, 141–148. http://dx.doi.org/10
.3109/00207459808986463
Rubinstein-Ávila, E., Sox, A. A., Kaplan, S., & McGraw, R. (2015).
Does biliteracy mathematical discourse binumerate develop-
ment? Language use in a middle school dual-language mathematics
classroom. Urban Education, 50, 899–937. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1177/0042085914536997
Ryan, J. J., Paolo, A. M., & Brungardt, T. M. (1990). Factor analysis of
the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-revised for persons 75 years and
older. Professional Psychology, Research and Practice, 21, 177–181.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.21.3.177
Sarant, J. Z., Harris, D. C., & Bennet, L. A. (2015). Academic outcomes
for school-aged children with severe-profound hearing loss and early
unilateral and bilateral cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language,
and Hearing Research, 58, 1017–1032. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/
2015_JSLHR-H-14-0075
Schleppegrell, M. J. (2007). The linguistic challenges of mathematics
teaching and learning: A research review. Reading & Writing Quarterly,
23, 139–159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10573560601158461
Schmitt, S. A., McClelland, M. M., Tominey, S. L., & Acock, A. C.
(2015). Strengthening school readiness for head start children: Evalua-
tion of a self-regulation intervention. Early Childhood Research Quar-
terly, 30, 20–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.08.001
Schult, J., & Sparfeldt, J. R. (2016). Do non-g factors of cognitive ability
tests align with specific academic achievements? A combined bifactor
modeling approach. Intelligence, 59, 96–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.intell.2016.08.004
Schwenck, C., Dummert, F., Endlich, D., & Schneider, W. (2015). Cog-
nitive functioning in children with learning problems. European Journal
of Psychology of Education, 30, 349–367. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s10212-014-0242-5
Seethaler, P. M., & Fuchs, L. S. (2006). The cognitive correlates of
computational estimation skill among third-grade students. Learning
Disabilities Research & Practice, 21, 233–243. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/j.1540-5826.2006.00220.x
Seethaler, P. M., Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Compton, D. L. (2012).
Predicting first graders’ development of calculation versus word-
problem performance: The role of dynamic assessment. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 104, 224–234. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0024968
Seethaler, P. M., Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Compton, D. L. (2016). Does
the value of dynamic assessment in predicting end-of-first-grade math-
ematics performance differ as a function of English language profi-
ciency? The Elementary School Journal, 117, 171–191. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1086/688870
Seethaler, P. M., Fuchs, L. S., Star, J. R., & Bryant, J. (2011). The
cognitive predictors of computational skill with whole versus rational
numbers: An exploratory study. Learning and Individual Differences,
21, 536–542. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.05.002
Sektnan, M., McClelland, M. M., Acock, A., & Morrison, F. J. (2010).
Relations between early family risk, children’s behavioral regulation,
and academic achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25,
464479. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.02.005
Sewell, R. D. (2008). Cognitive and behavioral correlates in mathemat-
ical ability subtypes (Doctoral dissertation). University of California,
Riverside, CA.
Sfard, A. (2008). Thinking as communicating: Human development, the
growth of discourses, and mathematizing. New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511499944
Shahaeian, A., Wang, C., Tucker-Drob, E., Geiger, V., Bus, A. G., &
Harrison, L. J. (2018). Early shared reading, socioeconomic status, and
children’s cognitive and school competencies: Six years of longitudinal
evidence. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22, 485–502. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1080/10888438.2018.1482901
Shankweiler, D., Crain, S., Katz, L., Fowler, A. E., Liberman, A. M.,
Brady, S. A.,...Shaywitz, B. A. (1995). Cognitive profiles of reading-
disabled children: Comparison of language skills in phonology, mor-
phology, and syntax. Psychological Science, 6, 149–156. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00324.x
Shaul, S., & Schwartz, M. (2014). The role of the executive functions in
school readiness among preschool-age children. Reading and Writing,
27, 749–768. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9470-3
Shaw, R. R. (2007). The relationship between pediatric HIV infection,
CD4 percentage, HAART, and WISC-III performance (Doctoral disser-
tation). Columbia University, New York City, NY.
Sholl, M. J., & Egeth, H. E. (1982). Cognitive correlates of map-reading
ability. Intelligence, 6, 215–230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0160-
2896(82)90015-0
Sidney, P. G., Hattikudur, S., & Alibali, M. W. (2015). How do contrasting
cases and self-explanation promote learning? evidence from fraction
division. Learning and Instruction, 40, 29–38. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.07.006
Simmons, F., Singleton, C., & Horne, J. (2008). Brief report-phonological
awareness and visual-spatial sketchpad functioning predict early arith-
metic attainment: Evidence from a longitudinal study. The European
Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 20, 711–722. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1080/09541440701614922
Simmons, F. R., Willis, C., & Adams, A. M. (2012). Different compo-
nents of working memory have different relationships with different
mathematical skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111,
139–155. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.08.011
Singer, V., Strasser, K., & Cuadro, A. (2019). Direct and indirect paths
from linguistic skills to arithmetic school performance. Journal of Ed-
ucational Psychology, 111, 434445. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
edu0000290
Skwarchuk, S. L., Sowinski, C., & LeFevre, J. A. (2014). Formal and
informal home learning activities in relation to children’s early nu-
meracy and literacy skills: The development of a home numeracy model.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 121, 63–84. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.11.006
Slate, J. R. (1995). Two investigations of the validity of the WISC-III.
Psychological Reports, 76, 299–306. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1995
.76.1.299
Slot, E. M., van Viersen, S., de Bree, E. H., & Kroesbergen, E. H. (2016).
Shared and unique risk factors underlying mathematical disability and
reading and spelling disability. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 803. http://
dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00803
Smiley, D. F. (2002). Problem solving ability in elementary school age
children with hearing impairment (Doctoral dissertation). University of
Tennessee, Knoxville, TN.
Smith, T. C., Smith, B. L., & Dobbs, K. (1991). Relationship between the
Peabody Picture Vocabulary test-revised, wide range achievement test-
revised, and Weschler Intelligence Scale for children-revised. Journal of
School Psychology, 29, 53–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-
4405(91)90015-J
Son, S. C., Kwon, K., Jeon, H., & Hong, S. (2013). Head start classrooms
and children’s school readiness benefit from teachers’ qualifications and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
37
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
ongoing training. Child and Youth Care Forum, 42, 525–553. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1007/s10566-013-9213-2
Sorhagen, N. S. (2013). Early teacher expectations disproportionately
affect poor children’s high school performance. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 105, 465–477. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031754
Soto-Calvo, E., Simmons, F. R., Willis, C., & Adams, A. M. (2015).
Identifying the cognitive predictors of early counting and calculation
skills: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 140, 16–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06
.011
Sowinski, C., LeFevre, J. A., Skwarchuk, S. L., Kamawar, D., Bisanz, J.,
& Smith-Chant, B. (2015). Refining the quantitative pathway of the
Pathways to Mathematics model. Journal of Experimental Child Psy-
chology, 131, 73–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.11.004
Spelke, E. S. (2017). Core knowledge, language, and number. Language
Learning and Development, 13, 147–170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
15475441.2016.1263572
Spelke, E. S., & Tsivkin, S. (2001). Language and number: A bilingual
training study. Cognition, 78, 45–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-
0277(00)00108-6
Speybroeck, S., Kuppens, S., Van Damme, J., Van Petegem, P., Lamote,
C., Boonen, T., & de Bilde, J. (2012). The role of teachers’ expectations
in the association between children’s SES and performance in kinder-
garten: A moderated mediation analysis. PLoS ONE, 7, e34502. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034502
Srinivasan, R. (1991). A cross-cultural study of subtypes of learning
disabilities in multilingual children residing in Lidia (Doctoral disser-
tation). California School of Professional Psychology, Berkeley/
Alameda, CA.
Stein, M. K., Grover, B. W., & Henningsen, M. (1996). Building student
capacity for mathematical thinking and reasoning: An analysis of math-
ematical tasks used in reform classrooms. American Educational Re-
search Journal, 33, 455–488. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00028312
033002455
Stevenson, H. W., & Newman, R. S. (1986). Long-term prediction of
achievement and attitudes in mathematics and reading. Child Develop-
ment, 57, 646659. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1130343
Sun, J., Zhang, L., Chen, E., Lau, C., & Rao, N. (2018). Preschool
attendance and executive function mediate early academic achievement
gaps in East Asia and the Pacific. Early Education and Development, 29,
1039–1060. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2018.1498999
Super, D. E. (1940). The A. C. E. psychological examination and special
abilities. The Journal of Psychology, 10, 221–226. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1080/00223980.1940.9917688
Susperreguy Jorquera, M. I. (2013). “Math talk” in families of preschool-
aged children: Frequency and relations to children’s early math skills
across time (Doctoral dissertation). The University of Michigan, Ann
Arbor, MI.
Swanson, H. L. (2004). Working memory and phonological processing as
predictors of children’s mathematical problem solving at different ages.
Memory & Cognition, 32, 648661. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/
BF03195856
Swanson, H. L. (2006). Cognitive processes that underlie mathematical
precociousness in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psy-
chology, 93, 239–264. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2005.09.006
Swanson, H. L. (2011). Working memory, attention, and mathematical
problem solving: A longitudinal study of elementary school children.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 821–837. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/a0025114
Swanson, H. L., & Beebe-Frankenberger, M. (2004). The relationship
between working memory and mathematical problem solving in children
at risk and not at risk for serious math difficulties. Journal of Educa-
tional Psychology, 96, 471–491. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663
.96.3.471
Swanson, H. L., & Fung, W. (2016). Working memory components and
problem-solving accuracy: Are there multiple pathways? Journal of
Educational Psychology, 108, 1153–1177. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
edu0000116
Swanson, H. L., Jerman, O., & Zheng, X. (2008). Growth in working
memory and mathematical problem solving in children at risk and not at
risk for serious math difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology,
100, 343–379. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.343
Swanson, H. L., Kong, J., & Petcu, S. (2018). Math difficulties and
working memory growth in English language learner children: Does
bilingual proficiency play a significant role? Language, Speech, and
Hearing Services in Schools, 49, 379–394. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/
2018_LSHSS-17-0098
Swanson, H. L., & Sachse-Lee, C. (2001). Mathematical problem solving
and working memory in children with learning disabilities: Both exec-
utive and phonological processes are important. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 79, 294–321. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2000
.2587
Swanson, L., & Kim, K. (2007). Working memory, short-term memory,
and naming speed as predictors of children’s mathematical performance.
Intelligence, 35, 151–168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.001
Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instruc-
tional design. Learning and Instruction, 4, 295–312. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/0959-4752(94)90003-5
Szucs, D., Devine, A., Soltesz, F., Nobes, A., & Gabriel, F. (2014).
Cognitive components of a mathematical processing network in 9-year-
old children. Developmental Science, 17, 506–524. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/desc.12144
Tan, M., Mourgues, C., Bolden, D. S., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2013).
Making numbers come to life: Two scoring methods for creativity in
Aurora’s cartoon numbers. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 48, 25–
43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jocb.39
Tang, W. (2007). Cognitive processes related to reading and arithmetic
(Doctoral dissertation). The University of British Columbia, Vancouver,
BC, Canada.
Tillinghast, B. S., Morrow, J. E., & Uhlig, G. E. (1984). Validity of the
PPVT—R using elementary school academic grades as criteria. Psychol-
ogy: A Journal of Human Behavior, 21, 43–47.
Tiu, R. D., Jr., Thompson, L. A., & Lewis, B. A. (2003). The role of IQ in
a component model of reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36,
424436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00222194030360050401
Tolar, T. D., Fuchs, L., Cirino, P. T., Fuchs, D., Hamlett, C. L., &
Fletcher, J. M. (2012). Predicting development of mathematical word
problem solving across the intermediate grades. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 104, 1083–1093. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029020
Toll, S. W. M., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2014). The developmental relation-
ship between language and low early numeracy skills throughout kin-
dergarten. Exceptional Children, 81, 64–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0014402914532233
Träff, U., Desoete, A., & Passolunghi, M. C. (2017). Symbolic and
non-symbolic number processing in children with developmental dys-
lexia. Learning and Individual Differences, 56, 105–111. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.10.010
Träff, U., & Passolunghi, M. C. (2015). Mathematical skills in children
with dyslexia. Learning and Individual Differences, 40, 108–114. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2015.03.024
Träff, U., Skagerlund, K., Olsson, L., & Östergren, R. (2017). Pathways
to arithmetic fact retrieval and percentage calculation in adolescents. The
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 647–663. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/bjep.12170
Tsushima, W. T. (1994). Short form of the WPPSI and WPPSI-R. Journal
of Clinical Psychology, 50, 877–880. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-
4679(199411)50:6877::AID-JCLP22705006103.0.CO;2-R
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
38 PENG ET AL.
Tupa, D. J., O’Dougherty Wright, M., & Fristad, M. A. (1997). Confir-
matory factor analysis of the WISC-III with child psychiatric inpatients.
Psychological Assessment, 9, 302–306. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-
3590.9.3.302
University of Iowa. (2008). ITBS research guide. Retrieved from https://
itp.education.uiowa.edu/ia/documents/itbs-research-guide.pdf
van Bergen, E., de Jong, P. F., Maassen, B., & van der Leij, A. (2014).
The effect of parents’ literacy skills and children’s preliteracy skills on
the risk of dyslexia. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42, 1187–
1200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014-9858-9
Vanbinst, K., Ansari, D., Ghesquière, P., & De Smedt, B. (2016). Sym-
bolic numerical magnitude processing is as important to arithmetic as
phonological awareness is to reading. PLoS ONE, 11, e0151045. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151045
van der Maas, H. L., Dolan, C. V., Grasman, R. P., Wicherts, J. M.,
Huizenga, H. M., & Raijmakers, M. E. (2006). A dynamical model of
general intelligence: The positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism.
Psychological Review, 113, 842–861. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-
295X.113.4.842
Van Rooijen, M., Verhoeven, L., & Steenbergen, B. (2016). Working
memory and fine motor skills predict early numeracy performance of
children with cerebral palsy. Child Neuropsychology, 22, 735–747.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2015.1046426
Verdine, B. N., Irwin, C. M., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2014).
Contributions of executive function and spatial skills to preschool math-
ematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126,
37–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.02.012
von Suchodoletz, A., & Gunzenhauser, C. (2013). Behavior regulation
and early math and vocabulary knowledge in German preschool chil-
dren. Early Education and Development, 24, 310–331. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1080/10409289.2012.693428
von Suchodoletz, A., Uka, F., & Larsen, R. A. A. A. (2015). Self-
regulation across different contexts: Findings in young Albanian chil-
dren. Early Education and Development, 26, 829– 846. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1080/10409289.2015.1012189
Vukovic, R. K. (2006). The development of numeracy: A longitudinal
study of children from first through fourth grade (Doctoral dissertation).
The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada.
Vukovic, R. K., Fuchs, L. S., Geary, D. C., Jordan, N. C., Gersten, R., &
Siegler, R. S. (2014). Sources of individual differences in children’s
understanding of fractions. Child Development, 85, 1461–1476. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12218
Vukovic, R. K., & Lesaux, N. K. (2013a). The language of mathematics:
Investigating the ways language counts for children’s mathematical
development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 227–244.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.02.002
Vukovic, R. K., & Lesaux, N. K. (2013b). The relationship between
linguistic skills and arithmetic knowledge. Learning and Individual
Differences, 23, 87–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.10.007
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Wade, M., Jenkins, J. M., Venkadasalam, V. P., Binnoon-Erez, N., &
Ganea, P. A. (2018). The role of maternal responsiveness and linguistic
input in pre-academic skill development: A longitudinal analysis of
pathways. Cognitive Development, 45, 125–140. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.cogdev.2018.01.005
Wagner, R., Torgesen, J., Rashotte, C., & Pearson, N. A. (1999). CTOPP:
Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (2nd ed.). Austin, TX:
Pro-ed.
Wallace, W. L. (1950). Prediction of grades in specific college courses.
The Journal of Educational Research, 44, 587–598. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1080/00220671.1951.10881895
Wang, A. Y., Fuchs, L. S., & Fuchs, D. (2016). Cognitive and linguistic
predictors of mathematical word problems with and without irrelevant
information. Learning and Individual Differences, 52, 7987. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.10.015
Wanless, S. B. (2008). Measuring behavioral regulation in young chil-
dren (Doctoral dissertation). Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR.
Wanless, S. B., Kim, K. H., Zhang, C., Degol, J. L., Chen, J. L., & Chen,
F. M. (2016). Trajectories of behavioral regulation for Taiwanese chil-
dren from 3.5 to 6 years and relations to math and vocabulary outcomes.
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 34, 104–114. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.ecresq.2015.10.001
Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Acock, A. C., Chen, F. M., & Chen,
J. L. (2011). Behavioral regulation and early academic achievement in
Taiwan. Early Education and Development, 22, 1–28. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1080/10409280903493306
Watson, C. S., Kidd, G. R., Homer, D. G., Connell, P. J., Lowther, A.,
Eddins, D. A.,...Watson, B. U. (2003). Sensory, cognitive, and
linguistic factors in the early academic performance of elementary
school children: The Benton-IU project. Journal of Learning Disabili-
ties, 36, 165–197. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221940303600209
Wechsler, D. Wechsler adult intelligence scale–fourth edition (WAIS–IV).
San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson, 22:498, 2008.
Wei, W., Lu, H., Zhao, H., Chen, C., Dong, Q., & Zhou, X. (2012).
Gender differences in children’s arithmetic performance are accounted
for by gender differences in language abilities. Psychological Science,
23, 320–330. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797611427168
Wei, W., Yuan, H., Chen, C., & Zhou, X. (2012). Cognitive correlates of
performance in advanced mathematics. The British Journal of Educa-
tional Psychology, 82, 157–181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279
.2011.02049.x
Weiland, C., & Yoshikawa, H. (2013). Impacts of a prekindergarten
program on children’s mathematics, language, literacy, executive func-
tion, and emotional skills. Child Development, 84, 2112–2130. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12099
West, G., Vadillo, M. A., Shanks, D. R., & Hulme, C. (2018). The
procedural learning deficit hypothesis of language learning disorders:
We see some problems. Developmental Science, 21, e12552. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12552
Wiese, M. J., Lamb, C., & Piersel, W. C. (1988). WISC-R factor scores
and student self-ratings of behavior as predictors of academic achieve-
ment. Psychology in the Schools, 25, 35–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
1520-6807(198801)25:135::AID-PITS23102501063.0.CO;2-B
Willoughby, M. T., Blair, C. B., Wirth, R. J., & Greenberg, M. (2012).
The measurement of executive function at age 5: Psychometric proper-
ties and relationship to academic achievement. Psychological Assess-
ment, 24, 226–239. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025361
Wilson, R. A., & Keil, F. (1998). The shadows and shallows of explana-
tion. Minds and Machines, 8, 137–159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:
1008259020140
Wright, D. (1987). Intelligence and achievement: A factor analytic and
canonical correlational study. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment,
5, 236–247. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/073428298700500306
Wright, J. C., Huston, A. C., Murphy, K. C., St. Peters, M., Piñon, M.,
Scantlin, R., & Kotler, J. (2001). The relations of early television
viewing to school readiness and vocabulary of children from low-
income families: The early window project. Child Development, 72,
1347–1366. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00352
Xin, Y. P., Jitendra, A. K., & Deatline-Buchman, A. (2005). Effects of
mathematical word problem-solving instruction on middle school stu-
dents with learning problems. The Journal of Special Education, 39,
181–192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00224669050390030501
Zagoloff, Z. (2010). The mediating effect of language skill on academic
achievement among African American, Caucasian, and Latino first–
third grade students (Doctoral dissertation). Illinois Institute of Tech-
nology, Chicago, IL.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
39
LANGUAGE AND MATHEMATICS
Zepp, R. A. (1981). Relationships between mathematics achievement and
various English language proficiencies. Educational Studies in Mathe-
matics, 12, 59–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00386046
Zhang, J., Fan, X., Cheung, S. K., Meng, Y., Cai, Z., & Hu, B. Y. (2017).
The role of early language abilities on math skills among Chinese
children. PLoS ONE, 12, e0181074. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal
.pone.0181074
Zhang, X. (2016). Linking language, visual-spatial, and executive func-
tion skills to number competence in very young Chinese children. Early
Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 178–189. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.ecresq.2015.12.010
Zhang, X. (2018). Letter-name knowledge longitudinally predicts young
Chinese children’s Chinese word reading and number competencies in a
multilingual context. Learning and Individual Differences, 65, 176–186.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2018.06.004
Zhang, X., Hu, B. Y., Ren, L., & Fan, X. (2017). Pathways to reading,
mathematics, and science: Examining domain-general correlates in
young Chinese children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 51,
366–377. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.09.004
Zhang, X., Koponen, T., Räsänen, P., Aunola, K., Lerkkanen, M. K., &
Nurmi, J. E. (2014). Linguistic and spatial skills predict early arithmetic
development via counting sequence knowledge. Child Development, 85,
1091–1107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12173
Zhang, X., & Lin, D. (2015). Pathways to arithmetic: The role of visual-
spatial and language skills in written arithmetic, arithmetic word prob-
lems, and non-symbolic arithmetic. Contemporary Educational Psychol-
ogy, 41, 188–197. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2015.01.005
Zhang, X., & Lin, D. (2017). Does growth rate in spatial ability matter in
predicting early arithmetic competence? Learning and Instruction, 49,
232–241. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.02.003
Zhang, X., & Lin, D. (2018). Cognitive precursors of word reading versus
arithmetic competencies in young Chinese children. Early Childhood
Research Quarterly, 42, 55–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017
.08.006
Zhang, X., Räsänen, P., Koponen, T., Aunola, K., Lerkkanen, M. K., &
Nurmi, J. E. (2017). Knowing, applying, and reasoning about arithmetic:
Roles of domain-general and numerical skills in multiple domains of
arithmetic learning. Developmental Psychology, 53, 2304–2318. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000432
Zhang, X., Räsänen, P., Koponen, T., Aunola, K., Lerkkanen, M., &
Nurmi, J. (2020). Early cognitive precursors of children’s mathematics
learning disability and persistent low achievement: A 5-year longitudinal
study. Child Development, 91, 7–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev
.13123
Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language
comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 162–185.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.123.2.162
Received April 13, 2019
Revision received February 20, 2020
Accepted March 5, 2020
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
40 PENG ET AL.
... Their findings indicate that mathematics vocabulary goes beyond simple concept retrieval and serves as a conduit for applying cognitive reasoning in mathematics. Furthermore, Peng et al. (2020) found that complex language and mathematical skills strengthened the language-math connection. Native speakers exhibited a stronger relationship between language and mathematics than second-language learners, although this difference disappeared after controlling for IQ and working memory. ...
Article
Full-text available
Mathematics requires both comprehension and competence, which, once developed, benefit learners in daily life. This descriptive-correlational and explanatory study filled the gap by determining the mediating effect of vocabulary skills on the relationship between comprehension abilities and mathematics performance of 146 grade 7 students, chosen through multi-stage sampling techniques, in two select schools in Cabuyao City, Philippines, in 2024–2025. The respondents answered valid and reliable instruments with ethical considerations. The study used weighted mean, standard deviation, multiple regression bootstrap technique, mediated regression bootstrap technique, and PLS-SEM. The outcomes showed high levels of mathematics vocabulary skills and reading comprehension abilities, while mathematics performance reached satisfactory levels among the students. Multiple regression bootstrap technique revealed mathematics vocabulary as the most important factor affecting students' mathematics performance, while reading comprehension abilities have no direct significant effect with the presence of mathematics vocabulary. Furthermore, mediated regression bootstrap technique and PLS-SEM displayed a significant total effect of reading comprehension abilities on mathematics performance and the indirect effect flowing through mathematics vocabulary skills was significant. Mathematical vocabulary skills entirely mediate the indirect effect of reading comprehension abilities on mathematics performance exemplifying full mediation. It suggests that mathematics vocabulary skills are essential in enhancing reading comprehension abilities, thereby reinforcing their crucial role in academic achievement. Educators are encouraged to integrate vocabulary-focused strategies, administrators to support professional development in math literacy, and local officials to fund programs that strengthen foundational language skills in mathematics. Recommended Citation: Munda, N.P., Endrinal, J.R.H., & Nequinto, M.C. (2025). Mediation effect of mathematics vocabulary skills between reading comprehension abilities and mathematics performance. International Journal of Educational Management and Development Studies, 6(2), 167-187. https://doi.org/10.53378/ijemds.353205
... Language is thought to be of particular importance because children use mathematics-specific vocabulary and syntax to make sense of mathematics. The use of their first language to understand mathematics meaning also plays a role in how this information is transferred to English (Kazima, 2007;Peng et al., 2020). This review helps teachers and clinicians evaluate the validity of available interventions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Clinical Question: Do bilingual school-age children (P) who receive reading strategy alone interventions (I) show improvement in math word problems (O) relative to a multiple-target intervention or control condition (C)? Method: Systematic Review Study Sources: PsycInfo ® , Web of Science, JSTOR, PubMed, ERIC Search Terms: "bilingual*" OR "second language learner" OR "language learner" OR "English as a second language" OR "English as a foreign language" AND "math word problem" OR "math applied problems" AND "intervention" OR "treatment" OR "therapy" AND "school-age" OR "elementary" OR "primary" OR "grade" Number of Included Studies: 12 Primary Results: Bilingual students with math difficulties showed improvement in math word problems from reading strategy interventions and reading strategy interventions with math vocabulary instruction, content knowledge priming, and culturally relevant approaches. Bilingual students and teachers generally had positive feedback on various intervention approaches. Conclusions: An overall aim of reading strategy interventions for bilingual students with math difficulties is to actively regulate their reading behaviors to reduce the cognitive load on linguistic information to focus on solving math word problems. There are two high-level pieces of evidence showing that bilingual students with math difficulties benefit from reading strategy interventions alone or in combination with content knowledge priming. However, additional research is needed to evaluate the effectiveness of including math vocabulary instruction, the use of a culturally relevant approach, and the comparison of different reading strategies. These approaches only have minimal evidence (primarily single-subject design studies) yet have high social validity.
... Students should articulate their ideas precisely, employing mathematically accurate vocabulary and definitions when explaining their mathematical models. Acquiring new words is vital for learning mathematics because it involves abstract and complex language and concepts (Peng et al., 2020). ...
Article
In this chapter, we present effective and evidence-based instructional practices to improve the mathematical literacy, concepts, knowledge, and skills of students with learning disabilities through (a) explicit instruction to teach mathematics vocabulary, (b) strategies promoting mathematical communication, (c) self-regulated strategy development to teach mathematical writing, (d) use of visual representations, (e) use of mathematical manipulatives, and (f) cognitive and metacognitive strategies to teach mathematical problem-solving. Mathematical literacy characterizes the ability to comprehend, communicate, and solve real-word problems in learning mathematics. However, many students with learning disabilities often face challenges in learning these mathematics abilities. As such, instructional practices that foster the development of mathematical literacy beyond mathematical proficiency for students with learning disabilities can lead to advancing their understanding and use of mathematical concepts, knowledge, and skills. We also describe inclusive practices and assessments that could be used as instructional frameworks for all learners, including those with learning disabilities. These include Universal Design for Learning, accommodations and modifications, data-based individualization, assistive technology, and emerging technology (e.g., artificial intelligence). As a provision of appropriate support tailored to mathematics instruction, these instructional practices will extend and promote students’ active engagement in learning mathematics.
... Performance IQ were each significantly correlated with core mathematical skills, including operations and problem-solving. These relationships support a growing body of literature suggesting that mathematical learning is not driven by a single cognitive ability but is instead the product of a dynamic interplay among linguistic, attentional, memory-based, and visual-spatial processes (Abikoff & Gallagher, 2009;Cirino, 2011;Peng et al., 2020;Swanson & Sachse-Lee, 2001;Stewart et al., 2025;Yang et al., 2021).). This integrative perspective moves beyond a narrow view of mathematics as a solely quantitative domain and reinforces the idea that cognitive strengths or weaknesses in non-numerical areas can significantly influence mathematical success. ...
Preprint
This study aimed to conduct a comparative assessment of mathematical competence-including vocabulary, operations, and problem-solving-in children diagnosed with Specific Learning Disability (SLD) and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Individual assessments were administered to evaluate intelligence quotient (IQ), mathematical competence, reading, and written language for both groups (SLD: N = 17; ADHD: N = 17). The results revealed statistically significant differences between the groups in several WISC subtests: Verbal IQ, Information (factual knowledge, long-term memory, recall), Arithmetic (attention, concentration, numerical reasoning), Digit Span (short-term auditory memory, concentration), Picture Arrangement (planning, logical thinking, social knowledge), Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), and Freedom from Distractibility Index (FDI), as well as in Mathematical Operations and Mathematical Problem Solving. Both groups demonstrated difficulties across all assessed domains. To explore these difficulties in greater depth, canonical correlation analyses (CCA) were conducted on three variable sets. Findings suggest that while both SLD and ADHD are associated with cognitive challenges, they present largely distinct patterns of cognitive deficits. The implications of these results are discussed in the context of educational assessment and intervention.
Chapter
This paper addresses disparities in Mathematics teaching between Indigenous and Eurocentric methods in South African primary schools, aiming to improve mathematical performance in underprivileged settings. Through systematic literature review, action research, and action learning, it identifies Afro-centric games like nhodo, tsoro, and pada as effective tools for teaching basic math concepts. The study reflects the potential of integrating these games into the South African curriculum to enhance learning outcomes.
Article
Full-text available
The current meta-analysis investigates the longitudinal association between prior and subsequent mathematics performance, involving mathematics measured at three time points, and to identify potential factors that could moderate this association, including age, time lag, and types of mathematics. Our analysis included 105 studies, comprising 111 independent samples and involving 413,541 individual students. Our findings revealed that early mathematics knowledge at Time 1 was associated with later performance at Time 3, both directly and indirectly through intermediate performance at Time 2, after controlling for working memory and language comprehension. Notably, the stability of the association between Time 1 and Time 3 mathematics remained robust across increasing age and time lag, while the association between Time 1 and Time 2 weakened as the time lag grew. Additionally, stronger associations were observed when mathematics at Time 1 and Time 3 involved similar or more comprehensive tasks. These findings suggest snowballing effect is more likely to occur within similar types of mathematics tasks, whereas a steppingstone effect is more likely between prior foundational mathematics and later higher level mathematical concepts.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to develop the Mathematics Vocabulary Test (MVT)—an instrument to identify young children with a limited mathematics vocabulary—in a multilingual majority country—South Africa. This study consists of two substudies with children aged 3–8.5 years (N = 988, ngirls = 429). In Study 1, a 26-item MVT was developed by multiprofessional expert panels and piloted in four languages, isiZulu (n = 229), Sesotho (n = 83), English (n = 89), and Afrikaans (n = 216). Study 1 provided evidence of content validity, and the MVT was further revised based on the results. In Study 2, additional samples were assessed with the revised version of the MVT in English (Stage 1, n = 270) and isiZulu (Stage 2, n = 101) to provide further evidence of reliability and validity for these two language versions of the test. Confirmatory factor analyses supported structural validity of a unidimensional structure, including 20 items in the English version and 16 items in the isiZulu version. The structure of the English MVT showed stability across time (T2 5 months after T1). Both versions showed good reliability in terms of internal consistency. Our findings also provided evidence of concurrent and known-group validity for both language versions, as well as predictive validity of the English version of the test. The English and isiZulu MVT can be used as a measure of mathematics vocabulary in educational practice and research with young children.
Preprint
Full-text available
This study explores how language shapes the teaching and learning of mathematics in Kalomo District, Zambia—a region where students grow up speaking local languages but are expected to switch to English as the language of instruction by Grade 5. Using a sequential mixed-methods approach, the research investigates how this linguistic shift impacts student performance, how teachers cope with language-related challenges, and what Zambia’s language-in-education policy means for everyday classroom practice. Data from mathematics tests and surveys involving 200 students and 30 teachers show a strong link between English proficiency and math achievement (r = 0.65, p < 0.05), with language ability explaining 34% of the differences in student performance. Interviews and classroom observations further reveal the difficulties students face in grasping abstract math concepts when taught in a language they are still learning. Teachers try to bridge the gap by code-switching, using visual aids, and incorporating culturally familiar examples. Despite their efforts, they continue to face challenges like limited bilingual resources and insufficient training. The study calls for a more gradual transition to English, backed by bilingual teaching materials and targeted teacher support. These findings highlight the importance of context-sensitive strategies in multilingual classrooms and offer practical recommendations for improving math education in linguistically diverse settings.
Article
Full-text available
To account for the large demands on working memory during text comprehension and expert performance, the traditional models of working memory involving temporary storage must be extended to include working memory based on storage in long-term memory. In the proposed theoretical framework cognitive processes are viewed as a sequence of stable states representing end products of processing. In skilled activities, acquired memory skills allow these end products to be stored in long-term memory and kept directly accessible by means of retrieval cues in short-term memory, as proposed by skilled memory theory. These theoretical claims are supported by a review of evidence on memory in text comprehension and expert performance in such domains as mental calculation, medical diagnosis, and chess.
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic correlates of mathematical achievement and mathematical disability (MD) are reviewed in an attempt to identify the core deficits underlying MD. Three types of distinct cognitive, neuropsychological, or cognitive and neuropsychological deficits associated with MD are identified. The first deficit is manifested by difficulties in the representation or retrieval of arithmetic facts from semantic memory. The second type of deficit is manifested by problems in the execution of arithmetical procedures. The third type involves problems in the visuospatial representation of numerical information. Potential cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic factors contributing to these deficits, and the relationship between MD and reading disabilities, are discussed. Finally, suggestions for the subtyping of mathematical disorders are offered.
Article
Full-text available
A theory of the way working memory capacity constrains comprehension is proposed. The theory proposes that both processing and storage are mediated by activation and that the total amount of activation available in working memory varies among individuals. Individual differences in working memory capacity for language can account for qualitative and quantitative differences among college-age adults in several aspects of language comprehension. One aspect is syntactic modularity: The larger capacity of some individuals permits interaction among syntactic and pragmatic information, so that their syntactic processes are not informationally encapsulated. Another aspect is syntactic ambiguity: The larger capacity of some individuals permits them to maintain multiple interpretations. The theory is instantiated as a production system model in which the amount of activation available to the model affects how it adapts to the transient computational and storage demands that occur in comprehension.
Article
Full-text available
We factor analyzed the WAIS-R protocols of 130 normal elderly. For age and education, Ms = 81.24 years (SD = 5.24) and 9.54 years (SD = 2.50) . Results indicated that the WAIS-R may be interpreted as a 1-, a 2-, or a 3-factor battery. In the 1-factor solution, interpretative emphasis was placed on the Full Scale IQ as a measure of g. In the 2-factor solution, a Verbal Comprehension factor consisted of Information, Digit Span, Vocabulary, Comprehension, and Similarities. A Perceptual Organization factor comprised Block Design, Object Assembly, and Digit Symbol. In the 3-factor solution, the subtest alignments for the Verbal Comprehension and Perceptual Organization factors remained highly similar, whereas Arithmetic constituted a Freedom From Distractibility factor. Overall, 1- or 2-factor models seem to offer the most plausible and parsimonious structures for this sample. 7 subtests had high or intermediate levels of specific variance.
Article
Full-text available
The main purpose of this study was to test the effects of word-problem intervention, with versus without embedded language comprehension instruction, on at-risk 1st graders' word-problem performance. We also isolated the need for a structured approach to word-problem intervention and tested the efficacy of schema-based instruction at 1st grade. Children (n=391; mean age = 6.53, SD = 0.32) were randomly assigned to 4 conditions: schema-based word-problem intervention with embedded language instruction, the same word-problem intervention but without language comprehension instruction, structured number knowledge intervention without a structured word-problem component, and a control group. Each intervention included 45 30-min sessions. Multilevel models, accounting for classroom and school effects, revealed the efficacy of schema-based word-problem intervention at 1st grade, with both word-problem conditions outperforming the number knowledge condition and the control group. Yet, word-problem performance was significantly stronger for the schema-based condition with embedded language comprehension instruction compared to the schema-based condition without language comprehension instruction. Number knowledge intervention conveyed no word-problem advantage over the control group, even though all 3 intervention conditions outperformed the control group on arithmetic. Results demonstrate the importance of a structured approach to word-problem intervention; the efficacy of schema-based instruction at 1st grade; and the added value of language comprehension instruction within word-problem intervention. Results also provide causal evidence on the role of language comprehension in word-problem solving.
Article
Full-text available
Despite the widespread use of Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN) tasks in reading and mathematics research, little is known about the direction of their relation. Thus, in this longitudinal study, we examined the direction of the relation between RAN and reading/ mathematics accuracy and fluency. One hundred eighty-three Grade 1 Chinese children from Shanghai (84 girls and 99 boys; Mage = 85.98 months, SD = 3.76) were followed until Grade 5 and were assessed annually on RAN, reading, and mathematics. The results of standard cross-lagged analyses indicated that the effects were unidirectional (earlier RAN predicted future reading and mathematics fluency), specific (earlier RAN predicted further growth only in fluency outcomes), and consistent (with one exception, earlier RAN predicted every subsequent reading and mathematics fluency). Theoretical implications of these findings are discussed in light of the language in which the study was conducted and the developmental span covered.
Article
Full-text available
Developing academic skills and cognitive abilities is critical for children's development. In this article, we review evidence from recent research on the bidirectional relations between academic achievement and cognitive abilities. Our findings suggest that (a) reading/mathematics and cognitive abilities (i.e., working memory, reasoning, and executive function) predict each other in development, (b) direct academic instruction positively affects the development of reasoning, and (c) such bidirectional relations between cognitive abilities and academic achievement seem weaker among children with disadvantages (e.g., those with special needs or low socioeconomic status). Together, these findings are in line with the theory of mutualism and the transactional model. They suggest that sustained and high‐quality schooling and education directly foster children's academic and cognitive development, and may indirectly affect academic and cognitive development by triggering cognitive‐academic bidirectionality. Developing academic skills and cognitive abilities is critical for children's development. In this article, we review evidence from recent research on the bidirectional relations between academic achievement and cognitive abilities. Our findings suggest that (a) reading/mathematics and cognitive abilities (i.e., working memory, reasoning, and executive function) predict each other in development, (b) direct academic instruction positively affects the development of reasoning, and (c) such bidirectional relations between cognitive abilities and academic achievement seem weaker among children with disadvantages (e.g., those with special needs or low socioeconomic status). Together, these findings are in line with the theory of mutualism and the transactional model. They suggest that sustained and high‐quality schooling and education directly foster children's academic and cognitive development, and may indirectly affect academic and cognitive development by triggering cognitive‐academic bidirectionality.
Article
Full-text available
Although studies have demonstrated a relationship between working memory and achievement in math and reading, it is still unclear why working memory might be related to these abilities. In the present article, we examined the viability of several possible theories in 2 separate analyses of math and reading. In each case, we contrasted the predictions of a cognitive filter model, a transactional model, and a positive manifold model using data from the 2011 Early Childhood Longitudinal Study Kindergarten (ECLS-K). Results of path analyses in a structural equation modeling (SEM) framework indicated an excellent fit for the transactional model, while a poor fit was shown for the other 2 models for both math and reading. Findings across these analyses suggested that working memory and achievement interact in a reciprocal, recursive manner over time. Findings are discussed in terms of their implications for theory, practice, and future research.