Available via license: CC BY
Content may be subject to copyright.
J Sleep Res. 2020;00:e12998.
|
1 of 10
https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12998
wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jsr
1 | INTRODUCTION
The dog (Canis familiaris) has been proved to be an interesting and
valid animal model of human socio-cognitive skills not just at the
behavioural level (Miklósi & Topál, 2013), but also in the area of
neurocognitive research, including sleep-related cognition (Bunford,
Andics, Kis, Miklósi, & Gác si, 2017). One prominent line of canine
neuroscience literature focuses on awake functioning, mainly using
Received:9December2019
|
Revised:23Januar y2020
|
Accepted:26Januar y2020
DOI : 10.1111 /js r.12998
REGULAR RESEARCH PAPER
Repeated afternoon sleep recordings indicate first-night-effect-
like adaptation process in family dogs
Vivien Reicher1 | Anna Kis2 | Péter Simor3,4 | Róbert Bódizs4,5 | Ferenc Gombos6,7 |
Márta Gácsi1,8
This is an op en access article under t he terms of the Creat ive Commons Attributio n License, which permits use, dist ribution and reproduc tion in any medium,
provide d the orig inal work is proper ly cited .
© 2020 The Authors . Journa l of Sleep Re searchpublishedbyJohnWiley&SonsLtdonbehalfofEuropeanSleepResearchSociety
1DepartmentofEthology,Institute
ofBiolog y,EötvösLorándUniversity,
Budapest, Hungary
2ResearchCentreforNaturalSciences,
Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and
Psychology, Budapest, Hungary
3InstituteofPsychology,EötvösLoránd
University,Budapest,Hungar y
4InstituteofBehaviouralSciences,
SemmelweisUniversity,Budapest,Hungary
5JuhászPálEpilepsyCenter,National
Instit ute of Clinical Neuroscien ce, Budapest,
Hungary
6Department of General Psychology,
PázmányPéterCatholicUniversity,
Budapest, Hungary
7MTA-PPKEAdolescentDevelopment
Research Group, Budapest, Hungary
8MTA-ELTEComparativeEthologyResearch
Group, Budapest, Hungary
Correspondence
VivienReicher,DepartmentofEthology,
InstituteofBiology,EötvösLoránd
University,PázmányPétersét ány1/C,1117
Budapest, Hungary.
Email:vivien.reicher@gmail.com
Funding information
HungarianScientificResearchFund,
Grant/AwardNumber:OTKAK115862,
OTKAK132372,OTKAFK128242and
NKFIFK128100;HungarianAcademy
ofSciences,Grant/AwardNumber:MTA
01031;BIALFoundation,Grant/Award
Number:169/16
Summary
The importance of dogs (Canis familiaris) in sleep research is primarily based on their
comparability with humans. In spite of numerous differences, dogs' comparable sleep
pattern, as well as several phenotypic similarities on both the behavioural and neural
levels, make this species a most feasible model in many respects. Our aim was to
investigate whether the so-called first-night effect, which in humans manifests as
a marked macrostructure difference between the first and second sleep occasions,
canbeobservedinfamilydogs.Weusedanon-invasivepolysomnographicmethod
to monitor and compare the characteristics of dogs' (N = 24) 3-hr-long afternoon naps
onthreeoccasionsatthesamelocation.Weanalyse dhowsle epmacrostructurevari-
ables differed between the first, second and third occasions, considering also the
effects of potential confounding variables such as the dogs' age and sleeping habits.
Our findings indicate that first-night effect is present in dogs' sleep architecture, al-
thoughitsspecificssomewhatdeviatefromthepatternobservedinhumans.Sleep
macrostructure differences were mostly found between occasions 1 and 3; dogs slept
more, had less wake after the first drowsiness episode, and reached drowsiness sleep
earlier on occasion 3. Dogs, which had been reported to sleep rarely not at home,
had an earlier non-rapid eye movement sleep, a shorter rapid eye movement sleep
latency, and spent more time in rapid eye movement sleep on occasion 3, compared
withoccasion1.Extendingpriordogsleepdata,theseresultshelpincreasethevalid-
ity of further sleep electroencephalography investigations in dogs.
KEY WORDS
dog model, neuroethology, non-invasive electroencephalography
2 of 10
|
REICHER E t al.
functionalmagneticresonanceimaging (Andics et al., 2016; Berns,
Brooks , & Spivak, 2012) an d electroe ncephalogr aphy (EEG)-based
(ERP)methods(Howell,Conduit,Toukhsati,&Bennett,2011;Kujala
et al., 2013). A somewhat independent line of research investigates
the characteristics of the dogs' sleep, mainly building on the fact that
thegeneralarchitectureofhuman sleepis better approximated by
dog sleep, and not by the most commonly used laborator y animals
(Toth&Bhargava,2013).Strong interrelatedness has been discov-
ered between dogs' sleep and awake functioning, including mem-
ory con solidation (Io tchev et al., 2017; Kis, Szak adát, et al., 2 017)
andemotionprocessing(Kis,Gergely,etal.,2017).Earlystudieson
dogs' sleep focused on neurological conditions (e.g. epilepsy), and
usedinvasivemethodsonlaboratorydogs(Shimazono etal.,1960;
Wauquier,Verheyen,Broeck,&Janssen,1979).Recently,anon-inva-
sivepolysomnography(PSG)methodhasbeen developedtoinves-
tigate the sleeparchitecture of familydogs (Kis,Szakadát,Kovács,
et al., 2014), which has been successfully used to monitor natural
sleepindogsasafunctionofpre-sleepexperiencesand/orindivid-
ual differences (Bunford et al., 2018; Kis, Gergely, et al., 2017; Kis,
Szakadát,etal.,2017).
However, the basic characteristic s of dogs' natural sleep patterns
(without pre-sleep treatment) have only been tangentially investi-
gated(Kis,Sz akadát,Kovács ,etal.,2014),wh ereasadescr iptiveanal-
ysis of sleep macrostructure was provided only for a single recording
session. To gain more insight into the basic mechanisms of natural
sleep in family dogs, we conducted three consecutive sleep record-
ings without affecting the dogs with any pre-sleep activity or han-
dling. Our primar y goal was to assess the so-called first-night ef fect
(FNE), a phe nomenon well k nown in human sl eep researc h, which
manifests in marked macrostructure differences between the first
andsecondsleepoccasionsmeasuredbyPS G.Themajorfactorsthat
contributetoFNEareunfamiliarsurroundings(e.g.sleeplaboratory),
discomfor t and limited mobility caused by electrodes, and psycho-
logical pressure of being under observation (Le Bon et al., 20 01).
It is a common practice in human sleep studies to consider the
first sleeping occasion as an adaptation session and thus discard
it from fur ther analysis without direct comparison to the follow-
ingoccasions.Studies thathaveassessedthedifferencesbetween
the sleep macrostructure of the first versus second night spent in
thelaboratoryhaveconsistentlyfound that FNEisassociatedwith
an increased state of alertness, which results in alterations in the
sleep pat tern: increased number of awakenings and more time spent
awakeaftersleeponset(WASO)leadingtolesstimespentsleeping,
increasedwakefulness,lessrapideyemovement(REM),longerREM
latencyandincreasedslow-wavesleep(SWS)latency(Agnew,Webb,
&Williams,1966;LeBonetal.,2001).Althoughthephenomenonis
calledtheFNE,humanstudiesindicatethat,forcertainparameters,
morethan1 nightisneededtoachievestability,forexampleREM-
relatedvariables(LeBonetal.,2001;Schmidt&Kaelbling,1971).
All previous non-invasive dogPSG measurements were either
carriedoutwiththeexclusion ofthefirstsleepoccasionandcoun-
terbalancing pre-sleep treatments between the second and third oc-
casions(Bunfordetal.,2018;Kis,Gergely,etal.,2017;Kis,Szakadát,
et al., 2017;Kis, Szakadát, Kovács, et al., 2014),or exclusively fo-
cused on one-time recordings (Iotchev et al., 2019). This was based
ontheassumptionthat,similarlytohumans(Agnewetal.,1966;Le
Bon et al., 2001), there must be specific differences in dogs' sleep
patterns and brain activity between the consecutive sleep occa-
sions. Additionally, some of these studies (Kis, Gergely, et al., 2017)
reported a lack of order effect between the second and third sleep
occasions, indicating that at that stage, adaptation effects could be
of smaller magnitude compared with the effect of pre-treatments
used. In light of our scarce knowledge on the number of occasions
needed for dogs to adapt to sleep with electrodes on their heads and
bodiesina newenvironment,amorethoroughanalysisofFNEhas
become essential.
Age is an important factor that both influences sleep−wake
rhythm(Takeuchi & Harada, 2002) and theEEGspectrum of dogs
(Kis, Szakadát, Kovács, et al.,2014;Takeuchi & Harada, 2002) and
humans (Carrier, Land, Buysse, Kupfer, & Monk, 20 01). Thus, in
order to gain pure insight into the effect of repeated laboratory
testing (PSG recordings) on dogs’ sleep structure, variables such
as age need to be included as potential confounding factors in the
analyses. Moreover, the sleep laboratory is a novel and potentially
perceived as a stressful environment, which might have a determi-
nantroleinsleepquality(Lima,Rattenborg,Lesku,&Amlaner,2005;
Voss, 2004). As dogs tend to vary with regard to their sleeping hab-
its(frequencyofsleepingawayfromhome),itcanbeassumedthat
differences will emerge bet ween dogs that rarely versus those that
often sleep away from home, making it imperative to control for such
environmental factors.
Similarlyto humans,dogs'sleepingpattern is sensitivetothe
timing of sleep: at night-time, dogs tend to sleep more, spend more
timeinnon-rapideyemovement(NREM) and REM andlesstime
indrowsiness,andwakeafterfirstdrowsiness(WASO1,formore
details onWASO1indogs,seeSection2.5) comparedwithday-
time (Bunford et al., 2018). However, during the day, dogs are also
prone to sleep, especially during the afternoon period (Tobler &
Sigg, 1986).Thus, forpractical reasons(e.g.inorder to reachan
adequatesamplesize),alldogstudiessofarhavebeenbasedonaf-
ternoon sleep recordings. Though we are not aware of any human
studiesdocumentingFNEin thecontextoftheafternoonnap,it
is plausible to assume that an adaptation effect is also present in
repeatedafternoonsleepsessions.Furtherdifferencescompared
with human studies arise from the fact that due to practical rea-
sons (availability of dog owners volunteering for the study), dog
PSG measurements are not recorded on consecutive days, but
with gaps of several days/weeks/months between occasions. The
effect of this procedural confound has not yet been addressed but,
basedongeneralhabituation−dishabituationtheory,thetimebe-
tween measurements might interfere with the general adaptation
(FNE)effect.
Taken together, prior research into human sleep indicates sig-
nificant and relevant differences in sleep structure between the
first two(andpotentiallymore)sleepoccasions.Wesuggest that
inorder to runvalidcomparative EEGstudies, it is an important
|
3 of 10
REICHER E t al.
precond ition to examin e the process of do gs' adaptatio n to the
PSG test si tuation. We assumed that human-li ke FNE could be
observed between the first/second and possibly the second/third
measurement in dogs, resulting in relatively similar changes in
their sleep structure to that of humans, including more intermit-
tentwaketime( WASO),decreasedsleepeff iciency(percentageof
timespentsleepingduringthe3-hr-longmeasurement),lessREM
sleep, lo nger REM and slee p latencies dur ing the firs t occasion.
Wealsoexaminedtheeffectsofageandsleephabitsontherele-
vant sleep parameters.
2 | METHODS
2.1 | Subjects
Wemeasured30familydogswhoattendedthecanineEEGlabwith
theirowners.Sixdogswereexcludedeitherduetothelackofsleep
(four dogs) or recording ar tefacts caused by high muscle tone (two
dogs). The 24 dogs whose data were used for the analysis were
7 months–9 years old; nine males and 15 females (14 out of the 15
female dogs had been neutered, and the remaining one female dog
was not in heat during any of the recording occasions); from nine dif-
ferent breeds and 10 mutts.
Owners were recruited from the Family Dog Project (Eötvös
Loránd University, Department of Ethology) database. All experi-
mentalprotocolswereapprovedbytheScientificEthicsCommittee
for Animal Experimentation (Állatkísérleti Tudományos Etikai
Tanács) of Budape st, Hungar y (number of ethic al permissio n: PE/
EA/853-2/2016). The location of the measurements was a fully
equippedlaboratoryforcanineEEGmeasurementsattheResearch
CentreforNaturalSciences,InstituteofCognitiveNeuroscienceand
Psychology.
2.2 | Procedure
ParticipationinthesleepEEGresearchdidnotrequirepriortrain-
ing. All subjects were measured on three occasions at the same
location. All the recordings were conducted during the afternoon,
withastarttimebetween12:00 hoursand17:00 hours.Foreach
individual dog, the starting time of the three different nap op-
portunities was kept within a ±2 hr interval between recording
occasions. Recordings were conducted within 11.03. 2017 and
22.12.2018 interval.For one dogall three recordings werecon-
ducted within the same season (during autumn); while for others
they were spread out from spring to summer N = 3; from spring
to autumn (first two recordings in spring, third recording in au-
tumn) N = 8; from autumn to winter N = 10; from summer to au-
tumn N = 2. Between occasions 1 and 2, 1–4 weeks passed, while
between occasions 2 and 3 for a subgroup (N = 10) 3–4 weeks
passed, and for the other subgroup (N=14)5weeks–6months
passed. Applying this set-up allowed us to investigate the effect of
time elapsed between the recordings.
All measurements were carried out after a relatively active day
(i.e. a physic ally and mentally loaded day due to advanced train-
ing,excursion).Thoughtheactivitylevelcouldbeslightlydifferent
between dogs, an individual's activity level was the same before
allsleepingoccasions.Beforethemeasurement,theexperimenter
explained the processto the owner while the dogcould explore
the room (5–10 min). Then the owner settled on the mattress with
the dog and held the dog's head gently while the experimenter
was placing the surface electrodes. During electrode placement,
dogs were rewarded using social (e.g. petting, praise) and/or food
reward.After the electrode placementandthecheckof the PSG
signals, owners were asked to mute their cell phones and engage
in a quiet activitysuch asreading, watching a movie on alaptop
withearphonesor sleeping duringthemeasurement.The experi-
menter lef t the room and monitored the measurement on a laptop
in the adjacent room. In case of the rare event of the malfunction
of an electrode, theexperimenter replaced or changed the elec-
trode. The canine sleep laboratory is a room with no window, thus
an inbuilt heating and air-conditioning system was set to keep the
temperature at the same level (about 22°C), and a reading lamp was
provided for those owners who wished to read (which was then on
for all recordings).
2.3 | PSG placement and monitoring
SleepwasmonitoredbyPSG,whichallowedtheparallelrecording
ofEEG,electrooculogram(EOG),electrocardiogram (ECG), respi-
ration (PNG) and electromyography (EMG). In thisresearch pro-
ject, wefollowedthe previouslyvalidated PSG method ondogs
(Kis,Szakadát,Kovács,etal.,2014)withthesingleadditionofan-
otherelectrodeontherightzygomaticarch.Withthisnewset-up
insteadoftwo,fourEEGchannelsandaneyemovementchannel
had been recorded. The two electrodes placed on the right and
left zygomatic archnext to the eyes(F8,F7) andthescalp elec-
trodes overtheanteroposterior midlineoftheskull(Fz,Cz)were
referred to the G2, a reference electrode that was in the posterior
midlineoftheskull(occiput;externaloccipitalprotuberance).The
ground electrode (G1) was attached to the left musculus tempo-
ralis. ECGelectrodeswereplacedbilaterally over the secondrib.
SeeFigu re1forphotoofadogwithelectrodeplacement;Figure2
fordetaileddrawingofadogwiththenamesandexactplacement
oftheelectrodes;andFigure3forexamplesofPSGdatafromthe
four different sleep stages.
Fortherecordings,gold-coatedAg/AgClelectrodeswereused,
secured bySigna SprayElectrode Solution (Parker)and EC2 Grass
Electrode Cream (Grass Technologies). The impedance values of
the EEGelectrodeswerekept under20kΩ during the recordings.
Thesignalswerecollected,pre-filtered,amplified anddigitizedata
samplingrate of 1,024 Hz perchannel,using the 25-channelSAM
4 of 10
|
REICHER E t al.
25R EEG System (Micromed), and the System PlusEvolutionsoft-
warewithsecond-orderfiltersat0.016Hz(highpass)and70Hz(low
pass).
2.4 | Questionnaire
We sent out an online questionnaire to all participating owners
about the sleeping habits of the dogs. This was done based on the
assump tiontha td ogs,w hichsleep frequ entlyou tside theirho meen-
vironment, might find it less stressful to sleep in a laboratory. In the
questionnaireownershadtoratetheirdogsona0–3(never;rarely;
often; ver y often) scale on the following statements: How often
does the dog sleep (a) in a novel environment in the presence of the
owner (who is engaged in other activities; work, meeting) during the
afternoon; (b) not at home but in a familiar environment in the pres-
ence of the owner (who is engaged in other activities; work, meeting)
during the afternoon; (c) in a novel environment in the presence of the
owner at night; (d) not at home but in a familiar environment in the
presence of the owner at night?
Most owners reported that their dogs never or rarely slept not
at home in a familiar (79%) or new environment (100%) during the
night. Moreover, most owners (75%) reported that their dogs never
or rarely slept in the afternoon in a new environment. Therefore,
only the second question yielded reasonable variability in re-
sponses, therefore we included it as a factor in our analysis, lumping
never and rarely responses (N=0 + 10; mean age=4.8±2.2)and
often and very often responses (N=10 + 4;meanage= 4.9±2.9)
so that in the end we had two categories for “sleep habits”: rarely
sleepin g away from home (RS AH); and often sl eeping away from
home(OSAH).
2.5 | Data analysis
Sleep re cordings were visua lly scored in accorda nce with standa rd
criteria (Berryetal., 2015), adapted for dogs(Kis, Szakadát,Kovács,
etal., 2014). A self-developed program (by FerencGombos;Fercio's
EEGPlus,2009–2019)was used to analyseandexportdata. The re-
cordings were manually scored, and the program provided data for
exportingmacrostructuralvariables.Thismanualcodingreliablyiden-
tifies the stages of wake, drowsiness, NREM and REM in dogs (Kis,
Szakadát,Kovács ,etal.,2014).Anota bledifferenc ei nt hecanin esleep
stage scoring, compared with human studies, arises from the fact that
in dogs there is a stage called drowsiness that bears characteristics
ofbothhuman Stage1 NREMsleepand quietawake.Drowsinessis
characteristic of insectivore and carnivore mammals (including dogs),
asinthesetaxathetransitionfromwakefulnesstosleepisnotasclear
as in humans ( Zepelin et al., 2005). Due to this difference, we used t wo
approaches/measurestodetermineWASO,sleeplatencyandREMla-
tency.IncaseofWASOandREMlatency,WASO1andREMlatency1
weremeasuredfromthefirstdrowsinessepisode,whileWASO2and
REMlatency2wereme asuredfromthefirstNREMepisode.Inc as eof
sleep latency, sleep latency 1 was measured until the first drowsiness
FIGURE 1 Photo of a dog with electrode placement before the
measurement.Electroencephalogram(EEG)andelectrooculogram
(EOG)electrodeswereplacedonthescalp,electrocardiogram
(ECG)electrodeswereplacedbilaterallyoverthesecondrib,and
electromyogram(EMG)electrodeswereattachedonthemusculus
iliocostalis dorsi. Respiration was recorded by a respirator y band.
Note: during the measurement the owner's hand was not on the dog
FIGURE 2 Placement of the electrodes
andtherespiratorybelt(Fz-Cz:frontal
andcentralmidline;F7-F8:rightandleft
electrodesplacedonthezygomaticarch;
G2: reference electrode; G1: ground
electrode;ECG:electrocardiographic
electrodes;EMG:electromyography
electrodes;PNG:respiratorybelt−
respiratory inductance plethysmography)
|
5 of 10
REICHER E t al.
episode,whilesleeplatency2wasmeasureduntilthefirstNREMepi-
sode. It also needs to be noted that in dog sleep research the different
stagesofNREMarenotdistinguished,buthandleduniformlyasSWS
orNREMsleep(Kis,Szakadát,Kovács,etal.,2014).
2.6 | Analytic plan
For alls tatisticalm odelsthe 10 dependent macrostructural variables
of interest were: sleep efficiency (the percentage of time spent asleep:
drowsiness+NREM+REMduringthe3-hr-longmeasurement),thedu-
ration of time spent awake after the first epoch scored as drowsiness
(WASO 1)and NREM (WASO2)sleep, the latencytofirstdrowsiness
(sleep latency 1) and NREM (sleep latency 2) sleep, theproportion of
time spent in drowsiness,NREM andREM sleep, the latency toREM
slee pa fterd ro wsiness(R EMlatency1)an dN REM(REMlatenc y2)slee p.
To measure the effect of the time elapsed between occasions,
weranseparategeneralizedlinearmixedmodels,wherethesubject
ID was included as a random factor, the macrostructural variables
were entered as target s, and time intervals between occasions were
enteredasfixedfactors.
TomeasureFNE,thedifferencebetweensleepmacrostructure
(for the above-described 10 variables of interest) on sleep occasions
1,2and3wasanalysedwithgeneralizedlinearmixedmodelswith
backward elimination. Statistical analyses were performed using
IBM's SPSS25.0 software.The subject ID wasincludedas a ran-
dom factor. The macrostructural variables were entered as targets,
whereas occasion, dogs' sleep habits, age (in years), and the inter-
actionofoccasionandsleephabitswereenteredasfixedfactors.
In case of sleep and REM latencies the statis tical analyses
wereconductedin R3.6.1(RCoreTeam,2014).Thesevariables
were analysed using Mixed Effects Cox Models (R package
‘coxme’;Therneau,2015),withoccurrenceofREMsleepastermi-
nal event. The subject ID was included as a random factor. In all
initial models, the effect of occasion, sleep habits, age (in years),
and interaction of occasion and sleep habits were included as
fixedfactors.
3 | RESULTS
Elapsedtimebetweenoccasionshadnoeffectonthesleepvariables
(all p > .05), thus we did not include it in further analyses.
3.1 | Sleep efficiency
Occasion had an effect on sleep efficiency (F2, 65= 19.874,p < .001),
which was greatest on occasion 3. Pairwise post hoc analysis revealed
a difference between occasions 1 and 3 (p < .001) and occasions 2 and
3 (p = .019), but no difference between occasions 1 and 2 (p = .111;
Figure4a).Dogs'sleephabitshadanoccasion-specificeffectonsleep
efficiency (F2, 65 = 3.655,p=.031):OSAHdogssleptmoreduringocca-
sion1comparedwith RSAHdogs(p=.016;Figure6).Moreover,age
had a main effect on sleep efficiency: older dogs slept more, compared
to younger dogs (F(2,65) = 8.382, p = .005).
3.2 | WASO 1 and WASO 2
Occasio n affected WASO 1 (F2 ,67 = 5.3 44, p = .007). More specifi-
cally, dogs spent less time awake after the first drowsiness episode
FIGURE 3 Representativepolysomnographic(PSG)tracesfromthesleepstagesof(a)wake,(b)drowsiness,(c)non-rapideyemovement
(NREM),(d)rapideyemovement(REM)
6 of 10
|
REICHER E t al.
on occasion 3, compared with occasion 1 (p = .013) and occasion 2
(p = .05), while occasion 2 did not differ from occasion 1 (p = .989;
Figure4b).AgeaffectedWASO1: olderdogsspentlesstimeawake
after their first drowsiness episode, compared to younger dogs
(F2,67=5.658,p =.020).Moreover,dogs’sleephabitshadatendency
effect (F2,67 = 3.725, p=.058):OSAHdogsspentlesstimeawake,
comparedtoRSAHdogs.
Occasio n showed a trend o n WASO 2 (F2,68 = 2.943, p = .059;
Figure 4c). In addition, age had a main effect on WASO 2
(F1,68 = 5.580,p = .021): older dogs spent less time awake after their
firstNREMepisode,comparedwithyoungerdogs.Dogs'sleephab-
its showed no effec t (F1,67 = 2.580,p = .113).
3.3 | Sleep latency 1
The Cox model proved to be significant in case of sleep la-
tency 1, and occasion had a main effect ( χ2
3=25.65; p < .001;
AIC = 19.65). Dogs reached drowsiness sleeplater onoccasion
1,comparedwithoccasions2(exp(β) = 0.379, z=−2.82,p = .0 01)
and3(exp(β) = 0.243, z=−3.89,p < .001), but no difference was
found bet ween occasions 2 and 3 (exp(β) = 0.384, z = −1.34,
p=.375;Figure5a).Moreover,ageandsleephabit shadnoeffect
(all p > .05).
3.4 | Sleep latency 2
TheCoxmodelprovedtobesignificantincaseofsleeplatency2,
including an interaction of occasion and sleep habits (χ2
6=23 .4 6;
p<.001;AIC=11.46).RSAHdogsreachedNREMsleeplateron
occasion1,comparedwithoccasions2(exp(β) = 0.251, z=−2.56,
p=.028)and3(exp(β) = 0.135, z=−3.53,p = .001), while occa-
sion 2 did notdifferfrom occasion 3 (exp(β) = 0.54, z = −1.24,
p=.4 33).Moreover,RSAHdogsre ac he dNREMsleepl ateronoc-
casion1,comparedwithOSAHdogs(exp(β) = 0.245, z=−2.019,
p=.043;Figure5b).Ageshowednoeffect(exp(β) = 1.087,
z = 0.81, p = .42).
FIGURE 4 The effect of occasion on (a) sleep efficiency, (b) wake after first drowsiness and (c) wake after first non-rapid eye movement
(NREM)episode.Note:sleepefficiency,wakeafterfirstdrowsinessandNREMsleepindogsindividuallyareindicatedwithcolouredlines;
mean values (± SE) are indicated with black
FIGURE 5 The effect of occasion on sleep latency 1 (a); and occasion-specific effects of dogs' sleep habits on sleep latency 2 (b)
|
7 of 10
REICHER E t al.
3.5 | Relative drowsiness duration
Occasion, age and dogs' sleep habits did not influence the relative
duration of drowsiness (all p > .05).
3.6 | Relative NREM duration
Occasion, age and dogs' sleep habits did not influence the relative
durationofNREMsleep(allp > .05).
3.7 | Relative REM duration
OccasiondidnotinfluencetherelativeREMduration(F2,66 = 1.005,
p = .372), but the dogs' sleep habits had a significant main effect
(F2,66 = 8.070, p=.006):OSAHdogs spent moretimeinREMsleep
compared with the RSAH dogs. A significant interaction of oc-
casion an d sleep habits o n relative REM dura tion was also foun d
(F2,66=3.265,p=.044);onoccasion1,OSAHdogsspentmoretime
inREMsl eepcomparedwithRSAHdogs(p<.001;Figure6).Agehad
no effec t (F2,65 = 0.321, p = .573).
As OSAH do gs did not seem to sh ow significant FN E, we ran
an additional model to assess the effect of occasion in relative
REMdurationonRSAH dogs.Wefoundamain effect ofoccasion
(F2,27=13.116,p< .001),thatis,RSAHdogshadlessrelativeREM
duration on occasion 1, compared with occasion 3 (p < .001), but no
difference between occasions 1 and 2 (p = .19), and between occa-
sions 1 and 3 (p=.92;Figure6b,blueline).
3.8 | REM latency 1
The Cox model proved tobesignificant in case of REM latency 1,
including an interaction of occasion and sleep habits (χ2
6 = 23.8;
p=.005;AIC = 11.8). Inthe subgroup of RSAHdogs we revealed
longer REM latency on occasion 1 compared with occasions 2
(exp(β)= 0.236,z=−2.19, p=.03)and3(exp(β) = 0.239, z=−2.34,
FIGURE 6 Occasion-specific effects of
dogs’sleephabitson(a)sleepefficiency
(mean±SE)and(b)relativerapideye
movement(REM)duration(mean±SE).
The effect of occasion on rarely sleeping
awayfromhome(RSAH)dogs(mean±SE)
(b). *p < .05; ***p < .0 01
FIGURE 7 Occasion-specificeffectsofdogs'sleephabitsonrapideyemovement(REM)latency1(a)and2(b)
8 of 10
|
REICHER E t al.
p=.02 ),but nodif f ere ncebet wee noccasions2and3(ex p(β) = 0.013,
z = 0.027, p=.99).Moreover,RSAHdogsreachedREMsleeplateron
occasion1,compared with OSAH dogs (exp(β) = 0.123, z = −3.24,
p=.001;Figure7a).Agehadnoeffect(exp(β) = 1.049, z= −0.53,
p = .59).
3.9 | REM latency 2
The Cox modelproved to besignificant in caseofREM latency 2,
including an interaction of occasion and sleep habits (χ2
6 = 20.29;
p = .002; A IC = 8.29). In the subg roup of RSAH dogs we n either
revealed difference between occasions 1 and 2 (exp(β) = 0.315,
z=−2.043,p=.10),norbetweenoccasions2and3(exp(β) = 0.951,
z=−0.100,p=.99),butdogstendedtohavelongerREMlatencyon
occasion 1, compared with occasion 3, although the effect did not
reachsignificance(exp(β) = 0.30 0, z = −1.93, p = .054). Moreover,
RSAHdogsreachedREMsleep lateronoccasion1,comparedwith
OSAHdogs(exp(β) = 0.180, z=−2.807,p=.005;Figure7b).Agehad
noeffect(exp(β) = 1.017, z=−0.20,p = .84).
4 | DISCUSSION
Acomplexpatternofdifferenceswasrevealedbetweensleepocca-
sions when conducting repeated afternoon sleep recordings in fam-
ily dogs. These adaptation effects on sleep macrostructure present
both similarities to and differences from the FNE phenomena de-
scribed in humans when conducting sleep recordings on consecutive
nights.Dogs experience (sleephabits)andagealso seemtoaffect
the sleep architecture, which parallels human findings.
Sleep occasion had an effect on dogs' sleep macrostructure;
however, contrar y to humans (Agnew et al., 1966; Le Bon e t al.,
2001), in dogs most significant differences were found not bet ween
the first two occasions, but between occasions 1 and 3. One possible
explanationforthemostly“second-night”insteadofthe“first-night”
effectmightbethevariation indogs' sleepinghabits (frequency of
sleeping away from home). These findings are in line with human
studies suggesting that a novel and potentially dangerous environ-
menthasadeterminantrole in sleepquality,for examplereducing
thetime spentinREMsleep(Lima et al., 2005).With NREM−REM
cycles,thelowandhigharousalthresholdsalternate,andREMsleep
with high arousal threshold results in low behavioural awareness
and high vulnerability to dangerous surroundings (Lima et al., 2005;
Voss,2004).Inhumanstudies,areverseversionoftheFNEhasalso
been reported; in contrast to healthy subjects, in whom the novel
environmentresultsinlowerqualityofsleep(Agnewetal.,1966),in
patients with insomnia the awareness of being watched had a pro-
motingeffectonsleepquality,presumablybecausetheenvironment
was interpreted as more secure (Hauri & Olmstead, 1989). Previous
studies on monkeys and laboratory rat s also found that sleep archi-
tecture (e.g. sleep efficiency, number of arousals and proportion of
time spent in REM) was sensitive to theperceived security of the
environment(e.g.laboratoryrats were exposedtoa laboratorycat
under var ious conditions ; Bert, Balz amo, Chase, & Pegra m, 1975;
Broughton, 1973). Our results regarding REM-related variables,
sleep latency and sleep efficiency suggest that dogs that rarely sleep
away from home are more sensitive to the new test situation (sleep-
ing in a new environment with electrodes) compared with dogs that
often sleep away from home.
However,this doesnot explain the lack of a linear habituation
process over the sleep occasions at the individual level. Dogs are
known to show individual-level variation that outnumbers humans'
by several magnitudes both in morphological and behavioural fea-
tures (McGreevy et al., 2013), as well as physiological parameters
(Báli ntetal.,2019).Suchindividualvariationsmightmaskalinearha-
bituation process, but as we did find differences between occasions
(1 and 3), it is unlikely that the noise caused by individual variability
would produce such a pattern.
Prior hum an FNE studies th at showed convincin g data on FNE
wereconductedon43(Agnewetal.,1966),26(LeBonetal.,2001)
and 12 (Lorenzo & Barbanoj, 2002) subjectson consecutive nights.
Our data were obtained from a sample of 24 dogs, which – in the
light of the remarkable individual differences – might not be large
enough. Moreover, due to practical reasons, our measurements were
conducted neither on three consecutive days nor during the night.
Althoughwe cannotexcludethat therelatively great time intervals
between the measurements interfered with our results, the present
data do not provide statistic al evidence for the ef fect of the elapsed
time between occasions. However, as our set-up followed that used
indogPSGstudies,theminimumtimeelapsedbetweenrecordings
was 1 week, thus our findings regarding this circumstance might not
beextendabletoasituationwhererecordingsareperformedoncon-
secutiv e nights. Lore nzo and Barban oj (2002) colle cted data on 12
nights, with a minimum of 1 month between three periods, and one
period consisted of4 consecutive nights. They foundthat the FNE
was only present in the first night of the first period (called the “very
first night”). This study is in line with earlier data obtained in a re-
searchimplyingthatFNEmightlas tformorethan1night,specifically,
REM -relatedparametersa remoresensitiveandtheirs tabilityprocess
might ext end up to 4 nights (L e Bon et al., 20 01). Interestin gly,w e
foundFNEinREMlatency,butmarkeddifferenceswerealsopresent
betwee n occasions 1 and 3. T hese findings s trengthen th e assumption
thatREM-relatedparametersneedmoresleepoccasionstostabilize.
Previous studies have already documented that several sleep pa-
rametersincluding EEGspectrum(Kis, Szakadát, Simor,etal.,2014)
and sleep spindles (Iotchev et al., 2019) co-vary with age. Here we
found that older dogs slept more and spent less time awake after the
first d rowsiness and NREM sl eep. The same mac rostructur al vari-
ables were also suggested in humans to be a marker of age-related
sleep changes (Carrier, Monk, Buysse, & Kupfer, 1997), but the rela-
tionship is opposite to the one we found in dogs. Human studies have
also documented anage-specific effect on FNE,for example it has
been reported that children and elderly need 3 instead of 2 nights
toadapt(Schmidt&Kaelbling,1971).Noindicationofaninteraction
between age and occasion was found here in dogs.
|
9 of 10
REICHER E t al.
In sum, our findings indicate that in case of dogs' afternoon sleep
recordings, the ef fect of adaptation during the first occasion is con-
siderably smaller thanwhat weexpected based on the humanlit-
erature.However,thehumanFNEliteratureisbasedondata from
consecutive night-time recordings, we could not find any study in-
vestigating adapt ation effects for afternoon recordings in humans.
Most macrostructural differences in our study were detected be-
tween sleep occasions 1 and 3, which raises issues for future dog
PSGresearch.Alternatively,theadaptationsleepmightnotbenec-
essary (considering the few significant effects between occasions
1 and 2). It is, however, imperative to control for dogs' sleep habits.
Futureresearch–evaluatingtheeffectsof,forexampleattachment,
personality and sensitivity – needs to confirm that a simplified mea-
surement procedurewithoutadaptationand/ortheinclusionofex-
perienced dogs is indeed feasible.
IthasbeensuggestedthatthewayFNEmanifestsinhumansat
the individual level could be used as a diagnostic criterion in certain
sleep disorders and psychiatric conditions (e.g. a more pronounced
FNE can be o bserved in pati ents with idiopa thic nightmares ; Kis,
Szakadát,Simor,et al., 2014),whilealesspronouncedFNE ischar-
acteristicofpatientswithdepression(Toussaint,Luthringer,Staner,
Muzet,&MacHer,2000).Consideringthatthefamilydogisincreas-
inglyrecognizedasamodelforhumanneuropsychiatricconditions,
including obsessive-compulsive disorder (Ledford, 2016), autism
(Topál, Román, & Turcsán, 2019), and sleep disorders, like narco-
lepsy(Ripley,Fujiki, Okura,Mignot,& Nishino, 2001), sleep-disor-
dered breathing (Hinchliffe, Liu, & Ladlow, 2019), our findings might
open up new directions for the investigations of the links between
environmental factors and brain mechanisms underlying cognitive
(dys)functions,whichcouldhelpbetterunderstandcomplexdogand
even human phenotypes.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Financia l support was prov ided to V. R. and M. G. by Hun garian
ScientificResearchFund(OTKAK115862,OTKAK132372)andthe
HungarianAcademyofSciences(MTA01031);toA.K.byHungarian
ScientificResearchFund(OTKAFK128242),BIALFoundation(grant
no.169/16), János Bolyai Research Scholarship of the Hungarian
Academy of Sciences; to P. S. by Hungarian Scientific Research
Fund(NKFIFK128100)oftheNationalResearch,Developmentand
InnovationOffice;R.B.byHigherEducationInstitutionalExcellence
Program of the Ministry of Human Capacities in Hungary, within the
frameworkof the Neurology thematicprogramof the Semmelweis
University.TheauthorsthankÁdámMiklósiforhiscommentsonan
earlier versionofthe manuscript, andTamás Faragó forhishelpin
the statistical analyses.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Conceptualization,V.R.,A.K.,P.S.,R.B.andM.G.;methodology,V.
R.,A.K.,P.S.,R.B.andM.G.;software,F.G.;validation,V.R.,A.K.
and M. G.; formal analysis, V. R. and A . K.; investigation, V. R.; data
curation V. R.; writing – original draft, V. R.; writing – review and
editing,allauthors;visualization, V. R.; supervision, M. G.;funding
acquisition,M.G.
ORCID
Vivien Reicher https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8285-826X
REFERENCES
Agnew, H.W.,Webb,W.B.,&Williams, R. L. (1966).Thefirstnight ef-
fect:AnEEGstudyofsleep.Psychophysiology, 2(3),263–266.https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1966.tb02650.x
Andics,A.,Gábor,A.,Gácsi,M.,Faragó,T.,Szabó,D.,&Miklósi,Á.(2016).
Neuralmechanismsforlexicalprocessingindogs.Science, 353(6303),
1030–1032.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf3777
Bálint , A., Eleőd , H., Körmendi , J., Bódizs, R ., Reicher, V., & Gácsi, M .
(2019). Potential physiological parameters to indicate inner states
indogs:TheanalysisofECG,andrespirator ysignalduringdifferent
sleep phases. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 13, 207. https ://
doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00207
Berns,G.S.,Brooks,A.M.,&Spivak,M.(2012).FunctionalMRIinawake
unrestrained dogs. PLoS ONE, 7(5), e38027. https ://doi.org/10.1371/
journ al.pone.0038027
Berr y,R . B., Gamaldo , C. E., Susan , M. H., Robin, M . L., Lloyd, C . L.,
Vaughn, B. V., & Brooks, R. (2015). The AASM Manual for the Scoring
of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical
Specifications, Version 2.2. Darien, Illinois: American Academy of
SleepMedicine.
Bert,J., Balzamo, E.,Chase, M., & Pegram, V.(1975).Thesleep ofthe
baboon, Papio papio, under natural conditions and in the laboratory.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 39(6),657–662.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0 013-4694(75)90079-6
Broughton, R. (1973) Confusional sleep disorders: interrelationship
with memory consolidation and retrieval in sleep. In T. Boag & D.
Campbell(Eds.),A Triune Concept of the Brain and Behaviour (pp. 115–
127).Toronto:TorontoUniversityPress.
Bunford , N., Andics , A., Kis, A ., Miklósi, Á ., & Gácsi , M. (2017). Canis
familiaris as a model for non-invasive comparative neuroscience.
Trends in Neurosciences, 40(7 ), 438– 452. htt ps://doi.or g/10.1016/j .
tins.2017.05.003
Bunford , N., Reicher, V., Kis, A ., Pogány, Á., G ombos, F., Bódiz s, R., &
Gácsi, M. (2018). Differences in pre-sleep activity and sleep location
are associated with variability in day time/nighttime sleep electro-
physiology in the domestic dog. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 7109. https ://
doi.org /10.1038/s41598-018-25546-x
Carrier,J.,Land,S.,Buysse,D.J.,Kupfer,D.J.,&Monk,T.H.(2001).The
effectsofageandgenderonsleepEEGpowerspectraldensityinthe
middle yearsof life(ages20–60years old).Psychophysiology, 38(2),
232–242.https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3820232
Carrier, J., Monk, T. H., Buysse, D. J., & Kupfer, D. J. (1997). Sleep
and morningness-eveningness in the “middle” years of life
(20–59 y). Journal of Sleep Research, 6(4), 230–237. https ://doi.
org /10.1111/j.1365-2869.1997.00230.x
Hauri,P.J.,&Olmstead,E.M.(1989).Reversefirstnighteffectininsom-
nia. Sleep, 12(2), 97–105. ht tps ://doi.or g/10.1093/sleep/ 12.2 .97
Hinchliffe,T.A.,Liu,N.,&Ladlow,J.(2019).Sleep-disorderedbreathing
in the Cavalier King Charles spaniel: A case series. Veterinary Surgery,
48(4), 497–504. https ://doi.org/10.1111/ vsu .1314 8
Howell,T.,Conduit,R.,Toukhsati,S.,&Bennett,P.(2011).Development
of a minimally-invasive protocol for recording mismatch negativity
(MMN) in the dog (Canis familiaris) using electroencephalography
(EEG).Journal of Neuroscien ce Methods, 201(2), 377–380. https ://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.08.005
10 of 10
|
REICHER E t al.
Iotchev,I.B.,Kis,A.,Bódizs,R.,VanLuijtelaar,G.,Kubinyi,E.,&Luijtelaar,
G.V.(2017).EEGtransientsinthesigmarangeduringnon-REMsleep
predic t learning in dogs. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–23. https ://doi.
org /10.103 8/s41598-017-13278-3
Iotchev, I. B. , Kis, A., Turcs án, B., Rodrigo , D., Fernández D e Lara, T.,
Reicher,V.,&Kubinyi,E.(2019).A ge-relateddifferencesandsexual
dimorphism in canine sleep spindles. Scientific Reports, 9, 10,092,
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019- 46434-y
Kis, A ., Gergely, A ., Galambo s, Á., Abd ai, J., Gomb os, F.,B ódizs, R ., &
Topál,J. (2017). Sleepmacrostructureismodulated bypositiveand
negativesocialexperienceinadultpetdogs.Proceedings of the Royal
Society B: Biological Sciences, 284 (1865), 20 ,171, 883. https://doi.
org /10.10 98/rspb .2017.1883
Kis,A.,Szakadát,S.,Gácsi,M.,Kovács,E.,Simor,P.,Török,C.S.,…Topál,
J.(2017).Theinterrelatedeffectofsleepandlearningindogs(Canis
familiaris);anEEGandbehaviouralstudy.Scientific Reports, 7, 41,873.
https ://doi.org/10.1038/srep4 1873
Kis, A., Szakadát, S., Kovács, E., Gácsi, M., Simor, P., Gombos, F., …,
Bódizs,R.(2014).Developmentofa non-invasivepolysomnography
technique for dogs (Canis familiaris). Physiology and Behavior, 130,
149–156.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.04.004
Kis,A .,Szakadát,S.,Simor,P.,Gombos,F.,Horváth,K.,&Bódizs,R.(2014).
Objective and subjective components of the first-night effec t in young
nightmare sufferers and healthy participants. Behavioral Sleep Medicine,
12(6),469–480.https://doi.org/10.1080/15402002.2013.829062
Kujala, M. V., Törnqvist , H., Somppi, S., Hännine n, L., Krause, C. M.,
Vainio,O.,&Kujala,J.(2013).Reactivityofdogs'brainoscillationsto
visual stimuli measured with non-invasive electroencephalography.
PLoS ONE, 8(5),1–8.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061818
Le Bon, O., St aner, L., Hoffmann, G., Dramaix, M., San Sebastian, I.,
Murphy,J.R.,…,Linkowski,P.(2001).Thefirst-nighteffectmaylast
more than one night. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 35(3),165–172.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3956(01)00019-X
Ledford,H.(2016).DogDNAprobedforcluestohumanpsychiatricills.
Nature, 529,446–447.https://doi.org/10.1038/529446a
Lima,S.L.,Rattenborg,N.C.,Lesku,J.A.,&Amlaner,C.J.(2005).Sleeping
under the risk of predation. Animal Behaviour, 70(4),723–736.https
://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.01.008
Lorenzo, J. L.,& Barbanoj,M.J.(2002). Variability of sleepparameters
across multiple laboratory sessions in healthy young subjects: The
“very first night effect”. Psychophysiology, 39(4), 409–413. https ://
doi.org /10.1017/S0 04 8577202394010
McGre evy, P. D., G eorgevsky, D., Car rasco, J., Valenzue la, M., Duff y,
D. L., & Ser pell, J. A. (2 013). Dog behavior co-v aries with heig ht,
bodyweight and skull shap e. PLoS ONE, 8(12), e80529. https ://doi.
org/10.1371/journ al.pone.0080529
Miklósi,Á.,&Topál,J.(2013).Whatdoesittaketobecome‘bestfriends’?
Evolutionarychangesincaninesocialcompetence.Tre nds in Cognitive
Sciences, 17(6),287–294.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.04.005
R Core Team (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical comput-
ing. Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from https ://cran.r-proje ct.org/doc/
FAQ/R FAQ.html
Ripley, B., Fujiki, N., Okura, M., Mignot, E., & Nishino, S. (20 01).
Hypocretin levels in sporadic and familial cases of canine narcolepsy.
Neurobiology of Disease, 8(3), 525–534. https://doi.org/10.1006/
nbdi.2001.0389
Schmidt,H.S.,&Kaelbling,R.(1971).Thedifferentiallaborator yadapta-
tion of sleep parameters. Biological Psychiatry, 3(1), 33–45.
Shimazono, Y., Horie, T., Yanagisawa, Y., Hori, N., Chikazawa, S., &
Shozuka, K. (1960). 6. The correlation of the rhythmic waves of
the hippocampus with the behaviors of dogs. Neurologia Medico-
Chirurgica, 2(1),82–88.https://doi.org/10.2176/nmc.2.82
Takeuchi, T., & Harad a, E. (2002). A ge-related change s in sleep-wake
rhythm in dog. Behavioural Brain Research, 136(1), 193–199. https ://
doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00123-7
Therneau, T. M. (2015). A Package for Survival Analysis in S.−R Package v,
2.38.
Tobler,I., &Sigg,H.(1986).Long-termmotoractivityrecording ofdogs
and the ef fect of sleep deprivation. Experientia, 42(9), 987–991. https
://doi.org/10.1007/BF01940702
Topál,J.,Román,V.,&Turcsán,B.(2019).Thedog (Canis familiaris) as a
translational model of autism: It is high time we move from prom-
ise to reality. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 10(4),
e1495. ht tps ://doi.o rg/10.100 2/wc s.1495
Toth, L. A., & Bhargava, P. (2013). Animal models of sleep dis-
orders. Comparative Medicine, 63(2), 91–104. https ://doi.
org /10.1007/978-981-10-5981-0 _12
Toussaint,M.,Luthringer,R.,Staner,L.,Muzet,A.,&MacHer,J.P.(2000).
ChangesinEEGpowerdensityduringsleeplaboratoryadaptationin
depressed inpatients. Biological Psychiatry, 47(7),626–633.https://
doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00161-4
Voss,U.(2004).Functionsofsleeparchitectureandtheconceptofpro-
tective fields. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 15(1),33–46.https://doi.
org/10.1515/REVNEURO.2004.15.1.33
Wauquier,A .,Verheyen,J.L.,VanDenBroeck,W.A.E.,&Janssen,P.
A.J.(1979).Visualandcomputer-basedanalysisof24hsleep-wak-
ing patterns in the dog. Electroencephalography and Clinical
Neurophysiology, 46(1),33–4 8.ht tps://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4 694
(79)90 0 47- 6
Zepelin,H.,Siegel,J.M., &Tobler,I.(2005).Mammalian sleep.In M.H.
Kryger,T.Roth&W.C.Dement(Eds.),Principles and prac tice of sleep
medicine(pp.91–100).Philadelphia,PA:WBSaunders.
How to cite this article:ReicherV,KisA,SimorP,BódizsR,
GombosF,GácsiM.Repeatedafternoonsleeprecordings
indicate first-night-effect-like adaptation process in family
dogs. J Sleep Res. 2020;00:e12998. http s ://doi.o rg /10.1111 /
jsr.12 99 8