ArticlePublisher preview available

The yellow stingray (Urobatis jamaicensis) can use magnetic field polarity to orient in space and solve a maze

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract and Figures

Elasmobranch fishes (sharks, skates, and rays) have been hypothesized to use the geomagnetic field (GMF) to maintain a sense of direction as they navigate throughout their environment. However, it is difficult to test the sensory ecology and spatial orientation ability of large highly migratory fishes in the field. Therefore, we performed behavioral conditioning experiments on a small magnetically sensitive species, the yellow stingray (Urobatis jamaicensis), in the laboratory. We trained individuals to use the polarity, or the north–south direction, of the GMF as a cue to orient in space and navigate a T-maze for a food reward. Subjects were split into two groups that learned to associate the direction of magnetic north or south as the indicator of the reward location. Stingrays reached the learning criterion within a mean (± SE) of 158.6 ± 28.4 trials. Subjects were then reverse trained to use the previously unrewarded magnetic stimulus of the opposite polarity as the new cue for the reward location. Overall, the stingrays reached the reversal criterion in significantly fewer trials (120 ± 13.8) compared to the initial procedure. These data show that the yellow stingray can learn to associate changes in GMF polarity with a reward, relearn a behavioral task when the reward contingency is modified, and learn a reversal procedure faster than the initial association. These data support the idea that the yellow stingray, and perhaps other elasmobranchs, might use GMF polarity as a cue to orient and maintain a heading during navigation.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Vol.:(0123456789)
1 3
Marine Biology (2020) 167:36
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-019-3643-9
ORIGINAL PAPER
The yellow stingray (Urobatis jamaicensis) can use magnetic eld
polarity toorient inspace andsolve amaze
KyleC.Newton1,2 · StephenM.Kajiura1
Received: 20 August 2019 / Accepted: 30 December 2019 / Published online: 6 February 2020
© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2020
Abstract
Elasmobranch fishes (sharks, skates, and rays) have been hypothesized to use the geomagnetic field (GMF) to maintain a
sense of direction as they navigate throughout their environment. However, it is difficult to test the sensory ecology and
spatial orientation ability of large highly migratory fishes in the field. Therefore, we performed behavioral conditioning
experiments on a small magnetically sensitive species, the yellow stingray (Urobatis jamaicensis), in the laboratory. We
trained individuals to use the polarity, or the north–south direction, of the GMF as a cue to orient in space and navigate a
T-maze for a food reward. Subjects were split into two groups that learned to associate the direction of magnetic north or
south as the indicator of the reward location. Stingrays reached the learning criterion within a mean (± SE) of 158.6 ± 28.4
trials. Subjects were then reverse trained to use the previously unrewarded magnetic stimulus of the opposite polarity as the
new cue for the reward location. Overall, the stingrays reached the reversal criterion in significantly fewer trials(120 ± 13.8)
compared to the initial procedure. These data show that the yellow stingray can learn to associate changes in GMF polarity
with a reward, relearn a behavioral task when the reward contingency is modified, and learn a reversal procedure faster than
the initial association. These data support the idea that the yellow stingray, and perhaps other elasmobranchs, might use
GMF polarity as a cue to orient and maintain a heading during navigation.
Introduction
Orientation is an integral part of animal navigation where
an organism aligns itself with respect to an external cue
(Berthold 2001; Gould 1998) to maintain a desired head-
ing. However, calculating a heading requires thatthe animal
know its current position relative to that of its goal so that it
can determine the correct direction in which to travel (Gould
1998; 2004). The animal can then use an appropriate envi-
ronmental cue, such as visual landmarks, localized sounds
or odor gradients, the position of the sun, or the direc-
tion of thegeomagnetic field (GMF) as an external point
of reference to maintain the correct orientation. Animals
can use different types of cues to form distinct cognitive
compasses and employ them as needed when environmen-
tal stimuli cease to propagate and become unreliable (Able
1991; Gould 1998). The physical nature of a cue and how it
behaves in a medium, such as seawater, will determine how
effective that cue is for navigating over a given spatiotem-
poral scale. Cues that originate from localized sources tend
to diminish rapidly with space and time, which makes them
effective beacons or landmarks (Shettleworth and Sutton
2005; Cheng 2012) for relatively short distance navigation.
Conversely, global cues such as celestial rotation or GMF
polarity fluctuate very little over small spatiotemporal scales
and are well suited for navigational tasks that can last several
months and span thousands of kilometers.
The GMF has polarity, or a north–south directional com-
ponent, because on the surface of the Earthit is emitted from
the magnetic north pole located in the southern hemisphere
and terminates atthe magnetic south pole in the northern
hemisphere. The GMF at any geographic location can be
described by a vector with an overall intensity of 20–70 µT
and an inclination angle (measured relative to the surface of
the Earth) that ranges from + 90° to 90°. These quantities
Responsible Editor: J. Carlson.
Reviewed by undisclosed experts.
* Kyle C. Newton
kyle.newton@wustl.edu
1 Department ofBiological Science, Florida Atlantic
University, 777 Glades Road, BocaRaton, FL33431, USA
2 Present Address: Department ofOtolaryngology, Washington
University School ofMedicine, 660 South Euclid Avenue,
Campus Box8115, St.Louis, MO63110, USA
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
... Many marine taxa are regarded as magneto-sensitive, whilst some organisms are electrosensitive and a select group, in particular elasmobranchs, are both (Anderson et al., 2017), (Meyer et al., 2005), (Keller et al., 2021a). Magneto-sensitivity is mainly assumed to be used for long-distance navigation or orientation (Kalmijn, 1978), (Molteno and Kennedy, 2009), (Klimley, 1993a), (Newton and Kajiura, 2020), which leads to the prediction that the EMFs from SPC export cables that transport electricity to the shore could interfere with migration along the coast. During migration adults will encounter multiple cables of varying EMF strengths which might cause delays as has been shown in migrating eel (Westerberg and Lagerfelt, 2008). ...
... For example, in the laboratory, yellow stingrays (Urobatis jamaicensis) can learn behavioral responses based on reward contingencies, indicating the ability to associate a stimulus with a reward and to discriminate among stimuli as to which yield rewards and which do not. They were also able to relearn the task upon modification of the reward structure, and do so at a faster rate than the initial learning curve (Newton and Kajiura, 2020). In the wild, blacktip sharks (Carcharhinus limbatus) integrate input from multiple sensory channels (smell, electroreception, vision, etc.) to navigate over both small-and large-scale distances (Gardiner et al., 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Keystone species populations, including shark and ray taxonomic groups, are declining due to numerous threats. A better understanding of how individual belief structures inform pro-conservation behavioral intentions is therefore critical to foster support for their conservation and the greater biodiversity of world ecosystems. This study conducted a survey to establish the general public’s cognitive beliefs about the mental and affective capacities of two species groups (sharks and stingrays) known to cause injury to humans, and their effects on different behavioral intentions to support their conservation. Results revealed that a range of different behavioral intentions to support animal conservation are driven by a complex interplay of cognitive beliefs, and some effects were specific to a particular species group while others were not. Beliefs in animals’ cognitive awareness and their emotional abilities were predictive of individuals’ self-reported desire to learn more about the animals and their conservation. Beliefs in animals’ cognitive and emotional capacities were predictive of supporting conservation in terms of voting and donating to their conservation. Finally, beliefs in animals’ reasoning abilities were predictive of different behavioral expressions of tolerance, and support for their conservation via voting only. Despite the potential injurious capabilities of these species, these findings have important implications for educational and conservation programs to emphasize or modify specific beliefs in order to facilitate particular pro-conservation behaviors for maximum benefits to conservation efforts.
... In addition, elasmobranchs are able to perceive magnetic field (e.g. Kalmijn, 1982;Meyer et al., 2005;Newton & Kajiura, 2020) and one study reported that a shark utilized its perception of magnetic field for navigation (Keller et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Aim: Despite extensive studies of phenological shifts in migration by climate change and driving factors of migration, a few issues remain unresolved. In particular, little is known about the complex effects of driving factors on migration with interactions and nonlinearity, and partitioning of the effects of factors into spatial, temporal, and spatio-temporal effects. Here, we aim to elucidate migration pattern as well as its driving factors under climate change. Location: Western North Pacific. Taxon: North Pacific spiny dogfish Squalus suckleyi. Methods: We first examined long-term changes in the timing and geographic location of migration by applying the Barrier model, a spatio-temporal model, to c. 5-decade time series data (1972–2019) for the presence/absence of spiny dogfish in the west- ern North Pacific. We then evaluated the spatial, temporal and spatio-temporal ef- fects of driving factors (fish productivity, sea surface temperature [SST], depth and magnetic field) on seasonal occurrence patterns using a machine learning model and an interpretable machine learning technique. Results: The migration area did not change over c. 5-decades, whereas the migra- tion timing advanced by a month after 2000. The spatial effects of magnetic field and depth were consistently large and the spatial and spatio-temporal effects of SST increased in the migration season, even though the temporal effect of SST was con- sistently weak. Main Conclusions: The migration area of spiny dogfish was stable over time because of the effect of magnetic field and a strong preference for submarine topography, whereas the migration timing advanced as a result of tracking a suitable location based on SST, which increased sharply after 2000. Therefore, temperature and other factors simultaneously influence migration under climate change, highlighting the im- portance of considering both biotic and abiotic factors and understanding the under- lying processes in predicting future impacts of climate change on species distribution.
Chapter
This chapter highlights the literature on electroreception before 1974 by Faraday, Lissmann & Machin, Murray, Parker &Van Heusen, and Dijkgraaf & Kalmijn. The author discusses the literature from 1974 to 2021 that conflicts with his own scientific writings. Included are the author’s criteria for establishing an electric and a magnetic sense in sharks and rays. That the ampullae of Lorenzini have the physical ability to provide the animals with navigational information cannot be properly grasped without a comprehensive understanding of the physical concepts from the animals’ perspective. The lack of the necessary understanding of the physical principles has led to erroneous physical interpretations and related unsound behavior experiments. Through step-by-step explanations of the underlying physics, this chapter addresses the inaccuracies, misunderstandings, and shortcomings of alternative theories that have been put forward in the literature.
Preprint
To mitigate the effect of climate change, the demand for marine renewable energy infrastructure is increasing worldwide. These facilities convert the kinetic energy of offshore wind, waves, tides or currents into electricity that is transported to shore through alternating current (AC), or direct current (DC), subsea cables. Energized cables emit magnetic fields radially into the surrounding seawater and induce secondary electric field artifacts. Thus, anthropogenic electromagnetic fields (EMFs) can alter the local geomagnetic landscape and may impact the behavior of EMF sensitive elasmobranchs that use the geomagnetic field (GMF) to navigate. We magnetically displaced big ( Beringraja binoculata ) and longnose ( Caliraja rhina ) skates to determine their response to GMF navigational cues. We then exposed skates to model AC and DC subsea cables to determine their response to fluctuating and constant EMFs. We then exposed skates to GMF and EMF to understand how EMF-AC and EMF-DC impacts GMF mediated behaviors. Experiments were recorded on video and 3D pose estimation software tracked the velocity, body angle, and spatial use of skates to quantify their behavioral response to magnetic stimuli. Big skates exposed to GMF and EMF were more active and showed the greatest changes in spatial use, velocity, and body angle. Conversely, longnose skates were less active but showed significant magnetic stimulus-specific changes in their movement kinematics. Thus, skates exposed to GMF and EMF showed stimulus- and species-specific changes in their behavior. Future studies will leverage these lab-based results to interpret the response of free-ranging elasmobranchs to EMF. GRAPHICAL ABSTRACT
Preprint
Full-text available
Subsea power cables cause electromagnetic fields (EMFs) into the marine environment. Elasmobranchs (rays, skates, sharks) are particularly sensitive to EMFs as they use electromagnetic-receptive sensory systems for orientation, navigation and locating conspecifics or buried prey. Cables may intersect with egg laying sites, foraging habitat and migration routes of elasmobranchs and the effects of encountering EMFs on species of elasmobranchs are largely unknown. Demonstrated behavioural effects are attraction, disturbance and indifference, depending on EMF characteristics, exposed life stage, exposure level and duration. We estimated exposure levels of elasmobranchs to subsea cable EMFs, based on modelled magnetic fields in the Dutch Continental Shelf and compared these to reported elasmobranch sensory sensitivity ranges and experimental effect levels. We conclude that the risk from subsea power cables has a large uncertainty and varies per life stage and species ecology. Based on estimated no-observed effect levels (from 10-3 to 10-1 microT) we discuss what will probably be the most affected species and life stage for six common benthic elasmobranchs in the Southern North Sea. We identify critical knowledge gaps for reducing the uncertainty in the risk assessments for EMFs effects on elasmobranchs.
Article
Full-text available
Using the expression of the immediate early gene (IEG) egr-1 as a neuronal activity marker, brain regions potentially involved in learning and long-term memory functions in the grey bamboo shark were assessed with respect to selected visual discrimination abilities. Immunocytochemistry revealed a significant up-regulation of egr-1 expression levels in a small region of the telencephalon of all trained sharks (i.e., ‘early’ and ‘late learners’, ‘recallers’) when compared to three control groups (i.e., ‘controls’, ‘undisturbed swimmers’, ‘constant movers’). There was also a well-defined difference in egr-1 expression patterns between the three control groups. Additionally, some staining was observed in diencephalic and mesencephalic sections; however, staining here was weak and occurred only irregularly within and between groups. Therefore, it could have either resulted from unintentional cognitive or non-cognitive inducements (i.e., relating to the mental processes of perception, learning, memory, and judgment, as contrasted with emotional and volitional processes) rather than being a training effect. Present findings emphasize a relationship between the training conditions and the corresponding egr-1 expression levels found in the telencephalon of Chiloscyllium griseum. Results suggest important similarities in the neuronal plasticity and activity-dependent IEG expression of the elasmobranch brain with other vertebrate groups. The presence of the egr-1 gene seems to be evolutionarily conserved and may therefore be particularly useful for identifying functional neural responses within this group.
Article
Full-text available
An impediment to a mechanistic understanding of how some species sense the geomagnetic field ("magnetoreception") is the lack of vertebrate genetic models that exhibit well-characterized magnetoreceptive behavior and are amenable to whole-brain analysis. We investigated the genetic model organisms zebrafish and medaka, whose young stages are transparent and optically accessible. In an unfamiliar environment, adult fish orient according to the directional change of a magnetic field even in darkness. To enable experiments also in juveniles, we applied slowly oscillating magnetic fields, aimed at generating conflicting sensory inputs during exploratory behavior. Medaka (but not zebrafish) increase their locomotor activity in this assay. Complementary brain activity mapping reveals neuronal activation in the lateral hindbrain during magnetic stimulation. These comparative data support magnetoreception in teleosts, provide evidence for a light-independent mechanism, and demonstrate the usefulness of zebrafish and medaka as genetic vertebrate models for studying the biophysical and neuronal mechanisms underlying magnetoreception.
Article
Full-text available
Elasmobranch fishes are among a broad range of taxa believed to gain positional information and navigate using the earth’s magnetic field, yet in sharks, much remains uncertain regarding the sensory receptors and pathways involved, or the exact nature of perceived stimuli. Captive sandbar sharks, Carcharhinus plumbeus were conditioned to respond to presentation of a magnetic stimulus by seeking out a target in anticipation of reward (food). Sharks in the study demonstrated strong responses to magnetic stimuli, making significantly more approaches to the target (p = < 0.01) during stimulus activation (S+) than before or after activation (S−). Sharks exposed to reversible magnetosensory impairment were less capable of discriminating changes to the local magnetic field, with no difference seen in approaches to the target under the S+ and S− conditions (p = 0.375). We provide quantified detection and discrimination thresholds of magnetic stimuli presented, and quantify associated transient electrical artefacts. We show that the likelihood of such artefacts serving as the stimulus for observed behavioural responses was low. These impairment experiments support hypotheses that magnetic field perception in sharks is not solely performed via the electrosensory system, and that putative magnetoreceptor structures may be located in the naso-olfactory capsules of sharks.
Article
Full-text available
Migration allows animals to track the environmental conditions that maximize growth, survival, and reproduction [1–3]. Improved understanding of the mechanisms underlying migrations allows for improved management of species and ecosystems [1–4]. For centuries, the catadromous European eel (Anguilla anguilla) has provided one of Europe’s most important fisheries and has sparked considerable scientific inquiry, most recently owing to the dramatic collapse of juvenile recruitment [5]. Larval eels are transported by ocean currents associated with the Gulf Stream System from Sargasso Sea breeding grounds to coastal and freshwater habitats from North Africa to Scandinavia [6, 7]. After a decade or more, maturing adults migrate back to the Sargasso Sea, spawn, and die [8]. However, the migratory mechanisms that bring juvenile eels to Europe and return adults to the Sargasso Sea remain equivocal [9, 10]. Here, we used a ‘‘magnetic displacement’’ experiment [11, 12] to show that the orientation of juvenile eels varies in response to subtle differences in magnetic field intensity and inclination angle along their marine migration route. Simulations using an ocean circulation model revealed that even weakly swimming in the experimentally observed directions at the locations corresponding to the magnetic displacements would increase entrainment of juvenile eels into the Gulf Stream System. These findings provide new insight into the migration ecology and recruitment dynamics of eels and suggest that an adaptive magnetic map, tuned to large-scale features of ocean circulation, facilitates the vast oceanic migrations of the Anguilla genus [7, 13, 14].
Article
Full-text available
Elasmobranch fishes (sharks, skates, and rays) have been hypothesized to use the geomagnetic field as a cue for orienting and navigating across a wide range of spatial scales. Magnetoreception has been demonstrated in many invertebrate and vertebrate taxa, including elasmobranchs, but this sensory modality and the cognitive abilities of cartilaginous fishes are poorly studied. Wild caught yellow stingrays, Urobatis jamaicensis (N = 8), underwent conditioning to associate a magnetic stimulus with a food reward in order to elicit foraging behaviors. Behavioral conditioning consisted of burying magnets and non-magnetic controls at random locations within a test arena and feeding stingrays as they passed over the hidden magnets. The location of the magnets and controls was changed for each trial, and all confounding sensory cues were eliminated. The stingrays learned to discriminate the magnetic stimuli within a mean of 12.6 ± 0.7 SE training sessions of four trials per session. Memory probes were conducted at intervals between 90 and 180 days post-learning criterion, and six of eight stingrays completed the probes with a ≥75% success rate and minimum latency to complete the task. These results show the fastest rate of learning and longest memory window for any batoid (skate or ray) to date. This study demonstrates that yellow stingrays, and possibly other elasmobranchs, can use a magnetic stimulus as a geographic marker for the location of resources and is an important step toward understanding whether these fishes use geomagnetic cues during spatial navigation tasks in the natural environment.
Article
Full-text available
Animal movements, in particular residency or return migrations (collectively defined as ‘philopatry’), can shape population structure and have implications for management. This review examines the evidence for philopatry in batoids, which are some of the least understood and most threatened vertebrates, and updates a prior review of the same in sharks. Evidence for philopatry in batoids was found in 46 studies, including 31 species that involve 11 species complexes. Batoid philopatry research has lagged behind shark philopatry research, with the annual publication rate of shark philopatry studies in the last 5 yr (17 yr-1) being more than twice that of batoids (7 yr-1). Philopatry research on both sharks and rays is taxonomically skewed: <50% of elasmobranch families are represented. Research is also skewed towards charismatic megafauna (white sharks, whale sharks, and manta rays), while the batoid philopatry literature is biased towards ‘Near Threatened’ species, even though approx. 47.5% of batoids are considered to be ‘Data Deficient’ by the IUCN. Limited evidence was found for residency in batoids, contrary to popular assumptions that they are sedentary, and there was limited evidence for sex-differentiated movements. Hypothesis-driven research, longer study durations, more taxon and life stage diverse studies, and consistent use of philopatry terminology are needed to advance the fields of batoid philopatry and conservation. Given strong evidence of philopatry in some species of batoids, management should proceed at the local scale until more studies are conducted.
Book
The question of how birds migrate over enormous distances with apparently minimal guidance continues to excite both professional and amateur ornithologists. Nearly ten years have elapsed since Peter Berthold, a leading researcher in the field, wrote the first edition of this highly readable and fascinating book. During that time the field has advanced by strides, so that this new edition has been extensively revised, expanded, and updated. No other book exists that brings together the vast amount of information that is available on the subject of bird migration, so that the book will be an inspiration to birdwatchers, naturalists, and ornithologists alike.
Article
Background: Previous studies have shown that stingrays can exploit a variety of spatial learning strategies, including directional, landmark, and place learning. In these studies, allocentric and global strategies were preferred over egocentric information and local strategies. Elasmobranchs represent the oldest extant vertebrate group. Considering their key phylogenetic position, we wished to address the evolution of spatial strategies as well as the significance of specific strategies over others. Aim: Investigate cue preferences in two types of maze experiments by assessing the significance of single landmarks and directional cues (both egocentric strategies) for orientation. Organism: Freshwater stingrays (Potamotrygon motoro). Methods: In two independent experiments, five juvenile stingrays were trained to find their way through a maze while being given both landmark and directional information.Transfer tests (in which conflicts between both kinds of information were created) showed which cues the rays used preferentially to reach their goal. Results: All rays successfully learned to master the mazes in both experiments, so all were capable of learning and the application of spatial memory. Rays placed more importance on directional information than on landmark cues. This applied to abstract cues (provided in the form of cards featuring symbols) as well as ‘natural’ landmark cues (in the form of plants). Once rays were trained to use directional information, it was not possible to reverse their preference and get them to use landmark information instead. Conclusion: Freshwater stingrays learn quickly and prefer directional information to single landmark cues.
Article
Preferred direction of motion under influence of geomagnetic field and its modifications was registered in zebrafish (Danio rerio) raised in laboratory culture and in roach (Rutilus rutilus) from the Rybinsk Reservoir. In the geomagnetic field, specimens of zebrafish prefer two opposite directions oriented towards the north and south, while they prefer towards east and west at 90° turning of the horizontal component of geomagnetic field. The specimens of roach in the geomagnetic field prefer only the direction oriented towards east–northeast. This direction coincides with the direction along the canal where roach was sampled to the main river channel part of the Rybinsk Reservoir. At 90° rotation of the horizontal component of geomagnetic field, the direction turns to the south–southeast. The reasons for selection of certain directions in the geomagnetic field are discussed.