Content uploaded by Gilad Feldman
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Gilad Feldman on Jul 02, 2020
Content may be subject to copyright.
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 1
Abstract
[Note: we wrote the registered report as if data was already collected and hypotheses were supported. This will
be changed after data search/coding/analyses if that is not the case]
In this meta-analysis registered report we examined the relationship between Dark Triad
personality traits (i.e., psychopathy, Machiavellianism, narcissism; Paulhus & Williams;
2002) and sadism (Chabrol, Van Leeuwen, Rodgers, & Séjourné, 2009) with personal values
(Schwartz, 1992). Across xx studies, we found consistent associations between four dark
traits and the four high-order values dimensions, with slightly different patterns for each of
the traits. Dark Triad traits showing the strongest positive correlations with self-enhancement
and openness to change dimensions, and negative associations with the opposite dimensions
on the values circumplex - self-transcendence and conservation. Shape consistency for the
dark triad associations was stronger for self-enhancement versus self-transcendence values
tension, than for the openness-to-change versus conservation values. Sadism too showed
similar relationship profiles with value dimensions.
Keywords: Dark triad, Personal values, meta-analysis, registered report
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 2
Values and the dark side: Meta-analysis of links between
Dark Triad and sadism traits with personal values
Introduction
The last two decades have witnessed the emergence of a large body of literature on the
Dark Triad model with empirical studies examining their structure, genetic origin (Vernon,
Villani, Vickers, & Harris, 2008), associations with other personality models (Jones &
Paulhus, 2011; Lee & Ashton, 2014), their impact on attitudes (Egan, Hughes, & Palmer,
2015; Kavanagh, Signal, & Taylor, 2013), and behavioural outcomes (Harms, Spain, &
Hannah, 2011; Hodson, Hogg, & MacInnis, 2009; Jonason, Li, Webster & Schmitt, 2009). In
recent years, researchers became especially interested in a motivational underpinning of the
Dark Triad, with initial studies mapping associations between the triad and values, long-term
motivational goals.
Presently, the intuitive predictions of the Dark Triad’s associations with the less social
and more hedonistic values received some support in the literature (Jonason, Strosser, Kroll,
Duineveld, & Baruffi, 2015; Kajonius, Persson, & Jonason, 2015). The relative strength and
direction of these associations tends to vary across traits and motivations, as for example the
association between narcissism and either conservation or self-transcendence values from the
Schwartz values framework (Balakrishnan, Plouffe, & Saklosfske, 2017; Güngör, Eksi, &
Arikak, 2012; Rogoza, Wyszyńska, Maćkiewicz, & Ciecuch, 2016). Thus far there have been
no systematic mappings of these associations and aggregated comparisons of their relative
strengths to help form a unified framework connecting distinct profiles for each trait, to
possibly try and reconcile conceptual conflicts and empirically overlapping findings (Muris,
Merckelbach, Otgaar, & Meljer; 2017). Here, we posit that whilst these traits may possess
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 3
some empirical overlap, each trait may differ in their motivational underpinning and values
associations.
In recent years, scholars have also begun examining trait sadism as an extension to the
Dark Triad model, showing its relative independence to the Dark Triad traits and showed its
prediction power towards socially undesirable behaviours as well (Buckels, Jones, & Paulhus,
2013; Chabrol et al., 2009). Sadism has also been examined both independently (Kinnunen,
Lindeman, & Verkasalo, 2016) as well as alongside the Dark Triad (Balakrishnan et al., 2017)
in terms of its relationship with value associations.
The present investigation aims to contribute to the literature on value-trait associations
(Güngör et al., 2012; Kajonius et al., 2015; Ljubin-Golub & Sokić, 2016; Persson, 2014;
Roccas, Sagiv, Schwartz, & Knafo, 2002; Rogoza et al., 2016) and extend to examine the
overall pattern of associations between personal values with Dark Triad traits and sadism. We
begin with a theoretical account of the Dark Triad model, the Schwartz value framework, and
their possible links, and then then proceed to report a pre-registered meta-analysis of the
literature on the relationships between Dark Triad traits and personal values.
Dark Triad
The Dark Triad is a cluster of three malevolent personality traits, characterised by
their anti-social and self-oriented attributes (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). Briefly, psychopathy
is characterised by impulsive, thrill-seeking, and callous nature, Machiavellianism by
manipulative, glibness, and cynic nature, and narcissism by self-loving, grandiose, and
entitled nature. The Dark Triad model identified these three key constructs as the most
prominent dark personality traits that had were covered by a large body of literature and were
highly aversive yet still not entirely outside accepted norms (Furnham, Richards, & Paulhus,
2013).
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 4
Over a wide range of contexts, empirical literature examining these traits mostly
associated them with undesirable outcomes such as exploitative mating strategies (Jonason,
Girgis, & Milne-Home, 2017), counterproductive work behaviour (O’Boyle Jr, Forsyth,
Banks, & McDaniel, 2012), abusive supervision at work (Wisse & Sleebos, 2016), and
differentiated cheating strategies (Baughman, Jonason, Lyons, & Vernon, 2014). However,
these traits have also been associated with positive outcomes that enable the individual to “get
ahead of” but not “get along with” others in the workplace (Furnham et al., 2013, p.206;
Hogan, 2007). This is particularly seen in the corporate world, where trait psychopathy and
narcissism have been found to be positively related to leadership (Mathieu, Neumann, Hare,
& Babiak, 2014; Rosenthal & Pittinsky, 2006).
Since then, scholars have sought to extend the Dark Triad model to include sadism,
the tendency to enjoy cruelty at the cost of others (Baumeister & Campbell, 1999). Examples
of everyday sadism include situations such as viewing violent movies or enjoying combat
fights (Baumeister & Campbell, 1999). Calling this the Dark Tetrad, validation work has
depicted sadism as being sufficiently distinct from the other traits, warranting it as a fourth
construct in this dark cluster of traits (Book et al., 2016; Mendedović & Petrović, 2015).
Sadism has been linked to undesirable outcomes, such as acts of cruelty (Buckels et al.,
2013), and violent video game preferences (Greitemeyer, 2015).
In Table 1 we summarize a list of common measures used for the Dark Triad.
Structurally, there has been increasing concern about the Dark Triad traits sharing high inter-
correlations (Muris et al., 2017). Whilst this supports the original conceptualisation that these
traits are “evil allies of personality” (Muris et al. 2017, p. 189), from a factor perspective it
remains contentious as to whether these traits are sufficiently distinct for the triadic model to
hold. Multiple meta-analytic examinations posit that the Dark Triad should be reduced to the
Dark Dyad, with Machiavellianism collapsed under psychopathy as they share substantial
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 5
overlap in variance, and narcissism remaining a distinct factor of its own (Muris et al. 2017;
O’Boyle Jr, Forsyth, Banks, Story, & White, 2014; Vize, Lynam, Collison, & Miller, 2018).
This proposal is reasonable, given that both psychopathy and Machiavellianism are more
oriented towards affecting others with their actions, such as political behaviour at work (Liu
& Liu, 2018), or cruelty to others (Dadds, Whiting, & Hawes, 2006), whereas narcissism is
more self-oriented and predicts more behaviours that affects self-presentation, such as
engagement in selecting Facebook profile pictures to enhance likeability (Kapidzic, 2013).
Especially when the shared variance between the Dark Triad traits have been taken into
accounted for, meta-analyses have also suggested that psychopathy appears to be the core of
the model and subsumes narcissism as well (Glenn & Sellbom, 2015, Muris et al., 2017).
Despite empirical findings showing substantial overlap between the Dark Triad traits,
many recent reviews and meta-analyses still assume that each trait remains conceptually
distinct, resulting in dissonance about the validity of this model (Furnham et al., 2013; Muris
et al. 2017; O’Boyle Jr et al., 2014; Vize et al., 2018). Further, although sadism is shown to be
empirically distinct, the high overlap it shares with the Dark Triad has been argued to be a
signal that it is a manifestation of the model, rather than a disposition (Book et al., 2016). A
clearer depiction of whether sadism stands as a construct of equivalent level to the Dark
Triad, or if it can be subsumed under the umbrella of traits as a shared secondary tendency is
needed. We hope that this meta-analysis may offer an additional perspective in understanding
where both similarity and variance can co-exist in this model – by examining associations
with personal value orientations.
Personal values
Personal values are long-term motivational goals that people deem important in their
lives (Schwartz, 1992). Values are generally stable in nature, yet they can also be reordered
depending on one’s social experiences and normative expectations (Bilsky & Schwartz, 1994,
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 6
Rokeach, 1973). Currently, the dominant model in values literature is Schwartz’s personal
values theory (1992). Traditionally, this model postulated ten universal values (power,
achievement, hedonism, stimulation, self-direction, universalism, benevolence, conformity,
security, and tradition). The values follow a consistent circular structure such that values that
underlie a similar universal motivation will be positioned closer to each other. Within the
Schwartz circumplex model, values that are positioned opposite to each other can be
considered as values with opposing motivations. The ten values are nested under four higher
order dimensions that are listed on two orthogonal axes and are considered as two sets of
underlying general motivations that oppose each other - self-transcendence versus self-
enhancement; and openness to change versus conservation. This value model has received
universal support (Schwartz, 2011), and has been found to be considered as a system of
related values, rather than independently distinct values (Schwartz, 1996). The values
circumplex structure is outlined in Figure 1 and sample items from the 10 values categories
are provided in Table 2. In Table 3 we summarize a list of commonly used measures for the
Schwartz values model.
The stable motivational quality of values distinguishes them from other goal-specific
constructs (Maio, 2010). Though considered abstract, values have been shown to predict a
range of value-serving attitudes and behaviours (Bardi & Schwartz, 2003; Grunert, & Juhl,
1995; Hitlin & Piliavin, 2004; Lipponen, Bardi, & Haapamäki, 2010; Roccas & Sagiv, 2010).
Of specific interest to this meta-analysis, values predict unethical decision making and
behaviour (Feldman, Chao, Farh, & Bardi, 2015; Fritzsche & Oz, 2007; Menesini, Nocetini,
& Camodeca, 2013), which have previously been associated with those higher on the Dark
Triad.
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 7
Relationship between traits and values
Despite the similar conceptualisation between traits and values, there is ample
empirical evidence suggesting these are distinct constructs (Fischer & Boer, 2015; Kajonius et
al., 2015; Roccas et al., 2002). Bilsky and Schwartz (1994, p.165) aptly distinguishes traits
and values in three ways, “(1) Personality traits are typically seen as descriptions of observed
patterns of behaviour, whereas values are criteria individuals use to judge the desirability of
behaviour, people, and events. (2) Personality-traits vary in terms of how much of a
characteristic individuals exhibit, whereas values vary in terms of the importance that
individuals attribute to particular goals. (3) Personality describe actions presumed to flow
from ‘what persons are like’ regardless of their intentions, whereas values refer to the
individual’s intentional goals that are available to consciousness.” Taken together, the main
difference between traits and values, is that traits signal the predisposition to act, but values
signal the motivations that one holds internally.
Recent meta-analyses (Fischer & Boer, 2015; Parks-Leduc, Feldman, & Bardi, 2015)
further elaborated on the similarities and differences between the two constructs and
presented theoretically meaningful associations between the Big Five personality traits and
Schwartz’s values. Personal values were shown to be more strongly related to cognitive based
traits than emotionally based traits (Parks-Leduc et al., 2015), and contextual factors were
shown to affect the association between these two constructs (Fischer & Boer, 2015).
Relationship between the Dark Triad and personal values
There is growing interest in the link between the Dark Triad and personal values
(Balakrishnan et al., 2017; Güngör et al., 2012; Jonason et al., 2015; Kajonius et al., 2015;
Persson, 2014). Existing studies have so far shown that Dark Triad traits are positively related
with self-enhancement and openness to change values, and negatively related to self-
transcendence values and conservation values (Kaufman, Yaden, Hyde, & Tsukayama, 2019).
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 8
These findings fit with the characterization of the Dark Triad as being self-centred, antisocial,
sensation-seeking (Crysel, Crosier, & Webster, 2013), and callous about the well-being or
interests of others (Jonason et al., 2015). However, despite the Dark Triad sharing similar
value orientations, research in this area has yet to evaluate how each trait is unique in how
strong they might be motivated by a specific value. We believe that this reconciles how the
Dark Triad can both share overlapping variance yet remain distinct by their difference in
value prioritisation.
In Figure 2 we outlined an initial simplified suggested mapping of the grouped three
traits on the values circumplex structure. In the next sections we hypothesize on finer-grained
associations further differentiating between the Dark Triad traits based on their expected
values associations.
Research aim and hypotheses
Broadly, we seek to examine: 1) The pattern of relationships between each of the Dark
Triad traits and personal values, and 2) factors that moderate the associations. At its core, the
Dark Triad has been shown to be strongly representative of or associated with low
agreeableness (Jonason, Kaufman, Webster, & Geher, 2013; Vize, Collinson, Miller, &
Lynam, 2020). Further, meta-analyses found negative associations between agreeableness and
self-enhancement value dimensions, and positive associations between agreeableness and
self-transcendence and conservation value dimensions (Fischer & Boer, 2015; Parks-Leduc et
al., 2015). As such, we would largely expect that the Dark Triad traits will be positively
related to self-enhancement, and negatively related to self-transcendence and conservation.
However, as we go in depth to theorise on the relation between each trait with value
dimensions, we might expect variation between our overall expectations and individual
hypotheses relating to each trait with each value dimension.
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 9
Psychopathy
Psychopathy is characterised by callousness, impulsivity, and a lack of remorse
(Paulhus & Williams, 2002). The lack of inhibition to engage in immoral acts for gains
(Schouten & Silver, 2012) can result in parasitic efforts to gain benefits at the expense of
others at work (Boddy, 2006; Babiak & Hare, 2006). This suggests that psychopathy is a
highly self-oriented trait, motivated by self-gains. This is supported by recent studies that
consistently show positive associations between psychopathy and values that are more
oriented towards enhancing oneself, such as power and achievement (Glenn, Efferson, Iyer, &
Graham, 2017; Jonason et al., 2015; Persson, 2014). Further, the need for stimulation and
hedonism are highly characteristic of individuals high in trait psychopathy, as exhibited by
higher tendencies to engage in substance abuse (Benning, Patrick, Hicks, Blonigen, &
Krueger, 2003). This is also supported by positive correlations found consistently shown
between psychopathy and openness to change values (Balakrishnan et al., 2017; Jonason et
al., 2015; Kajonius et al., 2015; Ljubin-Golub & Sokić, 2016).
Individuals high in trait psychopathy also tend to value the welfare of others to a much
lesser degree than their own. Jonason and Zeigler-Hill (2018) found negative associations
between psychopathy and the motivation to develop and maintain good relationships with
others. Psychopathy was also associated with being less motivated in focusing on the
partner’s needs during sex (Smith, Overup, & Webster, 2019). This supports existing findings
that psychopathy is negatively related to self-transcendence and conservation values (Jonason
et al., 2015; Kajonius et al., 2015; Ljubin-Golub & Sokić, 2016).
We hypothesise the following relationships between psychopathy with personal
values:
Hypothesis 1: Psychopathy is associated with self-enhancement (+), openness
to change, (+), self-transcendence (-), and conservation (-) values.
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 10
Based on our predicted orthogonal hypotheses for psychopathy with the value
dimensions, we also expect a sinusoidal pattern displayed when these observed value
associations are mapped on the circumplex structure of the value model. Following Boer and
Fischer’s shape consistency method (2013), and based on past literature finding strongest
associations with the self-enhancement and self-transcendence values dimensions (Kajonius
et al., 2015; Persson, 2014) we expect that psychopathy will be sinusoidal shaped where the
peaks and valleys are observed in the self-enhancement versus self-transcendence dimensions
(SET shaped).
Hypothesis 2: A sinusoid correlational pattern is observed between
psychopathy and the values in order of the circumplex structure, with the self-
enhancement and self-transcendence tension having stronger associations with
psychopathy compared to openness to change versus conservation tension (i.e.,
stronger negative SET consistency shape than CO consistency shape).
Machiavellianism
With increasing evidence that Machiavellianism highly overlaps with psychopathy in
variance, and that it might even be a subset of the latter trait (Muris et al., 2017; O’Boyle et
al., 2014), we predict that Machiavellianism will be similar to psychopathy in terms of the
relationships held with personal values. However, the strengths of the correlations for
Machiavellianism may be weaker than the correlations found for psychopathy, which we will
expand on more in the exploratory hypothesis in the next section below.
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 11
We hypothesise the following relationships between Machiavellianism with personal
values:
Hypothesis 3: Machiavellianism is associated with self-enhancement (+),
openness to change (+), self-transcendence (-), and conservation (-) values.
We again expect a sinusoidal pattern for Machiavellianism to be similar to that of
psychopathy. Therefore:
Hypothesis 4: A sinusoid correlational pattern is observed between
Machiavellianism and the values in order of the circumplex structure, with the
self-enhancement and self-transcendence tension having stronger associations
with Machiavellianism compared to openness to change versus conservation
tension (i.e., stronger negative SET consistency shape than CO consistency
shape).
Narcissism
Compared to psychopathy and Machiavellianism, narcissism is rather paradoxical in
that individuals high in this trait are characterised as “disagreeable extraverts” (Rogoza et al.
2016, p.88), as it is associated with both antisocial and prosocial behaviours (Kauten & Barry;
2016; Konrath, Ho, & Zarins, 2016; Grijalva et al., 2015). Grijalva et al. (2015) posits the
paradoxical nature of narcissism to stem from its different facets leading to different
outcomes separately. Similarly, studies have found that different facets of narcissism were
associated with different value dimensions (Güngör et al., 2012; Rogoza et al., 2016).
Whereas narcissism consistently positively correlates with self-enhancement and openness to
change values, there are mixed findings as to whether it is on the whole associated with
conservation or self-transcendence values, and if so, whether the association is positive or
negative (Balakrishnan et al., 2017; Güngör et al., 2012; Jonason et al., 2015; Kajonius et al.,
2015; Rogoza et al., 2016).
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 12
We hypothesise the following relationships between narcissism with personal values:
Hypothesis 5: Narcissism is associated with self-enhancement (+) and
openness to change (+) values.
The positive association between narcissism and extraversion further lends support to
our hypothesis (Muris et al., 2017), given that extraversion is consistently positively related to
self-enhancement and openness to change values (Fischer & Boer, 2015; Parks-Leduc et al.,
2015). Additionally, there is no consistent relationship found between extraversion and
conservation or self-transcendence. Further, given extant mixed findings on the associations
for narcissism with conservation and self-transcendence values, it is unclear what sinusoid
correlational pattern would emerge for the relationship between narcissism and the high-order
values dimensions on the circumplex structure. We thus make no predictions about the
correlational pattern.
Sadism
As the extension to the Dark Triad model, sadism is perceived as a tendency to engage
in cruelty towards others to derive pleasure. This trait is other-oriented and focuses on how
the individuals enjoy themselves at the cost of others. Studies examining relationships support
this argument, by showing negative associations with self-transcendence and conservation
values (Balakrishnan et al., 2017; Kinnunen et al., 2016). Further, because sadism is
associated with the act of achieving dominance (Balakrishnan et al., 2017; Buckels et al.,
2013; Hébert & Weaver, 2014), sadism could be associated with self-enhancement and
because they are motivated to achieve gaining power, in order to be in a state of hedonistic
enjoyment. However, as little is known about how sadism is related to openness to change
values, we make no prediction about this relationship.
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 13
We hypothesise the following relationships between sadism with personal values:
Hypothesis 6: Sadism is associated with self-enhancement (+), self-
transcendence (-), and conservation (-) values.
Further, given limited studies on sadism being associated with self-enhancement, self-
transcendence, and conservation values, it is also unclear what sinusoid correlational pattern
would emerge for the relationship between narcissism and the high-order values dimensions
on the circumplex structure. We thus make no predictions about the correlational pattern. We
note, that due to limited number of studies discovered examining this relationship in our pre-
test searches, we set to only conduct analyses on these hypotheses about the link between
sadism and values if we are able to identify at least five suitable studies.
Exploratory tests and moderators
Comparing associations of Machiavellianism and psychopathy
We will also examine whether the shape consistency and strength of relationships
between Dark Triad and personal values varies when comparing the different Dark Triad
traits. Based on the current literature suggesting the high overlap between Machiavellianism
and psychopathy (Muris et al., 2017), we expect the following:
Hypothesis 7a: The associations between Machiavellianism and personal
values is weaker than the associations found between psychopathy and
personal values (as indicated by comparison of shape consistency and the
strongest correlations).
Yet we also entertain the following competing hypothesis:
Hypothesis 7b: The associations between Machiavellianism and personal
values is stronger than the associations found between psychopathy and
personal values (as indicated by comparison of shape consistency and the
strongest correlations).
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 14
Comparing subscale measures of the Dark Triad
Further, there has been some debate as to the conceptualisation of the Dark Triad as a
unidimensional construct, where in fact they may represent rich multi-faceted personality
traits (Miller, Vize, Crowe & Lynam, 2019; Watts, Waldman, Smith, Poore, & Lilienfeld,
2017). In order to understand how each Dark Triad trait is associated with certain value
orientations, it may be valuable to also conduct moderator analyses using the different
subscale measures, such as NARQ-Admiration subscale (Back et al., 2013), or LSRP-F1
(Levenson et al., 1995). However, to our knowledge this has not been previously examined in
this literature, we therefore make no predictions as to what specific domains of each Dark
Triad trait will be associated with the value dimensions. We also note, that is it possible,
perhaps even likely, that there would not be sufficient studies reporting subscale level
associations for us to be able to conduct meaningful meta-analytic analyses. For this test, we
set a minimum number of studies to five in order to run such analyses.
Comparing measurement scales used for traits and values
Across empirical studies, different instruments were used to measure the Dark Triad
and personal values (see Tables 1 and 2). This may limit generalizability as different
instruments may tap into varying facets and capture different variance of the same construct
(Parks-Leduc et al., 2015). In a study by Balakrishnan et al. (2017), the use of a 27-item
composite Dark Triad measure (Jones & Paulhus, 2014) and individual measures of
psychopathy (Forth, Brown, Hart, & Hare, 1996), Machiavellianism (Christie & Geis, 1970),
and narcissism (Raskin & Hall, 1979), yielded different correlational strengths for the same
construct with personal values. For example, Machiavellianism and benevolence were found
to be negatively correlated using the individual measure (r = -0.26), but with weaker effects
when using the composite measure (r = -.13). Thus, the nature of personality measures
(individual or composite) measuring the Dark Triad may have a moderating effect on the
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 15
correlational strength of the relationships found between traits and values. We therefore set
the following exploratory hypothesis:
Hypothesis 9: Relationships between Dark Triad traits and personal values are
different (stronger or weaker) for studies when measured using individual
Dark Triad measures compared to studies using composite Dark Triad
measures (exploratory).
The type of value instruments used may also impact effects found for relationships
between values and traits. Parks-Leduc et al. (2015) found that more than half of the
relationships examined between values and the Big Five traits using null-hypothesis
significance testing were different between studies that utilised the Schwartz Values Survey
(SVS; Schwartz; 1992) versus studies that utilised the Portrait Values Questionnaire (PVQ;
Schwartz et al., 2001). Further, Boer and Fischer (2013) also found differences between
studies that used either of these two instruments on the association between attitudes and
values. Thus, we examine studies using these two specific types of instruments for possible
moderating effects.
Parks-Leduc et al. (2015) indicated that "in general [...] PVQ had stronger
relationships with traits than the SVS" (p. 19). They explained that SVS measures abstract
values directly whereas the PVQ was developed from the SVS with more concrete examples
asking participants to compare themselves to people's profiles and is therefore less cognitively
complex. They summarized that "As the PVQ has trait-like elements, it is likely to result in
inflated trait–value correlations compared with the SVS" (p. 19). As we are also examining
values-trait associations, we expected that:
Hypothesis 10: Relationships between Dark Triad traits and personal values
are stronger for studies that measure values using the PVQ compared to
studies using the SVS.
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 16
Statistical adjustments for values scale use
Parks-Leduc et al. (2015) suggested that analysing values requires a different approach
compared to traits. As values are perceived in relation to other values, controlling for the
mean importance of these values is encouraged (Schwartz, 1992). However, studies
measuring associations between Dark Triad and personal values do not consistently mean
centre. This may result in varying trait-value associations.
Hypothesis 11: Relationships between Dark Triad traits and personal values
are different (stronger or weaker) for studies that test values associations using
mean centering compared to studies that do not (exploratory).
Publication Status
Publication status will be examined for possible moderating effects on the
relationships between Dark Triad personality traits and personal values to examine for
possible publication biases in extant literature. Several meta-analyses (Flore & Wicherts,
2015; Lewis & Michalak, 2019; Nguyen & Ryan, 2008) have suggested signs of publication
bias, which can distort the true effect size of the relationships between the variables of
interest. Based on this, we would expect that studies that have published status are likely to
report stronger correlations than studies that are unpublished, yielding stronger effect sizes.
Hypothesis 12: Relationships between Dark Triad traits and personal values is
stronger for published studies compared to unpublished studies.
Demographic Variables
Demographic differences will also be examined for possible moderating effects on the
relationships between Dark Triad personality traits and personal values, as research has
demonstrated that there are population differences in the composition of the Dark Triad
depending on gender (Muris et al., 2017), WEIRD samples (Jonason et al., 2020), and age
groups (O'Boyle et al., 2013). However, as there is scarce theoretical backing for specific
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 17
differences between demographic groups on influencing the relationships of interest, we make
no hypotheses. Results for these moderator analyses can be found in the supplemental
materials.
Methods
[Note: Written in past tense to demonstrate methods section after completion, but has yet to be conducted. Will be filled and
updated after pre-registration and data collection.]
Open science
We shared all procedures, materials, datasets, and code on the Open Science
Framework https://osf.io/4z27f/.
[PLEASE NOTE: Coding sheet and example R code on simulated dataset based on the
suggested coding sheet is available on that page for your reference. We would very much
appreciate a code review and feedback on the coding sheet.]
Disclosures
We posted a notice on ResearchGate asking authors to alert us to possible related
articles yet systematic data collection has not yet commenced for this project. There are no
other unreported/unlinked pre-registrations for this meta-analysis project.
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 18
Design
For ease of analysis, interpretation, and presentation, reporting will focus on
aggregates and reported in terms of associations between the Dark Triad traits higher-value
dimensions (4 traits x 4 higher-value dimensions). Generalized details on lower level values
links are reported in the supplementary materials and provided in the accompanied dataset.
Eligibility Criteria
Studies including measures of Dark Triad traits and the Schwartz values will be
included in our study (see Tables 1 and 2 for a list of measures).
Search Strategy
The meta-analysis process is outlined in Figure 3.
Multiple strategies were used for gathering all studies that included measures of the
Dark Triad and Schwartz values (see Figure 4 for an overview of the search strategy). First,
we carried out a literature search on Google Scholar, which has been shown to be suitable for
gathering articles for meta-analyses (Gehanno, Rollin, & Darmoni, 2013), PsycINFO,
Proquest, and Web of Science. Combinations of keywords focusing on the Dark Triad and
Schwartz values were used to conduct systematic searches of the specified database. Exact
combinations and patterns that were used can be found in the associated coding sheet
document (“Final Search Patterns Record”). These search patterns were pre-tested, and each
finalised pattern to be used were pre-run to check for suitability of search results. More
information of the search pattern process can be found in the coding sheet under tabs prefixed
with “pretest1” and “pretest2”.
The following were the specific keyword stems to be used in the search: 1) Trait-
related keywords, 2) Value-related keywords. Boolean Logic operators such as “OR”, and
“AND” will be used in the search pattern to connect the trait and value related keywords. An
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 19
asterisk was appended to keywords to allow for searches of truncated variations of keywords
that may be spelled differently (e.g. “psychopathy” vs. “psychopaths”, Robinson & Dickersin,
2002). The asterisk, along with variations of keywords were also included in the search along
with original keywords specified to ensure that all studies examining the specific constructs
are captured in the search. The list of keywords can be found in the coding sheet (“Final
Keywords Record”). An example of the general format for our search pattern was as follows:
(“psychopathy” OR “psychopath*”) AND (“values” OR “value*”). Once all search patterns
had been exhausted and an initial list was specified, duplicate records input by the researcher
in the article list were identified and removed. Database searches for each search pattern was
terminated after combing through 30 records consecutively without potentially relevant
papers that fit the inclusion criteria.
Next, a search for relevant papers not listed in the first search was conducted, by
searching for papers listed under the “related articles” and “cited by” features where available
in the database using the identified list of articles. Another search for relevant papers was also
conducted, by searching for papers listed under the “cited by” feature where available in the
database using the common measurements identified based on a pre-review of existing
empirical studies conducted by researchers during the writing up of this registered report (see
Tables 1 and 2). Additionally, one more search for relevant papers was also conducted by
skimming the reference sections of identified articles from our primary search.
Further, we identified authors in the fields of the Dark Triad and Schwartz values
literature, searched through their available papers and contacted them, along with authors of
other identified articles to ensure full coverage and maximise access to unpublished data
and/or manuscripts that are too relevant (see Appendix A for a mail-merge Word template to
be sent to relevant authors identified).
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 20
Lastly, we issued a call for unpublished findings on online forums and research
platforms (e.g., ResearchGate, listservs). We set up a project on ResearchGate, and all
identified articles was added as references, where possible, to notify authors about this
project, and to provide an open access list of available studies (link:
https://www.researchgate.net/project/Dark-Triad-and-Personal-Values-Meta-analysis-
Registered-Report).
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
We included studies that reported correlations between at least one of the Dark Triad
traits and one of the Schwartz values/dimensions.
Studies were excluded if they a) experimentally manipulated value orientations, b)
failed to report the crucial statistics necessary for a meta-analysis (i.e., correlation coefficients
and sample size), and the results were not obtainable from the authors), or c) not written in
English (unless all necessary data and information for coding is provided in English or can be
obtained from authors).
Recording Studies during Searches
Studies that met our criteria were coded into “Searched Articles” tab within the coding
sheet. All preliminary studies included in the total search were saved into a cloud folder and
accessible from the project OSF link or directly via: [to be added later]. Articles were scanned
to determine whether they should be included in the main coding sheet or not. If excluded, a
reason will be documented along the article recorded.
Studies that require contacting the author for the dataset/further clarifications were
first included into the main coding sheet, but documented as to be excluded potentially,
should the author not respond by a given date. Authors of studies with missing needed
statistics were contacted for relevant datasets/information. If the dataset was provided, the
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 21
researchers conducted needed analyses for coding. This process and the relevant results are
documented in a tab within the coding sheet. If included, the article record will be added in
the main coding sheet within the tab (“Final Articles”) tab. All final studies included in the
total search are saved into a cloud folder and accessible from the OSF link or directly via:
[INSERT LINK].
Selection Procedure
During the database search, the title and abstract of the study were scanned to assess
for eligibility and recorded onto the list of records in the coding sheet if eligible. A total of
XX published articles, unpublished articles, and datasets were initially identified and
downloaded from the primary search.
In subsequent search strategies, the title and abstract of potential articles were also
scanned to assess for eligibility and added to the list, with indication as a secondary source.
These searches yielded another XX published articles, unpublished articles, and datasets. XX
duplicate articles were then identified and removed.
The remaining XX studies will then be assessed by the principal researcher to
determine its suitability for the meta-analysis, and the co-researcher verified the results. All
decisions on inclusion and exclusion is documented with reasons in the coding sheet. After all
studies were assessed, a final list of XX studies was derived.
Coding
Data extraction from the identified studies were recorded on a master coding sheet
(see Supplementary Materials Coding sheet, “Main Coding Sheet” tab). Where available, the
following key variables were recorded into the coding sheet: the correlations between each
Dark Triad and values, the intercorrelations among the Dark Triad traits, type of scales used,
sample demographics, and publication information. Any identified changes to the coding
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 22
sheet structure during the process were documented clearly. Any final decisions to resolve
issues that arose, such as further study exclusions were also documented as well in the coding
sheet (see “Decision Documentation” tab).
During the coding process, if the correlation between a trait variable and a value
variable (e.g. psychopathy and self-transcendence) were reported both on its own, and also as
a result when split by specified moderators (e.g. relationship when participants are male, and
the correlation when participants are female), these results will be listed as separate rows.
Moderator variables will be coded for each study where identified as present and analysed
using a meta-regression.
To ensure similarity in coding perception, the two researchers pre-tested and
conducted coding rounds of three random articles and reached at least 80% agreement before
the main coding began. The principal author coded all articles listed, with verification
conducted by the second author.
Analysis plan
Confirmatory analyses
We will use R/Rmarkdown, and the metafor packages for the statistical analyses
(Viechtbauer, 2010). We will meta-analyse the relationship of Dark Triad traits and personal
values, as measured by Pearson’s r. Whenever available, we will use correlations obtained
directly from authors of original papers. If correlations and regression coefficients from
multiple regression analyses are both provided in the paper, only the correlations will be used,
as we are only interested in the associations between traits and values, and not the causal
direction which is indicated by coefficients. We will also examine corrections for attenuation
to control for unreliability of measures used for the Dark Triad and Schwartz values. This is
by using the Hunter and Schmidt Method in the metafor package when conducting our meta-
regression analyses. For studies that did not report reliability, we will use the weight-averaged
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 23
reliability calculated for all studies that did report reliability. All conversions and coding
decisions will be documented, and the original text will be included in the coding sheet to
allow for reproducibility.
We will first report our meta-analysis of interrelations between the Dark Triad and
sadism traits. Then, for the main analyses, we will use a random effects model to examine the
relationship between traits and values. Forest plots presenting the effect size of each study
will be produced. Equivalence tests will be conducted to examine whether the effect size
indicates a meaningful association (Lakens, 2017), where a correlation of at least r = .10 is
expected. Additionally, based on the level of heterogeneous effects within this study, we will
run multi-level analyses. Depending on the level of heterogeneity, we will collapse the effects
(default analysis choice) and/or run a multi-level meta-regression. This decision will be
documented and justified based on our observations of the final set of papers coded.
Statistical heterogeneity will then be determined using the Tau2 test and quantified
using I2, which represents the percentage of total variation in a set of studies that is due to
heterogeneity (Higgins, Thompson, Deeks, & Altman, 2003). This meta-analysis will yield a
point estimate, confidence intervals, and p-value, along with statistics for heterogeneity,
assessed using the Q-statistics, and the I2 statistic. If there is indeed heterogeneity, we will
explore potential moderators. A meta-regression will then be conducted to examine the
suggested moderators.
Publication bias
To examine the presence of publication bias, we will use funnel plots and statistical
tests for publication bias and asymmetry. The Precision-Effect Test (PET) and Precision-
Effect Estimate with Standard Error (PEESE) are both regression techniques built on the idea
that publication bias results in a small study effect such that effect sizes from smaller studies
(i.e., smaller sample size) are larger and have larger standard errors than those from relatively
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 24
larger studies (Stanley & Doucouliagos, 2014). In other words, effect size positively
correlates with standard error. To adjust for this via the PET technique, the analyst regresses
effect size on standard error so that the intercept can be interpreted as the predicted effect size
when the standard error equals zero (i.e., a perfectly precise estimate).
We will also conduct a p-curve (Simonsohn, Nelson, & Simmons, 2014; Simmons &
Simonsohn, 2017) and a p-uniform test (van Aert & van Assen, 2018). A p-curve test is based
on the idea that the distribution of p- values (under p < .05) resulting from tests of “true”
effects will be right skewed (i.e., higher frequencies of small p-values). As effects and sample
sizes get bigger, small p-values become more frequent and the distribution of p-values—p-
curve—becomes more right-skewed. In contrast, null effects will produce p-values that are
uniformly distributed; that is, all p- values are equally likely when the null is true. Thus, an
analyst can glean evidential value of a phenomenon from the degree of right-skew in the
observed p-curve. A p-uniform test also assesses p-values (< .05), yet it employs a different
estimation algorithm. Because these techniques make similar assumptions about effect sizes
and selection bias, they perform equally well under similar conditions. However, unlike p-
curve, the p-uniform technique provides confidence intervals for the estimated effect size.
Additionally, we will also conduct Egger’s regression test (Egger, Smith, & Minder,
1997) to visually inspect for publication bias using funnel plots of study effect sizes against
sample size. A trim-and-fill test will also be conducted to test adjustment for publication bias
detected (Duval & Tweedie, 2000a, 2000b).
Additional exploratory analyses
We expect to include more variables that are not listed in the coding sheet as possible
moderators as we examine the literature. These additional moderator analyses will be
considered as exploratory and most likely will be conducted if tests of homogeneity reveal
significant heterogeneity among the studies included in our meta-analysis.
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 25
Decisions to conduct exploratory analyses will be documented in the coding sheet and
the results found will be clearly specified as exploratory.
Results
[TBD pending, to be completed after data analysis]
Discussion
[TBD pending, to be completed after data analysis]
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 26
References
Ames, D. R., Rose, P., & Anderson, C. P. (2006). The NPI-16 as a short measure of
narcissism. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 440-450.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2005.03.002
Babiak, P., & Hare, R. D. (2006). Snakes in suits: When psychopaths go to work. New York,
NY: Harper Collins.
Back, M. D., Küfner, A. C., Dufner, M., Gerlach, T. M., Rauthmann, J. F., & Denissen, J. J.
(2013). Narcissistic admiration and rivalry: Disentangling the bright and dark sides of
narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 1013-1037.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034431
Balakrishnan, A., Plouffe, R. A., & Saklofske, D. H. (2017). What do sadists values? Is
honesty-humility an intermediary? Replicating and extending findings on the link
between values and “dark” personalities. Personality and Individual Differences, 109,
142-147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.12.055
Bardi, A., & Schwartz, S. H. (2003). Values and behavior: Strength and structure of relations.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1207-1220.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167203254602
Baughman, H. M., Jonason, P. K., Lyons, M., & Vernon, P. A. (2014). Liar liar pants on fire:
Cheater strategies linked to the Dark Triad. Personality and Individual
Differences, 71, 35-38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.019
Baumeister, R. F., & Campbell, W. K. (1999). The intrinsic appeal of evil: Sadism,
sensational thrills, and threatened egotism. Personality and Social Psychology Review,
3, 210–221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0303_4
Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Hicks, B. M., Blonigen, D. M., & Krueger, R. F. (2003). Factor
structure of the psychopathic personality inventory: Validations and implications for
clinical assessment. Psychological Assessment, 15, 340-350.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.15.3.340
Bilsky, W., & Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Values and personality. European Journal of
Personality, 8, 163-181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.2410080303
Boddy, C. R. (2006). The dark side of management decisions: Organisational psychopaths.
Management Decisions, 44, 1461-1475.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/00251740610715759
Boer, D., & Fischer, R. (2013). How and when do personal values guide our attitudes and
sociality? Explaining cross-cultural variability in attitude-value linkages.
Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1113-1147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031347
Book, A., Visser, B. A., Blais, J., Hosker-Field, A., Methot-Jones, T. Gauthier, N. Y., Volk,
A. … D’Agata, M. T. (2016). Unpacking more “evil”: What is at the core of the dark
tetrad? Personality and Individual Differences, 90, 269-272.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.11.009
Buckels, E. E., Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2013). Behavioral confirmation of everday
sadism. Psychological Science, 24, 2201-2209.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177%2F0956797613490749
Buckels, E. E., & Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Comprehensive assessment of sadistic tendencies
(CAST). Unpublished instrument, Vancouver, Canada: University of British Columbia.
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 27
Butenko, T., & Schwartz, S. H. (2013). Relations of the new circle of 19 values to
behaviors. Higher School of Economics Research Paper No. WP BPR, 8.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2227218
Chabrol, H., Van Leeuwen, N., Rodgers, R., & Séjourné, N. (2009). Contributions of
psychopathic, narcissistic, Machiavellian, and sadistic personality traits to juvenile
delinquency. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 734–739.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.020
Christie, R., & Geis, F. (1970). Scale construction. Studies in machiavellianism, 34, 10-34.
Crysel, L. C., Crosier, B. S., & Webster, G. D. (2013). The Dark Triad and risk behavior.
Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 35-40.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.07.029
Dadds, M., Whiting, C., & Hawes, D. J. (2006). Associations among cruelty to animals,
family conflict, and psychopathic traits in childhood. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 21, 411-429. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0886260505283341
Dahling, J. J., Whitaker, B. G., & Levy, P. E. (2009). The development and validation of a
new Machiavellianism scale. Journal of Management, 35, 219-257.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2F0149206308318618
Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000a). A nonparametric “trim and fill” method of accounting for
publication bias in meta-analysis. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 95,
89-98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01621459.2000.10473905
Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000b). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of
testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics: Journal of the
International Biometric Society, 56, 455-463. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0006-
341X.2000.00455.x
Egan, V., Hughes, N., & Palmer, E. J. (2015). Moral disengagement, the dark triad and
unethical consumer attitudes. Personality and Individual Differences, 76, 123-128.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.11.054
Egger, M., Smith, G. D., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple,
graphical test. BMJ, 315, 629-634. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629
Feldman, G. (2020). Personal values and moral foundations: Examining relations and joint
prediction of moral variables. Social Psychological and Personality Science.
Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.32570.49600/4
Feldman, G., Chao, M. M., Farh, J.-L., & Bardi, A. (2015). The motivation and inhibition of
breaking the rules: Personal values structures predict unethicality. Journal of Research
in Personality, 59, 69-80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2015.09.003
Fischer, R., & Boer, D. Motivational basis of personality traits: A meta-analysis of value-
personality correlations. Journal of Personality, 83, 491-510.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12125
Flore, P. C., & Wicherts, J. M. (2015). Does stereotype threat influence performance of girls
in stereotyped domains? A meta-analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 53, 25-44.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2014.10.002
Forth, A. E., Brown, S. L., Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1996). The assessment of psychopathy
in male and female noncriminals: Reliability and validity. Personality and Individual
Differences, 20, 531-543. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(95)00221-9
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 28
Fritzsche, D. J., & Oz, E. (2007). Personal values' influence on the ethical dimension of
decision making. Journal of Business Ethics, 75, 335-343.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10551-006-9256-5
Furnham, A., Richards, S. C., & Paulhus, D. L. (2013). The dark triad of personality: A 10
year review. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7, 199-216.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12018
Gehanno, J. F., Rollin, L., & Darmoni, S. (2013). Is the coverage of Google Scholar enough
to be used alone for systematic reviews. BMC Medical Informatics and Decision
Making, 13, 7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1472-6947-13-7
Glenn, A. L., Efferson, L. M., Iyer, R., & Graham, J. (2017). Values, goals, and motivations
associated with psychopathy. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 36, 108-125.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2017.36.2.108
Glenn, A. L., & Sellbom, M. (2015). Theoretical and empirical concerns regarding the dark
triad as a construct. Journal of Personality Disorders, 29, 360-377.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2014_28_162
Glover, N., Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., Crego, C., & Widiger, T. A. (2012). The five-factor
narcissism inventory: A five-factor measure of narcissistic personality traits. Journal
of Personality Assessment, 94, 500-512.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2012.670680
Greitemeyer, T. (2015). Everyday sadism predicts violent video game preferences.
Personality and Individual Differences, 75, 19-23.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.10.049
Grijalva, E., Newman, D. A., Tay, L., Donnellan, M. B., Harms, P. D., Robins, R. W., & Yan,
T. (2015). Gender differences in narcissism: A meta-analytic review. Psychological
Bulletin, 141, 261-310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038231
Grunert, S., & Juhl, H. J. (1995). Values, environmental attitudes, and buying of organic
foods. Journal of Economic Psychology, 16, 39-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-
4870(94)00034-8
Güngör, I.H., Eksi, H., & Aricak, O.T. (2012). Value preferences predicting narcissistic
personality traits in young adults. Educational Sciences: Theory & Practice, 12, 1281–
1290.
Harms, P.D., Spain, S. M., & Hannah, S. T. (2011). Leader development and the dark side of
personality. The Leadership Quarterly, 22, 495-509.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.04.007
Hébert, A., & Weaver, A. (2014). An examination of personality characteristics associated
with BDSM orientations. Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, 23, 106-115.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3138/cjhs.2467
Higgins, J. P. T., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring
inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 327, 557-560.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557
Hitlin, S., & Piliavin, J. A. (2004). Values: Reviving a Dormant Concept. Annual Review of
Sociology, 30, 359-393. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.30.012703.110640
Hodson, G., Hogg, S. M., & MacInnis, C. C. (2009). The role of 'dark personalities'
(narcissism, Machiavellianism, psychopathy), Big Five personality factors, and
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 29
ideology in explaining prejudice. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 686-690.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2009.02.005
Hogan, R. (2007). Personality and the Fate of Organizations. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Inglehart, R., Haerpfer, C., Moreno, A., Welzel, C., Kizilova, K., Diez-Medrano, J., ... &
Puranen, B. (2014). World values survey: Round six-country-pooled datafile
version. Madrid: JD Systems Institute, 12.
Jonason, P. K., Girgis, M., & Milne-Home, J. (2017). The exploitive mating strategy of the
Dark Triad traits: Tests of rape-enabling attitudes. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 46,
697-706. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-017-0937-1
Jonason, P. K., Kaufman, S. B., Webster, G. D., & Geher, G. (2013). What lies beneath the
Dark Triad Dirty Dozen: varied relations with the Big Five. Individual Differences
Research, 11, 81-90.
Jonason, P. K., Li, N. P., Webster, G. D., & Schmitt, D. P. (2009). The dark triad: Facilitating
a short-term mating strategy in men. European Journal of Personality, 23, 5-18.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.698
Jonason, P. K., Strosser, G. L., Kroll, C. H., Duineveld, J. J., & Baruffi, S. A. (2015). Valuing
myself over others: The Dark Triad traits and moral and social values. Personality and
Individual Differences, 81, 102-106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.10.045
Jonason, P. K., & Webster, G. D. (2010). The dirty dozen: A concise measure of the dark
triad. Psychological Assessment, 22, 420–432. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019265
Jonason, P. K., & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2018). The fundamental social motives that characterize
dark personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 132, 98-107.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.05.031
Jonason, P. K., Żemojtel‐Piotrowska, M., Piotrowski, J., Sedikides, C., Campbell, W. K.,
Gebauer, J. E., ... & Atitsogbe, K. A. (2020). Country‐Level Correlates of the Dark
Triad Traits in 49 Countries. Journal of Personality.
Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2011). The role of impulsivity in the Dark Triad of
personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 679-682.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.011
Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3): A brief
measure of dark personality traits. Assessment, 21, 28-41.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191113514105
Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2011). The role of impulsivity in the Dark Triad of
personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 679-682.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.011
Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3): A brief
measure of dark personality traits. Assessment, 21, 28-41.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191113514105
Kajonius, P. J., Persson, B. N., & Jonason, P. K. (2015). Hedonism, Achievement, and Power:
Universal values that characterize the Dark Triad. Personality and Individual
Differences, 77, 173-178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.055
Kapidzic, S. (2013). Narcissism as a predictor of motivations behind Facebook profile picture
selection. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16, 14-19.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0143
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 30
Kaufman, S. B., Yaden, D. B., Hyde, E., & Tsukayama, E. (2019). The light vs. dark triad of
personality: Contrasting two very different profiles of human nature. Frontiers in
Psychology, 10, 467. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00467
Kauten, R. L., & Barry, C. T. (2016). Adolescent narcissism and its association with different
indices of prosocial behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 60, 36-45.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2015.11.004
Kavanagh, P. S., Signal, T. D., & Taylor, N. (2013). The Dark Triad and animal cruelty: Dark
personalities, dark attitudes, and dark behaviors. Personality and Individual
Differences, 55, 666-670. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.05.019
Kinnunen, S. P., Lindeman, M., & Verkasalo, M. (2016). Help-giving and moral courage on
the Internet. Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace, 10,
article 6. http://dx.doi.org/10.5817/CP2016-4-6
Konrath, S., Ho, M., & Zarins, S. (2016). The strategic helper: Narcissism and prosocial
motives and behaviors. Current Psychology, 35, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12144-
016-9417-3
Lakens, D. (2017). Equivalence tests: A practical primer for t tests, correlations, and meta-
analyses. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8, 355-362.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550617697177
Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2014). The Dark Triad, the Big Five, and the HEXACO
model. Personality and Individual Differences, 67, 2-5.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.048
Levenson, M. R., Kiehl, K. A., & Fitzpatrick, C. M. (1995). Assessing psychopathic attributes
in a noninstitutionalized population. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
151–158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.68.1.151
Lewis Jr, N., & Michalak, N. M. (2019). Has stereotype threat dissipated over time? A cross-
temporal meta-analysis. http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/w4ta2
Lilienfeld, S. O., & Andrews, B. P. (1996). Development and preliminary validation of a self-
report measure of psychopathic personality traits in noncriminal populations. Journal
of Personality Assessment, 66, 488–
524. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa6603_3
Lindeman, M., & Verkasalo, M. (2005). Measuring Values With the Short Schwartz's Value
Survey. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85(2), 170–
178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa8502_09
Lipponen, J., Bardi, A., & Haapamäki, J. (2010). The interaction between values and
organizational identification predicting suggestion-making at work. Journal of
Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 81, 241-248.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/096317907X216658
Liu, Y., & Liu, X-Y. (2018). Politics under abusive supervision: The role of Machiavellianism
and guanxi. European Management Journal, 36, 649-659.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.emj.2018.08.002
Ljubin-Golub, T., & Sokić, K. (2016). The relationships between triarchic psychopathic traits
and value orientations in men and women. Psihološka istraživanja, 19, 185-203.
Lynam, D. R., Gaughan, E. T., Miller, J. D., Miller, D. J., Mullins-Sweatt, S., & Widiger, T.
A. (2011). Assessing the basic traits associated with psychopathy: Development and
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 31
validation of the Elemental Psychopathy Assessment. Psychological Assessment, 23,
108–124. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021146
Lynam, D. R., Sherman, E. D., Samuel, D., Miller, J. D., Few, L. R. & Widiger, T. A. (2013).
Development of a short form of the elemental psychopathy assessment. Assessment,
20, 659-669. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177%2F1073191113502072
Maio, G. R. (2010). Mental representations of social values. Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology, 42, 1-43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(10)42001-8
Mathieu, C., Neumann, C. S., Hare, R. D., & Babiak, P. (2014). A dark side of leadership:
Corporate psychopathy and its influence on employee well-being and job
satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 59, 83-88.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.11.010
Mendedović, J. & Petrović, B. (2015). The dark tetrad: Structural properties and location in
the personality space. Journal of Individual Differences, 36, 228-236.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000179
Menesini, E., Nocentini, A., & Camodeca, M. (2013). Morality, values, traditional bullying,
and cyberbullying in adolescence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31,
1-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02066.x
Miller, J. D., Vize, C., Crowe, M. L., & Lynam, D. R. (2019). A critical appraisal of the dark-
triad literature and suggestions for moving forward. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 28, 353-360. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721419838233
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Otgaar, H., & Meijer, E. (2017). The malevolent side of human
nature: A meta-analysis and critical review of the literature on the dark triad
(narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy). Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 12, 183-204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691616666070
Neumann, C. S., Schmitt, D. S., Carter, R., Embley, I., & Hare, R. D. (2012). Psychopathic
traits in females and males across the globe. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 30, 557–
574. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/bsl.2038
Nguyen, H. H. D., & Ryan, A. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat affect test performance of
minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 93, 1314-1334. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012702
O'Boyle Jr, E. H., Forsyth, D. R., Banks, G. C., & McDaniel, M. A. (2012). A meta-analysis
of the Dark Triad and work behavior: A social exchange perspective. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 97, 557-579. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025679
O’Boyle, E. H., Forsyth, D., Banks, G. C., & Story, P. A. (2013). A meta-analytic review of
the Dark Triad–intelligence connection. Journal of Research in Personality, 47(6),
789-794.
O’Boyle Jr, E. H., Forsyth, D. R., Banks, G. C., Story, P. A., & White, C. D. (2014). A meta-
analytic test of redundancy and relative importance of the dark triad and five-factor
model of personality. Journal of Personality, 83, 644-664.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12126
O'Meara, A., Davies, J., & Hammond, S. (2011). The psychometric properties and utility of
the Short Sadistic Impulse Scale (SSIS). Psychological Assessment, 23, 523–
531. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022400
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 32
Parks-Leduc, L., Feldman, G., & Bardi, A. (2015). Personality traits and personal values: A
meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 19, 3-29.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868314538548
Patrick, C.J. (2010). Operationalising the Triarchic Conceptualisation of Psychopathy:
Preliminary Description of Brief Scales for Assessment of Boldness, Meanness and
Disinhibition. Unpublished Manual.
Paulhus, D. L., & Jones, D. N. (2015). Measures of dark personalities. In G. J. Boyle, D. H.
Saklofske, & G. Matthews (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological
constructs (p. 562–594). Elsevier Academic Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-
12-386915-9.00020-6
Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism,
Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 556-563.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6
Persson, B. (2014). Personality neuroscience and dark values. (Unpublished master’s thesis).
Pincus, A. L., Ansell, E. B., Pimentel, C. A., Cain, N. M., Wright, A. G. C., & Levy, K. N.
(2009). Initial construction and validation of the Pathological Narcissism
Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 21, 365–
379. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016530
Plouffe, R. A., Saklosfske, D. H., & Smith, M. M. (2017). The assessment of sadistic
personality: Preliminary psychometric evidence for a new measure. Personality and
Individual Differences, 104, 166-171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.07.043
Raskin, R. N., & Hall, C. S. (1979). A narcissistic personality inventory. Psychological
Reports, 45, 590. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1979.45.2.590
Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal-components analysis of the Narcissistic
Personality Inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 890–902. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
3514.54.5.890
Robinson, K. A., & Dickersin, K. (2002). Development of a highly sensitive search strategy
for the retrieval of reports of controlled trials using PubMed. International Journal of
Epidemiology, 31, 150–153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ije/31.1.150.
Roccas, S., & Sagiv, L. (2010). Personal values and behavior: Taking the cultural context into
account. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 30–
41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00234.x
Roccas, S., Sagiv, L., Schwartz, S. H., & Knafo, A. (2002). The Big Five personality factors
and personal values. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 789-801.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167202289008
Rogoza, R., Wyszyńska, P., Maćkiewicz, M., & Cieciuch, J. (2016). Differentiation of the two
narcissistic faces in their relations to personality traits and basic values. Personality
and Individual Differences, 95, 85-88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.02.038
Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York, NY: The Free Press.
Rosenthal, S. A., & Pittinsky, T. L. (2006). Narcissistic leadership. The Leadership Quarterly,
17, 617-633. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2006.10.005
Rosenthal, S. A., Hooley, J. M., Montoya, R. M., van der Linden, S. L., & Steshenko, Y.
(2019). The narcissistic grandiosity scale: A measure to distinguish narcissistic
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 33
grandiosity from high self-esteem. Assessment, 27, 487-507.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177%2F1073191119858410
Sandy, C. J., Gosling, S. D., Schwartz, S. H., & Koelkebeck, T. (2016). The development and
validation of brief and ultrabrief measures of values. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 99, 545-555. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2016.1231115
Schouten, R., & Silver, J. (2012). Almost a psychopath: Do I (or does someone I know) have a
problem with manipulation and lack of empathy? Minnesota, MN: Hazelden
Publishing.
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical
advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology (pp. 1-65). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Schwartz, S. H. (1996). Value priorities and behavior: Applying a theory of integrated value
systems. In C. Seligman, J. M. Olson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The Ontario symposium
on personality and social psychology, Vol. 8. The psychology of values: The Ontario
symposium, Vol. 8, pp. 1-24). Hillsdale, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Schwartz, S. H. (2003). A proposal for measuring value orientations across
nations. Questionnaire package of the european social survey, 259(290), 261.
Schwartz, S. H. (2011). Study values: Personal adventure, future directions. Journal of Cross-
Cultural Psychology, 42, 307-319. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022110396925
Schwartz, S. H., Melech, G., Lehmann, A., Burgess, S., Harris, M., & Owens, V. (2001).
Extending the cross-cultural validity of the theory of basic human values with a
different method of measurement. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 519 –
542. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022101032005001
Sellbom, M., Cooke, D. J., & Shou, Y. (2019). Development and initial validation of the
Comprehensive Assessment of Psychopathic Personality–Self-Report (CAPP-
SR). Psychological Assessment, 31, 878–894. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pas0000714
Simmons, J. P., & Simonsohn, U. (2017). Power posing: P-curving the evidence.
Psychological Science, 28, 687–693. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797616658563
Simonsohn, U., Nelson, L. D., & Simmons, J. P. (2014). P-curve: A key to the file-drawer.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 534–547.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033242
Smith, C. V., Overup, C. S., & Webster, G. D. (2019). Sexy deeds done dark? Examining the
relationship between dark personality traits and sexual motivation. Personality and
Individual Differences, 146, 105-110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.04.003
Stanley, T. D., & Doucouliagos, H. (2014). Meta-regression approximations to reduce
publication bias. Research Synthesis Methods, 5, 60-78.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1095
van Aert, R. C., & van Assen, M. A. (2018). Examining reproducibility in psychology: A
hybrid method for combining a statistically significant original study and a replication.
Behavior Research Methods, 50, 1515–1539. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-
0967-6
Vernon, P. A., Villani, V. C., Vickers, L. C., & Harris, J. A. (2008). A behavioral genetic
investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5. Personality and Individual Differences,
44, 445-452. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.007
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 34
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of
Statistical Software, 36, 1–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.18637/jss.v036.i03
Vize, C. E., Collison, K. L., Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2020). The “core” of the dark
triad: A test of competing hypotheses. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and
Treatment, 11, 91-99. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000386
Vize, C. E., Lynam, D. R., Collison, K. L., & Miller, J. D. (2018). Differences among dark
triad components: A meta-analytic investigation. Personality Disorders: Theory,
Research, and Treatment, 9, 101-111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/per0000222
Watts, A. L., Waldman, I. D., Smith, S. F., Poore, H. E., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2017). The
nature and correlates of the dark triad: The answers depend on the questions. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 126, 951–968. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/abn0000296
Wisse, B., & Sleebos, E. (2016). When the dark ones gain power: Perceived position power
strengthens the effect of supervisor Machiavellianism on abusive supervision in work
teams. Personality and Individual Differences, 99, 122-126.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.05.019
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 35
Appendix A: Template for Contacting Authors
Dear Dr. [name],
We are conducting a meta-analysis on the link between the Dark Triad traits and Schwartz
values. Your article was found through a comprehensive search of the literature and selected
as a study for inclusion.
The references for the studies you co-authored to be included are: [insert full citations]
We would like to ask for your help with the meta-analysis process.
We would appreciate references to any other published data on the relationship between the
dark triad traits and Schwartz values to make sure we included them in our meta-analysis.
More importantly, we are especially interested in any relevant unpublished manuscripts or
unpublished data that cannot be found through a regular literature search.
If you have unfinished or unpublished manuscripts, we would appreciate a copy. These will
be kept confidential and will not be used for any other purpose other than inclusion in the
meta-analysis.
Alternatively, for unpublished manuscripts and/or data, the information we require for each of
the studies/samples for inclusion is:
1. Brief description of the study and procedures
2. Correlation matrix between Dark Triad personality trait(s) and Schwartz value(s)
3. Sample size after exclusions
4. Independent variable :
1. Brief description of the measures/scales were used
2. Internal reliabilities if available/relevant.
5. Dependent variable:
1. Brief description of the measures/scales were used
2. Internal reliabilities if available/relevant.
6. Demographics and data collection details:
1. Date of data collection.
2. Country
3. Type of participants
4. Mean age (and SD if available).
7. Reference to be used when citing this data or manuscript.
If you only have raw data that has not yet been analysed, then we would be happy to help
analyse it for inclusion. In such a case, please send us the dataset and a description of the key
variables described above relevant for the analysis.
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 36
Please send all relevant information and/or data to us at this email [first author email]
If you are interested in more information, we will be happy to answer any further questions
and keep you posted on the project as we make progress and finalize the submission.
Best regards,
[authors]
[authors affiliation]
Dark Tetrad and personal values: Meta-analysis Registered Report 37
Appendix B: Formal call to publish on Research Gate
The aim of this project is to quantitatively summarize the literature on the relationship
between personal values and the Dark Triad.
If you have or are doing research on this, have any published or unpublished manuscripts or
data, or if you have questions or would like to offer your help or feedback, we would love to
hear from you. Please post a comment or contact us at [email address].
Table 1
Commonly used measures of the Dark Triad model
Dark Triad Factor
Measure Type
Measure Name
Citation
Items
Dark Triad
Composite
Dirty Dozen
Jonason & Webster, 2010
12
Dark Triad
Composite
Short Dark Triad
Jones & Paulhus, 2014
27
Psychopathy
Individual
Self-Report
Psychopathy Scales
Neumann et al., 2012
64
Psychopathy
Individual
Levenson Self-Report
Psychopathy Scale
Levenson, Kiehl, & Fitzpatrick, 1995
26
Psychopathy
Individual
Triarchic Psychopathy
Measure
Patrick, 2010
58
Psychopathy
Individual
Psychopathic
Personality Inventory
Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996
187
Psychopathy
Individual
Elemental Psychopathy
Assessment
Lynam et al., 2011
178
Psychopathy
Individual
Elemental Psychopathy
Assessment- Short Form
Lynam et al., 2013
72
Psychopathy
Individual
Comprehensive
Assessment of
Psychopathy
Selbom, Cooke, & Shou, 2019
33
Machiavellianism
Individual
MACH-IV
Christie & Geis, 1970
20
Machiavellianism
Individual
Machiavellian
Personality Scale
Dahling, Whitaker, & Levy, 2009
16
Narcissism
Individual
Narcissistic Personality
Inventory
Raskin & Terry, 1988
40
Narcissism
Individual
Narcissistic Personality
Inventory – 16 item
Ames, Rose, & Anderson, 2006
16
Dark Triad Factor
Measure Type
Measure Name
Citation
Items
Narcissism
Individual
Narcissistic Grandiosity
Scale
Rosenthal et al., 2019
7
Narcissism
Individual
Pathological Narcissism
Inventory
Pincus et al., 2009
52
Narcissism
Individual
Narcissistic Admiration
and Rivalry
Questionnaire
Back et al., 2013
18
Narcissism
Individual
Five-Factor Narcissism
Inventory
Glover et al., 2012
148
Sadism
Individual
Comprehensive
Assessment of Sadistic
Tendencies
Buckels & Paulhus, 2014
18
Sadism
Individual
Assessment of Sadistic
Personality
Plouffe, Saklofske, & Smith, 2017
9
Sadism
Individual
Short Sadistic Impulse
Scale
O’Meara, Davies & Hammond, 2011
10
Sadism
Individual
Varieties of Sadistic
Tendencies
Paulhus & Jones, 2015
16
Note. This table will be updated as we encounter other measures used in the studies analysed.
Table 2
Description of the 10 Schwartz values categories
Values
Description/Items: Individuals who value this believe in the importance of…
Power
…being in charge of people and resources and having money (social power, wealth, authority)
Achievement
… socially recognized successes (ambition, competence)
Hedonism
…sensual pleasure (fun, enjoying life)
Stimulation
…having stimulating experiences (daring, exciting life)
Self-direction
…independence of thought and action (creativity, freedom, independent, curious)
Universalism
… promoting the welfare of all people and nature (equality, social justice, protecting the
environment)
Benevolence
…promoting the welfare of people you are close to (helpfulness, loyalty, honesty, forgiving)
Conformity
…controlling impulses to fulfil others’ expectations (self-discipline, obedience)
Tradition
…maintaining traditions (moderation, respect for tradition, devout)
Security
…safety and security of self, family, and nation (family security, social order, clean)
Note. Adapted from Parks-Leduc, Feldman, and Bardi (2015)
Table 3
Commonly used measures of the Schwartz values model
Measure Type
Measure Name
Citation
Items
PVQ
Portrait Values Questionnaire (PVQ)
Schwartz et al., 2001
40
PVQ
Portrait Values Questionnaire Shortened (PVQ-
21/ESS)
Schwartz, 2003
21
PVQ
Portraits Values Questionnaire Revised (PVQ-
RR Revised)
Butenko & Schwartz, 2013
57
PVQ
World Values Survey (WVS)
Inglehart et al., 2014
10
PVQ
Ten Item Values Inventory (TIVI)
Sandy et al., 2016
10
PVQ
Twenty Item Values Inventory (TwIVI)
Sandy et al., 2016
20
SVS
Schwartz Value Survey (SVS)
Schwartz, 1992
57
SVS
Schwartz Values Scale – Short Version (SSVS)
Lindeman & Verkasalo, 2005
10