ArticlePDF Available

Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing: A proposal for a more inclusive measure

Authors:

Abstract

The science of wellbeing has come a long way from the early days of measuring wellbeing via a nation’s GDP, and wellbeing measures and concepts continue to proliferate to capture its various elements. Yet, much of this activity has reflected concepts from Western cultures, despite the emphasis placed on wellbeing in all corners of the globe. To meet the challenges and opportunities arising from cross-disciplinary research worldwide, the Well-Being for Planet Earth Foundation and the Gallup World Poll have joined forces to add more culturallyrelevant constructs and questions to existing Gallup modules. In this white paper, we review the discussion from the international well-being summit in Kyoto, Japan (August 2019), where nine such additions were proposed and highlight why a more global view of wellbeing is needed. Overall, the new items reflect a richer view of wellbeing than life satisfaction alone and include hedonic and eudaimonic facets of wellbeing, social wellbeing, the role of culture, community, nature, and governance. These additions allow for the measurement of a broader conceptualization of wellbeing, more refined and nuanced cross-cultural comparisons, and facilitate a better examination of the causes of variation in global wellbeing. The new Gallup World Poll additions will be trialled in 2020, with additional inclusions from this summit to be made in 2021.
Lambert, L., Lomas, T., van de Weijer, M. P., Passmore, H. A., Joshanloo, M., Harter, J., Ishikawa, Y.,
Lai, A., Kitagawa, T., Chen, D., Kawakami, T., Miyata, H., & Diener, E. (2020). Towards a greater global
understanding of wellbeing: A proposal for a more inclusive measure. International Journal of Wellbeing,
10(2), 1-18. doi:10.5502/ijw.v10i2.1037
Louise Lambert
United Arab Emirates University
ltlamber@yahoo.com
Copyright belongs to the author(s)
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
1
ARTICLE
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing: A
proposal for a more inclusive measure
Louise Lambert · Tim Lomas · Margot P van de Weijer · Holli Anne Passmore · Mohsen
Joshanloo · Jim Harter · Yoshiki Ishikawa · Alden Lai · Takuya Kitagawa · Dominique
Chen · Takafumi Kawakami · Hiroaki Miyata · Ed Diener
Abstract: The science of wellbeing has come a long way from the early days of measuring
wellbeing via a nation’s GDP, and wellbeing measures and concepts continue to proliferate to
capture its various elements. Yet, much of this activity has reflected concepts from Western
cultures, despite the emphasis placed on wellbeing in all corners of the globe. To meet the
challenges and opportunities arising from cross-disciplinary research worldwide, the Well-Being
for Planet Earth Foundation and the Gallup World Poll have joined forces to add more culturally
relevant constructs and questions to existing Gallup modules. In this white paper, we review the
discussion from the international well-being summit in Kyoto, Japan (August 2019), where nine
such additions were proposed and highlight why a more global view of wellbeing is needed.
Overall, the new items reflect a richer view of wellbeing than life satisfaction alone and include
hedonic and eudaimonic facets of wellbeing, social wellbeing, the role of culture, community,
nature, and governance. These additions allow for the measurement of a broader
conceptualization of wellbeing, more refined and nuanced cross-cultural comparisons, and
facilitate a better examination of the causes of variation in global wellbeing. The new Gallup World
Poll additions will be trialled in 2020, with additional inclusions from this summit to be made in
2021.
Keywords: positive psychology, wellbeing, hedonia, eudaimonia, life satisfaction, culture
1. Introduction
The science of wellbeing has come a long way. Initially anchored in the field of psychology,
it has since moved into fields like organizational development, health, education, economics, and
policy expansion. Indeed, global policy makers are progressively adopting wellbeing as an
overarching framework by which to assess, track, and respond to human development
challenges and opportunities. Indices by which wellbeing is measured are thus critical, and need
to be carefully reviewed and updated. For example, the World Happiness Report (WHR), which
has garnered international attention for its national happiness rankings, and the Happy Planet
Index, ranking environmentally sustainable wellbeing, assess wellbeing via the Gallup World
Poll (GWP). Both reports rely on a single question: the Cantril Self-Anchoring Striving Scale
(Cantril, 1965), also called Cantril’s Ladder, which asks respondents to rate themselves on their
current and future perceived quality or satisfaction with life, with the bottom of the ‘ladder
representing low satisfaction/quality of life and the top, representing high satisfaction/quality of
life.
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
2
Although the Cantril’s Ladder is a valid assessment of present perceived quality of life across
global cultures, it is an incomplete measure of well-being. At the country-level, Cantril’s Ladder
is highly correlated with a nation’s Gross Domestic Product (GDP) (Diener, Kahneman, Tov, &
Arora, 2010; Helliwell, Huang, & Wang, 2017; Joshanloo, 2018; Joshanloo, Jovanovic, & Taylor,
2019; Oishi & Schimmack, 2010). This single score is also linked to several factors (e.g., personal
freedom as well as healthcare, educational, and political functioning, Joshanloo et al., 2019). It is
further considered a Western-centric metric of wellbeing reflecting Western populations used in
most psychology research (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010) and is limited in its ability to
reflect ways in which wellbeing is experienced and understood worldwide (Lomas, 2015).
Scholars have found that subjective wellbeing can be ordered along a single dimension from
evaluative judgements of life (Cantril’s ladder) on one end to experienced affect on the other
(Diener, Kahneman, Arora, Harter, & Tov, 2009; Diener, Ng, Harter, & Arora, 2010). This
ordering reveals that evaluative judgements are more highly related to income, standards of
living, and luxury conveniences although meeting basic and psychosocial needs mediated the
effects of income on life evaluation to a degree, while affect is more highly related to
psychological needs, autonomy, social relationships, and fulfilment in daily tasks.
Wellbeing has also been thought of as a multidimensional set of constructs that are not
reducible to a single facet (such as quality of life) (de Chavez, Backett-Milburn, Parry, & Platt,
2005; Lomas, Hefferon, & Ivtzan, 2015); it includes not only satisfaction with life, a cognitive
appraisal of one’s life, but positive emotions and psychological resources (such as meaning in
life), together with considerations of social inequality, environmental degradation, and political
freedom. For example, Morrison, Tay, and Diener (2011) found, using Gallup World Poll data
that people tend to use proximate factors, such as one’s job, health, or standard of living to judge
their wellbeing when overall living conditions are satisfactory or when individualism is salient.
In contrast, individuals are more likely to use perceived societal success to judge life satisfaction
when life conditions are difficult or when collectivist norms form part of their culture. This
suggests additional dimensions need emphasis in wellbeing appraisals, preferably through
consensus and founded on empirical evidence proffered by the global academic community. This
approach also means capturing the diverse influences that have hitherto been overlooked in
existing measures and which matter to individuals and societies.
In the interests of advancing this aim, the authors recently participated in a three-day summit
convened in August 2019 in Kyoto, Japan. Funded and facilitated by the Well-Being for Planet
Earth Foundation (previously called the LiFull Foundation) and Gallup representatives, its
principal goal was to add new items to the GWP to ensure its representativeness in global
wellbeing perspectives. This means including additional concepts that have been omitted to date,
such as the role of culture, community, governance, and nature. This white paper offers a
summary of the proposed additions, including their rationale and future research potential. By
offering these additions, we hope to complement the work of the World Happiness Report
council and offer a truly comprehensive “World Wellbeing Report” in the years to come.
1.1 What’s wrong with the GDP as a measure of prosperity?
While income (GDP) is an indicator of prosperity, it is not the only one, and the realization
of its inadequacy in measuring social progress is growing (Adler & Seligman, 2016; Lambert,
Mulay-Shah, Warren, & Younis, 2019; Nikolova, 2016; Stiglitz, Sen, & Fitoussi, 2009; Uchida &
Oishi, 2016). Measuring the market value of goods and services tells us little about individual
wellbeing and happiness, or who benefits when a nation’s GDP increases. Costs to the
environment or health are also not calculated; in fact, this is a constant criticism of positive
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
3
psychology, the newly minted science of wellbeing, which focuses more on the psychological
makeup of individuals and less on societal, political, and natural contexts in which they live
(Kern et al., 2019; Mead et al., 2019). Further, while more income generates higher life satisfaction,
this relationship does not hold true everywhere or in all conditions (Easterlin, 2015). For instance,
a decrease in positive emotion due to rising ambitions and lost hopes for one’s economy, coupled
with failed leadership in delivering jobs and rising social equality, underscores what is known
as the “unhappy development paradox” (Arampatzi, Burger, Ianchovichina, Röhricht, &
Veenhoven, 2015). Money is not all that matters; factors such as equality, access to opportunity,
and feelings of respect can highlight what is happening in societies that a nation’s GDP cannot.
1.2 Why Does Wellbeing Matter?
While this question has been answered by many researchers, a recap is helpful for those who
remain unconvinced of its necessity as a matter of policy and global research. There are many
reasons why wellbeing matters. First, individuals with greater wellbeing are known to generate
greater social good and are easier on the public purse. For instance, they are more likely to save
and control expenditures by consuming less (Guven, 2012). They are more likely to show
compassion, empathy, and more prosocial behavior (Johnson & Fredrickson, 2005; Nelson, 2009;
Rand, Kraft-Todd, & Gruber, 2015), be more socially engaged (Mehl, Vazire, Holleran, & Clark,
2010; Richards & Huppert, 2011) and volunteer to a greater degree (Priller & Shupp, 2011; Son &
Wilson, 2012; Thoits & Hewitt, 2001). More likely to make more ethical decisions (James &
Chymis, 2004), they also engage in less risky behavior, smoke less, and exercise more (Goudie,
Mukherjee, De Neve, Oswald, & Wu, 2012; Grant, Wardle, & Steptoe, 2009; Huang &
Humphreys, 2012). Individuals with greater wellbeing also tend to be healthier and live longer
(Boehm & Kubzansky, 2012; Sin, 2016; Wiest, Schüz, Webster, & Wurm, 2011). At school, greater
wellbeing translates into better grades (Durlak, Weissberg, Dymnicki, Taylor, & Schellinger,
2011; Suldo, Thalji, & Ferron, 2011), while workplaces benefit with happier employees showing
more productivity, engagement attitudes, and less sick time and absenteeism (Bockerman &
Ilmakunnas, 2012; Edmans, 2012; Harter, Schmidt, Asplund, Agrawal, & Killham, 2010; Harter,
Schmidt, & Hayes, 2002; Judge, Thoreson, Bono, & Patton, 2001; Krause, 2013; Oswald, Proto, &
Sgroi, 2012; Walsh, Boehm, & Lyubomirsky, 2018).
1.3 The Gallup World Poll (GWP)
The Gallup World Poll (2005-present) contains a core survey component that carries over
from year to year covering the range of overall wellbeing measurements on a continuum from
evaluative judgements of life (Cantril’s Ladder) to measures of affect and daily experiences
(reflections on the previous day). The core instrument also includes measures of law and order,
food and shelter, work quality, health, standard of living, citizen engagement, migration
intentions, views of governance, and demographic variables. Additional items and modules are
added based on current events and the needs of sponsoring entities. This current initiative seeks
to expand the core content to fill gaps in wellbeing research that align with Eastern scholar’s
findings and views.
In World Poll countries, Gallup surveys residents using probability-based sampling
methods. The samples are representative of the civilian, non-institutionalized national
population, aged 15 and older in the vast majority of countries. Exceptions to national coverage
include unsafe areas, very remote locations and low human-density areas. Typically, the sample
size is 1,000 adults in most countries, while in the most populous nations such as China, India,
and Russia, Gallup uses sample sizes of at least 2,000. The sampling of respondents and countries
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
4
represents more than 95% of the global population on any given year. This broad coverage of the
global population has led to improved interpretations of the relationships among many
wellbeing variables, including life satisfaction, age, health, and income across societies for
example (Deaton, 2008).
Gallup maintains a centralized level of research management and quality assurance coupled
with country-specific knowledge provided by its Regional Research Directors. This centralisation
offers a single point of contact and strong processes to ensure consistency, quality and
transparency. The World Poll data collection is divided into seven regions. Each region (see box)
is led by a Regional Research Director who is responsible for all phases of the research process
in his or her portfolio of countries. The Regional Research Director also oversees data collection
efforts, which are carried out by the local partners’ field teams. Good logistics are key to
collecting quality data in the most time-efficient manner. In face-to-face countries, Gallup’s local
data collection partners use a field plan to deploy their field teams most effectively across
geographies. In telephone countries, local partners must also manage the phone sample carefully
for maximum efficiency.
1.4 Why Additions are Necessary
Empirical research around wellbeing is rapidly growing. Indices must keep pace with these
scientific developments by including broader constructs that contribute to wellbeing, such as the
natural, social, and political settings in which humans live and thrive (Kern et al., 2019; Mead et
al., 2019). Further, not only is the literature evolving, but non-Western research in particular is
emerging, making salient a dearth of cross-cultural diversity in the science and measurement of
wellbeing (Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson, 2018). The predominant Western view of
wellbeing has implications for which of its aspects are researched, upon whom the research is
based, and what resulting norms emerge for what constitutes a good life. For example, cross-
cultural differences influence how societies define the self (Joshanloo, 2014), with individualism
(a view of the self as independent and focused on the promotion of one’s success and personal
attributes) being predominant in the West, and collectivism (where self construals are
interdependent, with value placed on maintaining relationships, fulfilling social roles, and
sacrificing for a collective good) being predominant in the East.
With the exception of cross-cultural research, which tends to be relegated to the periphery of
research findings, and the Gallup World Poll as well as a handful of other data collecting entities,
much of the wellbeing research to date has largely been based on what have been called
“WEIRD” samples, i.e., Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic populations
(Henrich et al., 2010) reflected in nearly 90% of the published psychology research (Arnett, 2009;
Christopher, Wendt, Marecek, & Goodman, 2014; Rad, Martingano, & Ginges, 2018). This
Western-centricity of psychology needs to be challenged to make research and its findings more
representative of all humans; moreover, representative research populations and a broader range
of wellbeing constructs will align more accurately with how communities around the world view
themselves (Kim et al., 2018) and reflect what truly comprises a good life.
2. The Process
A team of seven researchers was invited to the summit on August 5, 6, and 7, 2019. The
summit took place at the Shunkoin Temple in Kyoto, Japan and was organized by the Well-Being
for Planet Earth Foundation, a non-profit dedicated to making wellbeing science truly global and
representative of all human views and perspectives. Invited participants were chosen for their
range of cross-cultural research expertise, experience in developing measures and scales, in-
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
5
depth knowledge of non-traditional and/or non-Western views of wellbeing, and leadership in
philosophical and theoretical developments in the field of wellbeing. Participants presented their
findings and proposed research initiatives, focused on addressing current gaps in the literature
through the provision of context and content from non-Western regions of the world. They also
collectively proposed alternative and/or additional topics for the existing GWP and formulated
these as questionnaire items. These topics and items were discussed, debated, and voted upon.
The list below includes the final selection. Questions 1 through 6 are those with the highest votes,
which were put forward to Gallup for consideration. The remaining items (7 to 9) are those we
felt were worthy of consideration for future editions of the poll.
2.1 Proposed additions: Constructs, questions, and rationales
Broadly, the constructs and items we recommend expand Gallup’s current wellbeing
measures of life satisfaction (i.e., Cantril’s Ladder) and the ratio of high-arousal positive to
negative emotions, measures which primarily assess hedonic wellbeing (Kahneman, Diener, &
Schwarz, 1999; Ryan & Deci, 2001). With its focus on feeling good, experiencing pleasure,
enjoyment, and comfort, and reducing pain, hedonic wellbeing is vital to human flourishing
(Huta & Ryan, 2010). Also crucial is eudaimonic wellbeing (Ryan & Deci, 2001; Ryff & Singer,
2008), using and developing the best in oneself (Huta & Ryan, 2010), a definition of wellbeing
with roots in Aristotle’s virtue ethics that is concerned with mastery, purpose in life, and personal
growth. While hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing overlap and are distinct, each contributes to
wellbeing in complementary ways (Huta & Waterman, 2014; Ryan & Huta, 2009) and are both
necessary to living a full life (Joshanloo, 2016; Keyes, Shmotkin, & Ryff, 2002; Ryan & Deci, 2001).
Our items reflect both orientations as well as the basic psychological needs posited by Self-
Determination Theory (SDT, Ryan & Deci, 2000) that must be satisfied for individuals to flourish,
namely: autonomy, competence, and relatedness.
While Gallup will unveil its final additions to the GWP in time for its 2020 wave and may
modify the wording of items further, we have put forward a short list of additions we felt were:
(1) reflective of a comprehensive conceptualization of wellbeing, augmenting other wellbeing
measures in the current literature;
(2) most pressing to capture as global data does not yet exist for these items;
(3) inclusive of a wider, richer, and more in-depth range of non-Western worldviews not
currently captured by the poll;
(4) of emergent and dynamic interest as their relationships with other proposed concepts
have not yet been examined;
(5) useful items from which policy makers and other decision makers could take action and,
(6) demonstrative of the true complexity of wellbeing, highlighting cultural, religious, or
regional differences allowing for an examination of the factors that contribute to wellbeing across
and within global societies.
2.1.1 Proposal 1Relationship to nature: “I feel connected to nature and all of life.”
The human need for relatedness goes beyond connecting with fellow humans; it extends to
connecting with the greater-than-human natural world. Nature connectedness refers to an
emotional sensibility that one is part of the larger cycle of life and broader natural environment
(Leary, Tipsord, & Tate, 2008; Mayer & Frantz, 2004; Nisbet, Zelenski, & Murphy, 2011). Across
samples spanning four continents, empirical evidence has demonstrated that individuals who
feel emotionally connected with nature enjoy enhanced levels of both hedonic and eudaimonic
wellbeing (see meta-analyses by Capaldi, Dopko, & Zelenski, 2014; Pritchard, Richardson,
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
6
Sheffield, & McEwan, 2019). Greater feelings of nature connectedness are associated with higher
levels of happiness, life satisfaction, and positive emotions, in addition to higher levels of
autonomy, personal growth, environmental mastery, meaning in life, and positive relations with
others. Moreover, nature connectedness emerges as a significant, distinct predictor of many
happiness indicators, over and above other types of human social relationships and connections
(Zelenski & Nisbet, 2014). With regard to eudaimonic wellbeing, nature connectedness emerges
as either similar to or greater in predictive magnitude than socio-demographic and economic
benchmarks (Martin et al., forthcoming).
In sum, nature connectedness matters to wellbeing and researchers have made a case for
nature relatedness as a basic human psychological need in its own right (Baxter & Pelletier, 2019;
Hurly & Walker, 2019). Further, meta-analytic results of studies utilizing samples from North
and South America, Europe, Australasia, and Asia provide evidence for a robust, causal link
between nature connectedness and pro-environmental behaviours and activities that protect the
planet’s wellbeing (Mackay & Schmitt, 2019; Whitburn, Linklater, & Abrahamse, 2019). Given
this evidence, and in line with the summit’s sponsor, the Well-Being for Planet Earth Foundation,
we propose to include the item “I feel connected to nature and all of life”.
2.1.2 Proposal 2—Mastery: “I am capable of dealing with life’s challenges.”
A sense of mastery is one aspect in the definition of eudaimonic wellbeing. Individuals who
score high on this factor “have a sense of mastery and competence in managing the environment,
control a complex array of external activities, make effective use of surrounding opportunities,
and are able to choose/create contexts suitable to personal needs and values” (Ryff, 1995). Noted
earlier, within Self-Determination Theory (Ryan & Deci, 2000), a sense of competence or mastery
is one of three basic human psychological needs. While some research supports these needs as
evident across cultures (e.g., Chen et al., 2015), it has been suggested that the dimension of
mastery may reflect more Western, individualistic notions, with non-Western cultures placing
more emphasis on harmony withrather than mastery oftheir environment (Christopher,
1999; Joshanloo, 2013). Due to a lack of global data, however, it has been difficult to investigate
this claim and its consequences. Measuring mastery on a global scale would enable a better
quantification of the relative importance of mastery across different cultures, as well as its
dynamic interplay with other aspects of well-being. We therefore propose the inclusion of the
following item to assess self-perceived mastery: “I am capable of dealing with life’s challenges”.
2.1.3 Proposal 3—Meaning in Life: “My daily activities seem worthwhile to me.”
The desire for meaning is widely characterized as a fundamental motivation (Frankl, 1963;
Maslow, 1968; Williams, 2007) and cornerstone of eudaimonic wellbeing (Diener & Seligman,
2004; Huta & Ryan, 2010; Ryff & Singer, 1998; Steger, 2012). High levels of meaning in life predict
various wellbeing benefits including life satisfaction, positive emotions, high morale, vitality,
resiliency after trauma, self-worth, personal growth, and environmental mastery, to name a few
(see review, Steger, 2009). A consensus has emerged detailing meaning in life as a
multidimensional construct comprising three distinct facets: purposefeeling directed and
motivated by goals, coherence—feeling as though one’s life makes sense, and significance—
feeling that one’s life and activities matter (George & Park, 2016; Heintzelman & King, 2014;
Martela & Steger, 2016; Steger 2012). Although research has examined the purpose and coherence
aspects of meaning, the significance aspect of meaning in life has been less studied. Still, research
is emerging which suggests that a sense of significance, feeling that one’s life matters and is
worthwhile, is a central aspect of meaning (George & Park, 2016). Having a life worth living is
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
7
intimately connected with eudaimonia and aligned with the Japanese notion of ikigai, translated
as a sense of “life worth living” (Sone et al., 2008, p. 709). It is important to note that “significance
is not merely about any kind of positive and negative feelings in life, but about the sense of value
that arises when we evaluate our lives” (Martela & Steger, 2016. p. 538). In order to measure
significance, this central aspect of meaning in life, we propose to include the item “My daily
activities seem worthwhile to me”.
2.1.4 Proposal 4Low-arousal emotions: “Did you feel calm and at peace yesterday?”
As noted above, hedonic forms of wellbeing—often referred to as ‘subjective wellbeing
(Diener, 1994)are frequently operationalized as having two elements: a cognitive component,
usually framed as ‘life satisfaction’ (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985) and an affective
component, involving the ratio of positive to negative affect (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988).
Research into the affective component tends to emphasize and prioritize high arousal forms of
positive affect. However, such trends reflect the influence of researchers and participants from
Western nations, since other cultures, particularly Eastern ones, may place greater value on low
arousal positive emotions (Leu, Wang, & Koo, 2011), such as calmness and contentment. Possible
explanations include a certain ‘fear’ or wariness of high arousal emotions in such cultures
(Joshanloo, 2013). That said, low arousal positive emotions may be more universally valued than
widely realized. For instance, analyzing lay perceptions of happiness across five continents, Delle
Fave et al. (2016) found that the most prominent psychological definition was a sense of ‘inner
harmony’, featuring three subcomponents: inner peace; contentment; and balance. We align with
such findings, although we prefer to separate these subcomponents into two items: peace and
contentment (this item), and balance (the next). In order to measure low arousal emotions, we
propose to include the item, “Did you feel calm and at peace yesterday?”.
2.1.5 Proposal 5—Balance and harmony: “The various aspects of my life are in balance.”
A key insight from the summit was the recognition of the importance of balance and
harmony (see Lomas (forthcoming) for a conceptual review). These concepts are central to
Eastern conceptualizations of wellbeing (Wong, 2011), reflecting the influence of traditions such
as Taoism (Fang, 2012). At the same time, Delle Fave et al.’s (2016) research suggests their
importance might be more universally recognized. While Della Fave et al. grouped balance
together with peace and contentment into an overall construct of inner harmony, we separate
low arousal positive emotions (the previous item) from our conceptualization of balance and
harmony. Our discussions of the latter centered on these qualities not as forms of low arousal
affect, but rather as forms of dynamic equilibrium between dialectically related-contrasts across
many aspects of life. These include (but are not limited to) subjective emotional states, character,
activities, and self-other relations.
First, a full life involves a range of emotions, including negatively-valenced ones, as
illustrated by the paradigm of ‘second wave’ positive psychology (Lomas & Ivtzan, 2016). Thus,
there is a time for joy and a time for sadness, for acceptance and for anger, and so on, and
flourishing involves an optimal subjective balance of these oppositional qualities. Second, in
terms of character, following Aristotle’s notion of the ‘golden mean’, virtue and excellence are
found in the optimal balance between extremes (courage, for instance, treading a middle line
between timidity and rashness) (Kristjánsson, 2006). Third, with activities, wellbeing involves an
appropriate balance between various life elements, as per the concept of ‘work-life balance’
(Guest, 2002). Finally, good self-other relations include striking a balance between prioritizing
one’s individual needs with those of others (and also of the natural world). A flourishing life
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
8
involves many such balancing acts, with the overall configuration of these ideally being in a
pattern of harmony. In order to measure this perceived balance and harmony in life, we propose
the item “The various aspects of my life are in balance” (adapted from Kjell, Daukantaitė,
Hefferon, and Sikström’s (2016) Harmony in Life Scale).
2.1.6 Proposal 6—Relationship to group: “My happiness depends on the happiness of people close to me.”
Although individualism is often seen as a characteristic of Western cultures, rates of
individualism are growing worldwide as a function of increasing globalization (Hamamura,
2012; Santos, Varnum, & Grossmann, 2017). Interestingly, parallel rates of collectivism are not in
decline (e.g., Mesoudi, Magid, & Hussain, 2016) and much of the world remains collectively
organized. Given that much of the psychology research stems from individualistic nations
(Henrich et al., 2010), the orientation to ‘other’ is often treated as more of an anomaly than a
norm. Collectivism reflects the view that the self is interdependent, related, and less
differentiated from others than is the case in more individualistic contexts. In collectivistic
settings, there tends to be a priority on relationships, social duties and roles, and ensuring that
group prosperity and harmony take precedence (Grossman & Na, 2014). These narratives are
expressed in norms, behaviors, socialization practices, and even how information is processed
(Mesoudi et al., 2016). To capture what is still the collectivistic norm in many cultures, the
proposed item reflects the interdependency individuals have with one another, and that
individual wellbeing is tightly bound to collective wellbeing. To measure this relationship to the
group, we propose to include the item “My happiness depends on the happiness of people close
to me”.
2.1.7 Proposal 7—Relationship with government: “To what extent do you feel that your government
and/or society respects people for who they are (for example, their culture, religion, sexual, or political
orientation)?”
As noted, SDT proposes that autonomy is a basic human need (Ryan & Deci, 2000). In line
with this, wellbeing tends to increase with greater political, economic, and personal freedoms
(Inglehart, Foa, Peterson, & Welzel, 2008; Ngamaba & Soni, 2018; Verme, 2009; Welzel &
Inglehart, 2010). As such, it matters greatly the extent to which governments and societies are
open, inclusive, and demonstrative of respect towards individuals with alternative lifestyles,
cultures, religions, political orientations, and socioeconomic status. For instance, studies show
that minorities (e.g., sexual minorities) are more satisfied in welcoming societies that are tolerant
and inclusive (versus discriminatory, indifferent, or hostile). Further, it seems that this
relationship between life satisfaction and tolerant, inclusive societies is true for everyone, not
only for individuals of minority groups (Berggren, Bjørnskov, & Nilsson, 2017; Kogan, Shen, &
Siegert, 2018). Studies support that social exclusion decreases subjective wellbeing (Verkuyten,
2008) and discrimination based on sexual minority status, for example, is costly to individuals in
terms of life satisfaction and subjective wellbeing as they suffer from disparities in employment,
friendships, physical health, income, and social supports (Conlin, Douglass, & Ouch, 2019;
Powdthavee & Wooden, 2015). These same relationships between subjective wellbeing and
discrimination against racial minorities is also evident (Cormack, Stanley, & Harris, 2018;
Paradies et al., 2015; Yoo, Kim, & Lee, 2018). In order to measure the relationship between
individuals and their government and society (i.e., as either upholding or infringing upon their
personal freedoms and dignities), we propose to include the item “To what extent do you feel
that your government and/or society respects people for who they are (for example, their culture,
religion, sexual, or political orientation)?”
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
9
2.1.8 Proposal 8—Leisure: “To what extent are you satisfied with how you spend your free time?”
Aristotle believed that leisure, “time away from unpleasant obligations” (Stebbins, 2001, pg.
4), was central to a satisfying life, in part because its activities were freely chosen. Modern
theories not only refer to the freedom of choice inherent in leisure activity (Gunter & Gunter,
1980; Iso-Ahola, 1988; Kelly, 1972), linking it to the satisfaction of our basic human need for
autonomy, but theories and research also refer to the satisfaction of needs related to meaning in
life, mastery, and relatedness that leisure offers (Newman, Tay, & Deiner, 2014). While frequency
of engaging in leisure activities is associated with happiness and life satisfaction (e.g., Newman
et al., 2014; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2004), it is leisure satisfaction, how pleased one is with those
experiences, that is paramount to wellbeing (see review, Kuykendall, Boemerman, & Zhu, 2018).
For instance, meta-analytic results of longitudinal and experimental studies suggest a causal
effect of leisure satisfaction on hedonic wellbeing (Kuykendall, Tay, & Ng, 2015). While such
studies involved samples from many countries (i.e., Australia, Canada, England, Finland,
Germany, Japan, Korea, Netherlands, Taiwan, and the United States), global data is needed.
Moreover, linkages between leisure satisfaction and markers of eudaimonic well-being have yet
to be examined. It is possible that cultural views regarding the value of work and leisure could
attenuate or enhance the relationship between leisure satisfaction and wellbeing. Yet, no
significant difference in the strength of this relationship was evident in Kuykendall and
colleagues’ (2015) meta-analysis across samples from countries that highly value leisure (e.g.,
European countries) compared to nations which value it to a lesser extent (e.g., United States).
Additional data is needed to examine if these results hold around the globe. In order to measure
leisure satisfaction, we propose to include the item “To what extent are you satisfied with how
you spend your free time?”.
2.1.9 Proposal 9—Resilience: “When life is difficult, I recover quickly.”
Resilient individuals are those who bounce back from difficulties, adapt to changing
demands of stressful experiences, and move forward with few apparent disruptions to their daily
functioning (Carver, 1998; Fletcher & Sarkar, 2013). Many studies demonstrate that resiliency is
associated with greater happiness and satisfaction with life in the short-term and over one’s
lifetime (e.g., Doyle et al., 2015; Haddadi & Besharat, 2010; Seery, Holman, & Silver, 2010; Smith
& Holllinger-Smith, 2015). Resilient individuals still experience a range of negative emotions
during stressful times, just as their less resilient counterparts do; indeed, highly resilient older
adults show greater emotional complexity (i.e., the co-occurrence of positive and negative
emotions) compared to the less resilient (Ong, Bergeman, & Boker, 2009).
Yet, as noted earlier in relation to balance and harmony, a full life necessarily includes a range
of emotion (Lomas & Ivtzan, 2016). The strength that resilient individuals exhibit during stressful
times comes not from ignoring negative emotions evoked by trauma and stress, rather their
strength seems to lie in their ability to draw on positive emotions to regulate their overall affect
(Tugade & Fredrickson, 2004). In turn, resiliency itself produces positive emotions, leading to an
upward cycle of wellbeing (Cohn, Fredrickson, Brown, Mikels, & Conway, 2009; Zautra,
Johnson, & Davis, 2005). Others have suggested that meaning in life may be a critical link to
resilience (Weathers, Aiena, Blackwell, & Schulenberg, 2016). While Southwick and Charney
(2018) identified meaning in life as one of many factors correlated with resiliency, there is a
paucity of studies examining the links between them. When added to Gallup’s existing measures
of positive affect and life satisfaction, inclusion of our suggested item on meaning in life and
resilience could help elucidate relationships among these factors across nations. To measure
resiliency, we propose to include the item “When life is difficult, I recover quickly.”
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
10
2.2 Research Potential of Our Proposed Additions
The research potential stemming from an expanded global measure of wellbeing is immense.
While the concept of wellbeing is much richer than life satisfaction alone, the lack of global data
on hedonic and eudaimonic facets of wellbeing has hampered progress in the field. The inclusion
of these newly proposed items not only allows for the measurement of a broader wellbeing
definition across the world, but also allows for refined and nuanced cross-cultural comparisons
of wellbeing. For example, having multiple items that measure different aspects of wellbeing
creates novel opportunities for examining the factor-structure or network-structure of wellbeing
across different countries and cultures. Additionally, given the abundance of other related
measures present in the GWP, the inclusion of these new wellbeing items will allow for a better
examination of the causes of variation in wellbeing across the globe. It is our hope that these new
additions will serve to push the wellbeing agenda to the forefront of government policy, and,
once data has been gathered, offer evidence to address current gaps in our knowledge.
3. Conclusion
The utility of the GWP’s assessment of wellbeing across the globe, and national reports which
stem from it such the WHR and Happy Planet Index, cannot be overlooked. The GWP has helped
to garner greater interest in wellbeing as a topic of serious academic concern, as well as capture
the attention of governments in its use as an overarching policy framework. As one of the first
global wellbeing assessments, it has raised the profile of wellbeing everywhere. However, it is
now time for a new, comprehensive measure of global wellbeing to be used to successfully reflect
the preoccupations and opportunities of societies and individuals across the world and truly
advance a representative human science of wellbeing. We hope to have captured these missing
nuances in our proposed items and stimulate the growing and ever evolving body of research to
capture such new developments in both conceptualizations and measurements.
Authors
Louise Lambert
United Arab Emirates University
ltlamber@yahoo.com
Tim Lomas
University of East London
Margot P van de Weijer
VU University
Holli Anne Passmore
University of Derby
Mohsen Joshanloo
The University of Melbourne
Jim Harter
Gallup
Yoshiki Ishikawa
Well-Being for Planet Earth Foundation
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
11
Alden Lai
New York University
Takuya Kitagawa
Rakuten
Dominique Chen
Waseda University
Takafumi Kawakami
Shunkoin Temple
Hiroaki Miyata
Keio University
Ed Diener
University of Virginia
University of Utah
Publishing Timeline
Received 27 October 2019
Accepted 20 January 2020
Published 1 June 2020
References
Adler, A., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2016). Using wellbeing for public policy: Theory, measurement, and
recommendations. International Journal of Wellbeing, 6(1), 1-35. https://doi.org/10.5502/ijw.v6i1.429
Arampatzi, E., Burger, M. J., Ianchovichina, E., Röhricht, T., & Veenhoven, R. (2015). Unhappy
development: Dissatisfaction with life on the eve of the Arab spring. World Bank Policy Research
Working Paper No. 7488. Retrieved from https://ssrn.com/abstract=2690017
Arnett, J. J. (2009). The neglected 95%, a challenge to psychology’s philosophy of science. American
Psychologist, 64(6), 571-574. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016723
Baxter, D. E., & Pelletier, L. G. (2019). Is nature relatedness a basic human psychological need? A critical
examination of the extant literature. Canadian Psychology, 60, 21-34. doi:10.1037/cap0000145
Berggren, N., Bjørnskov, C., & Nilsson, T. (2016). What aspects of society matter for the quality of life of a
minority? Global evidence from the new Gay Happiness Index. Social Indicators Research, 132(3),
11631192. doi:10.1007/s11205-016-1340-3
Bockerman, P., & Ilmakunnas, P. (2012). The job-satisfaction-productivity nexus: A study using matched
survey and register data. Industrial & Labor Relations Review, 65, 244-262.
https://doi.org/10.1177/001979391206500203
Boehm, J. K., & Kubzansky, L. D. (2012). The heart's content: The association between positive
psychological wellbeing and cardiovascular health. Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 655-691.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027448
Cantril, H. (1965). The pattern of human concerns. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Capaldi, C. A., Dopko, R. L., & Zelenski, J. M. (2014). The relationship between nature connectedness and
happiness: A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 976. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00976
Carver, C. S. (1998). Resilience and thriving: Issues, models and linkages. Journal of Social Issues, 54, 245
266. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1998.tb01217.x
Chen, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Beyers, W., Boone, L., Deci, E. L., der Kaap-Deeder, J., … Verstuyf, J. (2015).
Basic psychological need satisfaction, need frustration, and need strengths across four cultures.
Motivation and Emotion, 39, 216-236. doi:10.1007/s11031-014-9450-1
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
12
Christopher, J. C. (1999). Situating psychological well‐being: Exploring the cultural roots of its theory and
research. Journal of Counseling & Development, 77(2), 141-152. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-
6676.1999.tb02434.x
Christopher, J. C., Wendt, D. C., Marecek, J., & Goodman, D. M. (2014). Critical cultural awareness:
Contributions to a globalizing psychology. American Psychologist, 69(7), 645-655.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036851
Cohn, M. A., Fredrickson, B. L., Brown, S. L., Mikels, J. A., & Conway, A. M. (2009). Happiness unpacked:
Positive emotions increase life satisfaction by building resilience. Emotions, 9, 361368.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015952
Conlin, S. E., Douglass, R. P., & Ouch, S. (2019). Discrimination, subjective wellbeing, and the role of
gender: A mediation model of LGB minority stress. Journal of Homosexuality, 66(2), 238-259. doi:
10.1080/00918369.2017.1398023
Cormack, D., Stanley, J., & Harris, R. (2018). Multiple forms of discrimination and relationships with health
and wellbeing: Findings from national cross-sectional surveys in Aotearoa/New Zealand.
International Journal for Equity in Health, 17, Article 26. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12939-018-0735-y
de Chavez, A. C., Backett-Milburn, K., Parry, O., & Platt, S. (2005). Understanding and researching
wellbeing: Its usage in different disciplines and potential for health research and health promotion.
Health Education Journal, 64(1), 70-87. https://doi.org/10.1177/001789690506400108
Deaton, A. (2008). Income, health, and well-being around the world: Evidence from the Gallup World Poll.
Journal of Economic Perspectives, 22(2), 53-72. https://doi.org/10.1257/jep.22.2.53
Delle Fave, A., Brdar, I., Wissing, M. P., Araujo, U., Castro Solano, A., Freire, T., ... Nakamura, J. (2 016).
Lay definitions of happiness across nations: The primacy of inner harmony and relational
connectedness. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 30. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00030
Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-being: Progress and opportunities. Social Indicators Research,
31(2), 103-157. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01207052
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71-75. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
Diener, E., Kahneman, D., Arora, R., Harter, J., & Tov, W. (2009). Income’s differential influence on
judgments of life versus affective well-being. In E. Diener (Ed.), Assessing well-being (pp. 233-246),
Social Indicators Research Series (vol. 39). Dordrecht: Springer.
Diener, E., Kahneman, D., Tov, W., & Arora, R. (2010). Income’s association with judgments of life versus
feelings. In E. Diener, J. Helliwell, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), International differences in well-being (pp.
315). New York: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199732739.003.0001
Diener, E., Ng, W., Harter, J., & Arora, R. (2010). Wealth and happiness across the world: Material
prosperity predicts life evaluation, whereas psychosocial prosperity predicts positive feeling. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(1), 52-61. doi: 10.1037/a0018066.
Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Beyond money: Toward an economy of well-being. Psychological
Science in the Public Interest, 5, 1-31. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00501001.x
Doyle, N., MacLachlan, M., Fraser, A., Stilz, R., Lismont, K., McVeigh, J. (2015). Resilience and well-
being amongst seafarers: Cross-sectional study of crew across 51 ships. International Archives of
Occupational and Environmental Health, 89, 199-209. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00420-015-1063-9
Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, R. D., & Schellinger, K. B. (2011). The impact of
enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: A meta-analysis of school-based universal
interventions. Child Development, 82(1), 405432. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01564.x
Easterlin, R. A. (2015). Happiness and economic growth: The evidence. In W. Glatzer, L. Camfield, V.
Møller, & M. Rojas (Eds.), Global handbook of quality of life: Exploration of wellbeing of nations and
continents (pp. 283-299). New York, NY: Springer.
Edmans, A. (2012). The link between job satisfaction and firm value, with implications for corporate social
responsibility. Academy of Management Perspectives, 26(4), 1-19.
https://doi.org/10.5465/amp.2012.0046
Fang, T. (2012). Yin Yang: A new perspective on culture. Management and Organization Review, 8(1), 25
50. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1740-8784.2011.00221.x
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
13
Fletcher, D., & Sarkar, M. (2013). Psychological resilience: A review and critique of definitions, concepts,
and theory. European Psychologist, 18, 12-23. doi:10.1027/1016-9040/a000124
Frankl, V. E. (1963). Man’s search for meaning. New York, NY: First Washington Square Press.
George, L. S., & Park, C. L. (2016). Meaning in life as comprehension, purpose, and mattering: Toward
integration and new research questions. Review of General Psychology, 20, 205220.
doi:10.1037/pr0000077
Goudie, R., Mukherjee, S., De Neve, J.-E., Oswald, A. J., & Wu, S. (2012). Happiness as a driver of risk-
avoiding behavior. The Centre for Economic Performance, Discussion Paper No. 1126. Retrieved from
http://cep.lse.ac.uk/pubs/download/dp1126.pdf
Grant, N., Wardle, J., & Steptoe, A. (2009). The relationship between life satisfaction and health behavior:
A cross-cultural analysis of young adults. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 16, 259-268.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-009-9032-x
Grossmann, I., & Na, J. (2014). Research in culture and psychology: Past lessons and future challenges.
Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5, 114. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1267
Guest, D. E. (2002). Perspectives on the study of work-life balance. Social Science Information, 41(2), 255-
279. https://doi.org/10.1177/0539018402041002005
Gunter, B. G., & Gunter, N. C. (1980). Leisure styles: A conceptual framework for modern leisure. The
Sociological Quarterly, 21, 361-374. doi:10.1111/j.1533-8525.1980.tb00618.x
Guven, C. (2012). Reversing the question: Does happiness affect consumption and savings behavior?
Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(4), 701-717. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2012.01.002
Haddadi, P., & Besharat, M. A. (2010). Resilience, vulnerability and mental health. Procedia: Social and
Behavioural Sciences, 5, 639642. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.07.157
Hamamura, T. (2012). Are cultures becoming individualistic? A cross-temporal comparison of
individualism-collectivism in the United States and Japan. Personality and Social Psychology Review,
16, 324. doi:10.1177/10888 68311411587
Harter, J. K., Schmidt, F. L., & Hayes, T. L. (2002). Business-unit-level relationship between employee
satisfaction, employee engagement, and business outcomes: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 87(2), 268-279. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.87.2.268
Harter, J. K., Schmidt, F. L., Asplund, J. W., Killham, E. A., & Agrawal, S. (2010). Causal impact of employee
work perceptions on the bottom line of organizations. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(4), 378-
389. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610374589
Heintzelman, S. J., & King, L. A. (2014). Life is pretty meaningful. American Psychologist, 69, 561574.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035049
Helliwell, J., Huang, H., & Wang, S. (2017). Social foundations of world happiness. In J. Helliwell, R.
Layard, & J. Sachs (Eds.), World Happiness Report 2017. Retrieved from:
https://s3.amazonaws.com/happiness-report/2017/HR17.pdf
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 33(2-3), 61-83. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
Huang, H., & Humphreys, B. R. (2012). Sports participation and happiness: Evidence from US microdata.
Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(4), 776793. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2012.02.007
Hurly, J., & Walker, G. J. (2019). Nature in our lives: Examining the human need for nature relatedness as
a basic psychological need. Journal of Leisure Research, 50, 290-310.
doi:10.1080/00222216.2019.1578939
Huta, V., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). Pursuing pleasure or virtue: The differential and overlapping well-being
benefits of hedonic and eudaimonic motives. Journal of Happiness Studies, 11, 735-762.
doi:10.1007/s10902-009-171-4
Huta, V., & Waterman, A. S. (2014). Eudaimonia and its distinction from hedonia: Developing a
classification and terminology for understanding conceptual and operational definitions. Journal of
Happiness Studies, 15, 1425-1456. doi:10.100/s10902-013-9485-0
Inglehart, R., Foa, R., Peterson, C., & Welzel, C. (2008). Development, freedom and rising happiness: A
global perspective 1981-2006. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(4), 264-285. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-
6924.2008.00078.x
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
14
Iso-Ahola, S. (1988). The social psychology of leisure: Past, present, and future research. In L. A. Barnett
(Ed.), Research about leisure (pp. 76-93). Champaign, IL: Sagamo.
James H. S. Jr., & Chymis A. G. (2004). Are happy people ethical people? Evidence from North America
and Europe (Working Paper No. AEWP 2004-08). Department of Agricultural Economics. Retrieved
from http://ageconsearch.umn.edu/bitstream/26034/1/wp040008.pdf
Johnson, K. J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2005). "We all look the same to me": Positive emotions eliminate the
own-race bias in face recognition. Psychological Science, 16, 875-881. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
9280.2005.01631.x
Joshanloo, M. (2013). The influence of fear of happiness beliefs on responses to the satisfaction with life
scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(5), 647-651. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.011
Joshanloo, M. (2014). Eastern conceptualizations of happiness: Fundamental differences with Western
views. Journal of Happiness Studies, 15(2), 475-493. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-013-9431-1
Joshanloo, M. (2016). Revisiting the empirical distinction between hedonic and eudaimonic aspects of well-
being using exploratory structural equation modeling. Journal of Happiness Studies, 17(5), 2023-2036.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9683-z
Joshanloo, M. (2018). Optimal human functioning around the world: A new index of eudaimonic well-
being in 166 nations. British Journal of Psychology, 109(4), 637-655.
Joshanloo, M., Jovanović, V., & Taylor, T. (2019). A multidimensional understanding of prosperity and
well-being at country level: Data-driven explorations. PLoS ONE 14(10): e0223221.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223221
Judge, T. A., Thoresen, C. J., Bono, J. E., & Patton, G. K. (2001). The job satisfactionjob performance
relationship: A qualitative and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 127(3), 376-407.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.3.376
Kelly, J. R. (1972). Work and leisure: A simplified paradigm. Journal of Leisure Research, 4, 50-62.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00222216.1972.11970057
Kern, M. L., Williams, P., Spong, C., Colla, R., Sharma, K., Downie, A., Oades, L. G. (2019). Systems
informed positive psychology. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 111.
doi:10.1080/17439760.2019.1639799
Kristjánsson, K. (2006). “Emotional intelligence” in the classroom? An Aristotelian critique. Educational
Theory, 56(1), 39-56. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-5446.2006.00002.x
Kahneman, D., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (Eds.). (1999). Well-being: Foundations of hedonic psychology.
New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.
Keyes, C. L., Shmotkin, D., & Ryff, C. D. (2002). Optimizing well-being: The empirical encounter of two
traditions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 1007-1022. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
3514.82.6.1007
Kim, H., Doiron, K. M., Warren, M. A., & Donaldson, S. I. (2018). The international landscape of positive
psychology research: A systematic review. International Journal of Wellbeing, 8(1), 50 -70.
doi:10.5502/ijw.v8i1.651
Kjell, O., Daukantaitė, D., Hefferon, K., & Sikström, S. (2016). The harmony in life scale complements the
satisfaction with life scale: Expanding the conceptualization of the cognitive component of subjective
well-being. Social Indicators Research, 126(2), 893-919. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-015-0903-z
Kogan, I., Shen, J., & Siegert, M. (2018). What makes a satisfied immigrant? Host-country characteristics
and immigrants’ life satisfaction in eighteen European countries. Journal of Happiness Studies, 19(6),
1783 1809. doi:10.1007/s10902-017-9896-4
Krause, A. (2013). Don’t worry, be happy? Happiness and reemployment. Journal of Economic Behavior
& Organization, 96(C), 1-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2013.09.002
Kuykendall, L., Boemerman, L., & Zhu, Z. (2018). The importance of leisure for subjective well-being. In
E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.), Handbook of well-being. Salt Lake City, UT: DEF Publishers.
Kuykendall L., Tay, L., & Ng, V. (2015). Leisure engagement and subjective well-being: A meta-analysis.
Psychological Bulletin, 141, 364-403. doi:10.1037/a0038508
Lambert, L., Mulay-Shah, A., Warren, M. A., & Younis, Y. (2019). The pursuit of national wellbeing policies
across the GCC: The what, how, and why? In L. Lambert, & N. Pasha-Zaidi (Eds.), Positive psychology
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
15
in the Middle East/North Africa: Research, policy, and practise (pp. 11-35). Cham, Switzerland:
Springer.
Leary, M. R., Tipsord, J. M., & Tate, E. B. (2008). Allo-inclusive identity: Incorporating the social and natural
worlds into one’s sense of self. In H. A. Wayment & J. J. Bauer (Eds.), Transcending self-interest:
Psychological explorations of the quiet ego (pp. 137147). Washington, DC: APA.
https://doi.org/10.1037/11771-013
Leu, J., Wang, J., & Koo, K. (2011). Are positive emotions just as “positive” across cultures? Emotion, 11(4),
994-999. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021332
Lomas, T. (forthcoming). Life balance and harmony.
Lomas, T. (2015). Positive cross-cultural psychology: Exploring similarity and difference in constructions
and experiences of wellbeing. International Journal of Wellbeing, 5(4), 60-77. doi:10.5502/ijw.v5i4.437
Lomas, T., Hefferon, K., & Ivtzan, I. (2015). The LIFE model: A meta-theoretical conceptual map for applied
positive psychology. Journal of Happiness Studies, 16(5), 1347-1364. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-
014-9563-y
Lomas, T., & Ivtzan, I. (2016). Second wave positive psychology: Exploring the positivenegative dialectics
of wellbeing. Journal of Happiness Studies, 17(4), 1753-1768. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9668-
y
Mackay, C. M. L., & Schmitt, M. T. (2019). Do people who feel connected to nature do more to protect it?
A meta-analysis. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 65. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.101323
Martela, F., & Ryan, R. M. (2014). The benefits of benevolence: Basic psychological needs, beneficence, and
the enhancement of wellbeing. Journal of Personality, 84, 750-764. doi:10.1111/jopy.12215
Martela, F., & Steger, M. F. (2016). The three meanings of meaning in life: Distinguishing coherence,
purpose, and significance. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 11, 531-545.
doi:10.1080/17439760.2015.1137623
Martin, L., White, M. P., Hunt, A., Richardson, M., Pahl, S., & Burt, J. (forthcoming). Nature contact, nature
connectedness and associations with health, wellbeing and pro-environmental behaviors.
Maslow, A. H. (1968). Toward a psychology of being (2nd ed.). New York, NY: John Wiley.
Mayer, F. S., & Frantz, C. M. (2004). The Connectedness to Nature Scale: A measure of individuals’ feeling
in community with nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 504-515.
doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.10.001
Mead, J., Fisher, Z., Wilkie, L., Gibbs, K., Pridmore, J., Tree, J., & Kemp, A. (2019). Rethinking wellbeing:
Toward a more ethical science of wellbeing that considers current and future generations. Authorea,
https://doi.org/10.22541/au.156649190.08734276 Retrieved from
https://www.authorea.com/users/9886/articles/388348-rethinking-wellbeing-toward-a-more-ethical-
science-of-wellbeing-that-considers-current-and-future-generations
Mehl, M. R., Vazire, S., Holleran, S. E., & Clark, C. S. (2010). Eavesdropping on happiness: Wellbeing is
related to having less small talk and more substantive conversations. Psychological Science, 21(4), 539
541. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610362675
Mesoudi, A., Magid, K., & Hussain, D. (2016). How do people become W.E.I.R.D.? Migration reveals the
cultural transmission mechanisms underlying variation in psychological processes. PLoS ONE 11(1),
e0147162. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0147162
Morrison, M., Tay, L., & Diener, E. (2011). Subjective well-being and national satisfaction: Findings from a
worldwide survey. Psychological Science, 22(2), 166-171. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610396224
Nelson, D. (2009). Feeling good and open-minded: The impact of positive affect on cross cultural empathic
responding. Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 5363. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760802357859
Newman, D. B., Tay, L., & Diener, E. (2014). Leisure and subjective well-being: A model of psychological
mechanisms as mediating factors. Journal of Happiness Studies, 15, 555-578.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-013-9435-x
Ngamaba, K. H., & Soni, D. (2018). Are happiness and life satisfaction different across religious groups?
Exploring determinants of happiness and life satisfaction. Journal of Religion and Health, 57(6), 2118-
2139. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-017-0481-2
Nikolova, M. (2016). Happiness and development. IZA Discussion Paper No. 10088. Retrieved from
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
16
https://ssrn.com/abstract=2819369
Nisbet, E. K., Zelenski, J. M., & Murphy, S. A. (2011). Happiness is in our nature: Exploring nature
relatedness as a contributor to subjective well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 12, 303-322.
doi:10.1007/s10902-010-9197-7
Oishi, S., & Schimmack, U. (2010). Culture and well-being: A new inquiry into the psychological wealth of
nations. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 463471. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610375561
Ong, A. D., Bergeman, C. S., & Boker, S. M. (2009). Resilience comes of age: Defining features in later
adulthood. Journal of Personality, 77, 17771804. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00600.x
Ott, J. C. (2010). Good governance and happiness in nations: Technical quality precedes democracy and
quality beats size. Journal of Happiness Studies, 11(3), 353-368. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-009-
9144-7
Oswald, A. J., Proto, E., & Sgroi, D. (2012). Happiness and productivity. University of Warwick. Retrieved
from http://www.andrewoswald.com/docs/6MayOsProtoSgroi2012.pdf
Paradies, Y., Ben, J., Denson, N., Elias, A., Priest, N., Pieterse, A., & Gee, G. (2015). Racism as a determinant
of health: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PloS One, 10(9), e0138511.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138511
Powdthavee, N., & Wooden, M. (2015). Life satisfaction and sexual minorities: Evidence from Australia
and the United Kingdom. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 116, 107126.
doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2015.04.012
Priller, E., & Schupp, J. (2011). Social and economic characteristics of financial and blood donors in
Germany. DIW Economic Bulletin, 6, 23-30.
Pritchard, A., Richardson, M., Sheffield, D., & McEwan, K. (2019). The relationship between nature
connectedness and eudaimonic well-being: A meta-analysis. Journal of Happiness Studies.
doi:10.1007/s10902-019-00118-6
Rad, M. S., Martingano, A. J., & Ginges, J. (2018). Toward a psychology of Homo sapiens: Making
psychological science more representative of the human population. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 115(45), 11401-11405. doi:10.1073/pnas.1721165115
Rand, D. G., Kraft-Todd, G., & Gruber, J. (2015). The collective benefits of feeling good and letting go:
Positive emotion and (dis)inhibition interact to predict cooperative behavior. PLoS ONE, 10(1),
e0117426. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117426
Richards, M., & Huppert, F. A. (2011). Do positive children become positive adults? Evidence from a
longitudinal birth cohort study. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 6(1), 7587.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2011.536655
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation,
social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68-78. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-
066X.55.1.68
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic
and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 141-166.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141
Ryan, R. M., & Huta, V. (2009). Wellness as healthy functioning or wellness as happiness: the importance
of eudaimonic thinking (response to the Kashdan et al. and Waterman discussion). The Journal of
Positive Psychology, 4, 202-204. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760902844285
Ryff, C. D. (1995). Psychological well-being in adult life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4(4),
99-104. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772395
Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. (1998). The contours of positive human health. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 1-28.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0901_1
Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. H. (2008). Know thyself and become what you are: A eudaimonic approach to
psychological well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9(1), 13-39. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-006-
9019-0
Santos, H. C., Varnum, M. E. W., & Grossmann, I. (2017). Global increases in individualism. Psychological
Science, 28(9), 12281239. doi:10.1177/0956797617700622
Seery, M. D., Holman, E. A., & Silver, R. C. (2010). Whatever does not kill us: Cumulative lifetime adversity,
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
17
vulnerability, and resilience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 10251041.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021344
Sheldon, K. M., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2004). Achieving sustainable new happiness: Prospects, practices, and
prescriptions. In P. A. Linley & S. Joseph (Eds.), Positive psychology in practice (pp. 127145).
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470939338.ch8
Sin, N. L. (2016). The protective role of positive wellbeing in cardiovascular disease: Review of current
evidence, mechanisms, and clinical implications. Current Cardiology Reports, 11(18), 106.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11886-016-0792-z
Smith, J. L., & Hollinger-Smith, L. (2015). Savoring, resilience, and psychological well-being in older adults.
Aging & Mental Health, 19, 192-200. doi:10.1080/1360863.2014.986647
Son, J., & Wilson, J. (2012). Volunteer work and hedonic, eudemonic, and social wellbeing. Sociological
Forum, 27(3), 658-681. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1573-7861.2012.01340.x
Sone, T., Nakaya, N., Ohmori, K., Shimazu, T., Higashiguchi, M., Kakizaki, M., ... Tsuji, I. (2008). Sense of
life worth living (ikigai) and mortality in Japan: Ohsaki study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70, 709715.
https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e31817e7e64
Southwick, S., & Charney, D. (2018). Resilience: The science of mastering life’s greatest challenges (2nd
ed.). Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108349246
Stebbins, R. A. (2001). Serious leisure. Society, 38, 5357. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12115-001-1023-8
Steger, M. F. (2009). Meaning in life. In S. J. Lopez (Ed.), Oxford Handbook of Positive Psychology (2nd
ed.) (pp. 679-687). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195187243.013.0064
Steger, M. F. (2012). Experiencing meaning in life: Optimal functioning at the nexus if well-being,
psychopathology, and spirituality. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), Human Quest for Meaning: Theories,
Research, and Applications (pp. 165-184). Taylor & Francis: Florence.
Stiglitz, J., Sen, A., & Fitoussi, J.-P. (2009). The measurement of economic performance and social progress
revisited. Commission on the Measurement of Economic Performance and Social Progress, Paris,
France. Retrieved from http://www.ofce.sciences-po.fr/pdf/dtravail/WP2009-33.pdf
Suldo, S. M., Thalji, A., & Ferron, J. (2011). Longitudinal academic outcomes predicted by early adolescents'
subjective wellbeing, psychopathology, and mental health status yielded from a dual-factor model.
Journal of Positive Psychology, 6(1), 17-30. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2010.536774
Thoits, P. A., & Hewitt, L. N. (2001). Volunteer work and wellbeing. Journal of Health and Social Behavior,
42, 115-131. https://doi.org/10.2307/3090173
Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back
from negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 320333.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.3202004-10747-009
Uchida, Y., & Oishi, S. (2016). The happiness of individuals and the collective. Japanese Psychological
Research, 58(1), 125141. doi:10.1111/jpr.12103
Verkuyten, M. (2008). Life satisfaction among ethnic minorities: The role of discrimination and group
identification. Social Indicators Research, 89(3), 391404. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-008-9239-2
Verme, P. (2009). Happiness, freedom and control. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 71,
146-161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2009.04.008
Walsh, L. C., Boehm, J. K., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2018). Does happiness promote career success? Revisiting
the evidence. Journal of Career Assessment, 26, 199-219. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069072717751441
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive
and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063-1070.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
Weathers, L. N., Aiena, B. J., Blackwell, M. A., & Schulenberg, S. E. (2016). The significance of meaning to
conceptualizations of resilience and posttraumatic growth: Strengthening the foundation for research
and practice. In P. Russo-Netzer, S. E. Schulenberg, & A. Batthyany (Eds.), Clinical perspectives on
meaning (pp. 149-169). Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-41397-6_8
Towards a greater global understanding of wellbeing
Lambert, Lomas, van de Weijer, Passmore, Joshanloo, Harter, Ishikawa, Lai, Kitagawa, Chen, Kawakami, Miyata & Diener
www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org
18
Welzel, C., & Inglehart, R. (2010). Agency, values, and well-being: A human development model. Social
Indicators Research, 97, 43-63. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-009-9557-z
Whitburn, J., Linklater, W., & Abrahamse, W. (2019). Meta-analysis of human connection to nature and
proenvironmental behavior. Conversation Biology, doi:10.1111/cobi.13381
Wiest, M., Schüz, B., Webster, N., & Wurm, S. (2011). Subjective wellbeing and mortality revisited:
Differential effects of cognitive and emotional facets of wellbeing on mortality. Health Psychology,
30(6), 728-735. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023839
Williams, K. D. (2007). Ostracism. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 425-452.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085641
Wong, P. T. (2011). Positive psychology 2.0: Towards a balanced interactive model of the good life.
Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 52(2), 69-81. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022511
Yoo, H. C., Kim, A. Y., & Lee, R. M. (2018). Race and subjective well-being: Critical race perspective and
empirical review of key predictors. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.), Handbook of well-being.
Salt Lake City, UT (USA): DEF Publishers. DOI:nobascholar.com
Zautra, A. J., Johnson, L. M., & Davis, M. C. (2005). Positive affect as a source of resilience for women in
chronic pain. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 212220. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
006X.73.2.212
... In addition, researchers sometimes combine several single measurements to assess an individual's well-being [6]. The presence of multiple scales of well-being makes it necessary and vital to review and compare these scales [7,8] in order to develop a clearer conceptualisation of well-being assessment. For novice researchers in the field of wellbeing, this wide variety of types/domains of well-being creates an additional screening burden as they will need to understand the most appropriate scale to select for a specified area of research. ...
... Only 18 non-Western countries were included in studies (42 times in total), with China (9 times), India (6 times), and Japan (5 times) being the top three countries. Although non-Western research appears to be emerging and growing, more research is still needed in non-Western contexts since conceptualisations of well-being are likely to differ across cultural contexts [8,49,50]. ...
... Table 3 shows that there are 22 codes grouped into the 'hedonic' dimension. The Hedonic dimension was described as enjoyment and feelings of positive emotion along with the satisfaction of life [8,39,87]. Historically, almost all researchers tended to regard subjective wellbeing and affective well-being as hedonic well-being, and these concepts have often been used interchangeably [116,136,137]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to identify the conceptualisation of overall well-being used for well-being assessment through a review of the characteristics and key components and/or dimensions of well-being scales as presented in current literature. Scopus and Web of Science were searched, and thematic analysis was conducted inductively to analyse the identified components within scales, as well as the types of well-being these scales measure. 107 peer-reviewed articles from 2003 to 2022 were included, and 69 well-being scales were identified covering nine areas of well-being. Four final themes were identified as the foundational dimensions of overall well-being: hedonic; eudaimonic; physical health; and generic happiness. Notably, these 69 scales are mainly validated and adopted in the Western context. ‘4 + N’ frameworks of overall well-being are recommended for assessing overall well-being. This review provides researchers with a synthesis of what types of well-being have been measured and which measures have been used to assess these types of well-being for which research participants. Non-Western-based well-being research is called for that incorporates a broader range of research participants and cultural contexts in contributing to a more inclusive understanding of well-being.
... Based on the subjectivity and diversity of well-being concepts (Durayappah 2011), and therefore the difficulty of unifying a theory of well-being (Lambert et al. 2020), authors such as Hernández-Núñez et al. (2021) constructed a well-being indicator based on the balance of capital in cocoa-growing families in Colombia, which had human capital as the main driver of well-being. The main asset of small cocoa producers is themselves and their families, quantifiable through variables such as "labor forcé" or "availability of labor" (Chambers 1989). ...
Article
Full-text available
The concept of well-being of rural families is part of a theory under construction in which new theoretical elements are constantly being incorporated. This research aims to determine the influence of farmers’ knowledge on the well‑being of cocoa growing families in the departments of Santander, Huila, Meta and Caquetá, Colombia. Four categories of farmers were identified with different levels of knowledge in the management of cocoa cultivation obtained through a cluster analysis. The well-being of cocoa farmers, understood as the balance in the capital endowment of rural households, was obtained through the application of a semi-structured interview with 49 variables of human, cultural, social, political, natural, built, and financial capitals. The results show that cocoa knowledge is heterogeneous in the study area, with a slight improvement towards harvesting, post-harvest and transformation links. There is a positive relationship between cocoa knowledge and the well-being of cocoa farming families. Thus, producers with greater integral knowledge, with emphasis on post-harvest and bean transformation links, showed greater well-being. The Random Forest analysis identified that human capital (political, social, human, and cultural) made the greatest contribution to well-being. The findings show that cocoa knowledge contributes to the well-being of rural households to the extent that it favors vertical relationships (linkages with local governments) and horizontal relationships of producers (participation of association managers, sharing knowledge with friends, neighbors and partners, and cocoa training).
... Seligman's PERMA framework outlines core well-being elements-positive emotion, engagement, relationships, meaning, and accomplishment-crucial for a fulfilling life [7]. Well-being also involves a balance between personal resources and life challenges, influenced by cultural and environmental factors [8,9]. Moreover, it encompasses not only psychological states but also objective factors like financial security and social connections, which together enhance life quality [10,11]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Mobility, a vital part of daily life, significantly impacts human well-being. Understanding this relationship is crucial for shaping the future trajectory of mobility, a connection often overlooked in previous research. This study explores the complex relationship between mobility and well-being and proposes a holistic framework for mobility’s future, prioritizing individual and societal well-being. The motivation for this research stems from the growing need to balance technological advancements in transportation with the well-being of diverse populations, especially as the mobility landscape evolves with innovations like autonomous vehicles and intelligent mobility solutions. We employ bibliometric methods, analyzing 53,588 academic articles to identify key themes and research trends related to mobility and well-being. This study categorizes these articles into thematic clusters using the Louvain modularity maximization algorithm, which facilitates the formation of cohesive groups based on citation patterns. Our findings underline the significant impact of mobility on physical, mental, psychological, financial, and social well-being. The proposed framework features four pillars: vehicle, infrastructure and environment, mobility stakeholders, and policy. This framework underscores the importance of collaboration between institutional and individual actions in shaping a future mobility landscape that is technologically advanced, socially responsible, and conducive to an improved quality of life.
... Empirical studies suggest nature connection can have immense benefits for humans, including improving individual wellbeing (Lambert et al., 2020), with mood (MacKerron and Mourato, 2013), cognitive (Berman et al., 2008), health (Frumkin, 2001), and longevity (Mitchell and Popham, 2008) benefits, indicative of nature's importance for overall optimal health (Aberson et al., 2000). ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has found that nature connectedness, an experiential close connection to nature with cognitive, affective and physical benefits, profoundly impacts individual wellbeing and subsequently increases pro-environmental behaviors. However, little is known about the personal and contextual factors that predict nature connectedness. Testing theory derived from a qualitative interpretative phenomenological analysis study, this research addresses the lacuna in the literature. A structural equation model analysis finds that interoceptive awareness significantly predicts nature connection, that secure attachment to nature significantly explains this relationship, and that these inter-related constructs predict both pro-environmental behavior and wellbeing. This revised model of nature connection indicates important antecedents for the human-nature bond, illuminating in particular that the interpersonal relational processes foundational for close bonding with humans also occur in bonding with nature. Structural equation modeling indicates that emotional awareness is the dimension of interoceptive awareness that most significantly predicts nature connection, suggesting that the more aware a person is of the connection between inner bodily sensations and emotions, the more likely they can bond with nature. Given that interoceptive awareness indicates a coherent relationship with the self, including effective communication between body, mind and feelings, this process is therefore implicated in the capacity for humans to bond with nature. In sum, this present research points to the efficacy of an embodied, secure attachment with nature to help close both the human-nature disconnection chasm, and the environmental value-action gap. Theoretical and methodological implications for research and policy are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
This survey explores the landscape of well-being research in an urban context with focus on data. First, we conduct a review of well-being in general and efforts to measure it. Then, we perform an analysis of relevant research papers, reports, surveys, and standards published between January 2018 and September 2023 for the identification of reported factors contributing to urban well-being. These indicators were placed into distinct categories, creating a taxonomy of current efforts towards capturing urban well-being. This taxonomy, presented in a table format, details each indicator alongside its measurement method, data source, and other relevant information. Therefore, we are able to gain a deeper understanding of how urban well-being is assessed, what are the challenges, current research focus and gaps. This taxonomy could serve as a valuable tool for practitioners, researchers and policymakers in the field, providing a comprehensive framework to understand and assess various aspects of capturing well-being in urban context.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of the study is twofold: on the one hand, it assesses the association between well-being and the quality of government, on the other it evaluates the impact exerted on the same well-being by the different components into which the quality of government can be broken down. The analysis is conducted at the regional level. A composite indicator derived from OECD data on regional well-being is used to assess its association with the quality of government. The latter is measured using the European Quality of Government Survey Index (EQI) data. The EQI breaks down the quality of government into three key elements: the impartiality of administration, the quality of services provided and the degree of corruption. The association between well-being and quality of government is verified considering, at first, the EQI index as a whole, being the product of the three different components, and then separately considering its key elements. The results confirmed the existence of a strong positive association between well-being and the overall quality of government, as between the former and the single components of the EQI index. Furthermore, the same association is observed when the assessment is made on the whole sample, but also when subsamples are made, considering the geographical macro-area to which the regions belong.
Article
The interest of researchers in the areas of human sciences and health in subjective well-being (SWB) is growing. However, in the Brazilian context, there is still a shortage of scientific evidence that ensures the use of a measurement instrument for assessing this construct. The present study aimed to investigate the psychometric properties of the Brazilian version of the measurement of SWB composed of the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) and the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS), by testing the different factorial models described in the literature. The sample was composed of 1896 participants (77.8% female). Through confirmatory factor analysis, the fit indices of the single-factor, three-factor correlated (positive affections, negative affections, and life satisfaction), and bifactor models were compared. The results demonstrate better fits for the correlated multifactor model and invariance of the measurement model between groups according to gender and geographic region of the country. It was concluded that the Brazilian versions of the SWLS and PANAS compose an adequate measure of SWB, corresponding to the tripartite theoretical proposal that gave rise to the construct.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Health, safety, and well-being are concepts of significant interest to construction researchers and practitioners. Although the interconnected nature of the three is acknowledged, there is confusion over their definitions, management practices and perceptions. Based on a critical literature review, this paper attempts to clarify the constructs of health, well-being and safety in construction and highlight their interrelationships by analysing the different management approaches and perceptions. The review reveals that the management of safety hazards in construction often overshadows health and well-being issues, increasing the growing gap between management's focus on demonstrating safety measurements and employees' focus on staying safe on site. The implications of the findings emphasize the need for an integrated approach that considers the interplay between health, safety, and well-being, enhancing workplaces conditions by addressing work design. This review contributes to the ongoing discussion on the importance of health, safety, and well-being in the construction industry.
Article
Full-text available
Based on 7,939 business units in 36 companies, this study used meta-analysis to examine the relationship at the business-unit level between employee satisfaction–engagement and the business-unit outcomes of customer satisfaction, productivity, profit, employee turnover, and accidents. Generalizable relationships large enough to have substantial practical value were found between unit-level employee satisfaction–engagement and these business-unit outcomes. One implication is that changes in management practices that increase employee satisfaction may increase business-unit outcomes, including profit.
Article
Full-text available
The relevance of balance and harmony to wellbeing has been under-appreciated in psychology. Even though these concepts have received considerable attention across different contexts (e.g., work-life balance), this literature is fragmented and scattered. There have been few attempts to bring these disparate threads together, or to centre these concepts as foundational and important across all aspects of human functioning. This paper remedies this lacuna by offering a narrative review of these diverse works. Relevant literature is organised into four emergent categories: Affect, cognition, behaviour, and self-other relations. Throughout these, balance and harmony can be appreciated as not merely relevant to wellbeing, but arguably a defining principle, a ‘golden thread’ running through its myriad dimensions (though this thread is itself multifaceted, comprising a cluster of interlinked concepts). Based on this analysis, an overarching definition of wellbeing is offered: The dynamic attainment of optimal balance and harmony in any-and ideally all-aspects of life. This paper provides a foundation and stimulus for further work on these important topics.
Preprint
Full-text available
1 The construct of wellbeing has been criticised as a neoliberal construction of western individualism that ignores 2 wider systemic issues including increasing burden of chronic disease, widening inequality, concerns over environ-3 mental degradation and anthropogenic climate change. While these criticisms overlook recent developments, there 4 remains a need for biopsychosocial models that extend theoretical grounding beyond individual wellbeing, incor-5 porating overlapping contextual issues relating to community and environment. Our first GENIAL model (Kemp, 6 Arias, & Fisher, 2017) provided a more expansive view of pathways to longevity in the context of individual health 7 and wellbeing, emphasising bidirectional links to positive social ties and the impact of sociocultural factors. In 8 this paper, we build on these ideas and propose GENIAL 2.0, focusing on intersecting individual-community-9 environmental contributions to health and wellbeing, and laying an evidence-based, theoretical framework on 10 which future research and innovative therapeutic innovations could be based. We suggest that our transdisci-11 plinary model of wellbeing-focusing on individual, community and environmental contributions to personal 12 wellbeing-will help to move the research field forward. In reconceptualising wellbeing, GENIAL 2.0 bridges the 13 gap between psychological science and population health health systems, and presents opportunities for enhancing 14 the health and wellbeing of people living with chronic conditions. Implications for future generations including 15 the very survival of our species are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Social scientists have been interested in measuring the prosperity, well-being, and quality of life of nations, which has resulted in a multiplicity of country-level indicators. However, little is known about the factor structure of these indicators. We explored the structure of quality of life, using country-level data on tens of subjective and objective indicators. Applying factor analysis, we identified three distinct factors that exhibited both overlap and complementarity. This structure was replicated in data from previous years and with a partially different set of variables. The first factor, ‘socio-economic progress’, is dominated by socio-political and economic indicators but also includes life satisfaction, which thus appears to reflect objective living conditions. The second factor, ‘psycho-social functioning’, consists of subjective indicators, such as eudaimonic well-being and positive affective states. The third, ‘negative affectivity’, comprises negatively-valenced affective states. The three macro-factors of societal quality of life demonstrated moderate intercorrelations and differential associations with cultural and ecological variables, providing support for their discriminant validity. Finally, country and regional rankings based on the three societal factors revealed a complex picture that cautions against over-reliance on any single indicator such as life satisfaction. The results underline the need for a broadly-based approach to the measurement of societal quality of life, and provide an empirically-derived multidimensional framework for conceptualizing and measuring quality of life and well-being at country level. This study is thus an initial empirical step towards systematizing the multiple approaches to societal quality of life.
Article
Full-text available
Understanding what drives environmentally protective or destructive behavior is important to the design and implementation of effective public policies to encourage people's engagement in proenvironmental behavior (PEB). Research shows that a connection to nature is associated with greater engagement in PEB. However, the variety of instruments and methods used in these studies poses a major barrier to integrating research findings. We conducted a meta‐analysis of the relationship between connection to nature and PEB. We identified studies through a systematic review of the literature and used Comprehensive Meta‐Analysis software to analyze the results from 37 samples (n = 13,237) and to test for moderators. A random‐effects model demonstrated a positive and significant association between connection to nature and PEB (r = 0.42, 95% CI 0.36, 0.47, p < 0.001). People who are more connected to nature reported greater engagement in PEB. Standard tests indicated little effect of publication bias in the sample. There was significant heterogeneity among the samples. Univariate categorical analyses showed that the scales used to measure connection to nature and PEB were significant moderators and explained the majority of the between‐study variance. The geographic location of a study, age of participants, and the percentage of females in a study were not significant moderators. We found that a deeper connection to nature may partially explain why some people behave more proenvironmentally than others and that the relationship is ubiquitous. Facilitating a stronger connection to nature may result in greater engagement in PEB and conservation, although more longitudinal studies with randomized experiments are required to demonstrate causation.
Article
Full-text available
Nature connectedness relates to an individual’s subjective sense of their relationship with the natural world. A recent meta-analysis has found that people who are more connected to nature also tend to have higher levels of self-reported hedonic well-being; however, no reviews have focussed on nature connection and eudaimonic well-being. This meta-analysis was undertaken to explore the relationship of nature connection with eudaimonic well-being and to test the hypothesis that this relationship is stronger than that of nature connection and hedonic well-being. From 20 samples (n = 4758), a small significant effect size was found for the relationship of nature connection and eudaimonic well-being (r = 0.24); there was no significant difference between this and the effect size (from 30 samples n = 11,638) for hedonic well-being (r = 0.20). Of the eudaimonic well-being subscales, personal growth had a moderate effect size which was significantly larger than the effect sizes for autonomy, purpose in life/meaning, self-acceptance, positive relations with others and environmental mastery, but not vitality. Thus, individuals who are more connected to nature tend to have greater eudaimonic well-being, and in particular have higher levels of self-reported personal growth.
Article
Full-text available
Two primary goals of psychological science should be to under- stand what aspects of human psychology are universal and the way that context and culture produce variability. This requires that we take into account the importance of culture and context in the way that we write our papers and in the types of populations that we sample. However, most research published in our leading journals has relied on sampling WEIRD (Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic) populations. One might expect that our scholarly work and editorial choices would by now reflect the knowledge that Western populations may not be representative of humans generally with respect to any given psychological phenomenon. However, as we show here, almost all research published by one of our leading journals, Psychological Science, relies on Western samples and uses these data in an unreflective way to make inferences about humans in general. To take us for- ward, we offer a set of concrete proposals for authors, journal editors, and reviewers that may lead to a psychological science that is more representative of the human condition.
Article
Using meta-analysis, we examined whether there is evidence consistent with the idea that a subjective sense of “connection to nature” promotes pro-environmental behaviour (PEB; Mayer & Frantz, 2004; Nisbet, Zelenski, & Murphy, 2009). Analysis of correlational data (k = 75) provided compelling evidence for a strong and robust association between nature connection and PEB (r = 0.37). Nature connection was positively associated with PEB across different operationalizations of nature connection, across different PEB measures (i.e., behavioural intentions, self-reports of behaviour, and observed behaviour), and across various sample and demographic characteristics. We found no evidence of publication bias in correlational studies. For experimental manipulations of nature connection (k = 17), however, there was clear evidence of publication bias in favour of studies reporting positive effects. We addressed this bias by including unpublished studies in the meta-analysis, and found a small, significant causal effect of nature connection on PEB (d = 0.21, r = 0.10). We conclude that more experimental data with better quality manipulations of nature connection are needed. Nonetheless, the strong and robust association between nature connection and PEB, as well as evidence that nature connection causes PEB, suggest that nature connection is a promising avenue for promoting PEB.
Article
Is the need for nature relatedness a basic psychological need? Using Baumeister and Leary’s need criteria as comparators, we explored literature arcing across disciplines to find evidence that either supported or undermined our proposition that the human need for nature may be a fundamental psychological need. We found that while nature’s salubrious effects benefited humans across the lifespan, and largely transcended cultural boundaries, socioeconomic status, age, and state of health, the benefits of being in nature depended on choice, perception of safety, and absence of fear. Race, ethnicity, and gender tended to impede leisure in natural environments. In concluding that the need for nature relatedness is a basic psychological need, we discuss practical implications for leisure and recreation professionals, researchers, educators, architects, and city planners and proffer several practical interventions in several domains to benefit both the environment and humanity.