ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Handwriting has a low status and profile in literacy education and in recent years has attracted little attention from teachers, policy makers or researchers into mainstream educational processes. This article identifies a substantial programme of research into handwriting, including studies located in the domains of special needs education and psychology, which suggest that it is time to re-evaluate the importance of handwriting in the teaching of literacy. Explorations of the way handwriting affects composing has opened up new avenues for research, screening and intervention, which have the potential to make a significant contribution to children’s writing progress. In particular, the role of orthographic motor integration and automaticity in handwriting is now seen as of key importance in composing. Evidence from existing studies suggests that handwriting intervention programmes may have a real impact on the composing skills of young writers. In particular, they could positively affect the progress of the many boys who struggle with writing throughout the primary school years.
ISSN: 2320-5407 Int. J. Adv. Res. 7(12), 947-954
947
Journal Homepage: - www.journalijar.com
Article DOI: 10.21474/IJAR01/10237
DOI URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.21474/IJAR01/10237
RESEARCH ARTICLE
THE IMPORTANCE OF HANDWRITING IN EDUCATION
Jakhongir Shaturaev
Termez branch of Tashkent State Technical University.
……………………………………………………………………………………………………....
Manuscript Info Abstract
……………………. ………………………………………………………………
Manuscript History
Received: 12 October 2019
Final Accepted: 14 November 2019
Published: December 2019
Key words:-
Boys, Handwriting, Literacy, Writing,
Education
Handwriting has a low status and profile in literacy education and in
recent years has attracted little attention from teachers, policy makers
or researchers into mainstream educational processes. This article
identifies a substantial programme of research into handwriting,
including studies located in the domains of special needs education and
psychology, which suggest that it is time to re-evaluate the importance
of handwriting in the teaching of literacy. Explorations of the way
handwriting affects composing has opened up new avenues for
research, screening and intervention, which have the potential to make
a significant contribution to children’s writing progress. In particular,
the role of orthographic motor integration and automaticity in
handwriting is now seen as of key importance in composing. Evidence
from existing studies suggests that handwriting intervention
programmes may have a real impact on the composing skills of young
writers. In particular, they could positively affect the progress of the
many boys who struggle with writing throughout the primary school
years.
Copy Right, IJAR, 2019,. All rights reserved.
……………………………………………………………………………………………………....
Introduction:-
There has been little significant educational research into handwriting in England since the work of Sassoon et al
(1986) and Alston and Taylor (1987). Even the available research reviews (Graham & Weintraub, 1996) were
written over a decade ago and include little evidence from a British context. The way handwriting is taught in
English mainstream schooling is based on research and writing undertaken during the mid-1980s and early 1990s.
During the eighties and early nineties, a number of changes affected the teaching of handwriting. Firstly, a very
significant experiment took place in schools in England, involving a fundamental change in the handwriting script
taught to children in the primary years. Peters’ research into spelling (1985) emphasised that English spelling
provided a high degree of visual regularity and highlighted the link between visual and kinaesthetic learning of
spellings. A strong theoretical case was thus made for a link between correct spelling and the use of fluent, joined up
handwriting. By learning the movements of common spelling patterns by hand (kinaesthetically) as well as by eye, it
was suggested (Cripps & Cox, 1989; Peters & Smith, 1993) that writers improved their chances of producing correct
spellings. The popularisation of this theory in schools through spelling and handwriting schemes coincided with (or
caused) a change in the handwriting of children all over the country as handwriting schemes based on this theory
advocated the use of an alphabet including exit strokes right from the beginning of writing teaching, and the joining
of letters as early as possible (Cripps, 1988). Interestingly, there has been almost no empirical research to examine
Corresponding Author:- Jakhongir Shaturaev
Address:- Termez branch of Tashkent State Technical University.
ISSN: 2320-5407 Int. J. Adv. Res. 7(12), 947-954
948
the claims about the contribution of handwriting to correct spelling, to measure the effects of beginning writing
using different scripts or to examine the effects of early joining.
The importance of handwriting: writing assessment:
Whilst handwriting style streamlined across schools in England during the late 1980s and early 1990s, it was also
put firmly in its place in terms of its importance relative to other aspects of writing. The National Curriculum for
England (DfEE/QCA, 2000), for example, treats handwriting succinctly and deals with the development of
movement and style, with no mention of speed or efficiency. The attainment target for writing at level 4 (the target
for 11 year olds) demands only that: “Handwriting style is fluent, joined and legible”. No mention is made of speed.
Handwriting is statutorily assessed as part of the Standard Assessment Tasks and Tests (SATs), the marking
schemes for which allocate up to 40 marks for writing at age 7 (Key Stage 1) and 50 marks at age 11 (Key Stage 2).
At both ages, up to 3 marks can be awarded for handwriting. The assessment for these three marks is made on a
sample of handwriting done during a composition assessment and is a product analysis. Fluency is taken to mean
evidence of the effective joining of letters and speed of writing is not included in the assessment. In short, this is a
very imprecise assessment of handwriting style, not of handwriting efficiency.
The National Literacy Strategy also gives minimal attention to handwriting. It was included in the word level
objectives in the NLS Framework for Teaching (DfEE, 1998) from reception year (age 4-5) until Year 4 (aged 9),
after which handwriting does not appear as an objective. The assumption that handwriting will have been mastered
by this time is common across publications about writing (e.g. Wyse, 1998; Medwell et al, 2001; Nicholls et al,
1989). In the light of research in the areas of neuroscience, cognitive psychology and special needs education, it is
time to question this assumption and examine how research into handwriting can offer clues to improving
composition.
The importance of handwriting: teaching writing:
One reason for such a lack of attention to handwriting has been the perspectives on writing that have been popular in
schools and the emphasis (or lack of emphasis) these perspectives have placed upon handwriting. In early years’
education, evidence that children can write meaningful texts before they have mastered the writing system (Teale &
Sulzby, 1986) changed the way researchers and teachers looked at children’s early attempts at writing (Temple et al,
1982). Analysis of children’s early writing for evidence of understandings about the language system (Clay, 1975,),
spelling (Gentry, 1981) and audience (Hall, 1987; Czerniewska, 1992) shifted attention away from the teaching of
writing through copying, with its emphasis on correct letter formation and legibility.
Emergent writing (Hall, 1987; Teale & Sulzby, 1986), placed the focus of attention firmly on the meanings children
were able to create in their writing. Children were encouraged to write freely and to use their emerging, but
incomplete, understandings of language and writing skills to express themselves in writing. This was a corrective to
earlier emphasis on neatness and correct letter formation, which undoubtedly hindered the composition of beginning
writers.
The teaching of writing to older children has been strongly influenced by theoretical perspectives that emphasise the
difference between composing text and transcribing text. Graves’ (1983) account of the writing process as a series of
stages has been highly significant for theorised pedagogies of writing, even if these theories have not quite had the
practical effects that have been claimed for them (Medwell, 1998). Cognitive models of the writing process, such as
that of Hayes and Flowers (1980), also stress the planning and self-monitoring required by the writer, but these too
have had limited influence on mainstream school practice. More recently, a genre focused approach to writing,
emphasising the direct teaching of the structures of socially significant texts, was popularized by the work of Wray
and Lewis (1997) and included in the requirements of the National Literacy Strategy (1998). In none of these
perspectives on writing and its teaching does handwriting play a significant role. Indeed, current perspectives often
explicitly assign handwriting to a peripheral role in writing success.
A composition-led view of the writing process is very much part of the mainstream culture of literacy teaching in
England. The National Curriculum for English (DfEE, 2000) requires that children be taught to plan, draft, revise,
proof-read and present their work, a direct reflection of the process approach, and this is sustained in the National
Literacy Strategy (DfEE, 1998). Emphasis upon composing may, at times, have drawn attention away from
handwriting.
ISSN: 2320-5407 Int. J. Adv. Res. 7(12), 947-954
949
The importance of handwriting: the research evidence:
Despite its empirical rigour and replication, and its central concern with how children learn to write, the substantial
body of cognitive psychological research on the writing process has had little impact on classroom practice. This
may be because the largely experimental and non-naturalist design of such research makes its direct classroom
application problematic. However, in psychology, neuropsychology and special needs education the substantial
research into handwriting that has taken place in the last decade may offer insights into the composing processes of
mainstream children. It may also ensure that the role of handwriting in composition is reconsidered and even the
nature of handwriting itself reconceptualised.
A considerable amount of this research has focused on explorations of the role of working memory in writing.
Working memory denotes the temporary storage of the information necessary for carrying out tasks. Long-term
memory can store virtually unlimited amounts of material for many years, but working memory can hold only a few
items for a short time - it is a limited resource. Kellogg (1996; 1999; 2001) and Hayes (1996) have both given a
central role to working memory in their very influential models of the writing process. Understanding the ways in
which different writing processes draw on the same limited working memory resources could explain why some
writing processes are more difficult than others and how these processes may interfere with each other.
Identifying the role of working memory in writing may help us to understand the interference among memory
processes that contend for the same scarce memory resources, in this case, the way handwriting may actually affect
composition. The findings of Gathercole et al (2004) suggest that working memory is particularly associated with
the literacy scores of younger children. If young writers have to devote large amounts of working memory to the
control of lower-level processes such as handwriting, they may have little working memory capacity left for higher-
level processes such as idea generation, vocabulary selection, monitoring the progress of mental plans and revising
text against these plans.
Individuals can generally conduct only one cognitive task requiring attention at a time (Sweller, 1988; Sweller &
Chandler, 1994). This means that the way an individual manages cognitive resources to facilitate all the different,
attention-requiring aspects of a writing task is crucial to their success at writing (Saada-Robert, 1999). Christiansen
(2002) identifies two main strategies to limit the demands on working memory. The first is to sequence tasks so that
only one task is undertaken at a time. This has been a popular way to manage writing processes in classrooms and
planning, drafting, revising etc. have been sequenced as steps in the writing process for many children, in an attempt
to reduce their competing demands on young writers. However, models of writing (e.g. Hayes & Flower, 1980)
suggest that writing processes are recursive and that writing is not a step-by-step linear process at all. In this case,
sequencing tasks so that only one is undertaken at a time is unlikely to be a successful strategy for limiting demand
on working memory at a cognitive level, since writing simply does not proceed that way. Moreover, in writing it is
hardly possible to isolate or defer the handwriting element, since without it, nothing would actually be written!
An alternative solution to the problem of limited working memory capacity is to make some processes, such as
handwriting, automatic, in order to free up cognitive resources to deal with higher level processes. La Berge &
Samuels, (1974) define automaticity as having been achieved when a process can be effected swiftly, accurately and
without the need for conscious attention. The development of skill in writing may require the automatisation of
lower-level skills so that they use less of the available working-memory resources.
A major programme of research undertaken over the last ten to fifteen years (e.g. Berninger et al, 2006; Berninger,
1994; Berninger & Graham, 1998) has investigated the role of handwriting in writing and its findings are extremely
interesting. Firstly, it has been established that handwriting is far from a purely motor act. Berninger and Graham
(1998) stress that it is “language by hand” and point out that their research suggests that orthographic and memory
processes (the ability to recall letter shapes) contribute more to handwriting than do motor skills (Berninger &
Amtmann, 2004).
Orthographic-motor integration of handwriting - that is the ability to call to mind and write letter shapes, groups of
letters and words efficiently and effectively without allocation of cognitive attention, appears to be a very significant
part of writing that has been largely overlooked in education. It involves mentally coding and rehearsing visual
representations of these patterns and integrating them with motor patterns (Berninger, 1994). There is a growing
body of research which suggests that handwriting is critical to the generation of creative and well-structured written
text and has an impact not only on fluency but also on the quality of composing (Berninger & Swanson, 1994;
ISSN: 2320-5407 Int. J. Adv. Res. 7(12), 947-954
950
Graham et al, 1997). Lack of automaticity in orthographic-motor integration can seriously hamper the ability of
young children to express ideas in text. (Berninger & Swanson, 1994; De La Paz & Graham, 1995; Graham, 1990).
Studies in this area have experimented with the removal of some of the competing demands for children’s cognitive
attention during writing. De La Paz and Graham (1995), for example, found that when the children were able to
dictate their texts to an adult, thus freeing them from the task of handwriting, the quality of their composition
significantly improved. Other studies have confirmed this effect in primary aged children (e.g., Hidi & Hidyard,
1983; McCutchen, 1996, 1998; Scardamalia et al, 1982).
Research suggests that orthographic-motor integration accounts for more than 50% of the variance in written
language performance in children. Christensen and Jones (2000) put this as high as 67% for the 7-8 year old children
they studied. Some studies have suggested that the influence of orthographic-motor integration declines with age
(Berninger & Swanson, 1994), but there are suggestions that it continues to exert an influence on writing in
secondary school pupils (e.g. Christensen & Jones, 2000) and even in adults (Bourdin & Fayol, 2002).
If handwriting can have such an impact on writers’ abilities to generate sophisticated text, it appears critical that
children develop smooth and efficient handwriting. This raises two important questions. Firstly, for how many, and
for which, children might inefficient handwriting be affecting their composition? Secondly, what evidence is there
that teaching can make a difference to children’s performance in handwriting and in composition?
The importance of knowing who may have problems:
Ascertaining the numbers of children for whom lack of automaticity is a problem is difficult in Britain. Statutory
assessments do not assess handwriting speed and there is no national screening for handwriting problems. Graham
and Weintraub (1996) estimate that between 12 and 20% of school aged children experience handwriting
difficulties, and other estimates have been as high as 44% (Alston, 1985; Rubin & Henderson, 1982), although these
figures are based on teacher estimates and must be viewed with caution. Barnett et al (2006) suggest a figure as low
as 5% for schools in south east England, but this is based on teacher report in a very small survey and again must be
treated with caution. But if these figures are even approximately correct, it suggests that lack of handwriting
automaticity may affect a significant number of primary and secondary aged children. Such an unrecognised lack of
automaticity may interfere with the composing processes of these children. There is no evidence of concern about,
screening of or intervention in this aspect of writing in the British system.
Although we do not have enough evidence to estimate what proportion of children may be experiencing handwriting
difficulties in Britain, the research does suggest a strong gender effect. Boys are more likely to be identified as
having handwriting problems than girls (Hamstra-Bletz & Blote, 1993; Rubin & Henderson, 1982) and research in
the 1980s and 90s confirmed that girls are generally better handwriters than boys (Graham & Miller, 1980) both on
measures of overall quality and of letter formation (Hamstra-Bletz & Blote, 1990; Ziviani & Elkins, 1984). Girls
also tend to write faster than boys (Berninger & Fuller, 1992; Biemiller et al, 1993; Ziviani, 1984). This is an
important detail if handwriting does have an impact on children’s ability to compose. If boys are less likely to obtain
the necessary automaticity in handwriting at the expected age, it may be that this interferes with their ability to
compose.
At present, there is considerable concern in Britain about boys’ underachievement in writing (UKLA/PNS, 2004).
In the annual Standard Assessment Tests and Tasks, boys consistently do worse than girls at writing (Bearne &
Warrington, 2003) but the data that is collected cannot reveal how handwriting is implicated in this. The issue of
boys’ handwriting has not been a focus of the projects aimed at addressing underperformance in writing by boys. A
recent project in this area (UKLA/PNS, 2004) did not mention handwriting at all, despite the fact that the aspects of
writing most often cited by the boys in the study as a reason for disliking writing were technical including
handwriting and spelling.
For children who are slow to develop handwriting automaticity (as opposed to neatness), handwriting is slower and
demands more effort. This creates what Stanovich (1986) has called, in reading, the “Matthew effect” whereby those
who are more able, (given the above evidence in handwriting - usually girls) achieve more successful practice and,
in the case of orthographic-motor integration, have more attention available for composing processes. In turn, the
less able handwriters have less opportunity to engage with higher order composing processes and to make progress
in writing.
ISSN: 2320-5407 Int. J. Adv. Res. 7(12), 947-954
951
The importance of interventions:
If a lack of orthographic-motor integration can have such serious consequences for the development of composing
skills, it is important to know whether intervention can prevent these difficulties. There have been some studies of
orthographic-motor integration to try to ascertain the effects of focused handwriting practice. Two studies
undertaken in Australia (Jones & Christensen 1999; Christensen, 2005) used a relatively simple alphabet writing
task designed by Berninger et al (1991) to measure orthographic-motor integration and to identify children with
automaticity problems. One study measured the orthographic-motor integration, reading and written expression of
114 children in Year 2 (aged 7) before and after an eight week long handwriting programme. The children
undertaking the programme showed significant improvement in their handwriting and, crucially, in their composing
skills. More than half the variance in scores on written expression was accounted for by orthographic-motor
integration, even when reading scores were controlled. Christensen also reports a study of 50 older children (year 8
and 9 in secondary school) whose orthographic-motor integration and written expression were measured before and
after an intensive handwriting programme. A matched control group did journal writing for a similar period.
Although both the journal and handwriting groups were equivalent at pre-test, the scores for the handwriting group
after eight weeks of intervention were significantly better on all post-test measures, for example, 70% higher in
orthographic-motor integration and 46% higher in quality of written text than the journal group. The handwriting
group also wrote approximately twice as much text as the journal writers. These are startling findings at a secondary
level, where it might be expected that children who have not achieved automaticity would already have experienced
demoralizing failure. These studies offer strong evidence that handwriting intervention can make a difference to the
handwriting and, more importantly, the composition of children with poor automaticity. By improving their ability
to produce letters automatically, these young writers freed up their attention for other writing processes.
Conclusion:-
The research suggests that the role of handwriting in writing has been underestimated in mainstream education. The
concentration has been on the benefits to spelling of well formed, joined handwriting, and the necessity for speed
and automaticity has been neglected in our handwriting pedagogy. Educators have prioritised composing processes
in writing, in itself not necessarily a bad thing. But in doing so we may have neglected a skill which makes a strong
contribution to the composing we so value. The research suggests that it is time to re-consider.
Handwriting, and in particular the automaticity of letter production, appears to facilitate higher order composing
processes by freeing up working memory to deal with the complex tasks of planning, organizing, revising and
regulating the production of text. Research suggests that automatic letter writing is the single best predictor of length
and quality of written composition in the primary years (Graham et al, 1997) in secondary school and even in the
post-compulsory education years (Connelly et al, 2006; Jones, 2004; Peverley, 2006).
Enshrined in our pedagogic theory, practice and policy is the assumption that handwriting becomes automatic
relatively early on in writers’ development. This assumption unfortunately remains untested, as national testing does
not assess handwriting speed or fluency and addresses only writing style and neatness. We may be assessing the
wrong aspects of handwriting and failing to assess an aspect which is crucial.
We know that a significant number of children experience handwriting difficulties throughout their schooling,
although for most these are probably not judged as sufficiently serious to justify remedial action. More of these
children are boys than girls and their handwriting difficulties are likely to impact upon their ability to compose
written language. There is evidence that intervention to teach handwriting can improve not only the handwriting of
these children, but also their written composition.
There are a number of ways forward. We need to examine in more detail whether the findings about orthographic-
motor intervention can be generalised to the British context, where the extent of handwriting difficulty is unknown
and children are taught a simpler, more efficient script than those generally taught in America. One small study
(Connelly and Hurst, 2001) has suggested such generalisation is likely but a much larger sample and range of age
groups is necessary. We need to assess the extent and distribution of handwriting difficulties by looking at levels of
automaticity in primary and secondary school pupils. Establishing some bench-marks for orthographic motor-
integration through the school years would be the first step towards looking for a simple screening instrument that
could identify those children with handwriting difficulties who might benefit from interventions to improve their
automatic production of letters. For such children, a short handwriting programme may be what they need to
ISSN: 2320-5407 Int. J. Adv. Res. 7(12), 947-954
952
improve their composing. A research programme to consider what intervention might be most effective could then
be undertaken. Such a programme has the potential to benefit young writers, particularly boys, who struggle to
compose throughout their primary and secondary schooling.
Handwriting has not been an important aspect of literacy for teachers in the last decade, but it has been the subject of
important research. It is time for the research in this area to be made more accessible to educators and for it to be
considered in the planning of pedagogies for struggling writers.
References:-
1. Alston, J. and Taylor, J. (1987). Handwriting theory, research, and practice. New York: Nichols.
2. Alston, J. (1985) The handwriting of seven to nine year olds. British Journal of Special Educational Needs, 12,
68-72
3. Barnett, A., Stainthorp, R., Henderson, S. and Scheib, B. (2006) Handwriting policy and practice in English
primary schools. Institute of Education, London
4. Bearne, E. and Warrington, M. (2003). Boys and Writing. Literacy Today, 35,
http://www.literacytrust.org.uk/Pubs/bearne.html [12th May, 2006]
5. Berninger, V. W, and Amtmann, D. (2004). Preventing written expression disabilities through early and
continuing assessment and intervention for handwriting and/or spelling problems: research into practice. In L.
Swanson, K. Harris, and S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of research on learning disabilities (pp. 345363). New
York, Guilford Press.
6. Berninger, V., and Fuller, E. (1992). Gender differences in orthographic, verbal, and compositional fluency:
Implications for diagnosis of writing disabilities in primary grade children. Journal of School Psychology, 30,
363-382.
7. Berninger, V. W., and Graham, S. (1998). Language by hand: A synthesis of a decade of research on
handwriting. Handwriting Review, 12, 1125.
8. Berninger, V. W., and Swanson, H. L. (1994). Modifying Hayes and Flower’s model of skilled writing to
explain beginning and developing writing. In E. C. Butterfield (Ed.), Children’s writing: Toward a process
theory of the development of skilled writing (pp. 5781). Hampton Hill, UK: JAI Press.
9. Berninger, V. W., Mizokawa, D. T., and Bragg, R. (1991). Theory-based diagnosis and remediation of writing
disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 29, 5759.
10. Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R.D, Jones, J., Wolf, B., Gould, L., Anderson-Youngstrom, M., Shimada, S. and
Apel, K. (2006) Early development of language by hand: Composing, reading, listening and speaking
connections; Three letter writing modes and fast mapping in spelling. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29 (1),
61-92
11. Berninger, V. W. (1994). Reading and writing acquisition: A developmental neuropsychological perspective.
Dubuque, IA: Brown and Benchmark.
12. Biemiller, A., Regan, E., and Gang, B. (1993). Studies in the development of writing speed: Age, task, and
individual differences. Unpublished manuscript, University of Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
13. Bourdin, B.; Fayol, M. (2002) Even in adults, written production is still more costly than oral production.
International Journal of Psychology 37 (4), 219-227
14. Christensen. C. A., and Jones, D. (2000). Handwriting: An underestimated skill in the development of written
language. Handwriting Today, 2, 5669.
15. Christensen, C. A. (2005) The role of orthographic-motor integration in the production of creative and well
structured written text for students in secondary school, Educational Psychology. 25 (5), 441-453
16. Clay, M. (1975) What did I write? Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann
17. Connelly, V., and Hurst, G. (2001) The influence on handwriting fluency on writing quality in later primary and
early secondary education. Handwriting Today 2, 557.
18. Connelly, V., Campbell, S., MacLean, M., and Barnes, J. (2006). Contribution of lower order skills to the
written composition of college students with and without dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 175
196.
19. Cripps, C. and Cox, R. (1989) Joining the ABC: How and why handwriting and spelling should be taught
together. Cambridge: LDA Publications
20. Cripps, C. (1988) A hand for spelling. Cambridge: LDA Publications
21. Czerniewska, P. (1992) Learning about writing Oxford: Blackwell
ISSN: 2320-5407 Int. J. Adv. Res. 7(12), 947-954
953
22. De La Paz, S., and Graham, S. (1995). Dictation: Applications to writing for students with learning disabilities.
In T. Scruggs and M. Mastropieri (Eds.), Advances in learning and behavioral disorders (Vol. 9, pp. 227247).
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
23. Department for Education and Employment (1998) The National Literacy Strategy: Framework for teaching.
London: Department for Education and Employment.
24. Department for Education and Employment/Qualifications and Curriculum Authority (2000) The National
Curriculum Handbook for Primary Teachers in England: Key Stages 1 and 2. London: HMSO
25. Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Knight, C. and Stegmann, Z. (2004) Working memory skills and educational
attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 18, 1-16.
26. Gentry, J. R. (1981) Learning to spell developmentally. The Reading Teacher 34 (4), 378-81
27. Graham, S., and Miller, L. (1980). Handwriting research and practice: A unified approach. Focus on
Exceptional Children 13, 1-16.
28. Graham, S., and Weintraub, N. (1996). A review of handwriting research: Progress and prospects from 1980 to
1994. Educational Psychology Review, 8, 7-87.
29. Graham, S. (1990). The role of production factors in learning disabled students’ compositions. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 82, 781791.
30. Graham, S., Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Abbott, S., and Whitaker, D. (1997). The role of mechanics in
composing of elementary school students: A new methodological approach. Journal of Educational Psychology
89 (1), 170182.
31. Graves, D. (1983). Writing: Teachers and children at work. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann
32. Hall, N. (1987) The emergence of literacy. Sevenoaks: Hodder and Stoughton.
33. Hamstra-Bletz, L. and Blote, A., (1993). A longitudinal study on dysgraphic handwriting in primary school.
Journal of Learning Disability 26, 689699.
34. Hayes, J., and Flowers, L. (1980). Identifying the organization of writing processes. In L. Gregg and E.
Steinberg (Eds.) Cognitive processes in writing (pp. 330). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
35. Hayes, J. R. (1996). A new model of cognition and affect in writing. In C. Levy and S. Ransdell (Eds.) The
science of writing (pp. 1-30). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
36. Hidi, S. and Hilyard, A. (1984) The comparison of oral and written productions in two discourse modes,’ in
Discourse Processes, 6, 2, 91-105.
37. Jones, D., and Christensen, C. (1999). The relationship between automaticity in handwriting and students’
ability to generate written text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 44-49.
38. Jones, D. (2004) Automaticity of the transcription process in the production of written text. Unpublished Doctor
of Philosophy Thesis, University of Queensland, Australia.
39. Kellogg, R. T. (1996). A model of working memory in writing. In C. Levy and S. Ransdell (Eds.). The science
of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences, and applications (pp. 57-71). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
40. Kellogg, R. T. (1999). Components of working memory in text production. In Torrance, M. and Jeffery, G. C.
(Eds.) The cognitive demands of writing: Processing capacity and working memory in text production (pp. 42-
61). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Amsterdam University Press.
41. Kellogg, R. T. (2001) Competition for working memory among writing processes, American Journal of
Psychology, 114, pp. 175-191.
42. La Berge, D., and Samuels, S. J. (1974) Toward a theory of automatic information processing. Cognitive
Psychology, 6, 283323.
43. McCutchen, D. (1988) "Functional automaticity" in children's writing: A problem in metacognitive control.
Written Communication, 5, 306-324.
44. McCutchen, D. (1996) A capacity theory of writing: Working memory in composition. Educational Psychology
Review, 8, 299325.
45. Medwell, J., Moore, G., Wray, D. and Griffiths, V. (2001) Primary English: Knowledge and Understanding.
Exeter: Learning Matters
46. Medwell, J. (1998) The context of children’s writing in junior classes unpublished PhD thesis, Exeter:
University of Exeter
47. Nicholls, J., Bauers, A., Pettit, D., Redgwell, V., Seaman, E. and Watson, G. (1989) Beginning Writing Milton
Keynes: Open University Press
48. Peters, M. (1985) Spelling caught or taught: A new look. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul
49. Peters, M. and Smith, B. (1993) Spelling in context Slough: NFER Nelson
ISSN: 2320-5407 Int. J. Adv. Res. 7(12), 947-954
954
50. Peverley, S. (2006). The importance of handwriting speed in adult writing. Developmental Neuropsychology,
29, 197 216.
51. Rubin, N. and. Henderson S.E (1982). Two sides of the same coin: Variation in teaching methods and failure to
learn to write. Special Education: Forward Trends, 9, 1724.
52. Saada-Robert, M. (1999) Effective means for learning to manage cognitive load in second grade school writing:
a case study. Learning and Instruction, 9, 189-208
53. Sassoon, R., Nimmo-Smith, I., and Wing, A. (1986). An analysis of children’s penholds. In Kao, H. van Galen,
G. and Hoosain R. (Eds.), Graphonomics: Contemporary research in handwriting (pp. 93106). Amsterdam:
Elsevier.
54. Scardamalia, M., Bereiter, C., and Goleman, H. (1982). The role of production factors in writing ability. In
Nystrand, M. (Ed.), What writers know: The language, process, and structure of written discourse (pp. 173
210). New York: Academic Press.
55. Stanovich, K. (1986) Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the
acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360-470
56. Sweller, J., and Chandler, P. (1994). Why some material is difficult to learn. Cognition and Instruction, 12,
185233.
57. Sweller, J. (1988) Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12, 247285.
58. Teale, W. H., and Sulzby, E. (Eds.) (1986). Emergent literacy: Writing and reading. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
59. Temple, C., Nathan, R., Burris, N. and Temple, F, (1982) The beginnings of writing (second edition) Newton,
MA: Allyn and Bacon
60. United Kingdom Literacy Association/ Primary National Strategy (2004) Raising boys’ achievements in writing
Hertfordshire: UKLA
61. Wray, D. & Lewis, M. (1997) Extending Literacy London: Routledge
62. Wyse, D. (1998) Primary Writing. Milton Keynes: Open University Press
63. Ziviani, J. and Elkins, J. (1984). An evaluation of handwriting performance. Educational Review, 36, 249261.
64. Ziviani, J., (1984) Some elaborations on handwriting speed in 7- to 14-year-olds. Perceptual and Motor Skills
58, 535-539.
... Indonesia's education system comprises four levels of education: primary (grades 1-6), junior secondary (grades 7-9), senior secondary (grades [10][11][12], and higher education. The first two levels constitute 'basic education' as that term is used in the Indonesian context. ...
... This trend has served to cement Indonesia's place towards the bottom of the list of assessed countries in these tests and behind neighbouring countries such as Malaysia, Vietnam, and Thailand. [12] ...
Article
Full-text available
Indonesia's biggest challenge regarding education is no longer improving access but improving quality. The Indonesian Government hopes to develop a 'world-class' education system by 2025. However, numerous assessments of the country's education performance suggest that it has a long way to go before it will achieve that goal. Many Indonesian teachers and lecturers lack the required subject knowledge and pedagogical skills to be effective educators; learning outcomes for students are poor; and there is a disparity between the skills of graduates and the needs of employers. This Analysis explores the reasons behind these problems and the implications for Australian education providers. It argues that Indonesia's poor education performance has not simply been a matter of low public spending on education, human resource deficits, perverse incentive structures, and poor management. It has, at its root, been a matter of politics and power. Change in the quality of Indonesia's education system thus depends on a shift in the balance of power between competing coalitions that have a stake in the nature of education policy and its implementation. This barrier to improved educational performance is likely to limit the scope for Australian education providers to develop closer research linkages with Indonesian universities, offer Australian students more in-country study options in Indonesia, recruit greater numbers of Indonesian students, and establish branch campuses in Indonesia.
... Such as cited from (Shaturaev, 2019), during the last one decade, a big research They are the generation who have unlimited access to information, until Generation Z many a time called Generation C, which one of them means "connected", as a representative from "connected to the internet" (Dolot, 2018) or connected with internet globally. This is because their growth period is accompanied with digital transformation, which has also constituted the way they interact, study, and participate in literacy culture who are competent with the advancement of technology. ...
Article
Generation Z is coming of age in the digital era where computer technology and mobile devices have become very common. They are accustomed to computer keyboards and touchscreens rather than pens and paper. The ability to write on digital devices such as smartphones, tablets, or laptops has replaced much of the need for writing by hand. However, handwriting is a fundamental skill in literacy upgrades. This descriptive qualitative research explores how efforts to handwriting habituate Gen Z in enhancing literacy using the brainwriting method. The retrieval data methods used are observation, interviews, and literature analysis involving student samples with specific criteria. The study’s results indicate several factors of the respondents who infrequently write by hand, the exploration of the use of the brainwriting method, and its impact on the success of literacy among them. The outputs such as enhanced critical thinking, creativity, and communication that is not unilaterally dominated. Keywords: Gen Z, handwriting, brainwriting, literacy
... This is discussed related to: 1) characteristics of handwriting in general, 2) characteristics of handwriting in particular, and 3) the use of analysis results as a consideration of criteria for preparing teaching materials. This needs to be discussed because handwriting has a low status and profile in literacy and education in recent years has attracted little attention from teachers, policy makers or researchers to mainstream the education process (Shaturaev, 2019). Discussions related to student handwriting are positioned to focus on emphasising the meaning of students' meanings in written creation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Teaching materials may be considered to be effective if their preparation is adjusted to the level of students’ internal potential and cognitive abilities. Therefore, graphology is required to assist in identifying students’ inner potential for the results of the graphological analysis to be used as a basis for preparing materials. This research aims to discover students’ inner potentials using graphological sciences, which can later be used to consider the criteria for preparing teaching materials. This research used a descriptive qualitative research method since this research produces data in the form of descriptive data (writing). The research’s subject is the handwriting of students of X-1 class Bekasi Regency. The stages of this research are handwriting analysis, triangulation through interviews, grouping results. The data used is in the form of student handwriting with an assessment of the characteristics of student writing as a whole in terms of the slope of writing, the direction of the writing line, the size of the writing, the spacing between words, and the spacing between lines. Qualitative research data analysis techniques are carried out with the stages of analysis implementation according to Miles and Huberman with stages: (1) Data reduction, (2) Presentation of data; (3) Drawing conclusions. According to the study’s result, it was discovered that the students’ inner potentials were very diverse. As a result, the form in which teaching materials are prepared must likewise be varied.
... Throughout evolution, our brains have adapted to reading written sources and therefore processing information differently than when reading screens. Let us not forget handwriting, which is also a very important factor in education and personal development [84]. We propose an educational system (Figure 7) in which we balance different approaches by delivering part of the education with traditional written resources and another part with electronic written resources and closed and open hypertexts combined with modern online classrooms or massive online open courses (MOOCs). ...
Article
Full-text available
Humanity faces diverse technological, societal, and sociological challenges. Digitalization is being integrated into every aspect of our lives. Technologies are developing rapidly and the ways in which we live and learn are changing. Young people are acquiring information and learning in a different way than in the recent past. Education systems are no longer keeping up with the development of technology. Education systems need to adapt and introduce technologies that motivate students and ultimately contribute to higher learning goals. To this end, we need to develop modern learning models that support education and technological development. In previous research, we developed and evaluated a state-of-the-art learning model, the CPLM. We built on this with a new study, in which we assessed the difference between the cognitive activities of attention and meditation in students during the viewing of a classical educational video, a 360° video, and an AR app on a screen. We found that the 360° video had the greatest impact on students’ attention and is consequently suitable for initially motivating students in the proposed learning model. We made a proposal for a modern educational model and possibilities for further research.
... Such instruction in handwriting legibility may include learning letter names, learning to form each letter of the alphabet, copying letters with numbered arrows providing step-by-step guidelines, and reproducing letters after watching another person write the letter . As handwriting legibility instruction may be straightforward, the evaluation of handwriting legibility is more subjective and qualitative than the evaluation of handwriting fluency, potentially leading to inaccurate measurements (Shaturaev, 2019). Legibility may be evaluated using dichotomous coding to indicate whether the response was correct or incorrect. ...
Article
Full-text available
According to the simple view of writing (Berninger, Abbott, Abbott, Graham, & Richards, 2002), the two important components of transcription in writing are handwriting and keyboarding, the third one being spelling. The purpose of this paper is to review the contribution of two writing modes-handwriting and keyboarding to writing performance. In the first section, the contribution of handwriting fluency to writing performance was explored through moderator analyses. We found that handwriting fluency contributes to writing significantly and consistently, and significantly contributes to specific writing measures (e.g., writing quality, writing fluency, substantive quality). We then explored the relationship between handwriting and keyboarding and compared their contributions to writing. Results indicated that performance on fluency of handwriting and keyboarding were significantly related, particularly on speed. Writing qualities under each mode was relatively competitive; however, keyboarding allows for faster writing. The findings from the two sections emphasized the importance of handwriting in writing development even though keyboarding is accessible.
Chapter
The Common European Framework of Reference for Languages (CEFR) has impacted language learning, teaching, and assessment in Japan in many ways since its publication in 2001, however more research is needed with regards to its application. In this study, issues with the inadequacies of the CEFR descriptors for writing, as well as the gap in research at the CEFR Pre-A1 and A1 levels were addressed. In particular, the CEFR Action Research Model (CARM) was used to investigate how the CEFR illustrative descriptors for writing can be applied to develop new CEFR-informed instructional materials to achieve writing proficiency at Pre-A1 and A1 levels for a secondary school in Japan. To address the shortcomings of the CEFR at the Pre-A1 and A1 levels in writing, relevant ‘Can Do’ descriptors were added and then used as reference for developing new CEFR-informed instructional materials to improve the students’ observed orthographic control and graphomotor fluency problems—areas of concern that have not been sufficiently explored in previous research especially in the Japanese context. As marked improvements in the students’ written output were observed after the intervention, it is recommended that the new instructional materials continue to be improved upon and refined, and instructional materials for the next CEFR level (CEFR A2) be developed. Furthermore, analyses of the relationship between improvement in students’ orthographic and graphomotor skills, and the quality of their writing output would also be an interesting area to pursue for future research.
Article
Full-text available
: The textile industry is one of the most water-intensive sectors of the economy. A large amount of water is consumed in the technological processes of washing, dyeing, printing and finishing fabrics. As a result of the implementation of these processes, wastewater of complex composition is formed, containing mainly dyes, surface-active substances (surfactants), mineral salts and other impurities. The discharge of effluents into water bodies without bringing their composition to the accepted sanitary standards is unacceptable. The cleaning methods that currently exist in the textile industry, such as adsorption, flotation, coagulation, zonation, etc., require large capital and operating costs and do not always provide the desired effect. Most of the methods practically do not allow preserving or trapping useful components from wastewater for reuse, therefore, recently both in Uzbekistan and abroad extensive research is being carried out and new physicochemical methods for treating wastewater are being developed, among which reverse osmosis occupies a special place and ultrafiltration.
Article
Full-text available
The textile industry is one of the most water-intensive sectors of the economy. A large amount of water is consumed in the technological processes of washing, dyeing, printing and finishing fabrics. As a result of the implementation of these processes, wastewater of complex composition is formed, containing mainly dyes, surface-active substances (surfactants), mineral salts and other impurities. The discharge of effluents into water bodies without bringing their composition to the accepted sanitary standards is unacceptable. The cleaning methods that currently exist in the textile industry, such as adsorption, flotation, coagulation, zonation, etc., require large capital and operating costs and do not always provide the desired effect. Most of the methods practically do not allow preserving or trapping useful components from wastewater for reuse, therefore, recently both in Uzbekistan and abroad extensive research is being carried out and new physicochemical methods for treating wastewater are being developed, among which reverse osmosis occupies a special place and ultrafiltration.
Article
Full-text available
Indonesia, home to 273 million people, is the fourth most populous country in the world. It is also the largest archipelago on the globe. Its territory spans more than 17,000 islands that stretch for 3,181 miles along the equator between the Pacific and Indian Oceans. About 87 percent of Indonesia's population is Sunni Muslim, making Indonesia the largest majority Muslim country in the world. Compulsory education in Indonesia faces various major educational issues such as financing, poor academic results, shortage of teachers both in rural and urban areas. These obstacles put Indonesia far from being excellent academically even with a bigger expenditure on education in the state. Throughout the research, the author tried widely draw a picture of the education system of the Republic of Indonesia as it is a part of the dissertation. The researcher has chosen field observation and analysis of available data to define the current condition of compulsory education, financing towards the national education system, teaching-learning process, and academic achievements of Indonesian school children. Collected data shows that the Government of Indonesia needs further educational reforms.
Article
Full-text available
COVID-19 затронул людей независимо от национальности, дохода и даже пола. Образование не исключение. В таком случае этот вызов затронул давние шрамы в системе образования Индонезии и Узбекистана. Однако главное препятствие на пути человеческого развития в Узбекистане заключается в развитии квалифицированного и знающего персонала, который может облегчить переход к конкурентоспособной на международном уровне экономике и образованию. Наличие качественных человеческих ресурсов имеет решающее значение для осуществления этой трансформации. Правительство при содействии Азиатского банка развития (АБР) и других партнеров по развитию стремится ускорить эту трансформацию. Расходы на образование в Узбекистане по отношению к ВВП являются одними из самых высоких в мире. Государственные расходы на образование
Article
A framework for conceptualizing the development of individual differences in reading ability is presented that synthesizes a great deal of the research literature. The framework places special emphasis on the effects of reading on cognitive development and on “bootstrapping” relationships involving reading. Of key importance are the concepts of reciprocal relationships—situations where the causal connection between reading ability and the efficiency of a cognitive process is bidirectional-and organism-environment correlation—the fact that differentially advantaged organisms are exposed to nonrandom distributions of environmental quality. Hypotheses are advanced to explain how these mechanisms operate to create rich-get-richer and poor-get-poorer patterns of reading achievement. The framework is used to explicate some persisting problems in the literature on reading disability and to conceptualize remediation efforts in reading.