ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

The gender-neutral third-person pronoun singular hen was recently introduced in Swedish as a complement to she (hon) and he (han). The initiative to add hen initially received strong criticism. In the present study, we analyzed 208 arguments from 168 participants with critical attitudes toward hen. We used Blaubergs’ (1980) and Parks and Roberton’s (1998) taxonomies of critical arguments against past gender-fair language reforms in English in the 1970s and 1990s as a basis for coding the arguments. A majority of arguments (80.7%) could be coded into existing categories, indicating that criticisms of gender-fair language initiatives are similar across different times and cultural contexts. Two categories of arguments did not fit existing categories (19.3%): gender-neutral pronouns are distracting in communication and gender information is important in communication. Furthermore, we established four overarching dimensions that capture assumptions and beliefs underlying gender-fair language criticism: (a) Defending the Linguistic Status Quo (39.4%), (b) Sexism and Cisgenderism (27.4%), (c) Diminishing the Issue and Its Proponents (26.9%), and (d) Distractor In Communication (6.3%). These dimensions of criticisms should be considered and addressed in different ways when implementing gender-fair language.
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Four Dimensions of Criticism Against Gender-Fair Language
Hellen Petronella Vergoossen
1
&Emma Aurora Renström
2
&Anna Lindqvist
1,3
&Marie Gustafsson Sendén
1,4
#The Author(s) 2020
Abstract
The gender-neutral third-person pronoun singular hen was recently introduced in Swedish as a complement to she (hon)andhe
(han). The initiative to add hen initially received strong criticism. In the present study, we analyzed 208 arguments from 168
participants with critical attitudes toward hen.WeusedBlaubergs(1980) and Parks and Robertons(1998)taxonomiesofcritical
arguments against past gender-fair language reforms in English in the 1970s and 1990s as a basis for coding the arguments. A
majority of arguments (80.7%) could be coded into existing categories, indicating that criticisms of gender-fair language
initiatives are similar across different times and cultural contexts. Two categories of arguments did not fit existing categories
(19.3%): gender-neutral pronouns are distracting in communication and gender information is important in communication.
Furthermore, we established four overarching dimensions that capture assumptions and beliefs underlying gender-fair language
criticism: (a) Defending the Linguistic Status Quo (39.4%), (b) Sexism and Cisgenderism (27.4%), (c) Diminishing the Issue and
Its Proponents (26.9%), and (d) Distractor In Communication (6.3%). These dimensions of criticisms should be considered and
addressed in different ways when implementing gender-fair language.
Keywords Gender .Gender identity .Gender-fair language .Gender-inclusive language .Gender-neutral pronouns .Hen .
Language reforms .Pronouns .Sexism
Gender-fair language strategies often face resistance. Negative
attitudes have been documented against specific gender-fair
reforms, such as the replacement of the masculine generic he
with the paired form he/she (Blaubergs 1980), guidelines for
non-sexist language (Parks and Roberton 1998), and the in-
troduction of a gender-neutral pronoun (Gustafsson Sendén
et al. 2015;Bäcketal.2015,2018; Lindqvist et al. 2019).
Past research has discerned several arguments that are used
against adopting gender-fair language. Blaubergs (1980)
developed a taxonomy of arguments against gender-fair lan-
guage in the wake of the proposal in the 1970s to replace the
masculine generic he with he or she. Blaubergs analyzed a
sample of arguments in newspapers articles, scientific journals
and other media, and established eight categories of argu-
ments: Cross-Cultural, Language Is a Trivial Concern,
Freedom of Speech/Unjustified Coercion, Sexist Language
Is Not Sexist, Word Etymology, Appeal to Authority,
Change Is Too Difficult, Inconvenient, Impractical or
Whatever (hereafter shortened to Change Is Too Difficult),
and It Would Destroy Historical Authenticity and Literary
Works (hereafter shortened to Historical Authenticity). Parks
and Roberton (1998) extended Blaubergstaxonomy based on
undergraduates arguments against gender-fair language with
four more categories: Sexism Is Acceptable, Hostility Toward
Proponents of Change, Tradition, and Lack of Understanding.
In Swedish, the gender-neutral third-person pronoun
singular hen has been introduced as a complement to
she (hon)andhe(han) and is used as both a pronoun
to refer to individuals with non-binary gender identities
and as a generic pronoun (SAOL 2014). Hens introduc-
tion received a lot of media attention in 2011, and in
2014 the pronoun was included in the Swedish dictio-
nary. At first, there were heated debates in the media, in
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article
(https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-019-01108-x) contains supplementary
material, which is available to authorized users.
*Hellen Petronella Vergoossen
hellen.vergoossen@psychology.su.se
1
Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, Frescati Hagväg
14, 106 91 Stockholm, Sweden
2
Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg,
Gothenburg, Sweden
3
Department of Psychology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
4
Department of Social Sciences, Södertörn University,
Huddinge, Sweden
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-019-01108-x
Published online: 6 January 2020
Sex Roles (2020) 83:328–337
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
universities, and among laypeople (Milles 2013). In
contrast to past reforms that added gendered (often fem-
inine) forms to make women more salient, a gender-
neutral pronoun reform reduces gender information.
The present study examines whether arguments against
the gender-neutral hen are similar to arguments from
past gender-fair language reforms using feminization
strategies in other cultural contexts. To this aim, the
taxonomies of criticism developed by Blaubergs (1980)
and Parks and Roberton (1998)wereusedtocategorize
the arguments against the gender-neutral pronoun hen.
Gender-Fair Language Planning
Feminist scholars have promoted gender-fair language for
over half a century. In the 1970s, feminists considered the
generic use of masculine pronouns and masculine occupation-
al titles problematic and both a symptom and a source of
fundamental androcentrism(Braun et al. 2005,p.3).
Empirical studies have shown that masculine generics are an-
drocentric because they more readily evoke mental images of
men (Gastil 1990; Moulton et al. 1978). This connotation was
for example shown in German where the generic masculine
forms of roles and occupations were associated with men
more frequently than with women (Stahlberg et al. 2001).
Furthermore, masculine generics influence attitudes and
behavior, especially among women. For example, women
were less motivated to apply for a job when masculine ge-
nerics were used in a job advertisement (Bem and Bem
1973). Women also felt less belonging, motivation, and iden-
tification with a job when masculine generic pronouns were
used in a mock interview compared to gender-fair alternatives
(e.g., he or she;one; Stout and Dasgupta 2011). Social judg-
ments can also be influenced by masculine generics. Women
applying for jobs were judged as less suitable for a high-status
leadership position when masculine forms were used in com-
parison to paired forms (Horvath and Sczesny 2015).
To counter the negative effects of masculine generics,
many languages have introduced gender-fair alternatives
to masculine generics throughout the late twentieth cen-
tury. These initiatives have focused on creating either
paired forms that include references to both women
and men (called feminization because the feminine form
is added) or gender-neutral forms (called neutralization
because gender information is reduced; Sczesny et al.
2016).Themostcommonwayofpairinggenderedpro-
noun forms is he or she(Willis and Jozkowski 2017).
Gendered pronouns can also be paired as he/she,”“s/
he,”“he (she),or they can be alternated throughout a
text. Critiques of paired pronoun forms include awk-
wardness in use, that constructions like s/hecannot
be said out loud, and that there are no comparable
grammatical case forms, such as his or hers
(Madson and Hessling 1999). Even organizations that
deter the generic use of he,such as the American
Psychological Association, discourage the repeated use
of paired forms stating that the repetition can become
tiresomeand that forms such as he/she or (s)he are
awkward and distracting(American Psychological
Association 2009,p.74).
Paired forms also suffer from an androcentric effect called
male firstness(Willis and Jozkowski 2017)becausehe or
sheis more common than she or he.Male firstness has also
been documented for personal names (Hegarty et al. 2011)
and in scientific articles that present gender differences
(Hegarty and Buechel 2006; Willis and Jozkowski 2017). In
addition, paired forms emphasize gender as a dichotomy by
explicitly denoting gender as constituting the binary catego-
ries woman (she) or man (he), excluding individuals with non-
binary gender identities (Ansara and Hegarty 2016; Hyde
et al. 2018). Hen is used as both a pronoun to refer to individ-
uals with non-binary gender identities or as generic form
(SAOL 2014). In this way, gender-neutral pronouns are dif-
ferent from paired language reforms because they make non-
binary identities visible in language and decrease the dichot-
omous perceptions of gender (Wayne 2005).
The Introduction of Hen
The introduction of hen generated a heated debate in 2011
(Bäck et al. 2015,2018; Lindqvist et al. 2016; Gustafsson
Sendén et al. 2015), despite Sweden being an egalitarian
country with a history of adopting feminist values and lan-
guage reforms (Milles 2011). Past studies have shown that
egalitarianism is related to a more positive opinion of
gender-fair language (Formanowicz et al. 2015)andagreater
use of gender-fair language (Hodel et al. 2017). However,
during the debate in Sweden, the response was mostly nega-
tive. In a study documenting the attitudes in a representative
sample collected in 2014 in Sweden, 55% of the participants
expressed negative attitudes toward hen whereas only 14%
were positive (Bäck et al. 2018).
The present study focuses on the contents of arguments that
Swedish people use against gender-neutral pronouns. To this
aim, we use the categories of arguments against gender-fair
language established by Blaubergs (1980)andParksand
Roberton (1998) to investigate whether their taxonomies are
also valid for gender-fair language reforms that introduce
gender-neutral pronouns (neutralization in contrast to femini-
zation) in a different time and cultural context. If the argu-
ments fit past taxonomies, this continuity might indicate that
people are more averse to gender-fair language change than to
the content of the reforms themselves.
329Sex Roles (2020) 83:328–337
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Method
Participants and Procedure
In the present study, arguments against hen were the focus.
Therefore, the sample included participants who were familiar
with hen and expressed at least one argument against using
hen. First, participants read a definition of hen:Hen could be
used when it is not necessary to specify a gender, for example
to replace he/she,or for individuals that dontwanttocate-
gorize themselves as she or he.Subsequently, they described
in a free-text response why they did or did not want to use hen.
Participants (n= 168 Swedish speaking; M
age
=30.18, SD =
12.08, range = 1872) provided 208 arguments against using
hen. Most participants (88.7%, n= 149) provided one argu-
ment, whereas a minority (11.3%, n= 19) provided multiple
critical arguments in their response. The analyses focus on the
208 arguments rather than participants.
Participants were recruited on the university campus of a
large university in Sweden in 2014 (paper-and-pencil ques-
tionnaire)and through social media and other onlineplatforms
in 2015 (online survey). They completed the study without
compensation. Participants indicated their gender in a free-
text response to avoid normative gender categorizations
(Ansara and Hegarty 2014; Lindqvist et al. 2019b), with
54.2% (n= 91) indicating woman; 40.5% (n=68),man; and
5.4% (n= 9) not responding.
The study was completed in accordance with national
guidelines on ethical research (Swedish Research Council
2017). In accordance with these guidelines, participants were
informed about their voluntary and anonymous contribution
and their possibility to quit the survey at any point without
giving any reasons for quitting. They were also informed that
results are presented on aggregated levels with no possibility
to extract personal information. Next, participants gave their
informed consent and were forwarded to the questionnaire.
After answering the questionnaire, participants actively sub-
mitted their responses. A formal ethical approval was not
mandatory for this type of research because it did not include
any biodata nor did it intend to affect the participants physi-
cally or psychologically. It also did not involve any handling
of sensitive data as described in the Swedish data protection
law.
Coding
The presentation of the coding procedure and results follow
guidelines outlined by Chatfield (2018) for publication of
qualitative research in Sex Roles. To examine the content of
the criticism of the gender-neutral pronoun hen in Sweden, we
used a coding scheme based on Blaubergs (1980)andParks
and Roberton (1998). The initial coding scheme included 12
categories of arguments against gender-fair language. One
category was excluded (Lack of Understanding) because it
contained participantsjudgments of why others are critical
of gender-fair language, which was not the focus of the pres-
ent study. This initial coding scheme is reported in the online
supplement.
The 208 negative arguments were coded through thematic
analyses (Braun and Clarke 2006). The approach to the the-
matic analyses was both deductive and inductive. The deduc-
tive approach involved coding the arguments into the original
categories, whereas the inductive approach involved the cate-
gorization of the arguments that did not fit into the original
categories. First, two of the authors separately coded the argu-
ments into the 11 original categories. The inter-coder reliabil-
ity for coding into the original categories was indexed with
Krippendorffs alpha (Hayes and Krippendorff 2007) and was
.89 for all categories taken together (range = .571). The av-
erage agreement between coders for coding arguments into
the separate original categories ranged from 33.33% (this cat-
egory consisted of only 3 arguments) to 100% (M
agreement
=
80.34%, SD = 23.4). The 15 arguments disagreed upon were
categorized after joint discussion. This resulted in 45.7% (n=
95) of the arguments being placed in the original categories.
Second, the same two authors jointly subjected the argu-
ments that were partly matched to original categories (35.1%,
n= 73) to a second round of analysis, in accordance with how
Parks and Roberton (1998)modifiedBlaubergs(1980)tax-
onomy. The goal was to decide whether these arguments
could be included in the original categories if any of the cat-
egory labels and category definitions were modified. The out-
come from this procedure led to the inclusion of all arguments
in modified original categories.
Third, the arguments that did not match any of the 11 orig-
inal categories (19.2%, n= 40) were subjected to an inductive
thematic analysis (Braun and Clarke 2006) by the same two
authors. This led to the creation of two new categories and an
updated coding scheme. The titles and definitions of the new
categories were based on the explicit content of the comments
grouped into this category. Subsequently, a third author coded
all the arguments into the final coding scheme with 13 cate-
gories. In the third round ofanalyses, the Krippendorffsalpha
for the agreement between the third rater and the first two
raters for the categorization of all comments was .98. The
average agreement between coders for coding arguments into
each of the original categories ranged from 66.67% to 100%
(M
agreement
=94.88%, SD = 11.08). The four arguments
disagreed upon were categorized after joint discussion.
Finally, when all the comments were categorized, we used
an inductive thematic analysis to organize the categories ac-
cording to their latent content. To establish the themes, we
went beyond the explicit content of the categories and
searched for underlying ideas, assumptions, and ideologies
that shaped the semantic content of the data (Braun and
330 Sex Roles (2020) 83:328–337
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Clarke 2006). All authors were involved in this inductive
process.
Results
The coding of the 208 negative arguments against hen into the
categories showed that a majority of the arguments (n=160,
76.9%) fit the categories in the original taxonomies from past
reforms. Two original categories were modified: Sexism Is
Acceptable was modified into Sexism and Cisgenderism Are
Acceptable and Change Is too Difficult into Change Is Too
Difficult or Unnecessary. About a fifth of the arguments
(19.7%, n= 41) were coded into two new categories: Gender
Identification Is Important (13.0%, n= 27) and Distractor in
Communication (6.3%, n= 13). Two categories from the orig-
inal taxonomy did not occur in this sample: Cross-Cultural
and Historical Authenticity.
The inductive analyses of the latent content of the catego-
ries resulted in four dimensions of criticism of gender-fair
language: (a) Defending the Linguistic Status Quo (39.4%
of arguments, n= 82), (b) Sexism and Cisgenderism (27.4%,
n= 57), (c) Diminishing the Issue and Its Proponents (26.9%,
n= 56), and (d) Distractor in Communication (6.3%, n=13).
The dimensions and their underlying categories are presented
in Table 1with percentages and examples.
Defending the Linguistic Status Quo
The first dimension captures a variety of categories that justify
the current linguistic norm and the preference to keep the
current linguistic system unchanged. Over a third of the argu-
ments (39.4%, n= 82) fit into this dimension. This dimension
included four categories: (a) Change Is Too Difficult or
Unnecessary, (b) Appeal to Authority, (c) Word Etymology,
and (d) Tradition. Change Is Too Difficult was modified to
include the argument that change is not necessary. Comments
about hen being unnecessary were very common, for exam-
ple: Idont see the need for itand We already have a
neutral word for gender non-specified situations: person.
Responses about the difficulty of use included: Icannotget
used to itand I have still not grown accustomed to the word
and it is awkward to say.Of the arguments, 33.7% (n= 70) fit
into this category.
The Appeal to Authority (3.8%, n=8) and the Word
Etymology (1.0%, n= 2) categories rarely occurred and
contained a variety of references to authorities. Some partici-
pants stated that they did not want to use hen because they
found hens definition unclear or because they thought it
lacked proper case forms. In the two comments in the Word
Etymology category, it was argued that a generic use of hen is
not proper because hen was introduced as a pronoun for peo-
ple with trans or non-binary gender identities. The ambiguity
in these arguments is that they are negative toward the generic
use of hen, but not negative toward using hen for people who
use it as their personal pronoun. Like the Change Is Too
Difficult or Unnecessary category, the two arguments that fit
into the Tradition category (1.0%, n= 2) showed a preference
for the status quo. However, instead of referring to authorities,
they used the tradition of using certain gendered words as
justification for using them. For example, one argument stated
that Santa Claus had always been a heand that should not
change.
Sexism and Cisgenderism
The second dimension represents beliefs about gender and the
implications of the language reform. The term cisgenderism
was added to sexism and refers to the ideology that condemns
peoples own designations of their genders and bodies (Ansara
and Hegarty 2014). A fourth (27.4%, n= 57) of the arguments
fit this dimension which comprises the modified category
Sexist and Cisgenderist Language Is Acceptable (14.4%,
n= 30) and the new category Gender Identification Is
Important (13.0%, n= 27). In past taxonomies, criticism of
gender-fair language included classical and hostile forms of
sexism (e.g., men are superior to women). These forms of
sexism were absent in the current sample, but we found sim-
ilar hostility against people with non-binary identities. Trans
identities were explicitly or implicitly neglected in the content
of the comments (Idont see that a few hurt people should
change language). In other types of comments, the existence
of people outside the binary system was neglected. According
to these comments, pronouns for individuals with non-binary
gender identities are unnecessary because there are no indi-
viduals who are not either a woman or a man: Idont see the
need for a gender-neutral pronoun, because biologically you
are either a man or a womanor Girls are girls, boys are
boys.Instead of an acceptance of masculine dominance be-
ing expressed in language, these comments express an accep-
tance of binary gendering in language. Therefore, we expand-
ed the category Sexist Language Is Acceptable to Sexist and
Cisgenderist Language Is Acceptable.
The new category Gender Identification Is Important
emerged from comments focusing on the importance to use
gender labels in communication about others and concerns
that gender-neutral pronouns lead to depersonalization. This
category also comprises dichotomous beliefs about gender.
However, instead of explicitly denying the existence of indi-
viduals with non-binary identities, this category contains ar-
guments about gendered information being important in com-
munication (I think ones gender is part of who we are, and
that is why I like saying he or sheand one often wants to
know someonesgender). In this way, the arguments implic-
itly neglect non-binary gender identities.
331Sex Roles (2020) 83:328–337
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Table 1 Dimensions with subcategories, their definitions, examples, and frequencies
Dimensions Definitions Example quotes % of arguments
(n= 208)Categories
Defending Linguistic Status
Quo
Participants provide arguments to keep language unchanged 39.4% (n=82)
Change Is too Difficult and
Unnecessary
The current linguistic term is too deeply rooted in the language,
the suggested change is too devious, or breaking a language
habit is too difficult to justify implementing the change.
It doesnt feel natural to use it, neither in writing or
speaking.
It is somewhat unnecessary.
I think that in everyday language she/he is too deeply
rooted for
[hen] to become commonlanguage.
There are other words.
Personally, I prefer to avoid using hen by reformulating the
text
without using he/she.
33.7% (n=70)
Appeal to Authority Authorities in the field of language, such as linguists or dictionaries
but also teachers and family members, have a final say in what is
the correct way to use language.
Write Swedish correctly instead.
It is confusing because it has an unclear definition.
3.8% (n=8)
Word Etymology The original meaning of the word is the real meaning of the word,
regardless of modern interpretations.
The word hen was initially used to describe the gender
identity
of a person who didnt consider themselves to be male or
female.
Thus, hen is not a gender-neutral word, but rather the
opposite.
That is why I think it
feels wrong to use hen.
1.0% (n=2)
Tradition Language is the way it is and has been this way a long time,
and it should remain unaltered.
For example, Santa has traditionally been a he.I dontsee
a
reason to change that.
1.0% (n=2)
Sexism and Cisgenderism Participants indicate gender hierarchies where cisgender
identities or men are considered of greater importance.
27.4% (n=57)
Sexism and Cisgenderism Are
Acceptable
Men are superior to women, so it is acceptable if language
reflects this. In this study, we expanded this category with: there
are only two genders, and ultimately everyone belongs to one of the
two; there are differences between women and men, and language
should reflect this.
Ireallydont like it, because I think that men should be
men and
women, women,
so we can turn each other on, play the game with each
other.
Without it, life would be very boring.
There is only two, he and she
14.4% (n=30)
Gender Identification Is
Important
New category. Gender-neutral language is impersonal and objectifying;
the wishes of those that want to be referred to with a gendered pronoun
must be respected; gender is important for ones identity; gender is
important in communication.
The person talked about becomes some sort of object that
you
dont feel you can relate to. Impersonal, weird,
distanced.
I personally dont want to be labeled with hen.
13.0% (n=27)
Diminishing the Issue and Its
Proponents
Participants devalue or ridicule the word or the proponents of the reform 26.9% (n=56)
Hostility and Ridicule The topic of sexist language is ridiculous and potentially harmful.
Proponents of gender-fair language are malicious.
Reminds me of höna [hen in English]
It is ugly
12.5% (n=26)
332 Sex Roles (2020) 83:328–337
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Tab l e 1 (continued)
Dimensions Definitions Example quotes % of arguments
(n= 208)Categories
It is just a ridiculous and childish expression.
Freedom of Speech/
Unjustified Coercion
The proponents of linguistic change attempt to control or censure
freedom of speech through, for example, publication guidelines.
There is a queer feminist, politically correct agenda behind
it
that tries to erase gender. And this hen is one way.
It feels like an unpleasant and authoritarian imposition
from above.
9.6% (n=20)
Sexist Language Is a Trivial
Concern
People should focus on more important forms of societal injustice
than language, for example, on the realphysical and economic
oppression of women.
I dont think a word leads feminism forward.
The problem is not in the words heand shebut in the
perceptions
of women. There we need change.
3.8% (n=8)
Sexist Language Is Not Sexist Sexist language is not sexist when there is no intention
to be sexist. When words are perceived as sexist, the
bias is not in the language used by the speaker, but in
the person listening.
If I had to describe a construction worker, I think of a man,
and then I would
have said he.But I dont think this is something
demeaning toward women
or that this means that women cantbeorarent allowed
to be construction
workers. It just feels more natural.
1.0% (n=2)
Distractor in Communication Participants indicate that using hen takes attention from the message 6.3% (n=13)
New category. The word is too loaded; it is too much of a statement; it detracts from the
message; when using it, it may
invite hostility from opponents of gender-neutral language.
I dont use the word because there are such strong opinions
about it. Whether the
opinions are positive or negative, the person gets stuck on
the use
of the word hen
and it takes focus from the rest youre trying to
say/write.
Hen becomes a statement by a person instead of fitting in
naturally.
333Sex Roles (2020) 83:328–337
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Diminishing the Issue and its Proponents
The third dimension includes disparaging reactions to both
gender-fair language and the people advocating for its use. A
fourth of the arguments (26.9%, n= 56) fit this dimension. This
dimension comprises four original categories: (a) Hostility and
Ridicule (12.5%, n= 26), (b) Freedom of Speech/Unjustified
Coercion (9.6%, n= 20), (c) Sexist Language Is a Trivial
Concern (3.8%, n= 8), and (d) Sexist Language Is Not Sexist
(1.0%, n= 2). Common to these categories is the opinion that
sexist language is a non-issue and that proponents of gender-
fair language miss the larger picture of gender equality. The
largest category within this dimension, Hostility and Ridicule,
contains comments that undermine linguistic change by being
hostile or diminishing: It is totally ridiculous to use that word,
It reminds me of a hen [referring to the English meaning of the
word hen],”“Its frivolous,and Seriously, I get irritated.
Clearly negative words were common in this category (e.g.,
ugly, nonsense, annoying, stupid, fussy).
Most of the ridiculing arguments in the first category target
the word itself rather than the people using it. However, in
other categories the arguments also target proponents of hen.
For example, the Freedom of Speech/Unjustified Coercion
category included criticism that focused on proponents of
the linguistic change threatening or coercing others to change
their linguistic habits. Examples of typical arguments are:
Sweden has ended up as a self-limiting totalitarian state
where political correctness hides everything in societyand
It disturbs me that some abuse it, or even become aggressive
if you decide to distinguish between the genders.Some crit-
icism was milder but suggests that gender equality had gone
too far. An example of such a comment is: I can feel [gender
equality] has gone a bit too far if, for example, we will have to
divide toilets for he, she and hen.
The third category Sexist Language Is a Trivial Concern
included arguments that express the idea that a word does not
make a difference or that there are other more important steps
to reach gender equality. The fourth category Sexist Language
Is Not Sexist similarly included arguments that denied that
sexist language negatively affects anyone. An example of
such a comment is: [Using gendered language] does not nec-
essarily mean that certain characteristics or attributes are
forced on a person.
Distractor in Communication
The fourth dimension focused on participantsconcern that
the use of hen may lead to a less effective communication or
even reprimands because hen is seen as a political statement.
This theme does not concern the content or the implications of
hen, but rather the reactions from others to the new word. In
total, 6.3% of the arguments (n=13) fit into this dimension.
For example, participants mentioned that hen could be
distracting: The reader will get stuck on the use of the word
hen and it takes focus from the rest of what one is trying to
say/write.This category is similar to comments that diminish
the issue and its proponents because it contains a shoot the
messengerattitude. The difference is that the participants
express concern that they themselves might become the target
of hostility and ridicule when using hen because hen is per-
ceived as a political statement: Idont want to use it at work
during for example a presentation because there are many that
have strong opinions about the word and I dont want it to take
the attention from the rest of the presentation.
Discussion
The present study investigated whether criticism against
gender-neutral pronouns in Sweden fit into taxonomies based
on criticism of other gender-fair language reforms that aimed
to make women more visible in English language contexts.
The results showed that a majority of the arguments against
the use of the Swedish gender-neutral pronoun hen could be
categorized into existing taxonomies of criticism of gender-
fair language (Blaubergs 1980; Parks and Roberton 1998).
This similarity is noteworthy because the hen-reform differs
from the reforms central in past studies in that hen removes
gender information instead of adding paired gendered forms.
This finding indicates that despite the differences in context
and intentions of the gender-fair linguistic reforms, the con-
tents of criticisms largely remain the same.
In addition to validating the work by Blaubergs (1980)and
Parks and Roberton (1998), we established four dimensions of
beliefs on gender and language that underlie criticism of gender-
fair language reforms. The broadest dimension included argu-
ments that defend the linguistic status quo. This theme reflects
peoples tendency to be generally negative about anything that is
new and to prefer to keep things the way they are (Bäck 2013;
Bäck and Lindholm 2014;Jostetal.2004). This phenomenon is
called status quo bias (Jost et al. 2004; Samuelson and
Zeckenhauser 1988). For instance, studies have shown that peo-
ple preferred a candy that ostensibly has been sold a long time as
compared to if it was presented as a relatively new candy
(Eidelman et al. 2010). There is a similar resistance toward
changes in language, where studies have found that new words
and expressions are described as uglyor examples of bad
language(Andersson 2001; Kotsinas 1996).
Some of the participants in the current study did not oppose
the use of gender-neutral expressions per se (e.g., they were
positive toward using terms like the person), but specifically
opposed the use of hen. This indicates that these participants
prefer existing words and may therefore overestimate whether
words such as the personevoke gender-neutral or gender-
balanced mental representations. In fact, such supposedly neu-
tral words often carry a male bias (Bailey and LaFrance 2017).
334 Sex Roles (2020) 83:328–337
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
For example, in a recruitment situation, the supposedly neutral
the applicantwas found to have a male bias, whereas hen
did not (Lindqvist et al. 2019). It is possible that people who
express criticism with this underlying theme may be more
prone to change their attitudes when hen loses its novelty or
when made aware of the shortcomings of already existing
neutral terms.
The dimension Diminishing the Issue and Its Proponents
included the strongest negative attitudes and affect toward
gender-fair language. Criticism in this dimension targets both
the reform and the people advocating for the gender-fair lan-
guage. The hostile comments prevalent in our sample reflect
the heated public debate in the media and in comments on
social media when hen was introduced in 2011 (Wojahn
2015). Criticism within this category may be so strong be-
cause it is rooted in ideology; what is important in terms of
social justice, and what means should be used to attain social
justice. For example, participants may perceive gender-fair
language initiatives as an infringement on their freedom of
speech. They may perceive initiatives like hen as prescriptive
and a form of language policing. They may also relate hen to
other social justice initiatives that they may oppose, such as
initiatives to make public spaces such as toilets gender-neutral
or non-binary inclusive or campaigns for non-binary people to
self-determine their gender in official documentation.
Gender-neutral language may also lead to participants
experiencing their own social identities to be threatened. For
example, past research has shown that people with a strong
female or male gender identity are more negative toward hen
and use hen less often (Gustafsson Sendén et al. 2015).
Challenging social gender identities by introducing a gender-
neutral pronoun may be difficult for people and lead to harsh
reactions (Morgenroth and Ryan 2018). Arguments that di-
minish the issue and its proponents may therefore be based
on a persons perceived threat of their own social identity.
The theme Sexism and Cisgenderism might also be rooted
in ideology. This theme contained the arguments that gender-
neutral pronouns are unnecessary because there are only two
genders, that gendered language is important for ones identi-
ty, and that knowing someones gender is necessary in com-
munication. The content of this theme aligns with findings of
prejudice against transgender individuals (Tee and Hegarty
2006). One predictor for prejudice against trans individuals,
including individuals with non-binary gender identities, is the
idea that sex and gender are determined by genes, hormones,
and genitals (Tee and Hegarty 2006). Sex/gender is seen as
unchangeable and binary, and transgender or intersex condi-
tions are seen as abnormal. Classical forms of sexism that
typically target women are not as relevant when proposing
gender-neutral pronouns because they do not promote the vis-
ibility of women in language. However, it is possible that
people who hold classical sexist beliefs also have essentialist
beliefs on cisgenderism, similar in the way prejudice toward
one minority group tends to spill over to other groups (e.g.,
generalized prejudice; Akrami et al. 2011). Our study suggests
that criticism against gender-fair language may be rooted not
just in the perception of gender roles in society, but also in
cisgenderism. Whether cisgenderist motivations for being crit-
ical of gender-fair language are easy to change should be
investigated in future studies.
The dimension Distractor in Communication concerns the
motivation to not use gender-fair language because it is
distracting. Arguments in this dimension addressed a motiva-
tion that has not been part of previous taxonomies of
criticismnamely that participants are not necessarily critical
of gender-fair language themselves, but instead are concerned
about whether hen disrupts their communication with others
and about whether other people may judge them when using
hen. In a similar way, the American Psychological Association
(APA) guidelines for authors support gender-fair language, but
also recommend against using paired pronouns as they are
awkward and distracting(pp. 74, APA 2009). Linguistic
norms like guidelines and policies are important motivators
for using gender-fair language (Koeser and Sczesny 2014). It
is important that such guidelines are based on empirical evi-
dence, and future research should investigate whether paired or
gender-neutral pronoun forms truly are distracting in commu-
nication. It is also possible that the use of a word that is initially
perceived as distracting or ideologically charged might normal-
ize over time, as has been shown for paired masculine and
feminine professional titles (Horvath et al. 2016).
Limitations and Future Research Directions
Many participants were students, who in general are more
progressive and positive to hen as compared to the wider
Swedish population (Bäck et al. 2018). The distribution of
comments over the different categories and the themes may
thus differ from the general population. A follow-up study
could make use of a representative population to validate the
results found in our study.
Future studies should investigate whether variation be-
tween individuals predict the type of critical arguments that
are used. For example, high levels of sexism may predict
hostile and sexist arguments, whereas a preference for the
status quo may predict arguments concerning the preservation
of the current language and already available words.
Analyzing the dimensions of arguments over time may also
reveal whether arguments with ideological motivations perse-
vere longer than other arguments rooted in other motivations.
Practice Implications
Implementors of gender-fair language initiatives should be
aware of the arguments that exist against gender-fair language
to address resistance appropriately. Some arguments are based
335Sex Roles (2020) 83:328–337
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
on ideological beliefs and values, which might be harder to
change because they are associated with peoplessocialiden-
tities. Other arguments are based on the preference for status
quo or a concern about making a political statement by using
gender-fair language; these objections might change if the
word becomes more common. Each of these underlying mo-
tivations may be addressed in different ways, and some may
be more challenging to address than others. To facilitate the
acceptance of new gender-fair language initiatives, it is impor-
tant to provide knowledge about gender bias in language
(Koeser and Sczesny 2014) and knowledge about variations
in biological sex and gender identities (Hyde et al. 2018), as
well as give people the chance to get used to the word so it
loses its novelty. Guidelines for gender-fair languages for au-
thorities and the media will help make the proposed word
more familiar.
Conclusion
The introduction of the gender-neutral pronoun hen in the
Swedish language has received a lot of criticism. In compar-
ison to past gender-fair language reforms that made women
more salient, this reform attempts to make people with non-
binary identities visible in pronoun use and decreases dichot-
omous perceptions of gender. At large, critical arguments
against using hen were similar to arguments against using
paired forms. Four dimensions captured universal structures
of hesitance to gender-fair language: (a) defending the linguis-
tic status quo, (b) diminishing the issue and its proponents, (c)
sexism and cisgenderism, and (d) gender-fair language being
distracting in communication.
Acknowledgements Open access funding provided by Stockholm
University.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Conflict of Interest This work was supported by the Swedish Research
Council (grant 2014-1150). The authors declare that they have no conflict
of interest.
Research Involving Human Participants and/or Animals All procedures
performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance
with the ethical standards. No animals were involved in the research.
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual
participants included in the study.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons
Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing,
adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as
long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the
source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if
changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article
are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated
otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the
article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will
need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a
copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
References
Akrami, N., Ekehammar, B., & Bergh, R. (2011). Generalized prejudice:
Common and specific components. Psychological Science, 22(1),
5759. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610390384.
American Psychological Association. (2009). Publication manual of the
American Psychological Association (6th ed.). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Andersson, L. G. (2001). Fult språk: Svordomar, dialekter och annat ont
[Ugly language: Profanities, dialects, and other evils]. Stockholm:
Carlsson.
Ansara, Y. G., & Hegarty, P. (2014). Methodologies of misgendering:
Recommendations for reducing cisgenderism in psychological re-
search. Feminism & Psychology, 24(2), 259270. https://doi.org/10.
1177/0959353514526217.
Ansara, Y. G., & Hegarty, P. (2016). Misgendering in English language
contexts: Applying non-cisgenderist methods to feminist research.
International Journal of Multiple Research Approaches, 7(2), 160
177. https://doi.org/10.5172/mra.2013.7.2.160.
Bäck, E. A. (2013). Position toward the status quo:Explaining differences
in intergroup perceptions between left- and right-wing affiliates.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43,20732082. https://doi.
org/10.1111/jasp.12160.
Bäck, E. A., & Lindholm, T. (2014). Defending or challenging the status
quo: Position effects on biased intergroup perceptions. Journal of
Social and Political Psychology, 2(1), 7797. https://doi.org/10.
5964/jspp.v2i1.158.
Bäck, E. A., Lindqvist, A., & Gustafsson Sendén, M. (2015). Hen can do
it! Effects of using a gender-neutral pronoun in recruitment. In J.
Magnusson, K. Milles, & Z. Nikolaidou (Eds.), Könskonstruktioner
och Språkförändringar: En rapport från den åttonde nordiska
konferensen om språk och kön [Gender constructions and language
change: Report from the 8th Nordic Conference on Language and
Gender] (pp. 7190). Flemingsberg, Sweden: Södertörn University.
Bäck, E. A., Lindqvist, A., & Gustafsson Sendén, M. (2018). Hen.
Bakgrund, attityder och användande [Hen: Background, attitudes
and use] (Vol. 8). Lund: Lund University. Retrieved from https://
www.psy.lu.se/sites/psy.lu.se/files/plr_1801.pdf.
Bailey, A. H., & LaFrance, M. (2017). Who counts as human?
Antecedents to androcentric behavior. Sex Roles, 76(1112), 682
693. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0648-4.
Bem, S. L., & Bem, D. J. (1973). Does sex-biased job advertising aid
and abetsex discrimination? Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
3(1), 618. https://doi .org/10 .1111/j.1559- 1816.19 73.tb01290 .x.
Blaubergs, M. S. (1980). An analysis of classic arguments against chang-
ing sexist language. Wom ens Studies International Quarterly, 3(2
3), 135147. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0148-0685(80)92071-0.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology.
Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(3), 77101. https://doi.org/
10.1191/1478088706qp063oa.
Braun, F., Sczesny, S., & Stahlberg, D. (2005). Cognitive effects of mas-
culine generics in German: An overview of empirical findings.
Communications, 30(1), 121. https://doi.org/10.1515/comm.2005.
30.1.1.
Chatfield, S. (2018). Considerations in qualitative research reporting: A
guide for authors preparing articles for Sex Roles. Sex Roles, 79(3
4), 125135. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-018-0930-8.
336 Sex Roles (2020) 83:328–337
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Eidelman, S., Pattershall, J., & Crandall, C. S. (2010). Longer is better.
JournalofExperimentalSocialPsychology,46(6), 993998. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.07.008.
Formanowicz, M., Cisłak, A., Horvath, L. K., & Sczesny, S. (2015).
Capturing socially motivated linguistic change: How the use of
gender-fair language affects support for social initiatives in Austria
and Poland. Frontiers in Psychology, 6,19. https://doi.org/10.
3389/fpsyg.2015.01617.
Gastil, J. (1990). Generic pronouns and sexist language: The oxymoronic
character of masculine generics. Sex Roles, 23(1112), 629643.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00289252.
GustafssonSendén, M., Bäck, E. A., & Lindqvist, A. (2015). Introducing
a gender-neutral pronoun in a natural gender language: The influ-
ence of time on attitudes and behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 6,
893. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00893.
Hayes, A. F., & Krippendorff, K. (2007). Answering the call for a stan-
dard reliability measure for coding data. Communication Methods
and Measures, 1(1), 7789. https://doi.org/10.1080/
19312450709336664.
Hegarty, P., & Buechel, C. (2006). Androcentric reporting of gender
differences in APA journals: 19652004. Review of General
Psychology, 10(4), 377389. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.
10.4.377.
Hegarty, P., Watson, N., Fletcher, L., & McQueen, G. (2011). When
gentlemen are first and ladies are last: Effects of gender stereotypes
on the order of romantic partnersnames. The British Journal of
Social Psychology, 50,2135. https://doi.org/10.1348/
014466610X486347.
Hodel, L., Formanowicz, M., Sczesny, S., Valdrová, J., & Von
Stockhausen, L. (2017). Gender-fair language in job advertisements:
A cross-linguistic and cross-cultural analysis. Journal of Cross-
Cultural Psychology, 48(3), 384401. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0022022116688085.
Horvath, L. K., & Sczesny, S. (2015). Reducing womens lack of fit with
leadership positions? Effects of the wording of job advertisements.
European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 25(2),
316328. https://doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2015.1067611.
Horvath, L. K., Merkel, E. F., Maass, A., & Sczesny, S. (2016). Does
gender-fair language pay off? The social perception of professions
from a cross-linguistic perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 6,1
12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02018.
Hyde, J. S., Bigler, R. S., Joel, D., Tate, C. C., & van Anders, S. M.
(2018). The future of sex and gender in psychology: Five challenges
to the gender binary. American Psychologist, 74,171193. https://
doi.org/10.1037/amp0000307.
Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. B., & Nosek, B. A. (2004). A decade of system
justification theory: Accumulated evidence of conscious and uncon-
scious bolstering of the status quo. Political Psychology, 25(6), 881
919. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.823.
Koeser, S., & Sczesny, S. (2014). Promoting gender-fair language: The
impact of arguments on language use, attitudes, and cognition.
Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 33(5), 548560.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X14541280.
Kotsinas, U. B. (1996). Ungdomsspråk [Youth Language]. Uppsala:
Hallgren & Fallgren.
Lindqvist, A., Bäck, E. A., & GustafssonSenden, M. (2016). Vem tycker
om hen? [Who likes hen?]. Språk och Stil, 26,101129.
Lindqvist, A., Renström, E. A., & Gustafsson Sendén, M. (2019a).
Reducing a male bias in language? Establishing the efficiency of
three different gender-fair language strategies. Sex Roles, 81,109
117. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-018-0974-9.
Lindqvist, A., Gustafsson Sendén, M., & Renström, E. A. (2019b). What
Is Gender, Anyway: A Review of the Options for Operationalizing
Gender Psychology and sexuality, in press.
Madson, L., & Hessling, R. M. (1999). Does alternating between mascu-
line and feminine pronouns eliminate perceived gender bias in text?
Sex Roles, 41(7/8), 559575. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:
1018895321444.
Milles, K. (2011). Feminist language planning in Sweden. Current Issues
in Language Planning, 12(1), 2133. https://doi.org/10.1080/
14664208.2011.541388.
Milles, K. (2013). En öppning i en sluten ordklass? Den nya
användningen av pronomenet hen [An opening in a closed word
class? The new use of the pronoun hen]. Språk Och Stil, 23(1),
107140.
Morgenroth, T., & Ryan, M. K. (2018). Gender trouble in social psychol-
ogy: How can Butlers work inform experimental social psycholo-
gistsconceptualization of gender? Frontiers in Psychology, 9,
1320. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01320.
Moulton, J., Robinson, G. M., & Elias, C. (1978). Sex bias in language
use: Neutralpronouns that arent. American Psychologist, 33(11),
10321036. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.33.11.1032.
Parks, J. B., & Roberton, M. A. (1998). Contemporary arguments against
nonsexist language: Blaubergs (1980) revisited. Sex Roles, 39(5/6),
445461. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018827227128.
Samuelson, W., & Zeckenhauser, R. (1988). Status quo bias in decision
making. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 1(1), 759. https://doi.org/
10.1007/BF00055564.
SAOL. (2014). Svenska Akademins Ordlista [Dictionary of Swedish
Language]. Stockholm: Nordstedts.
Sczesny, S., Formanowicz, M., & Moser, F. (2016). Can gender-fair lan-
guage reduce gender stereotyping and discrimination? Frontiers in
Psychology, 7,111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00025.
Stahlberg, D., Sczesny, S., & Braun, F. (2001). Name your favorite mu-
sician: Effects of masculine generics and of their alternatives in
German. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 20(4), 464
469. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X01020004004.
Stout, J. G., & Dasgupta, N. (2011). When he doesnt mean you: Gender-
exclusive language as ostracism. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 36(6), 767769. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0146167211406434.
Swedish Research Council. (2017). Good research practice.Retrieved
from https://www.vr.se/download/18.
5639980c162791bbfe697882/1529480529472/Good-Research-
Practice_VR_2017.pdf
Tee, N., & Hegarty, P. (2006). Predicting opposition to the civil rights of
trans persons in the United Kingdom. Journal of Community and
Applied Social Psychology, 16(1), 7080. https://doi.org/10.1002/
casp.851.
Wayne, L. (2005). Neutral pronouns: A modest proposal whose time has
come. Canadian Woman Studies, 24(2/3), 8591.
Willis, M., & Jozkowski, K. N. (2017). Ladies first? Not so fast:
Linguistic sexism in peer-reviewed research. Journal of Sex
Research, 55(2), 137145. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2017.
1346058.
Wojahn, D. (2015). Språkaktivism: Diskussioner om feministiska
språkförändringar i Sverige från 1960-talet till 2015 [Language
activism: Discussions about feminist language changes in Sweden
from the 1960s until 2015] (Doctoral dissertation). Uppsala
University, Uppsala, Sweden.
PublishersNote Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to juris-
dictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
337Sex Roles (2020) 83:328–337
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
... A língua é um emblema de identidade (Okan, 2019) e, em princípio, deve incluir todos os indivíduos que a usam. Vergoosen et al. (2020) analisaram argumentos contra o pronome neutro e estabeleceram quatro categorias abrangentes que captam as crenças por trás das críticas à linguagem inclusiva. Vergoosen et al. (2020) propõem que essas categorias sejam levadas em consideração ao se tentar implementar uma linguagem inclusiva em relação a gênero. ...
... Vergoosen et al. (2020) analisaram argumentos contra o pronome neutro e estabeleceram quatro categorias abrangentes que captam as crenças por trás das críticas à linguagem inclusiva. Vergoosen et al. (2020) propõem que essas categorias sejam levadas em consideração ao se tentar implementar uma linguagem inclusiva em relação a gênero. Em uma análise de sistemas não binários disponíveis em blogs em língua portuguesa, De Tilio e Vieira (2021) destacam o papel da linguagem como ferramenta de questionamento de práticas sexistas e de inclusão de pessoas que pertencem a minorias sexuais, de gênero e de orientações sexuais. ...
Article
Resumo: Os debates sobre o uso de formas não binárias incluem a possibilidade ou não de alterar regras existentes ou adicionar novas formas ao conjunto de normas da língua que viabilizam a expressão de identidades e a possibilidade de existências. No entanto, o uso de formas não binárias é visto por muitas pessoas como algo que transgride a norma culta ou modifica a língua como um todo, apesar de as línguas vivas passarem por constantes mudanças. A sala de aula de língua pode ou fomentar ou impossibilitar a expressão de identidades que escapam a sistemas rígidos, dependendo da visão de quem leciona. Este artigo discorre a respeito da visão de profissionais de língua portuguesa sobre o possível uso de linguagem inclusiva em sala de aula. Os dados obtidos através da aplicação de um questionário anônimo revelam a familiaridade de profissionais de língua portuguesa (n=55) com questões relacionadas à não binariedade, bem como dificuldades e preocupações em relação à implementação de linguagem inclusiva em aula. Os resultados indicam que a maioria das pessoas que participaram da pesquisa já tinham ouvido falar sobre linguagem neutra/não binária (93%) e conheciam pessoa não binária (74,5%). A maior parte do grupo (72%) concordava com o acréscimo de formas não binárias ao português, mas um número menor se disse confortável com o seu uso em geral (64%) e em materiais didáticos (57%). Dentre as preocupações expressas por profissionais, destaca-se a necessidade de desenvolvimento profissional. Disponível em https://www.portugueselanguagejournal.com/
... Besides, we wanted to explore whether the sexist behavior, in particular the degree of positive and/or negative feminine discrimination, influenced the gender-congruency effect, especially when processing the generic masculine. Previous research (Vergoossen et al., 2020) found some dimensions that capture the assumptions and beliefs underlying reasons against gender-fair language, among them sexism and cisgenderism. To detect whether benevolent sexism (e.g., women are weaker and therefore need protection from men) or hostile sexism (e.g., women are inferior to men and therefore hopeless) influenced the processing of the masculine form as the generic gender we considered the scores obtained through the Ambivalent sexist inventory (Cárdenas et al., 2010 based on Glick andFiske, 2001). ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of the study was to explore the nature of the gender-congruency effect, characterized by a facilitation on the processing of congruent words in grammatical gender. Moreover, we explored whether resemblances between gender identities and gender attitudes with grammatical gender modulated lexical processing. We designed a gender-priming paradigm in Spanish, in which participants decided the gender of a masculine or feminine pronoun preceded by three different primes: biological gender nouns (mapping biological sex), stereotypical nouns (mapping biological and stereotypical information), and epicene nouns (arbitrary gender assignment). We found faster processing of gender congruent pronouns independently of the type of prime, showing that the grammatical gender feature is active even when processing bare nouns that are not conceptually related to gender. This indicates that the gender-congruency effect is driven by the activation of the gender information at the lexical level, which is transferred to the semantic level. Interestingly, the results showed an asymmetry for epicene primes: the gender-congruency effect was smaller for epicene primes when preceding the feminine pronoun, probably driven by the grammatical rule of the masculine being the generic gender. Furthermore, we found that masculine oriented attitudes can bias language processing diminishing the activation of feminine gender, which ultimately could overshadow the female figure.
... At the same time, it constantly changes. The intentional use of language in the sense of gender-sensitive and genderneutral communication is both a field of action for gender equality measures and a political point of debate in European societies (Burnett & Pozniak, 2021;Vergoossen et al. 2020). The rejection of gender-inclusive practices is a widespread response to efforts to make language and practices of communication gender-just, and to change traditional ways of writing and speaking accordingly. ...
Research
Full-text available
This Toolbox for gender-neutral, diversity-oriented institutional communication (D. 5.4) is submitted by RUB as part of Work Package 5 – Ensure a sustainable and cultural change by establishing a gender and diversity-friendly environment of the RESET project. This toolbox is the output of Task 5.4 – Promoting a unique standard of gender neutral, non-discriminatory, positive forms of communication. The toolbox builds on the insights and achievements that are reflected in the Guidelines for gender-sensitive communication in research and academia (2021) of the project SUPERA - Supporting the Promotion of Equality in Research and Academia. Additionally, this toolbox enriches the focus on gender-sensitive communication with the notion of gender-inclusive forms of communication as well as an intersectional perspective on further dimensions of diversity. For this, the Toolbox contains with a conceptual framework, that has a purpose to provide valuable information and knowledge around the necessity of gender-neutral, gender-sensitive as well as diversity-oriented forms of communication. Here, RESET’s intersectional perspective, and its understanding of diversity in the context of HEIs are explained. Information on social psychological and socio-linguistic aspects of communication are interpreted in relation to gender and diversity. Against these backgrounds the opportunities and challenges of visual aspects of communication are reflected. Part B contains the tools that have been developed in the context of the toolbox-creation, aligned with the conceptual framework, and based on institutional queries, interviews, and discussions within RESET consortium. They are meant to provide a starting point for making communication at our universities more inclusive, both in terms of gender diversity and other diversity factors. The Toolbox is a work-in-progress and closely interwoven with the ongoing work of RESET project. It will be enriched as the project evolves. At this stage, the toolbox has a strong focus on the aspect of gender-inclusive communication. In the future, it is going to entail more tools that emphasize the diversity aspect to a greater extent and that are going to be co-designed with our communities at our universities, to create safe and diversity friendly environments.
Chapter
This chapter focuses on increasing advocacy and visibility of identity through language usage in educational communities. Language is complex and constantly evolving. The journey of locating a community of acceptance is essential to feel empowered to present authentically, especially regarding the use of language that is inclusive and encompassing of each individual. Relationships contributing to the development of self-acceptance embrace the importance of language in recognizing and validating social identity. Relational influences and belonging within social communities enable people to present their authentic selves within educational settings, which can transform individuals, marginalized communities, and social norms. A reflective approach is taken within this chapter to increase awareness regarding inclusive language and to unpack personal beliefs of identity within the educational workplace and classroom constructs.
Article
This paper examines the effect of the recently introduced Dutch non-binary 3rd person pronouns hen and die on tesssxt comprehensibility and text appreciation in the context of newspaper reporting on non-binary persons. Moreover, it presents a first measurement of Flemish people’s familiarity with and attitudes towards this pronominal reform in Dutch in its early stage. In a survey experiment we compared the use of non-binary hen and die (both combined with hen as object and hun as possessive form) with established referential strategies. We also examined the potential mediating role of perceived awkwardness of the referential strategy used and tested the moderating effect of cueing, i.e. making readers aware of the fact that the person reported on identifies as non-binary and prefers non-binary pronouns. The results show that perceived awkwardness explains the lower comprehensibility and appreciation scores of non-binary hen and that cueing improves those scores. Overall, our findings suggest that especially the non-binary pronoun die has the potential to be successfully implemented.
Article
Full-text available
Our use of language conveys referential meanings and attitudes. The idea that women are unimportant to critical life issues that males handle first is the basis for "woman's language". Women's marginalization and helplessness are mirrored in all the views they are required to talk about, and they are speaking. Speaking in a way that is not proper for women, avoiding expressing strong emotions, favouring expressions of uncertainty, and elaborating on ways to explain ideas that are judged "trivial" to the "real" world Speech about women implies an object whose sexual nature necessitates euphemism, and whose social positions are derived and dependent about males. Thus, women’s identity is linguistically obscured; language works against treating women as important individuals with unique viewpoints. In this paper, the aspects of gender cover both the cultural and social strengths of a woman in this society. In general, the language deals with the adaptability of women in terms of gender, activities, and thoughts in their respective societies. By these ideologies, the paper highlights how to overcome equality in language and women.
Article
Full-text available
Before essential feminist contributions to legal drafting were made, legislative drafters adopted the use of the masculine rule, which established that all genders were implicitly included in the usage of the pseudo-generic third person masculine singulars such as 'he' and 'him.' In the 1960s, feminism acted as a nucleus for an approach to legal drafting that was inclusive of and thus avoided the erasure of women in constitutional and legislative language. Historically, the concept of gender neutrality has been approached from binary cisgendered and heteronormative perspectives. Legal drafters now have to take cognisance of this evolving reality as there is a growing need for legislation that is gender diverse and non-heteronormative. The Recognition of Customary Marriages Act 120 of 1998 has been subject to criticism for its use of gendered language that excludes queer couples. This article places the development of an understanding of inclusive legal drafting in South Africa within Afro-feminist theory. These theories present a more useful framework for thinking beyond a binary view of language in legal drafting. They also present an opportunity of placing inclusive legal drafting as African, in the face of continued marginalisation and subjugation of gender and sexual minorities on the continent. Using theories such as the coloniality of gender, the coloniality of being and the coloniality of knowledge for deconstructing Western and consequently binary notions of gender neutrality, I suggest an Afro-feminist understanding of drafting that will consequently be gender-neutral in a way that is inclusive of queer people.
Article
Full-text available
In this paper, the various arguments that have been presented with respect to usage of generic masculine forms and pair/feminine forms are discussed and analysed. The source of the data is provided by a questionnaire carried out in October 2019, including a sample of answers and comments from Slovak, Czech and Polish respondents. In the study, two dominant views on generic masculine forms, arbitrary and semantic, are introduced and discussed against empirical findings from many experiments and studies. The material from the questionnaire is qualitatively analysed with respect to the axiological reactions of the respondents. The language attitudes are further classified into eight categories: representation, offensiveness, addressing, economy, textual qualities, language naturalness, tradition and ideological markedness. The attitudes of participants from both “camps” are quoted to illustrate the argumentation process yielding to the acceptance or rejection of forms substantiating gender-inclusive language.
Article
Full-text available
Our use of language conveys referential meanings and attitudes. The idea that women are unimportant to critical life issues that males handle first is the basis for "woman's language". Women's marginalization and helplessness are mirrored in all the views they are required to talk about, and they are speaking. Speaking in a way that is not proper for women, avoiding expressing strong emotions, favouring expressions of uncertainty, and elaborating on ways to explain ideas that are judged "trivial" to the "real" world Speech about women implies an object whose sexual nature necessitates euphemism, and whose social positions are derived and dependent about males. Thus, women’s identity is linguistically obscured; language works against treating women as important individuals with unique viewpoints. In this paper, the aspects of gender cover both the cultural and social strengths of a woman in this society. In general, the language deals with the adaptability of women in terms of gender, activities, and thoughts in their respective societies. By these ideologies, the paper highlights how to overcome equality in language and women.
Article
Full-text available
In the social sciences, many quantitative research findings as well as presentations of demographics are related to participants’ gender. Most often, gender is represented by a dichotomous variable with the possible responses of woman/man or female/male, although gender is not a binary variable. It is, however, rarely defined what is meant by gender. In this article, we deconstruct the concept ‘gender’ as consisting of several facets, and argue that the researcher needs to identify relevant aspects of gender in relation to their research question. We make a thorough exposition of considerations that the researcher should bear in mind when formulating questions about each facet, in order to exemplify how complex this construct is. We also remind the researcher that gender is not a binary category and discuss challenges in the balance between taking existing gender diversity into account and yet sorting participants into gender categorisations that function in statistical analyzes. To aid in this process, we provide an empirical example on how gender identity may be categorised when using a free-text response. Lastly, we suggest that other measurements than participants’ gender might be better predictors of the outcome variable.
Article
Full-text available
Different strategies of gender-fair language have been applied to reduce a male bias, which means the implicit belief that a word describing an undefined person describes a man. This male bias might be caused by the words themselves in terms of generic masculine or masculine forms or by androcentrism (the conflation of men with humanity). In two experiments, we tested how different gender-fair strategies used as labels of an unknown social target (an applicant in a recruitment situation) could eliminate the male bias. The three types of gender-fair strategies tested were: (a) paired forms (he/she), (b) traditional neutral words (e.g., singular they, “the applicant”), or (c) gender-neutral third-person pronouns actively created to challenge the binary gender system (ze, Swedish hen). The two experiments were performed in Swedish with 417 undergraduates in Sweden and in English with 411 U.S. participants recruited online. In Swedish, the third-person gender-neutral pronoun singular (hen) was used. In English, several forms of such gender-neutral pronouns have been suggested (e.g., ze). In both experiments, results indicated that paired forms and actively created gender-neutral pronouns eliminated the male bias, whereas traditional neutral words contained a male bias. Thus, gender-fair language strategies should avoid using traditional words. Consequences of using paired forms and creating new gender-neutral words are discussed. We argue that an actively created gender-neutral pronoun is of highest value because it is more inclusive.
Article
Full-text available
A quarter of a century ago, philosopher Judith Butler (1990) called upon society to create “gender trouble” by disrupting the binary view of sex, gender, and sexuality. She argued that gender, rather than being an essential quality following from biological sex, or an inherent identity, is an act which grows out of, reinforces, and is reinforced by, societal norms and creates the illusion of binary sex. Despite the fact that Butler’s philosophical approach to understanding gender has many resonances with a large body of gender research being conducted by social psychologists, little theorizing and research within experimental social psychology has drawn directly on Butler’s ideas. In this paper, we will discuss how Butler’s ideas can add to experimental social psychologists’ understanding of gender. We describe the Butler’s ideas from Gender Trouble and discuss the ways in which they fit with current conceptualizations of gender in experimental social psychology. We then propose a series of new research questions that arise from this integration of Butler’s work and the social psychological literature. Finally, we suggest a number of concrete ways in which experimental social psychologists can incorporate notions of gender performativity and gender trouble into the ways in which they research gender.
Article
Full-text available
Qualitative inquiry is frequently used to deepen understanding, improve empathy, and inspire social change, making it particularly appropriate for researchers using feminist, critical or other frameworks that encourage readers to consider issues of power and privilege. Gender researchers using qualitative inquiry are able to select from within a multitude of qualitative approaches to address the purpose of any given research study. The availability of so many approaches makes it challenging to provide one set of “best practices” for qualitative inquiry. The purpose of the present paper is to address this challenge by providing a combination of general and approach-specific guidelines for authors who aspire to write qualitative original research articles for publication in Sex Roles. I begin by providing broad guidelines, follow with approach-specific considerations, and finish the paper with advice related to common practices in qualitative research reporting. I also provide a sample of authoritative sources authors might wish to consult and cite in their research papers.
Article
Full-text available
Swedish is the first language that has a gender-neutral third-person singular pronoun, hen, that has spread from transgender and queer communities to the broader society and now exists in parallel with the two traditional gendered pronouns representing 'she' and 'he'. Many strong emotions have arisen during this process, both for and against hen. This study aims at analyzing what background factors may explain the attitudes towards hen. In total, 240 individuals participated in our online questionnaire where they indicated their attitude towards hen, as well as responded to questions assessing attitudes towards sexist language (i.e. gender discriminating language), modern sexism (i.e. the belief that gender-discrimination is no longer an issue), political views (from left to right), interest in gender issues and their identification with their own gender identity. The results show no gender difference in attitudes towards hen, but participants strongly identifying themselves with their gender identity had a tendency to be more negative towards the word. Political view was not a significant predictor. However, participants with sexist attitudes had a tendency to dislike hen, whereas those who were interested in gender issues and were negative towards sexist language had a tendency to like the word. Finally, younger age implied a stronger tendency to like hen.
Article
Full-text available
Swedish is the first language that has a gender-neutral third-person singular pronoun, hen, that has spread from transgender and queer communities to the broader society and now exists in parallel with the two traditional gendered pronouns representing 'she' and 'he'. Many strong emotions have arisen during this process, both for and against hen. This study aims at analyzing what background factors may explain the attitudes towards hen. In total, 240 individuals participated in our online questionnaire where they indicated their attitude towards hen, as well as responded to questions assessing attitudes towards sexist language (i.e. gender discriminating language), modern sexism (i.e. the belief that gender-discrimination is no longer an issue), political views (from left to right), interest in gender issues and their identification with their own gender identity. The results show no gender difference in attitudes towards hen, but participants strongly identifying themselves with their gender identity had a tendency to be more negative towards the word. Political view was not a significant predictor. However, participants with sexist attitudes had a tendency to dislike hen, whereas those who were interested in gender issues and were negative towards sexist language had a tendency to like the word. Finally, younger age implied a stronger tendency to like hen.
Article
Full-text available
The present study investigates whether and how the use of gender-fair language is related to linguistic, cultural, and socioeconomic differences between countries with grammatical gender languages. To answer this question, we analyzed job titles in online job advertisements from four European countries differing in achieved gender equality and egalitarian versus hierarchical cultural values (Switzerland, Austria, Poland, and Czech Republic). Results show that gender-fair job titles were more frequent in more egalitarian countries with higher levels of socioeconomic gender equality (Switzerland, Austria) than in countries with a higher acceptance of hierarchies and inequalities (Poland, Czech Republic). In the latter countries, gender-specific (masculine or feminine) job titles predominated. Moreover, gender-fair job titles were more prevalent in a female-dominated branch (health care) and a gender-balanced economic branch (food services) than in a male-dominated branch (constructional steel and metal work). Thus, our findings suggest that the language use in job advertisements indeed corresponds with linguistic, cultural, and socioeconomic aspects and may contribute to the transmission of gender (in)equalities and gender stereotypes.
Article
The view that humans comprise only two types of beings, women and men, a framework that is sometimes referred to as the “gender binary,” played a profound role in shaping the history of psychological science. In recent years, serious challenges to the gender binary have arisen from both academic research and social activism. This review describes 5 sets of empirical findings, spanning multiple disciplines, that fundamentally undermine the gender binary. These sources of evidence include neuroscience findings that refute sexual dimorphism of the human brain; behavioral neuroendocrinology findings that challenge the notion of genetically fixed, nonoverlapping, sexually dimorphic hormonal systems; psychological findings that highlight the similarities between men and women; psychological research on transgender and nonbinary individuals’ identities and experiences; and developmental research suggesting that the tendency to view gender/sex as a meaningful, binary category is culturally determined and malleable. Costs associated with reliance on the gender binary and recommendations for future research, as well as clinical practice, are outlined.
Article
The words we use reflect and influence our interpretation of the world. The role of gender within a language varies; biases based on social gender are referred to as linguistic sexism. Male firstness is the practice of persistently ordering masculine terms before feminine terms. Because academic writing is mandated to be free of bias, peer-reviewed research should not contain any form of linguistic sexism. To assess the presence of male firstness in academic writing, we examined 862 contemporary articles from 10 social science journals across three disciplines: sexuality, health, and psychology. To assess male firstness, we tallied common gendered pairs (e.g., "women and men"; "male and female") and calculated percentages indicating how often men were presented before women. We found that male firstness bias was present in each of the 10 journals. For individual journals, the percentage of gendered phrases that presented masculine terms first ranged from 57.7% to 88.8%. Sexuality and health journals demonstrated less linguistic sexism than psychology; however, there were no consistent trends between journals within each discipline. We discuss the current presence of male firstness in academic journals and the issues this bias triggers. Recommendations for reducing linguistic sexism in peer-reviewed research are also discussed.