ArticlePDF Available

Psychological well-being and personality traits are associated with experiencing love in everyday life

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Everyday life presents many experiences that can make people feel connected to another and leave them feeling loved. We conducted two ecological momentary assessment studies (N = 52 and N = 160) to examine people's subjective perceptions of the impact of these experiences by capturing the extent to which they felt loved at several randomly sampled times during their daily life. Individual differences in loving feelings were characterized by baseline levels, within-person variabilities, and slow and fast time scale indicators of change. Results showed that there were considerable individual differences in these characteristics and these individual differences related systematically to both psychological well-being and personality: across two studies, higher felt love baseline levels were related to greater psychological well-being as well as to higher Extraversion personality scores, while people scoring high on Neuroticism showed lower baseline levels.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Personality and Individual Dierences
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid
Psychological well-being and personality traits are associated with
experiencing love in everyday life
Zita Oravecz
a,
, Jessica Dirsmith
b
, Saeideh Heshmati
c
, Joachim Vandekerckhove
d
,
Timothy R. Brick
a
a
Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, United States
b
Duquesne University, Pittsburgh, PA 15282, United States
c
Claremont Graduate University, Claremont, CA 91711, United States
d
University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697, United States
ARTICLE INFO
Keywords:
Feeling loved
Individual dierences
Psychological well-being
Personality
Ecological momentary assessment
ABSTRACT
Everyday life presents many experiences that can make people feel connected to another and leave them feeling
loved. We conducted two ecological momentary assessment studies (N= 52 and N= 160) to examine people's
subjective perceptions of the impact of these experiences by capturing the extent to which they felt loved at
several randomly sampled times during their daily life. Individual dierences in loving feelings were char-
acterized by baseline levels, within-person variabilities, and slow and fast time scale indicators of change.
Results showed that there were considerable individual dierences in these characteristics and these individual
dierences related systematically to both psychological well-being and personality: across two studies, higher
felt love baseline levels were related to greater psychological well-being as well as to higher Extraversion per-
sonality scores, while people scoring high on Neuroticism showed lower baseline levels.
1. Introduction
A friendly chat with a neighbor, a co-worker oering to help out on
a project, and a welcoming smile from a teacher. All of these are small
but potentially profound momentary experiences that can make one
person feel temporarily connected to another, and leave them feeling
loved. It has long been established that humans thrive on social con-
nectedness (see e.g., Baumeister & Leary, 1995), and languish in its
absence. A lack of aliation with others can lead to a rise in feelings of
loneliness and social isolation, which has been linked to poorer health
behaviors (i.e., smoking, physical inactivity, and poorer sleep) and
problematic health outcomes such as higher blood pressure and poorer
immune functioning (Cacioppo & Hawkley, 2009;Grant, Hamer, &
Steptoe, 2009) as well as increased risk of early mortality (Holt-
Lunstad, Smith, & Layton, 2010). In contrast, when people express
mutual care and concern for one another (in a romantic or non-ro-
mantic context), these daily interpersonal interactions might result in
the experience of momentary increases in feelings of love. We con-
ceptualize love broadly in this paper and assume that it emerges from
positivity resonance (Fredrickson, 2016). According to this framework,
people experience positivity resonance in their daily lives via biobe-
havioral synchrony, shared momentary experiences of positive emo-
tion, and mutual care. Positivity resonance has already been shown to
have an important impact on well-being (Major, Le Nguyen, Lundberg,
& Fredrickson, 2018).
Researchers have long sought out ways to operationally dene love
and to ascertain dierent types of love. In what follows, we review how
love has been studied in scientic research. We argue that while most of
the empirical work has centered on romantic love, non-romantic love
has also been considered in several studies. Moreover, in recent studies,
the importance of an ecologically valid way of studying love has been
brought into focus. Following this new perspective, our research pre-
sented below delves more deeply into everyday life love experiences
that are not limited to romantic love, by analyzing individual dier-
ences in love experiences via ecological momentary assessment (EMA;
Stone & Shiman, 1994) studies. By frequently assessing the degree to
which people feel loved (i.e., their levels of felt love) in natural settings
and deriving individual dierences in key characteristics of these ex-
periences over time, we aim to create a useful tool to capture the eect
of positivity resonances.
Results obtained in the two EMA studies described below indicate
considerable individual dierences in daily love experiences (e.g., in
baseline levels and within-person variability). These interindividual
dierences appear to relate systematically to psychological well-being.
To further position daily felt love experiences in existing theoretical
frameworks, we examined links to personality traits and gratitude
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.109620
Received 2 February 2019; Received in revised form 13 September 2019; Accepted 18 September 2019
Corresponding author at: 232 Health and Human Development Building, University Park, PA 16802, United States.



interindividual dierences in daily experiences of felt love were sys-
tematically related to both.
1.1. How to conceptualize love
Discussions of the characteristics of romantic and non-romantic love
date back at least as far as Aristotle (Nicomachean Ethics) and Plato
(Symposium), who distinguished philia (broadly, nonromantic love)
from eros (sexual or romantic love). Research in the modern eld of
psychology has primarily focused on romantic love, mostly either from
an evolutionary or biological perspective (e.g., Baumeister &
Finkel, 2010) or by considering its social aspects (e.g., Gottman, 1979;
Hendrick & Hendrick, 1986;Lee, 1977,1988). A social psychology
perspective also emerged with Kelley et al. (1983), who focused on
interdependence and attribution in intimate relationships. The resulting
interest in romantic love has continued to dominate the literature.
While empirical research on non-romantic love is scarce, some re-
searchers have construed love suciently broadly to include non-ro-
mantic relationships as well. For example, Sternberg (1986) dened
three basic components of love: intimacy, passion, and commitment;
this is a denition broad enough to include non-romantic love re-
lationships such as among family members and between friends. Others
have returned to Plato's approach and sought to identify types of love
(e.g., Berscheid, 2006) or have characterized love based on behavioral
observations of love acts(Hazan & Shaver, 1987) or loving acts (Buss,
1988) or through biological assessments of people who are in love
(e.g., Young, 2009). In each case, however, the construct remains lar-
gely focused on romantic love, with other types modeled as a means of
contrasting other social relationships with that common focal state.
At its core, however, the construct of love is also recognized in the
literature as an emotion or as a combination of emotion with a sense of
social connectedness. In fact, Izard (1977) noted that love is the com-
bination of the emotion joy with the interest that people feel in con-
nection with others, which again reinforces the idea that love is multi-
faceted and not limited to just romantic love. This broader under-
standing of love, as stemming generally from positive emotion and
social connectedness, and not only from romantic relationships, can be
seen as a synthesis of ndings in the relationship and emotion science.
1.2. Studying love in everyday life
Studying love, especially studying romantic relationships, has gen-
erated new challenges: getting people to fall in love while in the la-
boratory is dicult (Aron, Melinat, Aron, Vallone, & Bator, 1997), and
the timing of relationships is intricateconsider for example how short
interactions on dates might merge into more enduring emotions. To
better understand the underlying pathways of such processes, re-
searchers brought their work out of the laboratory to study people as
they lived their day-to-day lives. For example, Gable and Poore (2008)
assessed people's reections in long-term dating relationships by sig-
naling them several times a day to report their thoughts and feelings
about their partners. Emerging from these roots, researchers have
begun to place emphasis on everyday life experiences of love, while also
emphasizing a conceptualization of love that is not limited to romantic
relationships. Oravecz, Muth, and Vandekerckhove (2016) and
Heshmati et al. (2019) demonstrated that people tend to agree on what
makes them feel loved in everyday life, indicating that people's un-
derstanding of love does extend beyond strictly romantic love. This is in
line with Fredrickson's (2016) description of love as an emotion that
crosses both romantic and interpersonal non-romantic relationship
boundaries.
At present, empirical research focusing on experiences of love in
daily life (and their correlates) remain scarce. To our knowledge, only
Major et al. (2018) adopted Fredrickson's (2016) empirical framework
for modeling love in everyday life settings, using a daily diary study of
love experiences. Their work showed that higher perceived positivity
resonance was correlated with increased ourishing, fewer depression
symptoms, decreased loneliness, and fewer illness symptoms.
The research presented below takes a similar direction but uses the
method of ecological momentary assessment to gather ne-grained data
about everyday-life experiences of positivity resonance. We focus on
each individual's subjective perception of the impact of these experi-
ences by measuring what we call felt love. This measure is intended to
be broad enough to incorporate both romantic and non-romantic ex-
periences love.In this paper, we show that characteristics derived from
frequent self-report ratings of felt love are predictive of people's psy-
chological well-being and exhibit systematic associations with person-
ality characteristics.
1.3. Momentary and individual-specic aspects of daily love experiences
Since loving connections occur in everyday life context, an ecolo-
gically valid way to assess individualsfeelings of love is critical in
planning for meaningful ways to intervene and prevent subjective
feelings of loneliness and social isolation. Ecological momentary as-
sessment is an established research design to assess human emotions as
individuals they go about with their day-to-day lives. EMA and related
methods such as experience sampling (Csikszentmihályi &
Larson, 1987) have been shown to yield improved ecological validity
and reduce the potential for reporting bias in capturing psychological
processes compared to other methods (Shiman, Stone, & Huord,
2008).
We conducted two experience sampling studies to gather empirical
evidence on the impacts of loving feelings in everyday life. Over the
course of two weeks, participants reported several times a day about
their experienced level of love by responding to the question: How
much do you feel loved right now?From this rich within-person (intra-
individual) data, three important individual-specic characteristics of
daily love experience were derived: person-specic baseline levels,
uctuations or intra-individual (within-person) variability around the
baseline, and the inertia of love experiences. We also looked for asso-
ciations between inter-individual (between-person) dierences in these
intra-individual characteristics and established measures related to
psychological well-being and personality.
We propose that everyday life experiences of love can provide useful
insights into people's psychological well-being (captured by measuring
emotional well-being and ourishing). We hypothesized that people
with higher psychological well-being experience more love in their
everyday lives, with less variability in the degree to which they feel
loved over the course of the day. Having positive social relationships
has been shown to be an integral part of psychological well-being
(Diener & Seligman, 2009;Reis & Gable, 2003;Reis, Collins, &
Berscheid, 2000), and experiencing positive emotions such as love has
been linked to stronger immune systems (Barak, 2006;Pressman &
Cohen, 2005), and higher life expectancies (Holt-Lunstad et al., 2010).
We also explored the secondary hypothesis that gratitude is related
to daily love experiences as literature suggests that everyday acts of
gratitude contribute to positive social exchanges
(McCullough, Kimeldorf, & Cohen, 2008) and play an important role in
close relationships (Kubacka, Finkenauer, Rusbult, & Keijsers, 2011),
potentially aecting daily love experiences. Furthermore,
McCullough, Emmons, and Tsang (2002) found that individuals who
habitually report experiencing gratitude engage in prosocial behaviors
more frequently than those who do not.
Finally, correlates of everyday-life love experiences with the Big
Five personality traits were also analyzed. Individual dierences in
components of love have been connected to Big Five personality di-
mensions. For example, both Agreeableness and Conscientiousness have
been found to be positively associated with intimacy and commitment
(Ahmetoglu, Swami, & Chamorro-Premuzic, 2010;Engel, Olson, &
Patrick, 2002). Furthermore, Agreeableness along with Conscientious-
ness and Extraversion predicted various relationship aspects such as
Z. Oravecz, et al. 
conicts with peers, number of peer relationships, and falling in love
(Asendorpf & Wilpers, 1998). Additionally, Heshmati et al. (2019)
linked Neuroticism to cultural knowledge about situations in which
people might feel loved, and Openness to optimistically assuming love
when one is in doubt. However, to our knowledge no study has ad-
dressed the connection of daily love experiences and personality traits -
the current work is targeted at lling this gap.
2. Methods
2.1. Study settings
2.1.1. Community sample
The rst EMA study (Study 1), focusing on daily emotional and well-
being experiences, was conducted at a northeastern university. The
sample consisted of 52 individuals, and its demographics are displayed
in Table 1. The sample was collected during summer time and mainly
consisted of university staand international students. Because of the
relatively diverse age range, we refer to this sample as the community
sample. Participants were asked to complete a battery of personality
tests and demographics items during each of two laboratory sessions,
and to complete short web-based surveys via their own smartphones,
six times daily over the four weeks of the study. In the introductory
session, the participants provided informed consent and their respective
phone numbers were registered with the text messaging service Sur-
veySignal (SurveySignal, LLC, 2015). Text messages to complete the
web-surveys were sent to the participants' smartphones six times a day.
Survey timing was determined by dividing participants' self-reported
usual waking hours into six equal-length intervals, and survey prompts
were delivered at a random timing within each time interval, con-
strained so that no two prompts were less than 30 min apart. This
sampling scheme was chosen to reduce expectation biases in reporting
while providing a representative sampling of the individuals' context.
Web-based surveys were designed in and delivered by the Qualtrics
survey system (Qualtrics, Provo, UT, 2017). Over the course of the four
weeks, participants received and responded to up to 168 text-message
prompted web-based surveys. Each survey contained approximately
1012 questions (including items related to the level of accomplish-
ment, engagement etc.), but only data coming from the felt love in-
tensity question is analyzed here. Compliance was high, with partici-
pants completing an average of 157 (SD = 15) of the surveys.
Participants were paid proportional to their response rate, with a
maximum payment of $200.
2.1.2. Undergraduate sample
In the second study, the sample consisted of 160 undergraduate
students, recruited at the same location; we refer to these participants
as the undergraduate sample (see demographics in Table 1). They were
enrolled in an eight-week long study examining the eects of mobile
interventions on well-being, results of which are not discussed here.
The intervention was delivered at the beginning of week three, and the
data examined in the present study comprise only the rst two weeks
before any intervention occurred. With these rst two weeks of data
collection, we aimed to run a replication of Study 1. Self-report surveys
were delivered six times daily following the same protocol as Study 1,
and participants also completed a battery of personality assessments
and provided demographic information during biweekly lab sessions.
Compliance was high in these rst two weeks of the study, with par-
ticipants completing an average of 75 (SD = 6) of the 84 surveys.
Participants were proportionally compensated, contingent on response
rate, with a maximum payment of $65 for this portion of the study. The
complete eight-week long study was registered on the Open Science
Framework (OSF) website
1
. All participant interactions for both studies
were overseen by the University's Institutional Review Board.
2.2. Materials
Materials used in the analyses, namely the ecological momentary
assessment of felt love item and the battery of personality assessments,
were consistent across the two studies and the exact forms are available
via the above referenced OSF link (see Undergraduate Sample section).
Data coming from the ecological momentary assessment of felt love are
analyzed in detail later, therefore summary statistics are not provided
here. For the rest of the indicators, means, standard deviations and
reliability measures (Cronbach's alpha; Cronbach, 1951) are reported
below.
2.2.1. Ecological momentary assessment of felt love
The primary modeled outcomes are participant ratings regarding
the degree to which they felt loved at a given moment, as measured by
the question How much do you feel loved right now?. Participants
responded using a visual digital sliding scale, with the two extremes
labeled as Not at alland Extremely.Location on the visual slider
was mapped to integers between 0 and 100 respectively.
2.2.2. Flourishing
One measure used to capture psychological well-being was the
Flourishing Scale (Diener et al., 2010). This brief, eight-item scale
measures an individual's perception of success in several domains, in-
cluding relationships, self-esteem, purpose, and optimism. The nal
ourishing score ranges between 1 and 7, with higher values indicating
better psychological well-being (Study1: M= 5.68, SD = 1.11,
alpha = 0.93; Study2: M= 5.86, SD = 0.75, alpha = 0.88).
2.2.3. Emotional well-being
The other measure used to capture psychological well-being was the
emotional well-being subscale of the 36-Item Short Form Health Survey
(SF-36; Ware & Sherourne, 1992). The emotional well-being sub-scale
ranged from 0 to 100, with higher values indicating better well-being
(Study1: M= 71.79, SD = 18.59, alpha = 0.83; Study 2: M= 70.05,
SD = 17.44, alpha = 0.80).
2.2.4. Gratitude
Trait-level gratitude was assessed using the Gratitude Questionnaire
(McCullough et al., 2002). This six-item self-report questionnaire is
designed to assess individual dierences in the disposition for experi-
encing gratitude in daily life, scores ranging between 1 and 7 (Study1:
M= 3.04, SD = 1.23, alpha = 0.73; Study2: M= 6.15, SD = 0.79,
alpha = 0.84).
Table 1
Demographic characteristics of the two samples.
Demographics Community sample Undergraduate sample
Gender
% Male 33 32
% Female 67 68
Race
% White 80 78
% Asian 10 10
% Black 4 6
% Hispanic 6 4
% Other 0 1
Age
% Minors (< 18) 0 0
% Young adults (18 22) 21 100
% Adults (23 65) 79 0
% Seniors (> 65) 0 0
1
https://osf.io/rxd2p/?view_only=552dfa691bfa40e2a34b961f3e0ad098.
Z. Oravecz, et al. 
2.2.5. Personality
Personality was captured via the Big Five model of personality,
using the Big Five Inventory2 (BFI-2) scale (Soto & John, 2017). The
BFI-2 assesses personality in terms of ve domains (scores range be-
tween 1 and 5): extraversion (Study1: M= 3.43, SD = 0.62,
alpha = 0.83; Study2: M= 3.69, SD = 0.74, alpha = 0.87), agree-
ableness (Study1: M= 3.91, SD = 0.61, alpha = 0.85; Study2:
M= 3.84, SD = 0.56, alpha = 0.78), conscientiousness (Study1:
M= 4.03, SD= 0.59, alpha = 0.84; Study2: M= 3.70, SD = 0.66,
alpha = 0.85), negative emotionality (Study1: M= 2.72, SD = 0.84,
alpha = 0.90; Study2: M= 2.65, SD = 0.89, alpha = 0.91), and open-
mindedness (Study1: M= 3.84, SD = 0.66, alpha = 0.85; Study2:
M= 3.78, SD = 0.67, alpha = 0.84).
2.3. Data analysis
We illustrate the modeling framework for capturing individual dif-
ferences in the self-reports of how loved people feel by considering four
weeks of data from four participants in Study 1, as shown in Fig. 1.
Ratings are conveyed on a 0100 scale, with endpoints ranging from
Not at all to Extremely. Individual dierences are visually apparent, and
we propose four substantively motivated characteristics to describe
them. First, Participant 1 (top left) diers from Participant 2 (top right)
because the majority of the self-reports is centered on dierent areas of
the graph. More specically, on average, Participant 1s felt love level is
in the middle between the Not at all and Extremely endpoints, uctu-
ating around score 54, while Participant 2s indicates more intense love
experiences, uctuating around score 86. These are dierences in the
baseline levels. A second important characteristic is the degree of uc-
tuation of self-reported levels of felt love. This characteristic is often
referred to as intra-individual (or within-person) variability and quantied
as a variance or standard deviation parameter; individual dierences in
this are very pronounced when comparing Participant 1 (top left) and
Participant 3s data (bottom left). Third, even for a given level of intra-
individual variance, some people return quickly to their baseline levels,
while others linger longer in a given felt love state. This is a dierence
in the inertia of love experiences and is noticeable when comparing
Participant 1 (top left) and Participant 4s (lower right) data. These
dierences play out on the hour-to-hour time-scale: we can see that
Participant 1 returns quickly to their baseline, showing a peaked pat-
tern in the data graph, while for Participant 4 the changes in the felt
love levels are slower, showing a smoother pattern. In other words,
Participant 4 lingers longer in a given love state, with love intensities
changing more slowly than for Participant 1. These three characteristics
have been argued to suciently describe individual dierences in af-
fective experiences in everyday life (Kuppens, Oravecz, & Tuerlinckx,
2010) and have been found to be related to personality type (see, e.g.,
Oravecz, Tuerlinckx, & Vandekerckhove, 2011), aect dynamics in
psychiatric disorders (see, e.g., Westerman et al., 2017) and age-related
dierences in aect dynamics (Wood et al., 2017).
While less visible than the above three characteristics, we can also
observe an upward shift in baseline love levels in Participant 2 and 3s
data over the course of the study. This phenomenon might be due to
reactivity to the experience sampling (e.g., Conner and Reid, 2012;
Barta, Tennen, & Litt, 2012), which suggests that simply asking people
about their daily experiences raises their awareness, which in turn
might inuence the measured experience. If this is true in the context of
feeling loved, then the experience sampling design itself may work as
an intervention and elicit change in felt love levels. To account for this
possibility, we also compute and analyze the change in the baseline (or
baseline drift) as an additional person-specic characteristic. We note
that baseline drift and inertia (described in the previous paragraph)
capture two conceptually dierent phenomena: inertia captures the
hour-to-hour perseveration of events of felt love, while baseline drift
captures the aggregated eects of those events across days. We can
think of inertia as an indicator of fast time-scale change in felt love and
Fig. 1. Self-reported levels of love from 4 participants over the course of Study 1. Participants reported their momentary levels of felt love on a scale from 0 (Not at
all) to 100 (Extremely). Individual dierences in terms of baselines, change in baseline, variance around baselines and inertia (see explanation in text) are apparent.
Z. Oravecz, et al. 
baseline drift over the course of the study as a slow time-scale indicator.
A multilevel latent stochastic dierential equation model (MLSDEM,
based on the Ornstein-Uhlenbeck process model; see, e.g.,
Oravecz, Tuerlinckx, & Vandekerckhove, 2016) was used to t the self-
reported data on felt love. This approach captures the individual dif-
ferences described above, modeling dierences in the baseline, intra-
individual variability, inertia, and change in baseline over the course of
the study, and is robust to the intricate characteristics of the data that
stem from the EMA design. Specically, in order to reduce eects due to
the anticipation of the prompt, our prompts are unequally spaced
throughout the day, and each participant follows a dierent (rando-
mized) data collection schedule. This violates the assumptions of many
traditional modeling approaches, such as repeated measures analysis of
variance or autoregressive models. Self-reports about experienced felt
love might also be perturbed with measurement error, which needs to
be separated from meaningful within-person variance (intra-individual
variability measure). This can be done by estimating the four described
individual specic characteristics as latent model parameters (dyna-
mical process) and allowing it to be perturbed by error variance
(measurement part). Third, self-reports are naturally nested within
participants, which calls for a multilevel model (Raudenbush &
Bryk, 2002) that jointly analyzes dierences at the within and between
person levels. MLSDEM conforms to all these requirements. Given at
least 50 observations per person, MLSDEMs can reliably estimate the
substantively meaningful characteristics of the individual
(Oravecz et al., 2011). Our data meet this criterion; the average number
of responses was 157 (SD = 15) in the community sample and 75
(SD = 6) in the undergraduate sample.
3. Results
The MLSDEM was tted in the Bayesian framework (Gelman et al.,
2013), which allows for intuitive interpretation of the results by pro-
viding posterior probability distributions on the quantities of interest.
In the Bayesian framework, prior probability distributions on the model
parameters need to be set, which were chosen to be non-informative in
the current analysis. Data analysis was carried out in JAGS
(Plummer, 2003) and R (R Core Team, 2017), with rjags
(Plummer, 2016) to interface R with JAGS. Correlations between the
three person-specic love characteristics and the trait level measures
were calculated in JASP (version 0.7.5; JASP Team, 2016). Code, data
and full results are available on the OSF page
2
for the project.
3.1. Overview of the group-level trends and individual dierences in
everyday life experiences of love across the two experience sampling studies
We dene four key individual dierences characteristics, shown in
Table 2, which summarize the group-level trends in every day felt love.
Point estimates of the corresponding model parameters and their 95%
Credible Intervals (CIs) are displayed. In the Bayesian framework, the
Credible Interval plays a similar role to the Condence Interval in the
classical framework. By denition, the probability that the true para-
meter falls within the 95% Credible Interval is 95%. We remind the
reader that the scale for love experiences was between 0 and 100.
As seen in Table 2, the mean level of felt love across people is above
the midpoint of the scale in both studies (M= 63.24 and 70.46, cor-
responding 95% CIs exclude the midpoint 50), with the baseline about
7 points higher in the undergraduate sample than in the community
sample. At the same time, there is a considerable span of individual
dierences in these baselines, measured by the group-level standard
deviation estimate (M= 15.07 and 15.49), indicating that some people
do have baseline felt love levels that fall below the midpoint in both
studies.
The baseline level of loving feelings can change during the course of
the study. Statistically, this was captured by allowing the baseline to
vary as a linear
3
function of how long a participant has been in the
study; this is a slow time-scale indicator of change in felt love, referred
to as baseline drift. The posterior mean estimate for the group-level
baseline drift was positive and credibly dierent from 0 (95% Credible
Interval excludes 0) in both samples (Community M= 0.14; Under-
graduate M= 0.16), implying an overall gradual increase in felt love
over the course of the study. This value represents a 3.92-point (28
days*0.14) increase in love baseline during the course of the study for
the community sample, and a 2.24-point increase for the undergraduate
sample (14×0.16 = 2.24); a relatively small eect size in each case.
Individual dierences in the baseline changes were remarkable, with a
standard deviation of 0.21 across the community sample and more than
double this value in the college sample (0.51). These results indicate
that the overall linear increase across the study in experienced love was
not apparent for every participant; for example for some participants it
was much larger (maximum was around 0.60 in Study 1, and around
1.47 in Study 2, corresponding to an overall increase of 16.80 and
20.58 points in felt love levels, respectively).
In both samples, participants also showed a considerable amount of
intra-individual variability in their experience of felt love across days.
The intra-individual variability measure captures how much a given
person's felt love intensity uctuates around the baseline from day to
day and is displayed in Table 2. The group-level intra-individual
variability estimate quanties how much on average felt love levels
uctuate within-person: the community sample showed an average
within-person standard deviation of 14.33 points. Considering the
0100 scale and the 63.24 average baseline, this value indicates that on
average people experienced considerable ups and downs in how much
they felt loved. The average within-person felt love uctuations is only
slightly smaller for the college-age participants (M
SD
= 11.89). How-
ever, when compared to this group's 70.46 baseline, it was much less
likely for the undergraduates to fall below the midpoint of the scale,
indicative of more intense love experiences overall.
Finally, the inertia for momentary love states was computed to
capture the fast-time-scale changes in felt love. Inertia was assessed by
analyzing how quickly felt-love states return to the baseline after being
moved away (higher values in Table 2 correspond to less inertia).
Looking at the average inertia estimates in Table 2, the community
Table 2
Group level results on love experiences from two community and under-
graduate samples. Regulation is reported on a log-scale.
Characteristic Sample Point
estimate
95% Credible Interval
Average baseline Community 63.24 58.84 67.41
Undergraduate 70.46 68.00 72.93
Individual dierences in
baselines (SD)
Community 15.07 12.51 18.26
Undergraduate 15.49 13.87 17.31
Baseline drift per day Community 0.14 0.08 0.21
Undergraduate 0.16 0.07 0.26
Individual dierences in
average change in
baseline per day (SD)
Community 0.21 0.15 0.29
Undergraduate 0.51 0.43 0.60
Average intra-individual
variability (SD)
Community 14.33 12.21 17.30
Undergraduate 11.89 11.05 12.86
Individual dierences in
intra-individual
variability (SD)
Community 8.22 5.84 12.36
Undergraduate 5.59 4.73 6.67
Average inertia Community 0.71 0.36 1.82
Undergraduate 1.53 0.80 1.94
Individual dierences in
inertia (SD)
Community 2.12 1.40 3.01
Undergraduate 1.97 1.57 2.34
2
https://osf.io/rxd2p/?view_only=552dfa691bfa40e2a34b961f3e0ad098.
3
A more complex model allowing for quadratic curves was also tted to the
data but no credible quadratic trends were found.
Z. Oravecz, et al. 
sample participants, on average, exhibited somewhat more inertia, that
is a slower change in love states than college sample participants
(M= 0.71 and 1.53 respectively), with comparable amounts of in-
dividual dierences (M= 2.12 and 1.97). The scale of these estimates is
somewhat dicult to interpret directly: it is measured on a log scale
and its inuence plays out over time. One way to capture what these
numbers represent is to think of them as inducing correlation across
time. A participant in the community sample, with comparatively
slower changes in felt love time, would show lingering eects of past
felt love states after half an hour with a correlation of 0.36 (mid-sized
correlation by Cohen's criteria). By contrast, a participant in the college
sample no longer shows those eects a half hour laterthe correlation
with their state a half hour before is around 0.01. After 3 h this cor-
relation is negligible (less than 0.01) for both groups, implying that a
participant's current love state is not predictable from more distal past
experiences.
3.2. Do everyday life love experiences systematically relate to psychological
well-being and personality?
We hypothesized that individual dierences in daily life experiences
of loving feelings are systematically related to psychological well-being.
To study these associations, correlations (Pearson's r) were calculated
between indicators psychological well-being, such as ourishing and
emotional well-being; and daily felt love experience characteristics,
namely baseline levels, intra-individual variability, and inertia. We also
explored how these felt love characteristics relate to gratitude and
personality characteristics.
We quantied the evidence for these associations using Bayes
Factors (BF; Ly, Verhagen, & Wagenmakers, 2015). The Bayes Factor is
a statistical tool for hypothesis testing in the Bayesian statistical fra-
mework. With the BF, evidence in favor of no correlation (null hy-
pothesis), is quantied relative to evidence in favor of correlation (al-
ternative hypothesis). BF is computed on a continuous scale, expressing
the ratio of evidence between the alternative and the null hypothesis
(or vice versa, by taking the reciprocal). In order to summarize BFs in
terms of discrete categories for interpretation of evidence strength, a
classication scheme was proposed by Jereys (1961). According to
this classication, a BF in favor of a given hypothesis
4
whose value is
below 3 shows anecdotal or no evidence for the alternative, BF between
3 and 10 shows moderate evidence, BF between 10 and 30 shows strong
evidence, BF greater than 30 shows very strong evidence and BF greater
than 100 shows extreme evidence.
In both samples, psychological well-being measures and gratitude
had meaningful medium-size correlations with baseline levels of loving
feelings in everyday life (rs between 0.43 and 0.64), as shown in
Table 3. The corresponding BFs indicated at least strong evidence for
these correlations. Between the two samples, the undergraduate sample
had somewhat larger eect sizes (r-s between 0.5 and 0.6) and stronger
evidence (extreme) in favor of these eects. Intra-individual varia-
bility (IIV) in felt love yielded small negative correlations with psy-
chological well-being measures and gratitude, but the evidence sup-
porting these eects was weak (BFs are all below 3). Inertia showed a
small-size positive correlation with emotional well-being in the com-
munity sample, suggesting that participants with higher in emotional
well-being experienced a slower change in their love states, but the BF
indicated only moderate evidence in favor of this correlation, and this
eect was not replicated in the college sample. There was no evidence
for any other associations with respect to inertia and the well-being
measures.
Table 4 shows associations between felt love characteristics and
personality traits. In both samples, results indicated that more extra-
verted participants tended to have higher baseline levels of everyday-
life felt love experiences (r= 0.35, BF = 4.13 and r= 0.30,
BF = 146.60 in the community and undergraduate samples, respec-
tively). In contrast, participants who scored higher on the neuroticism
scale had lower baselines (r=0.42, BF = 16.32 and r=0.33,
BF = 829.52). In the community sample, we also found evidence that
neurotic individuals had more variability around this baseline
(r= 0.42, BF = 18.94). While there was a small positive correlation
suggesting the same tendency in the undergraduate sample, the evi-
dence for this eect was anecdotal. However, in the undergraduate
sample neuroticism was weakly associated with higher inertia
(r= 0.21, BF = 3.34), with moderate evidence for this eect. Finally,
undergraduate participants with Agreeable personalities tended to have
higher baselines (r= 0.35, BF = 2656.39), indicating more intense love
experiences for these participants. Correlations in the same directions
were found in the community sample as well, but they were smaller,
without sucient evidence to consider them credible.
4. Discussion
The two studies presented here expand upon existing research on
love by assessing how loved individuals felt throughout their day and
examining correlations with other psychological variables including
personality and psychological well-being. In both studies, ecological
momentary assessment via smartphone at random times throughout the
day was able to capture considerable individual dierences in felt love
experiences. We used a one item measure of loving feelings, adminis-
tered six times daily for several weeks to make sure we sample a variety
of everyday life experiences. Item conciseness is a useful characteristic
in EMA settings as it avoids unnecessary burden on participants, re-
sulting in better compliance and longer periods of measurement.
Individual dierences based on this concise love measure showed sys-
tematic relationships with other well-studied psychological variables,
suggesting that the ecological momentary assessment design is a viable
and useful method of learning about people's everyday life felt love
experiences.
We modeled self-reported levels of felt love in terms of a person-
specic baseline level, intra-individual variability around that baseline,
slow-timescale change in the baseline and fast-timescale inertia. The
results from these models showed a fairly similar picture across the two
studies, one with a broad age range (between 19 and 48) and another
consisting of undergraduate students (between 18 and 22). While most
of the results were consistent, the college-aged sample did exhibit a
slightly higher baseline with less intra-individual variability around it,
Table 3
Results on the associations between psychological well-being and everyday life
love experiences, in terms of Pearson's r correlations and corresponding Bayes
Factors quantifying the ratio of evidence in favor of the correlation. IIV stands
for intra-individual variability.
Predictor Sample Statistic Baseline IIV Inertia
Flourishing Community r0.55 0.26 0.20
BF 1031.54 0.94 0.45
Undergraduate r0.64 0.17 0.06
BF 1.20 ×10
17
0.93 0.13
Gratitude Community r0.53 0.28 0.31
BF 489.53 1.15 2.04
Undergraduate r0.55 0.01 0.02
BF 1.15 ×10
11
0.21 0.10
Emotional Well-
being
Community r0.43 0.22 0.33
BF 24.89 0.56 3.00
Undergraduate r0.51 0.19 0.06
BF 2.26 ×10
9
1.90 1.13
4
BF in favor of the alternative hypothesis is conventionally denoted BF
10
,
although in this paper we denote it simply as BF. This BF (BF
10
) can be con-
verted into BF
01
, which quanties evidence in favor of the null, by taking the
inverse: BF10 = 1/BF01, with interpretations identical, mutatis mutandis. We
do not refer to BF
01
s anywhere in this paper.
Z. Oravecz, et al. 
suggesting more consistently intense love experiences overall in that
age group.
Characteristics of everyday-life felt love, parameterized in terms of
baseline levels, variability, and inertia, showed credible relationships
with indicators of psychological well-being, such as ourishing and
emotional well-being, as well as with gratitude and personality traits.
Psychological well-being measures showed credible medium-sized
correlations with baseline levels of loving feelings in everyday life, with
strong evidence supporting these correlations, and this observation
replicated across the two studies: people who feel loved seemed to feel
happier as well. Similarly, we found evidence that higher trait gratitude
is related to higher daily love experiences, most likely via reciprocal
mechanisms as in close relationships (Kubacka et al., 2011). While
these relationships were evident in both samples, although we found
larger eect sizes and more evidence in favor of the eects in the col-
lege sample.
We also hypothesized that lower levels of intra-individual varia-
bility in felt love would be associated with higher levels of psycholo-
gical well-being. While we did nd small negative correlations between
levels of IIV and the psychological well-being measures and gratitude,
suggesting that low variability may have positive eects, the limited
evidence supporting these eects leaves us unable to draw any rm
conclusions about this relationship. We also explored the relationships
between psychological well-being and inertia, without any hypotheses
set on these beforehand. The community sample showed a medium-
sized positive correlation between inertia and emotional well-being,
suggesting that longer-lasting states of felt love are associated with
better emotional well-being; however, the evidence supporting this
barely reached a moderate level and the eect did not replicate in the
undergraduate sample. We conclude that further work is necessary to
better understand the relationship between psychological well-being
and felt love IIV and inertia measures.
Importantly, both of our studies found credible evidence that feeling
loved in daily life is associated with ourishing and emotional well-
being. These ndings highlight the importance of studying felt love in
its natural context and emphasize that for psychological well-being, it is
not only romantic relationships that matter, but also other forms of
positivity resonance people experience in their everyday life. While our
design does not permit causal inference, we theorize that these char-
acteristics demonstrate a mutual, cyclical inuence upon one another.
If so, nonromantic felt love may provide an avenue for intervention to
improve well-being if applied appropriately. Interestingly, we found
evidence of a small but credible drift in participantsbaselines of felt
love over the duration of the study. While small enough to be unlikely
to bias results over the course of a one- to two-month study, the eect
implies that raising awareness of felt love in day-to-day life may itself
be an intervention that raises levels of felt love over a longer period of
time. This suggests there may be merit in developing interventions to
improve well-being that are based in raising momentary awareness of
felt love using smartphone-based platforms.
Finally, we also explored associations between characteristics of
daily-life felt love and personality traits. Across both samples, higher
Extroversion individuals tended to have higher baseline levels of every
day felt love, while individuals with higher levels of Neuroticism de-
monstrated opposite patterns. These consistent ndings help us further
highlight the importance of everyday-life felt love construct and can be
used to facilitate the development of individualized intervention stra-
tegies.
5. Limitations and future directions
The study samples consisted of individuals demonstrating relatively
adaptive psychological well-being. Future studies may wish to explore
subgroups of individuals who may be in greater need of assessment of
day-to-day feelings in order to drive future treatment or intervention.
For example, it would be interesting to compare individuals with
mental health symptoms or diagnoses such as anxiety or depression to
the general population in order to determine if there are dierences in
their daily experiences felt love. These potential dierences could assist
in treatment and intervention to improve the health and well-being of
these subgroups of individuals. Additionally, these studies consisted of
adult participants only; future research may wish to explore the impact
of every day felt love in both younger and older populations. Examining
child and adolescent populations as well as aging adults could yield
meaningful information for both groups.
As mentioned above, one important limitation of the study is that
our design does not permit direct causal inference, and all associations
are correlational. Therefore more research is needed to determine for
example the causal direction and mechanism of the link between per-
sonality and felt love. Although our results show that extroverted,
outgoing people are more likely to report feeling loved than individuals
who are more anxious and neurotic, two possibilities exist. It is possible
that people who demonstrate higher neuroticism or anxiety are less
likely to interpret a given event as an expression of love, an eect that
training may be able to mitigate, and which may have nontrivial im-
pacts on relationship satisfaction and well-being. It seems likely that the
opposite eect may be true for people who exhibit high extraversion,
and indeed they may be incentivized to engage in social interaction
because they are more likely to view interpersonal acts as acts of love.
This heterogeneity across personality scores is underscored by the
large amounts of overall interpersonal variability in individual char-
acteristics that dene daily felt love. The dierences between age
groups further underscore this variability, suggesting that like the
weightings of feelings in Gable and Poore's (2008) study of relation-
ships, the role and inuence of felt love may vary both between in-
dividuals and across the lifespan.
Future research may also wish to explore the impact of gratitude
interventions on felt love. Gratitude, here dened as a virtue or as an
emotional state that involves an interpersonal connection between both
the benefactor and beneciary (Emmons, 2007), has a close relation-
ship with Fredrickson's (2016) conceptualization of love. Gratitude in-
terventions may increase or improve how much love people feel as they
go about their day-to-day lives. Liao and Weng (2018) found that
gratefulness impacts subjective well-being, mediated by social con-
nectedness and expanding meaning in life. Froh et al. (2009) found that
gratitude interventions were moderated by positive aect, with low-
positive-aect individuals showing more benet from these
Table 4
Results of the associations between personality traits and everyday life love
experiences, in terms of Pearson's r correlations and corresponding Bayes
Factors quantifying the ratio of evidence in favor of the correlation. IIV stands
for intra-individual variability.
Predictor Sample Statistic Baseline IIV Inertia
Extraversion Community r0.35 0.23 0.11
BF 4.13 0.61 0.23
Undergraduate r0.30 0.07 0.12
BF 146.60 0.15 0.28
Agreeableness Community r0.29 0.02 0.24
BF 1.32 0.17 0.73
Undergraduate r0.35 0.18 0.03
BF 2656.39 0.70 0.11
Conscientiousness Community r0.11 0.05 0.26
BF 0.23 0.18 0.99
Undergraduate r0.20 0.11 0.00
BF 2.49 0.24 0.01
Neuroticism Community r0.42 0.42 0.09
BF 16.32 18.94 0.21
Undergraduate r0.33 0.19 0.21
BF 829.52 1.62 3.34
Openness Community r0.05 0.17 0.33
BF 0.19 0.35 2.60
Undergraduate r0.09 0.03 0.05
BF 0.20 0.10 0.12
Z. Oravecz, et al. 
interactions. Our ndings imply that felt love may provide additional
pathways to both positive aect and overall well-being, and may,
therefore, be a valuable construct in the implementation of gratitude-
based interventions. More work is required to determine what role, if
any, felt love plays in these relationships.
We predict that many individuals will respond to or benet from a
simple intervention that makes them aware of everyday feelings of love
in their lives. In the current study, higher baseline experience of daily
felt love was linked to higher well-being, indicating that interventions
to increase well-being in adults can be oriented towards increasing
awareness of feelings of love. One example includes Loving Kindness
Meditation (Hutcherson, Seppala, & Gross, 2008), a meditation inter-
vention focused on increasing social connectedness. Interventions
centered on compassion and care through receiving care and expressing
care for others, might also be eective ways to increase felt love and
consequently increasing well-being (Dvořáková, Greenberg, &
Roeser, 2019).
However, the intricate interpersonal dierences in our data imply
that some individuals may require additional or dierent support in
order to achieve and maintain psychological well-being. Interventions
must, therefore, be tailored to their specic needs. We suggest that
additional work on the sources and correlates of between-person
variability in felt love may be benecial to researchers hoping to deploy
interventions using felt love.
6. Conclusions
We presented results from two experience sampling studies on felt
love in everyday life. Our results suggest that overall levels of felt love
have close relationships with psychological well-being, and may be
predictive of personality traits. These results are generally consistent
across studies, although some eects are stronger or weaker in the
college-student sample than in the broader community sample, sug-
gesting that the role of felt love may vary across the lifespan. We also
found substantial between-person variability in felt love. We suggest
that the construct of felt love may be useful, and recommend additional
research geared towards the development and customization of in-
dividualized felt-love interventions to improve well-being at all stages
of life.
References
Ahmetoglu, G., Swami, V., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2010). The relationship between
dimensions of love, personality, and relationship length. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
39, 11811190.
Aron, A., Melinat, E., Aron, E. N., Vallone, R. D., & Bator, R. J. (1997). The experimental
generation of interpersonal closeness: A procedure and some preliminary ndings.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 363377.
Asendorpf, J. B., & Wilpers, S. (1998). Personality eects on social relationships. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 15311544. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0022-3514.74.6.1531.
Barak, Y. (2006). The immune system and happiness. International Journal of
Epidemiology, 36(6), 1244.
Barta, W. D., Tennen, H., & Litt, M. D. (2012). Measurement reactivity in diary research.
In M. R. Mehl, & T. S. Conner (Eds.). Handbook of research methods for studying daily
life (pp. 108123). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
Baumeister, R. F., & Finkel, E. J. (2010). Advanced social psychology, the state of the science.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3),
497529.
Berscheid, E. (2006). Searching for the meaning of love. In R. Sternberg, & K. Weis
(Eds.). The New Psychology of Love (pp. 171183). New York: Yale University Press.
Buss, D. M. (1988). Love acts: The evolutionary biology of love. In R. J. Sternberg, & M. L.
Barnes (Eds.). The psychology of love(pp. 100118). New Haven, CT, US: Yale
University Press.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Hawkley, L. C. (2009). Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 13(10), 447454. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.005.
Conner, T. S., & Reid, K. A. (2012). Eects of intensive mobile happiness reporting in
daily life. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 315323.
Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coecient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika,
16, 297334.
Csikszentmihályi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the experience-
sampling method. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175(9), 526536.
Diener, E., & Seligman, M. P. (2009). Beyond money: Toward an economy of well-being.
In E. Diener (Ed.). The science of well-being: The collected works of Ed Diener (pp. 201
265). New Word: Springer Science+Business Media.
Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D. W., Oishi, S., & Biswas-Diener, R.
(2010). New well-being measures: Short scales to assess ourishing and positive and
negative feelings. Social Indicators Research, 97(2), 143156.
Dvořáková, K., Greenberg, M. T., & Roeser, R. W. (2019). On the role of mindfulness and
compassion skills in students' coping, well-being, and development across the tran-
sition to college: A conceptual analysis. Stress and Health, 35(2), 146156. https://
doi.org/10.1002/smi.2850.
Engel, G., Olson, K. R., & Patrick, C. (2002). The personality of love: Fundamental motives
and traits related to components of love. Personality and Individual Dierences, 32,
839853.
Emmons, R. A. (2007). Thanks! How the new science of gratitude can make you happier.
Boston: Houghton Miin Company.
Fredrickson, B. L. (2016). Love: Positivity resonance as a fresh, evidence-based perspec-
tive on an age-old topic. In L. F. Barrett, L. M., & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.).
Handbook of emotions (pp. 847858). Guilford Publications.
Froh, J. J., Kashdan, T., Ozimkowski, K., & Miller, N. (2009). Who benets the most from
a gratitude intervention in children and adolescents? Examining positive aect as a
moderator. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 19, 408422.
Gable, S. L., & Poore, J. (2008). Which thoughts count? algorithms for evaluating sa-
tisfaction in relationships. Psychological Science, 19, 10301036.
Gelman, A., Carlin, J. B., Stern, H. S., Dunson, D. B., Vehtari, A., & Rubin, D. B. (2013).
Bayesian data analysis (Third edition). Boca Raton (FL): Chapman & Hall/CRC.
Gottman, J. M. (1979). Marital interaction: Experimental investigations. New York:
Academic Press.
Grant, N., Hamer, M., & Steptoe, A. (2009). Social isolation and stress-related cardio-
vascular, lipid, and cortisol responses. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37(1), 2937.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-009-9081-z doi.org/.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment pro-
cess. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(3), 511.
Hendrick, C., & Hendrick, S. (1986). A theory and method of love. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 50, 392402.
Heshmati, S., Oravecz, Z., Sarah Pressman, S., Batchelder, W. H., Muth, C., &
Vandekerckhove, J. (2019). What does it mean to feel loved: Cultural consensus and
individual dierences in felt love. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 36(1),
214243. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407517724600.
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality
risk: A meta-analytic review. PLoS Med, 7(7), E1000316. https://doi.org/10.1371/
journal.pmed.1000316.
Hutcherson, C. A., Seppala, E. M., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Loving-kindness meditation in-
creases social connectedness. Emotion, 8(5), 720.
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press.
Jereys, H. (1961). Theory of Probability, 3rd ed. Oxford Classic Texts in the Physical
Sciences. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kelley, H. H., Berscheid, E., Christensen, A., Harvey, J. H., Huston, T. L., Levinger, G., &
Peterson, D. R. (1983). Close relationships. New York, NY: Freeman.
Kubacka, K. E., Finkenauer, C., Rusbult, C. E., & Keijsers, L. (2011). Maintaining close
relationships: Gratitude as a motivator and a detector of maintenance behavior.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(10), 13621375. https://doi.org/10.
1177/0146167211412196.
Kuppens, P., Oravecz, Z., & Tuerlinckx, F. (2010). Feelings change: Accounting for in-
dividual dierences in the temporal dynamics of aect. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 99, 10421060.
Lee, J. A. (1977). A typology of styles of loving. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
3, 173182.
Lee, J. A. (1988). Love-styles. In R. J. Sternberg, & M. Barnes (Eds.). The psychology of love
(pp. 3867). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Liao, K. Y., & Weng, C. Y. (2018). Gratefulness and subjective well-being: Social con-
nectedness and presence of meaning as mediators. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
65(3), 383393.
Ly, A., Verhagen, J., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2015). Harold Jereys's default bayes factor
hypothesis tests: Explanation, extension, and application in psychology. Journal of
Mathematical Psychology, 72, 1932. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jmp.2015.06.004.
Major, B. C., Le Nguyen, K. D., Lundberg, K. B., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2018). Well-being
correlates of perceived positivity resonance: Evidence from trait and episode-level
assessments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.https://doi.org/10.1177/
0146167218771324.
McCullough, M. E., Emmons, R. A., & Tsang, J. (2002). The grateful disposition: A con-
ceptual and empirical topography. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82,
112127.
McCullough, M. E., Kimeldorf, M. B., & Cohen, A. D. (2008). An adaptation for altruism?
The social causes, social eects, and social evolution of gratitude. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 17, 281284. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.
00590.x.
Oravecz, Z., Muth, C., & Vandekerckhove, J. (2016). Do people agree on what makes one
feel loved? A cognitive psychometric approach to explore consensus on felt love. PLoS
ONE, 11(4)http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0152803.
Oravecz, Z., Tuerlinckx, F., & Vandekerckhove, J. (2011). A hierarchical latent stochastic
dierential equation model for aective dynamics. Psychological Methods, 16,
468490. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024375.
Oravecz, Z., Tuerlinckx, F., & Vandekerckhove, J. (2016). Bayesian data analysis with the
bivariate hierarchical ornstein-uhlenbeck process model. Multivariate Behavioral
Research, 51, 106119.
Z. Oravecz, et al. 
Plummer, M. (2003). JAGS: A program for analysis of bayesian graphical models using
gibbs sampling. Proceedings of the 3rd international workshop on distributed statistical
computing (DSC 2003) (pp. 2022). .
Plummer, M. (2016). rjags: Bayesian graphical models using MCMC [Computer software
manual]. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rjags (R package
version 4-6).
Pressman, S. D., & Cohen, S. (2005). Does positive aect inuence health? Psychological
Bulletin, 131(6), 925971. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.925.
Qualtrics, Provo, UT. (2017). Qualtrics. Retrieved from http://www.qualtrics.com.
R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer
software manual]. Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/.
Reis, H. T., Collins, W. A., & Berscheid, E. (2000). The relationship context of human
behavior and development. Psychological Bulletin, 126(6), 844872.
Reis, H. T., & Gable, S. L. (2003). Toward a positive psychology of relationships. In C. L.
M. Keyes, & J. Haidt (Eds.). Flourishing: Positive psychology and the life well-lived (pp.
129159). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data
analysis methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Shiman, S., Stone, A. A., & Huord, M. R. (2008). Ecological momentary assessment.
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4(1), 132. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.
clinpsy.3.022806.091415.
SurveySignal, LLC. (2015). SurveySignal. Retrieved from http://www.surveysignal.com.
Soto, C. J., & John, O. P. (2017). The next big ve inventory (BFI-2): Developing and
assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, delity, and
predictive power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113, 117143.
Sternberg, R. J. (1986). A triangular theory of love. Psychological Review, 93(2), 119135.
Stone, A. A., & Shiman, S. (1994). Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in beha-
vioral medicine. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 16(3), 199202.
Team, J.A.S.P. (2016). JASP (Version 0.7.5.5)[Computer software].
Ware, J. E., Jr., & Sherbourne, C. D. (1992). The mos 36-Item short-form health survey
(SF-36): I. conceptual framework and item selection. Medical Care, 30(6), 473483.
Westerman, S., Grezellschak, S., Oravecz, Z., Moritz, S., Lüdtke, T., & Jansen, A. (2017).
Untangling the complex relationships between symptoms of schizophrenia and
emotion dynamics in daily life: Findings from an experience sampling pilot study.
Psychiatry Research, 257, 514518.
Wood, J., Oravecz, Z., Vogel, N., Benson, L., Chow, S.-. M., Cole, P., Conroy, D., E., Pincus,
A., L., & Ram, N. (2017). Modeling intraindividual dynamics using stochastic dif-
ferential equations: An examination of age-related dierences in aect regulation.
Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 73(1), 171184.
Young, L. J. (2009). Being human: Love: Neuroscience reveals all. Nature, 457, 148.
https://doi.org/10.1038/457148a 148.
Z. Oravecz, et al. 
... Individuals with high psychological well-being tend to be happier and experience greater life satisfaction, because they are better able to feel love and foster positive relationships in their daily lives (Oravecz et al., 2020). Research shows that parental social support has a significant positive relationship with psychological well-being (Sugiarto & Soetjiningsih, 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Parental social support is understood as emotional, instrumental, esteem, and informational assistance provided by parents to children in the context of interfaith dating relationships. This study aims to analyze the relationship between parental social support and psychological well-being in young adults in interfaith dating relationships in Indonesia. Psychological well-being was measured using Ryff's psychological well-being scale consisting of six main dimensions, namely self-acceptance, positive relationships with others, autonomy, environmental mastery, purpose in life, and personal growth. The research method used was quantitative with a correlational design, with 208 young adult participants in interfaith dating relationships. The results showed a significant positive relationship between parental social support and psychological well-being (r = 0.493, p <0.01). The average parental social support received was 115.16, in the moderate category with a percentage of 56.25% of participants. The average psychological well-being was 84.36, also in the moderate category with a percentage of 57.21%. Parental social support was also positively related to each dimension of psychological well-being. These findings underscore the importance of parental social support in maintaining the psychological well-being of young adults facing challenges in interfaith relationships.
... responding to repeated EMA prompts on well-being might impact someone's well-being. That is, the mere participation in such a study may have an effect on the participants' Meaning reports throughout the intervention -such changes in actions or feelings from repeated measurements throughout a study have been reported in previous studies (Clifford & Davis, 2012;Oravecz et al., 2020;Ottenstein et al., 2024;Runyan et al., 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Elméleti háttér: A szerelem világszerte ismert jelenség, amely hatással van az emberi élet számos aspektusára, különösen a romantikus partner kiválasztására, a vele való kapcsolat kialakítására és fenntartására. A szerelmi élmény tesztelésére szolgáló egyik legnépszerűbb eszköz Sternberg 45 tételes kérdőíve (STLS-45), amely három szerelmi komponenst mér: intimitást, szenvedélyt és elköteleződést. A tudományos konszenzus elérése, az eredmények megbízhatóságának, összehasonlíthatóságának és általánosíthatóságának javítása érdekében Kowal és munkatársai (2022) létrehozták a kérdőív rövid változatát (STLS-15). A kutatás célja: A kutatás célja az STLS-15 magyar változatának elkészítése, pszichometriai ellenőrzése és validálása volt. Módszer: Három online mintát használtunk. Az elsőn (1305 fő, 739 nő, 566 férfi) ellenőriztük a hármas faktorszerkezetet. A másodikon (465 fő, 319 nő, 146 férfi), amely az előző minta egy része volt, a Kapcsolati Elégedettség Kérdőív (RAS-H), a Páros Megküzdés Kérdőív (DCI-H) és a Szexuális Motiváció Kérdőív (YSEX?-HSF) alkalmazásával validáltuk az STLS-15-öt. E két minta az STLS-45 korábbi elemzéséből származik. Az ezekben szereplő 9 pontos válaszskálákat 5 pontossá transzformáltunk. A harmadik mintán (532 fő, 442 nő, 90 férfi) 5 pontos STLS-15-öt használtunk, újra ellenőriztük a faktorszerkezetet. Eredmények: A faktorelemzés megmutatta, hogy az STLS-15 magyar tételei ugyanolyan hármas szerkezetbe illeszkednek, mint a nemzetközi változatban. Az STLS-15 mindhárom skálája pozitív irányú, közepes erősségű korrelációt mutat a DCI-H összesített skáláival, valamint a RAS-H-val, és alacsony vagy értelmezhetetlenül alacsony korrelációt mutat a YSEX?-HSF-val. Következtetések: A Sternberg-féle Szerelem Kérdőív rövid magyar változata (STLS-15) az eredetivel megegyező faktorszerkezetű, magas belső megbízhatósági mutatókkal rendelkező valid eszköz, amely kiválóan alkalmas a magyar nyelvű kutatásokban a szerelem mérésére.
Article
Love is fundamental to human nature, transcending race and nation, with descriptions rooted in diverse cultural backgrounds. Thanks to some past research, the concept of love has evolved from abstract to concrete, making it increasingly “measurable.” Emerging studies focus on applying love, but research on how feeling loved influences green food purchase intention is still nascent. On the foundation of the Broaden-and-Build Theory, a research framework was created. It delves into how the emotion of love affects consumers’ purchase intention for green food. It uncovers the roles of self-esteem and social crowding in this process. The findings reveal that individuals’ increased green food purchase intention when feeling loved is particularly pronounced in less crowded environments and is achieved by boosting their self-esteem. Additionally, the study provides a deeper understanding of the impact of feeling loved on food marketing practices.
Preprint
Full-text available
Feeling and expressing love in daily life are interconnected and perhaps mutually influential experiences. In this study we examined the reciprocal dynamics of feeling and expressing love and its relation to well-being using an ecological momentary assessment design. Over a four-week period, we asked participants ( N = 52; 67% Female; 80% White) to report their levels of feeling loved and expressing love six times a day. Using a continuous-time process model, we explored individual differences in inertia (i.e. persistence of a process over time) and cross-influences of felt and expressed love over time. We found that increases in expressing love led to increased feelings of being loved over time; however, increases in felt love did not lead to increases in expressing love. Notably, participants with greater inertia in their felt love levels reported higher levels of overall flourishing. These results suggest new avenues for psychological well-being interventions which target increasing loving feelings through encouraging more expressions of love.
Article
Bu araştırmanın amacı şeflerin kişilik tiplerinin ve kişilik özelliklerinin iş tatminleri ve psikolojik iyi oluşları üzerindeki etkisini belirlemektir. Bu kapsamda İzmir’de faaliyet gösteren restoranlarda görev yapan şeflerden Kasım 2023 – Ocak 2024 döneminde anket formu aracılığıyla veri toplanmıştır. Anket formunda kişilik tipleri (A tipi ve B tipi), kişilik özellikleri (dışa dönüklük, duygusal denge, yumuşakbaşlılık, nevrotiklik, özdenetimlik), iş tatmini ve psikolojik iyi oluş ölçekleri yer almaktadır. Ayrıca şeflerin medeni durum, yaş, cinsiyet, eğitim düzeyi ve mesleki deneyim bilgilerini belirlemek amacıyla kategorik sorulara ver verilmiştir. Araştırmada ölçeklerin yapısal geçerlilikleri keşfedici faktör analizi değerleri, güvenirlikleri ise Cronbach alfa değerleri esas alınarak belirlenmiştir. Araştırmanın hipotezleri çoklu regresyon analizleri test edilmiştir. Analiz sonuçlarına göre şeflerin iş tatmini A tipi ve B tipi kişilik tiplerinden, dışadönüklük ve özdenetimlik kişilik özelliklerinden olumlu olarak etkilenmektedir. Şeflerin psikolojik iyi oluşları A ve B tipi kişilik tipinden, iş tatmininden ve deneyime açıklık kişilik özelliğinden olumlu olarak, nevrotiklik ve yumuşakbaşlılık kişilik özelliğinden ise olumsuz olarak etkilenmektedir. Ayrıca araştırma bulguları şeflerin iş tatminleri ve psikolojik iyi oluşlarının medeni durum, cinsiyet, yaş, mesleki deneyim ve eğitim düzeyine göre anlamlı farklılık göstermediğini göstermektedir.
Chapter
Full-text available
Scham kann zu einem starken Gefühl werden, wenn es am Arbeitsplatz und in Führungskontexten zu ersetze schamvollen mit herausfordernden Erfahrungen kommt. Frühere Forschungen haben die Auswirkungen von Scham auf Organisationen aufgezeigt und wie sie transformiert werden kann. In diesem Kapitel wird die Liebe in Form einer mitfühlenden, menschlichen Liebe als ein möglicher Weg zur Überwindung und Transformation von Scham erörtert, insbesondere in transkulturellen Arbeitsumgebungen. Das Kapitel präsentiert empirische Ergebnisse einer qualitativen Studie, die nach einem hermeneutischen Forschungsparadigma durchgeführt wurde. Für die Studie wurden strukturierte Interviews mit 22 Führungskräften aus verschiedenen Ländern und mit unterschiedlichem kulturellem Hintergrund geführt, um Scham und Liebe in der Führung, am Arbeitsplatz und in der Organisation zu erforschen. Die Ergebnisse zeigen, dass die (mitfühlende) Liebe von Führungskräften die Scham am Arbeitsplatz auf vier Arten transformiert. Die Studie zeigt außerdem, dass Liebe und Scham in gewisser Weise miteinander verknüpft sind, z. B. dass Liebeserfahrungen Scham verursachen, dass Liebe eine Ressource für die Überwindung von Scham ist und dass Scham und das Selbst integriert sind. Die Studie zeigt weiter, dass Liebesnarrationen eine mächtige Ressource für die Transformation von Scham und negativen Emotionen in Industrie 4.0-bezogenen Kontexten sind. Schließlich zeigen die Ergebnisse, welche Strategien der liebevollen Führung Führungskräfte anwenden, um toxische Scham am Arbeitsplatz zu transformieren, und betonen, dass die innere Haltung einer liebevollen Denkweise der Schlüssel zu gesunden und transformierten, positiven transkulturellen Arbeitsplätzen ist.
Article
Full-text available
Background Building a path aimed at the wellbeing of workers in the education sector is the fundamental basis to encourage quality education. To fill the gap in knowledge and address this aspect by understanding the behavior of the study population, it was proposed as with the objective of determining if servant leadership, brand love and work ethic predict the general health in workers. Methods A non-probability sampling was applied for convenience. For this purpose, a sample of 509 workers from Peru was submitted to study, who completed a questionnaire consisting of: scale of servant leadership, work ethic, GHQ-12 and brand love. By applying a quantitative method using a structural equation modeling partial least squares approach. Results The present study demonstrated that the three constructs (servant leadership, brand love, and work ethic) predict the general health of workers in a positive and significant way, in a sample of Peruvian workers in the education sector. Furthermore, the results suggest that these factors can be used to improve the health of employees in educational institutions in Peru and possibly in other contexts as well. Conclusion Given these results and after knowing the solidity of the predictions, the importance of promoting general health in workers in the education sector.
Article
Full-text available
A profound developmental experience is the emergence of adolescent romantic relationships and first feelings of love. However, the daily nature of feeling loved in adolescents’ everyday lives is poorly understood. We investigated how daily stress severity was associated with adolescents’ levels of feeling loved and whether romantic partner support moderated these associations. Furthermore, we investigated this for non‐interpersonal and interpersonal stressors. N = 97 mixed‐gender adolescent romantic couples (age M = 16.38, SD = 1.02) from an ethnically diverse sample (42.2% Hispanic/Latino/a/x, 44.7% White) participated in dyadic diary assessments twice a week for 12 consecutive weeks. Both partners independently completed open‐ended questions about a daily stressor, indicating stress severity, perceived partner support, and feeling loved. Daily stressors were coded for non‐interpersonal and various types of interpersonal stressors. Results from the dyadic multilevel model showed that days with lower than average support from a romantic partner amplify the adverse effects of stress severity on feeling loved, especially when the partner is involved in the stressor. We discuss the spillover of stress in romantic relationships and the stress‐buffering functions of adolescent romantic partner support. Adolescent romantic partners are potentially essential regulators of stress, increasing adolescents’ feelings of love. Research Highlights Romantic love is central in adolescents’ lives; we showed that adolescents generally feel loved by romantic partners. Feeling loved fluctuates daily as adolescents feel less loved on stressful days. However, when adolescents perceive their partner as supportive, there is no association between stress and feeling loved. Partner support is protective for feeling loved. The current study provides essential insights into when adolescents and why adolescents feel loved.
Article
Full-text available
In this paper, we aim to integrate the current conceptual approaches to stress and coping processes during the college transition with the potential role of mindfulness and compassion skills on students' well‐being and development. First, we provide an overview of the issues and challenges emerging adults are facing during the transition to college, drawing on the revised version of the transactional stress model by Lazarus and Folkman (1984). Second, we introduce a conceptual model of adaptive stress and coping processes enhanced by mindfulness and compassion (MC) skills to positively impact the appraisal and coping resources and emerging adults' mental health. Specifically, mindfulness and compassion skills may play an important role in promoting a healthy stress response by strengthening emerging adults' socioemotional competencies and supporting the development of adaptive appraisal and coping resources, including processes antecedent and consequent to a coping encounter. In particular, MC skills were hypothesized to enhance (1) preparedness to cope, (2) productive stress response through adaptive appraisals and skillful deployment of coping resources; and (3) healthy post‐coping reflections. Therefore, MC skills may be a useful preventive tool to strengthen emerging adults' ability to adjust to a new academic environment and fulfill the developmental tasks of this period.
Article
Full-text available
In four studies, the authors examined the correlates of the disposition toward gratitude. Study 1 revealed that self-ratings and observer ratings of the grateful disposition are associated with positive affect and well-being prosocial behaviors and traits, and religiousness/spirituality. Study 2 replicated these findings in a large nonstudent sample. Study 3 yielded similar results to Studies 1 and 2 and provided evidence that gratitude is negatively associated with envy and materialistic attitudes. Study 4 yielded evidence that these associations persist after controlling for Extraversion/positive affectivity, Neuroticism/negative affectivity, and Agreeableness. The development of the Gratitude Questionnaire, a unidimensional measure with good psychometric properties, is also described.
Article
Full-text available
The association between gratefulness and well-being is well established; however, few studies have examined the mechanisms that underlie this association. The broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 1998, 2001) posits that positive emotions broaden individuals' momentary thought-action repertoires, which serve to build personal resources that can be drawn upon during future stressful encounters. Based on this theory, the current study examined whether gratefulness, a positive emotion, would build social and cognitive resources in terms of social connectedness and presence of meaning in life (i.e., mediators), which subsequently contribute to subjective well-being (SWB). A total of 232 students participated in an online survey at 2 different time points (3 months apart). The mediational hypothesis was tested by latent change score analyses using structural equation modeling techniques. The results showed that changes in gratefulness predicted changes in social connectedness and presence of meaning in life, which, in turn, predicted changes in SWB. The study's findings provided further support for the broaden-and-build theory and suggested that gratefulness is an important positive emotion that contributes to SWB through increased social connectedness and a greater presence of meaning in life. (PsycINFO Database Record
Article
Full-text available
Cultural consensus theory is a statistical framework (CCT) for the study of individual differences in the knowledge of culturally shared opinions. In this article, we demonstrate how a CCT analysis can be used to study individual differences and cultural consensus on what makes people feel loved, or more generally any social behaviors that are governed by cognitive schemata. To highlight the advantages of the method, we describe a study in which people reported on their everyday experiences of feeling loved. Our unique approach to understanding this topic is to focus on people’s cognitive evaluations on what feeling loved (both romantically and nonromantically) entails by exploring the shared agreement regarding when one is most likely to feel loved and the individual differences that influence knowledge of these shared agreements. Our results reveal that people converge on a consensus about indicators of expressed love and that these scenarios are both romantic and nonromantic. Moreover, people show individual differences in (1) the amount of knowledge they have about this consensus and (2) their guessing biases in responding to items on love scenarios, depending on personality and demographics—all conclusions made possible by the CCT method.
Article
Full-text available
This pragmatic study examines love as a mode of communication. Our focus is on the receiver side: what makes an individual feel loved and how felt love is defined through daily interactions. Our aim is to explore everyday life scenarios in which people might experience love, and to consider people's converging and diverging judgments about which scenarios indicate felt love. We apply a cognitive psychometric approach to quantify a receiver's ability to detect, understand, and know that they are loved. Through crowd-sourcing, we surveyed lay participants about whether various scenarios were indicators of felt love. We thus quantify these responses to make inference about consensus judgments of felt love, measure individual levels of agreement with consensus, and assess individual response styles. More specifically, we (1) derive consensus judgments on felt love; (2) describe its characteristics in qualitative and quantitative terms, (3) explore individual differences in both (a) participant agreement with consensus, and (b) participant judgment when uncertain about shared knowledge, and (4) test whether individual differences can be meaningfully linked to explanatory variables. Results indicate that people converge towards a shared cognitive model of felt love. Conversely, respondents showed heterogeneity in knowledge of consensus, and in dealing with uncertainty. We found that, when facing uncertainty, female respondents and people in relationships more frequently judge scenarios as indicators of felt love. Moreover, respondents from smaller households tend to know more about consensus judgments of felt love, while respondents from larger households are more willing to guess when unsure of consensus.
Article
Positivity resonance is a type of interpersonal connection characterized by shared positivity, mutual care and concern, and behavioral and biological synchrony. Perceived positivity resonance is hypothesized to be associated with well-being. In three studies (N = 175, N = 120, N = 173), perceived positivity resonance was assessed at the trait level (Study 1) or the episode level, using the Day Reconstruction Method (Studies 2 and 3). Primary analyses reveal that perceived positivity resonance is associated with flourishing mental health, depressive symptoms, loneliness, and illness symptoms. These associations largely remain statistically significant when controlling for daily pleasant emotions or social interaction more generally. Ancillary analyses in Studies 2 and 3 support the construct validity of the episode-level assessment of perceived positivity resonance. The overall pattern of results is consistent with Positivity Resonance Theory. Discussion centers on avenues for future research and the need for behavioral interventions.
Book
The Collected Works of Ed Diener, in 3 volumes, present the major works of the leading research scientist studying happiness and well-being. Professor Diener has studied subjective well-being, people’s life satisfaction and positive emotions, for over a quarter of a century, and has published 200 works on the topic, many more than any other scholar. He has studied hundreds of thousands of people in over 140 nations of the world, and the Collected Works present the major findings from those studies. Diener has made many of the major discoveries about well-being, which are outlined in the chapters. The first volume presents the major theory and review papers of Ed Diener. These publications give a broad overview of findings in the field, and the theories of well-being. As such, the first volume is an absolute must for beginning scholars in this area, and offers a clear tutorial to the history of the field and major findings. The second volume focuses on culture. This volume is most unique, and could sell on its own, as it should appeal to cultural psychologists and anthropologists. The findings in the culture area are mostly all derived from the Diener laboratory and his students. Thus, the papers in this volume represent most of the major publications on culture and well-being. Furthermore, this is the area that is least well-known by most scholars. The third volume on measurement is the most applied and practical one because it discusses all the measures used, and presents new measures. Even for those who do not want to study well-being per se, but want to use some well-being measures in their research, this volume will be of enormous help. Volume 1: Gives a broad overview of findings and theories on subjective well-being. Volume 2: Presents most of the major papers on well-being and culture, and the international differences in well-being Volume 3: Presents discussions of measures of well-being and new measures of well-being, and is thus of great value to those who want to select measurement scales for their research Endorsements Over the past several decades Professor Diener has contributed more than any other psychologist to the rigorous research of subjective well-being. The collection of this work in this series is going to be of invaluable help to anyone interested in the study of happiness, life-satisfaction, and the emerging discipline of positive psychology. Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi, Professor of Psychology And Management, Claremont Graduate University Ed Diener, the Jedi Master of the world's happiness researchers, has inspired and informed all of us who have studied and written about happiness. His life's work epitomizes a humanly significant psychological science. How wonderful to have his pioneering writings collected and preserved for future students of human well-being, and for practitioners and social policy makers who are working to promote human flourishing. David G. Myers, Hope College, and author, The Pursuit of Happiness. Ed Diener's work on life satisfaction -- theory and research -- has been ground-breaking. Having his collected works available will be a great boon to psychologists and policy-makers alike. Christopher Peterson, Professor of Psychology, Univ. of Michigan By looking at happiness and well-being in many different cultures and societies, from East to West, from New York City to Calcutta slums, and beyond, Ed Diener has forever transformed the field of culture in psychology. Filled with bold theoretical insights and rigorous and, yet, imaginative empirical studies, this volume will be absolutely indispensable for all social and behavioral scientists interested in transformative power of culture on human psychology. Shinobu Kitayama, Professor and Director of the Culture and Cognition Program, Univ. of Michigan Ed Diener is one of the most productive psychologists in the world working in the field of perceived quality of life or, as he prefers, subjective wellbeing. He has served the profession as a researcher, writer, teacher, officer in professional organizations, editor of leading journals, a member of the editorial board of still more journals as well as a member of the board of the Social Indicators Research Book Series. As an admirer of his work and a good friend, I have learned a lot from him, from his students, his relatives and collaborators. The idea of producing a collection of his works came to me as a result of spending a great deal of time trying to keep up with his work. What a wonderful public and professional service it would be, I thought, as well as a time-saver for me, if we could get a substantial number of his works assembled in one collection. In these three volumes we have not only a fine selection of past works but a good number of new ones as well. So, it is with considerable delight that I write these lines to thank Ed and to lend my support to this important publication. Alex C. Michalos, Ph.D., F.R.S.C., Chancellor, Director, Institute for Social Research and Evaluation, Professor Emeritus, Political Science, Univ. of Northern British Columbia
Article
The unfolding of emotions over time (i.e., emotion dynamics) has been characterized by baseline, intraindividual variability and regulatory tendency (return time to baseline after deflection). Mounting evidence suggests that compared to healthy individuals, individuals with schizophrenia are characterized by a more negatively valenced baseline and a higher intraindividual variability. However, the regulatory tendency has not been investigated in schizophrenia. We hypothesize that the severity of positive symptoms is linked to increased emotional variability and that the severity of negative symptoms is associated with an increased regulatory tendency. Fifteen individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia took part in this pilot experience sampling study and reported their emotional state ten times a day. The dynamics of valence and arousal and their relationship with symptomatology were estimated with the DynAffect model. Regulatory tendency in valence and arousal was positively associated with negative symptom severity and negatively associated with positive symptom severity. However, the severity of positive symptoms was not credibly associated with the variability of valence. The study only partly corroborates findings on increased stress reactivity in schizophrenia, which might be due to the small sample size. However, results suggest that negative symptoms could stem from over-regulated emotion dynamics, which may impede goal-directed behavior.
Article
PART I THE LOGIC OF HIERARCHICAL LINEAR MODELING Series Editor 's Introduction to Hierarchical Linear Models Series Editor 's Introduction to the Second Edition 1.Introduction 2.The Logic of Hierarchical Linear Models 3. Principles of Estimation and Hypothesis Testing for Hierarchical Linear Models 4. An Illustration PART II BASIC APPLICATIONS 5. Applications in Organizational Research 6. Applications in the Study of Individual Change 7. Applications in Meta-Analysis and Other Cases where Level-1 Variances are Known 8. Three-Level Models 9. Assessing the Adequacy of Hierarchical Models PART III ADVANCED APPLICATIONS 10. Hierarchical Generalized Linear Models 11. Hierarchical Models for Latent Variables 12. Models for Cross-Classified Random Effects 13. Bayesian Inference for Hierarchical Models PART IV ESTIMATION THEORY AND COMPUTATIONS 14. Estimation Theory Summary and Conclusions References Index About the Authors
Article
Objectives: Life-span theories of aging suggest improvements and decrements in individuals' ability to regulate affect. Dynamic process models, with intensive longitudinal data, provide new opportunities to articulate specific theories about individual differences in intraindividual dynamics. This paper illustrates a method for operationalizing affect dynamics using a multilevel stochastic differential equation (SDE) model, and examines how those dynamics differ with age and trait-level tendencies to deploy emotion regulation strategies (reappraisal and suppression). Method: Univariate multilevel SDE models, estimated in a Bayesian framework, were fit to 21 days of ecological momentary assessments of affect valence and arousal (average 6.93/day, SD = 1.89) obtained from 150 adults (age 18-89 years)-specifically capturing temporal dynamics of individuals' core affect in terms of attractor point, reactivity to biopsychosocial (BPS) inputs, and attractor strength. Results: Older age was associated with higher arousal attractor point and less BPS-related reactivity. Greater use of reappraisal was associated with lower valence attractor point. Intraindividual variability in regulation strategy use was associated with greater BPS-related reactivity and attractor strength, but in different ways for valence and arousal. Discussion: The results highlight the utility of SDE models for studying affect dynamics and informing theoretical predictions about how intraindividual dynamics change over the life course.