ArticlePDF Available

Cat and Dog Ownership in Early Life and Infant Development: A Prospective Birth Cohort Study of Japan Environment and Children’s Study

MDPI
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health (IJERPH)
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Contact with companion animals has been suggested to have important roles in enhancing child development. However, studies focused on child development and pet ownership at a very early age are limited. The purpose of the current study was to investigate child development in relation to pet ownership at an early age in a nationwide prospective birth cohort study: the Japan Environment and Children’s Study. Associations between cat and dog ownership at six months and infant development at 12 months of age were examined in this study. Infant development was assessed using the Ages & Stages QuestionnairesTM (ASQ-3) at 12 months. Among participants of (Japan Environment and Children’s Study) JECS, those with available data of cat and dog ownership at six months and data for the ASQ-3 at 12 months were included (n = 78,868). Having dogs showed higher percentages of pass in all five domains measured by ASQ-3 (communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem-solving, and personal-social) compared to those who did not have dogs. Significantly decreased odds ratios (ORs) of developmental delays were observed in association with having dogs in all fix domains (communication: OR = 0.73, gross motor: OR = 0.86, fine motor: OR = 0.84, problem-solving: OR = 0.90, personal-social: OR = 0.83). This study suggested that early life dog ownership may reduce the risks of child developmental delays.
Content may be subject to copyright.
International Journal of
Environmental Research
and Public Health
Article
Cat and Dog Ownership in Early Life and Infant
Development: A Prospective Birth Cohort Study of
Japan Environment and Children’s Study
Machiko Minatoya 1,2 , Atsuko Araki 1, Chihiro Miyashita 1, Sachiko Itoh 1,
Sumitaka Kobayashi 1, Keiko Yamazaki 1, Yu Ait Bamai 1, Yasuaki Saijyo 3, Yoshiya Ito 4,
Reiko Kishi 1, * and The Japan Environment and Children’s Study Group
1Hokkaido University Center for Environmental and Health Sciences, Sapporo 060-0812, Japan;
mminatoya@cehs.hokudai.ac.jp (M.M.); AAraki@cehs.hokudai.ac.jp (A.A.);
miyasita@med.hokudai.ac.jp (C.M.); vzbghjn@den.hokudai.ac.jp (S.I.);
sukobayashi@cehs.hokudai.ac.jp (S.K.); kyamazaki@cehs.hokudai.ac.jp (K.Y.);
u-aitbamai@med.hokudai.ac.jp (Y.A.B.)
2Hokkaido University Faculty of Health Sciences, Sapporo 060-0812, Japan
3Department of Social Medicine, Asahikawa Medical University, Asahikawa 078-8510, Japan;
y-saijo@asahikawa-med.ac.jp
4Faculty of Nursing, Japanese Red Cross Hokkaido College of Nursing, Kitami 090-0011, Japan;
yito@rchokkaido-cn.ac.jp
*Correspondence: rkishi@med.hokudai.ac.jp
A complete list of the centres and investigators in the Japan Environment and Children’s Study Group is
provided in Appendix A.
Received: 4 December 2019; Accepted: 25 December 2019; Published: 27 December 2019


Abstract:
Contact with companion animals has been suggested to have important roles in enhancing
child development. However, studies focused on child development and pet ownership at a very
early age are limited. The purpose of the current study was to investigate child development in
relation to pet ownership at an early age in a nationwide prospective birth cohort study: the Japan
Environment and Children’s Study. Associations between cat and dog ownership at six months
and infant development at 12 months of age were examined in this study. Infant development was
assessed using the Ages & Stages Questionnaires
TM
(ASQ-3) at 12 months. Among participants of
(Japan Environment and Children’s Study) JECS, those with available data of cat and dog ownership
at six months and data for the ASQ-3 at 12 months were included (n=78,868). Having dogs showed
higher percentages of pass in all five domains measured by ASQ-3 (communication, gross motor, fine
motor, problem-solving, and personal-social) compared to those who did not have dogs. Significantly
decreased odds ratios (ORs) of developmental delays were observed in association with having
dogs in all fix domains (communication: OR =0.73, gross motor: OR =0.86, fine motor: OR =0.84,
problem-solving: OR =0.90, personal-social: OR =0.83). This study suggested that early life dog
ownership may reduce the risks of child developmental delays.
Keywords: pet ownership; child development; birth cohort; ASQ-3
1. Introduction
Childhood is a crucial phase in its contribution to the quality of health, emotional well-being,
learning, and behavior across the life span [
1
]. However, studies of child development have largely
been limited to relationships and interactions with other humans. Studies of the human–animal
interaction have proposed that there are health benefits associated with pet ownership. Pet ownership
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 205; doi:10.3390/ijerph17010205 www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 205 2 of 11
may provide emotional protection from the stresses and strains of life, and it may encourage a more
active life. Contact with companion animals has been suggested to have important roles in enhancing
child development [
2
,
3
]. The psychological benefits of attachment to pets have been found for a
variety of pet animals, particularly cats [
4
] and dogs [
5
]. Several studies have found a link between
pet ownership, pet attachment, positive attitudes to animals, compassion, empathy, and prosocial
behavior [
6
11
]. For many children, companion animals are likely powerful motivators for learning [
12
]
and development [
12
,
13
]. Although pet ownership may have the potential to positively influence child
development, these relationships have received little attention, and a need for research in this area
has been recognized [
12
,
14
]. Especially, studies focused on child development and pet ownership at a
very early age and are limited. This could be partly due to the limited availability of developmental
assessment tools at early ages.
The Ages & Stages Questionnaires
TM
(ASQ-3) is a developmental screening tool widely used
by clinicians, researchers, and intervention programs around the world [
15
]. The ASQ-3 pinpoints
developmental progress in children between the ages of 1 to 66 months. Because this tool is
parent-completed, it provides an ecient and cost-eective method of collecting information regarding
children’s development without the need for trained experts and considered to be an appropriate
method for large prospective cohort study.
The purpose of the current study was to investigate child development in relation to pet ownership
at an early age. In this study, associations between cat and dog ownership at six months and infant
development at 12 months of age were examined using the data from a nationwide prospective birth
cohort study; the Japan Environment and Children’s Study (JECS).
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Study Design
The JECS is an ongoing nationwide prospective birth cohort study in Japan. JECS study is
conducted at 15 regional centers (Hokkaido, Miyagi, Fukushima, Chiba, Kanagawa, Koshin, Toyama,
Aichi, Kyoto, Osaka, Hyogo, Tottori, Kochi, Fukuoka, and Minami Kyusyu/Okinawa) in Japan. Details
of the JECS project have been described elsewhere [
16
18
]. Briefly, pregnant women were recruited
between January 2011 and March 2014. Eligibility criteria for participation included residing in the
study area at the time of recruitment, an expected delivery date after August 2011, comprehension
of the Japanese language, and completing the self-administered questionnaire. In total, 104,065 fetal
records were included in the cohort, including multiple births. The present study used the dataset
jecs-an-20180131, which was released in January 2018 and revised in December 2018.
2.2. Ethical Statement
The JECS protocol was approved by the Ministry of the Environment’s Institutional Review
Board on Epidemiological Studies and by the Ethics Committees of each participating institution
(Appendix A) (ethical project identification code: Kanken19-117). All participants provided informed,
written consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
2.3. Study Participants
Of the 104,065 fetal records included in the cohort, 100,144 were live birth. Among participants of
JECS, those with available data of dog and/or cat ownership at six months and data for the ASQ-3 at
12 months were included in this study (n=78,868) (Figure 1).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 205 3 of 11
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, x FOR PEER REVIEW 3 of 11
Figure 1. Selection of study population.
2.4. SelfAdministered Questionnaires
Details of the self-administered questionnaire in this study have been described previously
[16,17]. Briefly, maternal smoking and drinking at the 2nd/3rd trimesters, maternal and paternal
education, and annual household income during pregnancy were obtained from the M-T2
questionnaire (answered by pregnant women at the 2nd/3rd trimesters); parity was obtained from
the Dr-T1 questionnaire (medical records transcripts at the 1st trimester); and maternal age at
delivery, delivery mode, infant sex, gestational age, and birth weight were obtained from the Dr-0m
questionnaire (medical records transcripts at delivery). Marital status was obtained from the C-6M
questionnaire (answered by mothers at 6 months of postpartum). Duration of breastfeeding, and
maternal mental illness (a Japanese version of the Kessler 6 (K6) scale) were obtained from the C-1Y
questionnaire (answered by mothers at 12 months of postpartum). Information regarding pet
ownership including cats and dogs was obtained from the C-6M questionnaire (answered by mothers
at 6 months of postpartum).
2.5. Outcome Definitions
The ASQ-3 is comprised of 21 age-specific questionnaires for children ages 1 to 66 months to
assess children’s progress in five developmental domains (communication, gross motor, fine motor,
problem-solving, personal-social). Each of the 5 domains has 6 questions, resulting in 30 items for
each age-interval. Each item describes a skill, ability, or behavior to which the parent responds “yes
(10 points),sometimes” (5), or “not yet (0). Parents sometimes omit items when they are unsure of
how to respond or because they have concerns about their child’s performance of the item. The ASQ-
3 scores were not calculated if there were three or more omitted items in a given domain. In the case
of one or two omitted items, an adjusted total domain score was calculated by adding the averaged
item score either once for one omission or twice for two omissions. Children who may potentially be
at-risk for developmental delays at each age-interval are identified by comparing their scores to cutoff
scores. The J-ASQ-3 cutoff scores for each domain were; communication = 15.64, gross motor = 21.49,
fine motor = 34.50, problem-solving = 27.32, personal-social = 21.73 based on the original ASQ-3 [19].
In this study, those who completed the J-ASQ-3 12-months questionnaire at 11 months 0 days through
Figure 1. Selection of study population.
2.4. Self-Administered Questionnaires
Details of the self-administered questionnaire in this study have been described previously [
16
,
17
].
Briefly, maternal smoking and drinking at the 2nd/3rd trimesters, maternal and paternal education, and
annual household income during pregnancy were obtained from the M-T2 questionnaire (answered
by pregnant women at the 2nd/3rd trimesters); parity was obtained from the Dr-T1 questionnaire
(medical records transcripts at the 1st trimester); and maternal age at delivery, delivery mode, infant
sex, gestational age, and birth weight were obtained from the Dr-0m questionnaire (medical records
transcripts at delivery). Marital status was obtained from the C-6M questionnaire (answered by
mothers at 6 months of postpartum). Duration of breastfeeding, and maternal mental illness (a
Japanese version of the Kessler 6 (K6) scale) were obtained from the C-1Y questionnaire (answered by
mothers at 12 months of postpartum). Information regarding pet ownership including cats and dogs
was obtained from the C-6M questionnaire (answered by mothers at 6 months of postpartum).
2.5. Outcome Definitions
The ASQ-3 is comprised of 21 age-specific questionnaires for children ages 1 to 66 months to
assess children’s progress in five developmental domains (communication, gross motor, fine motor,
problem-solving, personal-social). Each of the 5 domains has 6 questions, resulting in 30 items for
each age-interval. Each item describes a skill, ability, or behavior to which the parent responds “yes”
(10 points), “sometimes” (5), or “not yet” (0). Parents sometimes omit items when they are unsure of
how to respond or because they have concerns about their child’s performance of the item. The ASQ-3
scores were not calculated if there were three or more omitted items in a given domain. In the case
of one or two omitted items, an adjusted total domain score was calculated by adding the averaged
item score either once for one omission or twice for two omissions. Children who may potentially
be at-risk for developmental delays at each age-interval are identified by comparing their scores to
cutoscores. The J-ASQ-3 cutoscores for each domain were; communication =15.64, gross motor
=21.49, fine motor =34.50, problem-solving =27.32, personal-social =21.73 based on the original
ASQ-3 [
19
]. In this study, those who completed the J-ASQ-3 12-months questionnaire at 11 months
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 205 4 of 11
0 days through 12 months 30 days of ages were strictly included. According to recommend ASQ-3
procedures, adjusted age was used to determine the appropriate ASQ-3 for children who were preterm
(<37 weeks of gestational age).
2.6. Statistical Analyses
ASQ-3 scores of each domain were dichotomized based on the cutoscores of each domain.
Chi-squared test was used to determine whether there was a significant dierence in the frequency
of pass/fail of ASQ-3 scores at 12 months of age each domain in having or not having cats and dogs
at 6 months of age. t-test was used to compare the mean value of maternal age, birth weight, and
gestational age among those who had cats and dogs and those who did not. Logistic regression models
were used to investigate infant developmental delays in association with having cats and dogs. The
models were adjusted for maternal and paternal education, maternal smoking during pregnancy,
annual household income during pregnancy, maternal mental illness, and duration of breastfeeding
based on the previous literature and correlation between these variables and exposure and outcome.
Since there were a small number of missing values for each variable, analyses included only those
without missing variables. p<0.05 was considered as statistically significant. Statistical analyses were
performed using SPSS version 24 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA).
3. Results
Table 1shows the characteristics of parents and infants included in this study (n=78,868) and
comparison of characteristics between those who had cats and dogs. The pet ownership for cats and
dogs at six months was 6358 (8.1%) and 11,934 (15.1%), respectively. Prevalence of maternal smoking
at the 2nd trimester was higher in those who had cats and dogs. Both maternal and paternal education
levels and annual household income were lower in those who had cats and dogs. Prevalence of possible
maternal mental illness was higher in those who had cats but not dogs. Duration of breastfeeding was
shorter in those who had cats and dogs.
Table 1. Characteristics of participants.
Mean ±SD or n(%)
All Having Cat(s) Having Dog(s)
Parents n=
78,868
Yes (n=
6358)
No (n=
72,510) p-Value Yes (n=
11,934)
No (n=
66,934) p-Value
Maternal age
at delivery
(years)
30.7
±
5.1 30.8
±
5.1 30.7
±
5.1
0.832
30.6
±
5.0 30.8
±
5.1
0.034
Parity
nulliparous 31,078
(39.4)
2503
(39.4)
28,575
(39.4) 0.788 4721
(39.6)
26,357
(39.4) 0.754
multipara 32,986
(41.8)
3720
(58.5)
42,162
(58.1)
6932
(58.1)
38,950
(58.2)
missing
1908 (2.4)
135 (2.1)
1773 (2.4)
281 (2.4)
1627 (2.4)
Maternal
smoking at
2nd trimester
yes
2874 (3.6)
366 (5.7)
2508 (3.5)
<0.001 641 (5.4)
2233 (3.3)
<0.001
no 75,051
(95.2)
5897
(92.7)
69,154
(95.4) <0.001 11,128
(93.2)
63,923
(95.5) <0.001
missing 943 (1.2) 95 (1.5) 848 (1.2) 165 (1.4) 778 (1.2)
Maternal
drinking at
2nd trimester
yes
2134 (2.7)
191 (3.0)
1943 (2.7)
0.123 336 (2.8)
1798 (2.7)
0.415
no 75,789
(96.1)
6086
(95.7)
69,703
(96.1)
11,447
(95.9)
64,342
(96.1)
missing 945 (1.2) 81 (1.3) 864 (1.2) 151 (1.3) 794 (1.2)
Maternal
education
(years)
<13 26,859
(34.1)
2781
(43.7)
24,078
(33.2) <0.001 4905
(41.1)
21,954
(32.8) <0.001
13 51,249
(65.0)
3506
(55.1)
47,743
(65.8)
6889
(57.7)
44,360
(66.3)
missing 760 (1.0) 71 (1.1) 689 (1.0) 140 (1.2) 620 (0.9)
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 205 5 of 11
Table 1. Cont.
Mean ±SD or n(%)
All Having Cat(s) Having Dog(s)
Parents n=
78,868
Yes (n=
6358)
No (n=
72,510) p-Value Yes (n=
11,934)
No (n=
66,934) p-Value
Paternal
education
(years)
<13 33,115
(42.0)
3220
(50.6)
29,895
(41.2) <0.001 5940
(49.8)
27,175
(40.6) <0.001
13 44,570
(56.5)
3003
(47.2)
41,567
(57.3)
5750
(48.2)
38,820
(58.0)
missing
1183 (1.5)
135 (2.1)
1048 (1.4)
244 (2.0) 939 (1.4)
Annual
household
income at 2nd
trimester
<428,699
(36.4)
2581
(40.6)
26,118
(36.0) <0.001 4440
(37.2)
24,259
(36.2) <0.001
444,361
(56.2)
3115
(49.0)
41,246
(56.9)
6205
(52.0)
38,156
(57.0)
missing
5808 (7.4) 662 (10.4) 5146 (7.1)
1289
(10.8)
4519 (6.8)
Marital status
married 74,783
(94.8)
5952
(93.6)
68,831
(94.9) <0.001 11,167
(93.6)
63,616
(95.0) <0.001
divorced/
bereavement 529 (0.1) 72 (1.1) 457 (0.6) 149 (1.2) 380 (0.6)
missing
3556 (4.5)
334 (5.3)
3222 (4.4)
618 (5.2)
2938 (4.4)
Maternal
possible
mental illness
yes (K6
13)
2036 (2.6)
234 (3.4)
1802 (2.5)
<0.001 328 (2.7)
1708 (2.6)
0.205
no (K6 <13) 76,156
(96.6)
6067
(95.4)
70,089
(96.7)
11,493
(96.3)
64,663
(96.6)
missing 676 (0.8) 57 (0.9) 619 (0.9) 113 (0.9) 563 (0.8)
Infant
Sex
male 40,274
(51.1)
3255
(51.2)
37,019
(51.1) 0.721 6095
(51.1)
34,179
(51.1) 0.994
female 38,572
(48.9)
3100
(48.8)
35,472
(48.9)
5835
(48.9)
32,737
(48.9)
missing 22 (0.0) 3 (0.0) 19 (0.0) 4 (0.0) 18 (0.0)
Birth
weight (g)
3041 ±
384
3037 ±
383
3041 ±
384 0.428 3041 ±
386
3041 ±
383 0.926
Gestational
age (weeks)
39.4
±
1.2 39.4
±
1.2 39.4
±
1.3
0.003
39.4
±
1.2 39.4
±
1.3
0.001
Delivery
mode
vaginal 64,007
(81.2)
5157
(81.1)
58,850
(81.2) 0.796 9575
(80.2)
54,432
(81.3) 0.003
cesarean 14,665
(18.6)
1191
(18.7)
13,474
(18.6)
2335
(19.6)
12,330
(18.4)
missing 196 (0.2) 10 (0.2) 186 (0.3) 24 (0.2) 172 (0.3)
Duration of
breastfeeding
(months)
0
1978 (2.5)
205 (3.2)
1773 (2.4)
<0.001 346 (2.9)
1632 (2.4)
<0.001
1–6 16,582
(21.0)
1646
(25.9)
14,936
(20.6)
3000
(25.1)
13,582
(20.3)
7–12 60,308
(76.5)
4507
(70.9)
55,801
(77.0)
8588
(72.0)
51,720
(77.3)
Missing data: n(%), maternal age: n=93 (0.1), birth weight: n=41 (0.05). Chi-square test or t-test.
Table 2shows the distribution of the ASQ-3 12-months questionnaire scores in association
with having cats and dogs at six months of age. The mean scores of each domain were 37.68 for
communication, 42.78 for gross motor, 48.21 for fine motor, 42.35 for problem-solving, and 37.03
for personal-social, respectively. Numbers of infants who failed each domain were 5633 (7.1%) for
communication, 11,368 (14.3%) for gross motor, 8133 (10.2%) for fine motor, 12,505 (15.7%) for problem-
solving, 13,755 (17.3%) for personal-social. Those who had cats showed higher percentages (83.7%) of
pass in the personal-social domain compared to those who did not have cats (82.5%). Having dogs
showed higher percentages of pass in all five domains measured by ASQ-3 compared to those who did
not have dogs.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 205 6 of 11
Table 2.
Distribution of the ASQ-3 12-months questionnaire scores in association with having a cat and
dog at six months of age.
All Having Cat(s) Having Dog(s)
ASQ Subscale CutoYes No p-Value Yes No p-Value
Communication
15.64
Mean ±SD
37.68
±
13.43
Pass 73,216 (92.8) 5897
(92.8)
67,319
(92.9) 0.745 11,300
(94.7)
61,916
(92.6) <0.001
Fail 5601 (7.1) 458 (7.2) 5143
(7.1)
630
(5.3)
4971
(7.4)
Missing 51 (0.0)
Gross motor 21.49
Mean ±SD
42.78
±
17.55
Pass 67,556 (85.7) 5486
(86.3)
62,070
(85.6) 0.138 10,414
(87.3)
57,142
(85.4) <0.001
Fail 11,271 (14.3) 869
(13.7)
10,402
(14.4)
1511
(12.7)
9760
(14.6)
Missing 41 (0.0)
Fine motor 34.50
Mean ±SD
48.21
±
11.63
Pass 70,729 (89.7) 5729
(90.2)
65,000
(89.7) 0.236 10,840
(90.9)
59,889
(89.6) <0.001
Fail 8056 (10.2) 622 (9.8) 7434
(10.3)
1079
(9.1)
6977
(10.4)
Missing 83 (0.1)
Problem-
solving 27.32
Mean ±SD
42.35
±
13.61
Pass 66,317 (84.1) 5346
(84.3)
60,971
(84.2) 0.965 10,158
(85.3)
56,159
(84.1) 0.001
Fail 12,398 (15.7) 998
(15.7)
11,400
(15.8)
1754
(14.7)
10,644
(15.9)
Missing 153 (0.2)
Personal- social
21.73
Mean ±SD
37.03
±
14.57
Pass 64,927 (82.3) 5303
(83.7)
59,624
(82.5) 0.018 10,107
(84.9)
54,820
(82.2) <0.001
Fail 13,651 (17.3) 1032
(16.3)
12,619
(17.5)
1791
(15.1)
11,860
(17.8)
Missing 290 (0.4)
n(%). Chi-square test.
Table 3shows infant developmental delays at 12 months of age in association with having cats
and dogs at six months of postpartum. There was no significant association between having a cat
and infant developmental delays of any domains. Contrary, significantly decreased odds ratios (ORs)
were observed in association with having a dog in all five domains. ORs and 95% confidence intervals
(CIs) for each domain were 0.73 (0.67, 0.80) for communication, 0.86 (0.81, 0.92) for gross motor, 0.84
(0.78, 0.90) for fine motor, 0.90 (0.85, 0.96) for problem-solving, 0.83 (0.79, 0.88) for personal-social,
respectively. Further analysis of three groups (only cat ownership, only dog ownership, and both cat
and dog ownership) were conducted, and the results are shown in supplemental Table S3.
Table 3.
Infant development delays at 12 months of age in association with cat and dog ownership at
6 months of age.
OR (95% CI)
Cat Ownership Dog Ownership
Communication 0.94 (0.85, 1.04) 0.73 (0.67, 0.80) **
Gross motor 1.06 (0.98, 1.15) 0.86 (0.81, 0.92) **
Fine motor 1.07 (0.98, 1.18) 0.84 (0.78, 0.90) **
Problem solving 1.02 (0.95, 1.10) 0.90 (0.85, 0.96) *
Personal-social 1.07 (0.98, 1.16) +0.83 (0.79, 0.88) **
Adjusted for maternal and paternal education, maternal smoke during pregnancy, annual household income during
pregnancy, maternal mental illness (K6), duration of breastfeeding. +p<0.10, * p<0.05, ** p<0.001.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 205 7 of 11
4. Discussion
In this study, we found that having dogs at six months was associated with decreased risks of
infant showing developmental delays at 12 months in five domains of the ASQ-3. However, the
same was not observed among those who had cats. This was the first report on early childhood pet
ownership and child development based on the large-scale prospective birth cohort study data.
The mean scores of the ASQ-3 in this study were relatively lower compared to the recent cohort
study (n=318) as well as US national data [
20
]. The mean scores were also lower compared to the
original scores from the ASQ manual. As it was discussed in the recent literature from Japan, for
the questionnaires for younger children, the scores tended to be lower for the J-ASQ-3 [
21
]. This is
consistent with other developmental assessment tools that showed that Japanese children generally
develop slower than American children until about two years of age [22].
The ownership of cats and dogs in this study was 8.0% and 15.0%, respectively, and this is
comparable to the data from Japan Pet Food Association that reported the prevalence of cat and dog
ownership in Japan was 9.78% and 12.64%, respectively, in 2018 [
23
]. In this study, socio-demographic
characteristics of participants with cats or dogs were less educated parents and less household income,
and maternal age at delivery was significantly younger, which was similar to previous studies [
24
,
25
].
In general, higher parental education and parental social class, and the younger the mother was at
delivery, were related to less pet ownerships. This was only found in dog ownership but not in
cat ownership.
Our findings on pet ownership and child development are in line with findings from other
studies. A systematic review found evidence for an association between pet ownership and a wide
range of emotional health benefits, such as self-esteem and reduced depression from childhood
pet ownership [
26
]. A recent study from Japan suggested that pet ownership in toddlerhood may
contribute to the development of emotional expression of children [
24
]. The same study found that the
prevalence of children with poor emotional expression were in the protective direction for owning
dogs; contrary, the direction was opposite for cat ownership [
24
]. Infants could learn and develop skills
of expression from interacting with their dogs and result in a lower risk of having communication
and personal-social problems. Interaction with dogs may potentially help infants to have better gross
and fine motor development. For example, a study of dog-assisted therapies and activities in the
rehabilitation of children with cerebral palsy and physical and mental disabilities reported that the
children improved their abilities to use their bodies according to their capabilities [
27
]. Another study
showed that the presence of a therapy dog would aect the performance of a set of gross motor skill
tasks for a mixed group of language-impaired and typical preschool children [
28
]. Children in these
studies were older than those in the present study; however, these findings may be an indication of
how beneficial interacting with dogs can be for the motor development of children. The previous
studies suggested that early exposure to pets may contribute to child cognitive development [
29
] and
may facilitate language acquisition and potentially enhance verbal skills in children [
30
]. The presence
of animals has been shown to elicit immediate positive eects in testing situations of cognition, such as
memory, categorization, and attention [
28
,
31
35
]. Problem-solving assessed in this study is related to
cognitive function. Together with the previous studies, findings from this study suggested that the
presence of dogs at home may contribute to child cognitive development even at a younger age.
Literature included in the systematic review investigated various kind pets, including birds, fish,
horses, and so on; however, the evidence for the impact of pets on child development was mostly
found only with dogs [
26
]. It has been reported that children with pet dogs scored the highest on
attachment [
36
]. Dogs are also more likely to read and adopt their behavior in response to human
emotional signals [
37
]. These may explain why only ownership of dogs showed a significant decreased
risk of children to have developmental problems. In addition, a study suggested that pet ownership
may be associated with increased social skills, and the positive eect was only found in association
with dog ownerships [
38
]. Research over the past 30 years [
39
] and a systematic review [
40
] indicated
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 205 8 of 11
that dogs may oer physiological, emotional, social, physical support, and beneficial eect on a number
of behavioral processes for children.
Although the ownership of pets showed benefits, including better child development, certain
negative consequences, such as zoonotic infections [
41
], allergy and asthma [
42
], bites and other
injuries [
43
], and the psychological and emotional costs due to loss of pet [
44
] have been noted.
In addition, children are at a greater risk of animal bites from a household pet [
45
,
46
], and children
under five years of age are most at risk of serious injury [47,48].
The strengths of this study are that we used a dataset of large sample size, longitudinal design,
and included a variety of dierent factors. Health influence in association with pet ownership has
been discussed in case-control studies and cross-sectional studies [
26
,
49
]; however, these previous
studies had limitations in study designs and sample size. In addition, some of the studies did not
consider confounding factors. The current study was the first study to investigate child development
in association with early life pet ownership in a prospective birth cohort study. The characteristics of
parents and infants included in this study were compared to the whole cohort profile data and found
to be similar to the original cohort population [
16
]. However, a comparison between the current study
population (n=78,868) and those who were excluded from the current study due to no data of pet
ownership (n=860) found some significant dierences (Supplemental Tables S1 and S2). Percentage of
maternal smoking was higher, and both maternal and paternal education levels and annual family
income were lower among those who were excluded. These characteristics were also common among
those who had cats and dogs (Table 1). This may indicate that the prevalence of pet ownership was
possibly higher among those who were excluded. The mean ASQ-3 scores were higher among those
who were excluded in all domains, which may indicate that less developmentally delayed children
were in this group. These dierences in characteristics may impact the findings from this study;
however, the number of those excluded was small (1.1%); thus, the impact is also small even if it existed.
Some limitations of this study should be noted. First, the child developmental delays were
assessed by a parental report. Even though the ASQ-3 is one of the most widely used screening
tools for early childhood development, misclassification may have occurred. We have controlled for
possible maternal mental illness based on the validated Japanese version of the K6 score [
50
] at 12
months postpartum as ASQ-3 scores may have been influenced by maternal mental problems. Second,
the current study only investigated cat and dog ownerships but no other pets. Additionally, it is
important to recognize that pet attachment may be more important in exerting potential health eects
than pet ownership [
51
]. In this study, we could only examine the association between pet ownership
and child development. For further studies, assessment of pet attachment should be considered.
In addition to self-reported questionnaires, observational and behavioral methods would ensure the
accuracy of findings. Third, other important confounding factors, such as the quality of children’s
home environments, needed to be considered because children’s home environment has been linked
with both the concurrent and longitudinal cognitive development of children [
30
,
52
]. The current
study did not control for these confounding factors, which may possibly mask the true association
between dog ownership and infant development.
5. Conclusions
This study suggested that having dogs in early life may possibly reduce the risks of children
to have developmental delays. Further longitudinal follow-up studies are necessary to elucidate
associations between early life pet ownership and child development.
Supplementary Materials:
The following are available online at http://www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/17/1/205/s1,
Table S1: Comparison between the current study population and those who excluded from the current study due
to no data of pet ownership. Table S2: Comparison of ASQ-3 scores between the current study population and
those who excluded from the current study due to no data on pet ownership. Table S3: Infant development delays
at 12 months of age in association with cat and dog ownership at 6 months of age.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 205 9 of 11
Author Contributions:
Conceptualization, M.M. and R.K.; methodology, M.M.; investigation, M.M.; data curation,
M.M.; writing—original draft preparation, M.M.; writing—review and editing, A.A., C.M., S.I., S.K., K.Y., Y.A.B.,
Y.S., Y.I., R.K., The Japan Environment and Children’s Study Group; visualization, M.M.; supervision, R.K.; project
administration, R.K. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding:
The Japan Environment and Children’s Study is funded by the operating budget of the Ministry of the
Environment, Japan. The findings and conclusions of this article are solely the responsibility of the authors and do
not represent the ocial views of the Ministry of the Environment of the Japanese government.
Acknowledgments:
We thank the participants of all mothers and their children who participated in the JECS. We
wish to express our sincere appreciation to the collaborating hospitals and clinics. We also express our gratitude to
all the JECS stamembers in Hokkaido, Miyagi, Fukushima, Chiba, Kanagawa, Koshin, Toyama, Aichi, Kyoto,
Osaka, Hyogo, Tottori, Kochi, Fukuoka, and South-Kyushu and Okinawa regional centers and national center for
JECS, and the Medical Support Center.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Appendix A
Members of the Japan Environment and Children’s Study (JECS) Group 2019; Michihiro Kamijima
(principal investigator, Nagoya City University, Nagoya, Japan), Shin Yamazaki (National Institute
for Environmental Studies, Tsukuba, Japan), Yukihiro Ohya (National Center for Child Health and
Development, Tokyo, Japan), Reiko Kishi (Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan), Nobuo Yaegashi
(Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan), Koichi Hashimoto (Fukushima Medical University, Fukushima,
Japan), Chisato Mori (Chiba University, Chiba, Japan), Shuichi Ito (Yokohama City University,
Yokohama, Japan), Zentaro Yamagata (University of Yamanashi, Chuo, Japan), Hidekuni Inadera
(University of Toyama, Toyama, Japan), Takeo Nakayama (Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan), Hiroyasu
Iso (Osaka University, Suita, Japan), Masayuki Shima (Hyogo College of Medicine, Nishinomiya,
Japan), Youichi Kurozawa (Tottori University, Yonago, Japan), Narufumi Suganuma (Kochi University,
Nankoku, Japan), Koichi Kusuhara (University of Occupational and Environmental Health, Kitakyushu,
Japan), and Takahiko Katoh (Kumamoto University, Kumamoto, Japan).
References
1.
World Health Organization. Available online: https://www.who.int/maternal_child_adolescent/child/en/
(accessed on 29 August 2019).
2. Brodie, J.D. Health benefits of owning pet animals. Vet. Rec. 1981,109, 197–199. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
3.
Endenburg, N.; Baarda, B. The role of pets in enhancing human well-being: Eects on child deelopment.
In The Waltham Book of Human-Animal Interaction: Benefits and Responsibilities of Pet Ownership; Elsevier Ltd.:
Oxford, UK, 1995.
4.
Oyama, M.A.; Citron, L.; Cimino Brown, D.; Serpell, J.A.; Farrar, J.T. Measuring Quality of Life in Owners
of Companion Dogs: Development and Validation of a Dog Owner-specific Quality of Life Questionnaire.
Anthrozoös 2017,30, 61–75. [CrossRef]
5.
Straede, C.M.; Gates, R.G. Psychological Health in a Population of Australian Cat Owners. Anthrozoös
1993
,
6, 30–42. [CrossRef]
6.
Daly, B.; Morton, L.L. An investigation of human-animal interactions and empathy as related to pet preference,
ownership, attachment, and attitudes in children. Anthrozoös 2006,19, 113–127. [CrossRef]
7.
Knight, S.; Vrij, A.; Cherryman, J.; Nunkoosing, K. Attitudes towards animal use and belief in animal mind.
Anthrozoös 2004,17, 43–62. [CrossRef]
8.
Julius, H.; Beetz, A.; Kotrschal, K.; Turner, D.; Uvnäs-Moberg, K. Attachment to Pets: An Integrative View of
Human-Animal Relationships with Implications for Therapeutic Practice. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2015,76, e234.
9. Salomon, A. Animals and Children: The Role of the Pets; Canada’s Mental Health: Kenora, ON, Canada, 1981.
10.
Williams, J.M.; Muldoon, J.; Lawrence, A. Children and their pets: Exploring the relationships between pet
ownership, pet attitudes, attachment to pets and empathy. Educ. Health 2010,29, 12–15.
11.
Kruger, K.M.; Merrill, S.R. Walthman Pocket Book of Human-Animal Interactions; Beyond Design Solutions Ltd.:
Leicester, UK, 2012.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 205 10 of 11
12.
Melson, G.F. Child Development and the Human-Companion Animal Bond. Am. Behav. Sci.
2003
,47, 31–39.
[CrossRef]
13.
Svensson, A.S. The impact of the animals on children’s learning and their development—A study of what
children learn from and with pets: The example of dog and cat. Probl. Educ. 2014,59, 77–85.
14.
Endenburg, N.; van Lith, H.A. The influence of animals on the development of children. Vet. J.
2011
,190,
208–214. [CrossRef]
15.
Glascoe, F.P. Screening for developmental and behavioral problems. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev.
2005,11, 173–179. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
16.
Michikawa, T.; Nitta, H.; Nakayama, S.F.; Yamazaki, S.; Isobe, T.; Tamura, K.; Suda, E.; Ono, M.; Yonemoto, J.;
Iwai-Shimada, M.; et al. Baseline Profile of Participants in the Japan Environment and Children’s Study
(JECS). J. Epidemiol. 2018,28, 99–104. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
17.
Kawamoto, T.; Nitta, H.; Murata, K.; Toda, E.; Tsukamoto, N.; Hasegawa, M.; Yamagata, Z.; Kayama, F.;
Kishi, R.; Ohya, Y.; et al. Rationale and study design of the Japan environment and children’s study (JECS).
BMC Public Health 2014,14, 25. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
18.
Michikawa, T.; Nitta, H.; Nakayama, S.F.; Ono, M.; Yonemoto, J.; Tamura, K.; Suda, E.; Ito, H.; Takeuchi, A.;
Kawamoto, T.; et al. The Japan Environment and Children’s Study (JECS): A Preliminary Report on Selected
Characteristics of Approximately 10,000 Pregnant Women Recruited During the First Year of the Study.
J. Epidemiol. 2015,25, 452–458.
19.
Squires, J.T.; Bricker, E.; Potter, D.L. (ASQ-3) Ages & Stages Questionnaire; Brookes Publishing: Baltimore, MD,
USA, 2009; p. 256.
20.
Nozadi, S.S.; Li, L.; Cliord, J.; Du, R.; Murphy, K.; Chen, L.; Navajo Birth Cohort Study Team; Seanez, P.;
Burnette, C.; MacKenzie, D.; et al. Use of Ages and Stages Questionnaires (ASQ) in a Navajo population:
Comparison with the U.S. normative dataset. Child Care Health Dev. 2019,45, 709–718. [CrossRef]
21.
Mezawa, H.; Aoki, S.; Nakayama, S.F.; Nitta, H.; Ikeda, N.; Kato, K.; Tamai, S.; Takekoh, M.; Sanefuji, M.;
Ohga, S.; Oda, M.; et al. Psychometric profiles of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires, Japanese translation.
Pediatr. Int. 2019,61, 1086–1095. [CrossRef]
22.
Frankenburg, W.K. Denver II–Denver Developmental Screening, Second Version; Nihon Shoni Iji Shuppannsha:
Tokyo, Japan, 2009.
23.
Association, J.P.F. Ownership of Dogs and Cats Survey Results. 2018. Available online: https://petfood.or.jp/
topics/img/181225.pdf (accessed on 17 September 2019).
24. Sato, R.; Fujiwara, T.; Kino, S.; Nawa, N.; Kawachi, I. Pet Ownership and Children’s Emotional Expression:
Propensity Score-Matched Analysis of Longitudinal Data from Japan. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health
2019
,
16, 758. [CrossRef]
25.
Westgarth, C.; Heron, J.; Ness, A.R.; Bundred, P.; Gaskell, R.M.; Coyne, K.P.; German, A.J.; McCune, S.;
Dawson, S. Family pet ownership during childhood: Findings from a UK birth cohort and implications for
public health research. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2010,7, 3704–3729. [CrossRef]
26.
Purewal, R.; Christley, R.; Kordas, K.; Joinson, C.; Meints, K.; Gee, N.; Westgarth, C. Companion Animals
and Child/Adolescent Development: A Systematic Review of the Evidence. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health
2017,14, 234. [CrossRef]
27.
Elmaci, D.T.; Cevizci, S. Dog-assisted therapies and activities in rehabilitation of children with cerebral palsy
and physical and mental disabilities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015,12, 5046–5060. [CrossRef]
28.
Gee, N.R.; Harris, S.L.; Johnson, K.L. The role of therapy dogs in speed and accuracy to complete motor
skills tasks for preschool children. Anthrozoös 2007,20, 375–386. [CrossRef]
29.
Hurley, K.; Oakes, L.M. Infants’ Daily Experience with Pets and Their Scanning of Animal Faces. Front. Vet.
Sci. 2018,5, 152. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
30.
Poresky, R.H. Companion animals and other factors aecting young children’s development. Anthrozoös
1996,9, 159–168. [CrossRef]
31.
Hediger, K.; Turner, D.C. Can dogs increase children’s attention and concentration performance?
A randomised controlled trial. Hum. Anim. Interact. Bull. 2014,2, 21–39.
32.
Gee, N.R.; De, M.; Riley, W.; Belcher, J.M.; Grabski, J.L. The presence of a therapy dog results in improved
object recognition performance in preschool children. Anthrozoos 2012,25, 289–300. [CrossRef]
33.
Gee, N.R.; Church, M.T.; Altobelli, C.L. Preschoolers make fewer errors on an object categorization task in
the presence of a dog. Anthrozoös 2010,23, 223–230. [CrossRef]
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 205 11 of 11
34.
Gee, N.R.; Crist, E.N.; Carr, D.N. Preschool children require fewer instructional prompts to perform a memory
task in the presence of a dog. Anthrozoös 2010,23, 173–184. [CrossRef]
35.
Gee, N.R.; Sherlock, T.R.; Bennett, E.A.; Harris, S.L. Preschoolers’ adherence to instructions as a function of
presence of a dog and motor skills task. Anthrozoös 2009,22, 267–276. [CrossRef]
36.
Hawkins, R.D.; Williams, J.M.; Scottish, S. Society for The Prevention of Cruelty to Animals Scottish,
Childhood Attachment to Pets: Associations between Pet. Attachment, Attitudes to Animals, Compassion,
and Humane Behaviour. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017,14, 490. [CrossRef]
37.
Payne, E.; Bennett, P.C.; McGreevy, P.D. Current perspectives on attachment and bonding in the dog-human
dyad. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 2015,8, 71–79. [CrossRef]
38.
Carlisle, G.K. The social skills and attachment to dogs of children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism.
Dev. Disord. 2015,45, 1137–1145. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
39.
Friesen, L. Exploring Animal-Assisted Programs with Children in School and Therapeutic Contexts. Early
Child. Educ. J. 2010,37, 261–267. [CrossRef]
40.
Hall, S.S.; Gee, N.R.; Mills, D.S. Children Reading to Dogs: A Systematic Review of the Literature. PLoS ONE
2016,11, e0149759. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
41.
Robertson, I.D.; Irwin, P.J.; Lymbery, A.J.; Thompson, R.C. The role of companion animals in the emergence
of parasitic zoonoses. Int. J. Parasitol. 2000,30, 1369–1377. [CrossRef]
42.
Collin, S.M.; Granell, R.; Westgarth, C.; Murray, J.; Paul, E.S.; Sterne, J.A.; Henderson, A.J. Associations of Pet.
Ownership with Wheezing and Lung Function in Childhood: Findings from a UK Birth Cohort. PLoS ONE
2015,10, e0127756. [CrossRef]
43.
Voith, V.L. The impact of companion animal problems on society and the role of veterinarians. Vet. Clin.
N. Am. Small Anim. Pract. 2009,39, 327–345. [CrossRef]
44.
McNicholas, J.; Gilbey, A.; Rennie, A.; Ahmedzai, S.; Dono, J.A.; Ormerod, E. Pet ownership and human
health: a brief review of evidence and issues. BMJ 2005,331, 1252–1254. [CrossRef]
45.
Schalamon, J.; Ainoedhofer, H.; Singer, G.; Petnehazy, T.; Mayr, J.; Kiss, K.; Höllwarth, M.E. Analysis of dog
bites in children who are younger than 17 years. Pediatrics 2006,117, e374–e379. [CrossRef]
46.
Reisner, I.R.; Nance, M.L.; Zeller, J.S.; Houseknecht, E.M.; Kassam-Adams, N.; Wiebe, D.J. Behavioural
characteristics associated with dog bites to children presenting to an urban trauma centre. Inj. Prev.
2011
,17,
348–353. [CrossRef]
47.
Reisner, I.R.; Shofer, F.S.; Nance, M.L. Behavioral assessment of child-directed canine aggression. Inj. Prev.
2007,13, 348–351. [CrossRef]
48.
Daniels, D.M.; Ritzi, R.B.; O’Neil, J.; Scherer, L.R. Analysis of nonfatal dog bites in children. J. Trauma.
2009
,
66 (Suppl. S3), S17–S22. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
49.
Grandgeorge, M.; Tordjman, S.; Lazartigues, A.; Lemonnier, E.; Deleau, M.; Hausberger, M. Does pet arrival
trigger prosocial behaviors in individuals with autism? PLoS ONE 2012,7, e41739. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
50.
Furukawa, T.A.; Kawakami, N.; Saitoh, M.; Ono, Y.; Nakane, Y.; Nakamura, Y.; Tachimori, H.; Iwata, N.;
Uda, H.; Nakane, H.; Watanabe, M. The performance of the Japanese version of the K6 and K10 in the World
Mental Health Survey Japan. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 2008,17, 152–158. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
51.
Westgarth, C.; Boddy, L.M.; Stratton, G.; German, A.J.; Gaskell, R.M.; Coyne, K.P.; Bundred, P.; McCune, S.;
Dawson, S. Pet ownership, dog types and attachment to pets in 9–10 year old children in Liverpool, UK.
BMC Vet. Res. 2013,9, 102. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
52. Bradley, R.H.; Caldwell, B.M. The relation of infants’ home environments to achievement test performance
in first grade: A follow-up study. Child Dev. 1984,55, 803–809. [CrossRef]
©
2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
... regular yogurt consumption tended to be associated with a lower rate of sleep deprivation in a general pediatric population [4]. Sleep disorders are frequently associated with neurodevelopmental disorders, suggesting that there is some association between yogurt consumption habits and neurodevelopment [5,6]The mechanism by which the gut microbiota influences neurodevelopment is thought to involve the action of neurotransmitters such as GABA in favor of certain gut bacteria as well as the inhibition of inflammation in the gut [10][11][12][13][14]. ...
... The ASQ-3 consists of a set of well-validated questionnaires and has been recommended by the United Nations Children's Fund to verify whether children have typical neurological development. The Japanese version of the ASQ-3 has been validated [10] and has been used in several previous studies [11,12]. The ASQ-3 assesses development in the following five domains: (a) communication: language skills, such as babbling, vocalizing, listening, and understanding; (b) gross motor: arm, body, and leg movements during movement and play; (c) fine motor: hand and finger movements; (d) problem solving: problem-solving skills, learning, and playing with toys; and (e) personal-social: self-help skills, solitary social play, and play with toys and others. ...
... In addition, we selected several variables in terms of the possibility of impact on exposure and/or outcome. The categorization of these variables was performed according to usual medical practice or common practice in Japan and/or with reference to our previous studies [11,12,16,17]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Multiple observational studies have demonstrated the health benefits of yogurt, which are considered due to yogurt’s positive effects on the gut microbiota. It is also known that the intestinal microbiota is associated with cognitive and emotional functions. Although the intake of probiotics has been reported to improve neurodevelopmental symptoms in children, no large-scale studies have examined the association of yogurt consumption in early childhood with later neurodevelopmental status. In this study, we examined the association between frequency of such consumption at 1 year of age and the children’s subsequent neurodevelopmental status. Methods We studied children’s dietary consumption of yogurt at age 1 year and their neurodevelopment at age 3 years from data gathered from70,276 mother–child pairs enrolled in the Japan Environment and Children’s Study. We collected data from pregnant women whose consent was obtained after the study was explained to them at participating facilities in the target area. Frequency of yogurt consumption was categorized into 0, 1–2, 3–4, and ≥ 5 times/week based on a questionnaire about the child’s diet completed by the mother. Developmental delay was assessed using the Ages and Stages Questionnaires, Third Edition (ASQ-3™) in five domains: communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem solving, and personal-social. Using the results of the group that did not consume yogurt as a reference, multivariate logistic regression analysis was performed to compare the neurodevelopment of children according to frequency of yogurt consumption. For the covariates, items related to the socio-economic background and children’s neurodevelopment were selected with reference to previous studies. Results Consumption of yogurt 1–4 times/week was associated with a reduced risk of developmental delay in all ASQ-3 categories(adjusted odds ratios, 0.71–0.87). However, the risk of developmental delay was not necessarily reduced with yogurt consumption ≥5 times/week (adjusted odds ratios, 0.84–0.96). Conclusion Yogurt consumption habits at 1 year of age were associated with a lower risk of developmental delay at 3 years of age. However, the association was less apparent when yogurt was consumed more frequently. Possible mechanisms by which yogurt intake affects neurodevelopment include neurotransmitters produced by intestinal bacteria as well as the suppression of intestinal inflammation through improvements in the intestinal environment. Regular intake of yogurt in early childhood may have a positive association with neurodevelopment, but it is hoped that clearer links will be found in the future through intervention studies.
... In contrast to infants living without a dog, infants living with a dog showed decreased risks of developmental delay in the gross motor, problem-solving, communication, and personal-social domains. Interestingly, the authors did not find similar results in the case of cat owners (Minatoya et al., 2020). In young children, the positive effects of companion animals on cognitive development, and more specifically on language skills, might be explained by the fact that pets can function as both a receiver for the child's babble and as a source of environmental stimulation eliciting pet-directed communication (Endenburg & van Lith, 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
Dogs are popular pets among families with children. From the parents’ point of view, dog ownership can help children to become more mature and responsible. Children can develop a close relationship with the family dogs, becoming involved in their care and participating in daily activities together. Children can also meet dogs outside their homes, for example, as part of educational programmes. The aim of our study is to review how rich behavioural interactions with dogs (e.g., playing, petting, talking to the dog) can have an effect on typical children’s socio-cognitive development and the psychological and physiological mechanisms behind these effects. Positive effects include increased social competence, improved reading skills and higher motivation to learn, which may be explained by the child’s attachment to the pet and the effect of social buffering. In addition to the potential benefits, we also mention the costs of dog ownership in relation to child-dog interactions. Dog bites and allergies represent a significant risk, and in the case of problem behaviour or financial problems, the presence of a dog can lead to stress in the family, and the focus of the parents’ attention may be diverted from the child. We also review the main methodological limitations of studies on human-animal interactions, such as small sample sizes, small effect sizes, lack of control for confounding effects, self-report bias, lack of longitudinal studies, variability in dog behaviour and husbandry habits, and different study methods. We conclude that a large proportion of the studies on this topic are correlational, which does not allow causality to be established. Although dog ownership may be associated with certain developmental benefits, this does not necessarily mean that the presence of a dog directly causes these outcomes. Other factors, such as family environment, socioeconomic status and parental involvement, may also play a significant role, but these have rarely been examined in previous research.
... While the presence of a dog can yield beneficial outcomes for children in the classroom, living with a pet dog in the home is thought to provide even greater improvements in child well-being. Owning a pet dog with an infant child in the household was associated with a decrease in the risk of the infant experiencing developmental delays [28]. Children may be more motivated when in the presence of a pet dog due to the belief that the dog gives them full attention [29]. ...
Article
Full-text available
An increasing body of evidence indicates that owning a pet dog is associated with improvements in child health and well-being. Importantly, the degree of the social bond between child and dog may mediate the beneficial outcomes of dog ownership. The formation of social bonds is an intrinsically dyadic, interactive process where each interactor’s behavior influences the other’s behavior. For this reason, it is critical to evaluate the biological mechanisms of attachment in both children and their pet dogs as a socially bonded pair. Here, we review the physical, mental, and emotional outcomes that are associated with pet dog ownership or interaction in children. We then discuss the evidence that suggests that the strength of a social bond between a child and their pet dog matters for maximizing the beneficial outcomes associated with pet dog ownership, such as possible stress-buffering effects. We review the existing literature on the neural and endocrinological mechanisms of social attachment for inter-species social bonds that form between human children and dogs, situating this emerging knowledge within the context of the mechanisms of intra-species bonds in mammals. Finally, we highlight the remaining open questions and point toward directions for future research.
... Parents may skip an item if they are not sure how to answer it or concerned about their poor child's performance. If one or two scores were missing, the average score of the remaining items was substituted to calculate the overall score [15]. Following the recommended ASQ-3 procedures, modified age was used to determine the ASQ-3 for preterm infants (<37 weeks gestation). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Social relationships are essential in maintaining the physical and mental health of mothers and their children. However, there is limited evidence on how social support provided to the mother during pregnancy could impact child development. Herein, we examined whether maternal social support levels during pregnancy was associated with the risk of developmental delay in 3-year-old children. Methods: Overall, 68,442 mother-child pairs completed questionnaires on maternal social support during pregnancy and development delay in 3-year-old children. The maternal social support level was evaluated using four items. The risk of development delay was evaluated using the Japanese version of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire-3 (ASQ-3) with five domains of communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem-solving, and personal-social. Odds ratios (OR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) were calculated using logistic regression according to the quintiles of maternal social support levels after adjusting for potential confounding factors. Results: Social support during pregnancy was associated with a lower risk of development delay at 3 years of age. Beneficial effects were detected in all domains of the ASQ-3 (p for trend <0.001). Multivariable ORs (95% CIs) for the highest versus lowest quartiles of maternal social support level were 0.57 (0.50–0.65) for communication, 0.49 (0.43–0.55) for gross motor delay, 0.58 (0.53–0.64) for fine motor delay, 0.56 (0.51–0.62) for problem-solving delay, and 0.52 (0.45–0.60) for personal social delay. The associations remained unchanged when stratified by maternal education level, paternal education level, living with children, household income, and postpartum depression. Conclusion: Maternal social support during pregnancy was inversely associated with the risk of developmental delay at 3 years of age.
... The quantitative use of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires R , Third Edition (ASQ-3), a parent-completed method for screening children at risk of developmental delay in children between the ages of 1 month and 5½ years, has been validated in epidemiological studies (19). The Japanese version of the ASQ-3 has also been validated (20) and has been used in several studies (21,22). The ASQ-3 assesses the following 5 developmental domains: (a) communication: items related to language skills, such as babbling, vocalizing, listening, and understanding, including being able to answer to their own name and give simple directions using words; (b) gross motor: items related to arm, body, and leg movements during movement and play, such as being able to kick a ball and jump; (c) fine motor: items measuring hand and finger movements, such as the ability to use scissors to cut paper and to use pencils and crayons appropriately; (d) problem-solving: items related to problem-solving skills, learning, and playing with toys, such as reciting numbers and being able to arrange blocks in the same way as demonstrated; and (e) personal-social: items related to self-help skills, solitary social play, and play with toys and others, such as being able to dress oneself and keep order. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Results on the association between fish intake during pregnancy and a reduction in neurodevelopmental delays in children have been inconsistent, with some reports finding an association and others finding none. Because neurodevelopmental delays are more pronounced at the age of 3 years, their association needs to be examined at this age. Methods After exclusion and multiple imputation from a dataset comprising 104,057 records from the Japan Environment and Children's Study, logistic regression analysis was conducted in quintiles to evaluate the association between maternal fish intake during pregnancy and child neurodevelopment at age 3 years in 91,909 mother–child pairs. The Food Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ), validated in the Japan Public Health Center-Based Prospective Study for the Next Generation, was used to assess maternal fish intake during pregnancy. The Ages and Stages Questionnaires-3 was used to assess children's neurodevelopment in five domains: communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem-solving, and personal-social. Results Consistently lower odds were found for the highest vs. lowest quintile for the domains of communication, fine motor, problem-solving, and personal-social but not gross motor skills, with adjusted odd ratios (95% confidence intervals) of 0.89 (0.80–0.998), 0.90 (0.83–0.97), 0.86 (0.80–0.94), 0.87 (0.77–0.98), and 1.04 (0.94–1.16), respectively. The trend for lower odds of symptoms of neurodevelopmental delays across quintiles of higher maternal fish intake were significant for fine motor, problem-solving, and personal-social but not communication or gross motor. Conclusions Fish consumption during pregnancy may be associated with a reduced risk of neurodevelopmental delay in 3-year-olds, particularly in the fine motor, problem-solving, and personal-social domains. Continued investigation after the age of 3 could further clarify the association.
... Indeed, self-report studies have demonstrated links between pet ownership in childhood and higher self-esteem (Van Houtte & Jarvis, 1995), enhanced emotion regulation (Bryant & Donnellan, 2007), and greater empathy and theory of mind (e.g., Christian et al., 2020;Daly & Morton, 2006Jacobson & Chang, 2018;Svensson, 2014). Furthermore, longitudinal work has shown that living with a pet dog at 6 months of age was associated with a lower risk of showing developmental delays at 12 months in communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem-solving, and personal-social domains as measured via parent report (Minatoya et al., 2020). At age 3, having lived with a pet dog remained associated with a lower risk of developmental delays in the communication, gross motor, problem-solving, and personalsocial domains (Minatoya et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Living with a pet is related to a host of socioemotional health benefits for children, yet few studies have examined the mechanisms that drive the relations between pet ownership and positive socioemotional outcomes. The current study examined one of the ways that pets may change the environment through which children learn and whether childhood pet ownership might promote empathy and prosocial behavior through parent–child conversations about emotions and mental states in the presence of a pet dog. Participants included 123 parent (118 mothers, four fathers) and child (65 female, 58 male, Mage = 39.50 months, 75 White, not Hispanic, nine Asian/Pacific Islander, seven Hispanic, five Black/African American, two South Asian/Indian, two American Indian/Alaska Native, two “other,” 21 more than one race, 111 residing in the United States) dyads currently living with a pet dog (n = 61) or having never lived with a pet dog (n = 62). As hypothesized, we found that parents used a greater proportion of emotion and mental state language with their children when playing with their pet dog than with a lifelike toy, suggesting that the presence of a household pet may be one context used to promote conversations about emotions and mental states.
... Considering the scope of this study, the effect of animals on the risk factors that children attending secondary school may encounter is an important issue. In terms of the effect of animals on child development, it is emphasized that having animals at an early age can reduce the risk of developmental delay in children (Minatoya et al., 2020). ...
Article
With this group work, which includes activities for animal rights, it is aimed that children can raise awareness about animal rights and interact with animals. The research was carried out as a pretest-posttest experimental study in order to evaluate the effect of group work on children receiving counselling and training from a Guidance and Research Center in Istanbul. The study group of the research between May 2022 and June 2022 consists of 20 children (10 in the experimental group, 10 in the control group) between the ages of 11–12 and continuing their education in Istanbul. In addition to the Personal Information Form, which included questions such as gender, age, education status, number of siblings, socio-economic levels, and status of living with pets, the Oney Animal Love Scale and Scale of Behaviour for Animals were administered to the study group as pretest-posttest. The group work was held on Saturdays at 13:00, consisted of six sessions, and in each session, activities for socialization, animal rights and child–animal interaction were carried out. The collected data were analysed in the SPSS 22.0 package program. At the end of the group work, it was observed that there was a positive significant difference in the total scores of the Oney Animal Love and Scale of Behaviour for Animals of the children in the experimental group.
Article
Full-text available
Personal wellbeing is greatly influenced by our childhood and adolescence, and the relationships formed during those phases of our development. The human-dog bond represents a significant relationship that started thousands of years ago. There is a higher prevalence of dog ownership around the world, especially in households including children. This has resulted in a growing number of researchers studying our interactions with dogs and an expanding evidence base from the exploration of child-dog interactions. We review the potential effects of child-dog interactions on the physical, mental, and social wellbeing of both species. A search of the SCOPUS database identified documents published between January 1980 and April 2022. Filtering for key inclusion criteria, duplicate removals, and inspecting the references of these documents for additional sources, we reviewed a total of 393 documents, 88% of which were scientific articles. We were able to define the numerous ways in which children and dogs interact, be it neutral ( e.g ., sharing a common area), positive ( e.g ., petting), or negative ( e.g ., biting). Then, we found evidence for an association between childhood interaction with dogs and an array of benefits such as increased physical activities, a reduction of stress, and the development of empathy. Nonetheless, several detrimental outcomes have also been identified for both humans and dogs. Children are the most at-risk population regarding dog bites and dog-borne zoonoses, which may lead to injuries/illness, a subsequent fear of dogs, or even death. Moreover, pet bereavement is generally inevitable when living with a canine companion and should not be trivialized. With a canine focus, children sometimes take part in caretaking behaviors toward them, such as feeding or going for walks. These represent opportunities for dogs to relieve themselves outside, but also to exercise and socialize. By contrast, a lack of physical activity can lead to the onset of obesity in both dogs and children. Dogs may present greater levels of stress when in the presence of children. Finally, the welfare of assistance, therapy, and free-roaming dogs who may interact with children remains underexplored. Overall, it appears that the benefits of child-dog interactions outweigh the risks for children but not for dogs; determination of the effects on both species, positive as well as negative, still requires further development. We call for longitudinal studies and cross-cultural research in the future to better understand the impact of child-dog interactions. Our review is important for people in and outside of the scientific community, to pediatricians, veterinarians, and current or future dog owners seeking to extend their knowledge, and to inform future research of scientists studying dogs and human-animal interactions.
Preprint
Full-text available
Our wellbeing is greatly influenced by our childhood and adolescence, and the relationships that we form during those phases of our development. The human-dog bond started thousands of years ago. The higher prevalence of dog ownership around the world, especially in households including children along with the growing number of people studying dogs most likely explain the growing literature focusing on child-dog interactions. We review the potential effects of child-dog interactions on the physical, mental, and social wellbeing of both species. A scoping search of the SCOPUS database found several hundred documents meeting selection criteria. It allowed us to define the numerous ways in which children and dogs can interact, be it neutral (e.g., sharing a common area), positive (e.g., petting), or negative (e.g., biting). Then, we found evidence for an association between interacting with dogs during childhood and an array of health and mental benefits like stress relief and the development of empathy. Walking a dog and playing with one are perfect physical activity opportunities. Additionally, interacting with a dog can help lower stress and may have a role in the development of empathy. Nonetheless, a number of detrimental outcomes have also been identified in both humans and dogs. Children are the most at-risk population regarding dog bites and dog-borne zoonoses, which may lead to a subsequent fear of dogs or even death. Moreover, pet bereavement is generally inevitable when living with a canine companion and should not be trivialized. In terms of dogs, children sometimes take part in caretaking behaviors toward them which include going on walks. They are opportunities for dogs to relieve themselves outside, but also to exercise and socialize. In contrast, a lack of physical activity can lead to the onset of obesity. Dogs may present greater levels of stress when in the presence of children. Finally, the welfare of assistance, therapy, and free-roaming dogs remains underexplored. Overall, the study of the effects, positive as well as negative, on both species still requires further development. We call for more longitudinal studies and hope for cross-cultural research in the future in order to better understand the impact child-dog interactions might have.
Article
Full-text available
Background: This study aimed to determine the psychometric profiles of 10 questionnaires (every 6 months, from 6 to 60 months) from the Japanese translation of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires, third edition (J-ASQ-3). Methods: Data from 439 children in a birth cohort were used to identify the J-ASQ-3 score distribution, establish cutoff scores, and calculate the instrument's internal consistency. Data were also collected from 491 outpatients to examine J-ASQ-3 test-retest reliability and concurrent validity, which was examined using the Kyoto Scale of Psychological Development and the Japanese version of the Denver Developmental Screening Test. Both the original and the alternative screening criteria of the ASQ-3 were used (failure in at least one and at least two domains, respectively). Results: Cronbach's alpha for each J-ASQ-3 subscale on each questionnaire ranged from .45 to .89. Test-retest reliabilities were over .75 for the subscales on almost all questionnaires. Concurrent validity was also adequate. In comparison with the screening results of the Kyoto Scale of Psychological Development, the overall sensitivity and specificity were 96.0% and 48.8%, respectively, when the ASQ-3 original criterion was used, and 92.1% and 74.9%, respectively, when the alternative criterion was used. In comparison with the screening results of the Japanese version of the Denver Developmental Screening Test, the overall sensitivity and specificity were 75.6% and 74.7%, respectively, when the ASQ-3 original criterion was used, and 56.3% and 93.0%, respectively, when the alternative criterion was used. Conclusions: This study quantified the psychometric profiles of the Japanese translations of 10 ASQ-3 questionnaires. We demonstrated the validity of the J-ASQ-3 and determined new cutoff scores. Further studies with larger samples from a greater range of locations are required to clarify the suitability of this tool for all Japanese children.
Article
Full-text available
With many children and young adolescents reporting strong emotional bonds with their pets, the impact of pet ownership on child/adolescent health—especially on their emotional development—has garnered increasing scientific interest. We examined the association between pet ownership in toddlerhood (age 3.5 years) and poor emotional expression in later childhood (age 5.5 years) using propensity score matching within a longitudinal cohort dataset from Japan (n = 31,453). A propensity score for pet ownership was calculated by logistic models based on a comprehensive list of each child’s observed characteristics, including sex, household income, parental education, mother’s employment status, residential environment, number of siblings, and living arrangement. Log-binomial regression analyses using matched samples revealed that children who owned pets during the toddler years were 6% less likely to have a poor emotional expression in later childhood (prevalence ratio = 0.94, 95% confidence interval = 0.90–0.99) compared to those without pets. This suggests that owning pets may provide children with opportunities to control their emotions, and lead to a lower prevalence of poor emotional expression. Pet ownership in toddlerhood may contribute to the development of expression.
Article
Full-text available
Very little is known about the effect of pet experience on cognitive development in infancy. In Experiment 1, we document in a large sample (N = 1270) that 63% of families with infants under 12 months have at least one household pet. The potential effect on development is significant as the first postnatal year is a critically important time for changes in the brain and cognition. Because research has revealed how experience shapes early development, it is likely that the presence of a companion dog or cat in the home influences infants' development. In Experiment 2, we assess differences between infants who do and do not have pets (N = 171) in one aspect of cognitive development: their processing of animal faces. We examined visual exploration of images of dog, cat, monkey, and sheep faces by 4-, 6-, and 10-month-old infants. Although at the youngest ages infants with and without pets exhibited the same patterns of visual inspection of these animals faces, by 10 months infants with pets spent proportionately more time looking at the region of faces that contained the eyes than did infants without pets. Thus, exposure to pets contributes to how infants look at and learn about animal faces.
Article
Full-text available
Background: The Japan Environment and Children’s Study (JECS), known as Ecochil-Chosa in Japan, is a nationwide birth cohort study investigating the environmental factors that might affect children’s health and development. We report the baseline profiles of the participating mothers, fathers, and their children. Methods: Fifteen Regional Centres located throughout Japan were responsible for recruiting women in early pregnancy living in their respective recruitment areas. Self-administered questionnaires and medical records were used to obtain such information as demographic factors, lifestyle, socioeconomic status, environmental exposure, medical history, and delivery information. In the period up to delivery, we collected bio-specimens, including blood, urine, hair, and umbilical cord blood. Fathers were also recruited, when accessible, and asked to fill in a questionnaire and to provide blood samples. Results: The total number of pregnancies resulting in delivery was 100,778, of which 51,402 (51.0%) involved program participation by male partners. Discounting pregnancies by the same woman, the study included 95,248 unique mothers and 49,189 unique fathers. The 100,778 pregnancies involved a total of 101,779 fetuses and resulted in 100,148 live births. The coverage of children in 2013 (the number of live births registered in JECS divided by the number of all live births within the study areas) was approximately 45%. Nevertheless, the data on the characteristics of the mothers and children we studied showed marked similarity to those obtained from Japan’s 2013 Vital Statistics Survey. Conclusions: Between 2011 and 2014, we established one of the largest birth cohorts in the world.
Article
Full-text available
Attachment to pets has an important role in children’s social, emotional, and cognitive development, mental health, well-being, and quality of life. This study examined associations between childhood attachment to pets and caring and friendship behaviour, compassion, and attitudes towards animals. This study also examined socio-demographic differences, particularly pet ownership and pet type. A self-report survey of over one thousand 7 to 12 year-olds in Scotland, UK, revealed that the majority of children are strongly attached to their pets, but attachment scores differ depending on pet type and child gender. Analysis revealed that attachment to pets is facilitated by compassion and caring and pet-directed friendship behaviours and that attachment to pets significantly predicts positive attitudes towards animals. The findings have implications for the promotion of prosocial and humane behaviour. Encouraging children to participate in pet care behaviour may promote attachment between children and their pet, which in turn may have a range of positive outcomes for both children (such as reduced aggression, better well-being, and quality of life) and pets (such as humane treatment). This study enhances our understanding of childhood pet attachment and has implications for humane education and promoting secure emotional attachments in childhood.
Article
Full-text available
Childhood and adolescence are important developmental phases which influence health and well-being across the life span. Social relationships are fundamental to child and adolescent development; yet studies have been limited to children’s relationships with other humans. This paper provides an evidence review for the potential associations between pet ownership and emotional; behavioural; cognitive; educational and social developmental outcomes. As the field is in the early stages; a broad set of inclusion criteria was applied. A systematic search of databases and grey literature sources found twenty-two studies meeting selection criteria. The review found evidence for an association between pet ownership and a wide range of emotional health benefits from childhood pet ownership; particularly for self-esteem and loneliness. The findings regarding childhood anxiety and depression were inconclusive. Studies also showed evidence of an association between pet ownership and educational and cognitive benefits; for example, in perspective-taking abilities and intellectual development. Evidence on behavioural development was unclear due to a lack of high quality research. Studies on pet ownership and social development provided evidence for an association with increased social competence; social networks; social interaction and social play behaviour. Overall, pet ownership and the significance of children’s bonds with companion animals have been underexplored; there is a shortage of high quality and longitudinal studies in all outcomes. Prospective studies that control for a wide range of confounders are required.
Article
Full-text available
Background Despite growing interest in the value of human-animal interactions (HAI) to human mental and physical health the quality of the evidence on which postulated benefits from animals to human psychological health are based is often unclear. To date there exist no systematic reviews on the effects of HAI in educational settings specifically focussing on the perceived benefits to children of reading to dogs. With rising popularity and implementation of these programmes in schools, it is essential that the evidence base exploring the pedagogic value of these initiatives is well documented. Methods Using PRISMA guidelines we systematically investigated the literature reporting the pedagogic effects of reading to dogs. Because research in this area is in the early stages of scientific enquiry we adopted broad inclusion criteria, accepting all reports which discussed measurable effects related to the topic that were written in English. Multiple online databases were searched during January-March 2015; grey literature searches were also conducted. The search results which met the inclusion criteria were evaluated, and discussed, in relation to the Oxford Centre for Evidence Based Medicine levels of evidence; 27 papers were classified as Level 5, 13 as Level 4, 7 as Level 2c and 1 as Level 2b. Conclusion The evidence suggests that reading to a dog may have a beneficial effect on a number of behavioural processes which contribute to a positive effect on the environment in which reading is practiced, leading to improved reading performance. However, the evidence base on which these inferences are made is of low quality. There is a clear need for the use of higher quality research methodologies and the inclusion of appropriate controls in order to draw causal inferences on whether or how reading to dogs may benefit children’s reading practices. The mechanisms for any effect remain a matter of conjecture.
Article
The purpose of this study is to produce knowledge about the impact of pets on preschool children's desire to learn and their development. The research question asked is: What do children learn from and with pets: The example of dog and cat? Socio-cultural theories and developmental pedagogy are used in order to study children’s learning in the preschool. The socio-cultural theories are based on a model describing interactions between people presented by Vygotskij (1978) and further developed by Säljö (2011). This study employed a qualitative exploratory research method with the aim of gaining knowledge concerning a group of 24 children, aged 4-5 years. Individual interviews were used as the method of data collection. The results are presented as two main categories: 1) The pet supports the child in their learning and development process 2) The child is the teacher of the pet. Each main category comprised two subcategories: 1a) Developing empathy and emotions, 1b) Being good at school-related tasks, 2a) Teaching the pet playful exercises, 2b) Teaching the pet to obey. The results show that pets provide children with positive experiences and a sense of feeling good. The children said that they could talk to all the animals and that the animals are listening. When a child is doing activities with an animal or tries to teach an animal some exercises, the child receives a response from the animal. This leads on to new experiences and an increased knowledge regarding social behavior. Using pets for educational purposes in the home, preschool and school, might stimulate an interest in animal care and a commitment to the world´s wildlife and fauna in general. Key words: animal, children, development, preschool learning.
Article
Background: The Ages & Stages Questionnaires (ASQ-3) is a parent-completed screening to identify young children at-risk for developmental delays in the United States and internationally. Federal programs operating on Navajo Nation use the ASQ-3 to determine the need for early intervention services, even though the ASQ-3 national sample used to establish cut-off scores for referral included only 1% Native American children. Objectives: The current study aimed to compare the ASQ-3 results from a sample of Navajo infants to those from a representative national U.S. sample, and to examine the specificity and sensitivity of the ASQ-3 in Navajo population. Methods: The sample included 530 Navajo infants (47.3% males) aged between 1 and 13 months who lived in remote and rural areas across the Navajo Nation. Children's development was assessed during home visits at 2, 6, 9, and 12-month assessment windows. Results: Results showed that after 6 months, Navajo children had lower mean scores and higher percentages of children at-risk for developmental delays than those from the national sample. The sensitivities and specificities, estimated using a Bayesian diagnostic approach under both conservative and non-conservative prior range choices, suggested a comparable validity performance to that from other ASQ-3 studies. Discussion: The results of this study along with our ongoing comprehensive assessments at 4 years of age inform current programs working with Navajo children to improve early identification of developmental delays.
Article
Numerous studies have examined the link between companiondog ownership and improved physical or psychological health outcomes; however, few have examined the association between dog ownership and owners’ overall wellbeing or quality of life (QOL). Moreover, the potential for dog ownership to adversely affect owner QOL has been largely ignored. In general, little is known regarding the specific aspects of QOL that are most affected by dog ownership, and there are no instruments specifically designed to assess the QOL of dog owners. If such a tool was available, these effects could be better quantified, understood, and potentially utilized to further improve owner QOL. The aim of the present study was to develop and validate a QOL questionnaire specifically suitable for use in dog owners. A literature review, focus-group activities, and expert content review were used to generate an item pool for the instrument. Both positive and negative effects of dog ownership on QOL were considered. Developmental and validation versions of a QOL instrument were tested using exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis, respectively. The validity and reliability of the QOL instrument were tested using goodness of fit indices and Cronbach’s alpha. The study produced the 10-item Dog Owner Quality Of Life Questionnaire (DOQOL) comprised of 3 different factors: emotional wellbeing, social and physical activities, and stress and responsibility of dog ownership. The factor structure had good fit and internal reliability. The DOQOL represents a suitable tool to measure QOL in dog owners, and future use could improve our understanding of the relationship between dog ownership and QOL.