ArticlePDF Available

“Do as I do, not as I say”: Using social learning theory to unpack the impact of role models on students' outcomes in education

Authors:

Abstract

Role modeling has received much attention in education research, uncovering the mechanisms by which imitation occurs (e.g., the aspects of role models that make them more or less effective) and identifying the outcomes associated upon imitating the model. Nonetheless, certain social‐cognitive processes involved in role modeling tend to be overlooked. This oversight is puzzling given that these processes, such as retention and reproduction of modeled behavior, are of great importance to role modeling processes—the consideration and inclusion of such processes can provide crucial insight. This paper provides an overview of the role model research in education to date, detailing researchers' focus and emphasis on identifying aspects of role model effectiveness. We then analyze how including the component processes of social learning or observational theory can add value and application to advance role modeling research. Finally, we provide recommendations to close the gap between current research trends and what has been previously theorized on modeling to help inform ongoing future investigations.
ARTICLE
Do as I do, not as I say: Using social learning
theory to unpack the impact of role models
on students' outcomes in education
Janet N. Ahn
1
| Danfei Hu
2
| Melissa Vega
3
1
William Paterson University
2
The Pennsylvania State University
3
New York University
Correspondence
Janet N. Ahn, Department of Psychology,
William Paterson University, 300 Pompton
Road, Wayne, NJ 07470.
Email: ahnj9@wpunj.edu
Abstract
Role modeling has received much attention in education
research, uncovering the mechanisms by which imitation
occurs (e.g., the aspects of role models that make them
more or less effective) and identifying the outcomes associ-
ated upon imitating the model. Nonetheless, certain social-
cognitive processes involved in role modeling tend to be
overlooked. This oversight is puzzling given that these pro-
cesses, such as retention and reproduction of modeled
behavior, are of great importance to role modeling
processesthe consideration and inclusion of such pro-
cesses can provide crucial insight. This paper provides an
overview of the role model research in education to date,
detailing researchers' focus and emphasis on identifying
aspects of role model effectiveness. We then analyze how
including the component processes of social learning or
observational theory can add value and application to
advance role modeling research. Finally, we provide recom-
mendations to close the gap between current research
trends and what has been previously theorized on modeling
to help inform ongoing future investigations.
1|INTRODUCTION
It goes without saying that when people are asked to identify who or what greatly influenced them to become the
person they are today, many do not hesitate in naming specific individuals such as close family members, admired
celebrities, or whomever they deem to be role models. In the broadest sense, role models are people who have or
had a profound and significant (usually positive) impact on a person's life. Role models exemplify specific goals,
DOI: 10.1111/spc3.12517
Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2019;e12517. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/spc3 © 2019 John Wiley & Sons Ltd 1of12
https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12517
behaviors, and strategies that role aspirants (people exposed to role models who consciously and even unconsciously
follow in the latter's footsteps) internalize and imitate. Role models are particularly useful in the field of education as
a source of inspiration, providing roadmaps for possible career paths and enhancing motivation along the way.
There are two influential role model theories in the current literature: the stereotype inoculation model (SIM,
Dasgupta, 2011) and the motivational theory of role modeling (MTRM, Morgenroth, Ryan, & Peters, 2015). The ste-
reotype inoculation model focuses on in-group role models and defines them as accomplished individuals who share
common characteristic(s) (e.g., race and gender) with certain underrepresented and negatively stereotyped groups in
high-achieving domains (e.g., women in STEM). In-group role models influence role aspirants by alleviating their con-
cerns about group representation in the stereotyped domain and thereby inoculating them from stereotype threat.
Based on this definition, Dasgupta and colleagues argue that in-group role models can be anyone, from widely
renowned experts to peers whom one regularly interacts with in real life. While SIM focuses on how role models can
benefit minority or negatively stereotyped groups, the MTRM provides a much broader definition, describing role
models as individuals who influence role aspirants' achievements, motivation, and goals by acting as behavioral
models, representations of the possible, and/or inspirations(Morgenroth et al., 2015, p. 4). This definition delineates
three distinct functions that role models serve in influencing role aspirants: acting as behavioral models, representing
what is achievable, and being inspirational. In the current paper, we adhere to the MTRM's conceptualization of role
models and define them as individuals who exemplify specific goals, behaviors, and strategies that role aspirants
(people exposed to role models who consciously and even unconsciously follow in the latter's footsteps) internalize
and imitate.
As such, the literature on role models in education sheds light onto the various factors that impact role models'
effectiveness, particularly the aspects of the role model (e.g., competence and perceived similarity), and uncovers the
wide range of consequences that occur when role aspirants imitate role models. Despite its expansiveness and infor-
mative insights, the current role model literature in education tends to disproportionately focus on the attentional
and motivationalprocesses of role modeling (i.e., the aspects that tend to attract observers and role aspirants
1
and
the favorable and unfavorable outcomes associated with imitating the role model), and does not consider on balance
the other processes relevant in modeling that facilitate behavior change and/or imitation in the role aspirants
mainly the retentionand motor reproductionprocesses (see Bandura, 1972, 1997, 2005). In this vein, the current
understanding of role models does not draw on social-cognitive literature (e.g., social learning theory) to grasp how
role models function and work, even though role models are often seen as those who inspire others to exemplify
similar actions and behaviors. Addressing this disregard is pressing, since research on social-cognitive processes is
greatly informative for the most relevant processes to role modeling such as goal setting, goal striving, and behavior
change (e.g., Bandura, 1977; Fryling, Johnston, & Hayes, 2011; Manz & Sims, 1981; Schunk, 2012). Lastly, the pre-
dominant focus of research on role models in education has been to identify and examine the key attributes that role
models should possess to be considered effective (i.e., more directly focusing on role models). What tends to be lac-
king is the considerable influence role aspirants themselves can exert (e.g., their ambitions, motivation, choices, and
accomplishments) on the role modeling processes. That is, a role model's effectiveness cannot exclusively be viewed
unilaterallyinstead his or her effectiveness is largely contingent on and intertwined with the learning processes on
the side of role aspirants (Morgenroth et al., 2015). This dynamic process is often neglected in the current view of
role models in education but can be supported by integrating social-cognitive theories, specifically observational
learning theory (Bandura, 1965, 1969, 1977).
In this paper, we first review social or observational learning theory, specifying the relevant component pro-
cesses that influence the observer's ability to imitate the model. We then provide a targeted recapitulation of domi-
nant trends in the current work on role models, focusing on educational settings. Next, we focus on the disconnect
between these two streams of literature and shed light on how bridging the gap between these areas can help
researchers discern how to investigate modeling behavior going forward. Lastly, we provide suggestions for future
role model work and interventions to incorporate all component processes as laid out in observational learning the-
ory to better understand the functions and capabilities of role models.
2of12 AHN ET AL.
2|SOCIAL OR OBSERVATIONAL LEARNING THEORY
In the 60s and 70s, Albert Bandura and his colleagues made a concerted effort to evaluate and highlight the role of
observation as a necessary antecedent to social learning. In reaction to the predominant understandings of how indi-
viduals acquire knowledge via the unconscious in the psychoanalytic approach (Freud, 1949), the predetermined
sequence of stages in developmental theories (Erikson, 1950; Gesell, Ilg, Learned, & Ames, 1943; Piaget, 1948, 1954;
Sullivan, 1953) and even the direct experience of successive reinforcement in operant conditioning theory (Skinner,
1953), Bandura emphasized the role of social-cognitive processes associated with observing models. He very loosely
defined models as performerswhose behavior conveys information to observers about the characteristics of
appropriate responsesto display in a certain situation (Bandura, 1972, p. 37). Further, observers are to abstract
common attributes exemplifiedby this model and then generate a behavior that resembles that which the model is
inclined to exhibit under similar circumstances(Bandura, 1972, p. 37). These matching phenomena,of observers
attempting to exhibit similar behaviors as the model, are known as modeling.
At the core of observational learning theory is understanding the role of modeling in inspiring learning and imita-
tion in observers. Specifically, a desired behavior can be acquired by merely observing a model who demonstrates
that behavior, without (the observer) having to personally and directly experience the consequences associated with
engaging in that behavior (Bandura, 2005; Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963; S. K. Gibson, 2004b; Horn & Williams,
2004). In a seminal experiment, children were exposed to one of four types of models- a model who either displayed
acts of aggression and was rewarded for such behavior, a model who was aggressive then punished, or children were
alternately placed in one of two control conditions, in which they observed a non-aggressive model or no model at
all. Importantly, the greatest amount of imitative behavior was exhibited in the aggressive-rewarded group than in
the other three groups. Bandura concluded that by observing a model of the desired behavior, an individual forms
an idea of how response components must be combined and sequenced to produce the new behavior(Bandura,
1977, p. 35). Though such a notion seems rather simple and intuitive, the implications it has for learning and imitative
behavior is significant yet frequently overlooked.
2.1 |Processes of observational learning
According to Bandura (1969, 1971, 1977), there are four critical processes involved in observational learning:
(a) attention, (b) retention, (c) motor reproduction, and (d) motivation and reinforcement. First, attentional processes
matter because simply exposing persons to sequences of modeling stimuli does not in itself guarantee that they will
attend closely to the cues(Bandura, 1972, p. 41). That is, a particular model must attract the attention of an
observer, and the observer should perceive that model as worthy of imitation. This process is critical in determining
whether or not the observer will model the observed behavior. Second, in the retention process, the observer should
retain and register the original observational inputs or actions of the model in some symbolic form via imagery or
verbal coding. Additionally, repetitive rehearsal of registered imagery or verbal elements of the model's behavior can
significantly heighten the retention of observed behavior. Next, motor reproduction processes involves the use of
symbolic representations of modeled patterns to guide over performances(Bandura, 1972, p. 46). In other words,
an observer behaviorally follows a series of instructions to enact novel behaviors that are not externally prompted or
depicted but instead are reproduced internally through symbolic representations of the behavior as guides to appro-
priate action(Manz & Sims, 1981, p. 107). Lastly, motivational and reinforcement processes refer to the perceived
favorable or unfavorable consequences of mimicking the model's actions that are likely to increase or decrease the
likelihood of imitation.
According to Bandura, each of these components is crucial in determining whether or not imitation occurs upon
exposure to a model. It is noteworthy that observational theory placed much emphasis on the attentional, cognitive,
AHN ET AL.3of12
and motivational processes associated with the observer rather than the model. And yet, in the current view of role
modeling in educational settings, the opposite is trueheavy emphasis has been placed on the respective processes
associated with the model more than the observer (D. E. Gibson, 2004a; Morgenroth et al., 2015). Additionally, the
existing role model literature frequently hones in on the attentional and motivational processes relevant to modeling
(e.g., aspects that attract the observer to the model and the favorable or unfavorable outcomes associated with imi-
tating the model) while largely neglecting the other very relevant processes that matter for modeling according to
observational learning theory, such as the retention and motor production processes. In the following section, we
provide an overview of the current role model literature in education to highlight its preexisting disconnect from
observational learning theory and then suggest ways to integrate these two areas into a more coherent framework
for considering role modeling processes and outcomes.
3|ROLE MODELS IN EDUCATION
The majority of research investigating role models in education can be summed up in two streams: (a) identifying key
aspects of role models that make them effective, or ineffective (see Betz & Sekaquaptewa, 2012; Hurd,
Zimmerman, & Xue, 2009; Lockwood, Jordan, & Kunda, 2002), in influencing role aspirants and (b) examining the
outcomes (mostly favorable) associated with imitating the model. These streams reflect what Bandura refers to as
the attentionaland motivational and reinforcementprocesses of modeling.
3.1 |Attentional process: Aspects of role models that attractrole aspirants
First, an effective role model should demonstrate competence and attainable success in the desired or relevant
domain. Competence matters because role models are those who exhibit skills that others lack and are subsequently
motivated to learn from them (e.g., Kemper, 1968; Marx & Ko, 2012; Marx & Roman, 2002). For example, Marx and
Roman (2002) observed that exposure to a highly competent female role model in math tended to buffer women's
math test performance (a notably stereotypically threatening situation), as opposed to exposure to a less competent
female model in math. Additionally, a role model's accomplishments should be deemed attainable (e.g., Hoyt &
Simon, 2011; Lockwood & Kunda, 1997, 1999). Attainability is important because it influences role aspirants' expec-
tations regarding the kind of success that is possible to achieve. For instance, Lockwood and Kunda (1997) found
that freshmen in college, as compared to the seniors, were more motivated by a successful senior student's academic
achievements. The researchers reasoned that the freshmen were more motivated than their fourth year counterparts
because, with a few years still ahead of them, the role model's success seemed more attainable.
Second, an effective role model is someone that others can identify as similar or self-relevant (e.g., Lockwood &
Kunda, 1997; Marx & Ko, 2012). Perceived similarity is critical because upward social comparisons are particularly
self-enhancing when shared similarities with a superior other are highlighted (Collins, 1996, 2000). Perceived similar-
ity can be acquired from a number of sources, such as shared group membership (e.g., the same gender or race), simi-
lar past experiences, or common interests (Dasgupta, 2011; Marx & Goff, 2005; Marx, Ko, & Friedman, 2009;
Marx & Ko, 2012; O'Brien et al., 2017). For instance, Cheryan, Siy, Vichayapai, Drury, and Kim (2011) found that a
greater sense of perceived similarity engendered anticipated success in the specified field. Women who interacted
with models who seemed more similar and relatable (i.e., a model who seemed to defy his or her stereotypical role,
such as a computer scientist who dressed casually and appeared socially competent) versus those who were less sim-
ilar (i.e., a model who exemplified his or her stereotype, such as a computer scientist who dressed like a nerd and
appeared to lack interpersonal skills) increased their interest in computer science. Further, reminding women of other
women's achievements (e.g., role models) can alleviate the detrimental effects of women's mathematics stereotypes.
4of12 AHN ET AL.
For instance, McIntyre, Paulson, and Lord (2003) demonstrated that women performed significantly better on a
quantitative GRE exam when they first read biographical essays of women who succeeded in various professions.
A third aspect that is important in affecting a role model's effectiveness is how role models earn their success. It
has been suggested that people are more likely to benefit from a role model's success if the said success is attributed
to internal, controllable, and stable factors rather than success attributable to external, uncontrollable, and unstable
factors (Weiner, 1979, 1985). For example, effort, relative to inborn talent, is perceived as more controllable in the
sense that it is more subject to volitional control(e.g., Good, Aronson, & Inzlicht, 2003; Weiner, 1979, 1985).
Research has shown that girls performed as well as boys on a difficult math task when exposed to a hardworking role
model whose success was attributed to effort versus innate talent. Conversely, girls' performance worsened, com-
pared to boys, when exposed to a role model whose success was attributed to innate talent (Bàges, Verniers, &
Martinot, 2016).
3.2 |Motivational process: Outcomes associated with imitation
Another area that has received considerable attention in role model research is the examination and identification of
the outcomes (mostly favorable) for observers when models are imitated. This process most closely resembles
Bandura's motivational process, which is the process of reinforcing the likelihood of imitation because of the asso-
ciated favorable outcomes. That is, observers will be motivated to perform the modeled behaviors for its rewarding
outcomes. Current research identified several valued outcomes that provide such rewards: an enhanced sense of
belonging (e.g., Dasgupta, 2011; Rosenthal, Levy, London, Lobel, & Bazile, 2013), increased self-efficacy (Stout,
Dasgupta, Hunsinger, & McManus, 2011), and increased achievement (Lin-Siegler et al., 2016).
One valued outcome of modeling in education is that it leads to an enhanced sense of belonging, especially for
typically negative-stereotyped groups in STEM fields (e.g., women and racial minorities; Dasgupta, 2011; Steele,
1997; Williams, Phillips & Hall, 2014). Indeed, women are stereotyped as being unsuited for STEM, since STEM is
widely seen as masculine and thus incongruent with their gender role, while racist stereotypes categorize African
Americans and Latinos as intellectually inferior to European Americans (Drury, Siy, & Cheryan, 2011; Schmader &
Johns, 2003; Steele & Aronson, 1995; Steele, 1997). Thus, these groups are expected to perform poorly in STEM,
since succeeding in this intellectually rigorous, male-dominated domain is perceived as unlikely with their capabilities
(Dasgupta, 2011; Steele, 1997). However, exposure to competent, in-group role models challenges such stereotypes
for these individuals by demonstrating that people like them belong and can flourish in these seemingly inauspicious
domains (Dasgupta, 2011). In one study (Rosenthal et al., 2013), undergraduate women pursuing a premedical track
either read narratives about thriving female physicians who defied stereotypes by succeeding in a male-dominated
career or were given no reading material. Women exposed to the narratives, relative to those who were not,
reported a greater sense of belonging in their premedical track. Such favorable outcome of imitating a model
(i.e., enhanced sense of belonging) has been one emphasis of current research in modeling in education.
Another favorable outcome associated with the motivational process is increased self-efficacy among observers.
Female undergraduates were taught introductory calculus for a semester by either female or male professors (i.e., in-
group or out-group role models). To measure self-efficacy, students reported their expected course grade at the
beginning and end of the semester. Women taught by female professors reported greater self-efficacy at both time
points compared to those who were taught by male professors. Notably, female students' self-reported identification
with the female professors predicted their increased self-efficacy. Further, women taught by a female professor
demonstrated more proactive in-class participation (i.e., answering more questions asked by the professors) by the
end of the semester relative to those taught by male professors (Stout et al., 2011). Such findings indicate that con-
tact with an efficacious, in-group role model encourages women to view themselves as similarly efficacious, thus
reinforcing observers' motivations to emulate the model.
AHN ET AL.5of12
Additionally, researchers have examined increased performance and achievement. One set of studies utilized
famous scientists (i.e., role models) to show students that they can similarly succeed in STEM by working hard to
overcome the inevitable academic obstacles in one's educational career (Lin-Siegler et al., 2016). In this work, high
school students read narratives that highlighted either how famous scientists overcame their struggles and
succeeded or narratives that solely emphasized the scientists' achievements. The researchers observed that students
who read the former experienced improved science class performance relative to those who only read about the sci-
entists' achievements. In other words, vicarious reinforcement (i.e., learning by observing how others succeed
through hard work and failures) increased students' own performance.
Research also highlights how two processes (e.g., the attentional and motivational processes) can coexist or co-
occur. For instance, Bàges, Verniers, and Martinot (2016) demonstrated that depicting how role models earn their
success is an aspect that strongly impacts a role aspirant's performance, which speaks to a combination of both
attentional and motivational processes. Relatedly, Shapiro, Williams, and Hambarchyan (2013) found that exposure
to a successful Black role model, relative to exposure to a White role model, acted as a buffer for Black students
against the negative effects of an induced group stereotype threat, as evidenced by increased performance on a
GRE-like exam. These findings display a combination of the attentional and motivational processes.
In sum, most of the role model research in education has focused primarily on two processes crucial to modeling:
(a) the attentional processes and (b) the motivational processes. However, what is most notably missing are the other
two processes of modeling: the retention (encoding of role models' actions) and motor reproduction processes
(behavioral replication of role models' actions). In the following section, we consider this gap between what has been
theorized and constructed in early modeling and what has and is being targeted in the current role model literature.
We then explore and provide recommendations to potentially bridge the gap between these areas.
4|BRIDGING THE GAP AND RECOMMENDATIONS FOR FUTURE
RESEARCH
Although Bandura acknowledges that the retention process is one that has virtually been ignored in theories of imi-
tation(Bandura, 1972, p. 41), he writes about it considerably, even detailing the various components that are
involved in the process of symbolically representing the model's behavior in the mind of the observer. In order for
observers to reproduce the modeled behavior, the observer should retain the model's observational inputs in some
symbolic form(Bandura, 1972, p. 43). Observers need to actively code and organize (through both imaginal and ver-
bal representations) the model's behavioral outputs into more readily accessible and memorable templates that will
ultimately guide the observer for subsequent response reproduction (Schunk, 2012; Renkl, 2014). According to
Bandura, this process, of the observer cognitively coding and internalizing the modeled behavior, is precisely how
observational learning should be better understood, rather than solely emphasizing the observers' ability to enact the
sequences of the model's behavior. Bandura and colleagues stressed the importance of symbolic coding in the acqui-
sition and retention of modeled behavior in several studies. They demonstrated that children who verbally coded the
modeled behaviors of the model tended to reproduce more matching responses than those who were not encour-
aged to verbally code (Bandura, Grusec, & Menlove, 1966; see also Gerst, 1971). More explicitly, coders,or those
who were asked to learn each modeled activity by dividing it into its individual components, were better at
reconstructing the modeled behavior than noncoders,or those who were left to their own devices to learn the
modeled activity (Bandura & Jeffrey, 1973).
Two, more recent sets of studies also explicate how coders,relative to noncoders,are better at retaining,
and ultimately reproducing, previously modeled behavior. Yi and Davis (2003) directly tested Bandura's theory by
examining whether incorporating symbolic coding into a role modeling-based training intervention enhanced partici-
pants' computer skill learning via facilitation of the retention process. Participants enrolled in a computer training
6of12 AHN ET AL.
workshop series in which an actor modeled the exact steps needed to enact a target computer skill. After watching
this modeled behavior, coderparticipants were instructed to write out key, summarizing points from the videos
and reread their summaries while visualizing themselves act out the summarized (i.e., verbally coded) behaviors as
many times as possible within 5 min. Noncoderparticipants skipped this step. All participants then practiced the
modeled behavior before completing a measure of observational learning and a series of knowledge and perfor-
mance tests. Results indicate that the retention enhancement activities improved participants' computer learning
and skill acquisition by strengthening the retention process. In the second set of studies, Davis and Yi (2004) repli-
cated these findings, further demonstrating that the incorporation of symbolic coding into role modeling
interventions strengthens the latter's effects on learning and computer task performance. Such evidence suggests
that this internalized, cognitive process of initial coding, storing, rehearsing, and ultimately retrieving from memory
what the observer symbolically represented in their mind about the model's behavior is crucial for observational
learning. Despite some empirical indication of this process, it is, for the most part, lacking in the current role model
literature.
Another process of modeling that has not received much attention is the motor reproduction process, which
involves the use of symbolic representations of modeled patterns to guide overt performances(Bandura, 1972,
p. 47). It is the process where the observer attempts to externally and behaviorally reproduce the model's behavior
as guided by the symbolic representations the observer internalized. To illustrate, imagine an observer (e.g., a female
student) is exposed to a role model's (e.g., a female scientist) narrative/biography. In this narrative, the student learns
how the female scientist struggled and failed on her route to eventual success. The student learns about the difficul-
ties that the scientist encountered, the actions taken to overcome, and the accomplishments that the scientist
achieved. By proceduralizing the path to success, the student can encode a behavioral script that she can follow
when she encounters similar difficulties and allows her to reproduce those actions since they have been specified by
the model. For instance, the script might detail how the female role model, during her time a as a student, sought out
her class's teaching assistant after doing poorly on a test and asked for help with identifying her areas of weakness
and, together, created an individualized problem set for her to practice with and thus strengthen her learning
process.
Importantly, the various component processes should be associated with distinct outcomes in education
(i.e., learning vs. performance/achievement). Learning is defined as relatively permanent changes in comprehension,
understanding, and sets of acquired skills that will support long-term retention and transfer,whereas achievement or
performance refers to the temporary fluctuations in behavior or knowledge that can be observed and measured dur-
ing or immediately after the acquisition process(Soderstrom & Bjork, 2015). Thus, we interpret the retention and
motor reproduction processes as associated with learning. These processes involve a highly cognitive component
that enables observers to imitate the model by via a feedback loop in which the observer adjusts and adapts their
behavior based on what was internalized. Notably, an internal feedback loop is crucial for learning (Narciss, 2008).
On the other hand, the motivational process is associated with achievement/performance since this process empha-
sizes the tangible, favorable outcomes upon imitation that reinforces role modeling.
In sum, the current role model research heavily emphasizes the achievementoutcome for students, or the
motivational process, rather than the learning outcome (e.g., retention and reproduction processes). However,
there are simple experimental techniques that can be applied in future research to ensure that observers (or role
aspirants) are indeed retaining and reproducing the model's behaviors. For instance, researchers can increase the
dosage of role model exposure. It is typical in role model literature, particularly for lab studies, that role aspirants
tend to be exposed to role models in brief and limited ways. Indeed, role aspirants tend to view a profile of the
role model (Pietri et al., 2018), or a single-paged resume of the role model (Marx & Ko, 2012), or read brief biog-
raphies or struggle narratives of the role model (e.g., Hong & Lin-Siegler, 2012; Lin-Siegler et al., 2016;
Lockwood & Kunda, 1997; Rosenthal et al., 2013; Shin, Levy, & London, 2016). However, it is worth considering
whether these types of exposure are truly enough for role aspirants to be able to retain and process key and
AHN ET AL.7of12
relevant components about the model. Research has suggested that increased/extensive exposure to role models
may produce better role modeling outcomes. For example, Asgari, Dasgupta, and Cote (2010) showed that fre-
quent contact, in terms of both quality (i.e., support from faculty) and quantity, with female faculty predicted
greater in-class participation among female college students such that those students were more assertive in
intellectual settings (i.e., classrooms). Thus, aside from doing a quick manipulation check that role aspirants have
basic memory recall of the role model, researchers should delve into what role aspirants internalize and retain
about the role model to ultimately enact the modeled behaviors. Future research should focus on either doing
more extensive testing (e.g., involving more cognitive tests) to see what role aspirants internalize from the model
exposure or increase the dosage of role model exposure (by repeated or prolonged exposure) to enhance the
opportunity for retention processing.
On this note, the motor reproduction process suggests an internal feedback loop in which the observer recur-
sively adjusts and adapts their behavior based on what they internalized about the model's behavior. This internal-
ized process, of reproducing the model's behavior, is less emphasized in the current role model literature.
Conversely, the majority of empirical research has disproportionally focused on depicting the success of role models
(e.g., what they have achieved), rather than the actions taken to succeed. A detailed behavioral script—“a procedural
knowledge structure or schema for understanding and enacting behavior(Manz & Sims, 1981, p. 527)is important
because the intrapersonal interpretation and imitation of role models' behavioral scripts are fundamental to vicarious
learning (e.g., Gioia & Manz, 1985; Manz & Sims, 1981). Based on the recall of those scripts, observers can structure
expectations about the necessary/appropriate steps to be taken to accomplish goals and about the likely subsequent
consequences that will result from their actions. However, presentation of such behavioral scripts or proceduralizing
the model's behavior is less emphasized in the extant empirical role model research (with the exception of some,
e.g., Hong & Lin-Siegler, 2012; Lin-Siegler et al., 2016). Going forward, researchers can improve in this regard by
decomposing the model's behavioral process, making it externally accessible and explicit to role aspirants, and also
paying more attention to the behavioral sequences of the role aspirants.
Finally, though Bandura exerted much effort detailing the observer or role aspirants' cognitive and motivational
process, the current literature on role models in education tend to place less emphasis in that regard. Of course,
there is some research that has investigated the role aspirants' processes (e.g., the effects of role aspirants' self-
esteem on imitative behavior, see Weiss, 1977, and cultural differences in role aspirants affect role model choices,
see Lockwood, Marshall, and Sadler, 2005). However, the span of work does not parallel Bandura's emphasis of its
importance to the modeling process and outcome. For instance, assessing the dynamic and mutual processes of how
an observer externalizes a model's behavioral sequence (as well as how the former internalizes, cognitively processes,
and registers such behavioral sequences before subsequently reproducing such behaviors) is entirely lacking in the
current investigations. More research should further consider the process of imitative learning and behavior for role
aspirants.
5|CONCLUSION
The extant role model literature in the context of education covers a wide range of topics in depth, such as modeling
mechanisms and outcomes, but tends to neglect crucial social-cognitive processes that prove useful in providing fur-
ther insights. Drawing heavily upon social learning or observational theory (Bandura, 1965, 1969, 1977), we incorpo-
rate both the attentional, cognitive, and motivational processes of role models, as well as these same processes
associated with role aspirants, to provide a more comprehensive and integrated understanding of role models in edu-
cation. We argue that linking these two bodies of literature can generate new research questions that better under-
stand the dynamics of the role modeling process and thus yield greater benefits for role aspirants.
8of12 AHN ET AL.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
This research was supported (in part) by a Summer Stipend from the Research Center for the Humanities and Social
Sciences at William Paterson University.
ORCID
Janet N. Ahn https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3266-9684
ENDNOTE
1
The term observerhas been used in observational learning theory to refer to the person who watches and observes the
model. In research on role models in education, those who observe models have been referred to as role aspirants”—in
this manuscript, we use these two terms interchangeably.
REFERENCES
Ahn, J. N., Luna-Lucero, M. L., Lamnina, M., Nightingale, M., Novak, D., & Lin, X. D. (2016). Motivating students' STEM learn-
ing using biographical information. International Journal of Designs for Learning,7(1), 7185.
Asgari, S., Dasgupta, N. & Gilbert Cote, N. (2010). When does contact with successful in group members change self-
stereotypes? A longitudinal study comparing the effect of quantity vs. quality of contact with successful individuals.
Social Psychologyy,41(3), 203211. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000028
Bagès, C., & Martinot, D. (2011). What is the best model for girls and boys faced with a standardized mathematics evalua-
tion situation: A hardworking role model or a gifted role model? British Journal of Social Psychology,50, 536543.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2010.02017.x
Bagès, C., Verniers, C., & Martinot, D. (2016). Virtues of a hardworking role model to improve girls' mathematics perfor-
mance. Psychology of Women Quarterly,40(1), 5564. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684315608842
Bandura, A. (1965). Behavioral modification through modeling procedures. In L. Krasner & L. P. Ullman (Eds.), Research in
behavior modification. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Bandura, A. (1971). Social learning theory. New York: General Learning Press.
Bandura, A. (1972). Modeling theory: Some traditions, trends, and disputes. In R. D. Parke (Ed.), Recent trends in social learn-
ing theory (pp. 35-61). New York: Academic Press.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Macmillan.
Bandura, A. (2005). The evolution of social cognitive theory. In Great minds in management (pp. 935).Oxford:OxfordUniv.Press.
Bandura, A., Grusec, J. E., & Menlove, F. L. (1966). Observational learning as a function of symbolization and incentive set.
Child Development,37, 499506. https://doi.org/10.2307/1126674
Bandura, A., & Jeffrey, R. W. (1973). Role of symbolic coding and rehearsal processes in observational learning. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,26(1), 122130. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034205
Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1963). Vicarious reinforcement and imitative learning. The Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology,67(6), 601607. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0045550
Betz, D. E., & Sekaquaptewa, D. (2012). My fair physicist? Feminine math and science role models demotivate young girls.
Social Psychological and Personality Science,3(6), 738746. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550612440735
Cheryan, S., Siy, J. O., Vichayapai, M., Drury, B. J., & Kim, S. (2011). Do female and male role models who embody STEM ste-
reotypes hinder women's anticipated success in STEM? Social Psychological and Personality Science,2(6), 656664.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611405218
Collins, R. L. (1996). For better or worse: The impact of upward social comparison on self-evaluations. Psychological Bulletin,
119(1), 5169. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.51
Collins, R. L. (2000). Among the better ones: Upward assimilation in social comparison. In J. Suls & L. Wheeler (Eds.), The Ple-
num series in social/clinical psychology. Handbook of social comparison: Theory and research (pp. 159171). Dordrecht,
Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Dasgupta, N. (2011). Ingroup experts and peers as social vaccines who inoculate the self-concept: The stereotype inocula-
tion model. Psychological Inquiry,22(4), 231246. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2011.607313
Davis, F. D., & Yi, M. Y. (2004). Improving computer skill training: Behavior modeling, symbolic mental rehearsal, and the role
of knowledge structures. Journal of applied psychology,89(3), 509.
Dean, J. (2014, March 14). Girls need coolrole models in math and engineering. The Times. Retrieved from http://www.
thetimes.co.uk/tto/business/industries/engineering/article4033193.ece
AHN ET AL.9of12
Drury, B. J., Siy, J. O., & Cheryan, S. (2011). When do female role models benefit women? The importance of differentiating
recruitment from retention in STEM. Psychological Inquiry,22(4), 265269. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2011.
620935
Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.
Freud, S. (1949). An outline of psychoanalysis. Oxford, England: W. W. Norton.
Fryling, M. J., Johnston, C., & Hayes, L. J. (2011). Understanding observational learning: An interbehavioral approach. The
Analysis of Verbal Behavior,27(1), 191203.
Gerst, M. S. (1971). Symbolic coding processes in observational learning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,19(1),
717. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031029
Gesell, A., Ilg, F. L., Learned, J., & Ames, L. B. (1943). Infant and child in the culture of today. Oxford, England: Harper.
Gibson, D. E. (2004a). Role models in career development: New directions for theory and research. Journal of vocational
behavior,65(1), 134156.
Gibson, S. K. (2004b). Social learning (cognitive) theory and implications for human resource development. Advances in
Developing Human Resources,6(2), 193210.
Gioia, D. A., & Manz, C. C. (1985). Linking cognition and behavior: A script processing interpretation of vicarious learning.
Academy of Management Review,10(3), 527539. https://doi.org/10.2307/258134
Gonzales, P. M., Blanton, H., & Williams, K. J. (2002). The effects of stereotype threat and double-minority status on the test
performance of Latino women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,28(5), 659670. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0146167202288010
Good, C., Aronson, J., & Inzlicht, M. (2003). Improving adolescents' standardized test performance: An intervention to
reduce the effects of stereotype threat. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,24(6), 645662. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.appdev.2003.09.002
Hong, H., & Lin-Siegler, X. (2012). How learning about scientists' struggles influences students' interest and learning in phys-
ics. Journal of Educational Psychology,104, 469484. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026224
Horn, R. R., & Williams, A. M. (2004). Observational learning: Is it time we took another look? In A. M. Williams &
N. J. Hodges (Eds.), Skill acquisition in sport (pp. 199230). Abingdon: Routledge.
Hoyt, C. L., & Simon, S. (2011). Female leaders: Injurious or inspiring role models for women? Psychology of Women Quar-
terly,35(1), 143157. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684310385216
Hurd, N. M., Zimmerman, M. A., & Xue, Y. (2009). Negative adult influences and the protective effects of role models: A
study with urban adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,38, 777789.
Kemper, T. D. (1968). Reference groups, socialization and achievement. American Sociological Review,33,3145. https://doi.
org/10.2307/2092238
Lin-Siegler, X., Ahn, J. N., Chen, J., Fang, F. F. A., & Luna-Lucero, M. (2016). Even Einstein struggled: Effects of learning about
great scientists' struggles on high school students' motivation to learn science. Journal of Educational Psychology,108(3),
314328. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000092
Lockwood, P., Jordan, C. H., & Kunda, Z. (2002). Motivation by positive and negative role models: Regulatory focus deter-
mines who will best inspire us. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,83, 854864. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
3514.83.4.854
Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1997). Superstars and me: Predicting the impact of role models on the self. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology,73(1), 91103. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.91
Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1999). Increasing the salience of one's best selves can undermine inspiration by outstanding role
models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,76(2), 214228. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.214
Lockwood, P., Marshall, T. C., & Sadler, P. (2005). Promoting success or preventing failure: Cultural differences in motivation
by positive and negative role models. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,31(3), 379392. https://doi.org/10.
1177/0146167204271598
Manz, C. C., & Sims, H. P., Jr. (1981). Vicarious learning: The influence of modeling on organizational behavior. Academy of
Management Review,6(1), 105113. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1981.4288021
Marx, D. M., & Goff, P. A. (2005). Clearing the air: The effect of experimenter race on target's test performance and subjec-
tive experience. British Journal of Social Psychology,44(4), 645657. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466604X17948
Marx, D. M., & Ko, S. J. (2012). Superstars likeme: The effect of role model similarity on performance under threat.
European Journal of Social Psychology,42(7), 807812. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1907
Marx, D. M., Ko, S. J., & Friedman, R. A. (2009). The Obama effect: How a salient role model reduces race-based perfor-
mance differences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,45(4), 953956. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.
03.012
Marx, D. M., & Roman, J. S. (2002). Female role models: Protecting women's math test performance. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin,28(9), 11831193. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672022812004
10 of 12 AHN ET AL.
McIntyre, R. B., Paulson, R. M., & Lord, C. G. (2003). Alleviating women's mathematics stereotype threat through salience of
group achievements. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,39(1), 8390. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-1031(02)
00513-9
Morgenroth, T., Ryan, M. K., & Peters, K. (2015). The motivational theory of role modeling: How role models influence role
aspirants' goals. Review of General Psychology,19(4), 465483. https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000059
Narciss, S. (2008). Feedback strategies for interactive learning tasks. In J. M. Spector, M. D. Merrill, J. J. G. van
Merrienboer, & M. P. Driscoll (Eds.), Handbook of research on educational communications and technology: A project for the
association for educational communications and technology (3rd ed., pp. 125144). Mawah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
O'Brien, L. T., Hitti, A., Shaffer, E., Camp, A. R. V., Henry, D., & Gilbert, P. N. (2017). Improving girls' sense of fit in science:
Increasing the impact of role models. Social Psychological and Personality Science,8(3), 301309. https://doi.org/10.
1177/1948550616671997
Peacock, L. (2012, April 14). Women need role models who can juggle home and work life, says Rothschild MD Sian
Westerman. The Telegraph. Retrieved from https://www.telegraph.co.uk/finance/jobs/9203658/Women-need-role-
models-who-can-juggle-home-and-work-life-says-Rothschild-MD-Sian-Westerman.html.
Piaget, J. (1948). The moral judgment of the child. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child (M. Cook, Trans.). New York, NY, US: Basic Books. https://doi.org/10.
1037/11168-000
Pietri, E. S., Johnson, I. R., & Ozgumus, E. (2018). One size may not fit all: Exploring how the intersection of race and gender
and stigma consciousness predict effective identity-safe cues for Black women. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
74, 291306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.06.021
Rainey, K., Dancy, M., Mickelson, R., Stearns, E., & Moller, S. (2018). Race and gender differences in how sense of belonging
influences decisions to major in STEM. International Journal of STEM Education,5(10), 114. https://doi.org/10.1186/
s40594-018-0115-6
Renkl, A. (2014). Toward an instructionally oriented theory of example-based learning. Cognitive science,38(1), 137.
Rosenthal, L., Levy, S. R., London, B., Lobel, M., & Bazile, C. (2013). In pursuit of the MD: The impact of role models, identity
compatibility, and belonging among undergraduate women. Sex Roles,68(7-8), 464473.
Schmader, T., & Johns, M. (2003). Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,85(3), 440452. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.440
Schunk, D. H. (2012). Learning theories an educational perspective (6th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson Education Inc.
Shapiro, J. R., Williams, A. M., & Hambarchyan, M. (2013). Are all interventions created equal? A multi-threat approach to
tailoring stereotype threat interventions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,104(2), 277288. https://doi.org/
10.1037/a0030461
Shin, J. E., Levy, S. R., & London, B. (2016). Effects of role model exposure on STEM and non-STEM student engagement.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology,46(7), 410-427. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12371
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. Oxford, England: Macmillan.
Soderstrom, N. C., & Bjork, R. A. (2015). Learning versus performance: An integrative review. Perspectives on Psychological
Science,10(2), 176199. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615569000
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist,
52(6), 613629. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,69(5), 797811. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797
Stout, J. G., Dasgupta, N., Hunsinger, M., & McManus, M. A. (2011). STEMing the tide: Using ingroup experts to inoculate
women's self-concept in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM). Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology,100(2), 255270. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021385
Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York, NY, US: W. W. Norton & Co.
Weiner, B. (1979). A theory of motivation for some classroom experiences. Journal of Educational Psychology,71(1), 325.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.71.1.3
Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review,92(4), 548573.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.92.4.548
Weiss, H. M. (1977). Subordinate imitation of supervisor behavior: The role of modeling in organizational socialization. Orga-
nizational Behavior and Human Performance,19(1), 89105. https://doi.org/10.1016/0030-5073(77)90056-3
Williams, J., Phillips, K. W., & Hall, E. V. (2014). Double jeopardy? Gender bias against women of color in science. Hastings
College of the Law, Center for WorkLife Law.
Yi, M. Y., & Davis, F. D. (2003). Developing and validating an observational learning model of computer software training
and skill acquisition. Information Systems Research,14(2), 146169.
AHN ET AL.11 of 12
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHIES
Dr. Janet N. Ahn is an Assistant Professor of Psychology at William Paterson University and the Director of the
Motivation and Innovation (M+I) Lab. She received her BA in Psychology from Barnard College, Columbia Uni-
versity and completed her PhD in Social Psychology from New York University. She has also completed a post-
doctoral fellowship at Teachers College, Columbia University. Her research investigates human goal pursuit and
motivation. She has examined how people's goal pursuit is affected by what they infer about others' goals (via
projection) and what they believe about others based on their attributes (via transference and stereotyping). She
currently investigates interventions to increase students' motivation and persistence in STEM fields as well as
developing innovative technological tools to increase student retention in college. She has published in more
than a dozen journals including recent publications on gendered distributions of mental labor in heterosexual
relationships (Motivation Science, 2019) and self-assessment interventions in physics learning for high school stu-
dents (Educational Psychology, 2019). Additionally, her work has been featured in various media outlets such as
Women's Health Magazine, NPR, American Educator, the Business Insider, CBS, and USA Today.
Danfei Hu is a third year PhD student in the Department of Psychology at the Pennsylvania State University.
She obtained her MA in Cognitive Science and MS in Applied Statistics from Teachers College, Columbia Univer-
sity prior coming to Penn State. Danfei is broadly interested in the cognitive and motivational components of
emotional processing and emotion regulation. She is also interested in questions related to how stigmatized
group members are influenced by the stereotypes that they face.
Melissa Vega is a second year master's student in the General Psychology MA Program at New York University.
She graduated from Northwestern University in 2015 with a BA in Psychology and a minor in Gender Studies.
Prior to studying at NYU, she was the lab manager for the Education for Persistence and Innovation Center
(EPIC), an interdisciplinary research center at Teachers College, Columbia University. She is interested in the
influence of sociocultural group stereotypes on individuals' attitudes towards diversity and perceptions of threat
and discrimination. She is also interested in the environmental cues that aggravate gender and racial imbalances
in academic and organizational settings.
How to cite this article: Ahn JN, Hu D, Vega M. Do as I do, not as I say: Using social learning theory to
unpack the impact of role models on students' outcomes in education. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2019;
e12517. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12517
12 of 12 AHN ET AL.
... The simplicity of exposure to a role model renders this type of intervention more attractive than other complex interventions (e.g., growth mindset). There are two influential role model theories in the literature (Ahn et al., 2020): the stereotype inoculation model (Dasgupta, 2011) and the motivational theory of role modeling (Morgenroth et al., 2015). According to Dasgupta (2011), contact with in-group role models functions as a "social vaccine" that inoculates minority-group members against stereotype threat. ...
... This dynamic process is often overlooked in the current view of role models in education. Integrating socio-cognitive theories, in particular observational learning theory (Bandura, 1965(Bandura, , 1969(Bandura, , 1977 would then be a very promising avenue (Ahn et al., 2020). Further research is therefore needed to draw firmer conclusions about the impact of counter-stereotypical role models on role aspirants and to incorporate the other processes that may shape girls' aspirations and attitudes in STEM fields. ...
... The simplicity of exposure to a role model renders this type of intervention more attractive than other complex interventions (e.g., growth mindset). There are two influential role model theories in the literature (Ahn et al., 2020): the stereotype inoculation model (Dasgupta, 2011) and the motivational theory of role modeling (Morgenroth et al., 2015). According to Dasgupta (2011), contact with in-group role models functions as a "social vaccine" that inoculates minority-group members against stereotype threat. ...
... This dynamic process is often overlooked in the current view of role models in education. Integrating socio-cognitive theories, in particular observational learning theory (Bandura, 1965(Bandura, , 1969(Bandura, , 1977 would then be a very promising avenue (Ahn et al., 2020). Further research is therefore needed to draw firmer conclusions about the impact of counter-stereotypical role models on role aspirants and to incorporate the other processes that may shape girls' aspirations and attitudes in STEM fields. ...
Article
Full-text available
From 2015 to 2018, the math gender gap decreased primarily due to a decline in boys' performances (OECD, 2015, 2016, 2018). However, there is ample evidence that girls continue to be negatively stereotyped in math. Using a longitudinal design, we examined whether prolonged exposure to a counter-stereotypical role model embodied by a female top math scorer may prevent other girls in the class from experiencing stereotype threat. Multilevel analyses were conducted among 1,043 6th graders nested in 46 math classes. There was a decline in math performance throughout the school year for all students, but being a girl had a buffering effect against this decline. The results failed to support the main effect hypothesis (H1) which anticipated that student gender and top math scorer gender would be associated with student math achievement when jointly considered. The results supported the cross-level interaction hypothesis (H2) which anticipated that the greatest benefits would emerge for girls exposed to a counter-stereotypical role model; that is, in a class whose top math scorer was a girl. These results offer new insights regarding the extent to which a counter-stereotypical role model embodied by the top math scorer may influence differences in math performances.
... see alsoAhn et al., 2020), people do not copy any observed action per se. Instead, four constituent processes are crucial to obtain imitative behaviors and observational learning: (I) Models have to attract observers' interest and appear worthy of imitation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Observing how another person responds to a stimulus creates stimulus-response (SR) episodes. These can be retrieved from memory on later occasions, which means that observed responses are utilized for regulating one’s own actions. Until now, evidence for storage and retrieval of observationally acquired SR episodes was limited to dyadic face-to-face interactions between two partners who respond in alternating fashion. In two pre-registered studies (total N=252), we demonstrate for the first time that observational SR episodes can also be acquired in online interactions: Robust retrieval effects emerged when observers believe to be interacting with another person. In turn, retrieval effects were absent when observers believe to be interacting with a computer. Our findings show that feature-based binding and retrieval principles are pervasive and also apply to social interactions, even under purely virtual conditions. We discuss implications of our findings for different explanatory accounts of social modulations of automatic imitation. Keywords: observational stimulus-response bindings, event files, observational learning, episodic retrieval, online interactions.
... Panutan sangat penting pada bidang pendidikan sebagai sumber inspirasi. Panutan dapat menyediakan peta jalan untuk dilalui seseorang dan meningkatkan motivasi dalam usaha mencapai tujuan yang ditetapkan oleh orang tersebut (Ahn et al., 2020). Hal tersebut dialami oleh informan pada penelitian ini yang menemukan inspirasi seorang youtuber. ...
Article
Full-text available
Low achievement and motivation are problems that have often been faced in the field of mathematics education. The objective of this study was to explore how the student of prospective mathematics teachers get a starting point or turning point so that they had inspired to learn mathematics. The findings of this study had expected to help for solving the problem of low achievement and motivation. This type of research was a qualitative case study. The subjects of the research were mathematics education students who met certain criteria. The finding based on interviews and forms showed the inspiration had arisen when: the student had the opportunity to study with a teacher who was good at explaining, patient, friendly to students, humorous, giving compliments and reward when the student had gotten achievements; student realized that he or she good at mathematics; student realized that mathematics was challenging; student found motivators and role models for learning mathematics. The sources of inspiration were closely related to the source of self-efficacy. Educators had expected to integrate these sources of inspiration in learning mathematics. Key words: case study, inspiration, mathematics education, motivation, self-efficacy
... The performance area called Criteria Explanation corresponds to the planning of lessons and definition of objectives to be attained, whereas the Illustration performance area relates to the organization and development of the learning process. This last area of didactic performance of the professor implies that the professor demonstrates what students must do through actions and modeling (Ahn et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
The use of scales to assess the performance of professors from the students' standpoint is a generalized practice in higher education systems worldwide. The purpose of this study is to analyze the factorial structure and measure the invariance of the Scale of Teaching Performance of the Psychology Professor (EDDPsic) among groups according to gender, age, and academic stage. The sample of participants was composed of 316 Psychology students from the fourth and sixth semesters (basic cycles), and from the eighth and tenth semesters (disciplinary-professional cycles) of two renowned public universities in Lima, Peru. Two hundred and thirty-one participants were women (73%), and the mean age of students was 21.5 years old ( SD = 2.37). The measurement invariance of the scale in the three study variables was underpinned by a multigroup confirmatory factor analysis (MGCFA) conducted using a five-factor model that showed the best fitness indices. It is concluded that significant differences in measuring teaching performance areas of the professor depend on the students' age difference and on their academic stage (to attend the disciplinary-professional cycles).
... 22 It is built on social constructivist theories of learning but situates experience at the core of the learning process. 23 For example, a hands-on clinical course that teaches a student how to reinsert an avulsed tooth into the alveolar socket after a dental injury. Finally, Bandura a social learning theorist, 24 suggested that people learn best within social contexts, and concepts such as modelling, observational learning and imitation facilitate learning. ...
Article
Full-text available
The literature identifies that medical students receive little or no formal dental trauma assessment and management teaching during medical school. To bridge this important gap in medical education, medical educators should look to introduce basic dental trauma teaching into undergraduates’ final year of medical school. To set up a solid foundation for long-term learning, educators should consider which theories are most suited to transfer knowledge effectively to students in particular contexts. Similarly, medical educators should consider which theory or hybrid of learning theories best fits their teaching goals. In the absence of a dental educator at the medical school, offering an online learning approach is an ideal solution for filling medical students’ knowledge gap in assessing and managing dental trauma injuries.
... This role continuation suggests the importance for a counselor to learn, recognize, and continue a conscious role modeling effort despite the distant relationship (Epley et al., 2017). A continued camper-counselor relationship beyond camp may be ripe for additional research given that role models "exemplify specific goals, behaviors, and strategies" that youth may aspire to one day (Ahn et al., 2019). ...
Article
Camp programs hire counselors to fulfill multiple responsibilities and to role model positive behaviors for campers. Social-emotional learning (SEL) is a skill set that develops over time and through engagement with an array of individuals and opportunities that support social and emotional health across the lifespan. The social design of an overnight summer camp may be an ideal setting for nurturing SEL through camper-counselor relationships. The purpose of this study was to explore how counselors understood and modeled SEL with their campers and to what extent the campers recognized and interpreted those behaviors. Evidence suggests that counselors intended to role model SEL; however, they were inconsistent in how they modeled SEL during interactions with campers. Counselors’ own SEL may need more explicit support and training to more effectively role model SEL for campers.
Article
Mate copying occurs when individuals imitate the mate choice of other same‐sex individuals. These effects can be influenced by factors related to the rater and the model. The present study explored the roles of raters' interpersonal trust and models' romantic experiences in the decision‐making progress. Study 1 replicated mate copying effects and found the effect of interpersonal trust was nonsignificant. Study 2 further tested the interaction effect among interpersonal trust, model's romantic experiences, and model's mate choice decision. Results showed that change in targets' attractiveness varied with the model's romantic experiences under the model acceptance condition. The attractiveness of targets showed a greater increase when models had one or two romantic experiences than when they had no or multiple romantic experiences. A three‐way interaction effect occurred as well. Under the condition of model acceptance, for raters with a low or medium level of interpersonal trust, change in attractiveness increased with the model's romantic experiences and the relationship was non‐monotonic. For individuals with high levels of trust, models' romantic experiences did not have any effect. These results reveal that both rater's interpersonal trust and models' romantic experiences matter in individuals' willingness to copy other's mate preference.
Article
El aprendizaje social es la capacidad de aprender de los demás, como consecuencia de la observación de su comportamiento. Hoy, contamos con avances tecnológicos y medios digitales que nos permiten disponer de más información y herramientas de aprendizaje. Los usuarios que más han las aprovechado son los millennials, quienes utilizan las redes sociales y estas nuevas tecnologías en su ámbito social y también educativo. El método del caso es una metodología que ha demostrado ser muy eficaz en la educación de ejecutivos a partir de su creación en la Harvard Business School en 1908 y ha sido ampliamente difundida en numerosas escuelas de negocios. Esta investigación busca valorar el aprendizaje social a través de redes sociales como una alternativa a la metodología basada en casos. La hipótesis sostiene que la metodología basada en el aprendizaje social a través de redes sociales y la metodología basada en el método del caso permiten un nivel de aprendizaje equivalente. Se utilizó metodología experimental mixta con dos grupos de millennials, ambos expuestos a dichas metodologías. En los experimentos realizados para evaluar la eficacia de las metodologías, llegamos a dos conclusiones: (1) el promedio alcanzado en calificaciones utilizando el aprendizaje social es ligeramente superior al promedio alcanzado utilizando el método del caso; y (2) haciendo un análisis de medias, la diferencia no es estadísticamente significativa.
Article
Full-text available
Background Women and students of color are widely underrepresented in most STEM fields. In order to investigate this underrepresentation, we interviewed 201 college seniors, primarily women and people of color, who either majored in STEM or started but dropped a STEM major. Here we discuss one section of the longer interview that focused on students’ sense of belonging, which has been found to be related to retention. In our analysis, we examine the intersections of race and gender with students’ sense of belonging, a topic largely absent from the current literature. Results We found that white men were most likely to report a sense of belonging whereas women of color were the least likely. Further, we found that representation within one’s STEM sub-discipline, namely biology versus the physical sciences, impacts sense of belonging for women. Four key factors were found to contribute to sense of belonging for all students interviewed: interpersonal relationships, perceived competence, personal interest, and science identity. Conclusions Our findings indicate that students who remain in STEM majors report a greater sense of belonging than those who leave STEM. Additionally, we found that students from underrepresented groups are less likely to feel they belong. These findings highlight structural and cultural features of universities, as well as STEM curricula and pedagogy, that continue to privilege white males.
Article
Full-text available
Students’ beliefs that success in science depends on exceptional talent negatively impact their motivation to learn. For example, such beliefs have been shown to be a major factor steering students away from taking science and math courses in high school and college. In the present study, we tested a novel story-based instruction that models how scientists achieve through failures and struggles. We designed this instruction to challenge this belief, thereby improving science learning in classroom settings. A demographically diverse group of 402 9th and 10th grade students read 1 of 3 types of stories about eminent scientists that described how the scientists (a) struggled intellectually (e.g., made mistakes in investigating scientific problems, and overcame the mistakes through effort), (b) struggled in their personal life (e.g., suffered family poverty and lack of parental support but overcame it), or (c) made great discoveries (a control condition, similar to the instructional material that appears in many science textbooks, that did not describe any struggles). Results showed that participation in either of the struggle story conditions improved science learning postintervention, relative to that of students in the control condition. Additionally, the effect of our intervention was more pronounced for low-performing students. Moreover, far more students in either of the struggle story conditions felt connected to the stories and scientists than did students in the control condition. The use of struggle stories provides a promising and implementable instructional approach that can improve student motivation and academic performance in science and perhaps other subjects as well.
Article
Full-text available
Science instruction has focused on teaching students scientific content knowledge and problem-solving skills. However, even the best content instruction does not guarantee improved learning, as students’ motivation ultimately determines whether or not they will take advantage of the content. The goal of our instruction is to address the “leaky STEM pipeline” problem and retain more students in STEM fields. We designed a struggle-oriented instruction that tells stories about how even the greatest scientists struggled and failed prior to their discoveries. We describe how we have gone about designing this instruction to increase students’ motivation and better prepare them to interact and engage with content knowledge. We first discuss why we took this struggle-oriented approach to instruction by delineating the limitations of content-focused science instruction, especially from a motivational standpoint. Second, we detail how we designed and implemented this instruction in schools, outlining the factors that influenced our decisions under specific situational constraints. Finally, we discuss implications for future designers interested in utilizing this approach to instruction.
Article
In 3 studies, the authors demonstrated that individuals are motivated by role models who encourage strategies that fit their regulatory concerns: Promotion-focused individuals, who favor a strategy of pursuing desirable outcomes, are most inspired by positive role models, who highlight strategies for achieving success; prevention-focused individuals, who favor a strategy of avoiding undesirable outcomes, are most motivated by negative role models, who highlight strategies for avoiding failure. In Studies 1 and 2, the authors primed promotion and prevention goals and then examined the impact of role models on motivation. Participants' academic motivation was increased by goal-congruent role models but decreased by goal-incongruent role models. In Study 3, participants were more likely to generate real-life role models that matched their chronic goals.
Article
Black women are underrepresented in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) disciplines. Consequently, developing efficacious techniques to attract Black women to STEM companies is critical. Featuring successful scientists, who share an identity with Black women, on science companies' websites may be one way to signal to Black women they will be valued at that company. In two experiments, we explored who acts as an effective identity-safe cue for Black women at a fictitious science and technology company. Experiment 1 found Black women predicted they would feel more trust and belonging at a STEM company with a website featuring a Black woman or Black man scientist compared to a White woman scientist or no scientist. In Experiment 2, we found relative to viewing no scientist, Black women predicted they would feel more trust and belonging when a STEM company highlighted a Black woman scientist, or White woman scientist who expressed allyship with Black women (i.e., stating Black women bring important perspectives to science). Interestingly, across both experiments we found stigma consciousness (i.e., sensitivity to the possibility of experiencing discrimination) moderated these results, and had important implications for Black women's reported feelings of trust and belonging. Specifically, participants with higher stigma consciousness anticipated they would feel less trust and belonging at a company unless they viewed the profile featuring a Black woman scientist.
Article
A field experiment tested the effect of choosing a “favorite” role model on sense of fit in science among middle school girls. The girls participated in a 1-day science outreach event where they were exposed to multiple female role models. At the end of the event, participants were randomly assigned to choose and write about a favorite role model or to choose and write about their best friend. Girls in the role model choice condition experienced a significant increase in sense of fit in science, whereas girls in the control condition did not. Girls in the role model choice condition also tended to have stronger role model identification than girls in the control condition, and role model identification was correlated with sense of fit in science. Encouraging girls to actively choose and write about a favorite role model may help to maximize the impact of exposure to role models.