Content uploaded by Matthew Feinberg
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Matthew Feinberg on Jul 01, 2020
Content may be subject to copyright.
ARTICLE
Moral reframing: A technique for effective and
persuasive communication across political divides
Matthew Feinberg
1
| Robb Willer
2
1
Rotman School of Management, University
of Toronto
2
Stanford University
Correspondence
Matthew Feinberg, Rotman School of
Management, University of Toronto.
Email: matthew.feinberg@rotman.utoronto.ca
Abstract
The political landscape in the US and many other countries
is characterized by policy impasses and animosity between
rival political groups. Research finds that these divisions are
fueled in part by disparate moral concerns and convictions
that undermine communication and understanding between
liberals and conservatives. This “moral empathy gap”is par-
ticularly evident in the moral underpinnings of the political
arguments members of each side employ when trying to
persuade one another. Both liberals and conservatives typi-
cally craft arguments based on their own moral convictions
rather than the convictions of the people they target for
persuasion. As a result, these moral arguments tend to be
unpersuasive, even offensive, to their recipients. The tech-
nique of moral reframing—whereby a position an individual
would not normally support is framed in a way that is con-
sistent with that individual's moral values—can be an effec-
tive means for political communication and persuasion.
Over the last decade, studies of moral reframing have
shown its effectiveness across a wide range of polarized
topics, including views of economic inequality, environmen-
tal protection, same-sex marriage, and major party candi-
dates for the US presidency. In this article, we review the
moral reframing literature, examining potential mediators
and moderators of the effect, and discuss important ques-
tions that remain unanswered about this phenomenon.
DOI: 10.1111/spc3.12501
Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2019;13:e12501. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/spc3 © 2019 John Wiley & Sons Ltd 1of12
https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12501
1|INTRODUCTION
“It is necessary to have regard to the person whom we wish to persuade …what principles he acknowl-
edges …and then observe in the thing in question what affinity it has with the acknowledged principles.”
--Blaise Pascal, Pensees (1669/1976)
Politics in the US and many other countries are characterized by high levels of polarization (e.g., Boxell, Gen-
tzkow, & Shapiro, 2017; Groskopf, 2016; Pew Research Center, 2014; van der Eijk, Schmitt, & Binder, 2005), with
liberals and conservatives disagreeing on political issues and also harboring deep animosities toward one another.
1
Research finds that both attitude polarization and antagonism between political groups are associated with underly-
ing moral divisions (Haidt, 2012; Haidt & Graham, 2007; Koleva, Graham, Iyer, Ditto, & Haidt, 2012). Moral founda-
tions theory (Graham, Haidt, & Nosek, 2009) describes the moral domain in terms of five distinct foundations that
underlie moral attitudes and judgments: care—relating to the prevention and mitigation of suffering; fairness—relating
to equality and discrimination concerns; loyalty—relating to the prioritization of one's group and its needs; authority—
relating to showing respect for traditions and high ranking others; sanctity—relating to the protection of purity and
sacredness (Graham et al., 2009; Graham et al., 2011).
2
Survey results find that, relative to conservatives, liberals
more strongly endorse the care and fairness foundations, grounding many of their political positions in notions of
compassion, nurturance, and social justice. Conservatives, in contrast, more strongly endorse the loyalty, authority,
and sanctity foundations, and therefore ground many of their political stances in patriotism, traditionalism, and reli-
gious purity concerns (Caprara, Schwartz, Capanna, Vecchione, & Barbaranelli, 2006; Graham et al., 2009; Haidt &
Graham, 2007; Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003; Lakoff, 1996; but see also Voelkel & Brandt, 2019).
The rhetoric employed by advocates on the left and right often reflects these moral differences (Lakoff, 1996;
Marietta, 2008),
3
with liberals arguing for policy positions in terms of the care and fairness foundations, and conser-
vatives arguing for policy positions in terms of the loyalty, authority, and sanctity foundations (Feinberg & Willer,
2013, 2015). Use of moral rhetoric in this way may be an effective means for persuading, uniting, and motivating
those who already share one's moral and political convictions, but it is likely to be an ineffective, even counterpro-
ductive, strategy for persuading political rivals. Indeed, research suggests that exposure to the other side's moral rhe-
toric can even result in increased commitment to one's existing stance and greater animosity toward those on the
other side (Feinberg, Kovacheff, Teper, & Inbar, 2019; Gadarian & van der Vort, 2018; Skitka, Bauman, & Sargis,
2005; Tetlock, Kristel, Elson, Green, & Lerner, 2000), thereby exacerbating political polarization.
Recent research, however, suggests a means by which political advocates can persuade those who hold a differ-
ent political ideology. Moral reframing is a technique in which a position an individual would not normally support is
framed in a way that it is consistent with that individual's moral values. For example, an appeal in support of same-
sex marriage is likely to garner greater support from conservatives if it is framed in terms of patriotism (e.g., “Gay
Americans are proud and patriotic Americans”) than if it were framed in terms of equality (e.g., “Gay Americans
deserve equal rights”), because patriotism is a value more associated with conservatism, while equality is more asso-
ciated with liberalism. Below, we summarize the emerging literature on moral reframing, highlight key demonstra-
tions, and discuss when and why the technique is effective. We also point to some unanswered questions relating to
moral reframing, puzzles that we hope will inspire future research on the topic.
1.1 |A review of moral reframing research
Framing is the use of a “central organizing idea or story line that provides meaning (to an issue or event)”(Gamson &
Modigliani, 1987, p. 143; see also Druckman, 2001; Gitlin, 1980; Scheufele & Iyengar, 2012; Tversky & Kahneman,
1981). When the guiding force underlying the story line is grounded in moral concerns, this can be considered “moral
framing”(e.g., Lakoff, 1996, 2004). Results of moral framing research across disciplines points to its effectiveness as
a persuasion tool, especially in political discourse (e.g., Andrews, Clawson, Gramig, & Raymond, 2017; Barker, 2005;
2of12 FEINBERG AND WILLER
Dehghani, Sagae, Sachdeva, & Gratch, 2014; Feinberg et al., 2019; Hoover, Johnson, Boghrati, Graham, & Dehghani,
2018; Lakoff, 2004; Marietta, 2008; Valenzuela, Piña, & Ramírez, 2017; van Zant & Moore, 2015).
Of note, however, persuasion research highlights the fundamental importance of matching an argument or frame
to the message recipient's personal concerns and values (e.g., Boote, 1981; Hirsh, Kang, & Bodenhausen, 2012; Shen,
2004; Shen & Edwards, 2005; Watt, Maio, Haddock, & Johnson, 2008). Without such a match, the persuasive argu-
ment is less likely to resonate with the recipient and therefore will have little influence. Along these lines, the effec-
tiveness of moral framing will be bounded by how closely the morality underlying the argument fits with the moral
convictions of the target (Feinberg & Willer, 2013, 2015). Indeed, a moral frame that appeals to some individuals
(e.g., liberals) could even offend others (e.g., conservatives), if the two groups ascribe to different moral foundations
(Ditto & Koleva, 2011; Koleva et al., 2012). As a result, moral appeals that move one group, can actually backfire and
result in the other group becoming more strident in its opposition. For instance, because conservatives strongly
endorse the sanctity foundation, purity-based arguments such as those often made against immigration (“immigrants
pollute society”Cisneros, 2008; “immigrants contaminate the purity of our nation”Valverde, 2008) may uniquely res-
onate with conservatives. However, because liberals do not endorse this foundation and often view it as contradic-
tory to more foundations they do endorse (e.g., fairness), such arguments fail to persuade and may even backfire,
strengthening liberals' pro-immigration stances (Gadarian & van der Vort, 2018; Kahan, Braman, Slovic, Gastil, &
Cohen, 2007; Marietta, 2008; Tetlock et al., 2000).
A specific type of moral framing that can be effective for speaking across political divides is moral reframing
(Feinberg & Willer, 2013, 2015; Voelkel & Feinberg, 2018). In the political arena, moral reframing involves arguing in
favor of a political position that members of a political group would not normally support in terms of moral concerns
that the members strongly ascribe to. Fitting a message to a particular audience in this way is persuasive because it
makes the position relevant to and concordant with the audience's deeply held moral convictions. Seen through this
lens, the purpose of moral reframing is to transform positions that would otherwise seem morally wrong to an audi-
ence, into something morally acceptable or even desirable.
Evidence for the effectiveness of moral reframing has built over the last decade. Most moral reframing research
has focused on persuading liberals and conservatives to be more supportive of the other side's policy positions. In
particular, a series of articles have examined how morally reframed arguments about the environment and climate
change can persuade conservatives to be more supportive of pro-environmental policies and behavior (Feinberg &
Willer, 2013; Feygina, Jost, & Goldsmith, 2010; Kidwell, Farmer, & Hardesty, 2013; McCright, Charters, Dentzman, &
Dietz, 2016; Whitmarsh & Corner, 2017; Wolsko, Ariceaga, & Seiden, 2016). These studies have shown that conser-
vatives are more persuaded by pro-environmental arguments that appeal to the loyalty, authority, and sanctity foun-
dations. For example, Feygina et al. (2010) found that those more apt to be conservative (measured in their study in
terms of the highly related trait of system justification) reported greater pro-environmental intentions and were more
likely to sign a pro-environmental petition when presented with arguments emphasizing that being pro-
environmental is consistent with the “American way of life”and therefore a patriotic duty. Similarly, Feinberg and
Willer (2013) found that conservatives reported greater environmental concern, support for pro-environmental legis-
lation, and belief in climate change when presented with a purity-based argument that emphasized how dirty, dis-
gusting, and impure environmental degradation is, compared with a more typical, harm-focused argument that
emphasized the devastation and dangers a failing environment can cause (for similar findings, see also Kidwell et al.,
2013; Maibach et al., 2013; Wolsko et al., 2016).
Research shows that moral reframing is an effective means for persuasion on political issues far beyond the envi-
ronment (Bloemraad, Silva, & Voss, 2016; Feinberg & Willer, 2015; Franks & Scherr, 2019; Simon & Gilliam, 2013;
Thomas, 2017). For example, Feinberg and Willer (2015) found that liberals were more supportive of military spend-
ing when presented with fairness-focused arguments emphasizing the military's role in helping to overcome income
inequality and racial discrimination than they were presented with more typical, authority- and loyalty-focused argu-
ments that emphasized that the military demonstrated American patriotism and world dominance. In a representa-
tive sample of registered voters in California, Bloemraad et al. (2016) found that arguments in favor of legalizing the
FEINBERG AND WILLER 3of12
status of undocumented immigrants were effective in persuading conservatives only when the argument appealed
to the loyalty foundation, in particular to family unity. In a field experiment, Kalla, Levine, and Broockman (2019)
found that door-to-door canvassers' pro-choice appeals were persuasive if articulated in terms of a moral value can-
vassers' elicited from residents early in the interaction.
A second category of moral reframing research has focused on voters' support for political candidates. This
research builds on findings showing that individuals who more strongly endorse the care and fairness moral founda-
tions tend to vote for Democratic candidates while individuals who strongly endorse the loyalty, authority, and sanc-
tity foundations tend to vote for Republican candidates (Enke, 2018; Franks & Scherr, 2015). Voelkel and Feinberg
(2018) presented American participants with arguments for why they should not support Donald Trump for presi-
dent of the US. The arguments emphasized that Trump was unfit for office, either because he discriminates against
minorities and promotes prejudice (fairness frame), or because he showed disloyalty to the country by dodging the
draft during the Vietnam War (loyalty frame). Results showed that conservative participants were less supportive of
Donald Trump's candidacy when they were presented with the loyalty frame. Additionally, a preregistered, nationally
representative study found that an economically progressive candidate who articulated his platform in terms of more
conservative values like patriotism and cultural traditionalism was supported more by self-identified conservatives,
as well as moderates (Voelkel & Willer, 2019).
Finally, a third category of moral reframing research has explored its efficacy in shaping liberals' and conserva-
tives' attitudes on issues that are largely nonpolitical or, at least, not strongly aligned with either liberal or conserva-
tive political ideologies. These studies have targeted outcomes like vaccine hesitancy, diversity and inclusion, and
stem cell technology (Amin et al., 2017; Brannon, Carter, Murdock-Perriera, & Higginbotham, 2018; Clifford, Jerit,
Rainey, & Motyl, 2015; Hoover et al., 2018; Kljajic & Feinberg, 2019; Lee, Yoon, Lee, & Royne, 2018; Winterich,
Mittal, & Aquino, 2013). Along these lines, a handful of studies have examined the effects of moral reframing on lib-
erals' and conservatives' tendencies to donate money to different types of charities. In a broad examination of moral
reframing's impact on the donation behavior of liberals and conservatives, Kljajic and Feinberg (2019) found that cer-
tain charities were recognized as more “liberal charities”or “conservative charities,”and morally reframing these
charities' messaging led liberals to donate more to the “conservative charities”and conservatives to donate more to
the “liberal charities.”Similarly, Lee et al., (2018) examined the effects of different types of moral arguments in char-
ity advertising on both liberals' and conservatives' donation behaviors, finding that arguments appealing to equality-
based moral concerns were much more effective for liberals than conservatives (see also Winterich et al., 2013).
4
2|MECHANISMS DRIVING MORAL REFRAMING EFFECTS
Why does moral reframing work? The primary explanation is that morally reframed messages are influential because
targets perceive a “match”between their moral convictions and the argument in favor of the other side's policy posi-
tion. Moral convictions are one of the key underlying bases of people's attitudes. They are central and immutable
parts of one's identity (Aquino & Reed, 2002; Atran & Axelrod, 2008; Kovacheff, Schwartz, Inbar, & Feinberg, 2018;
Skitka et al., 2005; Strohminger & Nichols, 2014). Because moral convictions are so strongly held, arguments that
appeal to them are difficult to discount, even when used to argue for a position one would typically oppose. As a
result, when individuals face a morally reframed argument that resonates with their fundamental moral convictions,
they are more likely to evaluate the argument positively and revise their relevant attitudes as a result. In line with this
theorizing, moral reframing studies have found that perceived overlap between the message's argument and the
moral convictions of the target mediates the effect (Feinberg & Willer, 2015).
This perceived overlap between a message's moral basis and the target's moral convictions may also give rise to
subsequent mediating mechanisms. For instance, the overlap may evoke feelings of comfort or familiarity in the mes-
sage recipient, making the argument easier to comprehend and process, encouraging a positive evaluation. Along
these lines, Kidwell et al. (2013) found that conservatives' increase in pro-environmental intentions after reading
4of12 FEINBERG AND WILLER
morally reframed environmental messages was mediated by the extent to which they reported that the message was
clear and relatable (i.e., processing fluency; see also Gantman & Van Bavel, 2014). Additionally, moral reframing may
signal to the message recipient that the message source is a fellow ingroup member and therefore someone who is
credible and trustworthy (Cohen, 2003; Kalkhoff & Barnum, 2000). Wolsko et al., (2016), for example, found that
conservatives presented with morally reframed environmental appeals were persuaded largely because they per-
ceived the values being expressed in the message as coming from a conservative source.
2.1 |Potential moderators of moral reframing effects
Although a number of studies have shown the efficacy of moral reframing, potential moderating factors that either
strengthen or weaken the effect remain largely unexplored. One category of potential moderators concerns features
of the message itself. For instance, a morally reframed message that fails to connect a policy position to the moral
foundations of the target audience will likely be ineffective. This might occur because the association with that moral
foundation is not emphasized strongly enough to resonate with the target, because the argument made does not
make a compelling case for viewing the policy as promoting the moral foundation, or possibly because the moral
argument is obscured by other nonmoral arguments also included in the message. These possible moderating factors
may help explain some studies that have found inconsistent evidence for moral reframing (e.g., Day, Fiske, Down-
ing, & Trail, 2014; Peterson & Simonovits, 2017; van de Rijt, Akin, Willer, & Feinberg, 2016).
Conversely, a morally reframed argument might be particularly effective if it evokes strong moral emotions
related to the moral foundation being targeted. Research has shown that different moral emotions correspond with,
and reinforce, different foundations (Horberg, Oveis, & Keltner, 2011). For instance, the sanctity foundation corre-
sponds closely with the emotion of disgust (Horberg, Oveis, Keltner, & Cohen, 2009; Wisneski & Skitka, 2017). With
this in mind, morally reframed messages targeting conservatives via the sanctity foundation should be particularly
potent if the message elicits moral disgust in the target. Consistent with this reasoning, Feinberg and Willer (2013)
found that the persuasive effect of a purity-based argument in support of environmentalism (e.g., “pollution in our
environment inevitably contaminates us and our bodies”) among conservatives was mediated by conservatives' self-
reported disgust after reading the message. Although this research focused on disgust as a mediator rather than a
moderator, these results suggest that a sanctity message that elicits disgust may be more effective at persuading
conservatives than one that does not.
The effectiveness of a morally reframed message may also depend on which aspects of the targeted moral foun-
dation the message highlights. Although the relationship between political ideology and each of the five moral foun-
dations has proven robust (Graham et al., 2011), certain aspects of the different foundations are likely more
fundamental to liberal and conservative morality. For instance, although liberals very strongly endorse the care foun-
dation, this may be primarily due to liberals' emphasis on compassion and alleviating the suffering of those most in
need, rather than other aspects relating to protection and security concerns (Hirsh, DeYoung, Xu, & Peterson, 2010;
Piurko, Schwartz, & Davidov, 2011). As such, morally reframed messages that speak to concerns about being com-
passionate and alleviating suffering would likely be more persuasive for liberals than emphasizing national security
(Feinberg & Willer, 2015). Similarly, egalitarianism and social justice may be aspects of the fairness foundation that
resonate most strongly with liberals, compared to other aspects of fairness like proportionality (i.e., equity; Haidt,
2012; Jost et al., 2003). As a result, morally reframed messages that speak to these values are likely to be more per-
suasive for liberals. Indeed, in one moral reframing experiment, liberals were more likely to donate to a charity when
the message appealed to egalitarian concerns, but when the argument appealed to proportionality fairness concerns,
conservatives were more persuaded (Lee et al., 2018). Conversely, for conservatives, aspects of the loyalty founda-
tion pertaining to national pride and patriotism would likely be more effective in moral reframing than loyalty appeals
beyond the national level (see Feinberg & Willer, 2015; Feygina et al., 2010; Wolsko et al., 2016).
Message source will likely also have a strong moderating effect on the impact of moral reframing. For example, in
a study of moral messaging around environmental issues, Wolsko et al., (2016) found that one reason moral reframing
FEINBERG AND WILLER 5of12
worked was because message recipients perceived the source to be part of their political ingroup. But what would
happen if the message source was a known political outgroup member? One recent study (Voelkel & Willer, 2019)
found that conservatives were significantly more supportive of a Democratic presidential candidate if the candidate
appealed to conservative value concerns such as patriotism and respect for tradition than if he appealed to more lib-
eral value concerns like social justice and egalitarianism. This suggests that moral reframing effects can be effective
enough to be persuasive, even when seen as coming from a political outgroup. At the same time, it is unlikely that
moral reframing would be effective if the source was a reviled political outgroup member. For example, an argument
in favor of military spending that highlights how the military provides a level playing field for the poor and minorities
in the US would likely be unpersuasive to liberals if the messenger was Sean Hannity, Rush Limbaugh, or Donald
Trump. Political psychology research suggests that the effect of moral reframing would be substantially muted, if not
eliminated, in this situation (Cohen, 2003; see also Goren, 2005), because the target would approach the message
with outgroup bias and would be strongly motivated to reject the argument before even reading it (Kahan, 2015).
Lastly, national context is likely another important moderating factor. Although in many ways, political ideology
manifests similarly across cultural and national contexts (Lakoff, 1996), the bases of economic and social ideology
can vary markedly across nations and political systems (Malka, Soto, Inzlicht, & Lelkes, 2014). With this in mind, a
moral reframing strategy for economic policy in some countries might need to be quite different than it would be in
the US. One example might be post-Soviet countries, where conservatives often support greater government regula-
tion of the economy because of the historical legacy of communism as the traditional status quo. In these countries,
arguments for greater regulation of the economy would need to target liberals, linking government regulation with
the more liberal moral foundations to be effective.
2.2 |Effects of moral reframing on untargeted individuals
It is worth noting that most moral reframing studies to date have found nonsignificant effects for the untargeted
group—that is, the group that already supports the policy stance being argued for. For example, Feinberg and Willer
(2015) found that liberals showed similar levels of support for universal health care regardless of whether they read
a typical moral message appealing to equality or a reframed message appealing to purity concerns. Likewise, conser-
vatives showed similar levels of support for increased military spending regardless of whether they read a message
appealing to loyalty or egalitarian concerns. These null effects suggest that untargeted groups were not persuaded
by messages grounded in moral language they oppose, nor by messages grounded in moral language they endorse.
Bolstering this point, in the handful of moral reframing studies with a control condition, the untargeted group's level
of support did not differ across any condition. It appears that in situations where the argument is congruent with
one's policy stance, how that argument is made may matter little.
These null results for the untargeted group have important implications for individuals speaking to audiences
consisting of both liberals and conservatives. If the type of messaging has no effect on the untargeted group, but
morally reframed messages positively influence the targeted group, then speakers in these situations should rely on
morally reframed messages. However, we believe future research should explore this in more depth. Existing moral
reframing research may have found the null effects because it has focused primarily on highly polarized political
issues where the untargeted group's level of support is close to a ceiling, leaving little room for increases in support.
Nonetheless, in situations where the untargeted group's support is not crystallized, moral messaging could still have
an impact. Additionally, even if moral messages do not affect the untargeted group attitudinally, they may still have
behavioral effects. Considering moral motivations are important for collective action (van Zomeren, Postmes,
Spears, & Bettache, 2011), moral messages that appeal to existing supporters may not strengthen their support, but
still influence their willingness to act (Marietta, 2008, however, see Voelkel & Willer, 2019). With this in mind, it may
behoove individuals speaking to diverse audiences to employ a mix of moral frames—some appealing to existing sup-
porters, others appealing to nonsupporters. We hope future research might explore the benefits (or possible pitfalls)
of using such mixed frames.
6of12 FEINBERG AND WILLER
2.3 |Why people do not use moral reframing
Even though research suggests moral reframing is an effective strategy for persuading those on the other side of
political divides, people rarely employ the strategy spontaneously. Feinberg and Willer (2015) found that fewer than
10% of liberal or conservative participants wrote morally reframed appeals when asked to write political arguments
aimed at persuading their political counterparts and despite being offered a cash prize for effectively doing so. If
moral reframing is typically effective, why is it so rarely employed?
One possibility is that people do not realize that moral reframing is effective. Research indicates that individuals
experience their moral convictions as objective truths about the world (Skitka et al., 2005). As a result, it can be diffi-
cult to recognize that there are different “truths”that other people believe in (Ditto & Koleva, 2011; Kovacheff et al.,
2018). Indeed, polling data indicates that people are apt to perceive someone who does not endorse their morality
as simply immoral or evil, rather than morally different (Doherty & Kiley, 2016). Without recognizing that one's politi-
cal rivals possess different morals, and without a clear understanding of what those different morals are, using moral
reframing becomes impossible. Moreover, even if individuals are aware of the moral differences, they may still not
use moral reframing because a morally reframed argument might intuitively seem weak or ineffectual because it does
not resonate with them personally.
Alternatively, individuals may have the knowledge needed to craft morally reframed messages, but refuse to do
so on principle. Strategically making arguments that appeal to moral values that one does not strongly endorse may
feel unethical. In fact, research on the concept of “taboo trade-offs”(Tetlock, 2003) finds that even entertaining the
possibility of compromising one's moral convictions to achieve strategic ends—such as buying body parts for trans-
plants or hiring another person to serve one's prison sentence—can feel immoral and unacceptable. In the context of
moral foundations, for many liberals, aspects of the authority foundation may contradict moral beliefs about equality
and progressivism. As a result, making arguments grounded in the authority foundation could feel immoral. More-
over, individuals might also fear that others, particularly those who share their moral beliefs, will judge them harshly
for appealing to “false morals”. Finally, people might be averse to making morally reframed arguments because they
believe doing so could indirectly reinforce values they disagree with (Lakoff, 2004), perhaps leading to short run
gains on a given issue, but serving to validate moral values that are anathema to their moral and political worldview
in the long-term.
In initial explorations of why most individuals do not use moral reframing, Feinberg and Willer (2015) found that
when participants were asked to identify whether a morally reframed argument or a more typical moral argument
would be more persuasive to those on the other side of the political spectrum, 64% of liberal participants and 85%
of conservative participants correctly selected the morally reframed argument. This suggests that most individuals
can recognize the persuasive appeal of morally reframed arguments even if they do not spontaneously use them. In
addition, results indicated that when participants were asked to choose one of the arguments to send to a member
of the rival ideological group they were tasked with persuading, 80% of liberals and 94% of conservatives who previ-
ously indicated the morally reframed argument would be more persuasive also chose to send the morally reframed
argument, suggesting that most individuals would not object to using morally reframed arguments on principle.
Taken together, these findings suggest that the most likely barriers to individuals using moral reframing spontane-
ously are (a) they do not think to use it, or (b) they do not know how to, rather than an inability to recognize the per-
suasive power of these arguments, or an unwillingness to use them. However, more research is needed to fully
understand why few use morally reframed arguments.
3|CONCLUSION
In his classic of applied social psychology, How to Win Friends and Influence People, Dale Carnegie (2017 [1936])
underscored the importance of accounting for the interests and perspective of a target of persuasion:
FEINBERG AND WILLER 7of12
For example, one day Ralph Waldo Emerson and his son tried to get a calf into the barn. But they made
the common mistake of thinking only of what they wanted; so he stiffened his legs and stubbornly refused
to leave the pasture. The Irish housemaid saw their predicament. She couldn't write essays and books; but
on this occasion at least, she had more horse sense, or calf sense, than Emerson had. She thought of what
the calf wanted; so she put her maternal finger in the calf's mouth and let the calf suck her finger as she
gently led him into the barn.
This principle applies not only to the finger suckling of recalcitrant calves, but also—as we find in the above-
reviewed studies—to bridging one of the most fundamental divides in American society. A growing roster of studies
attest to the potential effectiveness of moral reframing as a persuasive tool.
In all, moral reframing offers a means for creating political consensus, which can have many benefits. In deeply
divided polities, such as the contemporary US, consensus may be a valuable end to itself, facilitating effective gover-
nance and preserving the legitimacy of democratic institutions. For advocates, moral reframing may be attractive as
an alternative to simple compromise on political issues, at present the dominant model for building political consen-
sus. Indeed, one prior study found that subjectively reframing the apparent moral virtues of a policy was more
impactful than the extremity of that policy on a liberal–conservative spectrum, suggesting compromise might often
be less effective than moral reframing (e.g., Voelkel & Willer, 2019).
Further, beyond being effective for building consensus, moral reframing may produce other valuable outcomes.
Moral reframing may reduce animosity individuals feel for those on the other side of a significant political divide
(Doherty & Kiley, 2016). Though people would likely use moral reframing for the strategic purpose of winning over
their rivals, effectively using the technique requires developing a clear understanding of what the other side values
and believes. Thus, inherent in using moral reframing is seeking out and understanding the moral perspective of one's
rivals. Developing a more accurate understanding of others' perspectives may also have downstream effects on
affective polarization, which research shows stems in part from exaggerated and inaccurate stereotypes of political
outgroup members (Farwell & Weiner, 2000; Graham, Nosek, & Haidt, 2012). When individuals have a better sense
of a rival group's perspective, they will be less likely to degrade them or view them as clearly evil. Future research
should explore the possibility that moral reframing of divisive political issues could be helpful in this way to reduce
antipathy and negative affect between rival political groups.
Of course, it would be naïve to suggest moral reframing will always have positive effects. In fact, many of the
greatest atrocities in human history have been supported by political projects that employed strategic moral
reframing. Where the Third Reich employed purity, loyalty, and authority arguments, Stalin and the Khmer Rouge
built their regimes on the values of authority and fairness. These and countless other examples show that, like any
pragmatic tool, moral reframing can be no more moral than the ends to which it is put. That said, while any technique
of political persuasion could in principle be used to ameliorate intractable and destructive political divides, or to jus-
tify cruelty and brutality, there is reason to hope that the everyday practice of moral reframing, because its effective
use requires careful attention to the perspective of one's political rivals, could foster that rarest of things in a bitterly
divided society—mutual understanding.
ENDNOTES
1
Whereas in the US, the political divide is between “liberals”and “conservatives”(or Democrats and Republicans), both the
substance of political divides and the terms used to describe them vary across cultural contexts (Malka et al., 2014). How-
ever, research suggests that the liberal–conservative divide on social issues in particular manifests in similar ways across
cultures (e.g., Feinberg, Wehling, Chung, Saslow, & Melvær Paulin, 2019; Graham et al., 2011).
2
There is also evidence of an additional moral foundation relating to freedom (see Iyer, Koleva, Graham, Ditto, & Haidt,
2012).
8of12 FEINBERG AND WILLER
3
Although we focus primarily on liberals versus conservatives in our review, the reasoning and insights from this review
apply as well to the highly related political divisions between the left and right and Democrats and Republicans.
4
In addition, they found that arguments appealing to proportionality-based rewards were more effective for influencing
conservatives' donation behavior. These findings speak to the possibility that the fairness moral foundation may have two
components—one that relates to egalitarian concerns and one that relates to proportionality concerns, with the former
being a central moral concern of liberals, and the latter being of concern to conservatives (Haidt, 2012).
ORCID
Matthew Feinberg https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8626-1620
REFERENCES
Amin, A. B., Bednarczyk, R. A., Ray, C. E., Melchiori, K. J., Graham, J., Huntsinger, J. R., & Omer, S. B. (2017). Association of
moral values with vaccine hesitancy. Nature Human Behaviour,1(12), 873–880. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-
0256-5
Andrews, A. C., Clawson, R. A., Gramig, B. M., & Raymond, L. (2017). Finding the right value: Framing effects on domain
experts. Political Psychology,38(2), 261–278. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12339
Aquino, K., & Reed, I. I. (2002). The self-importance of moral identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,83(6),
1423–1440. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1423
Atran, S., & Axelrod, R. (2008). Reframing sacred values. Negotiation Journal,24(3), 221–246. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
1571-9979.2008.00182.x
Barker, D. (2005). Values, frames, and persuasion in presidential nomination campaigns. Political Behavior,27(4), 375–395.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-005-8145-4
Bloemraad, I., Silva, F., & Voss, K. (2016). Rights, economics, or family? Frame resonance, political ideology, and the immi-
grant rights movement. Social Forces,94(4), 1647–1674. https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/sov123
Boote, A. S. (1981). Market segmentation by personal values and salient product attributes. Journal of Advertising Research,
21,29–35.
Boxell, L., Gentzkow, M., & Shapiro, J. M. (2017). Greater Internet use is not associated with faster growth in political polari-
zation among US demographic groups. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,114(40), 10612–10617. https://
doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1706588114
Brannon, T. N., Carter, E. R., Murdock-Perriera, L. A., & Higginbotham, G. D. (2018). From backlash to inclusion for all: Insti-
tuting diversity efforts to maximize benefits across group lines. Social Issues and Policy Review,12(1), 57–90. https://doi.
org/10.1111/sipr.12040
Caprara, G. V., Schwartz, S., Capanna, C., Vecchione, M., & Barbaranelli, C. (2006). Personality and politics: Values, traits, and
political choice. Political Psychology,27(1), 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2006.00447.x
Carnegie, D. (2017). How to win friends & influence people. Förlag: Musaicum Books.
Cisneros, J. D. (2008). Contaminated communities: The metaphor of "immigrant as pollutant" in media representations of
immigration. Rhetoric & Public Affairs,11(4), 569–601. https://doi.org/10.1353/rap.0.0068
Clifford, S., Jerit, J., Rainey, C., & Motyl, M. (2015). Moral concerns and policy attitudes: Investigating the influence of elite
rhetoric. Political Communication,32(2), 229–248. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2014.944320
Cohen, G. L. (2003). Party over policy: The dominating impact of group influence on political beliefs. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology,85(5), 808–822. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.808
Day, M. V., Fiske, S. T., Downing, E. L., & Trail, T. E. (2014). Shifting liberal and conservative attitudes using moral founda-
tions theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,40(12), 1559–1573. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0146167214551152
Dehghani, M., Sagae, K., Sachdeva, S., & Gratch, J. (2014). Analyzing political rhetoric in conservative and liberal weblogs
related to the construction of the “Ground Zero Mosque”.Journal of Information Technology & Politics,11(1), 1–14.
https://doi.org/10.1080/19331681.2013.826613
Ditto, P. H., & Koleva, S. P. (2011). Moral empathy gaps and the American culture war. Emotion Review,3(3), 331–332.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911402393
Doherty, C., & Kiley, J. (2016, June 22). Key facts about partisanship and political animosity in America. Retrieved from
http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2016/06/22/key-facts-partisanship/.
Druckman, J. N. (2001). The implications of framing effects for citizen competence. Political Behavior,23, 225–256. https://
doi.org/10.1023/A:1015006907312
Enke, B. (2018). Moral values and voting (No. w24268). National Bureau of Economic Research.
FEINBERG AND WILLER 9of12
Farwell, L., & Weiner, B. (2000). Bleeding hearts and the heartless: Popular perceptions of liberal and conservative ideolo-
gies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,26(7), 845–852. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167200269009
Feinberg, M., Kovacheff, C., Teper, R., & Inbar, Y. (2019). Understanding the process of moralization: How eating meat
becomes a moral issue. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,117,50–72. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000149
Feinberg, M., & Willer, R. (2013). The moral roots of environmental attitudes. Psychological Science,24(1), 56–62. https://
doi.org/10.1177/0956797612449177
Feinberg, M., & Willer, R. (2015). From gulf to bridge: When do moral arguments facilitate political influence? Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin,41(12), 1665–1681. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167215607842
Feinberg, M., Wehling, E., Chung, J. M., Saslow, L. R., & Melvær Paulin, I. (2019). Measuring Moral Politics: How Strict and
Nurturant Family Values Explain Individual Differences in Conservatism, Liberalism, and the Political Middle. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology. Advance online publication.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000255
Feygina, I., Jost, J. T., & Goldsmith, R. E. (2010). System justification, the denial of global warming, and the possibility of “sys-
tem-sanctioned change”.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,36(3), 326–338. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0146167209351435
Franks, A. S., & Scherr, K. C. (2015). Using moral foundations to predict voting behavior: Regression models from the 2012
US presidential election. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy,15(1), 213–232. https://doi.org/10.1111/asap.12074
Franks, A. S., & Scherr, K. C. (2019). Economic issues are moral issues: The moral underpinnings of the desire to reduce
wealth inequality. Social Psychological and Personality Science,10(4), 553–562.
Gadarian, S. K., & van der Vort, E. (2018). The gag reflex: Disgust rhetoric and gay rights in American politics. Political Behav-
ior,40(2), 521–543. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-017-9412-x
Gamson, W. A., & Modigliani, A. (1987). The changing culture of affirmative action. Research in Political Sociology,3,
137–177.
Gantman, A. P., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2014). The moral pop-out effect: Enhanced perceptual awareness of morally relevant
stimuli. Cognition,132,22–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.02.007
Gitlin, T. (1980). The whole world is watching: Mass media in the making & unmaking of the new left. Berkeley: University of
California Press.
Goren, P. (2005). Party identification and core political values. American Journal of Political Science,49(4), 881–896. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5907.2005.00161.x
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,96(5), 1029–1046. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015141
Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., & Haidt, J. (2012). The moral stereotypes of liberals and conservatives: Exaggeration of differences
across the political spectrum. PLoS ONE,7(12), e50092. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050092
Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology,101(2), 366–385. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021847
Groskopf, C. (2016). European politics is more polarized than ever, and these numbers prove it. Retrieved from https://qz.
com/645649/european-politics-is-more-polarized-than-ever-and-these-numbers-prove-it/
Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. New York: Vintage.
Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not rec-
ognize. Social Justice Research,20(1), 98–116. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-007-0034-z
Hirsh, J. B., DeYoung, C. G., Xu, X., & Peterson, J. B. (2010). Compassionate liberals and polite conservatives: Associations of
agreeableness with political ideology and moral values. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,36(5), 655–664.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210366854
Hirsh, J. B., Kang, S. K., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2012). Personalized persuasion: Tailoring persuasive appeals to recipients'
personality traits. Psychological Science,23(6), 578–581. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611436349
Hoover, J., Johnson, K., Boghrati, R., Graham, J., & Dehghani, M. (2018). Moral framing and charitable donation: Integrating
exploratory social media analyses and confirmatory experimentation. Collabra: Psychology,4(1), 9. https://doi.org/10.
1525/collabra.129
Horberg, E. J., Oveis, C., & Keltner, D. (2011). Emotions as moral amplifiers: An appraisal tendency approach to the influ-
ences of distinct emotions upon moral judgment. Emotion Review,3(3), 237–244. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1754073911402384
Horberg, E. J., Oveis, C., Keltner, D., & Cohen, A. B. (2009). Disgust and the moralization of purity. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology,97(6), 963–976. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017423
Iyer, R., Koleva, S., Graham, J., Ditto, P., & Haidt, J. (2012). Understanding libertarian morality: The psychological dispositions
of self-identified libertarians. PLoS ONE,7(8), e42366. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042366
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psycho-
logical Bulletin,129(3), 339–375. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339
10 of 12 FEINBERG AND WILLER
Kahan, D. M. (2015). The politically motivated reasoning paradigm, part 1: What politically motivated reasoning is and how
to measure it. Emerging trends in the social and behavioral sciences: An interdisciplinary, searchable, and linkable
resource, 1-16.
Kahan, D. M., Braman, D., Slovic, P., Gastil, J., & Cohen, G. L. (2007). The second national risk and culture study: Making
sense of-and making progress in-the American culture war of fact. GWU Legal Studies Research Paper,370,8–26.
Kalkhoff, W., & Barnum, C. (2000). The effects of status-organizing and social identity processes on patterns of social influ-
ence. Social Psychology Quarterly,63,95–115. https://doi.org/10.2307/2695886
Kalla, J. L., Levine, A. S., & Broockman, D. E. (2019). Personalizing moral reframing in interpersonal conversation: A field
experiment. Working paper.
Kidwell, B., Farmer, A., & Hardesty, D. M. (2013). Getting liberals and conservatives to go green: Political ideology and con-
gruent appeals. Journal of Consumer Research,40(2), 350–367. https://doi.org/10.1086/670610
Kljajic, M. & Feinberg, M. (2019). Moral foundations of charitable giving. Working paper.
Koleva, S. P., Graham, J., Iyer, R., Ditto, P. H., & Haidt, J. (2012). Tracing the threads: How five moral concerns (especially
Purity) help explain culture war attitudes. Journal of Research in Personality,46(2), 184–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jrp.2012.01.006
Kovacheff, C., Schwartz, S., Inbar, Y., & Feinberg, M. (2018). The problem with morality: Impeding progress and increasing
divides. Social Issues and Policy Review,12(1), 218–257. https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12045
Lakoff, G. (1996). Moral politics: What conservatives know that liberals don't. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lakoff, G. (2004). Don't Think of an Elephant! Know Your Values and Frame the Debate. White River Junction, Vermont:
Chelsea Green Publishing.
Lee, Y., Yoon, S., Lee, Y. W., & Royne, M. B. (2018). How liberals and conservatives respond to equality-based and
proportionality-based rewards in charity advertising. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing,37(1), 108–118.
Maibach, E., Roser-Renouf, C., Vraga, E., Bloodhart, B., Anderson, A., Stenhouse, N., & Leiserowitz, A. (2013). A national
survey of Republicans and Republican-leaning Independents on energy and climate change. George Mason University
Center for Climate Change Communication and Yale Project on Climate Change Communication.
Malka, A., Soto, C. J., Inzlicht, M., & Lelkes, Y. (2014). Do needs for security and certainty predict cultural and economic con-
servatism? A cross-national analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,106(6), 1031–1051. https://doi.org/10.
1037/a0036170
Marietta, M. (2008). From my cold, dead hands: Democratic consequences of sacred rhetoric. The Journal of Politics,70(3),
767–779. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022381608080742
McCright, A. M., Charters, M., Dentzman, K., & Dietz, T. (2016). Examining the effectiveness of climate change frames in the
face of a climate change denial counter-frame. Topics in Cognitive Science,8(1), 76–97. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.
12171
Pascal, B. (1669/1976). Thoughts. Paris: GF Flammarion.
Peterson, E., & Simonovits, G. (2017). Costly values: The limited benefits and potential costs of targeted policy justifications.
Journal of Experimental Political Science,4(2), 95–106. https://doi.org/10.1017/XPS.2017.3
Pew Research Center. (2014). Political polarization in the American public. Retrieved from http://www.people-press.org/
2014/06/12/political-polarization-in-the-american-public/
Piurko, Y., Schwartz, S. H., & Davidov, E. (2011). Basic personal values and the meaning of left-right political orientations in
20 countries. Political Psychology,32(4), 537–561. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2011.00828.x
Scheufele, D. A., & Iyengar, S. (2012). The state of framing research: A call for new directions. In The Oxford Handbook of
Political Communication Theories (pp. 1–26). New York: Oxford University Press.
Shen, F. (2004). Chronic accessibility and individual cognitions: Examining the effects of message frames in political adver-
tisements. Journal of Communication,54(1), 123–137. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2004.tb02617.x
Shen, F., & Edwards, H. H. (2005). Economic individualism, humanitarianism, and welfare reform: A value-based account of
framing effects. Journal of Communication,55(4), 795–809. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2005.tb03023.x
Simon, A. F., & Gilliam Jr., F. D. (2013). Framing and facts:Necessary synergies in communicating about public safety and crimi-
nal justice. Retrieved from https://www.frameworksinstitute.org/assets/files/pscj_values_and_facts.pdf
Skitka, L. J., Bauman, C. W., & Sargis, E. G. (2005). Moral conviction: Another contributor to attitude strength or something
more? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,88(6), 895–917. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.6.895
Strohminger, N., & Nichols, S. (2014). The essential moral self. Cognition,131(1), 159–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
cognition.2013.12.005
Tetlock, P. E. (2003). Thinking the unthinkable: Sacred values and taboo cognitions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,7(7),
320–324. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00135-9
Tetlock, P. E., Kristel, O. V., Elson, S. B., Green, M. C., & Lerner, J. S. (2000). The psychology of the unthinkable: Taboo
trade-offs, forbidden base rates, and heretical counterfactuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,78(5),
853–870. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.853
FEINBERG AND WILLER 11 of 12
Thomas, C. (2017, June). Universal Basic Income:The Meaning of Money Matters. Presentation at the Society for the Psycho-
logical Study of Social Issues 2017 Conference, Albuquerque, NM. Presentation.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science,211(4481), 453–458.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7455683
Valenzuela, S., Piña, M., & Ramírez, J. (2017). Behavioral effects of framing on social media users: How conflict, economic,
human interest, and morality frames drive news sharing. Journal of Communication,67(5), 803–826.
Valverde, M. (2008). Racial purity, sexual purity, and immigration policy. In The history of immigration and racism in Canada
(pp. 175–188). Toronto, ON: Canadian Scholars' Press.
Van De Rijt, A., Akin, I. A., Willer, R., & Feinberg, M. (2016). Success-breeds-success in collective political behavior: Evidence
from a field experiment. Sociological Science,3, 940–950. https://doi.org/10.15195/v3.a41
Van der Eijk, C., Schmitt, H., & Binder, T. (2005). Left-right orientation and party choice. In J. S. Thomassen (Ed.), The
European voter. A comparative study of modern democracies (pp. 167–191). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.
org/10.1093/0199273219.003.0007
Van Zant, A. B., & Moore, D. A. (2015). Leaders' use of moral justifications increases policy support. Psychological Science,26
(6), 934–943. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615572909
Van Zomeren, M., Postmes, T., Spears, R., & Bettache, K. (2011). Can moral convictions motivate the advantaged to chal-
lenge social inequality? Extending the social identity model of collective action. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations,
14(5), 735–753. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430210395637
Voelkel, J. G., & Brandt, M. J. (2019). The effect of ideological identification on the endorsement of moral values depends on
the target group. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,45(6), 851–863.
Voelkel, J. G., & Feinberg, M. (2018). Morally reframed arguments can affect support for political candidates. Social Psycho-
logical and Personality Science,9(8), 917–924. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617729408
Voelkel, J. G., & Willer, R. (2019). Resolving the progressive paradox: Conservative value framing of progressive economic
policies increases candidate support. SSRN working paper.
Watt, S. E., Maio, G. R., Haddock, G., & Johnson, B. T. (2008). Attitude functions in persuasion: Matching, involvement, self-
affirmation, and hierarchy. In R. Prislin & W. Crano (Eds.), Attitudes and Attitude Change (pp. 189–211). Frontiers of
Social Psychology Series. New York: Psychology Press.
Whitmarsh, L., & Corner, A. (2017). Tools for a new climate conversation: A mixed-methods study of language for public
engagement across the political spectrum. Global Environmental Change,42, 122–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
gloenvcha.2016.12.008
Winterich, K. P., Mittal, V., & Aquino, K. (2013). When does recognition increase charitable behavior? Toward a moral
identity-based model. Journal of Marketing,77(3), 121–134. https://doi.org/10.1509/jm.11.0477
Wisneski, D. C., & Skitka, L. J. (2017). Moralization through moral shock: Exploring emotional antecedents to moral convic-
tion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,43(2), 139–150. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167216676479
Wolsko, C., Ariceaga, H., & Seiden, J. (2016). Red, white, and blue enough to be green: Effects of moral framing on climate
change attitudes and conservation behaviors. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,65,7–19.
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHIES
Matthew Feinberg is an Assistant Professor of Organizational Behavior and Human Resource Management at
the Rotman School of Management at the University of Toronto. He earned his PhD in social-personality psy-
chology from the University of California, Berkeley.
Robb Willer is a Professor of Sociology, Psychology (by courtesy), and Organizational Behavior (by courtesy) at
Stanford University. He earned his PhD in sociology from Cornell University.
How to cite this article: Feinberg M, Willer R. Moral reframing: A technique for effective and persuasive
communication across political divides. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2019;13:e12501. https://doi.org/10.
1111/spc3.12501
12 of 12 FEINBERG AND WILLER