ArticlePDF Available

Howling by the river: howler monkey (Alouatta palliata) communication in an anthropogenically-altered riparian forest in Costa Rica

Authors:
  • University of Toronto Mississauga (UTM)

Abstract and Figures

The ways that forest edges may affect animal vocalization behaviour are poorly understood. We investigated the effects of various types of edge habitat on the loud calls (howls) of a folivorous-frugivorous primate species, Alouatta palliata, with reference to the ecological resource defence hypothesis, which predicts that males howl to defend vegetation resources. We tested this hypothesis across four forest zones — interior, riparian, anthropogenic, and combined forest edges — in a riparian forest fragment in Costa Rica. We predicted vegetation and howling would differ between forest zones, with riparian and interior zones showing the highest values and anthropogenic edge the lowest. Our results indicated that vegetation was richer and howling longer in riparian and interior zones compared to combined and anthropogenic edges, supporting the resource defence hypothesis and providing some of the first evidence in animal communication scholarship for differences in behavioural edge effects between natural riparian and anthropogenic edges.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Behaviour (2019) DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003582 brill.com/beh
Howling by the river: howler monkey (Alouatta palliata)
communication in an anthropogenically-altered
riparian forest in Costa Rica
Laura M. Bolt a,c,,Dorian G. Russell c,d,Elizabeth M.C. Coggeshall c,e,
Zachary S. Jacobson c,f,Carrie Merrigan-Johnson c,gand Amy L. Schreier b,c
aDepartment of Anthropology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, Canada N2L 3G1
bDepartment of Biology, Regis University, Denver, CO 80221, USA
cThe Maderas Rainforest Conservancy, P.O. Box 55-7519, Miami, FL 33255-7519, USA
dDepartment of Environmental Science, American University, Washington, DC, USA
eDepartment of Anthropology, Central Washington University, Ellensburg, WA, USA
fDepartment of Anthropology and Archaeology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB,
Canada
gDepartment of Anthropology, University of Toronto at Mississauga, Mississauga, ON,
Canada
*Corresponding author’s e-mail address: laurabolt@gmail.com
Received 21 June 2019; initial decision 3 August 2019; revised 1 October 2019;
accepted 4 November 2019
Abstract
The ways that forest edges may affect animal vocalization behaviour are poorly understood. We
investigated the effects of various types of edge habitat on the loud calls (howls) of a folivorous-
frugivorous primate species, Alouatta palliata, with reference to the ecological resource defence
hypothesis, which predicts that males howl to defend vegetation resources. We tested this hypoth-
esis across four forest zones interior, riparian, anthropogenic, and combined forest edges in
a riparian forest fragment in Costa Rica. We predicted vegetation and howling would differ be-
tween forest zones, with riparian and interior zones showing the highest values and anthropogenic
edge the lowest. Our results indicated that vegetation was richer and howling longer in riparian
and interior zones compared to combined and anthropogenic edges, supporting the resource de-
fence hypothesis and providing some of the first evidence in animal communication scholarship
for differences in behavioural edge effects between natural riparian and anthropogenic edges.
Keywords
river edge, natural forest edge, edge effects, habitat quality, ecological resource defence
hypothesis, roar, long-distance call, loud call.
©Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2019 DOI 10.1163/1568539X-00003582
2Behaviour (2019) DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003582
Resumen
Las formas en que los bordes del bosque pueden afectar el comportamiento vocal de los animales
son poco conocidas. Investigamos los efectos de varios tipos de hábitat de borde en los gritos (aulli-
dos) de una especie de primate folívoro-frugívora, Alouatta palliata, con referencia a la hipótesis
de la defensa de los recursos ecológicos, la cual predice que los machos aúllan para defender los
recursos vegetales. Probamos esta hipótesis en cuatro zonas forestales: interior, ribereña, antro-
pogénica y una combinación de bordes de bosque, en un fragmento de bosque ribereño en Costa
Rica. Predijimos que la vegetación y el aullido diferirían entre las zonas forestales, con zonas
ribereñas e interiores mostrando los valores más altos y el borde antropogénico los más bajos.
Nuestros resultados indicaron que la vegetación era más rica y el aullido prolongado en las zonas
ribereñas e interiores en comparación con los bordes combinados y antropogénicos, respaldando
la hipótesis de defensa de los recursos y proporcionando algunas de las primeras pruebas en la
investigación de la comunicación animal para diferencias en el comportamiento relacionadas con
los efectos de bordeentre los bordes naturales ribereños y los antropogénicos.
Palabras clave
borde del río, borde natural del bosque, efectos de borde, calidad del habitat, defensa de
recursos ecológicos hipótesis, rugido, llamada de larga distancia, llamada fuerte.
1. Introduction
When forest adjoins a different habitat type, the borders represent an edge
zone, where there is a transition in the biotic and abiotic properties of the
landscape (Laurance, 1991; Schwitzer et al., 2011). The amount of moisture,
solar energy, and wind in edge zones differs from forest interior (Lovejoy et
al., 1986; Harris, 1988; Cadenasso et al., 2003), leading to changes in the
vegetation, which may impact how wildlife use the space. Forest edges may
be human-caused and due to deforestation, but also occur naturally. Riparian
(river) edges, for example, represent one type of natural edge, marking the
transition from aquatic to terrestrial environments (Swanson et al., 1982).
Riparian edges extend as far as 50 m from a riverbank (Grebner et al., 2013)
and are less windy with more moisture in the soil compared to other nearby
habitats (Swanson et al., 1982). Consequently, riparian zone vegetation usu-
ally has more water-tolerant plant and tree species (Richardson & Danehy,
2007) and higher biodiversity than other types of forest edge (Grebner et al.,
2013). Riparian edges are important corridors for wildlife, with undisturbed
vegetation along river edges allowing animals to travel through fragmented
forests and otherwise modified landscapes (Payne & Bryant, 1998). These
riparian corridors mitigate long-term negative impacts on species living in
fragmented landscapes through preventing isolation and inbreeding, and are
therefore important habitat features to preserve for conservation purposes
L.M. Bolt et al. / Behaviour (2019) 3
(Payne & Bryant, 1998). However, the large-scale destruction of tropical
forests worldwide (Haddad et al., 2015) has drastically increased the pro-
portion of anthropogenic forest edge. To meaningfully gauge the impact of
anthropogenic edges on wildlife, we need to better understand the effects of
naturally-occurring forest edge such as riparian edge on animal behaviour
(Murcia, 1995).
Non-human primates, as indicator species whose global welfare is threat-
ened by anthropogenic deforestation (Mace & Balmford, 2000; Estrada et
al., 2017), are ideal study subjects for investigating how edge effects im-
pact behaviour across both natural and anthropogenic edges. Primate long-
distance vocal communication behaviour differed between forest interior and
anthropogenic edge in previous study (Bolt et al., 2019), suggesting that
communication patterns should also be compared across natural riparian
edges. Long-distance calls (loud calls) are produced by a range of animal
taxa including primates, and can carry for more than 1 km through for-
est environments (Bradbury & Vehrencamp, 1998; Delgado, 2006). Loud
calls are typically uttered by adult males, exchanged between individuals
and groups, and thought to have a variety of adaptive functions including
the defence of resources (Bradbury & Vehrencamp, 1998; Wich & Nunn,
2002). The ecological resource defence hypothesis for loud calling posits
that males call to defend their group’s access to preferred food or space re-
sources, such as feeding trees and preferred territory areas (Wich & Nunn,
2002). While this hypothesis has support from a number of primate species
(e.g., red howler monkey (Alouatta seniculus), Sekulic, 1982; Thomas lan-
gur (Presbytis thomasi), van Schaik et al., 1992; black-and-white colobus
monkey (Colobus guereza), Fashing, 2001; gibbon (Hylobates spp.) Wich
et al., 2002), no studies to date have compared primate loud-calling be-
haviour across natural edges, anthropogenic edges, and forest interior within
the same forest landscape; doing so will lead to increased awareness of how
diverse types of edge effects may impact primates and will help clarify the
effects of anthropogenic habitat destruction.
This study examines the effects of riparian and anthropogenic edges on
monkey loud calling behaviour within a fragmented tropical rainforest in
Costa Rica, the La Suerte Biological Research Station (LSBRS). LSBRS is
located in the Caribbean lowland region of northeastern Costa Rica which
has been mostly deforested since the 1970’s due to ranching and corporate
agriculture (Garber et al., 2010; Molina, 2015). LSBRS has been reforested
4Behaviour (2019) DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003582
since the 1990’s, and now has tall, secondary growth forest through much of
the site (Molina, 2015; Brandt & Singleton, 2018; Russell, 2018). It currently
represents one of the few relatively large (3 km2) forest fragments in the
broader region. At LSBRS, the forest interior consists of a dense, continuous
canopy of large trees (Pruetz & Leasor, 2002) while the anthropogenic forest
edge transitions sharply from continuous forest to roads, ranches and other
plantations (Molina, 2015). The La Suerte River flows through the interior of
LSBRS before joining the Tortuguero River and draining into the Caribbean
Ocean. The presence of natural riparian edges adjacent to the forest interior
as well as anthropogenic edges surrounding the fragment at LSBRS make
this an ideal site at which to examine differences in vocalization behaviour
across forest interior and various types of forest edge zones.
Previous investigation at this site (Bolt et al., 2019) compared the loud
calling (howling) behaviour of a platyrrhine primate species, the mantled
howler monkey (Alouatta palliata), across forest interior and 100 m an-
thropogenic edge zones, and found that howling bouts in the interior were
significantly longer and contained a greater number of howls than in an-
thropogenic edge. These results supported the ecological resource defence
hypothesis, with longer howling bouts found in areas of richer interior vege-
tation resources (Bolt et al., 2019). The present follow-up study divides the
forest at LSBRS into a greater number of landscape zones to further exam-
ine variation in howling behaviour and its potential causes, in order to more
accurately assess the effects of forest edge on monkeys inhabiting a forest
fragment.
1.1. Hypothesis 1: vegetation will differ in riparian vs. anthropogenic edges
Based on variation in vegetation across riparian and anthropogenic forest
zones (Richardson & Danehy, 2007; Grebner et al., 2013), we expected that
riparian edge would differ in vegetation characteristics compared to forest
interior and anthropogenic edge. Previous research at LSBRS on a separate
dataset of a smaller number of vegetation transects found lower tree canopy
cover and tree species richness in anthropogenic edge compared to forest in-
terior (Bolt et al., 2018, 2019), but did not examine vegetation differences
between riparian edge and other habitat types. Given these findings (Bolt et
al., 2018, 2019), we predicted that percentage of canopy cover, mean tree
diameter at breast height (DBH), and mean tree height would vary between
forest zones, with forest interior showing the highest vegetation values, ripar-
ian edge showing intermediate values, and anthropogenic edge showing the
L.M. Bolt et al. / Behaviour (2019) 5
lowest vegetation values. We considered the vegetation measures included
in this study to indicate high-quality vegetation for primates as previous re-
search has indicated that the size and abundance of trees in a forest area pre-
dict primate abundance (Ross & Srivastava, 1994; Mbora & Meikle, 2004),
while higher levels of canopy cover signify a better-quality primate habitat
(Arroyo-Rodríguez & Mandujano, 2006).
1.2. Hypothesis 2: howling for ecological resource defence
If, as previously suggested, howler monkeys at LSBRS howl to defend eco-
logical resources (Bolt et al., 2019), we would expect howling to occur more
often in areas of rich vegetation, which are likely to contain more preferred
resources for howler monkeys. Given previous findings at LSBRS showing
longer howling bouts containing a greater number of howls in forest interior
compared to anthropogenic edge (Bolt et al., 2019), we similarly predicted
that monkeys would howl at the highest rates, have the longest howling
bouts, and produce more howls per howling bout when in forest interior
regions and riparian edge regions, in order to defend the high-quality veg-
etation resources likely to be located there. Building on previous findings,
we also expected howling rate would be higher and howling bouts would be
longer with more howls in riparian edge compared to anthropogenic edge,
given the higher plant species richness found in natural edges compared to
anthropogenic edges (Ramos & Santos, 2006; Skrinyer, 2016).
2. Methods
2.1. Study site
We conducted this study at the La Suerte Biological Research Station (LS-
BRS) in northeastern Costa Rica (10°26N, 83°46W). This site is a tropical
lowland rainforest comprising approximately 3 km2of primary forest, sec-
ondary forest, and regenerating pastures (Pruetz & Leasor, 2002; Garber et
al., 2010). The main forest fragment where we conducted this study is com-
posed of two connected forest patches (‘Large Forest’ =0.94 km2to the
north end of the site and ‘Small Forest’ =0.35 km2to the south end of the
site) and a small deforested area for ‘camp’ (0.07 km2in the centre of the
site) (Molina, 2015; Bolt et al., 2018, 2019; Figure 1).
The mantled howler monkey is a group-living, folivorous-frugivorous
platyrrhine primate that is well known for its howl vocalizations (di Fiore
6Behaviour (2019) DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003582
Figure 1. Map of La Suerte Biological Research Station (LSBRS) showing 50 m edge zones
and vegetation survey plots.
et al., 2011; reviewed in Kitchen et al., 2015; described in detail in Bolt
et al., 2019). This species shares LSBRS with the Central American spider
monkey (Ateles geoffroyi) and the white-faced capuchin monkey (Cebus ca-
pucinus). Population survey estimates suggest that the Large Forest contains
L.M. Bolt et al. / Behaviour (2019) 7
6–8 groups of howler monkeys, while the Small Forest contains 2–3 groups
(Pruetz & Leasor, 2002; Garber et al., 2010). During the course of this study,
we observed and collected data from approximately 11 groups of mantled
howler monkeys (Bolt et al., 2019).
2.2. Vegetation data collection
Although previous investigations at LSBRS defined anthropogenic forest
edge as 100 m (Bolt et al., 2018, 2019), for the current study we defined
forest edge as 50 m from any natural or anthropogenic forest boundary, fol-
lowing previous research evaluating the depth of edge influence on plants, in-
sects, and birds in both natural and disturbed forest landscapes (e.g., Harper
& Macdonald, 2001; Magura, 2002; Harper et al., 2005; Roume et al., 2011;
Stone et al., 2018). This 50 m edge definition for riparian edge is better sup-
ported by existing literature (Laurance et al., 2009; Pohlman et al., 2009;
Grebner et al., 2013). We divided the forest at LSBRS into four different
zones for analysis: riparian forest edge (within 50 m of the La Suerte River),
forest interior (more than 50 m from any type of forest edge), anthropogenic
forest edge (within 50 m of deforested area including camp, roads, and neigh-
bouring cattle ranches and coconut plantations), and combined riparian and
anthropogenic forest edge (the edge area within 50 m of both the river and
anthropogenically deforested area that contains both (i.e., overlapping) types
of forest edge; referred to as combined edge; Figure 1).
We collected vegetation data from May–August 2017 and May–July 2018,
with a total of 44 vegetation plots conducted throughout the LSBRS for-
est fragment: 6 plots in riparian edge, 25 plots in forest interior, 9 plots in
anthropogenic edge, and 4 plots in combined edge (Figure 1). Vegetation
plots measured 5 ×10 m and were spread throughout LSBRS across ri-
parian edge, forest interior, anthropogenic edge, and combined edge zones
(Figure 1). Using a central transect line within each plot, at each one-meter
interval we estimated canopy cover with a cardboard scope on a scale of 1–4
(1 =0–25% cover, 2 =26–50%, 3 =51–75% and 4 =76–100%). Within
each vegetation plot, we measured all trees over 10 cm in circumference with
a standard forestry tape reel, then calculated tree diameter at breast height
(DBH). Within each plot we also measured tree height using a hypsometer
for all trees 10 m tall or higher. We only collected tree height data in 2018
(N=22 of 44 vegetation plots).
8Behaviour (2019) DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003582
2.3. Howling data collection
We collected data on mantled howler monkey howling behaviour from May–
August 2017 and 2018 (see Bolt et al., 2019 for a map of the site showing
behavioural sampling points). Researchers actively searched for and sampled
monkeys daily between 500 and 1800 hours. We followed groups as they
travelled across various edge and interior habitat zones, and all-occurrences
of howling data were collected for continuous durations of time ranging from
30 min to 5 h. Due to the dense foliage, high forest canopy, and poor visibility
at LSBRS, data collectors stayed as close as possible to monkey groups dur-
ing sampling to ensure that monkeys remained visible. All monkey groups
were well-habituated and did not react to researchers.
During all time spent with a mantled howler monkey group, we con-
ducted all-occurrences sampling (Altmann, 1974) for group-wide howling
behaviour (following Bolt, 2013a, b; Hopkins, 2013; Bolt et al., 2019).
Groups of monkeys ranged from three to 20 individuals of both sexes (Bolt
et al., 2019). When howling from one or more members of the focal group
was heard, we recorded the start time and end time of the howling bout,
the number of howls in the bout, the location of the howling bout (recorded
the location of the howling male by noting the closest trail marker to him
and taking a single GPS point as close to him as possible using a Garmin
GPSMAP 62s Handheld GPS Navigator), and any potential precursor(s) in
the seconds preceding the howl (Bolt et al., 2019). These known howling
triggers were recorded qualitatively, then grouped into one of six categories
for analysis: (1) no observed precursor, (2) other group howling, (3) envi-
ronmental factor (e.g., rain, wind, thunder), (4) intra-group social behaviour
(e.g., agonism, group travel, high arousal non-howl vocalization), (5) inter-
species interaction (e.g., dog, bird, or other monkey species was seen by
researchers and observed interacting with focal howler monkey group) and
(6) anthropogenic noise (e.g., lawn mower, chainsaw, airplane) (Baldwin &
Baldwin, 1976; Van Belle et al., 2013; reviewed in Kitchen et al., 2015; Bolt
et al., 2019).
Howls were defined as male-specific, high-amplitude Type 1/A loud calls
(Carpenter, 1934; Altmann, 1959; Baldwin & Baldwin, 1976) separated from
other long-distance vocalizations made by the caller or members of the same
group by at least 2 s. If individual vocal utterances were continuous or sepa-
rated by less than two seconds, they were considered part of the same howl
vocalization (Bolt et al., 2019). Howls were contained within howling bouts.
L.M. Bolt et al. / Behaviour (2019) 9
A howling bout comprised howls occurring less than 60 seconds apart
(following Sekulic, 1982; Bolt et al., 2019). Howling bouts could therefore
be seconds long and consist of a single howl vocalization by one male, or
many minutes long and consist of a large number of howl vocalizations by
multiple males from the same group. In addition to recording GPS points
to note locations of howling bouts, a GPS point was taken at the location
of every 30-minute period we spent in the presence of a monkey group,
regardless of whether howling occurred during the sample (Bolt et al., 2019).
These GPS sampling points were taken in the approximate centre of the
monkey group and allowed us to determine behavioural sampling frequency
across various forest zones at LSBRS.
2.4. Data analysis
To determine differences in vegetation and howling characteristics across
the four forest zones, we compared percentage of canopy cover, mean tree
DBH, mean tree height, mean length of howler monkey howling bouts, mean
number of howls in howling bouts, and mean howling rate using Kruskal–
Wallis H-tests. For significant values, we performed pairwise asymptotic
(2-sided) post-hoc tests with significance adjusted by the Bonferroni correc-
tion for multiple tests to determine which forest zones differed in vegetation
and howling characteristics. To compare whether howling precursors showed
different distributions than expected by chance across the four forest zones,
we used a Pearson chi-squared test. As a post-hoc test to determine which
precursors varied across forest zones, we examined adjusted residuals and
identified those with z-scores greater than ±1.97 as indicating differences
across forest zones. We used SPSS version 25 (IBM SPSS Statistics, IBM,
Armonk, NY, USA) for all statistical tests, and set the alpha level to 0.05.
3. Results
At LSBRS, overall mean level of canopy cover was 3.00 (approx. 51–75%
coverage; SD =0.98, range =0–100% coverage, N=494), mean tree DBH
was 16.85 cm (SD =28.49, range =3.18–256.24 cm, N=537 trees), and
mean tree height was 15.59 m (SD =5.06, range =10.2–30.2 m, N=70
trees).
Mean level of canopy cover differed significantly across forest zones
at LSBRS (Kruskal–Wallis H3=21.947, p=0.000, N=494; Figure 2).
10 Behaviour (2019) DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003582
Figure 2. Level of canopy cover across forest zones at LSBRS (p=0.000). Boxes repre-
sent inter-quartile ranges, lines represent median values, whiskers represent maximum and
minimum values and dots represent outliers. Similar asterisks indicate significant differences
between paired forest zones (riparian vs. combined edge, p=0.001; forest interior vs. com-
bined edge, p=0.000).
Canopy cover was significantly higher in riparian edge (3.13 (51–75%
canopy cover), SD =0.89, range =1–4 (0–100% canopy cover), N=76)
than combined edge (2.39 (26–50% canopy cover), SD =1.02, range =
1–4 (0–100% canopy cover, N=44); p=0.001), as was forest interior
(3.11 (51–75% canopy cover), SD =0.95, range =1–4 (0–100% canopy
cover), N=275; p=0.000; Figure 2). Mean level of canopy cover in an-
thropogenic edge (2.88 (26–50% canopy cover), SD =1.00, range =1–4
(0–100% canopy cover), N=99) did not differ significantly from any forest
zones, but showed the non-significant tendency to be higher than combined
edge and lower than riparian edge and forest interior.
Mean tree DBH also varied significantly across forest zones at LSBRS
(Kruskal–Wallis H3=7.894, p=0.048, N=537 trees; Figure 3). Mean
tree DBH was significantly higher in riparian edge (20.49 cm, SD =30.08,
range =3.18–156.55 cm, N=61 trees) than combined edge (12.64 cm,
SD =18.16, range =3.18–83.4 cm, N=49 trees; p=0.042). Mean tree
DBH in forest interior (16.86 cm, SD =26.95, range =3.18–256.24 cm,
N=309 trees) and anthropogenic edge (16.68 cm, SD =34.55, range =
3.18–254.65 cm, N=118 trees) did not differ significantly from other forest
L.M. Bolt et al. / Behaviour (2019) 11
Figure 3. Mean tree DBH across forest zones at LSBRS (p=0.048). Boxes represent
inter-quartile ranges, lines represent median values and whiskers represent maximum and
minimum values. Outliers were removed for graphical purposes. Asterisks indicate signifi-
cant differences between paired forest zones (riparian vs. combined edge, p=0.042).
zones, but showed the tendency of being lower than riparian edge and higher
than combined edge.
Finally, tree height did not differ significantly across forest zones
(Kruskal–Wallis H2=1.15, p=0.563, N=70 trees). Mean tree height
was highest in riparian edge (17.92 m, SD =6.99, range =10.3–29.2 m,
N=9 trees), lower in forest interior (15.30 m, SD =4.98, range =10.2–
30.2 m, N=48 trees), and lowest in anthropogenic edge (15.05 m, SD =
3.53, range =10.2–20.2 m, N=13 trees). There were no trees 10 m in
combined edge.
Howling bout length differed significantly across forest zones (Kruskal–
Wallis H3=14.355, p=0.002, N=641, Figure 4). Pairwise post-hoc
tests showed significant differences in mean howl bout length between ri-
parian edge (165.52 s, SD =388.98, range =1–2882 s, N=156) and
anthropogenic edge (104.62 s, SD =210.32, range =1–1641 s, N=197;
p=0.007) and between riparian edge and combined edge (117.38 s, SD =
251.27, range =1–1133 s, N=63; p=0.042), with longer howl bout
lengths at riparian edge than at combined and anthropogenic edges. Mean
howling bout length in forest interior (mean bout length =109.54 s, SD =
174.69, range =1–1260 s; N=225) did not differ significantly from any
12 Behaviour (2019) DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003582
Figure 4. Mean howl bout length in seconds across forest zones at LSBRS (p=0.002).
Boxes represent inter-quartile ranges, lines represent median values and whiskers represent
maximum and minimum values. Outliers were removed for graphical purposes. Similar aster-
isks indicate significant differences between paired forest zones (riparian vs. anthropogenic
edge, p=0.007; riparian vs. combined edge, p=0.042).
forest zones, but tended to be shorter than in riparian and combined edges
and slightly longer than in anthropogenic edge.
Mean number of howls per bout also varied significantly across forest
zones (Kruskal–Wallis H3=11.381, p=0.010, N=433, Figure 5). Pair-
wise post-hoc tests indicated significant differences in mean number of howls
per bout between forest interior (5.66 howls/bout, SD =8.90, range =1–60
howls/bout, N=147) and anthropogenic edge (3.77 howls/bout, SD =6.95,
range =1–53 howls/bout, N=158; p=0.035), but other forest zones did
not differ significantly in howling bout length when adjusted values were
compared pairwise. Mean number of howls per bout was highest in riparian
edge (7.86 howls/bout, SD =26.98, range =1–176 howls/bout, N=79),
second-highest in forest interior, smaller in combined edge (5.49 howls/bout,
SD =14.19, range =1–78 howls/bout, N=49) and lowest in anthropogenic
edge.
Howling rate between forest zones did not differ significantly (Kruskal–
Wallis H3=7.639, p=0.054, N=568.5 hours), although differences
closely approached significance. Mean howling rate was highest in ripar-
ian edge (1.36 howls/h, SD =2.43, range =0–12 howls/h, N=111.5h),
L.M. Bolt et al. / Behaviour (2019) 13
Figure 5. Mean number of howls per bout across forest zones at LSBRS (p=0.01). Boxes
represent inter-quartile ranges, lines represent median values and whiskers represent maxi-
mum and minimum values. Outliers were removed for graphical purposes. Asterisks indicate
significant differences between paired forest zones (forest interior vs. anthropogenic edge,
p=0.035).
second-highest in anthropogenic edge (1.29 howls/h, SD =2.57, range =
0–14 howls/h, N=159.5 h), lower in combined edge (1.23 howls/h, SD =
2.17, range =0–10 howls/h, N=50.5 h) and lowest in forest interior (0.895
howls/h, SD =2.02, range =0–18 howls/h, N=247 h).
Finally, there were significant differences in howling precursors across
forest zones (x2(15)=65.783, p=0.000, N=624). The strength of as-
sociation between variables was strong (Cramer’s V-test: ϕc=0.187, p=
0.000), and post-hoc examination of adjusted residuals indicated that when
adjusted for sample size, monkey howling bouts in riparian edge were pre-
ceded by no precursors more frequently than expected and preceded by
environmental factors and intra-group interactions less frequently than ex-
pected by chance (z>±1.97, Table 1). In forest interior, howling bouts were
preceded by inter-species interactions more frequently than expected and by
no precursors less frequently than expected by chance (z>±1.97, Table 1).
In anthropogenic edge, howling bouts were preceded by howls from other
groups and inter-species interactions less frequently than expected, and pre-
ceded by intra-group interactions, environmental factors, and anthropogenic
noise more frequently than expected (z>±1.97, Table 1). In combined
14 Behaviour (2019) DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003582
Table 1.
Mantled howler monkey (Alouatta palliata) howling precursors at LSBRS showing z-scores for adjusted residual values.
No howling
precursor
Howl from
other group
Environmental
factor
Intra-group
social behaviour
Inter-species
interaction
Anthropogenic
noise
Riparian edge 3.51.23.12.01.81.7
Forest interior 2.81.50.80.63.90.2
Anthropogenic edge 1.23.42.33.42.72.0
Combined edge 2.01.01.81.40.31.2
Asterisks show significant differences between zones (values >±1.97). Negative results indicate that howling occurrence following a
precursor was lower than expected by chance, while positive results indicate that howling occurrence following a precursor was higher than
expected by chance.
L.M. Bolt et al. / Behaviour (2019) 15
edge, howling was preceded by no precursor less frequently than expected
(z>±1.97, Table 1). Other observed values for howling precursors did not
differ from expected values across forest zones (z<±1.97, Table 1).
4. Discussion
Our results provided some support for the ecological resource defence hy-
pothesis, although alternate explanations are also feasible. Mean level of
canopy cover was higher at riparian edge and forest interior than in com-
bined edge, and mean tree DBH was higher at riparian edge than combined
edge. Tree height also showed a consistent though non-significant trend of
being highest at riparian edge and lowest at anthropogenic edge, indicating
that riparian zones tend to contain taller, larger trees than anthropogenic and
combined edge zones. Monkey howling behaviour also varied across habitat
zones, suggesting that differences in vegetation characteristics may be linked
to observed differences in monkey howling behaviour.
Monkey howl bout length, number of howls per bout, and howling rate
were all highest at riparian edge and bout length and number of howls per
bout were also high in forest interior, suggesting that monkeys may be howl-
ing to defend higher quality vegetation resources in these zones or some other
type of habitat feature, such as denser habitat. These results support previous
findings at LSBRS that mantled howler monkeys have longer howling bouts
containing a greater number of howls in forest interior areas with richer veg-
etation compared to anthropogenic edge areas with poorer vegetation (Bolt
et al., 2019).
Howler monkeys may howl for longer durations of time and utter bouts
containing a greater number of howls when in riparian edge zone due to
the greater proportion of primary forest containing mature feeding tree
species along the river in the largely secondary-growth forest at LSBRS
(Molina, 2015; Russell, 2018). Our vegetation surveys assessed overall tree
and canopy characteristics, but did not identify primary vs. secondary forest
areas, preferred howler monkey feeding tree species, or relative feeding tree
size across forest zones. Mantled howler monkeys preferentially eat from
trees and are selective about the tree species from which they feed (Estrada,
1984). It may be that the riparian zone contains larger, more mature preferred
feeding tree species than all other zones due to its comparative protection
from deforestation during LSBRS’s history as a cattle plantation prior to the
16 Behaviour (2019) DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003582
1990’s (Garber et al., 2010; Molina, 2015). In tropical regions, riparian zone
trees are more frequently left intact to prevent erosion and nutrient leach-
ing from the soil, even when a site’s other trees may be cut to enable use
of the land for cattle pasture or agriculture (Lees & Peres, 2008). LSBRS
followed this trend of leaving many riparian zone trees particularly those
closest to the river when clearing parts of the site for use as a ranch in
the 1960’s (Renee Molina, personal communication). Thus, riparian zones
at LSBRS likely contain a greater proportion of primary forest than other
edge and interior forest zones at LSBRS, which may be driving the observed
differences in howling behaviour. Howler monkeys spend more time in trees
that are larger than average within forests (Munoz et al., 2006) and at LS-
BRS fed from trees that were taller and had higher mean DBH than average
across each habitat zone (Russell, 2018; Schreier & Bolt, unpublished data),
thus exhibiting preference for large trees as food sources. The higher canopy
cover in riparian and interior forest zones at LSBRS suggests that there are
more of these large trees growing closely together in these habitats. However,
further research focused on howler monkey feeding tree usage at LSBRS is
needed.
Howler monkeys may also howl for longer durations in riparian edge and
forest interior for reasons unrelated to vegetation quality. Instead, observed
differences could be due to the acoustics of how this long-range vocalization
carries through vegetation in diverse forest zones. Howls typically carry for
longer distances through thick forest vegetation than through open space (de
Vore, 1979; Waser & Brown, 1986), but are more difficult to localize in these
areas due to differences in sound reverberation (i.e., sound dispersal through
propagation as a result of being reflected by objects; Naguib & Wiley, 2001;
da Cunha et al., 2015), meaning that howler monkeys may need to howl for
longer periods of time when in dense foliage to ensure they can be heard
and located by neighbouring groups. Loud calling is generally energetically
expensive for organisms, and costs are higher when vocalizations are longer
and/or repeated at higher rates (Zahavi, 1975; Prestwich, 1994; Bradbury
& Vehrencamp, 1998). It is adaptive for howler monkeys to utter longer
howling bouts containing a greater number of howls if doing so minimizes
close-range interactions with males from other groups, given that inter-group
encounters often harm mature males (Sekulic, 1982; Chiarello, 1995).
Howler monkeys at LSBRS howl for the longest durations in riparian edge
and forest interior, and may do so to enable better sound localization through
L.M. Bolt et al. / Behaviour (2019) 17
the higher level of canopy cover and thicker vegetation found in these zones,
thus making sure that other monkey groups know where they are. With the ri-
parian edge bordering the river and therefore containing more open space in
the adjacent environment than in forest interior, however, the more open river
environment would seem to contradict longer howls being necessary as com-
pensation for sound reverberation (Waser & Brown, 1986; Naguib & Wiley,
2001). In the present study, howler monkeys in riparian edge howled sponta-
neously (i.e., in response to no known precursor) more often than expected
by chance, while they howled in response to environmental noise less often
than expected. In contrast, monkeys in forest interior howled spontaneously
less often than expected, but howled in response to environmental noise no
more or less often than predicted. These differences between riparian and
interior forest zones may be due to the background noise provided by the La
Suerte River, which could act as a sound source that masks other types of en-
vironmental sound in the riparian edge (e.g., thunder or rain) and/or falsely
triggers howling due to monkeys not being able to clearly identify other types
of sound in their environment. The La Suerte River is fast-flowing, and while
not high in amplitude, is audible in many parts of the riparian edge zone at
LSBRS. This higher level of background noise in the riparian zone could
be stimulating monkeys to both howl spontaneously and utter longer howl-
ing bouts as a way of ensuring other monkey groups do not approach. This
phenomenon could also account for the trend towards higher howling rate
in riparian edge zone. Monkeys would thus howl more by the river than in
other forest zones as a precautionary measure, to discourage the approach of
other monkey groups which they would be less able to accurately detect in
the riparian zone due to a higher level of background noise.
Howl bout length and number of howls per bout may also differ across for-
est zones due to differences in predation pressure at LSBRS. Animals who
call more often and/or for longer periods of time are easier for predators to
locate (Bolt, 2016), which means that it is adaptive for potential prey species
to vocalize less in areas of higher risk. A variety of predators exist at LSBRS,
including ocelots (Leopardus pardalis), tayra (Eira barbara) and venomous
snakes (e.g., Bothrops asper) (Molina, 2015), so it is likely advantageous
for monkeys to be cautious of unfamiliar animals in this species-rich envi-
ronment. While predator abundance across habitat zones has not yet been
determined at LSBRS, increased sound reverberation in the forest interior as
well as the background noise caused by the river in riparian zones may help
18 Behaviour (2019) DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003582
conceal the location of calling animals from potential predators (Naguib &
Wiley, 2001; da Cunha et al., 2015), meaning that animals may call for longer
durations in these zones without increasing their risk of detection. Further
study is needed to examine the interaction between predator abundance and
howling behaviour across forest zones.
Another possibility is that monkey groups could be howling to defend
specific territory areas at La Suerte. While it is still unknown whether or not
the mantled howler monkey is a territorial species, mantled howler monkey
researchers at some other sites have observed territorial behaviour (e.g., Car-
penter, 1934; Chivers, 1969, but see Mitani & Rodman, 1979; Milton, 1980;
Crockett & Eisenberg, 1987), suggesting that howling behaviour could vary
at La Suerte based on variation in vegetation characteristics between group
territories. Of the 11 monkey groups we studied, we noticed that individual
groups did tend to range within the same general areas, with some overlap
among groups. However, since we were not able to reliably identify groups
during the study period, we were unable to systematically test if the howler
monkeys at La Suerte demonstrated territoriality and the potential connec-
tion between group-level howling behaviour and vegetation quality within a
group’s range. Future study should address these possibilities.
Finally, it is possible that the longer howling bouts in the riparian re-
gion may not relate directly to any one factor, but instead could be due to a
range of variables which are not mutually exclusive. These include a greater
concentration of rich feeding resources with a higher proportion of mature
feeding trees in the riparian zone, acoustic propagation factors due to land-
scape features causing howl bouts to be longer by the river and in forest
interior, and/or lower predator vigilance in the riparian zone.
We collected both howling and vegetation data in May–August over a
two-year period during the Costa Rican wet season. Data collected at other
times of year may yield different results. However, seasonality is not known
to influence feeding behaviour or habitat use in the mantled howler monkey
(Chapman, 1988; Bolt et al., 2018), suggesting that it should not impact
howling behaviour across forest zones (Bolt et al., 2019). We compared
group-level howling differences across habitat zones, but were not able to test
individual male or social group differences in howling behaviour at LSBRS.
We tested the ecological resource defence hypothesis in relation to overall
vegetation quality, but did not test how other forms of resource defence,
such as feeding or resting tree species richness or abundance, may relate
L.M. Bolt et al. / Behaviour (2019) 19
to howling behaviour. Future study should examine individual differences in
howling behaviour, as well as a broader range of ecological factors that may
influence habitat use in the mantled howler monkey.
Our results have important conservation considerations. Mantled howler
monkey howling differed significantly between natural riparian and anthro-
pogenic edge, suggesting that it is proximity to human-caused edge, rather
than to forest edge itself, that is altering monkey communication behaviour
at LSBRS (Bolt et al., 2019). While it is yet unknown what implications
these behavioural changes across different edge zones may have for monkey
fitness, any human-caused alteration of primate behaviour should be avoided
as a general animal welfare goal. Our results suggest that Central American
conservation initiatives should prioritize riparian edge and forest interior for
protection within forests, while anthropogenic edge should be reforested to
ensure the long-term well-being of the mantled howler monkey.
Overall, we found that mantled howler monkey howling properties var-
ied across forest zones at LSBRS. While our results may be interpreted in
several non-mutually exclusive ways, they provide some support for the eco-
logical resource defence hypothesis. In addition to contributing to the body
of scholarship on howler monkey howling function (reviewed in Kitchen et
al., 2015), we provide the first evidence of differences in animal loud calling
behaviour between natural riparian and anthropogenic forest edges.
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to Renee Molina and the Maderas Rainforest Conservancy
for their support and facilitation of our research at the La Suerte Biological
Research Station, Costa Rica. We thank Philip Quinn, Sophie Lieber, Micah
Adams, Stacy Hill and Mareike Janiak for howling data collection. We also
thank Aspen Russell and Stacy Hill for aid in conducting vegetation sur-
veys, and acknowledge Micah Adams, Carson Black, Seriana Gamble, Emily
Glotfelty, Montana Goldsmith, Heather Hicks, Alyssa MacAusland, Renee
Russell and Katie Travis for providing assistance. We acknowledge Ryan
Janzen and Michael Ennis for project support, Eugenia Tsao for technolog-
ical assistance, and Tristan Rhys Williams for inspiration. We also thank
María José Sandí for Spanish abstract translation. This research protocol
was approved by the Regis University Animal Care Committee and was con-
ducted with the permission of the Molina family. Our research was supported
20 Behaviour (2019) DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003582
by a University Research and Scholarship Council (URSC) Faculty Research
and Scholarship Grant (Regis University), a Cosmos Scholar Award (Cosmos
Club Foundation), an Explorer’s Club Exploration and Field Research Grant,
and the American University Mellon Fund for Graduate Student Research.
References
Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49:
227-265.
Altmann, S. (1959). Field observations on a howling monkey society. J. Mammal. 40:
317-330.
Arroyo-Rodríguez, V. & Mandujano, S. (2006). Forest fragmentation modifies habitat quality
for Alouatta palliata. Int. J. Primatol. 27: 1079-1096.
Baldwin, J. & Baldwin, J. (1976). Vocalizations of howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata)in
Southwestern Panama. Folia Primatol. 26: 81-108.
Bolt, L. (2013a). The function of howling in the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta). Int. J.
Primatol. 34: 157-169.
Bolt, L. (2013b). The relationship between dominance and vocal communication in the male
ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta). PhD Dissertation, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON.
Bolt, L. (2016). Alarm calling upon predator detection. In: Encyclopedia of evolutionary
psychological science (Shackelford, T. & Weekes-Shackelford, V., eds). Springer, New
York, NY. DOI:10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_1513-1.
Bolt, L., Schreier, A., Voss, K., Sheehan, E., Barrickman, N., Pryor, N. & Barton, M. (2018).
Influence of anthropogenic edge effects on primate populations and their habitat in a
fragmented rainforest in Costa Rica. Primates 59: 301-311.
Bolt, L., Schreier, A., Russell, D., Jacobson, Z., Merrigan-Johnson, C., Barton, M. & Cogge-
shall, E. (2019). Howling on the edge: mantled howler monkey (Alouatta palliata)howl-
ing behaviour and anthropogenic edge effects in a fragmented tropical rainforest in Costa
Rica. Ethology 125: 593-602.
Bradbury, J. & Vehrencamp, S. (1998). Principles of animal communication. Sinauer
Associates, Sunderland, MA.
Brandt, L. & Singleton, M. (2018). Record of Baird’s Tapir Tapirus bardii at the La Suerte
Biological Field Station in the Caribbean Lowlands of Costa Rica. PeerJ Prepr. 6:
e27128v1.
Cadenasso, M., Pickett, S., Weathers, K. & Jones, C. (2003). A framework for a theory of
ecological boundaries. BioScience 53: 750-758.
Carpenter, C. (1934). A field study of the behavior and social relations of howling monkeys
Alouatta palliata. Comp. Psychol. Monogr. 10: 1-168.
Chapman, C. (1988). Patterns of foraging and range use by three species of Neotropical
primates. Primates 29: 177-194.
Chiarello, A. (1995). Role of loud calls in brown howlers, Alouatta fusca.—Am.J.Primatol.
36: 213-222.
L.M. Bolt et al. / Behaviour (2019) 21
Chivers, D. (1969). On the daily behaviour and spacing of howling monkey groups. Folia
Primatol. 10: 48-102.
Crockett, C. & Eisenberg, J. (1987). Howlers: variations in group size and demography.
In: Primate societies (Smuts, B., Cheney, D., Seyfarth, R., Wrangham, R. & Struhsaker,
T., eds). University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, p. 54-68.
da Cunha, R., de Oliveira, D., Holzmann, I. & Kitchen, D. (2015). Production of loud
and quiet calls in howler monkeys. In: Howler monkeys. Developments in primatol-
ogy: progress and prospects (Kowalewski, M., Garber, P., Cortés-Ortiz, L., Urbani, B. &
Youlatos, D., eds). Springer, New York, NY, p. 337-368.
de Vore, I. (1979). A comparison of the ecology and behavior of monkeys and apes. In:
Primate ecology: problem oriented field studies (Sussman, R., ed.). Wiley, New York, NY,
p. 343-361.
Delgado, R. (2006). Sexual selection in the loud calls of male primates: signal content and
function. Int. J. Primatol. 27: 5-25.
di Fiore, A., Link, A. & Campbell, C. (2011). The atelines: behavioral and sociological
diversity in a New World monkey radiation. In: Primates in perspective (Campbell,
C., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K., Panger, M. & Bearder, S., eds). Oxford University Press,
New York, NY, p. 155-188.
Estrada, A. (1984). Resource use by howler monkeys in the rain forest of Los Tuxtlas,
Veracruz, Mexico. Int. J. Primatol. 5: 105-131.
Estrada, A., Garber, P., Rylands, A., Roos, C., Fernandez-Duque, E., Di Fiore, A., Nekaris,
K.A.-I., Nijman, V., Heymann, E.W., Lambert, J.E., Rovero, F., Barelli, C., Setchell,
J.M., Gillespie, T.R., Mittermeier, R.A., Verde Arregoitia, L., de Guinea, M., Gouveia,
S., Dobrovolski, R., Shanee, S., Shanee, N., Boyle, S.A., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K.C.,
Amato, K.R., Meyer, A.L.S., Wich, S., Sussman, R.W., Pan, R., Kone, I. & Li, B. (2017).
Impending extinction crisis of the world’s primates: why primates matter. Sci. Adv. 3:
e1600946.
Fashing, P. (2001). Male and female strategies during intergroup encounters in guerezas
(Colobus guereza): evidence for resource defense mediated through males and a com-
parison with other primates. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 50: 219-230.
Garber, P., Molina, A. & Molina, R. (2010). Putting the community back in community
ecology and education: the role of field schools and private reserves in the ethical training
of primatologists. Am. J. Primatol. 72: 785-793.
Grebner, D., Bettinger, P. & Siry, J. (2013). Introduction to forestry and natural resources.
Academic Press, New York, NY.
Haddad, N., Brudwig, L., Clobert, J., Davies, K., Gonzalez, A., Holt, R. & Townshend, J.
(2015). Habitat fragmentation and its lasting impact on Earth’s ecosystems. Sci. Adv.
1: e1500052.
Harper, K. & Macdonald, S. (2001). Structure and composition of riparian boreal forest: new
methods for analyzing edge influence. Ecology 82: 649-659.
Harper, K., Macdonald, S., Burton, P., Chen, J., Brosofske, K., Saunders, S., Euskirchen, E.,
Roberts, D., Jaiteh, M. & Esseen, P.-A. (2005). Edge influence on forest structure and
22 Behaviour (2019) DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003582
composition in fragmented landscapes. Conservation Biology. 19: 768-782. DOI:10.
1111/j.1523-1739.2005.00045.x.
Harris, L. (1988). Edge effects and conservation of biotic diversity. Conserv. Biol. 2: 330-
332.
Hopkins, M. (2013). Relative dominance and resource availability mediate mantled howler
(Alouatta palliata) spatial responses to neighbors’ loud calls. Int. J. Primatol. 34: 1032-
1054.
Kitchen, D., da Cunha, R., Holzmann, I. & de Oliveira, D. (2015). Function of loud calls
in howler monkeys. In: Howler monkeys. Developments in primatology: progress and
prospects (Kowalewski, M., Garber, P., Cortés-Ortiz, L., Urbani, B. & Youlatos, D., eds).
Springer, New York, NY, p. 369-399.
Laurance, W. (1991). Edge effects in tropical forest fragments: application of a model for the
design of nature reserves. Biol. Conserv. 57: 205-219.
Laurance, W., Goosem, M. & Laurance, S. (2009). Impacts of roads and linear clearings on
tropical forests. Trends Ecol. Evol. 24: 659-669.
Lees, A. & Peres, C. (2008). Conservation value of remnant riparian forest corridors of
varying quality for Amazonian birds and mammals. Conserv. Biol. 22: 439-449.
Lovejoy, T., Bierregaard, R., Rylands, A., Malcolm, J., Quintela, C., Harper, L., Brown,
K., Powell, A., Powell, G., Schubart, H. & Hays, M. (1986). Edge and other effects of
isolation on Amazon forest fragments. In: Conservation biology: the science of scarcity
and diversity (Soule, M., ed.). Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MA, p. 257-285.
Mace, G. & Balmford, A. (2000). Patterns and processes in contemporary mammalian ex-
tinction. In: Future priorities for the conservation of mammalian diversity (Entwhistle,
A. & Dunstone, N., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p. 27-52.
Magura, T. (2002). Carabids and forest edge: spatial pattern and edge effect. For. Ecol.
Manage. 157: 23-37.
Mbora, D. & Meikle, D. (2004). Forest fragmentation and the distribution, abundance and
conservation of the Tana River red colobus (Procolobus rufomitratus). Biol. Conserv.
118: 67-77.
Milton, K. (1980). The foraging strategy of howler monkeys: a study in primates eco-
nomics. Columbia University Press, New York, NY.
Mitani, J. & Rodman, P. (1979). Territoriality: the relation of ranging pattern and home range
size to defendability, with an analysis of territoriality among primate species. Behav.
Ecol. Sociobiol. 5: 241-251.
Molina, R. (2015). A brief history of the Molina family, and the birth of the Maderas Rainfor-
est Conservancy at the La Suerte and Ometepe Field Stations a narrative. In: Central
American biodiversity: conservation, ecology and a sustainable future (Huettman, F., ed.).
Springer Science +Business Media, New York, NY, p. 199-214.
Munoz, D., Estrada, A., Naranjo, E. & Ochoa, S. (2006). Foraging ecology of howler mon-
keys in a cacao (Theobroma cacao) plantation in Comalcalco, Mexico. Am. J. Primatol.
68: 127-142.
Murcia, C. (1995). Edge effects in fragmented forests: implications for conservation.
Trends Ecol. Evol. 10: 58-62.
L.M. Bolt et al. / Behaviour (2019) 23
Naguib, M. & Wiley, R. (2001). Estimating the distance to a source of sound: mechanism and
adaptations for long-range communication. Anim. Behav. 62: 825-837.
Payne, N. & Bryant, F. (1998). Wildlife habitat management of forest lands, rangelands, and
farmlands. Krieger, Malabar, FL.
Pohlman, C., Turton, S. & Goosem, M. (2009). Temporal variation in microclimatic edge
effects near powerlines, highways and streams in Australian tropical rainforest. Agric.
Forest Meteorol. 149: 84-95.
Prestwich, K. (1994). The energetic of acoustic signaling in anurans and insects. Am.
Zool. 34: 625-643.
Pruetz, J. & Leasor, H. (2002). Densities of primate species in forest fragments at La Suerte
Biological Field Station, Costa Rica. Neotrop. Primates 10: 4-9.
Ramos, F. & Santos, F. (2006). Floral visitors and pollination of Psychotria tenuinervis
(Rubiaceae): distance from the anthropogenic and natural edges of the Atlantic forest
fragment. Biotropica 38: 383-389.
Richardson, J. & Danehy, R. (2007). A synthesis of the ecology of headwater streams and
their riparian zones in temperate forests. Forest Sci. 53: 131-147.
Ross, C. & Srivastava, A. (1994). Factors influencing the population density of the Hanuman
langur (Prebytis entellus) in Sariska Tiger Reserve. Primates 35: 361-367.
Roume, A., Deconchat, M., Raison, L., Balent, G. & Ouin, A. (2011). Edge effects on
ground beetles (Coleoptera: Carabidae) at the woodlot-field interface are short-range and
asymmetrical. Agric. Forest Entomol. 35: 395-403.
Russell, D. (2018). The influence of edge effects on mantled howler monkey (Alouatta palli-
ata) food resource use and availability in a fragmented forest. M.Sc. Thesis, American
University, Washington, DC.
Schwitzer, C., Glatt, L., Nekaris, K. & Ganzhorn, J. (2011). Responses of animals to habitat
alteration: an overview focusing on primates. Endang. Species Res. 14: 31-38.
Sekulic, R. (1982). The function of howling in red howler monkeys (Alouatta seniculus).
Behaviour 81: 38-54.
Skrinyer, A. (2016). Living on the edge: an assessment of habitat disturbance and primate use
on the Osa Peninsula, Costa Rica. M.A. Thesis, Kent State University, Kent, OH.
Stone, M., Catterall, C. & Stork, N. (2018). Edge effects and beta diversity in ground and
canopy beetle communities of fragmented subtropical forest. PLoS ONE 13: e0193369.
Swanson, F., Gregory, S., Sedell, J. & Campbell, A. (1982). Land-water interactions: the
riparian zone. In: Analysis of coniferous forest ecosystems in the Western United States
(Edmonds, R., ed.). Hutchinson Ross, Stroudsburg, PA, p. 267-291.
Van Belle, S., Estrada, A. & Garber, P. (2013). Spatial and diurnal distribution of loud calling
in black howler monkeys (Alouatta pigra). Int. J. Primatol. 34: 1209-1224.
Van Schaik, C., Assink, P. & Salafsky, N. (1992). Territorial behavior in southeast Asian
langurs: resource defense or mate defense? Am. J. Primatol. 26: 233-242.
Waser, P. & Brown, C. (1986). Habitat acoustics and primate communication. Am. J.
Primatol. 10: 135-154.
Wich, S. & Nunn, C. (2002). Do male “long-distance calls” function in mate defense? A com-
parative study of long-distance calls in primates. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 52: 474-484.
24 Behaviour (2019) DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003582
Wich, S., Assink, P., Becher, F. & Sterck, E. (2002). Playbacks of long-distance calls to wild
Thomas langurs (Primates; Presbytis thomasi): the effect of location. Behaviour 139:
65-78.
Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection: a selection for a handicap. J. Theor. Biol. 53: 204-214.
... We collected mantled howler monkey social behavior data from 11 groups that ranged across both forest interior and anthropogenic edge zones at LSBRS . These same groups have also been the subjects for other behavioral investigations at LSBRS (Bolt et al., 2020a(Bolt et al., , 2021a(Bolt et al., , 2023bSchreier et al., 2022a), and comprise an unusually dense population of at least 149 individual monkeys or 109.5 individuals/km 2 , which is one of the highest population densities recorded at any site worldwide for this species Schreier et al., 2023). While we could not consistently identify individual monkeys between sampling seasons due to changing group demography, we consistently observed 11 different groups across sampling seasons (Bolt et al., 2020a(Bolt et al., , 2023bSchreier et al., 2021). ...
... These same groups have also been the subjects for other behavioral investigations at LSBRS (Bolt et al., 2020a(Bolt et al., , 2021a(Bolt et al., , 2023bSchreier et al., 2022a), and comprise an unusually dense population of at least 149 individual monkeys or 109.5 individuals/km 2 , which is one of the highest population densities recorded at any site worldwide for this species Schreier et al., 2023). While we could not consistently identify individual monkeys between sampling seasons due to changing group demography, we consistently observed 11 different groups across sampling seasons (Bolt et al., 2020a(Bolt et al., , 2023bSchreier et al., 2021). Howler monkey groups were habituated and did not react to observer presence . ...
... Both black and mantled howler monkeys at these sites did not alter social behavior or activity budget even when consuming higher-quality diets, suggesting that they may be limited in their ability to alter behavior (Estrada et al., 1999b;Horwich et al., 1993;Pavelka & Knopff, 2004 areas. Any human-caused alteration of primate behavior is a conservation concern and should be avoided in the interest of general animal well-being (Bolt et al., 2020a). With social behavior being a key adaptation for group-living mammals (Kappeler et al., 2013), its incorporation into edge effects studies is important. ...
Article
Full-text available
Social behavior is a key adaptation for group‐living primates. It is important to assess changes to social behavior in human‐impacted landscape zones to better understand the impact of anthropogenic disturbance on primate species. We investigated social behavior rate and type in three species of platyrrhines across 100 m anthropogenic edge and interior zones of a fragmented forest in Costa Rica, La Suerte Biological Research Station (LSBRS). Following results from other sites, we predicted that spider monkeys ( Ateles geoffroyi ), capuchin monkeys ( Cebus imitator ) and howler monkeys ( Alouatta palliata ) would show lower rates and fewer types of social behavior in forest edge compared to interior. We collected 1341 h of instantaneous focal data from 2017 to 2023 across the three monkey species. We found mixed support for our predictions, with spider and capuchin monkeys modifying some but not all aspects of social behavior across forest zones at LSBRS. Spider monkeys had lower rates of social behavior and capuchin monkeys performed different types of social behaviors in forest edge compared to interior at LSBRS. In contrast, howler monkeys did not modify social behavior. Two out of three platyrrhine species altered their social behavior when in anthropogenic edges, indicating behavioral adjustment when in human‐altered habitat areas at LSBRS.
... Home range sizes for this species vary widely, with research in Belize documenting home range sizes between 0.5 and 50 ha [29][30][31] . Howler monkeys emit loud, low-frequency calls, known as howls, which can be heard over long distances through the forest (> 1 km) [32][33][34] . Throughout the day, howler monkeys commonly engage in several bouts of howling, with each bout lasting on average around 10-15 min, and sometimes ranging up to an hour 33,35 . ...
Article
Full-text available
The combination of autonomous recording units (ARUs) and machine learning enables scalable biodiversity monitoring. These data are often analysed using occupancy models, yet methods for integrating machine learning outputs with these models are rarely compared. Using the Yucatán black howler monkey as a case study, we evaluated four approaches for integrating ARU data and machine learning outputs into occupancy models: (i) standard occupancy models with verified data, and false-positive occupancy models using (ii) presence-absence data, (iii) counts of detections, and (iv) continuous classifier scores. We assessed estimator accuracy and the effects of decision threshold, temporal subsampling, and verification strategies. We found that classifier-guided listening with a standard occupancy model provided an accurate estimate with minimal verification effort. The false-positive models yielded similarly accurate estimates under specific conditions, but were sensitive to subjective choices including decision threshold. The inability to determine stable parameter choices a priori, coupled with the increased computational complexity of several models (i.e. the detection-count and continuous-score models), limits the practical application of false-positive models. In the case of a high-performance classifier and a readily detectable species, classifier-guided listening paired with a standard occupancy model provides a practical and efficient approach for accurately estimating occupancy.
... For similar reasons, Stoner (1994) also conducted her population census throughout La Selva and then sampled seven of the groups; she calculated population density by multiplying the number of groups generated from the census by the mean group size of the groups she sampled, divided by the total size of the reserve. As part of a larger study examining monkey behavioral ecology across forest fragments and larger, continuous forests (Bolt et al. 2018(Bolt et al. , 2019(Bolt et al. , 2020a(Bolt et al. , 2020b(Bolt et al. , 2021a(Bolt et al. , 2021bBolt and Schreier 2022;Schreier et al. 2021Schreier et al. , 2022aSchreier et al. , 2022b, we collected howler monkey group demographic data between 6:00 and 17:00. Researchers located howler groups in the morning based on their vocalizations, known location the previous day, or opportunistically, and stayed with the group for as long as possible. ...
Article
Full-text available
In the face of widespread habitat destruction and forest fragmentation, it is critical to understand primate demography to assess population viability across populations inhabiting continuous forests and fragmented landscapes. While mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata) have traditionally been considered resilient to habitat destruction, their populations are declining and are now rated "Vulnerable" on the IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. In this study, we assessed the demography of the mantled howler monkey population at La Selva Research Station, a protected forest in northeastern Costa Rica-providing the first systematic study of population structure in 30 years. In 2022, we conducted a howler census to ascertain the number of groups at La Selva and recorded group size and composition for a subset of the groups. We then compared population density, group size and composition with a population in a nearby protected forest (La Suerte Biological Research Station, LSBRS) to help assess the viability of mantled howlers in a small forest fragment. The mantled howler population at La Selva has grown substantially since 1992 with, at the time of the survey, 25 groups and a population density of 23.4 individuals/km². Mean group size was 14.3 with a sex ratio of 1:2.2 and immature to adult ratio of 0.7-in line with mantled howler monkey populations throughout the species' range. The population density at La Selva was, however, almost five times lower than the forest fragment at LSBRS, although group size and composition did not differ across sites. Overall, our results highlight the importance of forest protection for mantled howler monkeys. Resumen: Ante la destrucción generalizada del hábitat y la fragmentación de los bosques, es fundamental comprender la demografía de los primates para evaluar la viabilidad de la población en las poblaciones que habitan bosques y continuos y paisajes fragmentados. Si bien los monos aulladores (Alouatta palliata) se han considerado tradicionalmente resistentes a la destrucción del hábitat, sus poblaciones están disminuyendo actualmente y ahora están clasificados como "vulnerables" en la Lista Roja de la UICN. En este estudio, evaluamos la demografía de la población de monos aulladores en la Estación de Investigación La Selva, un bosque protegido en el noreste de Costa Rica, proporcionando el primer estudio sistemático de la estructura de la población en 30 años. En 2022, realizamos un censo de aulladores para determinar la cantidad de grupos en La Selva y registramos el tamaño y la composición de los grupos para un subconjunto de los grupos. Luego comparamos la densidad de población, el tamaño de los grupos y la composición con un fragmento de bosque protegido cercano (Estación de Investigación Biológica La Suerte, LSBRS) para ayudar a evaluar la viabilidad de los aulladores en un pequeño fragmento de bosque. La población de aulladores en La Selva ha crecido sustancialmente desde 1992, con 25 grupos y una densidad de población de 23.4 individuos/km². El tamaño medio del grupo fue de 14.3 con una proporción de sexos de 1:2.2 y una pro-porción de inmaduros a adultos de 0.7, en línea con las poblaciones de monos aulladores en toda su área de distribución. La densidad de población en La Selva fue casi cinco veces menor que el fragmento de bosque en LSBRS, aunque el tamaño y la composición de los grupos no varió entre los sitios. Nuestros resultados destacan la importancia de la protección del bosque para los monos aulladores de manto. Palabras clave: densidad poblacional, fragmentación del bosque, Estación Científica La Selva, Estación Científica Biológica La Suerte, censo de primates, áreas protegidas Schreier et al. 36
... The long-calling patterns of howler monkeys may thus be ultimately related to the defense of food resources. This is further supported by evidence that the spatial location of groups also influences the long-distance vocalization patterns of howler monkeys, although such evidence is inconsistent (Altmann, 1959;Bernstein, 1964;Bolt et al., 2020;Chiarello, 1995;da Cunha & Byrne, 2006;da Cunha & Jalles-Filho, 2007;Whitehead, 1989). For instance, whereas in one study long-distance vocalizations were more frequent in border than in non-border areas (A. ...
Article
Objectives: Long-distance vocalizations are used by primates in a variety of contexts and may have different functions. The long-distance vocalizations of howler monkeys (Alouatta spp.) underlie the spatial regulation of neighboring groups and could be associated with the defense of food resources. Here, we test the hypothesis that the behavioral responses of mantled howler monkeys (A. palliata) to neighbor long-distance vocalizations are influenced by the potential for range defensibility while accounting for location within the home range and food availability. Methods: We studied two groups for 13 months and a total of 888 h at La Flor de Catemaco (Mexico). Group 1 had a 92-ha home range and Group 2 had a 24-ha home range. We recorded vocalizations (N = 178 calls) and movements (N = 74 movements) of focal groups following long-distance vocalizations produced by their neighbors. Results: Movement responses, but not vocal responses, were predicted by range defensibility, location, and food availability. As predicted, the group living in the smaller and more defendable range showed stronger movement responses than the group in the larger home range. These movement responses had a shorter latency and longer duration in more valuable spatial and temporal contexts (i.e., the core area and during periods of low food availability). Conclusions: These results suggest that the tradeoff between the costs and benefits of range defense varies according to the interactions between home range size and both the spatial (core areas) and temporal (food availability) abundance of resources. Thus, the responses of mantled howler monkeys to neighbor long-distance vocalizations could be related to home range defensibility.
... It is important to mention that by considering species that could perform a hole of bridge vector between sylvatic and urban transmission of YFV, Ae. albopictus has been identified as the most probably species, according to recent finds (Couto Lima, 2017;Damasceno-Caldeira et al., 2023. Howler monkeys, for example, one of the main YF hosts in South America (Bicca-Marques et al. 2020), circulate in these environments (Bolt et al., 2019(Bolt et al., , 2021. Therefore, this type of environment can be highly favorable to maintenance of YFV. ...
Article
The intense process of deforestation in tropical forests poses serious challenges for the survival of biodiversity, as well as for the human species itself. This scenario is supported by the increase in the incidence of epidemics of zoonotic origin observed over the last few decades. In the specific case of sylvatic yellow fever (YF), it has already been shown that an increase in the transmission risk of the causative agent (yellow fever virus - YFV) is associated with areas with a high degree of forest fragmentation, which can facilitate the spread of the virus. In this study we tested the hypothesis that areas with more fragmented landscapes and a higher edge density (ED) but a high degree of connectivity between forest patches favor YFV spread. To this end, we used YF epizootics in non-human primates (NHPs) in the state of São Paulo to build direct networks, and used a multi-selection approach to analyze which landscape features could facilitate YFV spread. Our results showed that municipalities with the potential to spread the virus exhibited a higher amount of forest edge. Additionally, the models with greater empirical support showed a strong association between forest edge density and the risk of occurrence of epizootic diseases, as well as the need for a minimum threshold of native vegetation cover to restrict their transmission. These findings corroborate our hypothesis that more fragmented landscapes with a higher degree of connectivity favor the spread of YFV, while landscapes with fewer connections tend to act as dead zones for the circulation of the virus.
... corresponding to howling near the edges of habitats)? Edge habitats may be used by vocalising individuals for behaviours such as territory defence (Taylor et al. 2008;Bolt et al. 2019). This additional landscape information provided by land cover maps thus further expands the possibilities of vocal behavioural research that can be carried out using land cover classification and acoustic localisation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Context The ecology of cryptic animals is difficult to study without invasive tagging approaches or labour-intensive field surveys. Acoustic localisation provides an effective way to locate vocalising animals using acoustic recorders. Combining this with land cover classification gives new insight into wild animal behaviour using non-invasive tools. Aims This study aims to demonstrate how acoustic localisation – combined with high-resolution land cover classification – permits the study of the ecology of vocalising animals in the wild. We illustrate this technique by investigating the effect of land cover and distances to anthropogenic features on coyote and wolf vocal behaviour. Methods We collected recordings over 13 days in Wisconsin, USA, and triangulated vocalising animals’ locations using acoustic localisation. We then mapped these locations onto land cover using a high-resolution land cover map we produced for the area. Key results Neither coyotes nor wolves vocalised more in one habitat type over another. Coyotes vocalised significantly closer to all human features than expected by chance, whereas wolves vocalised significantly further away. When vocalising closer to human features, coyotes selected forests but wolves showed no habitat preference. Conclusions This novel combination of two sophisticated, autonomous sensing-driven tools permits us to examine animal land use and behavioural ecology using passive sensors, with the aim of drawing ecologically important conclusions. Implications We envisage that this method can be used at larger scales to aid monitoring of vocally active animals across landscapes. Firstly, it permits us to characterise habitat use while vocalising, which is an essential behaviour for many species. Furthermore, if combined with additional knowledge of how a species’ habitat selection while vocalising relates to its general habitat use, this method could permit the derivation of future conclusions on prevailing landscape use. In summary, this study demonstrates the potential of integrating acoustic localisation with land cover classification in ecological research.
Article
Maderas Rainforest Conservancy (MRC) is a conservation non-profit organization that protects forests, offers primate field schools, and leads community outreach initiatives at sites in Central America, including La Suerte Biological Research Station (LSBRS) in Costa Rica. In 2018, MRC started Aula Verde, a conservation education initiative for elementary school students aged 5-12 at LSBRS. Groups of children from elementary schools and communities located within 50 km have visited LSBRS from 2018-2023 to participate in the 1.5-2 hour Aula Verde workshop, which consists of (1) an interactive lecture on conservation, (2) a guided forest walk, and (3) a snack, gift bag, and tree seedling for each participant. Workshops are funded by international donations to MRC and cost US $8-16 per student. Aula Verde has been successful, with multiple schools and communities making repeat visits, students’ families beginning to recycle their garbage and joining a WhatsApp group about conservation education that MRC staff initiated and lead, and teachers reporting improved learning outcomes for students who participate in the workshops. The success of Aula Verde is largely due to the ecological expertise and strong community ties of LSBRS staff. While the COVID-19 pandemic decreased Aula Verde offerings from 2020-2023, we expect workshops to resume at full capacity in 2024. In the future, MRC hopes to expand Aula Verde’s outreach to all children aged 5-12 living within 50 km of LSBRS.
Article
Fragmented forests contain natural edges, including riparian zones, and anthropogenic edges. Edges generally have lower plant density and fewer large trees than forest interior. Riparian edges, however, contain gap-specialist trees yielding leaves with high protein content, providing primates with important resources. We examined mantled howler monkeys’ behavioral responses to riparian and anthropogenic edges at La Suerte Biological Research Station (LSBRS), Costa Rica. We predicted the monkeys would spend more time resting and feeding and less time traveling, and be less spatially cohesive, in both anthropogenic and riparian edges compared to forest interior due to lower resource abundance in edges, and in anthropogenic compared to riparian edge due to higher leaf quality in riparian zones. From 2017 to 2020, we collected data across forest zones on activity and spatial cohesion patterns via focal sampling, recording data every 2 min. Howler monkeys were significantly more likely to rest and significantly less likely to travel in both anthropogenic and riparian edges compared to forest interior; however, there were no differences between these edge types. There were significantly more monkeys within a 5-m radius of focal subjects in both anthropogenic and riparian edges compared to forest interior, but no differences between these edge types. While prior research found no differences across zones when only anthropogenic edge and forest interior were compared, results of this study demonstrate that howler monkeys at LSBRS modify their activity patterns in anthropogenic and riparian edge zones compared to forest interior, highlighting the importance of focusing on both natural and anthropogenic edge zones to fully understand primates’ behavioral responses in fragmented landscapes.
Preprint
Full-text available
Context The intense process of deforestation in tropical forests poses serious challenges for the survival of biodiversity, as well as for the human species itself. This scenario is supported by the increase in the incidence of epidemics of zoonotic origin observed over the last few decades. In the specific case of sylvatic yellow fever (YF), it has already been shown that an increase in the transmission risk of the causative agent (yellow fever virus - YFV) is associated with areas with a high degree of forest fragmentation, which can facilitate the spread of the virus. Methods In this study we tested the hypothesis that areas with more fragmented landscapes and a higher edge density (ED) but a high degree of connectivity between forest patches favor YFV spread. To this end, we used YF epizootics in non-human primates (NHPs) in the state of São Paulo to build direct networks, and used a multi-selection approach to analyze which landscape features could facilitate YFV spread. Results Our results showed that municipalities with the potential to spread the virus exhibited a higher amount of forest edge. Additionally, the models with greater empirical support showed a strong association between forest edge density and the risk of occurrence of epizootic diseases, as well as the need for a minimum threshold of native vegetation cover to restrict their transmission. Conclusions These findings corroborate our hypothesis that more fragmented landscapes with a higher degree of connectivity favor the spread of YFV, while landscapes with fewer connections tend to act as dead zones for the circulation of the virus.
Article
Full-text available
The function of long calling is a subject of interest across animal behaviour study, particularly within primatology. Many primate species have male‐specific long‐distance calls, including platyrrhines like the folivorous howler monkey (Alouatta spp.). Howler monkeys may howl to defend resources such as feeding trees or areas of rich vegetation from other monkey groups. This study tests the ecological resource defence hypothesis for howling behaviour in the mantled howler monkey (Alouatta palliata) and investigates how anthropogenic forest fragmentation may influence howling behaviour. More specifically, this study examines how howling bout rate, duration, precursors and tree species richness, DBH, and canopy cover vary in 100 m anthropogenic edge and interior forest zones at La Suerte Biological Research Station (LSBRS), a fragmented tropical rainforest in Costa Rica. Results show that tree species richness and canopy cover are higher in forest interior at this site, suggesting that monkeys should howl at greater rates in the interior to defend access to these higher‐quality vegetation resources. Overall, our results supported the ecological resource defence hypothesis. The main howl precursor was howling from neighbouring groups. Although howling rate did not differ between forest zones, howling bouts from forest interior were longer, had a greater number of howls per bout and were preceded by different precursors than howls from anthropogenic edge zones, including more howls from neighbouring groups. Our findings provide some of the first evidence for behavioural edge effects in primate vocal communication behaviour.
Article
Full-text available
Clearing of dry forests globally creates edges between remnant forest and open anthropogenic habitats. We used flight intercept traps to evaluate how forest beetle communities are influenced by distance from such edges, together with vertical height, spatial location, and local vegetation structure, in an urbanising region (Brisbane, Australia). Species composition (but not total abundance or richness) differed greatly between ground and canopy. Species composition also varied strongly among sites at both ground and canopy levels, but almost all other significant effects occurred only at ground level, where: species richness declined from edge to interior; composition differed between positions near edges (<10 m) and interiors (> 50 m); high local canopy cover was associated with greater total abundance and richness and differing composition; and greater distances to the city centre were associated with increased total abundances and altered composition. Analyses of individual indicator species associated with this variation enabled further biological interpretations. A global literature synthesis showed that most spatially well-replicated studies of edge effects on ground-level beetles within forest fragments have likewise found that positions within tens of metres from edges with open anthropogenic habitats had increased species richness and different compositions from forest interior sites, with fewer effects on abundance. Accordingly, negative edge effects will not prevent relatively small compact fragments (if >10–20 ha) from supporting forest-like beetle communities, although indirect consequences of habitat degradation remain a threat. Retention of multiple spatially scattered forest areas will also be important in conserving forest-dependent beetles, given high levels of between-site diversity.
Article
Full-text available
Beyond the unique sound of howler monkey vocalizations, their vigorous loud calling displays are perplexing given the otherwise sedentary lifestyle of these primates. Here we provide potential explanations for this energetic investment by reviewing all available functional studies conducted to date. We highlight the variation among and even within species when we explore whether male loud calls are used in group cohesion, predator avoidance, attraction of females, or competition with other males or other groups over resources. In the competition scenario, we examine strategies of avoidance versus direct competition and whether contests are focused on defense of space, food, mates, or infants. We suggest that much of the debate surrounding the function of loud calls stems from methodological differences among researchers and from the varied levels of analyses used, although we also demonstrate that studies of form and function can be intertwined. We emphasize the need to examine different call types separately and discuss the role of howling in intragroup male relationships. Finally, we address the understudied role of female loud calling and the potential use of hybrid populations to examine the evolution of species-typical loud calls. We conclude with some practical hints for designing field tests to uncover functional significance.
Book
Full-text available
Introduction to Forestry and Natural Resources presents a broad overview of the profession of forestry. The book details several key fields within forestry, including forest health, economics, policy, utilization, and forestry careers. Chapters deal specifically with forest products and harvesting, recreation, wildlife habitats, tree anatomy and physiology, and ethics. These topics are ideal for undergraduate introductory courses and include numerous examples (mainly graphical) and questions for students to ponder. Unlike other introductory forestry texts, which focus largely on forest ecology rather than practical forestry concepts, Introduction to Forestry and Natural Resources encompasses economic, ecological, and social aspects providing a uniquely balanced text. The wide range of experience of the contributing authors equips them especially well to identify missing content from other texts in the area and address topics currently covered in corresponding college courses.
Article
When a forest is fragmented, this increases the amount of forest edge relative to the interior. Edge effects can lead to loss of animal and plant species and decreased plant biomass near forest edges. We examined the influence of an anthropogenic forest edge comprising cattle pasture, coconut plantations, and human settlement on the mantled howler (Alouatta palliata), white-faced capuchin (Cebus capucinus), Central American spider monkey (Ateles geoffroyi), and plant populations at La Suerte Biological Research Station (LSBRS), Costa Rica. We predicted that there would be lower monkey encounter rate, mean tree species richness, and diameter at breast height (DBH) in forest edge versus interior, and that monkeys would show species-specific responses to edge based on diet, body size, and canopy height preferences. Specifically, we predicted that howler monkeys would show positive or neutral edge effects due to their flexible folivorous diet, large body size, and preference for high canopy, capuchins would show positive edge effects due to their diverse diet, small body size, and preference for low to middle canopy, and spider monkeys would show negative edge effects due their reliance on ripe fruit, large body size, and preference for high upper canopy. We conducted population and vegetation surveys along edge and interior transects at LSBRS. Contrary to predictions, total monkey encounter rate did not vary between the forest edge and forest interior. Furthermore, all three species showed neutral edge effects with no significant differences in encounter rate between forest edge and interior. Interior transects had significantly higher mean tree species richness than edge transects, and interior trees had greater DBH than edge trees, although this difference was not significant. These results suggest that forest edges negatively impact plant populations at La Suerte but that the monkeys are able to withstand these differences in vegetation. http://rdcu.be/GqFs
Chapter
The Maderas Rainforest Conservancy (MRC) was founded in 2008 and manages two forested properties: (i) the La Suerte Biological Field Station, which is located in an Atlantic lowland rain forest in northeastern Costa Rica (4000 mm of annual rainfall) and (ii) the Ometepe Biological Research Station, which is part of a tropical dry forest found on the southeastern coast of Ometepe Island in Nicaragua (1400 mm of annual rainfall). Ometepe Island consists of two forested volcanoes, Volcán Concepción and Volcán Maderas, from which MRC gets its name. Ometepe Island itself is very important ecologically, and on June 5, 2010, it was declared a biosphere status by the United Nations Educational, Scientific, and Cultural Organization (UNESCO).