ArticlePDF Available

Motivational Effects of Enhanced Expectancies for Motor Learning in Individuals With High and Low Self-Efficacy

SAGE Publications Inc
Perceptual and Motor Skills
Authors:
  • Islamic Azad University of Islamshahr

Abstract and Figures

Previous research has shown that enhanced expectancies can foster a person’s motivational state and facilitate motor learning. However, the effects of enhanced expectancies on motivational state and subsequent motor learning in individuals with varied motivational states (e.g., self-efficacy; SE) are not well understood. This study examined the effects of enhanced expectancies on motivational state and motor learning in individuals with high and low SE by manipulating the type of knowledge of results (KR). We selected 60 participants from 251 male undergraduate students, based on their SE level, and assigned them to four groups: (a) Good Performance KR and High SE, (b) Poor Performance KR and High SE, (c) Good Performance KR and Low SE, and (d) Poor Performance KR and Low SE. We asked participants to throw beanbags at a target with their nondominant hand during an acquisition phase (10 blocks of six trials each) and during subsequent retention and transfer tests. During acquisition, the Good KR groups received KR from their three most accurate trials in each training block, whereas the Poor KR groups received KR from their three poorest attempts in each block. We measured accuracy scores and changes in SE as dependent variables. Results showed that KR from good trials, relative to KR from poor trials, led to better accuracy scores during acquisition and retention and transfer tests, independent of participants’ initial SE levels. Moreover, KR on good trials enhanced SE in the Good KR and Low SE group and kept SE at a high level in the Good KR and High SE group during acquisition and on the retention and transfer tests. These results provide support for the OPTIMAL (optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning) theory of motor learning, and we discuss results in terms of the potential effects of positive feedback on motor performance in professional athletes.
This content is subject to copyright.
Motivational Effects of Enhanced Expectancies for Motor Learning in
Individuals with High and Low Self-Efficacy
Saeed Ghorbani1* & Andreas Bund2
1. Department of Physical Education and Sport Science, Aliabad Katoul Branch, Islamic Azad
University, Aliabad Katoul, Iran
2. Institute of Applied Educational Sciences, University of Luxembourg, Esch-s.-Alzette L-
4365, Luxembourg, Luxembourg


How to cite: Ghorbani, S., & Bund, A. Motivational Effects of Enhanced Expectancies for

Motor Learning in Individuals with High and Low Self-Efficacy. Perceptual & Motor Skills,

https://doi.org/10.1177/0031512519892390.








Abstract

Previous research has shown that enhanced expectancies can foster a persons motivational

state and facilitate motor learning. However, the effects of enhanced expectancies on

motivational state and subsequent motor learning in individuals with varied motivational states

(e.g., self-efficacy; SE) are not well understood. The present study examined the effects of

enhanced expectancies on motivational state and motor learning in individuals with high and

low SE by manipulating the type of knowledge of results (KR). We selected 60 participants

from among 251 male undergraduate students, based on their SE level, and assigned them to

four groups: (a) Good Performance KR & High-SE, (b) Poor Performance KR & High-SE, (c)

Good Performance KR & Low-SE, and (d) Poor Performance KR & Low-SE. We asked

participants to throw beanbags at a target with their non-dominant hand during an acquisition

phase (10 blocks of six trials each) and during subsequent retention and transfer tests. During

acquisition, the Good-KR groups received KR from their three most accurate trials in each

training block whereas the Poor-KR groups received KR from their three poorest attempts in

each block. We measured accuracy scores and changes in SE as dependent variables. Results

showed that KR from good trials, relative to KR from poor trials, led to better accuracy scores

during acquisition, retention and transfer tests, independent of participants initial SE levels.

Moreover, KR on good trials enhanced SE in the Good-KR & Low-SE group and kept SE at a

high level in the Good-KR & High-SE group during acquisition and on the retention and

transfer tests. These results provide support for the OPTIMAL theory of motor learning, and

we discuss results in terms of the potential effects of positive feedback on motor performance

in professional athletes.

Keywords: Enhanced expectancies, self-efficacy, the OPTIMAL theory


Introduction

In recent years, research has shown that providing learners with positive performance

feedback can enhance learners’ expectancies and subsequently facilitate motor performance

and learning (Ghorbani, 2019; Gonçalves, Cardozo, Valentini, & Chiviacowsky, 2018; Lessa,

Tani, Chiviacowsky, 2018; Saemi, Wulf, Varzaneh, & Zarghami, 2011; Saemi, Porter, Ghotbi-

Varzaneh, Zarghami, & Maleki, 2012). Enhancing learners performance expectancies is

considered a motivational factor in the OPTIMAL (optimizing performance through intrinsic

motivation and attention for learning) theory of motor learning (Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2016),

whereby learners’ positive expectancies are operationalized by the self-efficacy (SE) construct.

SE is generally defined as the belief in one's ability to succeed in specific situations or to

execute a task (Bandura, 1977).

A common research method for enhancing learners motivational states is to provide

them knowledge of results (KR) from their more accurate (i.e., good) trials versus their less

accurate trials (i.e., poor). KR from good trials has been shown to lead to higher SE beliefs and,

thereafter, to more effective learning (Badami, VaezMousavi, Wulf, & Namazizadeh, 2011,

2012; Saemi et al., 2011, 2012). For example, in a golf-putting task, Badami et al., (2011, 2012)

found that KR from good trials led to significantly better motor learning and higher levels of

perceived competence and self-confidence than did KR from poor trials. In addition, Saemi et

al., (2011, 2012) demonstrated that KR from good trials led to higher SE and improved motor

learning. Chiviacowsky and Wulf (2007) found no significant differences in throwing beanbags

at a target with the non-dominant hand during an acquisition phase, but, on delayed retention,

KR from good trials resulted in significantly better performance than KR from poor trials.

Chiviacowsky, Wally, and Borges (2009) replicated these results with older adults.

Accordingly, Wulf and Lewthwaite (2016) proposed that motivational factors mediate

observed learning benefits following positive feedback (e.g., KR from good trials).

While previous research has identified this mediating role of enhanced motivational

state associated with in the benefits of positive performance KR on motor learning (Saemi et

al., 2011, 2012; Wulf, Chiviacoswsky, Cardozo, 2014), these prior studies did not address how

learners whose sense of self-efficacy varied might differentially respond to good or poor

performance KR. Previous studies (Badami et al., 2011, 2012; Saemi et al., 2011, 2012)

generally ignored the role of a priori differences in motivational state among individual

participants. But, if motivational state mediates the relationship between positive performance

feedback and motor learning, then a priori individual differences in motivational state must be

considered. Therefore, present study sought to examine the good and poor performance KR

effects on motivational state and motor learning in individuals who began motor learning with

high versus low SE levels. We hypothesized that individuals with high and low SE would differ

in their retention and transfer performance as a result of receiving of good or poor performance

KR. We used a 2 × 2 factorial design to compare individuals who reported either a high or a

low level of SE and who then randomly assigned them to good and poor performance KR,

creating four experimental groups: (a) Good KR & High-SE, (b) Poor KR & High-SE, (c) Good

KR & Low-SE, and (d) Poor KR & Low-SE. We used the motor learning task and procedure

in studies by Chiviacowsky and Wulf (2007) and Chiviacowsky et al., (2009).

Method

Participants

This research protocol was developed and performed in accordance with the

Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the university institutional review board.

Furthermore, all participants gave written informed consents.

First, we tested a large sample (N = 251) of male undergraduate students (bachelor

students of various disciplines who took part in first course of physical education) to determine

their self-reported SE levels. All participants were right-handed and had no prior experience

with the motor task. Before trying the motor task, participants rated themselves with respect to

their expected performance at it throwing a beanbag with the non-dominant arm to hit a 100-

point target on one of six trials. They used a rating scale ranging from 10 (not confident) to 100

(absolutely confident). We assigned students with low SE self-rating scores (between 10 and

20) to the Low-SE group and students with SE scores (between 90 and 100) to the High-SE

group, yielding a final participant sample of 60 male student (M age = 21.35, SD = 1.86 years),

30 in each SE group. We then randomly allocated these participants to separate groups that

would receive good performance KR and poor performance KR, creating four experimental

groups: (a) Good KR & High-SE (n = 15), (b) Poor KR & High-SE (n = 15), (c) Good KR &

Low-SE (n = 15), and (d) Poor KR & Low-SE (n = 15).

Motor Task

The motor task was similar to the task used by Chiviacowsky and Wulf (2007) and

Chiviacowsky et al., (2009). The participants were asked to throw beanbags with the non-

dominant arm at a target on the ground. The distance between the participant and the center of

the target was three meters. Concentric circles around the target with radiuses of 10, 20, 30, 40,

50, 60, 70, 80, 90, and 100 cm served to determine the accuracy of the throws. If the beanbag

landed in the center of the target, then the score was 100. If it landed in one of the other circles,

then the score was 90, 80, 70, 60, 50, 40, 30, 20, or 10 points, respectively. Finally, if it landed

outside the target, then a score of 0 was recorded (Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2007).

Procedure

The participants were tested individually on two consecutive days. Prior to data

collection, the participants were given general information about the experimental procedure

and asked to complete a questionnaire that asked for information such as age, laterality, and

previous experiences with the motor task. Finally, the participants were given brief instructions

about the beanbag-throwing task, which consisted of holding the beanbag with the non-

dominant hand and throwing it at the target. During the acquisition phase, participants

performed 10 blocks of six trials each, and one day later, they completed the retention and

transfer test, consisting of 10 trials each without KR. In the transfer test, the distance to the

target was changed from 3 to 4 meters. The participants were given six seconds to execute each

throw. The participants were allowed to look at the target before each block, but during the

acquisition, retention, and transfer phases, they were prevented from viewing the outcomes by

wearing opaque swimming goggles. At the end of each acquisition block, participants in the

Good-KR & High-SE and Good-KR & Low-SE groups received KR from their three most

accurate trials in that block, whereas those in the Poor-KR & High-SE and Poor-KR & Low-

SE groups received KR from their three poorest attempts in that block. All participants had

been told that, at the end of each block, they would be given KR about three throws, but they

had not been told whether the KR would refer to their best or worst attempts (Chiviacowsky &

Wulf, 2007). KRs from all three trials were written on a board and provided to the participant

for fifteen seconds. It included the number of attempts, the direction, and the score of the

landing relative to the center of the target. That is, if the beanbag landed in the upper part of

the target, then a plus sign was added to the throwing score (e.g., +50). Conversely, if the

beanbag landed in the lower part of the target, then a minus sign was presented before the

throwing score (e.g., -50). Therefore, KR included information about the throw’s distance from

the center of the target, as well as information about the direction of the error. We measured

time with a digital chronometer. During the experimental procedure, prior to each acquisition

block and before the retention and transfer tests, all participants completed the same SE scale

that had been previously used to select the participants with high and low SE levels.


Statistical Analysis

We averaged accuracy scores in the first acquisition block across all six trials and

analyzed as baseline performance in a 2 (SE: High vs. Low) × 2 (KR: Good vs. Poor) analysis

of variance (ANOVA). We further averaged accuracy scores in the acquisition phase across a

10 blocks of six trials each in a 2 (SE: High vs. Low) × 2 (KR: Good vs. Poor) × 10 (Block)

repeated measures ANCOVA including first Block scores as a covariate. Accuracy scores on

the single block retention and transfer tests were averaged across all 10 trials and analyzed in

a 2 (SE: High vs. Low) × 2 (KR: Good vs. Poor) ANCOVA including first Block scores as a

covariate. Initial (baseline) SE scores and SE scores on the retention and transfer tests were

analyzed in a 2 (SE: High vs. Low) × 2 (KR: Good vs. Poor) ANOVA. Finally, SE scores in

the acquisition phase were analyzed in a 2 (SE: High vs. Low) × 2 (KR: Good vs. Poor) × 10

(Block) repeated measures ANOVA. When there were significant group differences, we

calculated partial eta squared (η2) as the effect size. Moreover, we used simple linear regression

analyses to determine possible associations between the SE and accuracy scores during

acquisition and on the retention and transfer tests. First, the SE scores at baseline was used as

predictors of performances in the first block, and the SE scores obtained before the retention

and transfer tests were used as predictors of performance on the retention and transfer test,

respectively. Second, performance scores from the first block were used as predictors of SE in

second block, and performance scores from the last block were used as predictors of SE taken

before the retention test. Shapiro-Wilk test was used to examine normality of accuracy scores

at baseline. For all analyses, we set the statistical significance level a p < 0.05.




Results

Accuracy Scores

The results of Shapiro-Wilk test showed that the accuracy scores at baseline were

normally distributed (p = .641). The analysis of the first block showed no significant main

effects for KR or SE and there was no significant KR × SE interaction at baseline.

During the acquisition phase, there was a main effect for KR, F (1, 55) = 51.99, p

.001, η2 =.48. Moreover, there was a significant Block × KR × SE interaction, F (8, 440) =

2.152, p < .05, η2 =.03. However, the main effects of Block as well as SE were not significant.

As shown in Figure 1, the Good-KR groups achieved higher accuracy scores than the Poor-KR

groups. No other interactions were significant.

The data from the retention test indicated a main effect of KR, F (1, 55) = 20.03, p

.001, η2 = .26, with the Good-KR groups showing better throwing accuracy than the Poor-KR

groups. There was no significant main effect of SE and no significant interaction. On the

transfer test, there were significant main effects for KR and SE, F (1, 55) = 27.29, p ≤ .001, η2

= .33, and F (1, 55) = 9.52, p ≤ .01, η2 = .14, respectively. There was no significant KR × SE

interaction. With regard to KR, the Good-KR groups had higher accuracy scores compared

with the Poor-KR groups. The main effect of SE occurred because of the higher accuracy scores

of the performance of the Good-KR & Low-SE group (see Figure 1).


Figure 1. Accuracy scores across the acquisition phase and on the retention and transfer tests for all groups. GH:

KR-Good&High-SE, PH: KR-Poor&High-SE, GL: KR-Good&Low-SE, PL: KR-Poor&Low-SE groups. AQ:

Acquisition phase, RET: Retention test, TRA: Transfer test.

Self-Efficacy Scores

An analysis of the first block showed no significant main effect for KR. However, the

main effect for SE was statistically significant, F (1, 56) = 3705.68, p ≤ .001, η2 = .98. The KR

× SE interaction was not significant. Confirming the pre-study group assignment, participants

in the High-SE conditions reported much higher baseline SE scores than those in the Low-SE

conditions.

During the acquisition phase, there were significant main effects for Block, F (9, 504)

= 16.81, p ≤ .001, η2 =.23, for KR, F (1, 56) = 103.28, p ≤ .001, η2 =.64, and for SE, F (1, 56)

= 124.42, p ≤ .001, η2 =.69. Moreover, all of the interactions including the Block × KR, Block

× SE, KR × SE, and Block × KR × SE interactions were significant (each p .01). Here,

participants in the Good-KR groups had higher average SE scores than those in other groups.

Moreover, participants in the Good-KR & Low-SE group showed SE increases and those in

the Good-KR & High-SE group showed no SE changes. In addition, participants in the Poor-

50
60
70
80
90
100
AQ1 AQ2 AQ3 AQ4 AQ5 AQ6 AQ7 AQ8 AQ9 AQ10 RET TRA
Accuracy Scores
GH PH GL PL

KR & High-SE group showed SE decreases and those in the Poor-KR & Low-SE group showed

slight SE increases. Overall, participants reported increases in their SE scores during the

acquisition phase following exposure to the Good-KR conditions and decreases in SE scores

following exposure to the Poor-KR conditions during the acquisition phase.

Finally, analyses of the results from retention and transfer tests demonstrated significant

main effects for KR, F (1, 56) = 17.87, p ≤ .001, η2 = .24, and F (1, 56) = 31.32, p ≤ .001, η2 =

.35, respectively, and for SE, F (1, 56) = 6.02, p = .017, η2 = .09, and F (1, 56) = 8.73, p = .005,

η2 = .13, respectively. No significant interactions were observed. On both tests, participants in

the Good-KR groups reported higher SE levels than those in the Poor-KR groups. Moreover,

participants in the High-SE groups reported higher SE scores at retention and transfer than

those in the Low-SE groups; for example, the Good-KR & High-SE group had higher SE scores

than the Good-KR & Low-SE group, and similarly, the Poor-KR & High-SE group had higher

SE scores than the Poor-KR & Low-SE group.


Figure 2. SE scores across the acquisition phase and on the retention and transfer tests for all groups. HG: High-

SE&KR-Good, HP: High-SE&KR-Poor, LG: Low-SE&KR-Good, LP: Low-SE&KR-Poor groups. AQ:

Acquisition phase, RET: Retention test, TRA: Transfer test.

10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
AQ1 AQ2 AQ3 AQ4 AQ5 AQ6 AQ7 AQ8 AQ9 AQ10 RET TRA
Self-Efficacy Scores
GH PH GL PL

Regression Analyses

SE scores at baseline did not significantly predict performances in the first block, but

performance in first block significantly predicted SE before the second block, F (1, 58) = 8.16,

p = .006, Adjusted R2 = .108, β = .351. Performance in the last block also significantly predicted

SE before the retention test, F (1, 58) = 4.60, p = .036, Adjusted R2 = .058, β = .271. SE scores

before the retention test significantly predicted performance on the retention test, F (1, 58) =

11.44, p ≤ .001, Adjusted R2 = .15, β = .406, and, SE scores before the transfer test significantly

predicted performance on the transfer test, F (1, 58) = 8.22, p = .006, Adjusted R2 = .109, β =

.352.

Discussion

Previous research has clearly demonstrated that KR from well-performed (good) trials

versus KR from poorly performed (poor) trials enhances motivational state and motor learning

in novice performers (Badami et al., 2011, 2012; Saemi et al., 2011, 2012). However, no

research has yet investigated what role the learner`s initial motivational state plays in

responsiveness to KR type. The present study was designed to investigate this issue. Our results

replicated earlier findings that, during acquisition, KR from good trials resulted in significantly

better performance accuracy than KR from poor trials. Similarly, KR from good trials, relative

to KR from poor trials, led to significantly better accuracy on the retention test. Participants´

initial SE levels had no mediating influence on this beneficial effect of positive feedback on

motor performance and learning on acquisition and the retention test. However, on the transfer

test, the Good-KR & Low-SE group had higher accuracy scores than the other three groups.

These results are in line with previous studies in which enhanced expectancies benefitted motor

performance and motor learning (Ávila, Chiviacowsky, Wulf, & Lewthwaite, 2012; Badami et

al., 2012; Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2007; Chiviacowsky et al., 2009; Ghorbani, 2019; Gonçalves


et al., 2018; Grand, Daou, Lohse, & Miller, 2017; Lessa et al., 2018; Montes, Wulf, & Navalta,

2018; Ong, & Hodges, 2018; Pascua, Wulf, & Lewthwaite, 2015; Saemi et al., 2011, 2012;

Stoate, Wulf, Lewthwaite, 2012; Wulf et al., 2014), but they extend these findings to suggest

a particular benefit for individuals with low self-efficacy ratings.

In addition, our results showed that during acquisition, KR from good trials did not

(further) increase SE of participants in Good-KR & High-SE group (see Figure 2), as these

participants simply maintained their initially high level of SE during acquisition. On the other

hand, KR from good trials significantly enhanced Se for participants in the Good-KR & Low-

SE group during acquisition. During the second half of the acquisition phase (i.e., Blocks 6-

10), these low SE learners showed a similar SE level to learners in the Good-KR & High-SE

group (see Figure 2). Both of these Good-KR groups reported higher SE on the retention and

transfer tests than the Poor-KR groups. These results are in line with previous research showing

good-KR benefits (Badami et al., 2011, 2012; Saemi et al., 2011, 2012) and provide support

for the OPTIMAL theory of motor learning (Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2016), positing that KR from

good trials leads to increases in motivational state that might partially explain better motor

learning. Moreover, our results revealed that, for learners who reported high baseline SE

(presumably indicating high motivation), KR from good trials was associated with continued

high self-reported SE. This finding has important implications for the role of positive feedback

on the improved motor performance of professional athletes with high levels of SE.

OPTIMAL theorists assume that enhanced expectancies facilitate motor learning by

making dopamine available for memory consolidation and neural pathway development, thus

contributing to efficient goal-action coupling by preparing the motor system for task execution

(Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2016). In this way, the enhanced expectations of our low Se participants

and the maintained high expectancies of our high SE participants following good performance

KR may have led to more effective retention and transfer performance.


Of note, we found that KR from poor trials was associated with decreased SE during

acquisition for participants in the Poor-KR & High-SE group. These findings are in line with

previous research (Saemi et al., 2012) showing that poor performance reduces SE for

individuals with high baseline SE, perhaps explaining the lower performance of this group on

the retention test. Surprisingly, KR from poor trials slightly increased SE during acquisition in

the Poor-KR & Low-SE group, possibly because the specific data in their poor performance

KR showed them to be performing slightly better than the severely poor performance they

expected, as indicated by the nature of our SE rating scale. At the beginning of the study, these

learners rated their SE as 10 or 20, while their “poorperformance KR indicated that poor

performance beanbag throws were close to the scores from good trials (e.g., 50, 60). This might

have inspired the learners in this group to increase their confidence ratings in comparison to

their very low SE scores at the beginning of the study. Interestingly, at the end of the acquisition

phase, there were no significant differences in the SE scores of the two Poor-KR groups,

reflecting the significant gains of the low SE participants, even when exposed to this Poor-KR

condition.

According to the regression analysis, there were reciprocal effects between

participants’ performance and SE. Performance in the first and last acquisition blocks predicted

SE scores in the second block and before the retention test, respectively; and SE scores just

before the retention and transfer tests predicted performance on these tests. These results are in

accordance with Wulf et al.’s (2014) results and provide further support for predictions from

the OPTIMAL theory of motor learning (Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2016), again showing that

enhanced expectancies mediated motor learning KR benefits.

Among this study’s limitations, we used participants SE as both an independent

variable (with which to select participants) and as a dependent variable (to measure the effects

of positive feedback on motivational state). However, because we aimed to study how learners


with variant SE responded motivationally to positive or negative KR, we believe the double

function of SE to be acceptable in this case. Nevertheless, future studies might operationalize

participants’ a priori motivational state not only by SE but also by using other cognitive-

motivational variables such as expectancies, goal-orientation and self-concept. Such an

approach would provide additional information about other learner traits. Second, we used only

accuracy scores to measure motor performance and learning, while future research might be

improved by other variables such as movement form or kinematic and kinetic analysis.

In conclusion, this study replicated prior findings that KR from good trials facilitated

motor learning and further showed this to be true for both individuals with high and low a priori

SE. We further showed that KR from good trials was associated with particularly enhanced

motivational states of individuals with low self-reported SE at the beginning of the study, while

individuals with high SE maintained these high levels from positive performance KR but

reported lower SE after exposure to poor performance KR. These results support the

OPTIMAL theory of motor learning, positing that enhanced expectancies can facilitate motor

learning (Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2016). Practical implications of these results are the particular

benefits of positive performance KR for novices with low self-expectancies and the beneficial

maintenance effects of high SE from positive performance KR for professional athletes. A

specific focus of future research might be to investigate the relationship between SE and

positive performance KR on the motor performance of professional athletes. Additionally,

future research might investigate the effects of various other types of feedback (e.g., absolute

vs. comparative feedback, KR vs. KP) on different types of motivational states (e.g.,

intrinsic/extrinsic motivation, goal orientation) in order to gain further insights into the

complex relationships between feedback and individual motivation characteristics on motor

performance and learning.



Acknowledgment

We thank all the students who participated in this study.

Conflict of interest

No conflict.

References

Ávila, L. T. G., Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2012). Positive social-

comparative feedback enhances motor learning in children. Psychology of Sport and Exercise,

13, 849-853.

Badami, R., VaezMousavi, M., Wulf, G., & Namazizadeh, M. (2011). Feedback after

good trials enhances intrinsic motivation. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 82, 360-

364.

Badami, R., VaezMousavi, M., Wulf, G., & Namazizadeh, M. (2012). Feedback about

more accurate versus less accurate trials: Differential effects on self-confidence and activation.

Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 83, 196-203.

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a Unifying Theory of Behavioral

Change. Psychological Review. 84 (2), 191-215.

Chiviacowsky, S., & Wulf, G. (2007). Feedback after good trials enhances learning.

Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78, 40-47.

Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., Wally, R., & Borges, T. (2009). KR after good trials

enhances learning in older adults. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 80, 663-668.


Ghorbani, S. (2019). Motivational effects of enhancing expectancies and autonomy for

motor learning: An examination of the OPTIMAL theory. The Journal of General Psychology,

146(1), 79-92.

Gonçalves, G. S., Cardozo, P. L., Valentini, N. C., Chiviacowsky, S. (2018). Enhancing

performance expectancies through positive comparative feedback facilitates the learning of

basketball free throw in children, Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 36, 174-177.

Grand, K. F., Daou, M., Lohse, K. R., & Miller, M. W. (2017). Investigating the

mechanisms underlying the effects of an incidental choice on motor learning. Journal of Motor

Learning and Development, 5(2), 207-226.

Lessa, H. T., Tani, G., Chiviacowsky, S. (2018). Benefits of enhanced expectancies

through temporal-comparative feedback for motor learning in older adults. International

Journal of Sport Psychology, 49, 521-530.

Montes, J., Wulf, G., & Navalta, J. W. (2018). Maximal aerobic capacity can be increased

by enhancing performers’ expectancies. The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness,

58(5), 744-749.

Pascua, L. A., Wulf, G., Lewthwaite, R. (2015). Additive benefits of external focus and

enhanced performance expectancy for motor learning. Journal of Sports Sciences, 33(1), 58-

66.

Saemi, E., Wulf, G., Ghotbi-Varzaneh, A., & Zarghami, M. (2011). Feedback after good

versus poor trials enhances motor learning in children. Revista Brasileira de Educaçao Fisica

e Esporte (Brazilian Journal of Physical Education and Sport), 25(4), 673-681.

Saemi, E., Porter, J. M., Ghotbi-Varzaneh, A., Zarghami, M., & Maleki, F. (2012).

Knowledge of results after relatively good trials enhances self-efficacy and motor learning.

Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13, 378-382.


Stoate, I., Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2012). Enhanced expectancies improve

movement efficiency in runners. Journal of Sports Sciences, 30, 815-823.

Wulf, G., Chiviacowsky, S., & Cardozo, P. (2014). Additive benefits of autonomy

support and enhanced expectancies for motor learning. Human Movement Science, 37, 12-20.

Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2016). Optimizing performance through intrinsic

motivation and attention for learning: the OPTIMAL theory of motor learning. Psychonomic

Bulletin & Review, 23, 1382-1414.

... In the OPTIMAL theory, autonomy support refers to situations in which a person is allowed to control or choose some aspects of performance conditions. Several studies have tested the effects of the autonomy support on optimizing the motor skills and found that it positively affects performance and learning in a variety of motor tasks and across a range of age groups (Abdoshahi et al. 2022;Chiviacowsky, 2014;Chiviacowsky & Wulf 2002Chiviacowsky et al. 2008Chiviacowsky et al. , 2009Ghorbani & Bund, 2020;Wulf et al. 2014Wulf et al. , 2017. Moreover, it has been suggested that autonomy support and learners' expectancies are operationalized by the self-efficacy construct. ...
... That is, giving individuals with ADHD a choice to control practice condition resulted in higher motor performance and learning in comparison to yoked group. The results of the present experiment are in accordance with those of previous studies on healthy individuals (Chiviacowsky, 2014;Chiviacowsky & Wulf 2002Chiviacowsky et al. 2008Chiviacowsky et al. , 2009Ghorbani & Bund, 2020;Wulf et al. 2014Wulf et al. , 2017 indicating that autonomy support was clearly beneficial for motor learning in individuals with ADHD. ...
... The findings generalize prediction of the OPTIMAL theory that autonomy would affect motivational states such as selfefficacy in individuals with ADHD (Wulf Acquisition Phase Retention Self-efficacy Score Self-control Yoked & Lewthwaite, 2016). The results also are in accordance with the results of previous studies (Chiviacowsky, 2014;Chiviacowsky & Wulf 2002, Chiviacowsky et al., 2008Ghorbani & Bund, 2020;Wulf et al., 2014). Present findings indicate that autonomy support has clearly increased motivation during practice and it remained at a high level in no-KR retention condition, while that was not the case for yoked group. ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to investigate the effect of autonomy support (i.e., in the form of self-controlled feedback) on learning and self-efficacy in a throwing skill in adolescents with ADHD. The subjects were 40 adolescents with ADHD (14 to 17 years old) and were randomly and equally divided into two groups: self-controlled and yoked. Motor task included to throw beanbags with the non-dominant arm at a target on the ground. The participants executed the pretest (10 trials), an acquisition phase including 6 blocks of 10 trials, and a retention test consisting of 10 trials. The participants in the self-controlled group received KR anytime the requested. The yoked group was matched with self-controlled group, but without having a choice to request for feedback. Prior to pretest, each block, and before the retention test, all participants completed the self-efficacy scale. Dependent measures were throwing accuracy scores and self-efficacy. Independent t test and analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures were used to analyze the data. The results showed that participants in the self-controlled group had significantly higher throwing accuracy scores in the acquisition phase and the retention test than those in yoked group. Moreover, participants in the self-controlled group reported significantly higher self-efficacy scores in the acquisition phase and the retention test than those in yoked group. The results of this study show that people with ADHD benefit from autonomy support to learn a novel motor skill.
... On the other hand, in order to increase performance and learn skills, there are various techniques and strategies before training. Some of these techniques and strategies are included observing a model (Mohammadi et al. 2022;Hazrati et al. 2022;Hashemi Motlagh et al. 2022;Ghorbani & Bund, 2014Ghorbani, Ghanati, Dana, & Salehian, 2020;Mokhtari, Shojaei, & Dana, 2007), enhanced expectancies (Ghorbani, & Bund, 2020), and adopting an external focus of attention (Ghorbani, Dana, & Christodoulides, 2020;Ghorbani, Dana, & Fallah, 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study was designed to examine the effects of two kind of feedback presentation, namely KR and KP, on movement pattern and accuracy of a Badminton serve in children with autism. We used a causal-comparative method in the current study. Sixty children with autism with an age range of 7 to 12 years from special schools were selected based on a convenience sampling method and were randomly and equally assigned into four groups including KR, KP, KR+KP, and control groups. The motor task in the present study included the badminton serve, in which the movement patter and accuracy were measured as the dependent variable. The children participated in the pretest including 10 services, acquisition phase (5 training blocks, each of which included 10 services), and the retention test with 10 services. Respective feedback was provided before each practice block. We used ANOVA to analyze data. The results showed that both KR and KP feedback improve both the pattern and the accuracy of movement better than the condition without feedback. In addition, KP had better effects on the movement pattern and KR had better effects on the movement accuracy. Finally, children who were in the combination group performed better than all the groups in both execution of the movement pattern and service accuracy. Children with autism benefit from feedback to learn novel motor skills, indicating that they may have the necessary mechanisms to learn new skills through feedback.
... Attention of focus is the act of directing a person's attention to specific sources of information or topics of interest during movement performance. The focus of external attention is directing a person's attention to external sources of information (such as the path of an object or the result of movement on the environment) and the focus of internal attention is directing a person's attention to his own body movements (Chiviacowsky et al. 2010;Dana et al. 2019Dana et al. , 2021Ghorbani, & Bund, 2020;Ghorbani, Dana, & Fallah, 2019). They direct the attention of learners to body movements or environmental signs. ...
Article
Full-text available
Motor learning studies on adults have shown that directing the learners' attention to external cues is more effectual than internal cues. In this study, we investigated if this could be applied to children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD). 45 boys with developmental coordination disorder were selected using motor observation questionnaire for teachers. The task was static balance test that was measured in two experimental conditions including internal (focus on body limb) and external (focus on rex marker) focus of attention. For data analysis, ANOVA and Tukey's post hoc were used at the significant level of P < 0.05. Results showed that external focus could improve motor learning. However, there was no significant difference between internal focus and control groups. Thus children with DCD benefit from the external focus of attention to learning a static balance skill. According to the results of this study, therapists and coaches should adjust their rehabilitation methods and instructions based on external focus of attention.
... As tarefas utilizadas nas intervenções consistiram em: tarefa tempo coincidente com membros superiores (Chiviacowsky et al., 2012;Chiviacowsky, 2014;Drews et al., 2020e Drews et al., 2021, tarefas de arremesso, Pascua et al., 2015;Wulf et al., 2015;Ghorbani & Bund, 2020), de equilíbrio (Ong & Hodges, 2018;Chung et al., 2020), realidade virtual com membros superiores (Shuggi et al., 2019), desenho (Légal & Meyer, 2009), equilíbrio de bastão com membros superiores (Stevens et al., 2012), e esportivas incluindo, golf (Lebeau et al., 2018) e dardos (Shafizadeh et al., 2013e Ong et al., 2019. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective.The goal of this paper was to provide a comprehensive overview of the literature focusing on the influence of self-efficacy on performance and learning of motor skills in adults. Method. A systematic lit-erature review examining studies indexed in PubMed, Cochrane, Scielo, PsycArticles, and Pepsic databases was conducted. The following characteristics were extracted from the selected articles: authors and year of publica-tion, publication journal, hypothesis (predictor and criterion variables), sample characteristics, and risk of bias using the PEDro scale. Results. Analysis of the sixteen studies reveals a positive association between self-efficacy and motor performance, and in some studies, with learning. Strategies used to influence self-efficacy varied considerably among studies. There is no consensus on the best approach to influence the interpretation of this construct in adult motor skills training.
... Therapists can influence their participants' expectations in several ways. For example, providing them with positive feedback or encouragement will enhance their expectations (Ghorbani & Bund, 2020). Providing them with corrective feedback, on the other hand, may lower their expectations (van de Ridder et al., 2015). ...
Article
Effectiveness of motor interventions depends not only on learning content but also on the used teaching strategies. However, little is known regarding the application of teaching strategies in clinical practice. This study aimed to develop and assess psychometric properties of a new Dutch observational instrument to document teaching strategies: the Optimizing Performance through Intrinsic Motivation and Attention for Learning (OPTIMAL) Strategies Observational Tool (OSOT). Based on the OPTIMAL theory for motor learning from Wulf and Lewthwaite, the OSOT includes three variables: (a) expectancies, (b) autonomy, and (c) attention. The OSOT’s content was created by extracting relevant items from literature and existing instruments. To assess its psychometric properties, a convenience sample of 18 physiotherapy sessions for children with developmental coordination disorder was employed. Video recordings of these sessions were analyzed using Noldus The Observer XT. Relative duration (percentage of session time) was calculated for each item. Intraclass correlations were calculated to examine interrater and intrarater reliability. The design process resulted in 43 items in total. Interrater and intrarater intraclass correlations ranged from .48 to .99, with 81% (interrater), respectively, 95% (intrarater) of the items scoring above .75, indicating excellent agreement. The OSOT enables systematic and reliable observation of Optimizing Performance through Intrinsic Motivation and Attention for Learning teaching strategies used by therapists in Dutch clinical settings.
Article
Background and Purpose Older adults with chronic diabetes have been shown to exhibit reduced balance function and increased fear of falling; however, the contextual inter-relationships between diabetes and its psychological consequences on physical functioning are not fully understood. This study examined the relationships between diabetes disease status, balance confidence, fear of falling avoidance behavior, and changes in performance and confidence after massed practice of a balance task in participants with and without diabetes (PWD and PWOD). Methods Older adult PWD and PWOD were recruited for the pre-post control group study (n = 27 PWD, n = 26 PWOD). Participants underwent practice of a novel stabilometer-based balance task over a 2-day period (40 practice trials in 8 blocks). Changes in balance task performance and balance confidence were assessed pre- and post-training. Balance confidence and activity avoidance behavior associated with fear of falling were assessed using the Activities-Specific Balance Confidence Scale and Fear of Falling Avoidance Behavior Questionnaire, respectively. Repeated measures analysis of variance and mediation analyses were conducted to examine the effects of diabetes and training on balance performance and confidence, as well as how baseline balance confidence affects the training outcomes. Results and Discussion Fifty-three participants (27 with type II diabetes, 29 men, 23 women, and 1 gender nonconforming, mean age = 63.8, range 50-89 years) were enrolled in the study. Of them, 48 (90.6%) successfully completed the balance training with significant balance task performance improvement of approximately 30% in both groups (PWD: 3.04 [95% confidence interval, 1.77-4.31], P < .001; PWOD: 4.39 [95% confidence interval, 3.04-5.74], P < .001). Activities-Specific Balance Confidence Scale score significantly mediated the effect of diabetes on balance confidence after training and fear of falling avoidance behavior. Conclusions Despite the physical and psychological deficits associated with diabetes, individuals with chronic diabetes are capable of improving balance confidence and performance through targeted training. Balance confidence was identified as an important mediating factor, explaining the relationship between diabetes disease status and activity-related psycho-physical outcomes. Future research should focus on the potentially self-reinforcing effects of psycho-physical gains induced by exercise training.
Article
Full-text available
Background and Aim: As one of the popular English e-learning tools, Unipus customizes 1,700 universities, 40,000 teachers, and 8,000,000 students in China, thus, it is meaningful to invest the students’ perceived perceptions towards Unipus and has a significant impact on the blended learning environment implementation in higher education in China. This study aims to explore the factors that influence the non-English students Behavioral Intention to use Unipus and their actual Use Behavior by developing a framework based on the Unified Theory of Acceptance and Use of Technology (UTAUT), UTAUT2, and Diffusion of Innovation (DOI) model. Materials and Methods: The study collected 379 non-English student questionnaires in the study. The research utilized structural equation modeling (SEM) for hypothesis testing. Results: The result demonstrated the factors positively influencing intention to use which include Performance Expectancy(p<0.05), Social Influence(p<0.001), Learning Value(p<0.05), and Facilitating Conditions (FC) (p<0.001). Factors positively influencing Use Behavior include FC(p<0.05) and Behavioral Intention(p<0.001). The influence coefficients rank from highest to lowest as FC > SI > LV > PE, indicating that FC is the most significant factor driving student usage behavior among all factors, the attitudes of peers and teachers, along with time costs are significant influencing factors, whereas technical proficiency does not impact usage behavior or intention, a high level of behavioral intention typically encourages individuals to adopt and sustain the use of the technology, thereby leading to actual usage behavior. The findings offer insights into the factors that influence the intention and behavior of non-English students towards Unipus. Conclusion: Generally, if students perceive that Unipus can enhance their grades and is user-friendly, their intention to use the platform will increase. The influence of teachers and peers can positively affect students' attitudes and intentions toward using the platform. Teachers can assign tasks based on students' interests, increasing their motivation to engage with the platform. For platform designers, it is crucial to develop platforms that are efficient in terms of time investment and usability. Platforms characterized by high efficiency and ease of use are more likely to appeal to students.
Article
Full-text available
Motor learning is an important part of any sport. Various methods have been used to make motor learning more effective and improve it, and one of them is the enhanced expectancies method. The method assumes that if we induce a feeling in an individual that they can succeed in a task, they will perform better than individuals without the intervention. The main aim of the current systematic review was to explore the methods used to induce enhanced expectancies and to gain greater insight into research on the phenomenon. A total of 25 articles were included in the systematic review. Three main areas of methods used to induce enhanced expectancies emerged - feedback, differing criteria or assignments and visual illusions. Feedback appeared in 14 studies, while differing criteria or assignments and visual illusions each appeared in 8 studies. However, different methods fall under these three groups and are discussed in more depth. Another important finding is the effect of enhanced expectancies on self-efficacy, or other psychological components of the individual, which was found in 11 studies. Overall, research on enhanced expectancies is very diverse and the present review study depicts its forms.
Article
An external focus of attention and autonomy support are identified as key factors to optimize motor learning; however, research in children is limited. Moreover, research has failed to examine these factors in ecologically valid motor learning settings, like physical education. Therefore, the present study examined the effects of external focus of attention when delivered using autonomy-supportive or controlling instructional language on children’s motor learning. Thirty-three novice participants (10.30 ± 0.52 years) practiced a land-based curling task under supportive (external-focus instructions delivered with supportive language), controlling (external-focus instructions delivered with controlling language), or neutral (external instructions embedded in the task aim) conditions before completing a retention and transfer test. The supportive group produced higher positive affect after practice and greater accuracy in the retention test compared with the other groups. The findings provide support for the OPTIMAL (optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning) theory of motor learning that combining an external focus and autonomy support conditions improves motor learning.
Article
Full-text available
The present study investigated the effects of positive temporal-comparative feedback on the learning of a timing walk task in older adults. Thirty-four older adults practiced a task in which they were required to learn how to walk for a distance of 4 m using 50% of their maximal speed. Participants were divided into a positive temporal-comparative feedback (PTC) group and a control group, both of which received feedback about timing accuracy after every other trial during practice. In addition, after each block of 10 trials, participants in the PTC group were informed that their average temporal errors in the block were lower than the average error of the previous block. Retention and transfer tests were performed 24 hours after the practice phase. A customized questionnaire was also applied, which focused on verifying perceived competence, enjoinment, and nervousness levels. The results showed enhanced learning and lower levels of nervousness while adapting to the transfer task for the PTC group relative to the control group. These results provide evidence that positive temporal-comparative feedback can facilitate motor learning in older adults, and further support the motivational role of feedback in motor learning.
Article
Full-text available
One purpose of the present study was to examine whether self-confidence or anxiety would be differentially affected by feedback from more accurate rather than less accurate trials. The second purpose was to determine whether arousal variations (activation) would predict performance. On Day 1, participants performed a golf putting task under one of two conditions: one group received feedback on the most accurate trials, whereas another group received feedback on the least accurate trials. On Day 2, participants completed an anxiety questionnaire and performed a retention test. Skin conductance level, as a measure of arousal, was determined. The results indicated that feedback about more accurate trials resulted in more effective learning as well as increased self-confidence. Also, activation was a predictor of performance. Key words: Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2, golf putting, knowledge of results, motor learning
Article
Full-text available
Presents an integrative theoretical framework to explain and to predict psychological changes achieved by different modes of treatment. This theory states that psychological procedures, whatever their form, alter the level and strength of self-efficacy. It is hypothesized that expectations of personal efficacy determine whether coping behavior will be initiated, how much effort will be expended, and how long it will be sustained in the face of obstacles and aversive experiences. Persistence in activities that are subjectively threatening but in fact relatively safe produces, through experiences of mastery, further enhancement of self-efficacy and corresponding reductions in defensive behavior. In the proposed model, expectations of personal efficacy are derived from 4 principal sources of information: performance accomplishments, vicarious experience, verbal persuasion, and physiological states. Factors influencing the cognitive processing of efficacy information arise from enactive, vicarious, exhortative, and emotive sources. The differential power of diverse therapeutic procedures is analyzed in terms of the postulated cognitive mechanism of operation. Findings are reported from microanalyses of enactive, vicarious, and emotive modes of treatment that support the hypothesized relationship between perceived self-efficacy and behavioral changes. (21/2 p ref)
Article
Full-text available
The present study investigated whether motivation and augmented feedback processing explain the effect of an incidental choice on motor learning, and examined whether motivation and feedback processing generally predict learning. Accordingly, participants were assigned to one of two groups, choice or yoked, then asked to practice a non-dominant arm bean bag toss. The choice group was allowed to choose the color of the beanbag with which they made the toss, whereas the yoked group was not. Motor learning was determined by delayed-posttest accuracy and precision. Motivation and augmented feedback processing were indexed via the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory and electroencephalography, respectively. We predicted the choice group would exhibit greater motor learning, motivation, and augmented feedback processing, and that the latter two variables would predict learning. Results showed that an incidental choice failed to enhance motor learning, motivation, or augmented feedback processing. Additionally, neither motivation nor augmented feedback processing predicted motor learning. However, motivation and augmented feedback processing were correlated, with both factors predicting changes in practice performance. Thus, results suggest the effect of incidental choices on motor learning may be tenuous, and indicate motivation and augmented feedback processing may be more closely linked to changes in practice performance than motor learning.
Article
The OPTIMAL theory proposes that enhancing expectancies and autonomy facilitate motor performance and learning (Wulf & Lewthwaite). Present study with two experiments aimed to examine this proposition by using a modified dart throwing as motor task. In both experiments, motor learning (i.e., retention test) was enhanced by practice conditions, which enhance expectancies for future performance and support learners’ autonomy. Moreover, they led to significantly superior self-efficacy scores during all acquisition phase, retention, and transfer tests. Findings of the present study provided support for propositions of the OPTIMAL theory. Results are discussed in terms of motivational aspects of enhancing expectancies and autonomy and their role on facilitating motor learning.
Article
We report two experiments evaluating the impact of success-related feedback on learning of a balance task. In Exp. 1, we studied the influence of lax and conservative outcome feedback, as well as large vs. small improvements in outcome feedback on balance learning. Despite impacts on competency, there were no between-group differences in actual performance or learning. Because no comparative information was provided in Exp. 1, we tested four further groups that either did or did not receive positive or negative comparative feedback (Exp. 2). Although the manipulations influenced competency and arousal, again, there was no impact on balance outcomes. These data cast doubt on the assertions made in the OPTIMAL theory that perceptions of success are moderators of motor learning.
Article
Background: Maximum aerobic capacity (VO2max) is widely accepted as the best measure of cardiovascular fitness and aerobic power. The present study investigated whether enhancing participants' performance expectancies through positive social-comparative feedback would increase VO2max. Methods: Participants were experienced runners who regularly ran for exercise or competitively. All participants completed two VO2max tests within a 2-week period at similar times of the day. Before the second test, enhanced expectancy group participants were informed that their aerobic capacity on the first test was above the group average, whereas control group participants were told the second test was for validation purposes. Measurements taken were relative to VO2max, as well as pulmonary ventilation, respiratory exchange ratio, heart rate, and ratings of perceived exertion. Results: The enhanced expectancy group demonstrated a significant increase (+3.28%) in VO2max from Test 1 (61.1±2.8 mL·kg-1·min-1) to Test 2 (63.7±2.9 mL·kg-1·min-1, p = 0.007), whereas the control group's VO2max decreased significantly (-4.11%, Test 1 = 59.4±2.9 mL·kg-1·min-1, Test 2 = 57.8±2.3 mL·kg-1·min-1, p = 0.027). No group differences were found with respect to other performance measures (pulmonary ventilation p = 0.22, heart rate p = 0.97, respiratory exchange ratio p = 0.11, rate of perceived exertion p = 0.13). Conclusions: The results show that maximum aerobic capacity is, in part, a function of the performer's self-efficacy expectations. These findings add to the increasing evidence demonstrating social-cognitive-affective influences on (maximum) motor performance.