Content uploaded by Kelly-Ann Allen
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Kelly-Ann Allen on Dec 04, 2019
Content may be subject to copyright.
International Journal of Innovation, Creativity and Change. www.ijicc.net
Volume 5, Issue 1, November 2019
189
The Myth of Left- vs Right-Brain
Learning
Dr Kelly-Ann Allena & Dr Rick van der Zwanb
a Senior Lecturer, Monash University, Australia b Cognitive Neuro-Scientist
Consultant, Australia
It has been more than a decade since researchers began calling for
caution over certain brain-learning strategies supposedly based on
neuroscience. Nonetheless, misconceptions still persist. This paper
explores the myth of hemispheric dominance in learning, and
provides advice to educators, parents, and others in the field. It has
long been a popular view that some people favour one hemisphere
over the other and that such cognitive preferences have
implications for learning. Scientific research into the structures and
functions of normal brains, both as they develop and on into
adulthood, has demonstrated the fallacy of this belief. As such,
interventions and products that target left- or right-brain learning
should be treated with caution. It is unlikely that these interventions
successfully target one hemisphere over the other, or that they
improve learning outcomes in ways that depend on such perceived
differences. Educators, parents, and others in the field are urged to
inform themselves about the fundamental features of neuroscience,
and to look for significant independent research that supports
specific learning programs. While many school-based programs in
mainstream settings are supported by research, school leaders,
teachers and parents should take into consideration the quality of
evidence available, the purpose of the intervention and how the
intervention or program may fit the needs of the students and
context.
Keywords: Brain Learning, Brain Hemisphere, Education, Psychology, Research-based
Education
International Journal of Innovation, Creativity and Change. www.ijicc.net
Volume 5, Issue 1, November 2019
190
Introduction
Neuroscientists have long known that brains are ‘plastic’: If they were not, learning would
occur neither during development nor maturation (Blakemore & Cooper, 1970; Ismail, Fatemi,
& Johnston, 2017; Kolb, 2018; Munte, et al., 2002). As such, many educators have, for some
time, looked to neuroscience to help inform their practices. Despite that recognition, and as
Howard-Jones (2009) points out, “Decades without formal interdisciplinary communication
have allowed many unscientific ‘brain-based’ ideas to become established in the classroom”
(p. 550). While some of those ‘ideas’ have been developed by well-intentioned, classroom-
based teachers and school leaders, an industry that offers products and learning programs that
claim neuroscientific authenticity has also grown.
Discriminating between ideas and programs that have scientific merit and those that don’t can
be difficult. As a consequence, researchers have urged that so-called ‘brain-based’ learning
strategies be approached with caution (Goswami, 2006; Howard-Jones, 2009; Dekker, et al.
2012). The challenge in that approach is, of course, that myths still prevail, as do the so-called
learning strategies based on those myths. The aims of this paper are (i) to highlight the myth
of brain-dominance in learning, and (ii) to provide advice to educators, parents, and others on
how best to avoid programs and interventions based on this myth.
Hemispheric Asymmetry
Brains are among the most complex structures in the known universe. One hundred billion
neurons are distributed across the two hemispheres of the brain, with the hemispheres being
located on either side of our bilateral mid-line. Joining the two hemispheres is a massive bundle
of fibres called the corpus collosum. While the hemispheres are symmetrically distributed
within the brain, one on each side, they are morphologically asymmetrical, and develop
asymmetrically (Levin, 2005; Zaidel, 2001).
Perhaps unsurprisingly, in that context, the two hemispheres function asymmetrically as well.
That is, each hemisphere has functions that are not similarly represented in the other. Almost
International Journal of Innovation, Creativity and Change. www.ijicc.net
Volume 5, Issue 1, November 2019
191
all brain processes are undertaken by specialised groups of neurons in identifiable and
predictable locations. Certainly, all clusters of neurons are massively interconnected with other
parts of the brain, but specific areas are “tuned” for specific tasks and functions. Those areas
develop at specific times during maturation, and damage to an area can lead to a deficit in
whatever task it is that the neurons in that area do.
While some areas are bilaterally represented – the same place in each hemisphere exhibits the
same types of functions - the tuning of neurons in each hemisphere typically is not. For
example, visual processing occurs at the back of the brain, in the occipital lobes (put your hands
behind your head and your palms are resting above the occipital pole of each hemisphere).
Both the left and right occipital lobes process visual information, but the left hemisphere
processes information from the right visual field, and the right hemisphere processes
information from the left. That same pattern is seen for other functions too – the left frontal
lobe controls the right side of the body, and so on.
In addition to those processing asymmetries, more specific hemispheric specialisations, or
‘performance dominances’ occur. That is, there are examples of brain processes that occur
predominantly in one hemisphere, and much less so, or not at all, in the other. The most salient
example is language. In 1865, Paul Broca first noted that lesions in the left hemisphere affected
language ability in stroke patients. A hundred and fifty years later, functional magnetic
resonance imaging studies have unpacked the complex nature of language specialisation. In
95% of right-handed people, language is processed predominantly in the left hemisphere. For
the other 5%, language processing seems to be distributed bilaterally between the hemispheres.
In left-handers, the left-hemisphere performance dominance in language processing is reduced
to around 75%, while language is processed bilaterally in 14% (Pujol, Deus, Losilla, &
Capdevila, 1999), and a weak right-hemisphere predominance is found in the remaining 11%.
While various other functions have, over time, been cited as being specific to the left or the
right hemisphere, there is little evidence that any normal cognitive functioning occurs in only
one hemisphere or the other. The corpus collosum provides a connection that ensures massive
data-exchanges between the two hemispheres. Activation of neurons in any one location in one
International Journal of Innovation, Creativity and Change. www.ijicc.net
Volume 5, Issue 1, November 2019
192
hemisphere will lead to activity in many other regions of both hemispheres as well. While
diseased or damaged brains may function differently, in well-functioning brains, the notion that
one hemisphere can work independently of the other is false.
So… people really aren’t right-brain or left-brain thinkers?
While a simple model of hemispheric specialisation for learning is attractive as a heuristic, the
notion is a fallacy. There is currently no substantial empirical evidence to suggest that people
use their left or right brain more for thinking (Lindell & Kidd, 2011). It is certainly true that
some people tend to be stronger in analytical types of thinking, others are stronger in creative
thinking, and some are strong across multiple domains. But, there are no good grounds at all to
associate these traits with the predominance of one hemisphere over the other (Nielsen,
Zielinski, Ferguson, Lainhart, & Anderson, 2013).
That means that despite tuning asymmetries and despite hemispheric specialisations, there is
no research to date that has demonstrated that individuals are left- or right-brained learners. In
fact, the idea of right- or left-brain learning is possibly one of the oldest and most pervasive
neuromyths circulating today.
Origins of the left-brain/ right-brain learning myth
The origin of the myth of left/right-brain learning is unclear, but it may have emerged from the
scientific literature on split-brain patients. Split-brain surgery was initially developed in the
1940s as a treatment for seizures. The procedure involved severing the corpus callosum, the
massive neuron pathway connecting the right and left hemispheres, and offered one of the first
opportunities to study interactions between the hemispheres.
The procedure which is now replaced by more refined protocols allowed researchers at that
time to study a small population of split-brain patients (Gazzaniga, Bogen, & Sperry, 1962;
Sperry, 1961; Sperry, 1968). Gazzaniga (2008) has noted that split-brain surgery was
successful in decreasing seizure activity and, happily, produced no instances of ‘split
personalities’. He also noted that while cutting the corpus collosum produced little or no change
International Journal of Innovation, Creativity and Change. www.ijicc.net
Volume 5, Issue 1, November 2019
193
in cognitive activity in the ‘dominant’ left hemisphere of most patients, the right hemisphere
in split brain patients often showed a significant decrease in activity.
Of course, cutting the corpus collosum prevents language processes accessing the right
hemisphere, which among other things, specialises in making perceptual distinctions, and
processing and managing emotions (Gazzaniga, 2008). In other words, being able to articulate
ideas looks like a function of the left hemisphere, and handling perceptions and emotions looks
like a function of the right hemisphere. In normal, healthy individuals, that distinction is
obviously incorrect and based on gross simplifications of complex interactions between the
hemispheres. Nonetheless, it may be one source of the myth of left-brain/right-brain learning
styles.
Another source of the left/right brain learning myth may relate to misconceptions surrounding
hemispheric dominance. As explained above, hemispheric dominance relates to the sites of
origin of specific processes within the brain. The same term has colloquially become associated
with other functions; left or right handedness or what side of your body you choose to sleep
on, for example. In reality, it is difficult-to-impossible to identify by observation any types of
hemispheric dominance, and that is likely because of the vast amounts of information being
exchanged between the hemispheres at any one moment and all cognitive processes depend on
the coordination of complex interhemispheric processing (Doron, Bassett, & Gazzaniga, 2012).
Brain-based Education Based on Evidence
While it has been 70 years since the first split brain surgery and despite the almost exponential
growth in brain research and neuroimaging studies, there is a considerable time lag between
findings generated by ‘pure’ research and the implementation of that research into teaching
practice. That situation is not unique to education. The estimated lag in health research, which
is the time from initial research to practical healthcare solutions, is 17 years. In the case of
education, we can expect the lag to be much greater (Balas & Bohen, 2000).
International Journal of Innovation, Creativity and Change. www.ijicc.net
Volume 5, Issue 1, November 2019
194
One reason for that is there are a number of steps required to convert brain science into
impactful teaching strategies. Brain imaging or functioning needs to be linked, unambiguously,
to observable and teachable behaviours and skills. Those behaviours and skills need to be
linked to valuable learning outcomes, and then those learning outcomes need to be
implemented into curriculum. Each step requires professional expertise, and without someone
explicitly and systematically undertaking each of those steps, and supporting each transition
with scientifically meaningful evidence, myths arise and mistakes can occur (Donoghue &
Horvath, 2016; Horvath & Donoghue, 2016). For busy school staff, gaining the technical and
scientific expertise to translate the most up-to-date neuro-jargon into useful practices and
knowledge in the classroom can be difficult even if they do have the time to work through the
process. As consequence, teachers and other educators can, despite good intentions, mistake
misconceptions and outdated theories for scientifically-based innovations.
Morris, Wooding, and Grant (2011) note that the time between initial research and useful
practice does have the benefit of providing an opportunity to ensure the efficacy of any new
programs or approaches. But for that time to be an advantage, parents, schools, interested
others, and skilled, reputable researchers have to undertake some due-diligence. That is, they
need to carefully explore or conduct research investigating the claims made by brain training
programs or interventions (see, for example, Shipstead, Hicks, & Engle, 2012; Stong,
Togerson, Togerson, & Hulme, 2010). Educators should find opportunities to partner with
researchers to explore the efficacy of new programs and approaches. Parents and schools
interested in exploring the efficacy of new programs or approaches should carefully eschew
marketing brochures and websites to explore more carefully the claims of brain-based training
programs. An easy way to start is to look at how recent the science is behind the program. If
little time has passed, it is likely there have been little corroborating science, and certainly little
chance that the proposed programs have been rigorously tested.
International Journal of Innovation, Creativity and Change. www.ijicc.net
Volume 5, Issue 1, November 2019
195
What else do teachers and parents need to know?
Many mainstream educational programs are of a high standard and there are a number
supported by sound research. Nonetheless, parents and educational professionals should be
wary of educational programs that lack independent empirical evidence or that rely on the use
of personal stories and testimonials (Bowen & Snow, 2017). Some programs and products
muddy the waters with assertions like “neuroscience proves that our program grows your brain”
or “this program makes your child’s brain light up”. Whether, or not, such claims are true, they
are something of a distraction from a couple of important questions: The most important
question we can ask of any program that is supposed to improve student learning is “Does this
program improve student learning?” And the next question must be “Where is the science to
show it is so?” Those questions may sound obvious, and they are, but they often get lost in
irrelevant claims about supposed neural mechanisms. In the end, it matters little whether brains
get bigger or smaller; it matters little whether whole brains are more, or less active. What
matters is the impact of the learning mechanisms – do learners learn more? Or do they learn
more effectively? Is their retention better? And do they enjoy the learning process (a critical
step in becoming lifelong learners)? And, if they do, show us the evidence for that. Not
someone’s claim, but real, and valid data.
There are many excellent, expert researchers working to discover strategies that help make
learning more effective. A few minutes on Google Scholar, for instance, will reveal how much
formal work has been done to study or validate strategies, as well as the outcomes of that work.
A key message here is that if you type in the name of a commercially available program and
find nothing, then it is wise to be cautious about accepting its promises or effectiveness (Bowen
& Snow, 2017).
Useful guidelines are available for parents and educators in this area, especially for those
raising or working with children with learning difficulties or developmental delays (Bowen &
Snow, 2017; Stephenson, Wheldall, & Carter, 2017). Parents and schools need to do their own
homework to determine whether the broad claims made in a program’s promotional materials
International Journal of Innovation, Creativity and Change. www.ijicc.net
Volume 5, Issue 1, November 2019
196
are indeed true. They should ensure that interventions and strategies fit with their unique needs
and context, and suit the purpose at which they are aimed at.
How to Start?
Parents should be aware that early childhood is a time of rapid cognitive development and
growth. Brains are born with a full complement of undifferentiated neurons, and require normal
experiences to drive the development of processes and pathways.
The good news is that parents can drive development simply by providing rich and stimulating
environments and experiences. Those experiences do not need to be complex or expensive.
Parents can give their children a solid foundation for future learning by helping their children
build positive and supportive relationships with caregivers (including teachers); giving them
opportunities to play, socialise, read books, and listen to nursery rhymes; exposing them to
languages and music; giving them opportunities to learn social and emotional competencies;
providing a healthy diet; and keeping kids physically active (Allen, Kern, Vella-Brodrick,
Waters, & Hattie, 2018; Allen, Vella-Brodrick, & Waters, 2017; Barreto, de Miguel, Ibarluzea,
Andiarena, & Arranz, 2017; Grove, Reupert, & Maybery, 2015; Riebschleger, Grove,
Cavanaugh, & Costello, 2017).
Once kids reach formal learning settings, even as early as preschool, qualified teachers and
educators provide children with age- and stage-appropriate opportunities to develop into
functioning adults. Children who are able to embark on a safe and healthy developmental
trajectory, void of neglect, trauma or abuse, and surrounded by caring, trusting and nurturing
relationships are in a good position for healthy brain development to naturally occur.
Conclusions
Despite its fallacy, the myth of left- or right-brain dominance is problematically enduring.
Parents and educators should be extremely cautious when approaching educational programs,
interventions, phone apps, or books that claim to stimulate one hemisphere in preference to the
other (e.g., right-brain approaches).
International Journal of Innovation, Creativity and Change. www.ijicc.net
Volume 5, Issue 1, November 2019
197
Similarly, the claim that a program or resource is “whole-brain” or “brain-based” in its
approach should be a red flag for educators and parents, especially in the absence of rigorous
scientific research. This is because all learning environments and experiences drive the “whole-
brain” since it is the brain that learns.
Acknowledgements
To Gregory Donoghue, The University of Melbourne and the Australian Council for
Educational Research (ACER) for feedback and consultation.
International Journal of Innovation, Creativity and Change. www.ijicc.net
Volume 5, Issue 1, November 2019
198
References
Allen, K. A., Kern, P., Vella-Brodrick, D., & Waters, L. (2018). Understanding the priorities
of Australian secondary schools through an analysis of their mission and vision
statements. Educational Administration Quarterly, 54(2), 249-274.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0013161X18758655
Allen, K. A., Vella-Brodrick, D., & Waters, L. (2017). School belonging and the role of
social and emotional competencies in fostering an adolescent’s sense of connectedness
to their school (pp 83-99). In E. Frydenberg & A. Martin (Eds.), Social and Emotional
Learning in the Australasian Context (pp. 83-99). Melbourne, AU: Springer Social
Sciences
Balas, E. A., & Boren, S. A. (2000). Managing clinical knowledge for health care
improvement. In J. H. van Bemmel & A. T. McCray (Eds.), Yearbook of Medical
Informatics, 9(01), 65-70.
Barreto, F. B., de Miguel, M. S., Ibarluzea, J., Andiarena, A., & Arranz, E. (2017). Family
context and cognitive development in early childhood: A longitudinal study. Intelligence,
65, 11-22.
Blakemore, C. & Cooper, G.F. (1970). Development of the brain depends on the visual
environment. Nature, 228, 477-478.
Bowen, C., & Snow, P. (2017). Making sense of interventions for children with developmental
disorders: A guide for parents and professionals. Hampshire, United Kingdom: J & R
Press Limited.
Dekker, S., Lee, N.C., Howard-Jones, P., & Jolles, J. (2012) Neuromyths in education:
Prevalence and predictors of misconceptions among teachers. Frontiers in Psychology,
3, 429. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00429
Donoghue, G. M & Horvath, J.C. (2016). Translating neuroscience, psychology and education:
An abstracted conceptual framework for the learning sciences, Cogent Education, 3(1),
DOI: 10.1080/2331186X.2016.1267422.
International Journal of Innovation, Creativity and Change. www.ijicc.net
Volume 5, Issue 1, November 2019
199
Doron, K. W., Bassett, D. S., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2012). Dynamic network structure of
interhemispheric coordination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
109(46), 18661-18668.
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2008). Human: The science behind what makes us unique. New York, NY:
Ecco.
Gazzaniga, M. S., Bogen, J. E., & Sperry, R. W. (1962). Some functional effects of
sectioning the cerebral commissures in man. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 48(10), 1765-1769.
Goswami, U. (2006). Neuroscience and education: from research to practice? Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 7, 406-413.
Grove, C., Reupert, A. E., & Maybery, D. J. (2015). Gaining knowledge about parental mental
illness: how does it empower children? Child and Family Social Work, 20(4), 377 -
386. https://doi.org/10.1111/cfs.12086
Horvath, J. C. & Donoghue, G. M. (2016). A Bridge Too Far – Revisited: Reframing Bruer’s
Neuroeducation Argument for Modern Science of Learning Practitioners. Frontieres
Psychology, 7(377), DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00377.
Howard-Jones, P. (2009). Scepticism is not enough. Cortex, 45, 550-551.
Ismail, F. Y., Fatemi, A., & Johnston, M. V. (2017). Cerebral plasticity: windows of
opportunity in the developing brain. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology, 21(1),
23-48.
Kolb, B. (2018). Brain Plasticity and Experience. In R. Gibb & B. Kolb (Eds.), The
Neurobiology of Brain and Behavioral Development (pp. 341-389). Cambridge, MA:
Academic Press.
Levin, M. (2005). Left–right asymmetry in embryonic development: a comprehensive review.
Mechanisms of Development, 122(1), 3-25.
Lindell, A. K., & Kidd, E. (2011). Why right‐brain teaching is half‐witted: A critique of the
misapplication of neuroscience to education. Mind, Brain, and Education, 5(3), 121-127.
Morris, Z. S., Wooding, S., & Grant, J. (2011). The answer is 17 years, what is the question:
understanding time lags in translational research. Journal of the Royal Society of
Medicine, 104(12), 510-520.
International Journal of Innovation, Creativity and Change. www.ijicc.net
Volume 5, Issue 1, November 2019
200
Münte, T. F., Altenmüller, E., & Jäncke, L. (2002). The musician’s brain as a model of
neuroplasticity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 473-478.
Nielsen, J. A., Zielinski, B. A., Ferguson, M. A., Lainhart, J. E., & Anderson, J. S. (2013). An
evaluation of the left-brain vs. right-brain hypothesis with resting state functional
connectivity magnetic resonance imaging. PloS one, 8(8), e71275.
Pujol, J., Deus, J., Losilla, J. M., & Capdevila, A. (1999). Cerebral lateralization of language
in normal left-handed people studied by functional MRI. Neurology, 52(5), 1038-1038.
Riebschleger, J., Grove, C. J., Cavanaugh, D., & Costello, S. C. (2017). Mental health
literacy content for children of parents with a mental illness: Thematic analysis of a
literature review. Brain Sciences, 7(11), 1-19. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7110141
Shipstead, Z., Hicks, K. L., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Cogmed working memory training: Does
the evidence support the claims? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
Cognition, 1(3), 185-193.
Sperry, R. W. (1961). Cerebral organization and behavior. Science, 133(3466), 1749-1757.
Sperry, R. W. (1968). Hemisphere deconnection and unity in conscious awareness. American
Psychologist, 23(10), 723-33.
Stephenson, J., Wheldall, K., & Carter, M. (2017, September). Is it a scam? Nomanis Notes,
(1). Retrieved from
https://docs.wixstatic.com/ugd/81f204_56a95d9e0a7b4b13a9d42acaa7c714a4.pdf
Strong, G. K., Torgerson, C. J., Torgerson, D., & Hulme, C. (2011). A systematic meta‐
analytic review of evidence for the effectiveness of the ‘Fast ForWord’language
intervention program. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(3), 224-235.
Zaidel E. (2001). Brain asymmetry. In N.J. Smelser & P.B. Baltes (Eds.), International
Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 1st Ed (pp. 1321-1329). Oxford,
United Kingdom: Elsevier Ltd.