ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Meta-analytic findings indicate that the success of unmasking a deceptive interaction relies more on the performance of the liar than on that of the lie detector. Despite this finding, the lie characteristics and strategies of deception that enable good liars to evade detection are largely unknown. We conducted a survey (n = 194) to explore the association between laypeople’s self-reported ability to deceive on the one hand, and their lie prevalence, characteristics, and deception strategies in daily life on the other. Higher self-reported ratings of deception ability were positively correlated with self-reports of telling more lies per day, telling inconsequential lies, lying to colleagues and friends, and communicating lies via face-to-face interactions. We also observed that self-reported good liars highly relied on verbal strategies of deception and they most commonly reported to i) embed their lies into truthful information, ii) keep the statement clear and simple, and iii) provide a plausible account. This study provides a starting point for future research exploring the meta-cognitions and patterns of skilled liars who may be most likely to evade detection.
This content is subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Lie prevalence, lie characteristics and
strategies of self-reported good liars
Brianna L. VeriginID
1,2
*, Ewout H. Meijer
1
, Glynis BogaardID
1
, Aldert Vrij
2
1Forensic Psychology Section, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht,
Netherlands, 2Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, United Kingdom
*brianna.verigin@maastrichtuniversity.nl
Abstract
Meta-analytic findings indicate that the success of unmasking a deceptive interaction relies
more on the performance of the liar than on that of the lie detector. Despite this finding, the
lie characteristics and strategies of deception that enable good liars to evade detection are
largely unknown. We conducted a survey (n= 194) to explore the association between lay-
people’s self-reported ability to deceive on the one hand, and their lie prevalence, character-
istics, and deception strategies in daily life on the other. Higher self-reported ratings of
deception ability were positively correlated with self-reports of telling more lies per day, tell-
ing inconsequential lies, lying to colleagues and friends, and communicating lies via face-to-
face interactions. We also observed that self-reported good liars highly relied on verbal strat-
egies of deception and they most commonly reported to i) embed their lies into truthful infor-
mation, ii) keep the statement clear and simple, and iii) provide a plausible account. This
study provides a starting point for future research exploring the meta-cognitions and pat-
terns of skilled liars who may be most likely to evade detection.
Introduction
Despite the importance of being able to detect deception, research has consistently found that
people are unable to do so. In fact, the accuracy rates vary around chance level [1,2]. Lacking
good lie detectors, a growing body of evidence indicates that the accuracy of detecting decep-
tion depends more on the characteristics of the liar and less on the lie detection ability of the
judge [37]. The meta-analysis of Bond and DePaulo [3] provided robust evidence that liars
vary in their detectability. Their analysis showed that differences in detectability from sender
to sender are more reliable than differences in credulity from judge to judge, with reliability
coefficients of .58 and .30, respectively. This pattern of results was replicated by other research-
ers [5], lending support to the proposition that liar characteristics exert a powerful influence
on lie detection outcomes. Moreover, it has been shown that sender demeanour explained up
to 98% of the variance in detection accuracy [7].
Yet, only a handful of studies have attempted to determine individual differences in the
ability to lie credibly [812]. Research on what characterizes those who escape detection, i.e.,
good liars, would be highly beneficial in investigative settings. Thus, a focus on the liar, in
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 1 / 16
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Verigin BL, Meijer EH, Bogaard G, Vrij A
(2019) Lie prevalence, lie characteristics and
strategies of self-reported good liars. PLoS ONE 14
(12): e0225566. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
pone.0225566
Editor: Giuseppe Sartori, University of Padova,
ITALY
Received: August 12, 2019
Accepted: November 7, 2019
Published: December 3, 2019
Peer Review History: PLOS recognizes the
benefits of transparency in the peer review
process; therefore, we enable the publication of
all of the content of peer review and author
responses alongside final, published articles. The
editorial history of this article is available here:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566
Copyright: ©2019 Verigin et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: The data underlying
the results presented in the study are available
from https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/GB4RP.
Funding: This research was supported by a
fellowship awarded from the Erasmus Mundus
particular the skilled liar, was the aim of this study. Specifically, the present manuscript reports
an exploratory study addressing how self-reported deception ability is associated with lie prev-
alence and lie characteristics, and how self-reported good liars utilize strategies for deceiving.
First, we investigated the relationship between liars’ self-reported lie-telling frequency and
self-reported deception ability. The most widely cited research on deception prevalence esti-
mates the frequency at an average of once or twice per day [13,14]. More recent research, how-
ever, shows that the distribution of lies per day is considerably skewed. The majority of lies are
told by only a handful of prolific liars [1517]. Specifically, in a survey of nearly 3,000 partici-
pants, researchers found that 5% of respondents accounted for over 50% of all the lies reported
within the past 24 hours, whereas the majority of subjects reported telling no lies at all [15].
Several additional studies, as well as a reanalysis of DePaulo et al.’s [13] diary study, have vali-
dated that the majority of lies are told by a minority of people [14,16]. These few prolific liars
tend to tell more serious lies that carry significant consequences if detected [15]. Also, people
who self-reported to lie more often were more prone to cheating in laboratory tasks for per-
sonal profit [17]. It is possible that these prolific liars also perceive themselves as more skilled
at deceiving and tell more lies that they think will stay undetected, either because they believe
the receiver will not try to find out or they believe they are good enough to fool the receiver.
Second, our investigation examined whether characteristics of lies differ as a function of
self-reported deception ability. The first of these characteristics is the type of lie. This can refer
to the severity; at one end are white lies, which are relatively common [18] and often used to
ease social interactions (e.g., telling your mother-in-law that her baking is delicious when you
actually dislike sweets) [19], while at the other end are bold-faced fabrications, which are less
common and typically serve to protect the liar (e.g., denying having had an affair) [20]. The lat-
ter type of lies are also encountered more by the legal system [21]. Other taxonomies of lies
also exist, for example lies of omission or lies embedded into the truth; however, research has
yet to explore how the types of lies could differ as a function of deception ability. Is it that, for
example, good liars tend to utilize a certain type of lie which facilitates their success? The sec-
ond characteristic is the receiver of the lie. Lies can be communicated to a variety of individu-
als ranging from family, romantic partners, and friends to strangers, colleagues, or authority
figures. Previous research has shown that people lie less frequently in close relationships than
in casual relationships [22]. A third characteristic we are interested in is the medium of decep-
tion, as this can also influence the success of one’s lie. Some liars, for instance, prefer online
communication [23]. This would fit the liars’ (erroneous) belief their deception will leak out
via behavioural cues [24]. It is unknown, however, if or how good liars concentrate their lies to
specific individuals or communicate via certain mediums.
Finally, we examined how self-reported good liars utilize strategies for deceiving. The idea
that liars adopt strategies to enhance the likelihood of successfully deceiving stems from
research on impression and information management. Both forms of regulation relate to the
idea that much of social behaviour is controlled for the purpose of interpersonal presentation
[25,26]. In legal contexts, both liars and truth tellers are motivated to achieve a favourable
impression and attempt to do so by regulating their speech and behaviour, albeit liars more so
than truth tellers [27]. The topic of deceivers’ strategies has received some empirical attention
[2730]. For example, it was found that among the principal strategies of criminal offenders
were “Staying close to the truth,” and “Not giving away information” [31]. Researchers have
also capitalized on this increased awareness of liars’ and truth tellers’ strategies by developing
strategy-based lie detection tools. For instance, the Verifiability Approach (VA) [32,33]
exploits liars’ strategy of providing detailed statements that are embellished with unverifiable
information. Moreover, some researchers have speculated that good liars might use effective
strategies to conduct their behaviour, by attempting to act in line with people’s beliefs about
Self-reported good liars
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 2 / 16
Joint Doctorate Program The House of Legal
Psychology (EMJD-LP) with Framework
Partnership Agreement (FPA) 2013-0036 and
Specific Grant Agreement (SGA) 2016-1339 to
Brianna L. Verigin. The funders had no role in study
design, data collection and analysis, decision to
publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
how truth tellers behave while avoiding behaviour associated with liars [10]. Still, surveying
expert liars about their strategies as a source of insight into real-world deception remains a
highly underdeveloped research avenue [34].
Materials and methods
This study was approved by the ethical committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Neurosci-
ence at Maastricht University. Participants read and signed the informed consent in accor-
dance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Participants
The sample consisted of 194 participants (97 females; 95 males; 2 preferred not to say; M
age
=
39.12 years, SD
age
= 11.43) recruited via Amazon Mechanical Turk (mTurk). Most participants
reported being U.S. citizens (n= 175), whereas the remainder (n= 19) reported Indian citizen-
ship. Participants who completed the study were paid 1.75 USD. Participants could participate
in the study if they reported to be able to understand and write English at an advanced level.
To ensure data quality, participants were required to have the mTurk Masters Qualification
that is awarded to those who have demonstrated continual excellence across a wide range of
mTurk projects. An additional 133 participants began the questionnaire but did not complete
it, therefore their data were discarded. Data from nine participants were also removed because
of insufficient responses. We reached our sample size (n= 194) after these exclusions. The
study was approved by the standing ethical committee.
Procedure
The online questionnaire was created on Qualtrics online platform. After providing informed
consent, participants were provided definitions of lying and deception modelled from previous
research [2, 13; see Supporting Information). Participants were asked to read these definitions
carefully and to consider them while making responses throughout the questionnaire. In the
first part of the questionnaire, participants reported their experience with telling lies in daily
life. Participants rated on a 10-point Likert scale (1 –very poor to 10 –excellent) “How good
are you at successfully deceiving others (i.e., getting away with lies)?” Next, they reported the
estimated number of lies told during the past 24 hours. Participants then responded to multi-
ple-response questions about i) the types of lies told during the past 24 hours (options: white
lies, exaggerations, lies of omission/concealment, lies of commission/fabrications, embedded
lies; see the Supporting Information for the definitions provided to participants); ii) the receiv-
ers of their deception (options: family, friend, employer, colleague, authority figure, or other);
and iii) the mediums of their deception (options: face-to-face, over the phone, social media,
text message, email, or other).
The second part of the questionnaire probed the deceiver’s strategies. Participants provided
an open-ended response to explain “In general, what strategy or strategies do you use when
telling lies?” They were then asked to rate on a 10-point Likert scale (1 –not important to 10
very important) how important they consider verbal strategies of deception and nonverbal
strategies of deception to be for getting away with lies (for the definitions provided to partici-
pants, see the Supporting Information). Finally, participants indicated which verbal strategies
they use when telling lies in general from a pre-determined set (options: reporting from previ-
ous experience, providing details the person cannot check [i.e., unverifiable details], telling a
plausible story, etcetera). The options included in this list were drawn from empirical findings
regarding liars’ strategies and cues to deception [26,32,35]. Participants then provided demo-
graphic information regarding their age, sex, citizenship, ethnicity and education. We explored
Self-reported good liars
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 3 / 16
the association between laypeople’s self-reported deception ability and their sex and education
level. Finally, an additional part of the questionnaire asked participants to recall a time in
which they had told a serious lie and to report their rationale for lying and their strategies. We
examined how the deception rationale influenced their motivation, preparation, strategies,
and perceived success of the lie. To conserve manuscript length, the final section of the ques-
tionnaire is reported in the Supporting Information.
Qualitative analysis. To code the participants’ self-reported strategies into data-driven
categories, the first author performed a content analysis on the open-ended responses to the
question probing their use of strategies. First, each participant’s strategy or strategies was iden-
tified, then all overlapping responses were combined, and these strategies were condensed into
several dominant categories with theoretical similarities (i.e., relating to behavioural control or
verbal control, etcetera). The main coder completed each stage of this process and all authors
approved upon the final categories. Seven categories emerged from this coding method, for
example omitting certain information, relating to truthful information, or controlling behav-
iour (see Table 1).
To establish inter-rater reliability, the main coder and a second coder coded a randomly
selected 20% of the participants’ open-ended responses into the appropriate categories. A two-
way mixed effects model measuring consistency [36] showed that raters were highly consistent
across all categories (Single Measures ICCs ranged from .79 to 1.00). After confirming that the
raters were consistent, the main coder (first author) completed the remaining sample of partic-
ipant responses and only these scores were used in the analysis.
Results
We were interested in i) replicating previous findings regarding the distribution skewness of
lie-telling frequency and exploring how these patterns relate to self-reported deception ability;
ii) isolating lie characteristics as a function of deception ability; and iii) exploring the strategies
of deception used by self-reported good liars.
Lie prevalence and characteristics
We investigated how laypeople lie in daily life by examining the frequency of lies, types of lies,
receivers and mediums of deception within the past 24 hours. Overall, participants indicated
telling a mean of 1.61 lies during the last 24 hours (SD = 2.75; range: 0–20 lies), but the distri-
bution was non-normally distributed, with a skewness of 3.90 (SE = 0.18) and a kurtosis of
18.44 (SE = 0.35). The six most prolific liars, less than 1% of our participants, accounted for
38.5% of the lies told. Thirty-nine percent of our participants reported telling no lies. Fig 1 dis-
plays participants’ lie-telling prevalence.
Participants’ endorsement of the type, recipient, and medium of their lies are shown in Fig
2. Participants mostly reported telling white lies, to family members, and via face-to-face inter-
actions. All lie characteristics displayed non-normal distributions (see the Supporting Infor-
mation for the complete description).
Lie prevalence and characteristics as a function of deception ability. Next, we con-
ducted correlational analyses to examine the association of our participants’ lie frequency
and characteristics with their self-reported deception ability. An increase in self-reported
ability to deceive was positively correlated to a greater frequency of lies told per day, r(192)
= .22, p= .002, and with higher endorsement of telling white lies and exaggerations within
the last 24 hours (r(192) = .16, p= .023 and r(192) = .16, p= .027, respectively). There were
no significant associations between self-reported deception ability and reported use of
embedded lies, r(192) = .14, p= .051; lies of omission, r(192) = .10, p= .171; or lies of
Self-reported good liars
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 4 / 16
commission, r(192) = .10, p= .161. Higher self-reported deception ability was significantly
associated with telling lies to colleagues, r(192) = .27, p<.001, friends, r(192) = .16, p=
.026, and “other” receivers of deception, r(192) = .16, p= .031; however, there were no sig-
nificant associations between self-reported ability to lie and telling lies to family, employers,
or authority figures (r(192) = .08, p= .243; r(192) = .04, p= .558; and r(192) = .11, p= .133,
respectively). Finally, higher values for self-reported deception ability were positively corre-
lated to telling lies via face-to-face interactions, r(192) = .26, p<.001. All other mediums of
communicating the deception were not associated with a higher reported ability, as follows:
Via phone conversations, text messaging, social media, email, or “other” sources (r(192) =
.13, p= .075; r(192) = .13, p= .083; r(192) = .03, p= .664; r(192) = .05, p= .484; r(192) = .10,
p= .153, respectively).
Table 1. Endorsement of general qualitative deception strategies and descriptive statistics as a function of decep-
tion ability.
Interview Strategies N M SD X
2
Omitting certain information 76 0.39 0.49 χ
2
(2) = 3.00, p= .223, V= .124
Poor 25 0.49 0.51
Neutral 28 0.37 0.49
Good 23 0.34 0.48
Providing certain information 49 0.25 0.44 χ
2
(2) = 5.49, p= .064, V= .168
Poor 7 0.14 0.35
Neutral 20 0.27 0.45
Good 22 0.32 0.47
Relating to truthful information 49 0.25 0.44 χ
2
(2) = 5.02, p= .081, V= .161
Poor 7 0.14 0.35
Neutral 23 0.31 0.46
Good 19 0.28 0.45
Behavioural control 39 0.20 0.40 χ
2
(2) = 2.69, p= .260, V= .118
Poor 9 0.18 0.39
Neutral 12 0.16 0.37
Good 18 0.26 0.44
Miscellaneous strategies 44 0.23 0.42 χ
2
(2) = 1.29, p= .524, V= .082
Poor 9 0.18 0.39
Neutral 17 0.23 0.42
Good 18 0.26 0.44
No strategy 10 0.05 0.22 χ
2
(2) = 8.26, p= .016, V= .206
Poor 6 0.12 0.33
Neutral 4 0.05 0.23
Good 0 0 0
Not Applicable 15 0.08 0.27 χ
2
(2) = 1.23, p= .540, V= .080
Poor 4 0.08 0.27
Neutral 4 0.05 0.23
Good 7 0.10 0.31
Note. The Ncolumn represents the number of participants who endorsed each strategy, both in the total sample and
for Poor, Neutral and Good liars, respectively. The total number of endorsed strategies surpasses the sample size of
194 because each participant could report multiple strategies that may have fallen into more than one category. The
bolded numbers represent the group with the highest endorsement of each strategy.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566.t001
Self-reported good liars
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 5 / 16
Deception strategies of good liars
We were also interested in exploring the strategies of deception, particularly those of good
liars. To test this, we created categories representing participants’ self-reported deception abil-
ity, using their scores from the question asking about their ability to deceive successfully, as fol-
lows: Scores of three and below were combined into the category of “Poor liars” (n= 51);
scores of 4, 5, 6, and 7 were combined into the category of “Neutral liars” (n= 75); and scores
of eight and above were combined into the category of “Good liars” (n= 68).
Table 1 provides an overview of the exact values regarding the endorsement of each decep-
tion strategy that emerged from the qualitative coding. To examine whether there were associ-
ations between the reported strategies and varying deception abilities, we conducted a series of
chi square tests of independence on participants’ coded responses to the question regarding
their general strategies for deceiving. We did not observe any statistically significant associa-
tions between self-reported deception ability and the endorsement of any strategy categories
(see Table 1), apart from one exception. We observed a significant association between Poor,
Neutral and Good liars and the endorsement of using No strategy”. Pairwise comparisons
were performed using Dunn’s procedure [37] with a corrected alpha level of .025 for multiple
tests. This analysis revealed a significant difference in endorsing “No strategy” only between
the Good and Poor liars, p= .004. However, we did not meet the assumption of all expected
cell frequencies being equal to or greater than five and as such these data may be skewed.
Based on Cohen’s guidelines [38], all associations were small to moderate (all Cramer’s Vs <
.206).
Verbal and nonverbal strategies. To investigate whether participants differed in their
endorsement of the importance of verbal versus nonverbal strategies based on their self-
Fig 1. Scatterplot of participants’ self-reported lie-telling frequency during the past 24 hours. The distribution curve represents the mean
and standard deviation of the total sample. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566.g001
Self-reported good liars
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 6 / 16
Self-reported good liars
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 7 / 16
reported deception ability, we conducted two between-subjects ANOVAs with deception abil-
ity (Poor, Neutral, Good) on participants’ Likert scale ratings of the importance of verbal and
nonverbal strategies. Additionally, the data were examined by calculating Bayesian ANOVAs
with default prior scales, using JASP software. We report the Bayesian factors [BF; see 39,40]
in line with the guidelines by Jarosz and Wiley [39], adjusted from Jeffreys [41]. For ease of
interpretation, BF
10
is used to indicate the Bayes factor as evidence in favour of the alternative
hypothesis, whereas BF
01
is used to indicate the Bayes factor as evidence in favour of the null
hypothesis.
First, we found a significant effect of self-reported deception ability on participants’
endorsement of verbal strategies, F(2, 191) = 5.62, p= .004, η
P2
= .056; BF
10
= 7.11. Post hoc
comparisons indicated that Good liars rated verbal strategies as significantly more important
than Neutral liars (M
diff
= -0.82, 95% CI [-1.47, -0.18], p= .009), and Poor liars (M
diff
= -0.83,
95% CI [-1.54, -0.11], p= .018). Participants across groups did not differ with respect to their
endorsement of the importance of nonverbal strategies, F(2, 191) = .003, p= .997, η
P2
<.001;
BF
01
= 18.55.
Next, we examined which specific verbal strategies participants reported to use when lying.
We asked participants to indicate, from a list of ten options, which strategies they use. Table 2
provides an overview of the strategies endorsed by Poor, Neutral, and Good liars. Across all
groups, the most frequently reported strategies were Keeping the statement clear and simple
(endorsed by 17.6% of participants), Telling a plausible story” (15.1% of participants), “Using
avoidance/being vague about details” (13.2% of participants) and “Embedding the lie into an
otherwise truthful story” (13.1% of participants). To examine differences in the endorsement of
the strategies across Poor, Neutral, and Good liars we conducted a series of one-way between-
subjects ANOVAs. Significant differences emerged for eight of the strategies, as follows:
Embedding the lie, F(2, 191) = 11.97, p<.001, η
P2
= .111; BF
10
= 1438.20; “Matching the
amount of details in the deceptive component of the statement to the truthful component, F(2,
191) = 4.77, p= .010, η
P2
= .048; BF
10
= 3.32; “Matching the type of details of the deceptive com-
ponent of the statement to the truthful component, F(2, 191) = 3.56, p= .030, η
P2
= .036; BF
10
=
1.15; “Keeping the statement clear and simple, F(2, 191) = 5.07, p= .007, η
P2
= .050; BF
10
=
4.15; “Telling a plausible story, F(2, 191) = 5.48, p= .005, η
P2
= .054; BF
10
= 5.98; “Providing
unverifiable details, F(2, 191) = 4.95, p= .008, η
P2
= .049; BF
10
= 3.78, and Avoidance, F(2,
191) = 3.79, p= .024, η
P2
= .038; BF
10
= 1.43. Interestingly, Good liars reported using all of the
above strategies significantly more than Poor liars (all p’s <.025). The only exception was that
Poor liars reported using the avoidance strategy significantly more than Good liars (p= .026).
Finally, there were no significant differences between Good, Neutral, and Poor liars in endors-
ing “Reporting from previous experience/memory” (F(2, 191) = 1.32, p= .268, η
P2
= .014; BF
01
=
5.96), “Using complete fabrication” (F(2, 191) = 0.57, p= .565, η
P2
= .006; BF
01
= 11.36), and
“Using other strategies” (F(2, 191) = 0.51, p= .600, η
P2
= .005; BF
01
= 11.96). See Table 2 for the
exact values and applicable post hoc comparisons.
Exploratory testing of liar characteristics
Finally, we also explored the associations between sex and education level and laypeople’s self-
reported deception ability by conducting a series of chi square tests of association. We
Fig 2. Bar charts displaying the frequency of the types, receivers, and mediums of deception endorsed by
participants for their reported lies during the past 24 hours. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. For
deception recipients, “other” refers to individuals such as intimate partners or strangers; for deception mediums,
“other” refers to online platforms not included in the provided list.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566.g002
Self-reported good liars
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 8 / 16
observed a significant association between sex and deception ability, χ
2
(2) = 12.31, p= .002,
V= .253. Further examination revealed that, of those who self-reported to be Poor liars, 70%
Table 2. Endorsement of predetermined deception strategies and descriptive statistics as a function of deception ability.
Interview Strategies N M SD Bonferroni Comparisons
Poor Neutral
Keeping the statement clear and simple 112
Poor 20 0.39 0.49
Neutral 49 0.65 0.48 .010
Good 43 0.63 0.49 .025 1.00
Telling a plausible story 96
Poor 17 0.33 0.48
Neutral 36 0.48 0.50 .302
Good 43 0.63 0.49 .004 .195
Avoidance 84
Poor 28 0.55 0.50
Neutral 35 0.47 0.50 1.00
Good 21 0.31 0.47 .026 .167
Embedding the lie 83
Poor 13 0.26 0.44
Neutral 26 0.35 0.48 .850
Good 44 0.65 0.48 <.001 <.001
Providing unverifiable details 76
Poor 12 0.24 0.43
Neutral 29 0.39 0.49 .251
Good 35 0.52 0.50 .006 .338
Matching the type of details between lies and truths 71
Poor 12 0.24 0.43
Neutral 27 0.36 0.48 .453
Good 32 0.47 0.50 .025 .503
Reporting from previous experience 55
Poor 10 0.20 0.40
Neutral 23 0.31 0.46 .535
Good 22 0.32 0.47 .387 1.00
Matching the amount of details between lies and truths 38
Poor 5 0.10 0.30
Neutral 12 0.16 0.37 1.00
Good 21 0.31 0.47 .012 .072
Using complete fabrication 14
Poor 2 0.04 0.20
Neutral 6 0.08 0.27 1.00
Good 6 0.09 0.29 .930 1.00
Using other strategies 7
Poor 3 0.06 0.24
Neutral 2 0.03 0.16 1.00
Good 2 0.03 0.17 1.00 1.00
Note. The Nrepresents the number of participants who endorsed each strategy per group. Post hoc comparisons were conducted with the Bonferroni correction, and the
p-values are displayed in the table. The bolded numbers represent the significant cell comparisons.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566.t002
Self-reported good liars
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 9 / 16
(n= 35) were female compared to 30% (n= 15) male. Additionally, of those who identified
themselves as Good liars, 62.7% (n= 42) were male whereas 37.3% (n= 25) were female. Both
column proportions were significantly different at the .05 level. We did not observe a signifi-
cant association between participants’ education level and their self-reported deception ability,
χ
2
(4) = 9.09, p= .059, V= .153. The complete analyses are presented in the Supporting Infor-
mation to conserve manuscript length.
Discussion
We found that self-reported good liars i) may be responsible for a disproportionate amount of
lies in daily life, ii) tend to tell inconsequential lies, mostly to colleagues and friends, and gener-
ally via face-to-face interactions, and iii) highly rely on verbal strategies of deception, most
commonly reporting to embed their lies into truthful information, and to keep the statement
clear, simple and plausible.
Lie prevalence and characteristics
First, we replicated the finding that people lie, on average, once or twice per day, including its
skewed distribution. Nearly 40% of all lies were reported by a few prolific liars. Furthermore, higher
self-reported ratings of individuals’ deception ability were positively correlated with self-reports of:
i) telling a greater number of lies per day, ii) telling a higher frequency of white lies and exaggera-
tions, iii) telling the majority of lies to colleagues and friends or others such as romantic partners,
and iv) telling the majority of lies via face-to-face interactions. Importantly, skewed distributions
were also observed for the other lie characteristics, suggesting that it may be misleading to draw
conclusions from sample means, given that this does not reflect the lying behaviours of the average
person. A noteworthy finding is that prolific liars also considered themselves to be good liars.
The finding that individuals who consider themselves good liars report mostly telling
inconsequential lies is somewhat surprising. This deviates from the results of a previous study,
which showed that prolific liars reported telling significantly more serious lies, as well as more
inconsequential lies, compared to everyday liars [15]. However, small, white lies are generally
more common [18] and people who believe they can get away with such minor falsehoods
may be more inclined to include them frequently in daily interactions. It is also possible that
self-reported good liars in our sample had inflated perceptions of their own deception ability
because they tell only trivial lies versus lies of serious consequence.
Regarding the other lie characteristics, we found a positive correlation between self-
reported deception ability and telling lies to colleagues, friends and others (e.g., romantic part-
ners). This variation suggests that good liars are perhaps less restricted in who they lie to, rela-
tive to other liars who tell more lies to casual acquaintances and strangers than to family and
friends [22]. Our results also showed that good liars tended to prefer telling lies face-to-face.
This fits the findings of one of the only other studies to examine characteristics of self-reported
good versus poor liars, which found that self-perceived good liars most commonly lied via
face-to-face interactions versus through text chat [42]. This could be a strategic decision to
deceive someone to their face, since people may expect more deception via online environ-
ments [43]. As researchers continue to examine the nature of lying and to search for ways of
effectively detecting deception, it is important to recognize how certain lie characteristics may
influence individuals’ detectability as liars.
Deception strategies
We also isolated the lie-telling strategies of self-reported good liars. People who identified as
good liars placed a higher value on verbal strategies for successfully deceiving. Additional
Self-reported good liars
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 10 / 16
inspection of the verbal strategies reported by good liars showed that commonly reported
strategies were embedding lies into truthful information and keeping their statements clear,
simple and plausible. In fact, good liars were more likely than poor liars to endorse using these
strategies, as well as matching the amount and type of details in their lies to the truthful part/s
of their story, and providing unverifiable details. A common theme among these strategies is
the relation to truthful information. This fits with the findings of previous literature, that liars
typically aim to provide as much experienced information as possible, to the extent that they
do not incriminate themselves [35,44]. Additionally, good liars used plausibility as a strategy
for succeeding with their lies. This reflects the findings of the meta-analysis by Hartwig and
Bond [45] that implausibility is one of the most robust correlates of deception judgements,
and the results of DePaulo et al. [26] that one of the strongest cues to deception is liars’ ten-
dency to sound less plausible than truth tellers (d= -0.23).
We also found that self-reported poor liars were more likely than good liars to rely on the
avoidance strategy (i.e., being intentionally vague or avoiding mentioning certain details). Pre-
vious research suggests that this is one of the most common strategies used by guilty suspects
during investigative interviews [46]. Additionally, all liars in our study expressed behavioural
strategies as being important for deceiving successfully. This could be explained by the wide-
spread misconceptions about the associations between lying and behaviour, for example that
gaze aversion, increased movement or sweating are behaviours symptomatic of deception [2,
47].
There was inconsistency in our data between the responses to the qualitative strategy ques-
tion and the multiple-response strategy question. Based on the qualitative strategy data it
seems that Good, Neutral, and Poor liars do not differ in their use of strategies. However,
robust differences emerged when we evaluated participants’ endorsement of the predeter-
mined strategies. One explanation for this finding is the difficulty people perceive when they
have to verbalize the reasons for their behavior. Ericsson and Simon [48] suggest that inconsis-
tencies can occur especially when the question posed is too vague to elicit the appropriate
information, which might have been the case in our study. Another explanation for the dis-
crepancy in the data between the two measurement procedures is that data-driven coding is
inherently susceptible to human subjectivity, error, and bias [49,50]. Such limitations apply to
a lesser extent to coding based on predetermined categories that are derived from psychologi-
cal theory, an approach which has been heavily used within the deception literature [2]. In any
case, future research should continue exploring the deception strategies of good liars using a
variety of methodological approaches. In particular, it would be beneficial to establish more
reliable techniques for measuring interviewees’ processing regarding their deception strategies.
One potential idea could be to explore the effectiveness of using a series of cued questions to
encourage the recall of specific aspects of interviewees’ memory or cognitive processing.
Another suggestion is to combine the data-driven and theory-driven approaches, whereby the
coding system is generated inductively from the data but the coders draw from the theoretical
literature when identifying categories [50].
Limitations
Some methodological considerations should be addressed. First, the results of the present
study are drawn from participants’ self-reports about their patterns of deception in daily life.
Sources of error associated with such self-report data limit our ability to draw strong infer-
ences from this study. However, previous research has validated the use of self-report to mea-
sure lying prevalence by correlating self-reported lying with other measures of dishonesty [17].
Moreover, self-report data may not be as untrustworthy as critics argue, and in some
Self-reported good liars
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 11 / 16
situations, it may be the most appropriate methodology [51]. This study was intended as an
initial examination of the strategies and preferences of good liars, and surveying liars for their
own perspectives provided a novel source of insight into their behaviour. A constraint to the
generalizability of this research is that we did not establish the ground truth as to whether self-
reported good liars are indeed skilled deceivers. Future research could attempt to extend our
findings by examining deceivers’ lie frequency, characteristics, and strategies after systemati-
cally testing their lie-telling ability within a controlled laboratory setting.
Second, one of the most frequent concerns about using Amazon MTurk relates to low com-
pensation and resulting motivation [52,53]. We took measures to ensure that our remunera-
tion to participants was above the fair price for comparable experiments. Importantly, data
collected through MTurk produces equivalent results as data collected from online and student
samples [52,5458]. As well, mTurk surveys have been shown to produce a representative
sample of the United States population that yields results akin to those observed from more
expensive survey techniques, such as telephone surveys [57]. It speaks to the validity of our
data, for example, that the self-reported prevalence of lies, and the endorsement of nonverbal
deception strategies, replicates previous research. Nonetheless, the results of this study could
be advanced if future research i) directly replicates our survey amongst different populations,
for instance university students, and ii) conceptually replicates this research by evaluating dif-
ferent methodological approaches for measuring deception ability (e.g., via controlled evalua-
tion) and good liars’ strategies for deceiving (e.g., via cued recall).
Implications and future research
This study explored the deception characteristics and strategies used by self-reported good
liars. Deception researchers generally agree that the most diagnostic information is found in
the content of liars’ speech [59]. Content-based cues to deception, however, may be less effec-
tive for detecting good liars who rely highly on verbal strategies of deception. This could even
offer an explanation for the modest effect sizes observed in the deception literature [60]. For
instance, good liars in our study reported to strategically embed their lies into truthful infor-
mation. This finding has potential implications for the reliability of credibility assessment
tools that derive from the assumption that truth tellers’ statements are drawn from memory
traces whereas liars’ statements are fabricated from imagination [61,62]. If good liars draw on
their memory of truthful previous experiences, then their statements may closely resemble
those of their truth telling counterparts. Another interesting observation was that self-reported
good liars were more likely than poor liars to provide unverifiable details. This fits with the
findings of previous literature on the VA, which contends that liars provide information that
cannot be verified to balance their goals of being perceived as cooperative and of minimizing
the chances of falsification by investigators [32,33]. A fruitful avenue of future research could
be to further explore liars’ strategic inclusion of truthful information and unverifiable details.
Doing so may give lie detectors an advantage for unmasking skilled liars. It would also be
interesting for future research to examine how good versus poor liars are affected by certain
interview techniques designed to increase the difficulty of lying such as the reverse-order tech-
nique [63].
Conclusion
In sum, this study yields new insights into the deception prevalence, characteristics, and strate-
gies used by self-reported good liars. We replicated the finding that a minority of individuals
account for the majority of lies told in daily life, and we provide evidence that these prolific
liars also consider themselves good liars. We unveiled several lie characteristics of good liars:
Self-reported good liars
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 12 / 16
They lean towards telling inconsequential lies, mostly to colleagues and friends, and generally
via face-to-face interactions. Additionally, our results showed that self-reported good liars may
attempt to strategically manipulate their verbal behaviour to stay close to the truth and to tell a
plausible, simple, and clear story. This study provides a starting point for further research on
the meta-cognitions and patterns of skilled liars, who may be more likely to evade detection in
investigative settings.
Supporting information
S1 File. Questionnaire definitions: Deception and strategies.
(DOCX)
S2 File. Lie characteristics and distribution skewness.
(DOCX)
S3 File. Exploratory testing of liar characteristics.
(DOCX)
S4 File. Questionnaire part III: Recalling a serious lie.
(DOCX)
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Brianna L. Verigin, Ewout H. Meijer, Glynis Bogaard.
Data curation: Brianna L. Verigin.
Formal analysis: Brianna L. Verigin.
Funding acquisition: Brianna L. Verigin.
Investigation: Brianna L. Verigin.
Methodology: Brianna L. Verigin.
Project administration: Brianna L. Verigin.
Resources: Brianna L. Verigin.
Supervision: Ewout H. Meijer.
Visualization: Brianna L. Verigin.
Writing original draft: Brianna L. Verigin.
Writing review & editing: Brianna L. Verigin, Ewout H. Meijer, Glynis Bogaard, Aldert
Vrij.
References
1. Bond C, DePaulo B. Accuracy of Deception Judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Review.
2006; 10(3):214–234. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_2 PMID: 16859438
2. Vrij A. Detecting lies and deceit: Pitfalls and opportunities. 2nd ed. Hoboken, NJ; Chichester, West
Sussex, England: John Wiley & Sons; 2008 Feb 19.
3. Bond C, DePaulo B. Individual differences in judging deception: Accuracy and bias. Psychological Bul-
letin. 2008; 134(4):477–492. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.477 PMID: 18605814
4. Bond C, Kahler K, Paolicelli L. The miscommunication of deception: An adaptive perspective. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology. 1985; 21(4):331–345.
Self-reported good liars
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 13 / 16
5. Law MK, Jackson SA, Aidman E, Geiger M, Olderbak S, Kleitman S. It’s the deceiver, not the receiver:
No individual differences when detecting deception in a foreign and a native language. PloS One. 2018
May 3; 13(5):e0196384. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196384 PMID: 29723243
6. Levine TR. Examining sender and judge variability in honesty assessments and deception detection
accuracy: Evidence for a transparent liar but no evidence of deception-general ability. Communication
Research Reports. 2016 Jul 2; 33(3):188–94.
7. Levine TR, Serota KB, Shulman H, Clare DD, Park HS, Shaw AS, et al. Sender demeanor: Individual
differences in sender believability have a powerful impact on deception detection judgments. Human
Communication Research. 2011 Jul 1; 37(3):377–403.
8. DePaulo B, Rosenthal R. Telling lies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1979; 37
(10):1713–1722. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.37.10.1713 PMID: 512835
9. Riggio RE, Tucker J, Throckmorton B. Social skills and deception ability. Personality and Social Psy-
chology Bulletin. 1987 Dec; 13(4):568–77.
10. Vrij A, Granhag PA, Mann S. Good liars. The Journal of Psychiatry & Law. 2010 Mar; 38(1–2):77–98.
11. Wright GR, Berry CJ, Bird G. “You can’t kid a kidder”: association between production and detection of
deception in an interactive deception task. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2012 Apr 17; 6:87. https://
doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00087 PMID: 22529790
12. Wright GR, Berry CJ, Bird G. Deceptively simple. . . The “deception-general” ability and the need to put
the liar under the spotlight. Frontiers in neuroscience. 2013 Aug 29; 7:152. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.
2013.00152 PMID: 24009549
13. DePaulo B, Kashy D, Kirkendol S, Wyer M, Epstein J. Lying in everyday life. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology. 1996; 70(5):979–995. PMID: 8656340
14. George JF, Robb A. Deception and computer-mediated communication in daily life. Communication
Reports. 2008 Nov 14; 21(2):92–103.
15. Serota KB, Levine TR. A few prolific liars: Variation in the prevalence of lying. Journal of Language and
Social Psychology. 2015 Mar; 34(2):138–57.
16. Serota KB, Levine TR, Boster FJ. The prevalence of lying in America: Three studies of self-reported
lies. Human Communication Research. 2010 Jan 1; 36(1):2–25.
17. Halevy R, Shalvi S, Verschuere B. Being honest about dishonesty: Correlating self-reports and actual
lying. Human Communication Research. 2014 Jan 1; 40(1):54–72.
18. Feldman RS, Forrest JA, Happ BR. Self-presentation and verbal deception: Do self-presenters lie
more?. Basic and applied social psychology. 2002 Jun 1; 24(2):163–70.
19. Vrij A. Deception: A social lubricant and a selfish act. Social communication. 2007:309–42.
20. Craig D. The Right to Silence and Undercover Police Operations. International Journal of Police Sci-
ence & Management. 2003; 5(2):112–125.
21. Vrij A, Edward K, Roberts KP, Bull R. Detecting deceit via analysis of verbal and nonverbal behavior.
Journal of Nonverbal behavior. 2000 Dec 1; 24(4):239–63.
22. DePaulo B, Kashy D. Everyday lies in close and casual relationships. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology. 1998; 74(1):63–79. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.74.1.63 PMID: 9457776
23. Van Swol LM, Braun MT, Kolb MR. Deception, detection, demeanor, and truth bias in face-to-face and
computer-mediated communication. Communication Research. 2015 Dec; 42(8):1116–42.
24. Vrij A, Granhag PA, Porter S. Pitfalls and opportunities in nonverbal and verbal lie detection. Psycholog-
ical science in the public interest. 2010 Dec; 11(3):89–121. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100610390861
PMID: 26168416
25. DePaulo B. Nonverbal behavior and self-presentation. Psychological Bulletin. 1992; 111(2):203–243.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.111.2.203 PMID: 1557474
26. DePaulo B, Lindsay J, Malone B, Muhlenbruck L, Charlton K, Cooper H. Cues to deception. Psychologi-
cal Bulletin. 2003; 129(1):74–112. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.74 PMID: 12555795
27. Hartwig M, Granhag PA, Stro¨mwall LA. Guilty and innocent suspects’ strategies during police interroga-
tions. Psychology, Crime & Law. 2007 Apr 1; 13(2):213–27.
28. Colwell K, Hiscock-Anisman C, Memon A, Woods D, Michlik PM. Strategies of impression management
among deceivers and truth-tellers: How liars attempt to convince. American Journal of Forensic Psy-
chology. 2006(24):31–38.
29. Hartwig M, Granhag PA, Stro
¨mwall LA, Doering N. Impression and information management: On the
strategic self-regulation of innocent and guilty suspects. The Open Criminology Journal. 2010 Jul; 3
(1):10–6.
Self-reported good liars
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 14 / 16
30. Stro
¨mwall LA, Hartwig M, Granhag PA. To act truthfully: Nonverbal behaviour and strategies during a
police interrogation. Psychology, Crime & Law. 2006 Apr 1; 12(2):207–19.
31. Stro
¨mwall LA, Wille
´n RM. Inside criminal minds: Offenders’ strategies when lying. Journal of Investiga-
tive Psychology and Offender Profiling. 2011 Oct; 8(3):271–81.
32. Nahari G, Vrij A, Fisher RP. Exploiting liars’ verbal strategies by examining the verifiability of details.
Legal and Criminological Psychology. 2014 Sep; 19(2):227–39.
33. Nahari G, Vrij A, Fisher RP. The verifiability approach: Countermeasures facilitate its ability to discrimi-
nate between truths and lies. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2014 Jan; 28(1):122–8.
34. Nahari G, Ashkenazi T, Fisher RP, Granhag PA, Hershkowitz I, Masip J, et al. ‘Language of lies’: Urgent
issues and prospects in verbal lie detection research. Legal and Criminological Psychology. 2019 Feb;
24(1):1–23.
35. Leins DA, Fisher RP, Ross SJ. Exploring liars’ strategies for creating deceptive reports. Legal and Crim-
inological Psychology. 2013 Feb; 18(1):141–51.
36. Koo TK, Li MY. A guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability
research. Journal of chiropractic medicine. 2016 Jun 1; 15(2):155–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcm.
2016.02.012 PMID: 27330520
37. Dunn O. Multiple Comparisons Using Rank Sums. Technometrics. 1964; 6(3):241.
38. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd ed. New York: Psychology
Press; 1988.
39. Jarosz AF, Wiley J. What are the odds? A practical guide to computing and reporting Bayes factors.
The Journal of Problem Solving. 2014; 7(1):2–9.
40. Lee MD, Wagenmakers EJ. Bayesian cognitive modeling: A practical course. Cambridge university
press; 2014 Apr 3.
41. Jeffreys H. Theory of probability. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
42. Van Swol LM, Paik JE. Deciding how to deceive: differences in communication and detection between
good and bad liars. Communication Quarterly. 2017 Oct 20; 65(5):503–22.
43. Whitty MT, Carville SE. Would I lie to you? Self-serving lies and other-oriented lies told across different
media. Computers in Human Behavior. 2008 May 1; 24(3):1021–31.
44. Nahari G, Vrij A. Systematic errors (biases) in applying verbal lie detection tools: richness in detail as a
test case. Crime Psychology Review. 2015 Jan 1; 1(1):98–107.
45. Hartwig M, Bond CF Jr. Why do lie-catchers fail? A lens model meta-analysis of human lie judgments.
Psychological bulletin. 2011 Jul; 137(4):643. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023589 PMID: 21707129
46. Granhag PA, Hartwig M. A new theoretical perspective on deception detection: On the psychology of
instrumental mind-reading. Psychology, Crime & Law. 2008 Jun 1; 14(3):189–200.
47. Bogaard G, Meijer E, Vrij A, Merckelbach H. Strong, but Wrong: Lay People’s and Police Officers’
Beliefs about Verbal and Nonverbal Cues to Deception. Plos One. 2016; 11(6):e0156615. https://doi.
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0156615 PMID: 27258014
48. Ericsson K, Simon H. Verbal reports as data. Psychological Review. 1980; 87(3):215–251.
49. Morrissey ER. Sources of error in the coding of questionnaire data. Sociological Methods & Research.
1974 Nov; 3(2):209–32.
50. Syed M, Nelson SC. Guidelines for establishing reliability when coding narrative data. Emerging Adult-
hood. 2015 Dec; 3(6):375–87.
51. Chan D. So why ask me? Are self-report data really that bad. Statistical and methodological myths and
urban legends: Doctrine, verity and fable in the organizational and social sciences. 2009:309–36.
52. Landers RN, Behrend TS. An inconvenient truth: Arbitrary distinctions between organizational, Mechan-
ical Turk, and other convenience samples. Industrial and Organizational Psychology. 2015 Jun; 8
(2):142–64.
53. Chandler J, Shapiro D. Conducting clinical research using crowdsourced convenience samples. Annual
review of clinical psychology. 2016 Mar 28;12.
54. Casler K, Bickel L, Hackett E. Separate but equal? A comparison of participants and data gathered via
Amazon’s MTurk, social media, and face-to-face behavioral testing. Computers in Human Behavior.
2013; 29(6):2156–2160.
55. Feitosa J, Joseph D, Newman D. Crowdsourcing and personality measurement equivalence: A warning
about countries whose primary language is not English. Personality and Individual Differences. 2015;
75:47–52.
Self-reported good liars
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 15 / 16
56. Simons DJ, Chabris CF. Common (mis) beliefs about memory: A replication and comparison of tele-
phone and Mechanical Turk survey methods. PloS one. 2012 Dec 18; 7(12):e51876. https://doi.org/10.
1371/journal.pone.0051876 PMID: 23272183
57. Fleischer A, Mead AD, Huang J. Inattentive responding in MTurk and other online samples. Industrial
and Organizational Psychology. 2015 Jun; 8(2):196–202.
58. Crump MJ, McDonnell JV, Gureckis TM. Evaluating Amazon’s Mechanical Turk as a tool for experimen-
tal behavioral research. PloS one. 2013 Mar 13; 8(3):e57410. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.
0057410 PMID: 23516406
59. Bond CF Jr, DePaulo BM. Accuracy of deception judgments. Personality and social psychology Review.
2006 Aug; 10(3):214–34. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_2 PMID: 16859438
60. Luke TJ. Lessons from Pinocchio: Cues to deception may be highly exaggerated. Perspectives on Psy-
chological Science. 2019 Jul; 14(4):646–71. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619838258 PMID:
31173537
61. Johnson MK, Raye CL. Reality monitoring. Psychological review. 1981 Jan; 88(1):67.
62. Steller M, Ko
¨hnken G. Criteria-based content analysis. In: Raskin DCed. Psychological methods in
criminal investigation and evidence. New York: Spring Publishing Company. P. 217–45.
63. Evans JR, Michael SW, Meissner CA, Brandon SE. Validating a new assessment method for deception
detection: Introducing a Psychologically Based Credibility Assessment Tool. Journal of Applied
Research in Memory and Cognition. 2013 Mar 1; 2(1):33–41.
Self-reported good liars
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225566 December 3, 2019 16 / 16
... The demographic questionnaire obtained information regarding sex, age, grade level, and batch (year) of the students. Additionally, to increase the accuracy of the data, there were four attention and honesty check items included based on scientific recommendations [147,148]. The first item, "Please choose 'agree' for this question", was used to test whether the subject was careless or inattentive. ...
... The first item, "Please choose 'agree' for this question", was used to test whether the subject was careless or inattentive. If the subject did not choose the fixed response of "agree", their data was excluded [147][148][149]. The next three items which were rated on a 4-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree)-"I answered all the questions truthfully", "I never lied", and "I never hid myself"-were intended to assess the honesty and truthfulness of the subject's responses. ...
... The next three items which were rated on a 4-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree)-"I answered all the questions truthfully", "I never lied", and "I never hid myself"-were intended to assess the honesty and truthfulness of the subject's responses. If a person scored low (≤2 on the 4-point Likert scale) on any of these three items, their data was also excluded, as this would suggest a tendency toward self-reported deception [147][148][149]. By including these validity checks, the researchers aimed to ensure the truthfulness and reliability of the self-reported data collected through the demographic questionnaire. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Social media addiction (SMA) and internet addiction (IA) are increasingly prevalent, impacting mental health (MH) globally. This study investigates the mediating roles of mindfulness and social capital (SC) in the relationship between SMA, IA, and MH among Ethiopian high school and university students, contributing to the Sustainable Development Goal (SDG) 3 of good health and well-being. Methods: A cross-sectional study was conducted with 1160 university and 1473 high school students in Dessie, Ethiopia. Participants completed validated questionnaires assessing SMA, IA, mindfulness, SC, and MH. Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) with a multi- mediation Model (SMM) was used to examine the hypothesized relationships. Results: SEM revealed that both SMA and IA had a direct negative effect on mindfulness, SC, and MH in in both high school and university students. Notably, mindfulness and SC significantly and positively predicted MH, indicating their protective role against the negative effects of SMA and IA. Furthermore, both mindfulness or SC fully or partially mediated the relationship between SMA, IA, and MH, highlighting their crucial role in explaining the association. Conclusions: This study provides evidence for the crucial roles of mindfulness and SC in buffering the negative effects of SMA and IA on MH among Ethiopian students. The findings highlight the need for educational and health interventions that foster mindfulness and SC to enhance student mental health and promote a healthy digital environment. These results offer valuable insights for educators, health professionals, and policymakers in Ethiopia and other developing countries facing similar challenges.
... Los sospechosos que mienten gestionan estratégicamente la información, optando por una estrategia de evitación o una de escape/negación. La estrategia de evitación se caracteriza por tácticas evasivas, como la ambigüedad deliberada o la omisión de detalles específicos(Verigin et al., 2019). Por ejemplo, un sospechoso podría omitir deliberadamente mencionar su presencia en un lugar específico en un momento determinado, cuando se le solicita un relato libre y espontáneo en respuesta a una pregunta abierta. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Esta guía de entrevista de investigación está diseñada para ayudar a los investigadores a realizar entrevistas efectivas. Cubre una amplia gama de temas, que incluyen establecer una buena relación, hacer preguntas productivas y evitar errores en la identificación de testigos. La guía también analiza la importancia de considerar los factores que pueden afectar la memoria, como el estrés, la intoxicación y el paso del tiempo. Proporciona orientación sobre cómo utilizar las pruebas de forma estratégica durante las entrevistas para obtener información y detectar el engaño. La guía enfatiza la importancia de usar técnicas de entrevista basadas en la ciencia para obtener información precisa y confiable.
... Additionally, the dataset under analysis was designed to collect outright false statements. However, a more frequent and ecological form of deception is constituted by embedded lies (Caso et al., 2023;Verigin et al., 2019), where people interwire truth and lies together. As a consequence, the detection rates found in our series of studies may be even lower when considering this type of deception. ...
Article
Detecting deception in interpersonal communication is a pivotal issue in social psychology, with significant implications for court and criminal proceedings. In this study, four experiments were designed to compare the performance of natural language processing (NLP) techniques and human judges in detecting deception from linguistic cues in a dataset of 62 transcriptions of video-taped interviews (32 genuine and 30 deceptive). The results showed that machine-learning algorithms significantly outperform naïve (accuracy = 54.7%) and expert judges (accuracy = 59.4%) when trained on features from the reality monitoring (RM) and cognitive load frameworks (accuracy = 69.4%) or on features automatically extracted through NLP techniques (accuracy = 77.3%) but not when trained on the RM criteria alone. This evidence suggests that NLP algorithms, due to their ability to handle complex patterns of linguistic data, might be useful for better disentangling truthful from deceptive narratives, outperforming traditional theoretical models.
... Everyone engages in some form of deception daily [1]. Rather than fabricating entirely false accounts, however, most individuals tend to combine elements of truth with elements of falsehood [2]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background: Verbal deception detection research relies on narratives and commonly assumes statements as truthful or deceptive. A more realistic perspective acknowledges that the veracity of statements exists on a continuum with truthful and deceptive parts being embedded within the same statement. However, research on embedded lies has been lagging behind. Methods: We collected a novel dataset of 2,088 truthful and deceptive statements with annotated embedded lies. Using a within-subjects design, participants provided a truthful account of an autobiographical event. They then rewrote their statement in a deceptive manner by including embedded lies, which they highlighted afterwards and judged on lie centrality, deceptiveness, and source. Results: We show that a fined-tuned language model (Llama-3-8B) can classify truthful statements and those containing embedded lies with 64% accuracy. Individual differences, linguistic properties and explainability analysis suggest that the challenge of moving the dial towards embedded lies stems from their resemblance to truthful statements. Typical deceptive statements consisted of 2/3 truthful information and 1/3 embedded lies, largely derived from past personal experiences and with minimal linguistic differences with their truthful counterparts. Conclusion: We present this dataset as a novel resource to address this challenge and foster research on embedded lies in verbal deception detection.
... However, studies investigating the link between ToM and Lying -the intentional attempt at instilling a false belief in others (Sip et al., 2012) -is a prevalent phenomenon carrying potentially important consequences. Interestingly, evidence suggests that the successful detection of a lying attempt depends more on the ability of the liar, than on the performance of the lie detector (Bond Jr & DePaulo, 2008;Levine et al., 2011;Verigin et al., 2019). However, with most of the deception literature focused on deception detection (Masip, 2017;Sternglanz et al., 2019;Viji et al., 2022), the factors contributing to one's ability to lie remain unclear. ...
Article
Full-text available
While a large part of the deception literature focuses on lying detection, the factors contributing to one’s ability to lie remain unclear. The present study examined the contribution of Theory of Mind (ToM) and interoception on our ability to lie using a directed lie paradigm with two conditions (“Interrogation” and “Polygraph”), designed to enhance each of the two mechanisms. Given the relatively small sample size (n = 26 × 40 trials), special steps were taken to avoid false positives. Our results suggest that various facets of interoceptive abilities are positively related to the self-rated confidence in one’s own lies, especially when under the belief that bodily signals are being monitored (i.e., in the “Polygraph” condition). Beyond providing evidence for the role of the body in lying and raising interesting questions for deception science, these results carry practical implications for criminology and lie detection protocols.
... According to the RM approach (Johnson & Raye, 1981), experienced events contain more perceptual and contextual (spatial and temporal) information than imagined events. Truth tellers report about experienced events, whereas lie tellers report at least partially imagined events (Leins et al., 2013;Verigin et al., 2019). Hence, truth tellers' accounts should be richer in details than lie tellers' accounts (Amado et al., 2016;Bogaard, Colwell, et al., 2019;DePaulo et al., 2003;Sporer & Sharman, 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
Lie detection research comparing manual and automated coding of linguistic cues is limited. In Experiment 1, we attempted to extend this line of research by directly comparing the veracity differences in manual coding and two coding software programs (Text Inspector and Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count [LIWC]) on the linguistic cue “total details” across eight published datasets. Mixed model analyses revealed that LIWC showed larger veracity differences in total details than Text Inspector and manual coding. Follow-up classification analyses showed that both automated coding and manual coding could accurately classify honest and false accounts. In Experiment 2, we examined if LIWC’s sensitivity to veracity differences was the result of honest accounts including more redundant (repeated) words than false accounts as LIWC—but not Text Inspector or manual coding—accounts for redundancy. Our prediction was supported, and the most redundant words were function words. The results implicated that automated coding can detect veracity differences in total details and redundancy, but it is not necessarily better than manual coding at accurately classifying honest and false accounts.
... Lying is an intrinsic feature of human behavior [1]. We all lie and we have all been lied to [2][3][4]. When people are asked to discriminate between truth and lie based on their perceptions, they correctly notice lies in about 47% of cases and classify truths as nondeceptive in about 61% of cases-which is close to chance level [5,6]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Deception, a complex aspect of human behavior, is inherently difficult to detect directly. A valid alternative involves memory detection, particularly through methods such as the Reaction-Time based Concealed Information Test (RT-CIT). The RT-CIT assesses whether an individual possesses specific knowledge by presenting various probe (familiar) items amidst irrelevant (unfamiliar) items. The task-required "unfamiliar" response to probes may induce a response conflict. Resolving this conflict, by inhibiting the automatic "familiar" response, takes time and slows probe RTs–a phenomenon known as the RT-CIT effect. Notably, secondary psychopathy is characterized by disinhibition and impulsivity, traits which may hinder the ability to effectively manage experienced conflict. Therefore, we hypothesized that secondary psychopathy would be associated with an elevated RT-CIT effect. To investigate this hypothesized relation, we conducted a pre-registered study (n = 86, student sample), employing a novel CIT paradigm that incorporates no-go trials to assess response inhibition capacity. Psychopathic traits were measured using the Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy (LSRP) scale, while the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) assessed impulsivity. The novel CIT paradigm revealed impressive detection efficiency. However, contrary to our expectations, we observed no significant correlation between the RT-CIT effect and secondary psychopathic traits (BF01 = 6.98). This cautiously suggests that while secondary psychopathic tendencies do not improve RT-CIT validity, they also do not compromise it. Although future investigations should explore more diverse contexts and populations, this tentative finding is reassuring and underscores the robustness of the CIT paradigm.
Article
Deception is a multi-faceted social behaviour that is pervasive in human communication. Due to differences in social communication and experiences, autistic and non-autistic adults may contrast in how they respond to situations that elicit deceptive decision-making. This study examined whether autistic and non-autistic adults differed in their general lie frequency, their inclination to produce different lie types, and their emotional experiences of lying. Fifty-eight non-autistic and fifty-six autistic university students matched on age and gender completed self-report measures of their general lying patterns, how often they lied in the past 24 hours, and whether they would lie across hypothetical scenarios with differing beneficiaries (self, other, group) and motivations (protective, beneficial). The groups did not significantly differ in their general lying behaviour or frequency of lies told over 24 hours. Yet, autistic adults indicated that they would be significantly less likely to lie in group scenarios and would experience increased difficulty, more guilt, and greater concerns about their believability when lying. These results advance theoretical understanding by suggesting that autistic adults’ deceptive decision-making may be context-dependent. Future research may benefit from examining autistic deception across numerous social situations as more general lie frequency measures may be insensitive to nuanced population differences. Lay Abstract Lying, a universal social behaviour, is frequent in everyday communication. Due to differences in social communication and experiences, autistic and non-autistic adults may react differently in situations where they must decide whether to lie or tell the truth. We investigated whether autistic and non-autistic adults differ in their general lying behaviour (e.g. how often they lie) and their likelihood of lying in a range of hypothetical social scenarios with different motivations (why people lie – to benefit or protect) and orientations (who people lie for; themselves, other, a group). We also examined participants’ emotional experiences of lying and truth-telling. We found that autistic and non-autistic adults’ general lying frequencies and emotional experiences were similar. However, the social scenario responses revealed that autistic adults would be less likely to lie to benefit or protect a social group they are part of. Moreover, autistic adults indicated that they would find lying more difficult across all social scenarios, experience more guilt, and would be less confident that their lie would be believed. This research highlights how autistic adults’ lying may be context-dependent and considers how a reduction in the likelihood of lying for their social group could increase strain on autistic adults’ social relationships.
Article
Latar Belakang: Sebagian daerah pedalaman sering kali menghadapi tantangan tersendiri dalam hal akses dan kualitas pelayanan kefarmasian, peran apoteker maupun tenaga kefarmasian di Puskesmas masih menjadi pertanyaan. Berdasarkan hal tersebut, peneliti sangat tertarik untuk mengetahui bagaimana penerapan pelayanan kefarmasian di Puskesmas di tempat asal peneliti yaitu Puskesmas di Kecamatan Parenggean.Tujuan: Penelitian ini bertujuan untuk Mengobservasi secara mendalam mengenai penerapan standar pelayanan kefarmasian di Puskesmas Parenggean I dan Puskesmas Parenggean II berdasarkan Permenkes RI nomor 74 tahun 2016 dan Permenkes RI nomor 26 tahun 2020Metode: Metode yang digunakan yaitu observasi dan wawancara mendalam yang dilakukan secara langsung kepada Apoteker dengan penyerahan lembar observasi yang berisi pertanyaan untuk mendapatkan informasi mengenai Pengelolaan Sediaan Farmasi dan Bahan Medis habis Pakai, Pelayanan Farmasi Klinik, Sumber Daya Manusia dan Sarana Prasarana.Hasil: Terdapat dua Puskesmas yang berada di Kecamatan Parenggean. Semua puskesmas telah menerapkan Pengelolaan Sediaan Farmasi dan Bahan Medis Habis Pakai sesuai dengan Permenkes nomor 74 tahun 2016, namun dalam aspek Pelayanan Farmasi Kinik belum sepenuhnya dilakukan seperti visite pasien karena di dua Puskesmas tersebut tidak memiliki ruang rawat inap.Simpulan: Pelayanan kefarmasian di Puskesmas Parenggean I mencapai 96% dan Puskesmas Parenggean II mencapai 100% sesuai dengan Permenkes nomor 74 tahun 2016 dan Permenkes nomor 26 tahun 2020. Nilai tersebut menunjukkan bahwa secara keseluruhan kedua Puskesmas telah sesuai standar dengan kategori baik dalam melakukan pelayanan kefarmasian di wilayah kerjanya
Article
Full-text available
Deception researchers widely acknowledge that cues to deception-observable behaviors that may differ between truthful and deceptive messages-tend to be weak. Nevertheless, several deception cues have been reported with unusually large effect sizes, and some researchers have advocated the use of such cues as tools for detecting deceit and assessing credibility in practical contexts. By examining data from empirical deception-cue research and using a series of Monte Carlo simulations, I demonstrate that many estimated effect sizes of deception cues may be greatly inflated by publication bias, small numbers of estimates, and low power. Indeed, simulations indicate the informational value of the present deception literature is quite low, such that it is not possible to determine whether any given effect is real or a false positive. I warn against the hazards of relying on potentially illusory cues to deception and offer some recommendations for improving the state of the science of deception.
Article
Full-text available
Men and women (20 each) were videotaped while describing someone they liked, someone they disliked, someone they were ambivalent about, someone they were indifferent about, someone they liked as though they disliked him or her, and someone they disliked as though they liked him or her. Accuracy at detecting that some deception had occurred was far greater than accuracy at detecting the true underlying affect, and Ss who were good at detecting that deception was occurring were not particularly skilled at reading the speakers' underlying affects. However, Ss whose deception attempts were more easily detected by others also had their underlying affects read more easily. Speakers whose lies were seen more readily by men also had their lies seen more readily by women, and observers better able to see the underlying affects of women were better able to see the underlying affects of men. Skill at lying successfully was unrelated to skill at catching others in their lies. A histrionic strategy (hamming) was very effective in deceiving others, and this strategy was employed more by more Machiavellian Ss, who also tended to get caught less often in their lies. (32 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2006 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Individual differences in lie detection remain poorly understood. Bond and DePaulo’s meta-analysis examined judges (receivers) who were ascertaining lies from truths and senders (deceiver) who told these lies and truths. Bond and DePaulo found that the accuracy of detecting deception depended more on the characteristics of senders rather than the judges’ ability to detect lies/truths. However, for many studies in this meta-analysis, judges could hear and understand senders. This made language comprehension a potential confound. This paper presents the results of two studies. Extending previous work, in Study 1, we removed language comprehension as a potential confound by having English-speakers (N = 126, mean age = 19.86) judge the veracity of German speakers (n = 12) in a lie detection task. The twelve lie-detection stimuli included emotional and non-emotional content, and were presented in three modalities–audio only, video only, and audio and video together. The intelligence (General, Auditory, Emotional) and personality (Dark Triads and Big 6) of participants was also assessed. In Study 2, a native German-speaking sample (N = 117, mean age = 29.10) were also tested on a similar lie detection task to provide a control condition. Despite significantly extending research design and the selection of constructs employed to capture individual differences, both studies replicated Bond and DePaulo’s findings. The results of Study1 indicated that removing language comprehension did not amplify individual differences in judge’s ability to ascertain lies from truths. Study 2 replicated these results confirming a lack of individual differences in judge’s ability to detect lies. The results of both studies suggest that Sender (deceiver) characteristics exerted a stronger influence on the outcomes of lie detection than the judge’s attributes.
Article
Full-text available
The present study investigated the beliefs of students and police officers about cues to deception. A total of 95 police officers and 104 undergraduate students filled out a questionnaire addressing beliefs about cues to deception. Twenty-eight verbal cues were included in the questionnaire, all extracted from verbal credibility assessment tools (i.e., CBCA, RM, and SCAN). We investigated to what extent beliefs about nonverbal and verbal cues of deception differed between lay people (students) and police officers, and whether these beliefs were in agreement with objective cues known from research. Both students and police officers believed the usual stereotypical, but non-diagnostic (nonverbal) cues such as gaze aversion and increased movement to be indicative of deception. Yet, participants were less inclined to overestimate the relationship between verbal cues and deception and their beliefs fitted better with what we know from research. The implications of these findings for practice are discussed.
Article
To study deception, participants were randomly assigned the role of allocator or recipient in an ultimatum negotiation game. Allocators “earned” 7 dollars and divided the money between themselves and recipient and communicated the decision either face-to-face or through text chat. Recipients were unaware the amount the allocator had, and therefore, allocators could deceive. Most allocators used deception. We hypothesized that participants who self-identified as good liars would communicate more face-to-face than through text chat when deceiving, and this was supported for deceptive omission but not fabrications. Good liars were more likely to have their truths correctly detected than bad liars.
Chapter
People are generally poor at detecting deceit when observing someone’s behaviour or listening to their speech. In this chapter I will discuss the major factors (pitfalls) that lead to failures in catching liars: the sixteen reasons I will present are clustered into three categories: (i) a lack of motivation to detect lies; (ii) difficulties associated with lie detection; and (iii) common errors made by lie detectors. Discussing pitfalls provides insight into how lie detectors can improve their performance (for example, by recognising common biases and avoiding common judgment errors). The second section of this chapter discusses 11 ways (opportunities) to improve lie detection skills. Within this section, I first provide five recommendations for avoiding common errors in detecting lies. Next, I discuss recent lie detection research that introduces novel interview styles aimed at eliciting and enhancing verbal and nonverbal differences between liars and truth tellers. The recommendations are relevant in various settings, from the individual level (e.g., “Is my partner really working late?”) to the societal level (e.g., “Can we trust this suspect when he claims that he is not the serial rapist the police are searching for?”).
Article
A round-robin experiment unpacked message veracity, sender believability (demeanor), judge truth-bias, sender transparency, and judge deception detection accuracy. Generally, more variance was observed in senders than in judges. The data were suggestive of the existence of an unusually transparent liar, but the data were not consistent with a deception-general ability. The results highlight the importance of considering variability in addition to central tendency and the importance of individual differences in senders in deception detection.