ArticlePDF Available

Symmetry and Balance as Factors of Aesthetic Appreciation: Ethel Puffer’s (1903) “Studies in Symmetry” Revised

MDPI
Symmetry
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Symmetry and balance are basic concepts in art theory for the composition of pictures. It is assumed that well-balanced pictures are preferred to unbalanced ones. One of the first experimental studies to test this assumption was conducted more than a century ago by Ethel Puffer. By applying a production method, she found little evidence for the hypothesis that balance is favorable for the aesthetical appreciation of pictures. Instead, she observed that other construction principles competing with balance, such as bilateral symmetry and closeness, were applied. The aim of the present study was to repeat some of Puffer’s experiments with modern methods and to examine whether her results are replicable. In two experiments, we also found little to no evidence for balance. Moreover, as in Puffer’s study, participants used closeness and bilateral symmetry as principles. However, compared to that study, the relative frequency of use of these principles was quite different.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468; doi:10.3390/sym11121468 www.mdpi.com/journal/symmetry
Article
Symmetry and Balance as Factors of Aesthetic
Appreciation: Ethel Puffer’s (1903) “Studies
in Symmetry” Revised
Ronald Hübner * and Katja Thömmes
Department of Psychology, Universität Konstanz, 78464 Konstanz, Germany;
katja.thoemmes@uni-konstanz.de
*
Correspondence: ronald.huebner@uni-konstanz.de
Received:16 November 2019; Accepted: 30 November 2019; Published: 2 December 2019
Abstract: Symmetry and balance are basic concepts in art theory for the composition of pictures. It
is assumed that well-balanced pictures are preferred to unbalanced ones. One of the first
experimental studies to test this assumption was conducted more than a century ago by Ethel Puffer.
By applying a production method, she found little evidence for the hypothesis that balance is
favorable for the aesthetical appreciation of pictures. Instead, she observed that other construction
principles competing with balance, such as bilateral symmetry and closeness, were applied. The aim
of the present study was to repeat some of Puffer’s experiments with modern methods and to
examine whether her results are replicable. In two experiments, we also found little to no evidence
for balance. Moreover, as in Puffer’s study, participants used closeness and bilateral symmetry as
principles. However, compared to that study, the relative frequency of use of these principles was
quite different.
Keywords: empirical aesthetics; preference; symmetry; balance; composition
1. Introduction
Pictorial composition is one of the most important concepts in art and design [1–5]. It concerns
the harmonious arrangement of components of an artwork into a unified whole. Of the various
principles of composition, geometric or static bilateral symmetry is one of the simplest ones, i.e., the
exact duplication of elements about the medial vertical axis. Pictures and designs with this property
are usually pleasing to look at [6]. However, also non-symmetric pictures can be aesthetically
appealing. Therefore, in order to maintain symmetry as a general principle of good composition, it
has been assumed that such pictures often have some hidden symmetry, i.e., are symmetric with
respect to the perceptual weights of the elements in a picture. This kind of symmetry has been called
aesthetic symmetry [7], or dynamic symmetry [8] and is closely related to the concept of perceptual
balance [9].
In his influential book on design, Ross [1] provided a rather general definition of balance by
stating that a balanced arrangement of elements in a picture “… is a Harmony of Positions due to the
coincidence of two centers, the center of the attractions and the center of the framing” (p. 24).
Obviously, this definition treats both horizontal and vertical balance equally. However, such a two-
dimensional concept of balance, where both dimensions are combined and treated equally, has been
investigated empirically only recently [10–16]. The first empirical studies on balance were mainly
concerned with the horizontal arrangement of elements in a picture, although some researchers also
examined vertical balance [17].
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 2 of 18
With respect to horizontal balance, it has been assumed that a picture is liked, if the perceptual
weights on one side of a picture counterbalance those on the other side. Thus, the elements in a picture
are considered as weights on a beam scale, or seesaw. This mechanical metaphor has often been
applied to investigate how balanced a picture is. McManus et al. [18], for instance, asked their
participants to position a fulcrum underneath a picture so that the perceptual weights in the picture
were horizontally balanced. If a picture is well balanced towards the midline, then the fulcrum should
be placed underneath the center of the frame. McManus et al. found, however, that for many pictures
the balance point was slightly left of center.
This example demonstrates that mechanical balance can be a helpful metaphor for investigating
perceptual balance. However, its application is based on several assumptions. First of all, it has to be
assumed that persons attribute weights to each element in a picture. This is not implausible, because
by lifting real objects in our everyday activities, we steadily associate their weight with certain object
features such as color, brightness, or other properties. Nevertheless, measuring the perceptual
weights of picture elements is not straightforward, unless one makes simplifying assumptions. For
instance, if one believes that brightness plays a crucial role for the perceptual weight of an object,
then a simple approach is to assume that dark elements are heavier than bright ones, and, given an
equal brightness, that large elements are heavier than small ones. Accordingly, the weight of an object
can be computed by a simple gray-value integration method, i.e., by integrating the gray values over
the object’s area [e.g., 14]. This type of perceptual weight has to be distinguished from weights based
on higher level properties such as perceptual salience, which is related to visual attention [19,20].
However, irrespective of how weight is determined, we further have to assume that persons are able
to perceive the common center of ‘mass’ of all the perceptual element weights in a picture. At least
for simple configurations, it has been shown that this assumption is justified [21,22].
Early experimental research on perceptual balance [7,23] tested the mechanical metaphor and
found more or less support. Although the methods and procedures applied in these studies were
state of the art at that time, from our perspective, they are improvable. Moreover, they were rather
effortful, and therefore, only a few participants were observed. Therefore, the aim of the present
study was to repeat some of the early experiments with modern methods and more participants to
examine to what extent the early results are replicable.
As far as we know, the first experimental study on perceptual balance was conducted by Edgar
Pierce [7] during his time as a graduate student in Hugo Münsterberg’s lab at Harvard University.
Pierce was concerned with the question: “Can a feeling of symmetry, that is, of aesthetical equality
of the two halves, remain when the two sides are not geometrically identical, and if so, what are the
conditions under which this can result—what variations of one side seem aesthetically equal to the
variations of the other side?“ (p. 484).
For his experiments, he used a 100 × 100 cm board covered with a black rubber surface. It had
two invisible slits from one side to the other, which were used to hold and move objects by means of
a slider on the backside. In one of the simpler arrangements, the physical midline was marked with
a permanent 20 cm long white line. A 10 cm long vertical line was positioned on the right half of the
display at a fixed distance of 20 cm from the midline, while there were movable objects such as a star,
a square, or a vertical line on the left side, respectively. The experimenter could slide the movable
object alternately towards and from the midline and the participants were asked to say “stop” when
it reached the most “agreeable” position. After this decision, the participants were asked whether the
composition gave any aesthetical feeling of symmetry or balance.
As a result, some participants made adjustments in accord with mechanical balance. For
instance, a variable short line was stopped at a greater distance from the center than a long vertical
line, whereas others ignored the size differences and simply placed the variable element at the
laterally symmetric location. Further persons chose locations closely related to the golden section.
Despite noting individual differences, Pierce [7] derived some general relations from his results.
For instance, he concludes that a short line must be farther out than a long one, a thin line farther out
than a bold line, etc. Thus, by and large, Pierce’s results can be interpreted as supporting the concept
of mechanical balance.
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 3 of 18
A few years later, Ethel Puffer [23], who was also a graduate student in Hugo Münsterberg’s
Harvard Psychology Lab, criticized Pierce’s [7] study for several reasons. First, by explicitly asking
his participants whether the adjusted configuration was balanced, Pierce excluded any other
possibility than that of mechanical balance. Second, by permanently showing a line at the center, he
strongly accentuated the midline and divided the frame into two clearly visible halves. Finally, by
focusing on averaged data, performance was not represented adequately, given the large individual
differences.
In her own study on symmetry and balance, Puffer [23] used arrangements of lines and other
figures that were attached to a rectangular black surface (60 × 40 cm) with a gray frame (see Figure
1). There was a fixed element F on one side (left or right) of the surface, and a variable element V on
the other side. F was placed successively at different distances from the center, respectively. In her
various experiments, Puffer used elements of different size (short, long, or double lines), elements
that suggested movement (oblique or curved lines), elements that arouse what she called “intrinsic
interest”, i.e., attracted attention, and elements with perspective depth. In all cases, Puffer’s nine
participants were asked to horizontally move the variable element V, which was always a vertical
long or short line, until they found a position that made the arrangement most aesthetically pleasing.
Finding such a position of V was the result of a long series of trials, so that each participant made
only about ten choices per hour. They were never asked whether the resulting arrangement was
balanced.
Figure 1. Three examples of arrangements applied in Puffer’s [23] study. The black surface had an
extension of 60 × 40 cm. The width of the elements was 1 cm and their length 16 or 8 cm, respectively.
Overall, Puffer’s results provided little support for mechanical balance as a design principle. As
in Pierce’s study, there were large individual differences, which is why she did not compute averages
but discussed her results on an individual level. Some persons behaved in accord with mechanical
balance, at least in some conditions, whereas others even placed V close to F on the same side, which
is incompatible with mechanical balance.
The preferred positions also depended on the element type. Preference for mechanical balance
occurred most often when F was oblique (Exp. III, see Figure 2). For lines of different sizes (Exp. I (a)
and I (b), see Figure 2), in some cases there was even a reversal of mechanical balance. For instance,
with a short fixed line near the center, a larger movable line was positioned at a greater distance. For
double lines (Exp. II, see Figure 2), reversals were even more pronounced. However, they only
occurred when mechanical balance would have brought both elements near the center of the frame.
From the comments of her participants, Puffer concluded that they considered the whole picture and
often tried to avoid a large black space. In any case, even if mechanical balance was reversed, the
participants had a feeling of equilibrium.
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 4 of 18
Figure 2. Example displays for all main conditions (combinations of F and V) used in Experiment 1,
based on Puffer’s (1903) original study (Exp. I–III). The shapes of the lines are simplified.
In addition to the preference for mechanical balance and its reversal, there were also other
preferred positions. Some of them were close to the center, while others approached bilateral
symmetry. The latter, however, did not occur for double lines.
Taken together, Puffer’s results do not support mechanical balance as an important principle of
aesthetic composition. Nevertheless, they have been interpreted as compatible with this metaphor
[e.g., 5,24]. Since the exact role of mechanical balance for composition is still under dispute, the
purpose of the current study was to repeat some of Puffer’s pioneering experiments with modern
methods and a larger sample of participants.
2. Experiment 1
In our first experiment we wanted to repeat Puffer’s [23] experiments I to III and examine to
what extent the results are replicable. Of the considered elements, one was fixed on one side at one
out of five positions. As variable element V served a short or a long line on the opposite side,
respectively. The task of the participants was to place V at a position so that the configuration was
aesthetically pleasing. Figure 2 shows examples of the corresponding main conditions.
In view of the results of Pierce [7] and Puffer [23], we derived several hypotheses. The first one
was that the concept of mechanical balance plays an important role in placing the variable element.
Curiously, for some of Puffer’s conditions, F can only be balanced when V is moved to a position
outside the frame. The second hypothesis concerned lateral symmetry. If we consider the distance
between the midline and the location of F, then V should be placed opposite at that distance. Finally,
inspired by Puffer’s results, we hypothesized that some persons prefer positions close to the fixed
element, which we call “closeness”. As will be shown, after inspecting our results, we also came up
with one further hypothesis to explain some preferences.
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Participants
Fifty-one persons (12 males, 39 females; mean age 25.5, SD = 7.96), mostly students from different
disciplines of the University of Konstanz, were recruited via a local online system [ORSEE, 25]. For
participation, they received a voucher worth €3. The study was performed in accordance with the
ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki (1964) and its later amendments and with the ethics
and safety guidelines of the University of Konstanz. Participants were informed that they are free to
withdraw from the study at any point without any negative consequences. Informed consent was
obtained from all participants by check-marking a box on the informed-consent page before the actual
experiment started.
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 5 of 18
2.1.2. Stimuli
As fixed element F, we used one of five different element types, which had the same proportions
as in Puffer: A short vertical line (10 × 80 pixels), a long vertical line (10 × 160 pixels), a double line
(two lines at 10 × 80 pixels, 10 pixels apart), an oblique line (10 × 80 pixels) pointing in at 45°, and an
oblique line (10 × 80 pixels) pointing out at 45°. The oblique lines were pointed at the upper end. As
variable element V, the short vertical and the long vertical line were used, respectively. Each F type
was combined with each of the two V types, except that of its own type, which resulted in eight
conditions: Long-short, short-long, double-short, double-long, oblique-in-short, oblique-in-long,
oblique-out-short, and oblique-out-long, where the first term always denotes the F type (see Figure 2
for all combinations).
For each element type, F was successively presented at one of ve locations (40, 80, 120, 160, and
200 pixels from the center) along the horizontal midline, respectively. As Puffer was not clear in this
respect, we decided to present the center of F at these locations. Consequently, the inner edge distance
could differ between different F, e.g., between single-line and double-lines. The five F locations
combined with the eight F–V combinations resulted in 40 different conditions. Additionally, there
was a left (F-left, V-right) and a right (F-right, V-left) version, as in Puffer’s study. The stimulus area
(without gray frame) had an extension of 600 × 400 pixels and was horizontally centered on the screen
with a light blue background (see Figure 3).
Figure 3. Example displays for some conditions (short-long, double-long, oblique-in-long, and
oblique-out-long) in Experiment 1 (Block 1: F-left, V-right).
2.1.3. Procedure
The program for the online experiment was written in Javascript. Participants could choose
between German and English instructions. They were asked to use a notebook or desktop computer
and instructed how to scale their browser window (full-screen mode) so that all relevant elements
were optimally visible. The actual experiment started with a short instruction that informed about
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 6 of 18
the task and continued by requiring the participants to work through two main blocks (F-right, F-
left) at 40 stimuli, whose order was randomized. Within each block, the eight element-type
combinations were presented in a randomized order. The five different locations for F for each
combination, however, were always presented in a stepwise manner from the location closest to the
center to the most outward location.
Participants’ task was to move V with the computer mouse (via drag and drop) to a position that
produced the most pleasing arrangement. V could be moved horizontally anywhere between the
location of F and the opposite gray border with a minimum distance of 2 pixels to the fixed element
and to the frame, respectively. The initial position of V was chos en ran domly withi n that range. There
was no time limit. The participants could end a trial and forward to the next one by pressing a NEXT-
button. However, this was possible only after they had moved V. At the end, the participants had the
opportunity to write a comment. Altogether, the experiment lasted about 12 to 15 min.
2.2. Results
Fifty-one participants completed the online experiment. Unfortunately, due to technical issues,
some data were missing. As a consequence, the data of four persons were discarded completely. Of
the remaining 47 persons (11 males, mean age 25.09, SD = 7.43), six had missing data for the F-right
block and five for the F-left block, respectively. Therefore, 41 data sets were analyzed for F-right, and
42 data sets for F-left. Data are complete for 39 participants. A first analysis revealed that the data
from the two blocks F-left and F-right are rather similar, as has also been the case in Puffer’s [23]
study. Therefore, the positions in the F-right data were mirrored and added to the F-left data,
resulting in 83 data sets.
Example data are shown in Figure 4, where each row shows the data for two different locations
of F for all eight conditions, respectively. As can be seen, the lines are not uniformly distributed.
Rather, as expected, they cluster at some locations. To see to what extent the clusters coincide with
our hypotheses, we computed the corresponding positions and marked them in the figure by thick
colored lines. To calculate the positions for mechanical balance, we used the simple gray-value
integration method (see above). Merely for the closeness hypothesis, we show no marks, because
there was no single location.
As mentioned above, for the two most outer F positions (160 and 200 pixels) in the long-short
and double-short conditions, there are no positions for mechanical balance within the frame.
Moreover, for double-long and the oblique-short conditions, the positions for mechanical balance and
symmetry coincide.
By visually inspecting the data in Figure 4 alone, one can already see that the lines do not cluster
around the positions for mechanical balance (turquoise line). In most of the conditions there is some
clustering around the symmetry position. Most striking, though, is the frequent occurrence of
closeness, i.e., the clustering near F, which is especially pronounced for the double-line element. The
small down-pointing arrows above the V areas indicate the mean of the adjusted locations. The means
clearly support Puffer’s critique to Pierce [7] that they do not represent the performance of the
participants.
By visual inspection, we also had the impression that in some cases, participants placed the line at
the center of the frame. Therefore, we also tested this hypothesis.
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 7 of 18
(continued)
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 8 of 18
Figure 4 This figure shows two examples of results for each of the eight main conditions in
Experiment 1. Each row corresponds to one condition. The left and right columns represent the F
location of 80 and 160 pixels, respectively. Each black line indicates an adjusted position for the
variable element. The arrow above the lines indicates the mean of all V positions and the cross
indicates the center of the frame. The turquoise line represents the location for mechanical balance,
and the magenta line that for bilateral symmetry (relative to F). The distribution at the bottom reflects
the estimated intensities (see text for details).
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 9 of 18
2.2.1. Testing the Hypotheses
Although the validity of some of our hypotheses can already be assessed by visual inspection,
we also wanted to do some statistical tests. For this objective, we considered the adjusted line
positions for each condition as a spatial point (line) pattern [26]. Specifically, we assumed that the
lines were placed in accordance with an inhomogeneous Poisson process. For such a process, the
number of points that one can expect in a given region is expressed by its intensity function. For a
homogeneous process the intensity is constant across all regions. This is certainly not the case for our
data. To estimate the intensity for each region, we applied the R [27] function “density.ppp” from the
library “spatstat” [26], where the standard deviation (sigma) of the smoothing kernel was set to 10.
The results are shown as intensity plot at the bottom of each panel in Figure 4. The estimated intensity
is more informative than the shown lines, because multiple lines might superimpose at some
positions.
For testing our hypotheses, we analyzed whether the region of interest for a hypothesis has a
higher mean intensity, i.e., contains more points (lines), than the outer region. As the region of
interest, we defined 10% of the adjustment range around the position of the respective hypothesis.
The intensity in this region was contrasted with that of the same size surrounding this region. One
exception was closeness. For this hypothesis we used the range (10%) next to F as region of interest
and contrasted its intensity with the range (10%) next to that region. By this method, we were able to
detect local clusters. For testing whether the intensity in the region of interest was significantly higher
than that in the outer region, we applied the R-function “poisson.test”.
All four hypotheses were tested separately for all eight element conditions, where we considered
only three out of the five F locations: 80, 120, and 160 pixels, respectively. One reason for this selection
was that the regions of interest of some hypotheses overlap, depending on the length of the
adjustment area. The possible overlap is greatest for the F location closest (40 pixels) to the center. To
reduce the overlap, we excluded this condition from the analysis. We also excluded the 200 pixel
location, because here the position for mechanical balance was outside the frame for the conditions
long-short and double-short. Moreover, for these two conditions, the location for mechanical balance
was also outside the frame for the 160 pixel location. Therefore, for these two conditions we excluded
this F location and included the innermost F location (40 pixels) instead. Thus, for each of the eight
main conditions, we considered three out of the five F locations. It should also be noted that for some
conditions, the positions for mechanical balance and symmetry coincide (for examples see Figure 4).
For each hypothesis, we performed a Poisson test (α = 0.05) for each of the eight main conditions
and the respective three F locations, to see whether more lines were positioned in the corresponding
region of interest than in the surrounding region. Since it would have been very cumbersome to
consider each of the 24 tests for each hypothesis individually, the outcomes were summed over the
main conditions and considered F locations. The results are listed in Table 1, where the single-digit
numbers represent the number of significant differences (max = 3 for each main condition, max = 24
overall) between the region of interest and the outer region. The numbers in parenthesis are the
corresponding summed numbers of lines.
As can be seen in Table 1, for mechanical balance, the differences were significant only for three
of the 24 tests. More specifically, they occurred only in conditions 5 and 7. In these conditions, F was
an oblique line and V a short line, respectively. Thus, in these cases the regions of interest for balance
and symmetry were overlapping. The region of interest for symmetry was significant in six of the 24
tests, mostly, when V was short, but not when F consisted of double lines. Closeness was produced
most frequently (significant in 14 of 24 tests), especially in conditions, where F was a double line or F
was oblique and V was short. Centering was produced in nine of the 24 conditions. It should be noted,
however, that one of the significant tests occurred in the double-short condition, where we included
the F location closest to the center, for which the region of interest was rather similar to that for
closeness.
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 10 of 18
Table 1. Number of significant tests (maximum 3) in each of the eight element-type conditions for the
four hypotheses, respectively, in Experiment 1. For further details, see the text.
Balance Symmetry Closeness Centering
1 short-long 0(19:28) 1(56:27) 1(58:17) 1(35:19)
2 long-short 0(10:17) 1(59:42) 0(61:20) 0(39:45)
3 double-short 0(8:4) 0(33:19) 3(153:7) 1(67:11)
4 double-long 0(42:28) 0(42:28) 2(72:16) 3(42:13)
5 obl.-in-short 2(52:22) 2(52:22) 3(94:20) 0(28:24)
6 obl.-in-long 0(20:26) 1(40:23) 0(71:23) 1(41:22)
7 obl.-out-short 1(40:25) 1(40:25) 3(98:19) 1(35:16)
8 obl.-out-long 0(17:26) 0(32:19) 2(92:19) 2(51:24)
Sum 3(208:176) 6(354:205) 14(699:141) 9(338:174)
The large number of tests for each hypothesis might be considered as problematic, because of
the inflation of false positives. For the present number of 24 tests, this means that, given our
significance level of 5%, one test can be expected to be significant due to an alpha error. However,
because the situation is the same for all hypotheses, their comparison should nevertheless be
meaningful. Moreover, we also tested the difference in the number of lines between the inner and
outer regions for each hypothesis across all main conditions and F locations. For these overall tests
the risk of false positives does not inflate. The results are shown in the last row in Table 1. For balance
we have a rate ratio of 1.18, p = 0.057, for symmetry of 1.73, p < 0.001, for closeness of 4.96, p < 0.001,
and for centering of 1.94, p < 0.001. The latter also remains significant (ratio = 1.66, p < 0.001), if we
exclude the problematic double-short condition.
2.2.2. Adjustment Times
We also registered how much time the participants spent on their adjustments. Since some
adjustment times were rather long (max = 4 h), presumably due to a long break between two
adjustments, and others quite short (min = 1.54 s), we trimmed the data by excluding the 2% shortest
and 2% longest adjustment times for each condition. The resulting mean adjustment times and their
standard deviation (in parenthesis) for the eight conditions are: 5.88 (3.44), 5.87 (3.19), 6.21 (3.22), 5.93
(3.04), 5.57 (3.52), 5.58 (3.51), 5.83 (3.96), and 5.57 (3.56) s. Their average is 5.80 s (3.43 s). Summing up
the adjustment times over all trials resulted in 7.74 min.
2.2.3. Individual Performance
We also explored individual performance to see how consistent each participant positioned the
lines. The individual data for two example conditions are shown in Figure 5. With such data, we
analyzed which strategies the individual participants applied, but only with respect to the most
prevalent strategies: Symmetry and closeness. To categorize the produced positions, we used the
same regions of interest as before (see also Figure 5). For testing which strategy the individuals
preferred, we tested the difference in production, again with the R-function “poisson.test”. As a
result, we found that 11 persons produced more positions according to symmetry than to closeness,
whereas 17 showed the opposite pattern (see Figure 6). Eleven persons could not be assigned to either
of the two strategies. It should be noted that these classifications only mean that participants chose
one of these two strategies significantly more often than the other one.
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 11 of 18
Figure 5. Individual performance of our 39 participants (Experiment 1) in the long-short (upper panel)
and double-short (lower panel) conditions (F location: 80 pixels). The filled rectangles represent the
fixed elements. The left and right border indicate the center and inner edge of the fixed element,
respectively. The red and blue lines show the adjusted positions in the F-right and F-left block,
respectively. The long vertical lines indicate the regions of interest for closeness and symmetry.
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 12 of 18
Figure 6. This figure shows preferences of the 39 participants (Experiment 1) with respect to symmetry
and closeness. Each point represents one participant and indicates how many of her or his 80
adjustments are located in the region of interest for symmetry or closeness, respectively. The blue
triangles represent participants who more often chose positions according to the closeness hypothesis
than those in accord with symmetry. The red squares represent persons with the reversed preference.
The black circles stand for participants that preferred neither of the two strategies. The single digit
indicates that the adjacent point represents four participants.
2.3. Discussion
Although our participants took only seconds for each line adjustment rather than minutes, as in
Puffer’s [23] study, the data are similar in several aspects. First of all, there is little evidence for
mechanical balance as a strategy for creating pleasing compositions. Although there was a significant
number of corresponding adjustments in the two oblique-short conditions, this can also be explained
by symmetry, as the locations predicted by the two hypotheses coincided for these conditions. Since
symmetry was also used as a strategy in other conditions, it is likely that the positions in the oblique-
short conditions were not produced to realize mechanical balancing, but symmetry.
Symmetric configurations were produced for at least one F location in five of the eight main
(element-type) conditions, while closeness occurred in six of the main conditions. If we consider the
test for all three F locations in each of the eight main conditions, then 14 of the 24 tests (eight main
conditions × three F locations) are significant for closeness, but only six for symmetry. Symmetry was
mainly produced when V was short, but not when F consisted of double lines. The latter was also
observed by Puffer [23].
Although closeness has already been reported by Puffer [23], we were surprised by its frequency
in our results. It was very frequently produced in conditions, where F was a double line or an oblique
line, and V was short, i.e., when the length of all line elements was similar. Concerning our ex-post-
facto centering hypothesis, the analyses revealed that this strategy was applied in nine of the 24
conditions. However, the regions of interest overlapped in some conditions (where F was near the
center) with that of closeness.
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 13 of 18
The dominance of closeness was also reflected by individual performance. In contrast, only less
than one third of the persons preferred symmetry over closeness. Moreover, the dominance of the
preferred strategy was much more pronounced in the first group. About one third of participants
applied both strategies equally often. It should be noted that Figure 6 can also be used to assess how
frequently other strategies were applied. For instance, if a person produced closeness on 30 trials and
symmetry on 16 trials, then the difference to 80, i.e., 34, gives the number of trials where another
strategy was used.
Taken together, our data indicate that, although symmetry and centering were also applied as
principles of good composition, closeness occurred by far most frequently. A possible reason for this
unexpected result could have been that the distance of F from the center increased stepwise for each
element type of V. Consequently, at the beginning of a given main condition, F appeared near the
center, so that for various reasons the participants also placed V close to the center, which might have
produced some nice configurations. When F was then sequentially placed further away from the
center, many participants could have wanted to maintain the configuration. Therefore, closeness
could be the result of a carry-over effect from the first trial in a condition to the subsequent trials.
Although Puffer [23] also increased the distance of F from the midline in a stepwise manner, we
reasoned that the corresponding effect on closeness was stronger in our case, because the much
shorter adjustment times might have favored carry-over effects. To test whether this was indeed the
case, we conducted a second experiment.
3. Experiment 2
This experiment was similar to Experiment 1, except that all trials, i.e., also the F locations, were
randomized in each block. If positional carry-over effects between trials have been responsible for
the frequent closeness in our first experiment, then this principle should be applied less frequently in
the present experiment.
Moreover, because we proposed the centering hypothesis only ex post facto in Experiment 1,
this experiment was also an opportunity to see whether this result can be replicated.
3.1. Method
Forty persons (14 males, 26 females; mean age 24.2, SD = 6.20), mostly students from different
disciplines of the University of Konstanz, were recruited via our local online system as in Experiment
1. They participated for a voucher worth €3. No participant had previously participated in
Experiment 1. The procedure was identical to that of Experiment 1, except that the F locations were
randomized.
3.2. Results
The data set of one participant was incomplete due to technical issues, and additionally three
participants reported a small displacement between the variable line and the mouse cursor.
Therefore, their data were excluded from the analysis (remaining 36 participants: 14 males, mean age
24.3, SD = 6.52).
The data were analyzed in the same way as in Experiment 1. The results of the statistical tests
with respect to the four hypotheses are shown in Table 2. As can be seen, tests for mechanical balance
are significant only in one of 24 conditions tested, and this was a condition where the location for
balance coincides with that for symmetry. Whereas symmetry was significant four times, closeness
was again by far the most frequently applied strategy (11 of 24 test were significant). The majority of
significant tests occurred with oblique F and short V, but again also for double-line F. This time,
centering was significant only in two main conditions, and only for one F location, respectively. One
of these conditions was double-short, where we included the F location closest to the center. Insofar,
this result should be taken with care.
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 14 of 18
Table 2. Number of significant tests in each of the eight main conditions for the four hypotheses,
respectively, in Experiment 2. For further details, see text.
Balance Symmetry Closeness Centering
1 short-long 0(11:18) 2(45:21) 1(70:18) 0(21:17)
2 long-short 0(13:14) 1(44:28) 0(59:28) 0(36:33)
3 double-short 0(7:5) 0(14:12) 3(142:15) 1(57:15)
4 double-long 1(36:22) 1(36:22) 0(73:19) 0(25:15)
5 obl.-in-short 0(26:14) 0(26:14) 2(98:14) 0(24:21)
6 obl.-in-long 0(11:19) 0(16:21) 1(89:23) 0(32:22))
7 obl.-out-short 0(22:13) 0(22:13) 3(100:14) 1(39:19)
8 obl.-out-long 0(13:17) 0(19:16) 1(96:26) 0(33:20)
Sum 1(139:122) 4(222: 147) 11(727:157) 2(267:162)
Concerning our overall analyses, the rate ratios for balance was 1.14, p = 0.161, for symmetry
1.51, p < 0.001, for closeness 4.63, p < 0.001, and for centering 1.65, p < 0.001. Even if we ignore the
problematic double-short condition for the latter strategy (see above), the ratio is still significant, 1.43,
p < 0.001.
3.2.1. Adjustment Times
The trimmed mean adjustment times for the eight conditions were: 5.21 (2.70), 5.29 (3.05), 5.66
(2.94), 5.60 (3.36), 4.84 (2.90), 5.12 (3.58), 5.08 (3.27), and 4.90 (2.88) s. Their average is 5.21 s (3.09 s).
Across all trials, the adjustment times sum up to 6.95 min.
3.2.2. Individual Performance
As in the previous experiment, we analyzed how many participants preferred the symmetry or
closeness strategy. As a result, five persons produced a symmetric arrangement more often, whereas
22 placed more lines in accord with the closeness hypothesis. Nine persons could not be assigned to
either of the two strategies. Figure 7 illustrates these findings.
Figure 7. This figure shows the performance of the 36 participants in Experiment 2. Each point
represents a participant and indicates how many of her or his 80 adjustments are located in the region
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 15 of 18
of interest for symmetry or closeness, respectively. The blue triangles represent participants who more
often adjusted positions according to the closeness hypothesis than to the symmetry hypothesis. The
red squares represent participants for which the relation was reversed. The black circles represent
participants that preferred neither of the two strategies. The digits indicate that the adjacent point
represents two participants.
3.3. Discussion
Even though the F locations were randomized in this experiment, the results are, by and large,
similar to those from the previous experiment. Different from our expectation, closeness was not only
again the most frequently applied strategy, its dominance was even more pronounced than in
Experiment 1. The analysis of the individual data revealed that more than half of the participants (22
of 36) preferred closeness over symmetry. These results demonstrate that the frequency of closeness
in Experiment 1 was not due to carry-over effects caused by the stepwise increase of the distance
between the fixed element and the midline.
Symmetry and centering also occurred less frequently than in our previous experiment.
Centering was significant only in two tests, compared to nine tests in Experiment 1. Nevertheless, the
overall tests were again significant for both strategies, indicating that they still played some role. In
line with Experiment 1, there was again no evidence for the application of mechanical balance.
4. General Discussion
The aim of the present study was to repeat some of the experiments on symmetry and balance
conducted by Ethel Puffer [23] more than a century ago. She used a large (60 × 80 cm) board with an
element of a certain form attached on one side at a fixed location and asked her nine participants to
position a variable line so that the configuration was aesthetically most pleasing. Here, we updated
her method and used an online experiment, where the variable line had to be moved on a screen with
a computer mouse. A first striking difference in performance between the two experiments was that
Puffer’s participants took about 6 min for each adjustment, whereas ours spent only about 5 s. Despite
these large differences in adjustment times and the whole setup, some of the results obtained in
Experiment 1 were similar.
First of all, as in Puffer, we found little to no evidence for the application of mechanical balance
as a principle for producing aesthetically pleasing compositions. Rather, geometric symmetry was
applied as principle, in the sense that participants often moved the variable element to a position that
was laterally symmetric to that of the fixed element, but without paying attention to size and form
differences between the fixed and variable elements. A similar performance was observed by Pierce,
[7] as well as by Puffer [23]. A possible reason of why size and form differences were largely ignored
could have been that they were relatively small. This is supported by the fact that symmetry was
practically absent for double lines, which differed most in this respect. That lateral symmetry played
an important role as design principle might have several reasons. For instance, there is not only a
relatively high general preference for symmetry, it is also relatively easy to realize. This corresponds
to the idea that people prefer symmetry, because it facilitates fluent processing, which generates
positive subjective feelings [28].
Furthermore, after visual inspection of our data, we had the impression that participants often
placed the variable line at the center of the frame. Therefore, we also considered centering as a
strategy, which was confirmed by our statistical analysis.
An unexpected result was that the variable line was often placed very close to the fixed element.
Although Puffer had already reported that some of her participants placed the variable line near the
fixed element, it occurred by far not as frequently as in our experiment. Here, closeness occurred for
almost all element types. Moreover, the analysis of individual performance revealed that several
participants exclusively used closeness as a strategy. Moreover, more participants preferred closeness
over symmetry than vice versa. How can the strong tendency to produce closeness be explained?
Puffer concluded from comments of her participants that they wanted to construct a configuration
without necessarily forming a figure. Moreover, for them the clustered elements were set off against
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 16 of 18
the blackness on the other side. Puffer had the impression that the black space on the opposite side
had some meaning such as a deep vista or an expanse of sky.
Although Puffer’s account of closeness is interesting, we questioned that it accounts for the
frequent application of this principle in our first experiment. Therefore, we hypothesized that the
stepwise increase of the distance between the fixed element and the midline in combination with the
short adjustment times might have produced carry-over effects from the shortest distance, where
closeness is an obvious strategy, to longer distances.
To test our hypothesis, we conducted a second experiment, which was similar to the first one,
except that the location of the fixed element and the element type were randomized across trials. As
a result, the number of significant tests decreased substantially for all of our hypotheses (from 29 in
Experiment 1, to 17 in Experiment 2, counting tests for overlapping hypotheses only once). The
largest decrease occurred for centering, which was now practically absent. However, different from
our expectation, closeness occurred again very frequently. The relative number of persons that
preferred closeness over symmetry even increased. These results indicate that the stepwise procedure
had an effect on the frequency of use of specific design principles, but that it was not responsible for
the strong tendency in Experiment 1 to produce compositions based on closeness.
What else could explain the high frequency of closeness in our experiments? One reason might
be that this principle was also relatively easy to apply, presumably, even easier than symmetry, and
therefore strongly met the needs of our participants who are used to act quickly on the computer.
However, an alternative and more plausible account is to assume that the participants adopted a
specific perspective. The comments given by our participants suggest that some of them took a local
perspective, i.e., focused more on composing a pleasing configuration from the elements, which
usually resulted in closeness, while others adopted a global perspective and tried to produce a
pleasing picture as a whole. Given these two perspectives, it is reasonable to assume that our
computer-based method, where the participants had to focus on the variable line to drag it to another
position, and where we had no control of the viewing distance, favored a local perspective, and,
therefore, the production of closeness. The situation might be similar to drawing, where a local focus
is also particularly useful [29]. This account might explain the difference between our and Puffer’s
results with respect to the frequency of closeness.
As already mentioned, similar to Puffer, we found only weak support for mechanical balance—
at least if one does not assume empty space to have perceptual weight. This result could also have
been due to our specific method. Both Puffer’s and our study applied the method of production,
which is one of the three methods, besides the method of choice and the method of use, proposed by
Fechner [30]. The method of production has the advantage that it offers a large range of possibilities.
In contrast, if the method of choice is applied for investigating balance, the researcher presents a more
and a less balanced configuration and asked the participants which one they prefer. In such situations
the more balanced one is usually preferred, and it is concluded that balance is important, and that
people generally prefer balanced pictures. The present study, however, shows, as did Puffer’s study
a century ago, that even for relatively simple arrangements, almost no person produces mechanically
balanced configurations, when she or he is free also to apply other principles. One reason could be
that balance is more difficult to produce than symmetry and closeness. Perhaps, art experts might
have produced more balanced compositions, because of their knowledge about balance, and because
they prefer symmetry less than non-experts [31,32].
If the positive role of mechanical balance for liking was only measurable in settings where all
other variables are held constant, the importance of balance as a design principle was largely reduced.
However, the attraction of balance as an aesthetic principle might depend on the applied method and
the stimuli. As mentioned in the Introduction, more recent experiments did not restrict their
investigation to the horizontal balance of simple elements, but examined to what extent the center of
mass of multiple and more complex elements deviates from the center of the frame irrespective of
direction [10–16]. All these studies find support for a positive relation between balance and liking.
Interestingly, also for this two-dimensional approach, there is a study that used the method of
production. Locher et al. [33] asked their participants to construct interesting and pleasant designs
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 17 of 18
from nine identical shapes of different sizes within a square frame located on a table. As a result, for
most of the compositions the center of gravity was close to the geometric center of the frame.
Taken together, these results indicate that the answer to whether balance is an important design
principle or not, depends on the stimulus material and the specific task. If multiple elements can be
positioned freely in a frame, then mechanical balance is usually taken into account. In this case the
geometric center of a display functions as an “anchor” around which the construction is organized
[2]. However, if there are only two elements in a frame, and one of them is fixed on one side, and the
other can be moved along the horizontal midline only, then there are other, more attractive principles
for designing a pleasing composition, such as closeness and lateral symmetry of the location of these
two elements no matter whether the composition as a whole is balanced or not.
Author Contributions: Conceptualization, R.H. and K.T.; Methodology, R.H. and K.T.; Software, R.H.;
Validation, R.H. and K.T.; Formal analysis, R.H.; Investigation, R.H.; Resources, R.H.; Data curation, R.H.;
Writing—original draft preparation, R.H.; Writing—review and editing, K.T. and R.H.; Visualization, R.H. and
K.T.
Funding: This research received no external funding.
Acknowledgments: We thank Martin G. Fillinger for his helpful comments.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
1. Ross, D.W. A Theory of Pure Design: Harmony, Balance, Rhythm; Houghton, Mifin: Boston, MA, USA, 1907.
2. Arnheim, R. The Power of the Center: A Study of Composition in the Visual Arts; University of California Press:
Berkeley, CA, USA, 1983.
3. Bouleau, C. The Painter's Secret Geometry: A Study of Composition in Art; Dover Publications Inc.: Dover,
Kent, UK, 2014.
4. Kandinsky, W. Point and Line to Plane; Translated by H. Dearstyne and H. Rebay; Dover Publications Inc:
New York, USA, 1926/1979.
5. Langfeld, H.S. The Aesthetic Attitude; Harcourt Brace & Company: New York, USA, 1920. doi:10.1037/10922-
010.
6. Pecchinenda, A.; Bertamini, M.; Makin, A.D.J.; Ruta, N. The Pleasantness of Visual Symmetry: Always,
Never or Sometimes. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e92685. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0092685.
7. Pierce, E. Studies from the Harvard Psychological Laboratory (II): The aesthetics of simple forms. I.
Symmetry. Psychol. Rev. 1894, 1, 483–495. doi:10.1037/h0069000.
8. Locher, P.J.; Nodine, C. The perceptual value of symmetry. Comput. Math. Appl. 1989, 17, 475–484.
9. Locher, P.J. Contemporary experimental aesthetics: Procedures and findings. In Handbook of the Economics
of Art and Culture; Victor, A.G., David, T., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2014; Volume 2, pp.
49–80.
10. Hübner, R.; Fillinger, M.G. Perceptual balance, stability, and aesthetic appreciation: Their relations depend
on the picture type. i-Perception 2019, 10, 1–17. doi:10.1177/2041669519856040.
11. Fillinger, M.G.; Hübner, R. The relations between balance, prototypicality, and aesthetic appreciation for
Japanese calligraphy. Empir. Stud. Arts 2018. doi:10.1177/0276237418805656.
12. Hübner, R.; Fillinger, M.G. Comparison of objective measures for predicting perceptual balance and visual
aesthetic preference. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 335. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00335.
13. Wilson, A.; Chatterjee, A. The assessment of preference for balance: Introducing a new test. Empir. Stud.
Arts 2005, 23, 165–180. doi:10.2190/B1LR-MVF3-F36X-XR64.
14. McManus, I.; Stöver, K.; Kim, D. Arnheim’s Gestalt theory of visual balance: Examining the compositional
structure of art photographs and abstract images. i-Perception 2011, 2, 615–647. doi:10.1068/i0445aap.
15. Thömmes, K.; Hübner, R. Instagram Likes for Architectural Photos Can Be Predicted by Quantitative
Balance Measures and Curvature. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01050.
16. Locher, P.J.; Gray, S.; Nodine, C. The structural framework of pictorial balance. Perception 1996, 25, 1419–
1436.
17. Pierce, E. Studies from the Harvard Psychological Laboratory (V): The aesthetics of simple forms. II. The
functions of the elements. Psychol. Rev. 1896, 3, 270–282. doi:10.1037/h0073983.
Symmetry 2019, 11, 1468 18 of 18
18. McManus, I.; Edmondson, D.; Rodger, J. Balance in pictures. Br. J. Psychol. 1985, 76, 311–324.
19. Jahanian, A.; Vishwanathan, S.V.N.; Allebach, J.P. Learning visual balance from large-scale datasets of
aesthetically highly rated images. In Proceedings of the SPIE, Human Vision and Electronic Imaging XX,
San Francisco, 2015; Volume 9394, pp. 93940Y-93940Y-93949.
20. Abeln, J.; Fresz, L.; Amirshahi, S.A.; McManus, I.C.; Koch, M.; Kreysa, H.; Redies, C. Preference for Well-
Balanced Saliency in Details Cropped from Photographs. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016, 9, 704.
21. Friedenberg, J.; Liby, B. Perceiving the center of three-body displays: The role of size-ratio, symmetry,
elongation, and gravity. Open Behav. Sci. J. 2008, 2, 13–22.
22. Friedenberg, J.; Liby, B. Perception of two-body center of mass. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2002, 64, 531–
539.
23. Puffer, E.D. Studies in symmetry. Psychol. Rev. Monogr. Suppl. 4 Harv. Psychol. Stud. 1 1903, 1, 467–539.
24. Cohn, J. ‘Puffer, E.D. Studies in Symmetry. Psychol. Rev. Monograph. Suppl. 4 Harv. Psychol. Stud. 1 1903’.
Zeitschrift für Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane 1906, 40, 215–218.
25. Greiner, B. Subject pool recruitment procedures: Organizing experiments with ORSEE. J. Econ. Sci. Assoc.
2015, 1, 114–125. doi:10.1007/s40881-015-0004-4.
26. Baddeley, A.; Rubak, E.; Turner, R. Spatial Point Patterns: Methodology and Applications with R; Chapman and
Hall/CRC: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2015.
27. R Core Team R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical
Computing: https://www.R-project.org/, 2017.
28. Reber, R.; Schwarz, N.; Winkielman, P. Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the
perceiver’s processing experience? Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2004, 8, 364–382.
29. Chamberlain, R.; Wagemans, J. Visual arts training is linked to flexible attention to local and global levels
of visual stimuli. Acta Psychol. 2015, 161, 185–197.
30. Fechner, G.T. Zur experimentalen Ästhetik. Hirzel: Leipzig, 1871.
31. Leder, H.; Tinio, P.P.; Brieber, D.; Kröner, T.; Jacobsen, T.; Rosenberg, R. Symmetry is not a universal law
of beauty. Empir. Stud. Arts 2019, 37, 104–114. doi:10.1177/0276237418777941.
32. Weichselbaum, H.; Leder, H.; Ansorge, U. Implicit and Explicit Evaluation of Visual Symmetry as a
Function of Art Expertise. i-Perception 2018, 9, 1–24. doi:10.1177/2041669518761464.
33. Locher, P.J.; Stappers, P.J.; Overbeeke, K. The role of balance as an organizing design principle underlying
adults’ compositional strategies for creating visual displays. Acta Psychol. 1998, 99, 141–161.
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
... A The different axes and lines used for computing the Balance score (Wilson & Chatterjee (2005); see text for details). The image underneath shows the portrait of Ethel Puffer (1872-1950, from a 1995 publication) who was one of the first psychologists to experimentally study balance (Puffer, 1903;Hübner and Thömmes, 2019). The Balance score of the image is 7.31 (bright pixels are assumed to be heavier), which indicates an excellent bal-ance. ...
Article
Full-text available
Over the past two decades, researchers in the field of visual aesthetics have studied numerous quantitative (objective) image properties and how they relate to visual aesthetic appreciation. However, results are difficult to compare between research groups. One reason is that researchers use different sets of image properties in their studies. However, even if the same properties are used, the image pre-processing techniques may differ, and researchers often use their own customized scripts to calculate the image properties. To provide better accessibility and comparability of research results in visual experimental aesthetics, we developed an open-access and easy-to-use toolbox called Aesthetics Toolbox . The Toolbox allows users to calculate a well-defined set of quantitative image properties popular in contemporary research. The properties include image dimensions, lightness and color statistics, complexity, symmetry, balance, Fourier spectrum properties, fractal dimension, self-similarity, as well as entropy measures and CNN-based variances. Compatible with most devices, the Toolbox provides an intuitive click-and-drop web interface. In the Toolbox, we integrated the original scripts of four different research groups and translated them into Python 3. To ensure that results were consistent across analyses, we took care that results from the Python versions of the scripts were the same as those from the original scripts. The toolbox, detailed documentation, and a link to the cloud version are available via GitHub: https://github.com/RBartho/Aesthetics-Toolbox . In summary, we developed a toolbox that helps to standardize and simplify the calculation of quantitative image properties for visual aesthetics research.
... In the early days of experiential aesthetics, researchers gathered diverse materials and developed new paradigms to explore how stimulating changes in certain aspects led to changes in appreciation. Line direction, length, curvature, thickness [12], proportion [13], the complexity of polygons [14], the level of curvature [15], symmetry [16] or the uniformity of patterns and arrangements [17] led to differences in perceived beauty or pleasure. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Previous studies have suggested human preference for artworks with greater image complexity. Here we aim to further explore the impact of complexity on aesthetic experiences in both visual and auditory domains. Twenty-eight college students were tasked with viewing pairs of abstract paintings by Ely Raman and indicating their preference using a two-alternative forced choice procedure. Following this, they listened to Western tonal music for five seconds and completed a simple choice reaction task. A longer reaction time was taken as an indication of greater immersion in the music and, consequently, a stronger preference. Our findings confirmed a preference for complexity in both visual and auditory modalities. Additionally, a notable correlation was observed between the inclination towards complexity in the visual domain and that in the auditory domain, lending support to the Taste Typicality in explaining individual differences in aesthetic experiences.
... Psychology and empirical aesthetics were concerned with symmetry work very early on, as early as the beginning of the twentieth century, providing important data regarding aesthetic preference for symmetry. In her PhD work later published in the influential book "Psychology of Beauty", psychologist and suffragette Ethel Puffer observed in her study that symmetry composition is one of the preferred strategies for generating a pleasant composition, and these results were also confirmed more than one hundred years later (Hübner & Thömmes, 2019). Besides empirical aesthetics and neuroaesthetics, other disciplines also focus on symmetry and are very informative for these fields, even if their focus is on the aesthetic experience. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Why are we fascinated by flowers? Why are we fascinated by synchronous dance? What do they have in common? The overview on Symmetry Research in Neuroaesthetics discusses the importance of studying aesthetic primitives in holistic contexts. Using the current results on symmetry preference from various fields, one of the better-understood aspects of aesthetic preference, this overview highlights what we can hope to achieve by thoroughly applying neuroaesthetic research methods and how results in this field can contribute to the understanding human behaviour at the phylogenetic scale.
... When composing an MB, several relevant factors must be taken into consideration, including not only the positions of visual elements within the board, oriented to aesthetically balanced composition (Arnheim, 2004), but also: their colours (P. Locher et al., 2005); orientations (Locher & Stappers, 2002); sizes, and shapes (Hübner & Thömmes, 2019;Wilson & Chatterjee, 2005). A balanced composition points to a tendency to distribute visual elements throughout the physical space of the board (Leyssen et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Mood Boards (MB) are a visual tool used to provide references for inspiration and decision-making in the design development process. However, it isn't easy to obtain objective and precise measures about the MB organization if exclusively qualitative approaches identify how the image composition can affect the visual reading process. In this sense, this study aimed to evaluate how the position and physical image aspects influence an attentive MB visual reading. The results presented that variations in image contrast and positioning influence the observer's visual reading. Besides, the observers' identified tendencies were to concentrate the visualisations in the boards' central, lower and left areas, indicating the importance of this region's MB construction. Moreover, the results evidence that the visual reading of an MB presents a different pattern from those reported by studies directed to analysing the visual reading of websites. Thus, changes in image positioning influenced the viewer's perception, being a relevant point to be considered when assembling the MB's image composition. The results of this study contribute to creating criteria for the MB construction process.
Article
Full-text available
The Barnes Foundation is a traditional art collection and it is one of a kind as for the assorted hanging of the paintings. The sophisticated wall compositions by Albert Barnes were created as a tool for art education, and they have not been altered since 1951. Today, we are interested whether Barnes’ taste withstood the test of time. We asked participants in an online study to create their own hangings on five of the original gallery walls, and also tested whether beholders are able to identify a missing painting from a selection of suitable alternatives. Results show that Barnes’ motifs are reproduced by a significant number of participants and that experts produce more Barnes-like displays than naive participants. We conclude that Albert Barnes based his choices at least to some degree on universally valid visual aspects that are still understood today, especially by people trained in the visual domain.
Article
Spirals, with their widespread presence in both nature and culture, are universally admired. Although there are different types, such as the Archimedean, logarithmic, and golden spirals, they are indiscriminately considered as beautiful. This universal view might explain the lack of studies investigating aesthetic differences among spirals. To show that there are indeed differences, the beauty of the golden spiral was compared with that of the Fibonacci spiral in this empirical study. Since the curvature of the golden spiral changes continuously, whereas that of the Fibonacci spiral does so in steps, the golden spiral was predicted to be aesthetically preferred. The results clearly confirmed this prediction. That the difference in preference was really based on the continuity versus discontinuity of the curvature is supported by the further result that an Archimedean spiral was preferred over a Dürer spiral, which similarly differed in their continuity.
Thesis
Full-text available
Visual aesthetic experiences are omnipresent in our everyday life: from the joy that arises from looking at untouched natural landscapes or beautifully designed objects and artworks, to the conscious or unconscious aesthetic preferences that influence ordinary purchasing decisions for food or clothing, all the way to important life decisions like selecting a partner. It is thus no wonder that empirical aesthetics is a research field that scientists, artists, humanists, and marketers alike are interested in. The increasingly interdisciplinary field of empirical aesthetics offers manifold methods and a vast range of perspectives on aesthetic issues. This doctoral thesis examines the aesthetic appeal of photographs from the disciplinary viewpoint of psychology. In three consecutive research papers it addresses the fundamental issue of how to define an aesthetic measure for a suitable set of images in order to investigate aesthetic principles. This is done by leveraging data from a real-world source: the social media platform Instagram. The first research paper addresses the question whether and how Instagram Likes can be leveraged to investigate aesthetic principles. It provides insights from an explorative investigation of liking data and low-level image features in a confined set of about 700 photographs derived from five large Instagram accounts of architecture photographs. It is shown that Instagram Likes can serve as a proxy for the aesthetic appeal of images if important confounds are controlled for through careful data selection. Based on the promising results from this initial study, in research paper 2 the database is broadened to over 15,000 images from different genres (architecture, dancer portraits, and landscapes) and a comprehensive method is developed to compute an aesthetic score reflecting the aesthetic appeal of an image based on Instagram Likes. This method systematically reflects and addresses the need to control for influential confounds, above all the growing numbers of followers of an account over time. Finally, in the 3rd research paper 30,000 photographs from another genre, namely bird photography, are investigated, lending further proof for the broad applicability of the method from research paper 2. The investigation of several form, content, and context variables that were hypothesized to affect the aesthetic appeal of such photos underlines the potential of leveraging Instagram data in empirical aesthetics. This doctoral thesis proposes a new approach to tap a valuable source of data for empirical aesthetics and develops a measure for the aesthetic appeal of photographs. Several previously studied aesthetic principles were replicated and generalized using Instagram data, providing empirical evidence for both universal and domain-specific aesthetic effects. The methodological contributions of this thesis provide a sound basis for the use of real-life data from Instagram to investigate aesthetic principles with good ecological validity in mind.
Article
Full-text available
It is widely assumed that the aesthetic appreciation of a picture depends, among others, on how well the picture’s composition is perceptually balanced, where “perceptual balance” is often defined analogous to mechanics. To what extent this metaphor holds for different picture types, however, is still open. Therefore, in this study, we examined the relationship between balance, liking, and some objective measures with pictures from an aesthetic sensitivity test. These stimuli could be divided into single-element, multiple-element, and dynamic-pattern pictures. The results show that “balance” is interpreted differently, depending on the stimulus type. Whereas “mechanical” balance was applied to assess single-element pictures, the balance of multiple-element and dynamic-pattern pictures was rated more in the sense of gravitational stability. Only for the multiple-element stimuli, there was a positive relation between balance/stability and liking. Together, our findings show that there are different types of balance, and that their relation with liking depends on the picture type.
Article
Full-text available
“3,058 people like this.” In the digital age, people very commonly indicate their preferences by clicking a Like button. The data generated on the photo-sharing platform Instagram potentially represents a vast, freely accessible resource for research in the field of visual experimental aesthetics. Therefore, we compiled a photo database using images of five different Instagram accounts that fullfil several criteria (e.g., large followership, consistent content). The final database consists of about 700 architectural photographs with the corresponding liking data generated by the Instagram community. First, we aimed at validating Instagram Likes as a potential measure of aesthetic appeal. Second, we checked whether previously studied low-level features of “good” image composition also account for the number of Instagram Likes that architectural photographs received. We considered two measures of visual balance and the preference for curvature over angularity. In addition, differences between images with “2D” vs. “3D” appearance became obvious. Our findings show that visual balance predicts Instagram Likes in more complex “3D” photographs, with more balance meaning more Likes. In the less complex “2D” photographs the relation is reversed, more balance led to fewer Likes. Moreover, there was a general preference for curvature in the Instagram database. Together, our study illustrates the potential of using Instagram Likes as a measure of aesthetic appeal and provides a fruitful methodological basis for future research.
Article
Full-text available
Scientific disciplines as diverse as biology, physics, and psychological aesthetics regard symmetry as one of the most important principles in nature and one of the most powerful determinants of beauty. However, symmetry has a low standing in the arts and humanities. This difference in the valuation of symmetry is a remarkable illustration of the gap between the two cultures. To close this gap, we conducted an interdisciplinary, empirical study to directly demonstrate the effects of art expertise on symmetry appreciation. Two groups of art experts—artists and art historians—and a group of non-experts provided spontaneous beauty ratings of visual stimuli that varied in symmetry and complexity. In complete contrast to responses typically found in non-art experts, art experts found asymmetrical and simple stimuli as most beautiful. This is evidence of the effects of specific education and training on aesthetic appreciation and a direct challenge to the universality of symmetry.
Article
Full-text available
In perception, humans typically prefer symmetrical over asymmetrical patterns. Yet, little is known about differences in symmetry preferences depending on individuals’ different past histories of actively reflecting upon pictures and patterns. To address this question, we tested the generality of the symmetry preference for different levels of individual art expertise. The preference for symmetrical versus asymmetrical abstract patterns was measured implicitly, by an Implicit Association Test (IAT), and explicitly, by a rating scale asking participants to evaluate pattern beauty. Participants were art history and psychology students. Art expertise was measured using a questionnaire. In the IAT, art expertise did not alter the preference for symmetrical over asymmetrical patterns. In contrast, the explicit rating scale showed that with higher art expertise, the ratings for the beauty of asymmetrical patterns significantly increased, but, again, participants preferred symmetrical over asymmetrical patterns. The results are discussed in light of different theories on the origins of symmetry preference. Evolutionary adaptation might play a role in symmetry preferences for art experts similarly to nonexperts, but experts tend to emphasize the beauty of asymmetrical depictions, eventually considering different criteria, when asked explicitly to indicate their preferences.
Article
Full-text available
The aesthetic appreciation of a picture largely depends on the perceptual balance of its elements. The underlying mental mechanisms of this relation, however, are still poorly understood. For investigating these mechanisms, objective measures of balance have been constructed, such as the Assessment of Preference for Balance (APB) score of Wilson and Chatterjee (2005). In the present study we examined the APB measure and compared it to an alternative measure (DCM; Deviation of the Center of “Mass”) that represents the center of perceptual “mass” in a picture and its deviation from the geometric center. Additionally, we applied measures of homogeneity and of mirror symmetry. In a first experiment participants had to rate the balance and symmetry of simple pictures, whereas in a second experiment different participants rated their preference (liking) for these pictures. In a third experiment participants rated the balance as well as the preference of new pictures. Altogether, the results show that DCM scores accounted better for balance ratings than APB scores, whereas the opposite held with respect to preference. Detailed analyses revealed that these results were due to the fact that aesthetic preference does not only depend on balance but also on homogeneity, and that the APB measure takes this feature into account.
Article
Full-text available
Photographic cropping is the act of selecting part of a photograph to enhance its aesthetic appearance or visual impact. It is common practice with both professional (expert) and amateur (non-expert) photographers. In a psychometric study, McManus et al. (2011b) showed that participants cropped photographs confidently and reliably. Experts tended to select details from a wider range of positions than non-experts, but other croppers did not generally prefer details that were selected by experts. It remained unclear, however, on what grounds participants selected particular details from a photograph while avoiding other details. One of the factors contributing to cropping decision may be visual saliency. Indeed, various saliency-based computer algorithms are available for the automatic cropping of photographs. However, careful experimental studies on the relation between saliency and cropping are lacking to date. In the present study, we re-analyzed the data from the studies by McManus et al. (2011a,b), focusing on statistical image properties. We calculated saliency-based measures for details selected and details avoided during cropping. As expected, we found that selected details contain regions of higher saliency than avoided details on average. Moreover, the saliency center-of-mass was closer to the geometrical center in selected details than in avoided details. Results were confirmed in an eye tracking study with the same dataset of images. Interestingly, the observed regularities in cropping behavior were less pronounced for experts than for non-experts. In summary, our results suggest that, during cropping, participants tend to select salient regions and place them in an image composition that is well-balanced with respect to the distribution of saliency. Our study contributes to the knowledge of perceptual bottom-up features that are germane to aesthetic decisions in photography and their variability in non-experts and experts.
Chapter
Full-text available
An aesthetic experience with visual art and the resulting perceived 'beauty' of it depend on a complex interaction among characteristics of the art object, the observer, and the physical, social, and historical contexts in which the experience takes place. Experimental aesthetics is devoted to the study of forms of behavior that center around observers' interactions with works of art and other aesthetic phenomena using a variety of research techniques and controlled observation. The purpose of this chapter is to provide the reader with an overview of the diversity of research techniques and procedures employed in the last decade to understand and explain the underlying processes that contribute to an observer's aesthetic experiences with a variety of visual art forms, including painting, film, photography, sculpture, cuisine, design products, and dance. The chapter is divided into the following sections to achieve its goal: artworks as stimuli, processes underlying an aesthetic experience with different visual art forms, the contribution of viewer characteristics to his or her experience with art, and the art museum as laboratory.
Article
Aesthetic appreciation of pictures partly depends on the perceptual balance of their elements. This relation has also been supported by objective measures predicting balance ratings as well as preference. Gershoni and Hochstein, however, applied these measures to Japanese calligraphies and failed to find such a relation, which questions the generality of these balance concepts. In our first experiment, we, therefore, tried to replicate these results with a slightly different method. In addition, we calculated further balance measures and collected liking ratings. As result, perceptual balance was again uncorrelated with the measures and with liking. In a second experiment, participants assessed the perceptual stability of the calligraphies, which was considered as alternative concept of balance, and their prototypicality. After discounting the effects of prototypicality on liking, there were correlations between liking and stability and between liking and one of the balance measures. However, the correlations were reliable only for atypical calligraphies.