ArticlePDF Available

The effectiveness of Nurture Groups in improving outcomes for young children with social, emotional and behavioural difficulties in primary schools: An evaluation of Nurture Group provision in Northern Ireland

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Concerns have grown regarding the increased prevalence of social, emotional and behavioural difficulties observed in young children in primary school settings. Contributory factors are multiple and varied but one consistent emphasis has been on the negative effects of children’s poor attachments with significant others which, due to contextual factors, may not have developed sufficiently. Some groups of children are more at risk of not developing strong attachments, particularly children in care whose ‘pre-care’ and ‘in care’ experiences make it more likely that their attachments will have been adversely impacted. Reflecting this increasing concern, there has been a growth of school-based interventions that aim to strengthen attachments in order that children can develop social and emotional skills; thus enabling them to be better placed to access learning opportunities and reach levels of educational achievement and attainment similar to their peers. One such intervention is Nurture Groups, modelled on attachment theory. Nurture Group provision is a short-term, schools-based intervention targeted at individual children beginning school who are already displaying social, emotional and/or behavioural difficulties. This article reports the findings of one of the first larger-scale evaluations of the effectiveness of Nurture Group provision in improving outcomes of children, and the first to explore the differential effects of Nurture Groups in relation to school-, pupil- and programme-level characteristics. The evaluation used a non-random control group design, involving a total sample of 384 children, aged 5–6 years, from 30 Nurture Group schools and a further 14 matched schools with no Nurture Group provision in Northern Ireland. The trial found effect sizes ranging in magnitude from g = 0.528 to 1.352 for a range of social, emotional and behavioural outcomes, using the Boxall Profile and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. However, no evidence of effects was found for academic outcomes. The article discusses the implications for policy and practice of the findings.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Children and Youth Services Review
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/childyouth
The eectiveness of Nurture Groups in improving outcomes for young
children with social, emotional and behavioural diculties in primary
schools: An evaluation of Nurture Group provision in Northern Ireland
Seaneen Sloan
a,b
, Karen Winter
a,
, Paul Connolly
a
, Aideen Gildea
a
a
Centre for Evidence and Social Innovation, Queens University Belfast, United Kingdom
b
School of Education, University College Dublin, Ireland
ARTICLE INFO
Keywords:
Nurture Groups
Young children
Children in care
Social, emotional and behavioural diculties
Educational intervention
ABSTRACT
Concerns have grown regarding the increased prevalence of social, emotional and behavioural diculties ob-
served in young children in primary school settings. Contributory factors are multiple and varied but one
consistent emphasis has been on the negative eects of childrens poor attachments with signicant others
which, due to contextual factors, may not have developed suciently. Some groups of children are more at risk
of not developing strong attachments, particularly children in care whose pre-careand in careexperiences
make it more likely that their attachments will have been adversely impacted. Reecting this increasing concern,
there has been a growth of school-based interventions that aim to strengthen attachments in order that children
can develop social and emotional skills; thus enabling them to be better placed to access learning opportunities
and reach levels of educational achievement and attainment similar to their peers. One such intervention is
Nurture Groups, modelled on attachment theory. Nurture Group provision is a short-term, schools-based in-
tervention targeted at individual children beginning school who are already displaying social, emotional and/or
behavioural diculties. This article reports the ndings of one of the rst larger-scale evaluations of the ef-
fectiveness of Nurture Group provision in improving outcomes of children, and the rst to explore the dier-
ential eects of Nurture Groups in relation to school-, pupil- and programme-level characteristics. The evalua-
tion used a non-random control group design, involving a total sample of 384 children, aged 56 years, from 30
Nurture Group schools and a further 14 matched schools with no Nurture Group provision in Northern Ireland.
The trial found eect sizes ranging in magnitude from g = 0.528 to 1.352 for a range of social, emotional and
behavioural outcomes, using the Boxall Prole and the Strengths and Diculties Questionnaire. However, no
evidence of eects was found for academic outcomes. The article discusses the implications for policy and
practice of the ndings.
1. Introduction
Denitions of social, emotional and behavioural diculties vary. In
a UK context, they are commonly referred to as behaviours or emotions
that deviate so much from the norm that they interfere with the childs
own growth and development and/or the lives of others(Cooper, 2017,
p. 13). International denitions emphasize similar themes, namely:
responses from children that are very dierent from normative age-
appropriate responses and that result in impairments socially, educa-
tionally and developmentally (Linsell et al., 2019; Poulou, 2015;
Wichstrøm et al., 2011). Broadly speaking, emotional diculties (also
referred to as internalising problems) include phobias, anxiety,
emotional regulation and depression, whilst behavioural diculties
(also referred to as externalising problems) refer to aggression, agi-
tation, deant, oppositional and confrontational behaviour (Poulou,
2015). Reecting changes in the dominant policy discourse, the con-
stellation of issues is now more likely to be subsumed under the broader
category of social, emotional and mental health(SEMH) rather than
social, emotional and behavioural diculties (SEBD) (Bayer, Hiscock,
Ukoumunne, Price, & Wake, 2008). Notwithstanding challenges in op-
erationalising concepts, existing evidence suggests that: social, emo-
tional, behavioural and/or mental health diculties experienced by
young children are on the increase; diculties are noticeable in pre-
school settings; they continue into primary school settings; and they can
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.104619
Received 4 September 2019; Received in revised form 14 November 2019; Accepted 15 November 2019
Corresponding author at: School of Social Sciences, Education and Social Work, Queens University Belfast, 6 College Park, Belfast BT7 1PS, Northern Ireland,
United Kingdom.
E-mail address: k.winter@qub.ac.uk (K. Winter).
Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104619
Available online 28 November 2019
0190-7409/ Crown Copyright © 2019 Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/BY-NC-ND/4.0/).
T
have adverse impacts into adulthood (Doyle, Hegarty, & Owens, 2018;
Linsell et al., 2019; Poulou, 2015).
In terms of contributory factors, there is some evidence to indicate a
connection between social, emotional and behavioural diculties and
the intrapersonal characteristics of children; including their genetics,
disposition and temperament (Wichstrøm et al., 2011). However, there
is also research illustrating that childrens social, emotional and beha-
vioural diculties may well be an understandable response by young
children to poor emotional bonds with their main carers formed in a
context of familial and social structural adversities including exposure
to: familial neglect, abuse and violence; inconsistent parenting; familial
separation/deaths; racism; poverty; and social exclusion/margin-
alisation (Doyle et al., 2018). It is known that these challenging social
circumstances can impact on attachments between primary carers and
their children and that attachments are not always secure (Linsell et al.,
2019). Furthermore, research has identied that some groups of young
children are more prone to social, emotional and behavioural dicul-
ties than others and these include young children in care (Sempik,
Ward, & Darker, 2008). For this particular group, these diculties are
exacerbated by experiences of poor attachments in the pre-care home
environment and also by the diculties forming strong, positive at-
tachments when in care, with the result that attachments are not always
secure.
1.1. Attachment theory
Attachment theory, rst proposed by Bowlby (1969, 1988), builds
upon this concern regarding the importance of emotional bonds and
focuses on how individuals respond when in need of comfort and/or
under threat. It is suggested that children, from birth, seek out the
proximity of an adult with the expectation and anticipation that they
will receive the comfort, safety and support they need. In situations
where, over time, the caregiver is attuned that is alert, responsive,
consistent and sensitive the developing attachment is more likely to
be secure. Furthermore, Bowlby (1969, 1988) contends that where
children cannot rely on their carers for regular, consistent, sensitive
responses, children have to learn ways of managing in those relation-
ships since they cannot exit them. Ainsworth and Bell (1970), building
on Bowlbys work, proposed that in such contexts, three types of at-
tachment style could be identied: secure; insecure avoidant; and in-
secure ambivalent. The latter is characterised by a child feeling anxious
when separated from their caregiver but not reassured by their return;
since their presence cannot be relied on as an indication that needs will
be met. By contrast, in relation to avoidant attachment, the child avoids
their carer.
It is suggested that secure attachments provide the secure base
from which children can develop their social and emotional skills. For
example, from a secure base, Bowlby (2008) contended that a child is
more likely to be able to: condently explore their environment; ac-
quire skills such as emotional regulation (managing stress, empathy,
controlling impulses such as anger, rage); and to acquire social skills
(establishing rapport, turn taking, listening and sharing). There is evi-
dence to suggest that where a child has not had the opportunity to form
secure attachments, their ability to soothe themselves, regulate their
emotions and form relationships has been adversely impacted (Linsell
et al., 2019). Furthermore, their self-esteem and self-condence can be
low; not believing themselves to be worthy of attention, love and care
and not believing others to be trustworthy, safe and dependable (NICE,
2015). These diculties can become amplied within school settings
where certain emotional and social skills within group settings are re-
quired to progress and where access to learning opportunities can be
hindered as the social, emotional and behavioural diculties act as
barriers to learning (Boxall, 2002; Steinsbekk & Wichstrøm, 2018).
Within this context, and as noted by Boxall (2002), issues regarding
childrens attachments are associated with wider disadvantage and
their prevalence more likely to be associated with particular subgroups,
most notably children and young people in care. Furnivall and Grant
(2014), for example, highlight the challenging circumstances faced by
this group of children in particular, that lead them to come into care,
including abuse, neglect and witnessing domestic violence that are all
then compounded by loss and trauma experienced through being re-
moved from their familial home and placed in care. Ford and Courtois
(2013) and Francis et al., (2017) note that such experiences of abuse
can aect physical health and cause hyperarousal, poor attention span
and attachment problems. To address attachment issues, a range of
interventions has been designed that seek to strengthen attachments
between children in care and their carers (Dickes, Kemmis-Riggs, &
McAloon, 2018; Kerr & Cossar, 2014). While the role played by schools
in developing secure attachments has been the subject of debate (Parker
& Levinson, 2018), it is now widely acknowledged that attachment
theory is of relevance to schools; reecting an acceptance that children
tend to have multiple attachments at any one time (Parker & Levinson,
2018) and that these may change over time. Moreover, the opportunity
to develop a secure attachment can help mitigate against attachments
that are not secure and that schools can play a pivotal role given the
amount of time children spend in school each day (Cameron & Maginn,
2011; Geddes, 2006). With this in mind, the development of schools-
based interventions that focus on childrens attachments as a means of
enabling them to better access learning opportunities are an important
development, especially for young children in care whose attachment
relationships because of abuse, neglect and challenging social cir-
cumstances are more likely to have been impaired. Nurture group
provision is among these programmes.
1.2. Nurture Group provision
Established by Marjorie Boxall, who worked as an educational
psychologist in Hackney (London) in the 1960s, Nurture Groups were
originally established in certain primary schools in socially and eco-
nomically deprived areas in London, in response to high levels of early
childhood psychosocial disorders and the attendant emotional and be-
havioural problems that was evident in some children on entry to pri-
mary school (Boxall, 2002). In identifying the source of these dicul-
ties Boxall (2002) argued that the main parent/carer-child attachment
relationship had been compromised because of the impact of challen-
ging social circumstances in the family home; the source of which could
be traced back to a range of structural factors including poverty, racism
and social marginalisation. She suggested that the resulting emotional,
behavioural and social diculties experienced by children impacted on
their ability to adjust to the demands of the classroom setting and to
access learning opportunities.
In response, Boxall designed Nurture Groups to take a small group
of children (1012 maximum) out of the mainstream classroom for a
certain period each day and for a limited length of time over the course
of one school year and, within a small group setting, to model out
positive attachment relationships and provide opportunities for social
learning and the development of emotional literacy, whilst simulta-
neously enabling children to access educational learning opportunities.
Underpinned by Bowlbys attachment theory, the role of the key adult
in the school setting was regarded as critical in terms of establishing
routines and relationships in a safe, predictable and nurturing en-
vironment. Accompanying this, the Nurture Group model placed a
strong emphasis on the physical environment, as Colley (2009: 291-2)
outlines:
The nurture room sets out to provide a safe, welcoming and caring
environment for learning and will replicate the home environment
with a comfortable seating area, a kitchen facility for preparing food
and a working area to address formal curriculum demands. A range
of activities are undertaken which aim to help the young people to
develop trust, communication skills and the growth of condence
and self-esteem. This might involve the sharing of news, emotional
S. Sloan, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104619
2
literacy sessions, turn-taking games, group activities, formal curri-
culum tasks or the nurture breakfast.
The proposed theory of change underpinning Nurture Groups is that
if childrens attachment relationships can be enhanced then their
emotional and social wellbeing will improve and this, in turn, will lead
to improved behaviour that will better place children to access learning
opportunities with the end result that their academic scores should
increase. Whilst there is some variation in Nurture Group provision
between the classic model(outlined above) and variants(Cooper,
Arnold, & Boyd, 2001), they are all underpinned by the same long-term
aim to improve childrens educational outcomes. To plot these im-
provements, Boxall developed a bespoke measure, known as the Boxall
Measure (Bennathan, 1998) which has a diagnostic and developmental
strand, to plot childrens baseline scores and progress over time. This,
combined with the use of the Strengths and Diculties Questionnaire,
have tended to be the key measures used to plot change in children
participating in Nurture Group provision to date.
Across the UK as a whole, there are currently over 2,000 Nurture
Groups UK (Nurtureuk Policy Brieng, 2019) and, in Northern Ireland,
there were, in 2016, 32 groups (https://www.theyworkforyou.com/
ni/?id=2016-11-15.1.70 accessed 28/10/19). As indicated above,
dierent variants of Nurture Group have been identied. Variant 1
Nurture Groups (the classicmodel) are classes of about 1012 children
looked after by a teacher and teaching/classroom assistant (Boxall,
2002; Cooper & Whitebread, 2007). Children spend most of the school
week in this Group, receiving highly structured and supported learning
experiences and, where possible, re-joining their mainstream class for
registration, assembly, break, lunch and home time. Typically, children
attend the Group for between two and four terms, after which the ul-
timate aim is that they can reintegrate into their mainstream class on a
full-time basis. Variant 2Nurture Groups adhere to the principles of
the classic model but dier in terms of structure and organisation
(Cooper & Whitebread, 2007). They may run on a part-time basis,
possibly involving a group of children aged 56 in the Nurture Group in
the morning sessions and involving an older group (aged 711 years) in
Nurture Group in the afternoons. Such models are often seen as a more
feasible option for schools as more pupils can be supported while, at the
same time, children spend more time accessing the mainstream curri-
culum with their peers. Both Variant 1 and 2 Nurture Groups are re-
cognised by the Nurture Group Network (NGN) as meeting the quality
standard of nurture provision. Variant 3Groups have been described
as groups radically departing from the principles and practice of Nur-
ture Provision based on, for example, lunch-time or after-school groups
that tend to focus on social and emotional issues but have no focus on
teaching the curriculum in the way that Variant 1 and 2 Groups would
do. Cooper and Whitebread (2007) dene Variant 4groups as those
that bear the name but that do not adhere to the nurture group prin-
ciples in practice and therefore essentially should not be labelled as
Nurture Groups. The Nurture Groups in Northern Ireland are based on
Variants 1 and 2.
2. Current evidence regarding the eectiveness of Nurture Group
provision
There now exists a wide-ranging literature related to Nurture
Groups. Using a thematic framework, the current literature that spans
from the mid-1970s onwards can be organised into three main cate-
gories. First are the descriptive accounts of Nurture Groups that explore
issues such as the background, theoretical underpinnings, component
parts, composition, processes, practices and intended outcomes of
Nurture Group provision (Bennathan, 1997; Bennathan & Boxall, 2000;
Bishop, 2008;Boxall, 1976;Boxall & Lucas, 2010; Cooper & Tiknaz,
2007). Second, there are studies that detail the perspectives, experi-
ences and views of parents and children (Cefai & Pizzuto, 2017; Kirk,
2018; Kirkbride, 2014; Morris, 2019). Third are the studies that
consider the eectiveness, impact and outcomes of Nurture Group
provision. It is this latter category of research study that provides the
main focus for this present article and is appraised in further detail
below. As noted in the reviews conducted by Hughes and Schlösser
(2014) and Bennett (2015), these studies typically (although not ex-
clusively) focus on children in primary schools and involve pre- and
post-tests using a range of measures including the Boxall prole
(Bennathan, 1998), the Strengths and Diculties Questionnaire
(Goodman, 1997) and the Behavioural Indicators of Self-esteem (BIOS).
With regards to the measures that have tended to be used, the Boxall
prole (Bennathan, 1998) comprises a developmental and a diagnostic
strand. The diagnostic strand contains 34 items describing behaviours
that act as barriers to the childs full and satisfactory participation in
school. Items are organised under three clusters: self-limiting features;
undeveloped behaviour; and unsupported development. The develop-
mental strand consists of 34 items describing aspects of the develop-
mental process in the early years that lays the foundation for being able
to function socially, emotionally, behaviourally and academically in
school. Items are organised under two clusters: organisation of ex-
perience; and intermalisation of controls. The measure aids in the
identication of priority areas for intervention for each child, such as
areas of social skills development, which can then be targeted de-
pending on the needs of each pupil. The Strengths and Diculties
Questionnaire (Goodman, 1997) consists of 20 statements describing
negative behaviours, which can be summed to give a total diculties
score and also broken down into the following subscales (which each
contain 5 items): conduct problems (e.g. often has temper tantrums);
emotional symptoms (e.g. often unhappy); hyperactivity (e.g. con-
stantly dgeting); and peer problems (e.g. rather solitary, tends to play
alone). The Behavioural Indicators of Self-esteem (BIOS) is used by
teachers to measure the frequency of behaviours that are associated
with positive self-esteem. It comprises 13 items which are rated from
neverto always. Scores are associated to these ratings and the
average of these scores provides the measure of self-esteem.
Regarding the studies currently available, some consider impact on
outcomes by tracking the progress of various cohorts of children but
where there is no control group (Binnie & Allen, 2008; OConnor &
Colwell, 2002). Binnie and Allen (2008), for example, reported the
ndings from a study involving 36 children (28 male and 8 female,
mean age 7 years and 2 months) attending Nurture Group provision in
six schools in one Local Authority between 2006 and 2007. The design
involved repeated measures (Boxall prole, SDQ and BIOS) with eight
months between pre and post-tests and questionnaires with staand
parents. In the ndings, the children showed statistically signicant
improvements, regarding scores obtained in the developmental strand
of the Boxall prole, with a pre-intervention mean of 79 increasing to a
post intervention mean of 114 (standard deviations were not reported
by the authors). Improvements were also noted in the diagnostic strand
of the Boxall prole, with a reported pre-intervention mean 63 com-
pared with a post-intervention mean of 35. On the BIOS, children in all
schools demonstrated a positive change in childrens self-esteem, with a
pre-intervention mean 33 and a post-intervention mean of 42. Although
there was some attrition, the teacher and parent completed SDQ
showed that parents and stareported similar positive impacts of
Nurture Group attendance on the children concerned.
Other studies track the progress of cohorts using various types of
control groups (Cooper et al., 2001; Cooper & Whitebread, 2007; Iszatt
& Wasilewska, 1997; Reynolds, MacKay, & Kearney, 2009; Saunders,
2007). An early study, often referred to, is that by Iszatt and
Wasilewska (1997). The study concerned 308 children who had been
placed in Nurture Groups between 1984 and 1988. They noted that the
vast majority (87%) were returned to their mainstream class within one
academic year. This compared very favourably with a small group of 20
non-matched pupils who were deemed suitable for Nurture Group
provision but no place was available and where higher levels of chronic
diculties were found with 35% placed in special schools and only
S. Sloan, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104619
3
55% coping well in mainstream schools. Nurture Groups appeared to
impact positively on childrens social and emotional skills and their
levels of self-condence.
A later study by Cooper et al. (2001) reported preliminary ndings
of a then ongoing longitudinal study involving 342 children (aged be-
tween 4 and 10 years) in 25 schools (23 primary and 2 secondary
schools), in eight local education authorities (LEAs). Of the 342 chil-
dren, 216 children were in Nurture Groups and their outcomes were
compared with two control groups. Control Group 1 comprised 64
matchedchildren with social, emotional and behavioural diculties
but who remained in their mainstream class within the school that was
delivering Nurture Group provision. Control Group 2 comprised 62
children without social, emotional and behavioural diculties in
mainstream classes. Teachers completed the Strengths and Diculties
Questionnaire and the Boxall prole. In the snapshot ndings, Cooper
et al. (2001) reported that children in Nurture Groups made gains
compared with children with diculties in mainstream classes and the
non-social, emotional and behavioural controls. On the SDQ, and at
entry to Nurture Group, 92% children were scored as abnormal/bor-
derline, compared with 84% of the children in Control Group 1. By the
third term (i.e. within the year), this had changed to 63% for Nurture
Group pupils compared with 75% in Control Group 1 and was noted as
statistically signicant. On the Boxall Prole, the mean improvements
from the rst test (term one) to the second test (term three) were noted
as statistically signicant. Furthermore, on the qualitative data gath-
ered from parents and teachers regarding their perceptions of the
childrens progress, Nurture Groups were positively received and be-
lieved to be making a positive impact (Cooper et al., 2001, p. 164).
More recently, research by Reynolds et al. (2009), using a quasi-
experimental research design, notwithstanding its design limitations,
has been cited as the most robust study available (EIF, 2019). A total of
221 children (142 males and 79 females, ages 57 years) were involved
in the study. They attended one of 32 schools, 16 of these had Nurture
Group provision (117 children in the study attending these) and 16
were matched schools without Nurture Group provision (104 children
attended these). The childrens outcomes were assessed via a pre-test
and post-test (with tests being 6 months apart) and using the Boxall
prole, the Strengths and Diculties Questionnaire and the Beha-
vioural Indicators of Self Esteem (BIOS). Findings indicated that: on the
Boxall Prole, signicant benets were found for the nurture groups in
comparison with the controls on all ve strands, with signicance levels
ranging from p = 0.003 to p < 0.001(Reynolds et al., 2009, p. 208).
Regarding the SDQ, the ndings indicated that there was improvement
but that this was not statistically signicant. With regards to the Be-
havioural Indicators of Self Esteem (BIOS), signicant eects were
noted for children attending the nurture group compared with those in
the control groups (p = 0.001). This study also used a Baseline As-
sessment for Early Literacy which was completed by teachers and as-
sessed basic literacy skills and early reading readiness(Reynolds et al.,
2009, p. 207). The measure has four sub sections and includes concepts
of print, phonological awareness, early reading skills and develop-
mental tasks. The ndings indicated that children attending the Nurture
Groups had made statistically signicant gains as measured by their
nal scores following their baseline assessment. Further analysis using
multiple regression to detect which variable was the best predictor of
educational attainment indicated that unsupported development (on
the Boxall measure) was the best predictor accounting for almost a
quarter of the variance(Reynolds et al., 2009, p. 208) and that together
with organisation of experience and internalisation of controls these
accounted for half of the variance in baseline improvements in this
area.
As noted by Hughes and Schlösser (2014) and Bennett (2015) in
their reviews of research regarding Nurture Group provision, the
overall message emerging from quasi experimental studies (see Table 1)
and other studies, appears to suggest that: Nurture Group provision has
positive impacts on childrens social, emotional and behavioural
diculties; that improvements made are generally quite well-sustained
over time; and that because children are better able to access the cur-
riculum, they make related gains in achievement and attainment.
However, there are notable limitations to the current evidence base,
including the fact that there are currently no randomised controlled
trials of the eectiveness of Nurture Group provision. Furthermore, of
the evidence that does exist, studies that include control groups are still
limited in number. Even then, of those with control groups, most are
small scale studies with only a handful of studies having a sample size
of over 200 children. Moreover, and for these studies, there is little
reported evidence of sub group analyses. The study reported in this
article seeks to address some of these limitations by reporting the
ndings of a larger-scale non-randomised control group trial involving
384 children from 44 primary schools in Northern Ireland where the
control group comprised matched schools and where sub analyses was
undertaken. Although not reported here (due to word restrictions), the
study also included a secondary data analysis, a process evaluation and
a cost eectiveness analysis (Anon, 2016), therefore making this one of
the most detailed studies available on the implementation, experience
and impact of Nurture Group provision.
3. The present study
In Northern Ireland there are a number of established Nurture
Groups that have been operating for many years, with some schools
self-funding or accessing funds through the former Department for
Social Development (now known as the Department for Communities
(Northern Ireland)) and the Neighbourhood Renewal Investment Fund.
In 2012, the Oce of the First Minister and Deputy First Minister an-
nounced funding for 20 new Nurture Groups, through the governments
Delivering Social Change (DSC) Signature Projects. Furthermore, the
Department of Education Northern Ireland (DENI) also invested funds
for the continued provision of 10 established Nurture Groups in schools
where funding was coming to an end. To ensure a consistent approach
to the set up and delivery of nurture provision, Nurture Guidelines
developed collaboratively were published and, as part of the Signature
Project, each region of the Education Authority received funding
through DENI for a Nurture Support Ocer/Nurture Advisor and
Educational Psychologist hours to provide support and advice to the
Signature Project schools.
3.1. The research design and ethical approval
This present article reports the ndings of a non-randomised con-
trolled group trial evaluation of Nurture Group provision in Northern
Ireland, funded by DENI. The trial formed part of a larger study that
also included: secondary analysis of existing data on the progress of
children that had previously participated in Nurture Groups; a cost-ef-
fectiveness analysis; and a qualitative process evaluation involving in-
terviews with key professionals, parents and children as well as ob-
servations of the Nurture Groups in practice. The aims and objectives of
the research were to: assess the eectiveness of nurture provision in
improving child social, emotional and behavioural development, and
ability to learn, both within the Nurture Group and following re-
integration with the mainstream class; and to assess the cost-eec-
tiveness of nurture provision in achieving its objectives. The research,
which took place in 20152016 and was approved by the research
teams institutional research ethics committee.
The trial component of the study, that provides the focus for this
present article, tracked 384 pupils attending nurture groups in 30
schools during one school year (2014/2015) and compared those with a
control group of pupils who had a similar level of need from 14 mat-
ched schools without nurture provision. These schools were identied
from the list of schools that satised the original criteria for allocation
of Signature Project funding (i.e. schools with above average proportion
of pupils eligible for free school meals, below average attendance,
S. Sloan, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104619
4
below average attainment at ages 56 and 1011 years, when tests are
undertaken, and above average numbers of children with a statement of
special educational needs). The recruitment of participants and their
ow through the trial is illustrated by Fig. 1.
Table 2 compares the intervention and control groups in relation to
key demographic characteristics. As can be seen, there were some
(statistically signicant) dierences between the two groups, with the
intervention group tending to have a higher proportion of children that
were: eligible for free school meals; looked after; known to social ser-
vices; and/or on the child protection register. In contrast, a higher
proportion of pupils in the control group were from minority ethnic
backgrounds and had English as an additional language. As described
further below, these dierences were controlled for in the analysis by
including dummy variables for each of these characteristics listed in
Table 1 as covariates in the multilevel regression models tted.
3.2. Outcome measures
For comparability, the outcome measures selected were: the Boxall
measure (using both its developmental and diagnostic components); the
Strengths and Diculties Questionnaire (SDQ); data held by the schools
regarding tracking assessment scores; and a bespoke measure for chil-
dren to indicate school enjoyment. Whilst widely use for previous stu-
dies of Nurture Groups, there is currently no reliable psychometric data
reported on the Boxall measure (EEF, 2019). However, there is some
evidence to suggest that it has strong concurrent validity when com-
pared to the SDQ (Couture, Cooper, & Royer, 2019). Using the baseline
data collected for this present study, the ve sub-scales were found to
reliable (alpha ranging from 0.632 to 0.906) and the two main com-
ponents were highly reliable (alpha = 0.931 and 0.919 respectively).
In contrast, the SDQ is a widely-used outcome measure with strong
and consistent psychometric properties (EEF, 2019; Husky et al., 2018).
The SDQ has ve sub-scales and four of these focus on negative beha-
viour (conduct problems, emotional symptoms, peer problems and
hyperactivity) and combine to create a total diculties score. The fth
sub-scale, provides a stand-alone measure of prosocial behaviour.
Analysis of the baseline data for this present study indicated that the
main total diculties score and prosocial behaviour scores were reli-
able (alpha = 0.798 and 0.818 respectively) as were the four sub-do-
mains (alpha ranging from 0.654 to 0.843). For the target age range of
this present study, the SDQ has versions for teachers and parents to
complete. The teacher version was used to minimise missing data.
Enjoyment of School was measured by children completing a be-
spoke self-report on the following 11 aspects of school: reading; writing;
spelling; numeracy; using the computer/iPad; working by yourself;
outdoor play with your class; break/lunchtime in the playground; lunch
time in the dinner hall; golden time; and coming to school. Children
were invited to rate each aspect on a 5-point scale by point to or putting
a circle around one of a set of ve smiley facesand labelled from dont
like it at all(scored 1)t
olike it very much(scored 5), meaning that
total scores could range from 11 to 55. Inspection of the baseline data
indicates that the scale as a whole has good reliability (alpha = 0.799).
Finally, the data derived from schools comprised results of assess-
ment tests carried out with children at Key Stages 1 and 2 on com-
munication, using mathematics and using ICT.
With regards to the analysis, baseline dierences between pupils in
the intervention and control group (in terms of both core characteristics
and baseline scores on measures of social, emotional and behavioural
functioning, enjoyment of school, attendance and academic attainment
in literacy and numeracy) were explored. Raw changes in these out-
come measures from pre-test to post-test in both the intervention group
and control group were then examined and compared. For the main
analysis, data were analysed in a series of multi-level statistical models
for each outcome to account for the clustered nature of the data
(children clustered within schools). These models compared post-test
mean scores for those in Nurture Groups with those in the control
group, accounting for any dierences at baseline in terms of pre-test
scores and other key pupil characteristics (i.e. gender, year group, free
Table 1
A summary of the ndings of evaluations of Nurture Groups employing quasi experimental designs.
a
Study (year) Location Design Sample Developmental
Pre-post scores
Eect size Diagnostic
Pre-post scores
Eect size
The Present Study Northern
Ireland
Single-group pre-post-test; average
time between assessments = 3 terms
N = 507 pupils in 27
schools
Pre: 77.28
Post: 108.96
+1.49 Pre: 53.17
Post: 26.13
0.97
Shaver and McClatchey
(2013)
Northern
Scotland
Single-group pre-post-test; 8 weeks to
1 year between assessments
N = 32 pupils in 2
schools
Age not reported
Pre: 89.21
Post: 112.10
+0.88 Pre: 33.18
Post: 22.77
0.31
Reynolds et al. (2009) Glasgow Pre-post-test with matched control
group; 6 months between assessments
N = 97 pupils in 16
schools
b
Age 57 years
Pre: 81.25
Post: 102.10
*Pre: 41.11
Post: 28.20
*
Binnie and Allen (2008) West Lothian Single-group pre-post-test; 8 months
between assessments
N = 36 pupils in 6
schools
Age 510 years
Pre: 79
Post: 114
*Pre: 63
Post: 35
*
Cooper and Whitebread
(2007)
Various sites,
England
Pre-post-test with matched control
group; time between assessments
ranges from 2 to 4 terms
N = 359 pupils in 23
schools
b
Age 414 years
After 2 terms
(n = 253):
Pre: 77.92
Post: 96.19
After 4 terms
(n = 86):
Pre: 73.37
Post: 105.53
+0.75
+1.07
After 2 terms
(n = 253):
Pre: 51.58
Post: 41.34
After 4 terms
(n = 86):
Pre: 51.24
Post: 33.72
0.34
0.68
OConnor and Colwell
(2002)
London Single-group pre-post-test; average of 3
terms between assessments
N = 68 pupils in 5
schools
Mean age = 5.25 years
Pre: 71.22
Post: 110.16
+1.66 Pre: 49.15
Post: 23.43
0.63
Cooper et al. (2001) Various sites,
England/
Wales
Pre-post-test with matched control
group; average time between
assessments = 2 terms
N = 216 pupils in 25
schools
Age 410 years
Pre: 77.14
Post: 95.21
+0.73 Pre: 50.83
Post: 39.98
0.38
a
Published UK research by Sanders (2007); Seth-Smith et al. (2010) and Scott and Lee (2009) are not included as raw mean scores were not reported.
b
Only intervention group data reported.
* Standard deviations not reported in original publication therefore eect size could not be calculated.
S. Sloan, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104619
5
school meal eligibility, neighbourhood deprivation, looked-after status,
if pupils family was known to social services). Eect sizes (Hedgesg)
were calculated as the standardised mean dierence in outcomes be-
tween the intervention and control groups. Hedgesg was used in
preference to Cohens d as it is a more reliable measure of standardised
mean eects for smaller sample sizes. These post-test mean scores were
estimated from the regression models and thus were adjusted for any
dierences at pre-test. In addition, each model was extended by
including additional interaction terms to assess whether there was
evidence of dierential eects of Nurture Groups in relation to a set of
school-level and child-level characteristics. These are explained further
in the next section.
For the purposes of the main analysis, the two interventions group
(Fig. 1) were combined to ensure improved statistical power for com-
parisons between those receiving Nurture Group provision (whether via
the existing Nurture Groups or the new Groups funded through the
Signature Project) and the control group. However, potential dier-
ences between the two intervention groups were explored as part of the
further sub-group analysis and no evidence was found that the eects
associated with both intervention groups diered (see Table 7 reported
later).
4. Results
Table 3 provides a summary of simple descriptive statistics from the
trial of the mean scores (with standard deviations) for the intervention
and control groups for each of the outcome variables at pre-test and
post-test. In comparing the pre-test mean scores of both groups, it can
be seen than the core developmental, social and educational outcomes
for those in the intervention group tended to be slightly lower than
those in the control group. Again, these dierences were eectively
controlled for in the main analysis as described above by including the
childrens pre-test scores as covariates in each model.
In considering the gains made by children during the period of the
trial, a visual inspection of the progress made by both groups from pre-
Intervenon Group 2:
Pupils (n=66) from 10 funded
‘established’ Nurture Groups
Control Group:
Pupils (n=88) from 14 schools
meeng the Signature Project
criteria but with no nurture
provision
All primary schools in NI
assessed against Nurture
Unit Signature Project
eligibility criteria
Schools meeng criteria
(N=44) ranked according to
%FSM
20 schools with highest
%FSM oered funding for
nurture unit
Intervenon Group 1:
Pupils (n=232) from 20
Signature Project Nurture
Groups
Boxall Prole at pre-test
(n=232) and post-test
(n=197)
SDQ at pre-test (n=219) and
post-test (n=163)
Enjoyment of School at pre-
test (n=106) and post-test
(n=137)
Boxall Prole at pre-test (n=63)
and post-test (n=56)
SDQ at pre-test (n=45) and
post-test (n=46)
Enjoyment of School at pre-
test (n=57) and post-test
(n=61)
Boxall Prole at pre-test (n=86)
and post-test (n=84)
SDQ at pre-test (n=86) and
post-test (n=84)
Enjoyment of School at pre-
test (n=84) and post-test
(n=82)
10 primary schools already
funded under the exisng
Nurture Group provision in
Northern Ireland
14 primary schools
purposively selected from
the remaining sample of
schools that met the Nurture
Unit Signature Project criteria
but were not selected and
had no nurture provision
Fig. 1. Flow diagram of recruitment and data collection.
Table 2
Comparison of the characteristics of the intervention group and control group
(%).
Pupils Intervention
Group
Control
Group
Sig.
a
Male 64 70 0.359
Eligible for FSM 87 66 < 0.001
EAL 3 15 < 0.001
Non-white 5 22 < 0.001
Looked after 8 3 0.185
Known to social services 36 16 < 0.001
On Child Protection Register 9 2 0.043
Special Educational
Needs
Not on SEN
register
3 29 < 0.001
Stage 1 12 19
Stage 2 54 17
Stage 3 26 24
Stage 454 5
a
Based on chi-squared tests.
S. Sloan, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104619
6
test to post-test would appear to indicate notable (and statistically
signicant) positive improvements for the children in the intervention
group across all of the social, emotional and behavioural outcomes;
with reductions in negative behaviours and increases in prosocial be-
haviour. This compared to limited evidence of change for those in the
control group. Interestingly, there only appeared to be smaller (and
non-signicant) changes in academic outcomes; namely their enjoy-
ment of school (measured using the total score) and their literacy and
numeracy outcomes (using school-assessed key stage results).
As noted above, the data were more formally analysed using mul-
tilevel regression models and the ndings from these are summarised in
Table 4 that presents the adjusted mean post-test scores for the inter-
vention and control groups, estimated using the multilevel models.
As can be seen from Table 4, there is clear and consistent evidence
of improvements in social, emotional and behavioural outcomes for
children attending Nurture Groups compared to those in the control
group, using both the Boxall Prole and the Strengths and Diculties
Questionnaire (SDQ). Typically, eect sizes of around 0.2 are con-
sidered to be small, those around 0.5 to be mediumand those above
0.8 to be large(Cohen, 1977). It can be seen from Table 4 that the
eects associated with attending Nurture Groups were found to be large
for the developmental strand (g = 1.352) and the diagnostic prole
(g = -0.904) of Boxall and the total diculties score (g = -1.303) and
prosocial scores (g = 0.926) of the SDQ. Interestingly, there is no
evidence of an eect for Nurture Groups on the childrens academic
outcomes in literacy or numeracy.
In addition, exploratory analyses were undertaken to explore whe-
ther Nurture Groups had dierential impacts for: boys and girls; chil-
dren with English as an additional language; from areas with diering
levels of deprivation; children known to socials services, looked after
by social services; and at dierent stages of the Codes of Practice. The
exploratory analysis also examined whether the Nurture Group provi-
sion had dierential eects in relation to schools with diering pro-
portions of children eligible for free school meals; children with special
education needs; diering levels of deprivation; and diering sizes.
Analyses also took account of dierential eects regarding full/part
time Nurture Group provision; size of Nurture Group and how long it
had been established; and type.
In relation to school level variables, the evidence of possible sub-
group dierences are summarised in Table 5, by simply reporting the
statistical signicance of the interaction term from each of the multi-
level regression models in turn. Given the multiple statistical tests un-
dertaken, and thus the increased risk of Type I errors, these ndings are
presented simply to provide an overall sense of the potential existence
of sub-group dierences. It would be potentially misleading to use the
current analysis to generate more specic summary statistics for any of
the potential interaction eects found, especially given that many of
these rely on smaller sub-samples.
It can be seen that there was some potential evidence that the size of
the school may be a mediating variable. Further analysis of the tted
models suggested that there was an inverse relationship between school
size and amount of progress, in that pupils in larger schools made less
Table 3
Comparison of mean pre-test and post-test scores for the intervention and control groups for each outcome.
Outcome Intervention
Mean (sd)
Eect size (d)
[sig]
Control
Mean (sd)
Eect size (d)
[Sig]
Pre Post Pre Post
Boxall Prole
Developmental Strand 74.11 (19.78) 110.05 (18.41) +1.817
[p < .001]
84.29 (24.35) 85.04 (23.35) -0.031
[p = .686]
Diagnostic Prole 54.13 (25.36) 25.53 (21.91) 1.128
[p < .001]
47.43 (29.44) 46.76 (27.44) -0.023
[p = .746]
Organisation of experience 37.65 (12.10) 58.10 (11.02) +1.690
[p < .001]
45.27 (13.26) 44.72 (12.73) -0.041
[p = 625]
Internalisation of controls 36.50 (10.41) 52.00 (10.16) +1.489
[p < .001]
39.01 (13.14) 40.31 (12.05) +0.099
[p = .157]
Self-limiting features 12.31 (5.15) 5.87
(4.60)
1.250
[p < .001]
9.20
(5.07)
9.68 (5.66) +0.094
[p = .292]
Undeveloped behaviours 13.30 (8.15) 6.18
(6.43)
-0.873
[p < .001]
11.06 (8.31) 10.95
(7.58)
-0.013
[p = .874]
Unsupported development 28.17 (17.01) 13.48 (13.42) -0.884
[p < .001]
27.17 (16.60) 26.13 (17.70) -0.054
[p = .418]
Strengths and Diculties Questionnaire
Total Diculties 19.64 (5.75) 10.30 (6.08) 1.622
[p < .001]
17.54 (6.79) 17.65 (5.64) +0.018
[p = .815]
Conduct problems 3.42
(2.59)
1.67 (2.14) -0.681
[p < .001]
3.26 (2.63) 3.33 (2.65) +0.027
[p = .627]
Emotional symptoms 4.64
(3.00)
2.20 (2.16) -0.813
[p < .001]
3.92 (3.53) 4.25 (2.97) +0.094
[p = .236]
Peer problems 3.95
(2.26)
2.20 (2.10) -0.852
[p < .001]
2.90 (2.11) 3.24 (2.12) 0.158
[p = .126]
Hyperactivity 7.62
(2.56)
4.41 (2.88) 1.256
[p < .001]
7.45 (2.69) 6.83 (2.25) -0.230
[p = .003]
Prosocial behaviour 4.28
(2.74)
7.03 (2.53) +1.008
[p < .001]
5.01 (2.69) 5.11 (2.78) +0.035
[p = .675]
Academic outcomes
Enjoyment of school 42.26 (9.41) 45.45 (7.83) +0.338
[p < .001]
44.61 (7.51) 43.29 (7.89) 0.211
[p = .103]
Attendance 90.39 (8.91) 93.08 (5.69) +0.303
[p < .001]
89.06 (13.43) 90.85 (8.79) +0.134
[p = .122]
Literacy 80.19 (9.96) 82.85 (9.34) +0.267
[p = .054]
82.00 (7.78) 78.50 (9.15) 0.450
[p = .001]
Numeracy 82.74 (12.34) 87.00 (11.05) +0.345
(p = .077)
78.57 (9.89) 79.00 (10.80) 0.043
[p = .890]
S. Sloan, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104619
7
progress compared to those in smaller schools. In terms of the Peer
problems subscale of the SDQ, Nurture Group pupils in larger schools
tended to have more peer problems at post-test compared to the control
group, whereas Nurture Group pupils in smaller or medium schools
tended to have fewer peer problems compared to the control group. In
contrast, and as can also be seen, there was no evidence found of any
mediating eects for: the proportion of children at school eligible for
free school meals: the proportion of pupils with special educational
needs; or the average level of deprivation for pupils at the school.
With regards to pupil level variables, the ndings are presented in a
similar format in Table 6. In this case, evidence was explored as to
whether there was any evidence to suggest that the eects of Nurture
Groups varied in relation to: a childs gender; their age; their rst
language; their levels of familial deprivation; whether they were
looked afteror known to social services; their special educational
needs stage; and their behaviour when entering the Nurture Group
(measured by their baseline score). Findings revealed that Nurture
Groups had similar positive eects for pupils, on average, regardless of
their individual characteristics. One exception to this was while pupils
were likely to make positive progress regardless of their baseline score,
the evidence would appear to suggest that, for most outcomes, pupils
with lower scores at baseline made more progress compared to those
with higher scores. In addition, there is perhaps some evidence to
suggest that Nurture Groups were having some dierential eects for
boys and girls with a larger eect in terms of the reduction of total
diculties for girls compared to boys. Within this, the dierence ap-
pears to be explained largely in terms of gender dierences in emo-
tional symptoms and peer problems.
Lastly, Nurture Group eects were explored (see Table 7). Findings
indicate that pupils made similar progress across outcomes independent
Table 4
The adjusted post-test mean scores (and standard deviations) for the intervention and control groups and their associated eect sizes for each outcome.
Outcome Adjusted post-test means
(standard deviation)
Sig. Eect size (Hedgesg)
[95% CI]
Intervention Control
Boxall Prole
Developmental Strand 110.70
(18.46)
84.04
(23.35)
< 0.001 +1.352
[+0.098, +1.728]
Diagnostic Prole 25.94
(21.97)
47.13
(27.67)
< 0.001 0.904
[1.251, 0.557]
Organisation of experience 58.70
(11.03)
43.74
(12.73)
< 0.001 +1.306
[+0.913, +1.708]
Internalisation of controls 52.03
(9.19)
40.40
(12.05)
< 0.001 +1.170
[+0.843, +1.497]
Self-limiting features 5.79
(4.61)
10.09
(5.66)
< 0.001 0.882
[1.312, 0.452]
Undeveloped behaviours 6.24
(6.46)
10.86
(7.58)
< 0.001 0.685
[1.002, 0.369]
Unsupported development 13.88
(13.44)
25.84
(17.70)
< 0.001 0.821
[1.133, 0.511]
Strength and Diculties Questionnaire
Total diculties 10.07
(6.05)
17.80
(5.64)
< 0.001 1.303
[1.696, 0.909]
Conduct Problems 1.68
(2.00)
3.09
(2.65)
< 0.001 0.638
[0.926, 0.350]
Emotional Symptoms 2.24
(2.15)
4.33
(2.97)
< 0.001 0.865
[1.242, 0.489]
Peer Problems 1.95
(2.10)
3.35
(2.12)
< 0.001 0.663
[1.045, 0.281]
Hyperactivity 4.15
(2.88)
7.12
(2.25)
< 0.001 1.093
[1.445, 0.740]
Prosocial Behaviour 7.33
(2.53)
4.92
(2.78)
< 0.001 +0.926
[+0.571, +0.1.281]
Academic outcomes
Enjoyment of School 46.54
(7.44)
42.62
(7.42)
0.002 +0.528
[+0.199, +0.857]
Attendance rate 93.02
(5.72)
91.18
(6.52)
0.101 +0.308
[0.060, +0.675]
Literacy 87.54
(11.39)
80.74
(17.41)
0.230 +0.559
[0.354, 1.472]
Numeracy 85.43
(12,15)
86.94
(13.70)
0.822 0.119
[1.154, 0.915]
Table 5
A summary of the evidence of possible dierential eects of Nurture Groups in
relation to school-level mediating variables.
Outcome Mediating Variables Explored
a
School %
FSM
School %
SEN
School
deprivation
School size
Boxall Prole
Developmental 0.682 0.141 0.266 0.005
Diagnostic 0.897 0.182 0.775 0.023
Strengths and Diculties Questionnaire
Total diculties 0.911 0.048 0.499 0.049
Conduct Problems 0.873 0.202 0.650 0.388
Emotional Symptoms 0.580 0.051 0.495 0.181
Peer Problems 0.298 0.585 0.181 < 0.001
Hyperactivity 0.765 0.057 0.476 0.046
Prosocial Behaviour 0.664 0.864 0.627 0.990
Education-Related
Enjoyment of
School
0.529 0.572 0.878 0.959
Attendance 0.302 0.187 0.208 0.317
a
Statistical signicance of interaction terms.
S. Sloan, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104619
8
of whether they attended full-time or part-time or in relation to the
length of time the Nurture Group had been running or whether it was
part of the Signature Project or an existing Group.
5. Limitations of the study and discussion
This paper has presented the ndings of a non-randomised, control
group trial evaluation of the eects of Nurture Group provision on
outcomes for primary school aged children in Northern Ireland. The
study makes a signicant contribution to the national and international
literature in the area, representing one of only a handful of larger-scale
non-randomised control of Nurture Group provision which, combined
with a secondary data analyses, a cost eectiveness analysis and a
process evaluation (reported elsewhere) provides one of the most de-
tailed analyses available adding to existing knowledge.
Notwithstanding the limitations to the methodology employed for this
present study, most notably the fact that the study was not a rando-
mised controlled trial and that measures employed rely on teacher
ratings, the ndings would appear to be encouraging and contributing
as they do to a body of related evidence that indicates positive eects.
Moreover, the size of the eects found from this present study are re-
latively large; typically suggesting improvements of around one stan-
dard deviation across a range of developmental outcomes. Furthermore,
and unique to this present study, there is signicant potential for NG
provision to benet looked-after children in particular, given that they
tend to have the lowest developmental outcomes currently and that our
evidence suggested that NGs achieve the highest eects for those with
the lowest baseline scores.
In relation to these positive ndings, four points are worthy of
further consideration. First, although our own study reports positive
eects, there remains further work required to help ascertain exactly
why and how these positive eects have occurred. All the literature on
Nurture Group provision stresses the signicance of positive attach-
ments for children in a classroom setting. Within this context, the study
serves as an important reminder that attachment theory has evolved
away from an emphasis solely on the main child-carer attachment to a
consideration of childrens attachments in their wider context where it
is possible for children to experience multiple attachments with people
in dierent positions and relationships around them including teachers
(Bombèr, 2007; Geddes, 2006; Kennedy & Kennedy, 2004). If it is in-
deed positive attachments that make the dierence, further theorisation
and exploration of these in a school context is needed. Making a notable
contribution to this is the English-based Attachment Aware Schools
programme that combined a whole-school training programme re-
garding attachment, attunement and trauma informed practice (and
associated strategies, in particular the use of Emotion Coaching) with
the testing of the eectiveness of attachment based interventions to
young children (not just those in care) including Nurture Groups and
Theraplay. Findings from the independent evaluations (Dingwall &
Sebba, 2018; 2018a; Fancourt & Sebba, 2018) and researchers involved
in the programme (Rose, McGuire-Snieckus, Gilbert, & McInnes, 2019)
indicate positive ndings worthy of further research.
Second, if positive attachments can be formed in schools, the
question remains as to how specically do they relate to improvements
in academic performance and achievements? In this present study there
was no evidence that Nurture Groups were having an eect on atten-
dance or academic outcomes compared to those attending control
schools. However, Nurture Group children did report signicantly
greater enjoyment of school compared to pupils in the control group. It
is therefore plausible that improvements in academic attainment could
be more medium to longer-term outcomes of Nurture Group provision
that follow once engagement with learning and school in general is
achieved. As such, further research is required to track educational
attainment and achievement outcomes over longer periods of time.
Third, and arising from our study and related studies, is the broader
context regarding the development of secure attachments. Schools do
not operate in isolation and rather than Nurture Groups operating on a
decit model (e.g. teachers making up for poor attachments at home),
eorts are put into practical approaches so that schools and parents can
Table 6
A summary of evidence of possible dierential eects of Nurture Groups in relation to pupil-level mediating variables.
Outcome Mediating Variables Explored
a
Boys vs Girls Year group EAL Deprivation LAC Known to SS SEN Stage Baseline score
Boxall Prole
Developmental Strand 0.528 0.087 0.606 0.294 0.391 0.352 0.948 < 0.001
Diagnostic Prole 0.198 0.977 0.663 0.953 0.449 0.287 0.727 0.006
Strengths and Diculties Questionnaire
Total Diculties 0.013 0.528 0.990 0.928 0.360 0.979 0.929 0.118
Conduct problems 0.513 0.673 0.956 0.213 0.499 0.057 0.151 < 0.001
Emotional symptoms < 0.001 0.617 0.605 0.061 0.429 0.603 0.484 < 0.001
Peer problems 0.008 0.184 0.278 0.940 0.129 0.887 0.640 0.049
Hyperactivity 0.346 0.796 0.537 0.702 0.822 0.575 0.988 0.236
Prosocial behaviour 0.149 0.110 0.769 0.174 0.802 0.996 0.656 0.002
Education-Related
Enjoyment of School 0.563 0.834 0.508 0.811 0.354 0.259 0.096 0.146
Attendance 0.936 0.538 0.647 0.114 0.710 0.445 0.825 0.024
a
Statistical signicance of interaction terms.
Table 7
A summary of evidence of possible dierential eects of Nurture Groups in
relation to programme characteristics variables.
Outcome Nurture group (NG) characteristics explored
a
Full-time Years NG
running
NG size Signature Project
NG
Boxall Prole
Developmental Strand 0.605 0.859 0.275 0.874
Diagnostic Prole 0.770 0.932 0.807 0.604
Strengths and Diculties Questionnaire
Total Diculties 0.613 0.166 0.450 0.983
Conduct problems 0.250 0.278 0.100 0.774
Emotional symptoms 0.098 0.436 0.847 0.498
Peer problems 0.682 0.792 0.066 0.329
Hyperactivity 0.914 0.103 0.107 0.753
Prosocial behaviour 0.946 0.354 0.162 0.931
Education-Related
Enjoyment of
School
0.214 0.890 0.331 0.633
Attendance 0.500 0.588 0.644 0.349
a
Statistical signicance of interaction terms.
S. Sloan, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104619
9
together build positive and strong attachments with children. One of
the relatively unexplored elements of Nurture Group provision, as
noted in the Nurture Group process evaluation that accompanied our
own study, was the importance of the relationship between the Nurture
Group teacher and teaching assistant and the parents/main carers.
Rather than keep parents at arms length, the Nurture Groups in our
study, proactively sought to build and nurture their relationships with
parents/main carers, spending time with them, inviting them in to
breakfast sessions and other events, to support them and to equip them
with the strategies and techniques being employed in the classroom
setting that could also be used in the family home.
The role of parent/main carer/signicant other is an important and
yet relatively unexplored area. It is all the more important in the con-
text of particular groups of young children, most notably those in care.
The English NICE guidelines Childrens Attachment (NG26, NICE, 2015)
make specic recommendations regarding how best to support children
in care with attachment diculties in schools. These include making
training on attachment available for all stawho come into contact
with children in care; and ensuring that all stawork together re-
garding the management of and interventions regarding the attachment
diculties. At present, it is not clear that social workers, care givers,
educational and indeed other professionals working with children in
care have had the opportunity to: a) be made fully aware of the im-
portance of attachment theory; b) receive adequate training in the core
principles of attachment theory; c) develop a working knowledge of
school based interventions that might assist the children they are re-
sponsible for; and to d) develop a meaningful relationship with a par-
ticular school to support the delivery of an intervention. This is an area
that requires attention and it seems imperative that interdisciplinary
training (and indeed refresher training) on attachment relationships
should form part of the bread and butterof carers and/or profes-
sionalstraining proles; that there should be renewed eorts to
strengthen the daily, lived out relationships between educational,
health, social work professionals and carers and that ndings from
practice developments should be made available in shared inter-
disciplinary fora. This may help prevent missed opportunities for chil-
dren in care to benet from developments in educational practice and
will build on the work that highlights the importance of this issue and
the calls for further evidence (Fernandez, 2019).
Fourth and regarding evidence, it is the case that there are no
randomised controlled trials regarding Nurture Groups. It is noted by
Reynolds et al. (2009) that it is challenging to undertake a full trial
given the number of Nurture Groups that have been established and the
diculties then caused in the random allocation of children to control
and intervention groups. This was certainly the case in Northern Ireland
where decisions had already been taken in relation to the provision of
existing Nurture Groups and a second round of additional Groups under
the Signature Project. The selection of such schools was already made
before the current research team were commissioned, and hence the
impossibility of applying a randomised controlled trial (RCT) design in
this present case. This, combined with the pre-determined teacher rated
measures, are limitations to the study. It is worth stressing that this
inability to use an RCT design is a signicant limitation and yet it is
avoidable if commissioners and deliverers of services engage with re-
searchers at an earlier stage. In most cases, including this present one, it
would have been possible to establish a robust RCT design for the
evaluation of Nurture Groups in Northern Ireland whilst still ensuring
that the level of provision made was maintained and was still targeted
at those schools in most need. This is a critical point that has implica-
tions far beyond this present evaluation regarding the urgent need for
much better coordination and joined-up approaches between govern-
ments and service providers and independent research teams if we are
to build a strong and robust evidence-based for social and educational
interventions.
Funding
The research study was funded by Department of Education
Northern Ireland.
CRediT authorship contribution statement
Seaneen Sloan: Conceptualization, Methodology, Funding acqui-
sition, Investigation, Formal analysis, Writing - original draft. Karen
Winter: Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, Writing - ori-
ginal draft, Writing - review & editing. Paul Connolly:
Conceptualization, Methodology, Funding acquisition, Formal analysis,
Writing - review & editing. Aideen Gildea: Conceptualization,
Methodology, Investigation, Writing - review & editing.
Declaration of Competing Interest
The authors have no competing interests.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.104619.
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bell, S. M. (1970). Attachment, exploration, and separation:
Illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Child Development,
41,4967.
Anon (2016). The Impact and Cost Eectiveness of Nurture Groups in Primary Schools in
Northern Ireland. Centre for Evidence and Social Innovation/Queen's. Belfast; Northern
Ireland: University Belfast/Department of Education.
Bayer, J. K., Hiscock, H., Ukoumunne, O. C., Price, A., & Wake, M. (2008). Early child-
hood aetiology of mental health problems: A longitudinal population-based study.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(11), 11661174. https://doi.org/10.
1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01943.x.
Bennathan, M. (1997). Eective intervention in primary schools: What nurture groups
achieve. Emotional and Behavioural Diculties, 2(3), 2329.
Bennathan, M. (1998). The boxall prole: Handbook for teachers. Maidstone, UK: AWCEBD.
Bennathan, M., & Boxall, M. (2000). Nurture groups: Eective intervention in primary
schools. London: David Fulton.
Bennett, H. (2015). Results of the systematic review on nurture groupseectiveness. The
International Journal of Nurture in Education, 1(1), 37. https://www.nurtureuk.org/
sites/default/les/the_international_journal-vol1.pdf#page=5.
Binnie, L. M., & Allen, K. (2008). Whole school support for vulnerable children: The
evaluation of a part-time nurture group. Emotional and Behavioural Diculties, 13(3),
201216. https://doi.org/10.1080/13632750802253202.
Bishop, S. (2008). Running a nurture group. Sage.
Bombèr, L. M. (2007). Inside I'm hurting: Practical strategies for supporting children with
attachment diculties in schools. Worth.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Volume I: Attachment. Attachment and loss:
Volume I: Attachment (pp. 1401). London: The Hogarth Press and the Institute of
Psycho-Analysis.
Bowlby, J. (1988). Developmental psychiatry comes of age. The American Journal of
Psychiatry.
Bowlby, J. (2008). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development.
Basic books.
Boxall, M. (1976). The Nurture Group in the Primary School. Inner London Education
Authority.
Boxall, M. (2002). Nurture groups in school: Principles & practice. Sage.
Boxall and Lucas (2010).
Cameron, S., & Maginn, C. (2011). Living psychology: The 'emotional warmth' dimension
of professional childcare. Educational and Child Psychology, 28(3), 44.
Cefai, C., & Pizzuto, S. A. S. (2017). Listening to the voices of young children in a nurture
class. Emotional and Behavioural Diculties, 22(3), 248260. https://doi.org/10.
1080/13632752.2017.1331987.
Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. London: Routledge.
Colley, D. (2009). Nurture groups in secondary schools. Emotional and Behavioural
Diculties, 14(4), 291300. https://doi.org/10.1080/13632750903303120.
Cooper, P. (2017). Evidence based approaches to mental health issues in schools. In C.
Cefai, & P. Cooper (Eds.). Mental Health Promotion in Schools. Cross Cultural Narratives
and Perspectives (pp. 1125). Rotterdam: SensePublishers.
Cooper, P., Arnold, R., & Boyd, E. (2001). The eectiveness of Nurture Groups:
Preliminary research ndings. British Journal of Special Education, 28(4), 160166.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8527.t01-1-00219.
Cooper, P., & Tiknaz, Y. (2007). Nurture Groups in school and at home: Connecting with
children with social, emotional and behavioural diculties,Vol. 1. Jessica Kingsley
Publishers.
S. Sloan, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104619
10
Cooper, P., & Whitebread, D. (2007). The eectiveness of nurture groups on student
progress: Evidence from a national research study. Emotional and Behavioural
Diculties, 12(3), 171190. https://doi.org/10.1080/13632750701489915.
Couture, C., Cooper, P., & Royer, E. (2019). A study of the concurrent validity between
the Boxall Prole and the Strengths and Diculties Questionnaire. The International
Journal of Emotional Education, 3(1), 2029.
Dickes, A., Kemmis-Riggs, J., & McAloon, J. (2018). Methodological challenges to the
evaluation of interventions for foster/kinship carers and children: A systematic re-
view. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 21(2), 109145. https://doi.org/10.
1007/s10567-017-0248-z.
Dingwall, N., & Sebba, J. (2018). Evaluation of the attachment aware schools programme
nal report. Rees Centre, University of Oxford, England.
Doyle, O., Hegarty, M., & Owens, C. (2018). Population-based system of parenting sup-
port to reduce the prevalence of child social, emotional, and behavioural problems:
Dierence-in-dierences study. Prevention Science, 19(6), 772781. https://doi.org/
10.1007/s11121-018-0907-4.
Early Intervention Foundation. (2019). Guidebook. Nurture Groups. https://guidebook.
eif.org.uk/programme/nurture-groups (accessed 08.12.19).
EEF (Education Endowment Foundation) (2019) Early Years Measures Database.
Education Endowment Foundation. https://educationendowmentfoundation.org.uk/
projects-and-evaluation/evaluating-projects/early-years-measure-database/early-
years-measures-database/. Data accessed: 25/10/19.
Fancourt, N., & Sebba, J. (2018). The Leicestershire Virtual Schools Attachment Aware
Schools Programme: Evaluation Report. http://www.education.ox.ac.uk/wp-
content/uploads/2019/05/Leicestershire-Attachment-Aware-Schools-Programme-
Evaluation-Report.pdf.
Fernandez, E. (2019). Working towards better education for children in care:
Longitudinal analysis of the educational outcomes of a cohort of children in care in
Australia. Oxford Review of Education, 45(4), 481501. https://doi.org/10.1080/
03054985.2019.1612345.
Ford, J. D., & Courtois, C. A. (Eds.). (2013). Treating complex traumatic stress disorders in
children and adolescents: Scientic foundations and therapeutic models. Guilford Press.
Furnivall, J., & Grant, E. (2014). Trauma sensitive practice with children in care. Iriss
Insight, 27.
Geddes, H. (2006). Attachment in the classroom: The links between children's early experience,
emotional well-being and performance in school. Worth Pub.
Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Diculties Questionnaire: A research note.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581586. https://doi.org/10.1111/
j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x.
Hughes, N. K., & Schlösser, A. (2014). The eectiveness of nurture groups: A systematic
review. Emotional and Behavioural Diculties, 19(4), 386409. https://doi.org/10.
1080/13632752.2014.883729.
Husky, M. M., Otten, R., Boyd, A., Pez, O., Bitfoi, A., Carta, M. G., ... Kovess-Masfety, V.
(2018). Psychometric properties of the Strengths and Diculties Questionnaire in
children aged 512 years across seven European countries. European Journal of
Psychological Assessment.https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000489.
Iszatt, J., & Wasilewska, T. (1997). Nurture groups: An early intervention model enabling
vulnerable children with emotional and behavioural diculties to integrate suc-
cessfully into school. Educational and Child Psychology, 14,6370.
Kennedy, J. H., & Kennedy, C. E. (2004). Attachment theory: Implications for school
psychology. Psychology in the Schools, 41(2), 247259. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.
10153.
Kerr, L., & Cossar, J. (2014). Attachment interventions with foster and adoptive parents: A
systematic review. Child Abuse Review, 23(6), 426439. https://doi.org/10.1002/car.
2313.
Kirk, J. (2018). An exploratory study of the curriculum in primary school nurture groups:
from a pupil, parent and practitioner perspective (Doctoral dissertation, The Open
University). http://oro.open.ac.uk/55037/.
Kirkbride, R. (2014). They were a little family: An exploratory study of parental in-
volvement in nurture groupsfrom a practitioner and parent perspective. British
Journal of Special Education, 41(1), 82104. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8578.
12047.
Linsell, L., Johnson, S., Wolke, D., Morris, J., Kurinczuk, J. J., & Marlow, N. (2019).
Trajectories of behavior, attention, social and emotional problems from childhood to
early adulthood following extremely preterm birth: A prospective cohort study.
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(4), 531542. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s00787-018-1219-8.
Morris, J. M. (2019). Children's Constructions Of Their Experiences In A Primary School
Nurture Group (Doctoral dissertation, School of Education). https://lra.le.ac.uk/
handle/2381/43326.
National Institute for Clinical Excellence (2015). Childrens attachment: attachment in
children and young people who are adopted from care, in care or at high risk of going
into care. NICE Guideline [NG26]: November. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/
pubmed/26741018.
OConnor, T., & Colwell, J. (2002). Research Section: The eectiveness and rationale of
the nurture groupapproach to helping children with emotional and behavioural
diculties remain within mainstream education. British Journal of Special Education,
29(2), 96100. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8527.00247.
Parker, R., & Levinson, M. P. (2018). Student behaviour, motivation and the potential of
attachment-aware schools to redene the landscape. British Educational Research
Journal, 44(5), 875896. https://doi.org/10.1002/berj.3473.
Poulou, M. S. (2015). Emotional and behavioural diculties in preschool. Journal of Child
and Family Studies, 24(2), 225236. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-013-9828-9.
Reynolds, S., MacKay, T., & Kearney, M. (2009). RESEARCH SECTION: Nurture groups: A
large-scale, controlled study of eects on development and academic attainment.
British Journal of Special Education, 36(4), 204212. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
8578.2009.00445.x.
Rose, J., McGuire-Snieckus, R., Gilbert, L., & McInnes, K. (2019). Attachment Aware
Schools: The impact of a targeted and collaborative intervention. Pastoral Care in
Education, 123.https://doi.org/10.1080/02643944.2019.1625429.
Sanders, T. (2007). Helping Children Thrive at School: The Eectiveness of Nurture
Groups. Educational Psychology in Practice, 23(1), 4561. https://doi.org/10.1080/
02667360601154600.
Scott, K., & Lee, A. (2009). Beyond the classicnurture group model: An evaluation of
part-time and cross-age nurture groups in a Scottish local authority. Support for
Learning, 24(1), 510. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9604.2009.01391.x.
Sempik, J., Ward, H., & Darker, I. (2008). Emotional and Behavioural Diculties of
Children and Young People at Entry into Care. Clinical Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 13(2), 221233. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104507088344.
Shaver, I., & McClatchey, K. (2013). Assessing eectiveness of nurture groups in Northern
Scotland. Support for Learning, 28(3), 97102.
Steinsbekk, S., & Wichstrøm, L. (2018). Cohort Prole: The Trondheim Early Secure Study
(TESS)A study of mental health, psychosocial development and health behaviour
from preschool to adolescence. 1401-1401i International Journal of Epidemiology,
47(5), https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyy190.
Wichstrøm, L., Berg-Nielsen, T. S., Angold, A., Egger, H. L., Solheim, E., & Sveen, T. H.
(2012). Prevalence of psychiatric disorders in preschoolers. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(6), 695705.
S. Sloan, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 108 (2020) 104619
11
... Perkembangan emosi dasar pada setiap anak perlu untuk dilakukan evaluasi [9]. Melalui evaluasi perkembangan, guru dapat mengidentifikasi permasalahan dan kesulitan yang dihadapi anak usia dini, khususnya dalam pengelolaan emosi diri pada anak usia dini [10]. Sehingga, pada program pembelajaran selanjutnya guru dapat merencanakan dan menyusun kegiatan yang dapat menstimulasi perkembangan emosi pada anak usia dini. ...
... Evaluasi dapat meningkatkan kualitas pendidikan yang diberikan kepada anak usia dini. Evaluasi dapat membantu anak untuk mencapai potensi dan standar perkembangan anak usia dini secara maksimal [11], [12], [10], [13]. Anak usia dini yang memiliki emosi dasar tidak matang maka akan berdampak pada kesulitan dalam mengendalikan emosi hal ini ditandai anak usia dini sering merasa marah dan sedih tanpa alasan yang jelas [14]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Penelitian ini adalah sebuah langkah dasar bidang pendidikan dalam mengikuti arah perkembangan zaman yaitu penggunaan alat digital untuk mendukung kegiatan pembelajaran. Oleh karena itu, menciptakan sebuah media digital yang digunakan untuk mengevaluasi perkembangan anak usia dini dapat membantu guru untuk memperbaiki kualitas pembelajaran agar lebih efektif dan efisien. .Penelitian ini bertujuan untuk mengetahui implementasi evaluasi digital pada perkembangan anak usia dini, baik dalam konten audio, video, dan audio-video. Metode penelitian yang digunakan adalah deskriptif kualitatif dengan pendekatan studi pustaka melalui literatur perkembangan emosi dasar dengan rancangan studi kasus fenomenologis. Sumber data dalam penelitian ini antara lain sumber data insani dan sumber data non insani. Teknik pengumpulan data dalam penelitian ini adalah observasi, wawancara, dan dokumentasi dengan menggunakan teknik analisis data analisis tematik. Hasil dari penelitian ini adalah evaluasi digital dengan menggunakan konten audio-visual. Audio-visual dapat membantu anak untuk mengekspresikan emosi wajah dan memberikan respon reseptif pada audio-visual yang sudah disaksikan, dalam menafsirkan audio-video anak bebas menentukan deskripsi dari sosok manusia dengan tujuan menilai pemahaman anak tentang emosi dasar dan memeriksa kapasitas anak dalam memberikan pemahaman emosi dasar
... Notes 1. "A nurture room sets out to provide a safe, welcoming and caring environment for learning and will replicate the home environment with a comfortable seating area, a kitchen facility for preparing food and a working area to address formal curriculum demands." (Sloan et al., 2020) 2. Sensory rooms, are tailored spaces that caters to students' sensory needs, helping them self-regulate through individualized sensory experiences to enhance readiness for learning and social interaction. It engages primary senses such as sight, hearing, touch, and smell, along with vestibular and proprioceptive senses. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose This study aims to capture children’s interpretation of holistic well-being within Irish primary schools and add to the development of a comprehensive systems-informed positive education model. Design/methodology/approach This study utilized visual participatory research methods, including PhotoVoice and one-on-one interviews, to assess children’s (n = 16) well-being, guided by Von Unger’s comprehensive seven-step framework. Data analysis was anchored within grounded theory, beginning with data collection, initial coding, focused coding and culminating in identifying themes and subthemes. Data were interpreted using the mosaic approach by integrating visual and verbal data. Findings This analysis uncovered three primary themes that affect student well-being: relationships, space and physical environment and learning and curriculum, each with detailed subthemes. For instance, student–teacher relationships, peer relationships, safety, learning spaces, the creative curriculum including arts and music and the experiential richness of outdoor learning are crucial to students' educational growth and well-being. These aspects are seen as interconnected, shaping a holistic educational experience beyond academic learning to encompass students’ comprehensive well-being. The students' narratives demonstrated that learning is not merely an academic exercise but a vital component of their well-being. Originality/value This study significantly departs from traditional educational research by advocating for a dynamic, action-oriented understanding of “well-being.” It challenges the static, possessive interpretations of well-being and introduces the concept of well-being as a fluid and ever-evolving process. This reconceptualization positions well-being as a complex construct, influenced by an intricate web of relationships, spanning human and non-human interactions, organizational and environmental structures, personal desires, behavioral practices and broader societal and cultural frameworks.
... Many researchers conclude that some young children are more susceptible than others to emotional, behavioural, and social challenges; these include children in foster care. Problems with attachment are exacerbated by experiences of poor attachment in their initial home setting, and by the difficulty of building strong and positive bonds during their time in care (Sloan et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotional, social, behavioral, and mental health disorders in young children are increasingly prevalent and may have long-term effects, persisting from preschool into primary school (Carroll & Hurry, 2018). Such challenges can significantly impact children's ability to adapt to the classroom and access learning opportunities. To address these challenges, Boxall (2002) developed the concept of nurture groups, where a small group of students is removed from their regular classes for a limited period during the school year to model positive attachment relationships, develop emotional literacy, and provide opportunities for social learning while maintaining access to education (Loinaz, 2015). This literature review aims to explore the effectiveness of nurture groups in improving outcomes for young children with emotional, behavioral, and social difficulties in primary schools. The study reviews existing literature, examining the positive effects of nurture groups on children's well-being, while also critiquing current research. The results suggest that children with a broad spectrum of social, emotional, and behavioral difficulties may benefit from nurture groups, thereby making a strong case for creating nurturing schools. However, one potential concern is that some students who thrive in nurturing schools may struggle when they return to mainstream classes. Therefore, further comparable studies on a larger scale are required.
Chapter
This chapter will not only delve into the rising interest in mental health among children and adolescents but also provide a comprehensive examination of school-based mental health programs and interventions, providing practical guidance for stakeholders.The aim is to assess their effectiveness in supporting students facing mental health challenges. Importantly, this chapter will offer practical strategies and evidence-based solutions that are readily applicable in helping school districts implement effective mental health interventions tailored to the diverse needs of students. By targeting prevalent challenges such as anxiety, depression, ADHD, trauma, and others, school communities can advocate for the well-being and safety of their students. Educators, mental health practitioners, and policymakers can benefit from understanding the most effective programs and areas for improvement in current interventions. Ultimately, this discussion will contribute to enhancing the efficacy and future implementation of school-based mental health interventions
Article
This research delves into the intricate relationship between emotional abuse, emotional competence, self-concept, and academic achievement among adolescents in Kashmir, with a specific focus on gender differences. Adolescence, marked by vulnerability, witnesses the profound impact of emotional abuse on cognitive, emotional, and psychological development. A comprehensive survey involving 300 high school students reveals significant gender disparities in emotional abuse experiences, emotional competence levels, self-concept, and academic achievement. Boys are shown to be more susceptible to emotional abuse, particularly in dimensions such as rejection, terrorizing, isolation, ignoring, and corruption. Conversely, girls exhibit higher emotional competence, self-concept, and academic achievement, emphasizing the role of societal and familial dynamics. The study underscores the need for targeted interventions recognizing gender-specific nuances, aiming to mitigate emotional abuse's adverse effects and foster a conducive environment for adolescents' emotional and academic well-being. The findings contribute to a nuanced understanding of the multifaceted challenges faced by adolescents in the context of emotional well-being and academic success.
Article
Full-text available
Aims This scoping review sought to identify and characterize measurement of self-regulation in preschool and elementary aged children. Methods The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses-Scoping Review (ScR) guidelines were applied. Databases from the fields of allied health, education, medicine, and social sciences were searched including: CINAHL, Education Database (ProQuest), Education Research Complete, EMBASE, ERIC, iNFORMIT Combined, Medline, PsychINFO, Social Sciences (ProQuest), Teacher Reference Center, and Web of Science. Articles published between 2015 and 2020 were included. Dual review was utilized at all stages and a third reviewer resolved any conflicts. Results Sixty-seven studies were included in this review. A range of observational, self-report, teacher report, caregiver report, and observational measures of self-regulation were identified. Included studies were primarily published in education and psychology disciplines, with no studies by occupational therapists identified. Conclusions Although a range of measures were identified in this scoping review, the results highlight the lack of consensus regarding self-regulation measurement that occupational therapists use to design and implement therapy programs to address child emotional and behavioral needs.
Article
Nurturing approaches (NA) derived from the need to support children’s social, emotional and behavioural development and their ability to build secure relationships with others (Boxall, 2002). This systematic review follows Hughes and Schlösser’s (2014) review of the effectiveness of nurture groups (NGs). The purpose was two-fold: To assess whether the issues highlighted by Hughes and Schlösser (2014) have been addressed; and to evaluate the quality of evidence supporting the implementation of NAs in primary schools across Scotland. Both NGs and whole-school nurture were included, reflecting current nurturing practices in Scotland. 647 articles were initially sourced from a range of databases. Following eligibility screening, eight peer-reviewed articles were identified for appraisal and synthesis using the Downs and Black (1998) research quality checklist and National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (2012) guidelines. A range of methodological approaches and findings were reported, such as: Improvements in relationships, social skills, emotional regulation, academic engagement and attitudes towards school. Overall, NAs have been shown to relate to positive outcomes for primary school-aged children. However, due to a number of methodological issues, the reliability and generalisability of these findings were questionable. The findings were discussed in relation to the current Scottish context. The rise of whole-school approaches; the importance of inclusive practice; considerations for the use of quantitative and qualitative research in education; and considerations around the impact of the current Covid-19 pandemic were included. Implications for educational psychology practice and future research were discussed, in addition to the limitations of the current review.
Article
Full-text available
The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) has been used extensively to screen for possible mental disorders in epidemiological studies around the world. The present study aimed to compare the internal consistency of both the parent- and teacher-SDQ across seven European countries: Italy, Germany, the Netherlands, Lithuania, Bulgaria, Romania, and Turkey, and to determine the ability of the SDQ to discriminate cases from non-cases of disorders against the well-established Development and Well-Being Assessment (DAWBA). The sample included 541 assessments of children aged 5–12 years. Internal consistency ranged from .74 to .85 for the teacher-SDQ, and .60 to .85 for the parent-SDQ with significant between-country differences. The SDQ further proved to be an adequate screening instrument for the detection of any mental disorder (area under the receiving operator characteristic [AUROC] = .74, 95% CI: .69–.78), and for externalizing disorders in particular (AUROC = .80, 95% CI: .76–.84). There were no differences in AUROC between countries (p = .09), yet sample sizes were limited thus restricting our ability to detect between-country differences in AUROCs. The results reinforce existing research on the SDQ and support its use in detecting probable cases of psychiatric disorders in children across Europe.
Article
Full-text available
To investigate trajectories of behavior, attention, social and emotional problems to early adulthood in extremely preterm survivors compared to a term-born comparison group. Longitudinal analysis of a prospective, population-based cohort of 315 surviving infants born < 26 completed weeks of gestation recruited at birth in 1995, from the UK/Republic of Ireland, and a term-born comparison group recruited at age 6. The parent-report Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire was completed at age 6, 11, 16 and 19 years. The Total Behavioral Difficulties Score was 4.81 points higher in extremely preterm individuals compared to their term-born peers over the period (95% CI 3.76–5.87, p < 0.001) and trajectories were stable in both groups. The impact of difficulties on home life, friendships, school or work and/or leisure activities was greater in the EPT group (RR 4.28, 95% CI 2.89–6.35, p < 0.001), and hyperactivity/inattention and peer problems accounted for the largest differences. A clinically significant behavioral screen at age 2.5 was associated with a higher Total Behavioral Difficulties Score from 6 years onwards in extremely preterm participants (Mean difference 6.90, 95% CI 5.01–8.70, p < 0.0.01), as was moderate/severe cognitive impairment at last assessment (Mean difference: 4.27, 95% CI 2.76–5.77, p < 0.001). Attention, social and emotional problems in extremely preterm individuals persist into early adulthood with significant impact on daily life. A positive behavioral screen in infancy and moderate/severe cognitive impairment are associated with early adult outcomes. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (10.1007/s00787-018-1219-8) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Article
The transition from the social world of home to that of school can be a difficult and sometimes treacherous journey. Each child will arrive in school with an internal model of the world based on early experiences, through which they will try to make sense of school life. Children whose home circumstances are very stressful, who have suffered repeated disruptions during their early years, are often ill-equipped to cope when they come into school. Some give up and withdraw into their own world, others act out their distress in ways that can be enormously challenging for their teachers. They underachieve and can quickly get caught up in cycles of failure. Many seem destined for special education and some may even be excluded. The nurture group model grew out of concerns about how to help such vulnerable children and support their teachers and parents. It draws on ideas developed in multi-disciplinary child guidance work and is a preventative approach aimed at successful integration. As a stage three resource, it operates as an integral part of the school A teacher and welfare assistant work with a group of infants for a maximum of four terms. The emphasis is on fostering a trusting relationship and providing a secure, predictable setting where the children can gradually learn to control their own behaviour and use their curiosity constructively so that they can learn. Six nurture groups are currently in operation in Enfield. Evaluation indicates that this provision is both preventative and cost effective, with many statutory assessments being averted. The nurture group serves the function of bringing home and school together and plays an important role in supporting the school, parents and children. It is a model that we have found to be particularly effective in meeting the needs of some of our most vulnerable and challenging children.
Article
Internationally, reviews of educational outcomes of children in care reveal consistent findings highlighting the need for considerable policy attention to be paid to their education to ensure they achieve optimal economic and psychosocial life outcomes. Important to enhancing the educational achievement of children in care are support, stability of environment, teaching resources and the recognition by all stakeholders – birth parents, foster carers, social workers and statutory authorities — that education is paramount to a child’s future well-being. This paper highlights the perspectives of carers, teachers and children on the fostering experience with particular focus on educational outcomes drawing on a prospective longitudinal study of children in long-term care. Using a multimethod, repeated measures multi-informant design, children were assessed by carers and teachers using the Achenbach Child Behaviour Checklist and its companion, the Teacher Report. The findings indicate demonstrated gains in terms of improved scores and adaptive functioning at subsequent assessments following two years of stable care. Discussion of the findings and implications for practice will focus on the need for a coordinated strategy to improve placement continuity and focused professional and organisational support with respect to the emotional and educational needs of children in care.
Article
The Attachment Aware Schools (AAS) project was a targeted and collaborative intervention between academics and school-based practitioners. The aim of the project was to promote practitioner awareness of attachment in relation to child behaviours and learning. It focused on using relational-based strategies and interventions to address the needs of children and young people. The AAS framework promoted Emotion Coaching as a universal, relational-based practice approach, with specialised targeted support for children with additional needs. Supportive managerial strategies and setting policies sustained the integration and maintenance of attachment-informed practice and school ethos. This article reports the findings from the project which included over 200 participants (107 teaching and support staff and 94 pupils aged 5 to 16 years), from 40 schools, in two different Local Authorities within the UK. Adopting a mixed methods approach, qualitative and quantitative data provided hard and soft indicators of improved pupil and adult outcomes. Findings demonstrated significant improvements in pupils’ academic achievement in reading, writing and maths. There were significant decreases in sanctions, exclusions and overall difficulties. Practitioners reported a positive impact on professional practice, adult self-regulation and emotional self-control, and were more confident when talking with children about emotions. This project contributes to the growing evidence based on the effectiveness of whole school attachment-based strategies and is already demonstrating policy implications at a national level.
Article
This article is a position paper drawing on the findings of several studies into attachment‐aware approaches in schools and other settings. In seeking to promote pedagogical positions that place greater emphasis on the emotional landscape of students, the authors locate this in an historical perspective. They outline the ways in which educationalists have come to separate the learning being from the feeling being, to the extent of evolving separate discourses on behaviour and wellbeing. On the contrary, the authors contend that these elements are intertwined, a reality that needs to be understood at policy level for ‘outcomes’ to change radically. They frame their argument in the light of conclusions drawn from studies in which they were involved of attachment‐awareness and emotion‐coaching approaches (2011–2017). The authors’ position is further informed by research exploring attitudes of excluded youngsters towards education (2015–2016), and also in light of the wider international literature around progressive education.
Article
The quality of parenting is recognised as an important determinant of children’s mental health. Parenting interventions typically target high-risk families rather than adopting a universal approach. This study examined the population impact of the Triple P Positive Parenting Programme on the prevalence of children’s social, emotional, and behavioural problems. A propensity score matching difference-in-differences method was used to compare intervention and comparison regions matched on socio-demographic characteristics in midlands Ireland. The pre-intervention sample included 1501 and 1495 parents of children aged 4–8 years in the intervention and comparison regions respectively. The post-intervention sample included 1521 and 1544 parents respectively. The primary outcome measure was parental reports on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. There were some significant reductions in the prevalence rates of social, emotional, and behavioural problems in the intervention regions compared to the comparison regions. Children in the intervention sample experienced lower total difficulties, emotional symptoms, and conduct problems than children in the comparison sample, and they were less at risk of scoring within the borderline/abnormal range for total difficulties, conduct problems, and hyperactivity. The programme reduced the proportion of children scoring within the borderline/abnormal range by 4.7% for total difficulties, 4.4% for conduct problems, and 4.5% for hyperactivity in the total population. This study demonstrated that a universal parenting programme implemented at multiple levels using a partnership approach may be an effective population health approach to targeting child mental health.
Book
An empirically based account of the nature, effectiveness and challenges relating to Nurture Groups.