ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Abstract and Figures

In this comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis of group design studies of nonpharmacological early interventions designed for young children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), we report summary effects across 7 early intervention types (behavioral, developmental, naturalistic developmental behavioral intervention [NDBI], TEACCH, sensory-based, animal-assisted, and technology-based), and 15 outcome categories indexing core and related ASD symptoms. A total of 1,615 effect sizes were gathered from 130 independent participant samples. A total of 6,240 participants, who ranged in age from 0-8 years, are represented across the studies. We synthesized effects within intervention and outcome type using a robust variance estimation approach to account for the nesting of effect sizes within studies. We also tracked study quality indicators, and report an additional set of summary effect sizes that restrict included studies to those meeting prespecified quality indicators. Finally, we conducted moderator analyses to evaluate whether summary effects across intervention types were larger for proximal as compared with distal effects, and for context-bound as compared to generalized effects. We found that when study quality indicators were not taken into account, significant positive effects were found for behavioral, developmental, and NDBI intervention types. When effect size estimation was limited to studies with randomized controlled trial (RCT) designs, evidence of positive summary effects existed only for developmental and NDBI intervention types. This was also the case when outcomes measured by parent report were excluded. Finally, when effect estimation was limited to RCT designs and to outcomes for which there was no risk of detection bias, no intervention types showed significant effects on any outcome. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
Content may be subject to copyright.
Running head: AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 1
©American Psychological Association, 2019. This paper is not the copy of record and may
not exactly replicate the authoritative document published in the APA journal. Please do
not copy or cite without author's permission. The final article is available, upon
publication, at: https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000215
Project AIM: Autism Intervention Meta-Analysis for Studies of Young Children
Micheal Sandbank
The University of Texas at Austin, Department of Special Education
Kristen Bottema-Beutel
Boston College, Lynch School of Education and Human Development
Shannon Crowley
Boston College, Lynch School of Education and Human Development
Margaret Cassidy
Vanderbilt University, College of Arts and Sciences
Kacie Dunham
Vanderbilt University, Vanderbilt Brain Institute
Jacob I. Feldman
Vanderbilt University, Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences
Jenna Crank
The University of Texas at Austin, Department of Special Education
Susanne A. Albarran
The University of Texas at Austin, Department of Special Education
Sweeya Raj
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 2
Vanderbilt University, College of Arts and Sciences
Prachy Mahbub
Mount Holyoke College, Department of Neuroscience and Behavior
Tiffany G. Woynaroski
Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Vanderbilt Brain Institute, Vanderbilt Kennedy Center
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 3
Author Note
Micheal Sandbank, PhD, is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Special Education at The
University of Texas at Austin. Kristen Bottema-Beutel, PhD, is an Associate Professor in the
Lynch School of Education and Human Development at Boston College. Shannon Crowley is a
doctoral student in the Lynch School of Education and Human Development at Boston College.
Margaret Cassidy and Sweeya Raj are undergraduate students in the College of Arts and
Sciences at Vanderbilt University. Kacie Dunham is a doctoral student in the Vanderbilt Brain
Institute at Vanderbilt University. Jacob I. Feldman, MS, CCC-SLP, is a doctoral student in the
Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences at Vanderbilt University. Jenna Crank and Susanne
A. Albarran are doctoral students in the Department of Special Education at the University of
Texas at Austin. Prachy Mahbub is an undergraduate student at Mount Holyoke College. Tiffany
Woynaroski, PhD, CCC-SLP, is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Hearing and Speech
Sciences at Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Vanderbilt Brain Institute, and Vanderbilt
Kennedy Center. This work was funded in part by Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of
Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) of the National Institutes of Health
(U54HD083211; PI: Neul), the Training Program in Fundamental Neuroscience of the National
Institutes of Health (T32 MH064913; Winder) and the National Institute on Deafness and Other
Communication Disorders (1R21DC016144; Woynaroski). The content is solely the
responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the funding
agencies. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Micheal Sandbank via
email at msandbank@austin.utexas.edu, or via phone at 512-232-3589.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 4
Abstract
In this comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis of group design studies of
nonpharmacological early interventions designed for young children with autism spectrum
disorder (ASD), we report summary effects across seven early intervention types (behavioral,
developmental, naturalistic developmental behavioral intervention [NDBI], TEACCH,
sensory-based, animal-assisted, and technology-based), and 15 outcome categories indexing core
and related ASD symptoms. A total of 1,615 effect sizes were gathered from 130 independent
participant samples. A total of 6,240 participants, who ranged in age from 0-8 years, are
represented across the studies. We synthesized effects within intervention and outcome type
using a robust variance estimation approach to account for the nesting of effect sizes within
studies. We also tracked study quality indicators, and report an additional set of summary effect
sizes that restrict included studies to those meeting pre-specified quality indicators. Finally, we
conducted moderator analyses to evaluate whether summary effects across intervention types
were larger for proximal as compared to distal effects, and for context-bound as compared to
generalized effects. We found that when study quality indicators were not taken into account,
significant positive effects were found for behavioral, developmental, and NDBI intervention
types. When effect size estimation was limited to studies with randomized controlled trial (RCT)
designs, evidence of positive summary effects existed only for developmental and NDBI
intervention types. This was also the case when outcomes measured by parent report were
excluded. Finally, when effect estimation was limited to RCT designs and to outcomes for which
there was no risk of detection bias, no intervention types showed significant effects on any
outcome.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 5
Keywords:
autism spectrum disorder, intervention, treatment, meta-analysis, robust
variance estimation, study quality
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 6
Public Significance Statement
This comprehensive meta-analysis of interventions for young children with autism
spectrum disorder (ASD) suggests that naturalistic developmental behavioral interventions and
developmental intervention approaches have amassed enough quality evidence to be considered
promising for supporting children with ASD in achieving a range of developmental outcomes.
Behavioral intervention approaches also show evidence of effectiveness, but methodological
rigor remains a pressing concern in this area of research. There is little evidence to support the
effectiveness of TEACCH, sensory-based interventions, animal-assisted interventions, and
interventions mediated solely through technology at this time. Additional high quality
randomized-controlled trials that feature assessments administered by naive examiners are
needed to more firmly establish the effectiveness of any intervention type for this clinical
population. Stakeholders should consider the nature of outcomes being tracked in intervention
studies and interpret findings cautiously.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 7
Project AIM: Autism Intervention Meta-Analysis for Studies of Young Children
Autism Spectrum Disorder
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a relatively common neurodevelopmental disorder
with a varied impact. Current prevalence estimates suggest that 1 in 59 meet the criteria for
ASD, though this prevalence varies by sex, with males having a higher (approximately four
times greater) likelihood of being affected (Baio et al., 2018). The diagnosis is primarily
associated with core challenges in social communication, as well as restricted interests and
repetitive behaviors and differences in sensory function (American Psychological Association
[APA], 2013). Individuals with ASD, however, may also exhibit difficulty in a number of related
areas, such as language, adaptive behavior, and academic achievement.
A substantial portion of autistic individuals report drawing a sense of identity and
1
empowerment from the diagnosis, and advocate for a neurodiversity conceptualization of ASD
as a natural form of human difference (Houting, 2019). Researchers have recently articulated a
view of early intervention that is consistent with a neurodiversity framework (e.g.,
Fletcher-Watson, 2018). Specifically, early intervention services provided throughout childhood
may support children with ASD in developing competencies that will allow them to navigate into
adulthood in ways they see fit. At present, long-term life outcomes of autistic individuals vary
widely. Though a number of individuals that receive early diagnoses go on to develop adaptive
and communicative skills within the average range , most require at least some support, and
many require substantial support into adulthood (Renty & Roeyers, 2006). Importantly, quality
1Though researchers and clinicians often feel more comfortable with and advocate for using person-first language
such as “individuals with autism,” some autistic individuals and their parents have endorsed identity-first language
that incorporates autism as a component of their identity over person-first language (Gernsbacher, 2017; Kenny et
al., 2016). In this manuscript, we flexibly use identity-first and person-first language to acknowledge the diversity of
opinions on this issue within the broader autism community (see Robison, 2019).
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 8
of life among autistic adults also varies between individuals (Howlin & Magiati, 2017).
Improving the quality of intervention provided in early childhood may be one way to increase
the likelihood that long term life-satisfaction is attainable for all autistic people.
Research on Interventions in Early Childhood
Common intervention recommendations. Recommendations abound regarding the
nature and amount of intervention that should be provided to support development in children
with ASD. Scholars and professionals have routinely asserted that intervention should be
provided as early as possible, beginning at or even before diagnosis in toddlerhood or infancy;
that intervention should be intensive (i.e., provided for 25-40 hours per week for over a year or
longer); and that it should be comprehensive (i.e., targeting broader development rather than
specific skills; Boyd, Odom, Humphreys, & Sam, 2010; Lovaas, 1987; McEachin, Smith, &
Lovaas, 1993; Lord et al., 2001; Odom, Boyd, Hall, & Hume, 2010). These recommendations are
motivated by the theory that interventions provided in early childhood are likely to yield the
most optimal effects by capitalizing on the neuroplasticity of the developing brain (Dawson &
Zanolli, 2003; Kolb & Gibb, 2011), and are rooted in early influential studies which suggested
that intensive intervention yielded substantial cognitive gains, and that such gains varied
according to age at the onset of intervention (e.g., Lovaas, 1987; McEachin, Smith & Lovaas,
1993). However, it is notable that some subsequent studies exploring putative predictors of
treatment response have reported that age at intake was not significantly associated with
intervention outcomes (e.g., Eikeseth, Smith, Jahr, & Eldevik, 2007; Eikeseth, Klintwall, Jahr,
and Karlsson, 2012).
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 9
Types of intervention approaches. Several approaches to intervention aim to address
the core and related challenges associated with ASD. These approaches vary in their underlying
theories on the nature of ASD and development, as well as in their procedures and instructional
modalities.
Behavioral approaches.
Behavioral interventions were among the first developed and
clinically tested approaches for improving outcomes for children with autism (Ferster &
DeMeyer, 1962). These approaches are derived from operant learning theory and are
characterized by the discrete presentation of information (i.e., a stimulus), the prompted
exhibition of target responses (i.e., desired academic, adaptive, and communicative behaviors),
and the provision of extrinsic positive reinforcement (e.g., edible treats, toys, stickers, etc.) in the
presence of those responses. Target skills are chosen based on functional areas of child need.
Skills tend to be initially targeted in highly structured interactions within isolated clinical
contexts (e.g., in the course of one-on-one interactions at a clinic with a therapist), but more
natural settings and interaction partners (e.g., mainstream classrooms and other children) are
gradually integrated as a child demonstrates progress. Initial studies suggested that Early
Intensive Behavioral Intervention (EIBI) could yield marked improvements in cognitive and
academic placement outcomes for children with ASD, especially when provided before school
age and with sufficient intensity (Lovaas, 1987; McEachin, Smith, & Lovaas, 1993). In the wake
of such research, a number of behavioral approaches were further developed and refined, and the
Behavior Analyst Certification Board (BACB) was established to oversee the clinical
certification associated with this approach. Other behavioral interventions include Discrete Trial
Training (DTT), Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS), and Positive Behavioral
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 10
Supports (PBS). Together, these interventions are sometimes loosely described as Applied
Behavior Analysis (ABA) Therapy and now constitute the primary approach used in clinical
practice, according to parent and provider reports (Green et al., 2006; Stahmer, Collings, &
Palinka, 2005).
Developmental approaches.
At times viewed in contrast to the aforementioned
traditional behavioral interventions are those derived from developmental theories of learning
(e.g., Ospina et al., 2008; Prizant & Wetherby, 1998). Developmental interventions are rooted in
constructivist theory, which posits that development is the result of children’s active exploration
of their physical and social surroundings. This exploration is far from being a solitary endeavor,
as children are supported in social and language development by their interactions with more
competent interaction partners such as caregivers (Bruner, 1982; Vygotsky, 1978). Foundational
research on ASD within the developmental tradition has suggested that early deficits in social
processes (joint attention being of particular importance) in children with ASD may in turn lead
to difficulties in early caregiver-child social interactions. These early deficits are thus viewed as
disrupting the primary context for subsequent language and social communication development.
As such, developmental interventions focus on improving the synchrony, reciprocity, and
duration of parent-child or child-child interactions as a pathway for ameliorating deficits in
social communication and generating cascading improvements in developmentally related skills.
These interventions are primarily delivered in the context of everyday routines such as play, and
intervention goals are chosen based on the typical sequences of social communication and
language development. Examples of classically developmental interventions include
DIR/Floortime (Greenspan & Wieder, 2007) and Hanen models (Carter et al., 2011).
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 11
Naturalistic Developmental Behavioral Interventions (NDBIs).
In 2015, several
interventions were categorized as belonging to a third type of intervention approach which has
theoretical underpinnings in both behavioral and developmental theories of learning and
development. Naturalistic Developmental Behavioral Interventions (NDBIs) involve the use of
behavioral principles of learning to teach skills chosen from a developmental sequence in
naturalistic environments and using natural rewards (Schreibman et al., 2015). Skills selected as
relevant for intervention are those that allow the child to participate more fully within reciprocal
interactions with the adult. These interventions are delivered primarily in the context of play, but
control of interactions within this context is shared by both the child and the adult, through
balanced turn-taking. Interventions categorized as NDBIs include the Early Start Denver Model
(Rogers & Dawson, 2010); Enhanced Milieu Teaching (EMT; Kaiser, 1993); Pivotal Response
Treatment (Koegel, Koegel, & Carter, 1999); and Joint Attention, Symbolic Play, Engagement,
and Regulation (JASPER; Kasari, Freeman, & Paparella, 2006).
TEACCH.
The TEACCH (Treatment and Education of Autistic and related
Communication-handicapped Children) program was developed in 1972 by Eric Schopler and is
based primarily in the state of North Carolina (Mesibov, Shea, & Schopler, 2004). We consider
this specific intervention as distinct from other approaches because of the explicit focus on
structured environmental design and self-monitoring, which is not the emphasis of any of the
other interventions of interest to the present synthesis. The theoretical foundations of TEACCH
are rooted neither in behavioral nor in developmental theories of learning. Rather, TEACCH
procedures were designed according to Schopler’s theorized profile of the learning strengths,
preferences, and needs of individuals with ASD, which include relative visual strength and
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 12
comfort with consistent routines. Thus, the TEACCH program is characterized by highly
structured work routines and a heavy reliance on the visual presentation of information.
TEACCH “work systems” organize individual student tasks to visually convey four pieces of
information: (1) What activity the student will complete, (2) How many items need to be
completed, (3) How to identify when the work is finished, and (4) What will happen after task
completion. TEACCH classrooms tend to feature carefully planned and structured environmental
arrangements, work areas with minimal distractions, consistent routines, and the extensive use of
visual schedules and supports.
Sensory-based interventions.
Sensory-based interventions are motivated by the theory
that sensory function is foundational in nature, and that sensory disruptions, particularly early in
life, may produce cascading effects on development across a number of domains, ultimately
yielding the constellation of core and related characteristics associated with ASD (e.g., Bahrick
& Todd, 2012). Within this framework, it is hypothesized that targeted treatments may thus have
the potential not only to ameliorate reported sensory differences, but also to translate to effects
on higher-order social, communication, and cognitive skills in children with ASD (Cascio,
Woynaroski, Baranek, & Wallace, 2016). The most well-known of these sensory-based
approaches to treatment is Sensory Integration Therapy, in which children are presented with a
series of individualized sensory-motor experiences intended to build foundational skills that will
facilitate their engagement and participation in a range of activities of daily living (Ayres, 1979;
Ayres, 2005). Other sensory-based interventions, as broadly conceptualized, may include
activities such as brushing, swinging, the use of weighted vests and blankets to improve sensory
processing, and music therapy and auditory integration training approaches that aim to scaffold
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 13
motor, social, and emotional development (e.g., Baranek, 2002; Case-Smith and Arbesman,
2008). Sensory based approaches are most often provided by occupational therapists in clinical
contexts but may also be delivered by caregivers, educators, and/or other service providers
across a broader range of home and community settings.
Animal-assisted interventions.
Animal-assisted interventions are those that rely on
interactions with animals as the primary context for facilitating developmental change (e.g.,
O’Haire, 2013; 2017; Trzmiel, Purandare, Michalak, Zasadzka, & Pawlaczyk, 2018). In the ASD
intervention literature, the intervention most prominently represented in this category is
equine-assisted activities and therapy (EAAT; see Gabriels et al., 2012 for a review of related
terminology). Proponents of EAAT contend that the activities of horse-riding and horse care
provide a multisensory experience that allows children the opportunity to practice skills across
multiple domains. More broadly, animal-assisted interventions are theoretically motivated by the
possibility that human-animal interactions are highly motivating and provide calming contexts
which may support improved psychological wellbeing and social function.
Technology-based interventions.
Technology-based interventions employ one or more of
a variety of technologies (e.g., computers, videos, video games, robots) as the primary medium
for delivery of instruction. These interventions attempt to capitalize on the reported special
interest that many autistic individuals have in computer technology (Grynszpan, Weiss,
Perez-Diaz, & Gal, 2014) and predictable formats of information delivery (Baron-Cohen, Golan,
& Ashwin, 2012), which allow users to control the pace of the interaction (Knight, McKissick, &
Saunders, 2013). Examples of technology-based interventions include computer-assisted
instruction and The Transporters™ DVD series (e.g., Young & Posselt, 2012).
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 14
Previous Syntheses of Intervention Literature
The National Professional Development Center (NPDC) on Autism Spectrum Disorders
generated a list of 27 evidence-based practices for improving outcomes in individuals with ASD,
based on prior reviews of single subject and group design research (Wong et al., 2015).
Similarly, the National Standards Project (NSP, 2015) described 14 intervention practices as
established for children with ASD and an additional 18 as emerging, based on a review of single
subject and group design literature. In 2011, Warren and colleagues systematically reviewed 34
group design studies examining interventions in children with ASD. Notably, only two of the
studies included in the review by Warren et al. (2011) were randomized controlled trials (RCTs),
and only one of those was rated as high quality. Very recent systematic reviews suggest the
publication of RCTs has precipitously increased in ASD since the publication of the
aforementioned synthesis by Warren and collaborators. For example, French and Kennedy
(2017) systematically reviewed RCTs of interventions targeting any outcome in children with
ASD below age 6, and found a total of 48 RCTs, 40 of which had been published since 2010.
Previous efforts to synthesize this literature have a number of shortcomings. First, NPDC
and NSP review procedures attempted to synthesize evidence from RCTs, quasi-experimental
studies, and single subject design studies (SSDs), when there is currently no agreed upon way of
doing so. Though multiple methodologies can contribute to knowledge about effective practices,
studies employing group designs, in particular high-quality RCTs, are the best equipped to
control for alternative explanations and threats to internal validity. Syntheses that attempt to
combine RCTs, quasi-experimental studies, and SSDs may overestimate the effectiveness of a
given intervention approach. Inclusion of SSDs also limits the extent to which summary effects
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 15
of intervention can be quantified with meta-analytic approaches. Though effect sizes that
quantify change observed in SSDs have been proposed, many of these approaches fail to account
for first order autocorrelation of data, ignore the logic of within study replication that is critical
to interpretation of SSD data, and yield highly inflated and positively biased effect sizes which
are not comparable to mean group differences that index treatment effects in group design
(Wolery, Busick, Reichow, & Barton, 2010; Zimmerman et al., 2018).
Second, in previous reviews, limited consideration was given to the nature of outcomes
measured. That is, prior syntheses of intervention literature have predominantly sought to
ascertain whether various approaches to interventions are “evidence-based,” but they have
largely failed to summarize the extent to which interventions effected meaningful change.
Interventions that were shown to effect change that was overly specific to intervention targets
were generally not distinguished from those that impacted scores on broader standardized
assessments of developmentally advanced skills as administered by independent assessors. A
synthesis is needed which asks not only “what works and for whom,” but also, “for what?”
Third, none of the prior reviews seeking to synthesize effects for the broad range of
interventions geared towards young children with ASD attempted to identify the summary
effects of varied interventions on any outcomes using meta-analytic tools. Although a narrative
synthesis approach allows for tallying the number of studies that have shown an effect for a
given outcome, they do not allow for deriving an estimate of the combined magnitude of the
effect, or determining whether or not the combined effect is significantly different from zero.
Additionally, narrative synthesis methods are unable to offer information about variables that
may moderate effect sizes. Moderator effects offer vital information for understanding for whom
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 16
interventions are effective, and for identifying study design features that result in potentially
inflated effect sizes.
Crucial Quality Considerations
Although systematic reviews and meta-analyses are purported to provide the most
reliable summary of evidence of intervention effects, their conclusions are limited by the quality
of evidence which they summarize (Higgins et al., 2011; Murad, Asi, Alsawas, & Alahdab,
2016). Several aspects of study design pose risk of biasing outcomes. Thus, examination of any
set of intervention literature must include an assessment of several study-level quality indicators.
We outline here those that are particularly important in studies of nonpharmacological
interventions of children with ASD.
Random assignment. Though some have questioned the feasibility of conducting
randomized controlled trials to test the effects of “real world” interventions with individuals with
disabilities (Oliver et al., 2002), random assignment remains the most rigorous control for rival
explanations of findings. Though random assignment does not ensure pretreatment statistical
equivalence between groups on all variables, it is the best procedural guard against systematic
differences between groups that would limit confidence in conclusions about causal associations
between the intervention and dependent variables (Kasari, 2002). Historically, randomized tests
of interventions have been exceptionally rare in ASD research (Warren et al., 2011). However,
the recent proliferation of RCTs in this field suggests that random assignment is feasible and
employed frequently enough to permit an evaluation of evidence from randomized trials versus
quasi-experimental studies.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 17
Independence of assessors. Detection bias refers to the risk of bias that arises when
assessors are aware of the group assignment of individual participants. This type of bias
manifests in different ways in studies of autism intervention, and the degree of risk may vary
depending on the extent to which non-independent assessors are involved in outcome
assessment. It is likely that detection bias poses the greatest threat when caregivers participate in
outcome assessment, either as reporters or interaction partners, though the threat is still
substantial in situations wherein outcomes are assessed or coded by professionals that are aware
of group assignment.
Caregiver/teacher report
.
It is common for researchers to rely on parents or teachers to
assess outcomes via standardized interviews and/or report forms in pediatric psychology and
adjacent fields. Because caregivers observe and engage with children for extended periods of
time across a variety of contexts, they can draw on their cross-context knowledge of a child’s
abilities when reporting on an outcome, and may therefore produce scores that are more
representative of a child’s generalized abilities, compared to scores derived from brief
assessments administered by unfamiliar examiners. However, parents and teachers are virtually
always aware of the extent and nature of a child’s participation in an intervention study.
Moreover, they are likely to be personally invested in the outcome of intervention. This
combination of awareness of group assignment and strong investment in positive outcomes can
yield a “placebo by proxy” effect, which can positively bias results in favor of the treatment
group (Grelotti & Kaptchuk, 2011). Prior placebo-controlled studies of pharmacological
interventions such as secretin have demonstrated that these effects can be rather large (Williams,
Wray, & Wheeler, 2012), and present even in simulated clinical trials where no intervention was
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 18
provided (Jones, Carberry, Hamo, & Lord, 2017). Thus, outcomes from caregiver report are
highly subject to systematic measurement error and may positively bias summary estimates of
intervention effects.
Outcomes assessed in interactions with caregivers.
Even in situations that do not involve
standardized report, caregivers can exert undue influence on outcome measurement. This occurs
when caregivers participate as interaction partners in observational measures of outcomes of
interest. Autism researchers frequently use observational measurement to capture social
communication and related skills in the natural contexts in which they arise. For example, scores
of language and communication are often derived from free play sessions with parents, or from
interactions with teachers in the classroom. These scores are fundamentally dyadic; though they
are often assumed to solely represent the skills or behavior of the child, they actually index the
child’s response to the interaction partner. When interaction partners are aware of the
administration of a treatment, they may subconsciously or consciously shift their behavior to
better elicit skill demonstration from the child. Though this threat arises often in studies of
interventions on language and communication outcomes, it is not limited to measures of those
domains. Therefore, outcomes measured in the context of natural interaction with caregivers are
also subject to bias and may influence intervention effect sizes.
Outcomes assessed or coded by professionals aware of group assignment.
Even
unfamiliar professionals can influence outcomes when administering standardized assessments
or coding observational measures of behaviors. A recent systematic review of medical literature
that contained assessment of binary outcomes from both independent and non-independent
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 19
assessors found that assessors that were aware of group assignment exaggerated odds ratios as
much as 36% (Hróbjartsson et al., 2012).
Influential Outcome Characteristics
The Cochrane Collaboration has delineated a set of quality indicators that are applicable
to intervention literature in most fields, but additional field-specific sources of bias also exist for
autism early intervention literature. Further, when it comes to studies of intervention for children
with ASD, we contend that various aspects of outcome measurement can also serve as sources of
bias and should therefore be considered (CITATION REMOVED FOR THE PURPOSES OF
MASKED REVIEW). We summarize two particularly important dimensions of outcome
variables below (boundedness and proximity), and we review one additional source of bias
related to study design that we hypothesize has the potential to influence effect sizes observed
across studies of treatment effects on outcomes of young children with ASD (correlated
measurement error [CME] that arises when parents or teachers are trained in the intervention and
then participate in the data collection).
Boundedness of outcomes to intervention context. Whether or not an intervention
effects change that generalizes beyond the context of an intervention is a question of great
importance. While the context of intervention is generally contrived and temporary, changes
effected by intervention are often assumed to (or at least intended to) extend to natural
environments and the routines of daily life. However, dependent variables vary in the extent to
which they index generalized change. Those that are measured within the context of intervention,
or in a context that is similar to intervention across several dimensions (i.e., materials, setting,
interaction partners, interaction style), may reflect changes that are potentially bound to the
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 20
intervention context. In contrast, dependent variables that are measured in a context that differs
from the intervention on several dimensions should reflect highly generalized changes. For
example, in the hypothetical study of an intervention that is administered during play with a
therapist, outcomes measured in a play-based interaction with a familiar therapist and similar
toys may index change that is bound to that context. The outcome measure does not afford any
degree of confidence that the treatment has induced changes in child behavior that would
generalize to other contexts. In contrast, outcomes measured using standardized assessment
procedures (i.e., different interaction style and materials) administered by an unfamiliar examiner
(i.e., different interaction partner) would likely reflect change that reaches across a wide range of
contexts. Similarly, outcomes measured in the home environment in an interaction with a parent
(i.e., different setting, interaction partner, and interaction style, assuming the parent has not been
trained in the intervention), would serve as a naturalistic assessment of highly generalized
change in this hypothetical study. In theory, generalized change is more difficult to effect than
context-bound change, so effect sizes for generalized outcomes are likely to be smaller relative
to effect sizes of outcomes that are potentially context-bound.
Proximity of outcomes to intervention targets. Outcomes may also vary by their
proximity to the targets or goals of the intervention. Ideally, interventions would be able to
demonstrate change not only on outcomes that are directly taught or addressed by the
intervention (i.e., proximal
outcomes), but also on outcomes that are developmentally
downstream from what is directly taught or addressed (i.e., distal
outcomes). When interventions
are able to demonstrate growth on distal outcomes, they are essentially providing evidence that
the intervention is influencing children’s development, which may mean that the intervention
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 21
will continue to have effects long after the intervention has stopped. However, prior best
evidence syntheses have shown that early interventions for children with ASD show much larger
effects for proximal as compared to distal outcomes (Yoder et al., 2013).
Correlated measurement error in parent/teacher mediated interventions. In addition
to other commonly cited sources of bias, studies of autism-specific interventions are frequently
threatened by CME that occurs when parents or teachers are the interventionists and also
participate in assessment procedures. Parents and teachers are primary figures in the lives of
children with ASD, and this makes them ideal mediators of intervention. For this reason,
researchers have developed a number of interventions that target parents and teachers as
interventionists, and tested their effectiveness in parent- or teacher-training studies. Trouble
arises when natural interaction partners are trained as interventionists over the course of a study
while simultaneously participating as assessors, either by rating child outcomes via a
standardized report, or by serving as the interaction partner in an observational assessment
context. The risk of bias posed by this specific study design flaw extends beyond that posed by
detection bias related to the non-independence of assessors. This is because, in addition to being
aware of group assignment, the assessors and assessment context has also changed from pre to
post intervention in a manner that favors the intervention group. For example, a study might test
the effects of parent-training for improving communication in children with ASD by examining
the frequency of child communication during free play with parents. Prior to intervention, the
assessment context in both groups would feature a parent naive to strategies for eliciting
communication. However, after intervention, the assessment context in the treatment group
would feature a parent who is more adept at eliciting communication while the assessment
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 22
context in the control group remained the same. Though these two assessment contexts seem
identical, they are fundamentally different. Though studies of parent- and teacher-led
interventions are not unique to this population, they are well-represented in autism intervention
literature. As such, any assessment of study quality should include an evaluation of the potential
influence of this field-specific source of bias.
Study Purpose and Research Questions
The purpose of this study is to gather and synthesize all available studies of
nonpharmacological interventions targeting any outcome in children with ASD below the age of
8 years. Our specific research questions were:
1. Across all eligible quasi-experimental and experimental studies, are summary effects
positive and significant for targeted outcomes for each of seven intervention types
(behavioral, developmental, NDBI, TEACCH, sensory-based, animal-assisted, and
technology-based)?
2. Are summary effects positive and significant for targeted outcomes for each of the
aforementioned seven intervention types when only outcomes from studies with basic
quality controls (i.e., random assignment, independent assessors) are included?
3. Across intervention and outcome types, are summary effects for proximal outcomes
larger than summary effects for distal outcomes?
4. Across intervention and outcome types, are summary effects for outcomes that measure
context-bound behaviors larger than summary effects for outcomes that measure more
highly generalized characteristics?
Method
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 23
Search
Search Terms and Databases. To gather the peer-reviewed literature included in the
current meta-analysis, the following nine online databases were searched: Academic Search
Complete, CINAHL Plus with Full Text, Education Source, Educational Administration
Abstracts, ERIC, MEDLINE, PsycINFO, Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection, and
SocINDEX with Full Text. Search terms were used in various combinations to capture the
diagnostic criteria and intervention designs included within the search. The individual databases
were searched using the following terms: autis*, ASD, PDD, Aspergers, intervention, therapy,
teach*, treat*, program, package, assign*, control group, BAU, “wait list”, RCT, random*,
quasi, “treatment group”, “intervention group”, “group design”, and trial. This initial search
yielded 12,933 results from academic journals, dissertations, books, reports, conference
materials, and reviews.
To gather grey literature, or studies not published in peer-reviewed journals, investigators
who received federal grants to study autism were identified through a search of the National
Database for Autism Research (NDAR), the National Institutes of Health (NIH) Matchmaker,
and Institute of Education Sciences (IES) websites. A list of researchers (n
= 106) was generated,
and 90 of these investigators were emailed with a request for eligible data. The contact
information for the remaining investigators could not be found.
Screening process. A preliminary screen of abstracts was first completed using abstrackr
(Wallace, Small, Brodley, Lau, & Thomas, 2012). Studies were screened at the full-text level if
they met the following inclusion criteria: (a) published in English, (b) published from 1970 -
present, (c) group design that included both an intervention and control group, (d) a simple
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 24
majority of participants were reported to have a diagnosis of ASD, and (e) the average age of
included participants was between 0 and 8 years. In many instances, though studies met inclusion
criteria; insufficient information was provided to enable the extraction of unadjusted effect sizes.
In these cases, authors were identified and emailed with a request to provide unadjusted
post-intervention means and standard deviations. The PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for
Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) diagram in Figure 1 summarizes the search process and
provides justifications for exclusion of articles.
Coding Procedures
Included studies were coded for participant characteristics, intervention characteristics,
study characteristics (including quality indicators), outcome characteristics, and effect size
information. The coding manual is available upon request from the first author.
Participant characteristics. Participant characteristics coded from studies included
average age of participant samples in months, percentage of sample that was male, and average
language age in months (either receptive, expressive, or total) whenever it was reported.
Intervention characteristics. Intervention approaches were categorized based on the
specific techniques used and the underlying philosophies that motivated the approach. A set of
candidate categories (behavioral, developmental, NDBI, sensory-based, technology-based,
cognitive behavior therapy, other) were drafted in the first instantiation of the coding manual for
this synthesis based on authors’ knowledge of intervention literature. Based on the results of our
literature search and screening process, as well as the range of intervention approaches
encountered across our team’s initial training on coding precision and reliability, intervention
categories were further refined to include ‘animal-assisted therapy’. This intervention approach
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 25
was found to be motivated by a distinct theoretical framework and to have amassed a sufficient
number of group design studies to permit prior systematic review and meta-analysis (O’Haire,
2013; 2017; Trzmiel, Purandare, Michalak, Zasadzka, & Pawlaczyk, 2018). Thus, interventions
were initially coded as animal-assisted therapy, behavioral, developmental, NDBI, cognitive
behavior therapy, sensory-based, technology-based, or other. After completion of coding, the set
of interventions coded as ‘other’ were re-examined to determine whether there existed a
sufficient set of similar studies (e.g., 5 or more) that could be meaningfully combined to
comprise an additional category. This was the case for studies of the TEACCH intervention.
Studies of TEACCH that were initially coded as ‘other’ were, therefore, re-coded as ‘TEACCH’.
Animal-assisted therapy.
Interventions coded as animal-assisted therapy were those
mediated through the presence of an animal. Equine Assisted Therapy was an example listed in
the coding manual.
Behavioral.
Interventions were coded as behavioral if authors described the intervention
as being heavily situated in operant theories of learning, or if they relied heavily on behavior
analytic techniques, such as didactic instruction, prompting, shaping, and extrinsic
reinforcement. Examples of behavioral interventions listed in the coding manual included EIBI,
PECS, Discrete Trial Training, Verbal Behavior, Autism Partnership, and the Lovaas Model.
Developmental.
Interventions were coded as developmental if they were described as
being motivated by constructivist theories of learning, or if they were heavily child-led and
implemented according to a typical developmental sequence, with the goal of facilitating the
development of foundational skills that would translate to gains in developmentally downstream
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 26
domains. Examples of developmental interventions featured in the coding manual included
DIR/Floortime, Hanen models, and Responsive Teaching.
NDBIs.
Interventions were coded as NDBIs if they were one of any of the named
interventions in the consensus paper on this intervention approach (Schreibman et al., 2015), or
if they combined adult-led, behavioral teaching methods with child-led routines and taught to a
natural developmental progression within naturalistic settings. These included Incidental
Teaching, Pivotal Response Treatment, Early Start Denver Model, Enhanced Milieu Teaching,
Reciprocal Imitation Training, Project ImPACT, JASPER, SCERTS, Early Achievements, and
Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching. Although Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching is not explicitly listed as
an example of an NDBI in the consensus paper by Schriebman and colleagues, we contacted
Paul Yoder, a leading researcher of this intervention, while drafting the coding manual for this
meta-analysis to verify that this would be the appropriate category for this intervention approach
(Personal communication, March 29, 2018).
Cognitive behavior therapy.
Interventions were coded as cognitive behavior therapy if a
study explicitly named the intervention as such.
Sensory-based interventions.
Interventions were coded as sensory-based if they
incorporated targeted exposure to sensory or multisensory (e.g., auditory, visual, tactile,
olfactory) stimuli. Examples listed in the coding manual included sensory integration, music
therapy, massage, acupuncture, auditory integration, and weighted blankets. This category was
drafted based on precedent across prior reviews of sensory-based interventions (Baranek, 2002;
Case-Smith and Arbesman, 2008; Weitlauf et al., 2017).
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 27
Technology-based interventions.
Interventions were coded as technology-based if the
intervention was primarily delivered on a computer or electronic device (i.e., iPad, DVD).
TEACCH.
Interventions were re-coded as TEACCH if a study explicitly identified using
this method.
Other.
Interventions that did not fit into the previously defined categories were coded as
other.
Study characteristics. Study-level characteristics that were coded include design type
(i.e., randomized control trial [RCT] or quasi-experimental), publication status (i.e., indexed,
non-indexed, unpublished), and several features of study quality. Studies were coded as a
randomized controlled trial if the text indicated that participants were randomly assigned to an
intervention group and a control group or contrasting treatment, or if the authors referred to the
study as “randomized.” Studies were coded as quasi-experimental when authors made no
indication that the process of group allocation was random. If a contrasting treatment model was
used, the group receiving the treatment that was hypothesized by the authors to effect greater
change was considered the treatment group. In the case of studies testing multiple active
treatment groups compared to a passive control, treatment characteristics and effects were coded
separately in contrast to control.
Publications were coded for whether they were published or unpublished. Published
studies included indexed and non-indexed journals, and unpublished studies included
dissertations and theses. Despite our extensive attempts to locate, obtain, and include
unpublished data apart from dissertations and theses, no researchers provided us with
unpublished data sets or effect sizes.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 28
Studies were coded for several indicators of study quality. These indicators included
those specified by the Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias (e.g., selection
bias, performance bias, detection bias, reporting bias; Higgins et al., 2011), as well as additional
indicators which we proposed in prior work (e.g., potential presence of CME related to
parent/teacher training, sufficient number of participants to justify statistical analysis, reliance on
parent or teacher report; Yoder et al., 2013). Selection bias related to insufficient randomization
procedures and allocation concealment was coded as “high”, “low”, or “unclear” for studies
coded as randomized controlled trials, and as “not applicable” for quasi-experimental studies.
For subsequent Cochrane quality indicators, risk of bias was coded as “high” or “low” if studies
explicitly indicated or provided sufficient information to ascertain the presence or absence of
such risk, and as “unclear” if information related to risk potential was not detailed. Risk of
selective reporting bias was coded as high if outcomes were reported to have been collected at
post but were not reported in results, or if an entire assessment was administered but only
selective subscores were reported without sufficient justification. Performance bias risk was
assessed in consideration of the participants’ and families’ awareness of their group assignment.
Detection bias accounted for the independence of assessors and coders. We elected to include
interaction partners in naturalistic observational measures as “assessors,” given that they may
transiently influence child behavior during interactions. Attrition bias was coded with respect to
the number of participants recruited and the number of participants included in analysis.
Specifically, attrition bias was considered low if attrition was lower than 20% or if intent-to-treat
analysis was utilized.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 29
Outcome characteristics. In addition to the above quality indicators, we coded for
outcome-level quality indicators that are especially important for research on intervention in
young children with ASD. These quality indicators captured the boundedness and distality of
outcomes, as well as the potential presence of CME related to parent or teacher training.
Outcomes were coded as context-bound if they were measured in or very near the context of the
intervention, and as generalized if they were measured in a context that differed from the context
of intervention on multiple dimensions (e.g., interaction partners, materials, setting, interaction
style). Outcomes taken from standardized parent/teacher reports were coded as potentially
context-bound if reporters were also the primary mediators of intervention, based on the rationale
that their report could be based on their observance of the outcome as it occurred within the
context of the intervention they provided. Outcomes were coded as proximal if they indexed
skills that were directly taught, modeled, or prompted during the intervention, and otherwise as
distal. Outcomes indexed by developmentally scaled assessments were automatically coded as
distal, based on the reasoning that these assessments are meant to tap generalized development
rather than specific skills. We recognize that it is possible for an intervention to directly target
specific items of a developmentally scaled assessment, but reasoned that in the absence of an
extremely detailed description of intervention procedures, we should assume these assessments
captured constructs beyond what was directly taught in intervention. Decision trees used to judge
distality and boundedness are presented in Figures 2 and 3, respectively. Correlated measurement
error related to parent/teacher training was coded as potentially present when parents or teachers
operated as both the mediators of intervention as well as the outcome assessors.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 30
Outcome categorization. Each dependent variable was categorized as either a core
feature of ASD (i.e., social communication; restricted/repetitive patterns of behaviors, interests,
or activities; sensory) or a related outcome (i.e., language, motor, adaptive, cognitive, academic,
play, sleep, brain imaging, social emotional/challenging behavior). If outcomes were reported at
multiple time points, immediate and follow-up outcomes were coded separately.
Effect size information. Unadjusted means, SDs, and n
s were extracted from all eligible
studies that reported a group difference between participants receiving the specified intervention
and those not receiving the specified intervention. Group difference effect sizes were calculated
for each outcome using the standardized mean difference (d
), as derived via the Campbell
Collaboration Practical Meta-Analysis Effect Size Calculator (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001) and then
converted to the effect size metric used for analyses, Hedge’s g (g
). Effect sizes were reported in
such a way that higher g
scores indicated superior performance in the treatment group.
We were unable to extract effect sizes from some eligible studies due to insufficient
information (e.g., authors did not report means and SDs, reported only mean change scores, or
reported means and SDs that were adjusted for baseline covariates and therefore could not be
meta-analyzed with unadjusted means and SDs). When this occurred for articles published
within the last ten years, we contacted the corresponding author(s) in an attempt to obtain either
the unadjusted post means and SDs, or any other statistical information that would allow us to
calculate the standardized mean difference between treatment and control/contrast groups after
intervention. Fifty-five studies did not have sufficient information to allow effect size extraction
for all outcomes. In the case of nine of these studies, effect size extraction was possible for some
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 31
but not all outcomes, so eligible outcomes were included from those studies. Authors responded
and supplied effect size information for 14 additional studies.
Reliability
A primary coder (the first author) read and coded all studies. All studies were also
independently coded for reliability by one coder from a team of nine. Both coding sheets were
then sent to a separate coding auditor who examined codesheets for discrepancies and reported
any disagreements between coders. Original primary and reliability codes were then saved for
reliability analyses in a separate folder, and all disagreements were addressed in discrepancy
discussions between the primary and reliability coders. Discrepancies were considered resolved
once both coders agreed to a final consensus code, which was then added to the dataset used for
the final analyses. Therefore, we are able to report reliability data from the original coding and
also confirm that all
disagreements were resolved prior to statistical analysis.
All reliability calculations were completed in R studio (R Core Team, 2017) using the irr
package (Gamer, Lemon, Fellows, & Singh, 2012). Reliability was indexed using unweighted
kappa for all categorical variables (Cohen, 1960) and one-way random intraclass correlation
coefficients for all continuous variables (ICC; Shrout & Fleiss, 1979). Kappas ranged from
0.602-0.923, and average kappa across all categorical variables was 0.751. ICCs ranged from
0.676-0.999, and average ICC across all continuous variables was 0.916.
Analysis
All analyses were conducted in R (R Core Team, 2017). To account for the nesting of
multiple effect sizes within overlapping participant samples, we used robust variance estimation
(RVE) with small sample adjustments when synthesizing effect sizes and conducting
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 32
meta-regressions (Hedges, Tipton, & Johnson, 2010; Tanner-Smith, Tipton, & Polanin, 2016).
These procedures account for the non-independence of effect size statistics drawn from
overlapping samples, and provide test statistics and confidence intervals that are adjusted based
on how the effect sizes are clustered.
Effect sizes were aggregated based on type of outcome (see Outcome characteristics)
within each type of intervention (see Intervention characteristics). Aggregating the results in this
manner provided a summary statistic for the effect of each intervention type on each outcome
type. Meta-regression analyses were conducted on the coded variables of distality and
boundedness (see Outcome characteristics) to determine whether the magnitude of the effects
across intervention and outcome types were moderated by these categorical characteristics
related to measurement. The threshold level of significance for these tests was set at p
< .10,
given that we had clear directional hypotheses for each potential moderator, meriting one-tailed
tests of significance. To examine the potential presence of publication bias, we examined funnel
plots of effect size estimates against their standard errors, and corresponding Egger’s tests of
funnel plot asymmetry, for each summary effect estimate. Due to the large number of
significance tests this demanded, we applied the Benjimini-Yekutieli false discovery rate
correction to the significance values from the Egger’s tests to correct for spurious findings using
the Hmisc package in R (Harrell, 2018). The Robumeta package in R (Fisher, Tipton, &
Zhipeng, 2017) was used to conduct these analyses while the Metafor package (Viechtbauer,
2010) was used to graph the forest plots and funnel plots.
Results
Descriptives of Included Study Samples and Outcomes
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 33
The search and screening process yielded 1,615 effect sizes gathered from 130
independent study samples (from a total of 150 reports) representing 6,240 participants. Across
all studies, the average age of participants was 54.21 months (SD =
18.98), the average
proportion of male participants per sample was 0.84 (SD
= 0.07), and the average language age
of participants in studies for which it was reported was 22.68 months (SD
= 11.91). An average
of 12.4 outcomes were reported for a single study sample (MIN
= 1, MAX
= 100, MDN
= 8).
Participant characteristics according to intervention type are reported in Table 1. There were 27
studies of behavioral interventions, 14 studies of developmental interventions, 26 studies of
NDBIs, seven studies of sensory-based interventions, ten studies of technology-based
interventions, and six studies of TEACCH included in the synthesis. The RVE approach requires
that at least five studies contribute to the generation of effect sizes, so the studies representing
animal-assisted intervention (n
= 4), cognitive behavioral therapy (n
= 2), and other varied
approaches that could not be meaningfully combined into intervention types (n
= 29) were
excluded from summary effect estimation.
Study Quality
Figures 4 and 5 illustrate the proportion of studies or outcomes that received each quality
rating (i.e., low risk of bias, high risk of bias, unable to determine) for seven key quality
indicators, according to intervention type. These figures include only studies that contributed to
summary effect estimation. Because it is almost always impossible for participants to be naive to
intervention delivery in studies of nonpharmacological interventions for ASD, performance bias
was rated as high for all but one study included in summary effect estimation and, thus, is not
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 34
reported separately for each intervention type (see Corbett, Schickman, & Ferrer, 2008 for the
lone exception).
Behavioral intervention studies. Figure 4 reflects information regarding quality
indicator ratings for studies of behavioral interventions. Notably, only 29.63% of studies of
behavioral interventions were RCTs. Detection bias was rated as high for 77.05% of outcomes in
behavioral studies. High detection bias in this set of studies was largely driven by an overreliance
on reports completed by individuals who were aware of intervention assignment – 60.33% of
outcomes were based on parent or teacher report. Correlated measurement error related to
parent/teacher training threatened 53.77% of outcomes reported in behavioral studies. Since
many of the studies relied on standardized report forms, and because most of these studies only
loosely described intervention targets, 86.23% of outcomes tracked in behavioral intervention
studies were categorized as distal to the intervention targets. Half (50.49%) of outcomes were
categorized as generalized, and 10.49% were classified as context bound. The remaining 39.02%
of outcomes were categorized as potentially context bound, because they were derived from
caregiver reports in studies where caregivers participated as interventionists (meaning that it is
unclear if the outcome could be demonstrated in interactions with individuals who were not
trained as interventionists). Bias related to substantial attrition (i.e., > 20% of the study sample)
was rated as high for 15.41% of all outcomes.
Developmental intervention studies. Figure 4 reflects quality indicator ratings for
studies of developmental interventions. A large majority (78.57%) of included developmental
studies were RCTs. Detection bias was rated as high for 53.97% of outcomes, but this was not
due entirely to over-reliance on caregiver report. Nearly a third (29%) of outcomes were taken
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 35
from parent/teacher report. The remainder of outcomes flagged for high detection bias
(approximately half of the outcomes tracked in these studies) reflects the common practice of
measuring language and communication outcomes in the context of interactions with natural
communication partners (primarily parents, who were aware of group assignment). CME related
to parent/teacher training threatened three quarters (75%) of all outcomes in developmental
studies. Since many of the developmental interventions were explicitly described as targeting
language and social communication, and many of the outcomes were observational measures of
language and social communicative behaviors, approximately half (53.57%) of outcomes were
categorized as proximal to intervention targets. Approximately a quarter (27.84%) of outcomes
were categorized as generalized, a quarter (25%) were categorized as potentially context-bound,
and approximately one half (47.16%) were categorized as context-bound. Over a third (34.66%)
of all outcomes were subject to high bias from substantial attrition.
Naturalistic developmental behavioral intervention studies. Figure 4 illustrates
quality indicator ratings for included studies of NDBIs. A large majority (76.92%) of included
studies of NDBIs were RCTs. Detection bias was rated as high for 59.42% of outcomes. This
was due, in part, to the common use of observational measures of skills coded from natural
interactions with interaction partners who were aware of group assignment. Only 17% (the
lowest of any intervention type) of outcomes were collected from parent/teacher report.
However, CME related to parent/teacher training threatened 47.09% of outcomes, due to a
prevalence of parent-training studies which included outcomes derived from parent-child
interactions. Because many NDBIs were described as specifically targeting symbolic play, early
social communication, and language, researcher-created measures of these skills were coded as
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 36
proximal to intervention targets. Thus, nearly half (47.59%) of outcomes in NDBI studies were
categorized as proximal. Nearly a quarter (22.22%) were categorized as generalized, 52.41%
were categorized as potentially context-bound, and another quarter (26.36%) were categorized as
context-bound. Only 7.25% of outcomes were subject to bias from high attrition.
Sensory-based intervention studies. Figure 5 reflects quality indicator ratings for
sensory-based intervention studies that were included in summary effect size estimation. All of
the seven studies included in effect size estimation were RCTs. Since language was the only
outcome category for which there were a sufficient number of sensory-based intervention studies
to permit summary effect size estimation, the following outcome-level quality indicator ratings
apply only to the language outcomes (n = 13) tracked in these studies. Detection bias was rated
as high for nearly half (46.15%) of all language outcomes. Nearly a third (30.77%) of all
outcomes were based on parent/teacher report, and these same outcomes were also subject to
CME related to parent training. The overwhelming majority (92%) of outcomes were categorized
as distal, because few sensory-based interventions were described as directly targeting language.
Nearly a third (30.77%) were categorized as generalized, 53.86% were categorized as potentially
context-bound, and 15.38% of outcomes were categorized as context-bound. Attrition bias was
rated as high for 15.38% of outcomes.
TEACCH studies. Figure 5 illustrates quality indicator ratings for studies of TEACCH
that were included in summary effect size estimation (n = 6). Only two (33%) of these studies
were RCTs. Detection bias was rated as high for the majority (81.81%) of outcomes, and this
was largely driven by an over-reliance on parent/teacher report, from which 77.27% of outcomes
were derived. CME related to parent/teacher training threatened half (50%) of all outcomes.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 37
Given that the explicit individual intervention targets of TEACCH were not thoroughly
described, and that the majority of outcomes were taken from standardized parent/teacher
reports, almost all (95.45%) outcomes were assumed to be distal. Nearly half (45.45%) of
outcomes were categorized as generalized, half (50%) were categorized as potentially
context-bound, and the remaining 4.54% were categorized as context-bound. None (0%) of the
studies reported substantial attrition.
Technology-based intervention studies. Figure 5 illustrates quality indicator ratings for
studies of technology-based interventions. Of ten technology-based intervention studies included
in summary effect estimation, eight (80%) were RCTs. Detection bias was rated as high for
64.28% of all outcomes. Over a third (38.1%) of outcomes were taken from parent/teacher
report. CME related to parent/teacher training threatened 30.95% of outcomes. Over half
(53.57%) of outcomes were categorized as distal. Nearly a third (30.95%) of outcomes were
categorized as generalized, nearly half (47.62%) were categorized as potentially context-bound,
and 21.43% were categorized as context-bound. Bias related to substantial attrition was rated as
high for 15.38% of outcomes.
Summary Effects by Intervention and Outcome Type
Summary effects across all studies without consideration of quality indicators.
Figure 6 reflects summary effect size estimates within interventions and outcome types. These
estimates were derived using all available effect sizes, both from quasi-experimental studies and
RCTs. Summary effects were computed when effect sizes associated with a given outcome and
intervention type were available from at least five independent participant samples. Thus, we
were able to estimate the summary effects of behavioral interventions on adaptive outcomes,
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 38
cognitive outcomes, language outcomes, motor outcomes, social communication outcomes,
social emotional/challenging behavior outcomes, and outcomes quantifying broader autism
symptomatology. Summary effects for behavioral interventions across outcome types ranged
from 0.24 to 0.46 and were all statistically significant. For developmental interventions, only
language and social communication outcomes were measured in a sufficient number of studies to
permit the estimation of summary effects. The summary effects of developmental interventions
on these outcomes were 0.06 and 0.30, respectively, and only the estimate for social
communication was statistically significant. The summary effects of NDBIs were separately
estimated for adaptive outcomes, cognitive outcomes, language outcomes, play outcomes,
restrictive and repetitive behaviors, social communication outcomes, social
emotional/challenging behavior outcomes, and outcomes that quantified broader autism
symptomatology. These summary effects ranged from -0.01 to 0.35. The summary effect
estimates of NDBIs on cognition, language, play, and social communication outcomes were
statistically significant. For sensory-based interventions, only language outcomes were measured
in a sufficient number of studies to permit the estimation of summary effects. This summary
effect estimate was 0.28, and was not significant. For TEACCH, summary effects could be
generated only for social communication outcomes. This summary effect estimate was -0.11 and
was not significant. For technology-based interventions, the most frequently tracked outcomes
were social communication and social emotional/challenging behavior. Summary effect
estimates for these outcomes were 0.05 and 0.42, respectively, and neither were significant.
Summary effects from RCTs. Figure 7 reflects summary effect size estimates derived
exclusively from outcomes extracted from RCTs, according to intervention and outcome type.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 39
There were not enough RCTs of behavioral interventions to permit summary effect estimation
for any outcome type. For developmental interventions, the summary effect across social
communication outcomes from RCTs was 0.27 and significant. For NDBIs, a sufficient number
of RCTs permitted the estimation of summary effects on cognition, language, play, and social
communication. These estimates ranged from 0.18 to 0.42, and were significant for language,
play, and social communication. All of the studies tracking the effect of sensory-based
interventions on language outcomes were RCTs. Therefore, this summary effect estimate
remains identical to that of the initial model. For technology-based interventions, there were only
enough RCTs to permit estimation of a summary effect for social communication. This was 0.06
and was not significant. There were no RCTs examining the effects of the TEACCH intervention
on any outcome.
Summary effects from RCTs excluding outcomes from caregiver reports. Figure 8
reflects summary effects estimated exclusively from outcomes that were extracted from RCTs
and that were not based on caregiver report. For developmental interventions, a sufficient
number of studies and outcomes permitted the estimation of a summary effect for social
communication, which was 0.31 and statistically significant. For NDBIs, summary effect
estimation was possible for cognition, language, play, and social communication. These effects
ranged from 0.18 to 0.47, and were significant in the cases of play and social communication
outcomes. For sensory-based interventions, summary effect estimation was possible for language
only. This estimate was 0.28 and was not significant.
Summary effects from RCTs excluding all outcomes subject to a high threat of
detection bias. Figure 9 reflects summary effects estimated exclusively from outcomes that were
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 40
extracted from RCTs where assessors were unaware of group assignment. There were enough
studies/effect sizes of this nature to permit estimation of the summary effects of NDBIs on
language and social communication only. These estimates were 0.17 and 0.17, respectively, and
were not significant.
Publication Bias Analyses
Funnel plots and Egger’s test results are included in the supplementary materials
accompanying this report. Corrected p
-values for Egger’s tests for funnel plot asymmetry were
significant for adaptive and social communication outcomes from studies of NDBIs, suggesting
that publication bias may have threatened these summary estimates.
Moderator Analyses
Meta-regression analyses across the entire dataset suggested that summary effects were
significantly larger for outcomes that were proximal compared to those that were distal (β
=
0.171, p
= 0.024). Boundedness was also a significant source of effect size variance; effect sizes
coded as generalized (β =
-0.170, p
= .076) were smaller than those coded as potentially
context-bound or context bound (β =
-0.115, p
= 0.22).
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to locate, evaluate, and synthesize all available
quasi-experimental and RCT investigations of nonpharmacological interventions for children
with ASD in terms of methodological quality and summary effect. Results suggest that some
intervention approaches show promise for improving a range of outcomes, while others have
amassed relatively limited evidence of effectiveness to date. The number of RCT investigations
in this area have increased precipitously, but low methodological rigor remains a concern.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 41
Promising Intervention Types
We consider intervention types for which significant summary effects were shown for at
least one outcome, when two important quality indicators were taken into account
(randomization and abstention from using caregiver reports) to be ‘promising’. NDBIs and
developmental interventions meet these criteria.
NDBI. This is the first paper to report summary effects of NDBIs since the 2015
consensus paper that established this new category of intervention as a blend of traditional
behavioral and developmental approaches. By far, NDBIs have emerged as the intervention type
most supported by evidence from RCTs. These studies suggest NDBIs may be particularly useful
for supporting development of social communication, language, and play skills. Studies of
NDBIs were also the least likely to rely on caregiver report as a primary index of intervention
effectiveness. However, we note that when outcomes subject to all forms of detection bias were
excluded from summary effect estimation, there was no category of outcomes for this
intervention type that reached significance. In addition, our results suggest that publication bias
may have threatened overall summary estimates for adaptive and social communication outcome
types. However, asymmetry in these funnel plots may also be due to other methodological design
flaws, such as the presence of detection bias.
Developmental. Evidence suggests that developmental interventions may be particularly
effective for supporting the acquisition of social communication skills, which represents a core
challenge for young children with ASD. This conclusion is supported even when outcomes from
quasi-experimental studies and caregiver report are excluded. However, a substantial portion of
outcomes were subject to high detection bias due to interaction partners or assessors that were
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 42
aware of group assignment. When these outcomes are excluded, the remaining studies are too
few in number to permit summary effect estimation for any outcome type. A key assumption of
developmental interventions is that targeted gains in social communication will facilitate
cascading developments in the domain of language. This assumption was not supported by our
meta-analysis, as the summary effect of developmental interventions on language outcomes was
not significant. However, we did locate compelling evidence suggesting that early targeted
improvements in the synchrony of parent-child interactions can yield longitudinal improvements
in the core challenges associated with ASD, which are detectable with standardized,
independently administered assessments (Green et al., 2010; Pickles et al., 2016). Green and
colleagues (2010) study of the Preschool Autism Communication Trial (PACT) supports the
notion that proximal changes effected by intervention can facilitate long-term change in
developmentally distal outcomes, even in the absence of continued intervention. It also provides
an example of methodological rigor to which the field should aspire, as it employed random
assignment, pre-registered analyses, independent evaluators, and clearly defined proximal and
distal outcomes.
Intervention Types with Some Evidence of Effectiveness
Behavioral. Behavioral intervention, specifically EIBI and related variants, is the most
commonly recommended intervention approach for children with ASD, with many states
specifying behavioral interventions explicitly in insurance coverage mandates (“Autism and
insurance coverage,” 2018). Indeed, the large number of behavioral intervention studies (n
= 27)
that met our search criteria also suggests this is the most studied intervention approach for this
population. Considered as a whole, without regard to quality of evidence, these studies support
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 43
the effectiveness of behavioral interventions for improving a wide range of outcomes for
children with ASD. However, only a fraction of past studies exploring the effects of traditional
behavioral interventions were RCTs, and the majority of outcomes contributing to summary
effect sizes were taken from caregiver report. Thus, the relatively low quality of this set of
intervention literature limits our confidence in the accuracy of the summary effect sizes
estimated in the initial model. A notable exception is the sole RCT which examined the effects of
EIBI on standardized measures of cognition and language administered by independent
evaluators (Smith, Groen, & Wynn, 2000). Though the positive results of this study are
encouraging, they have persisted without replication for nearly 20 years. The dramatic increase
in published RCTs in the intervening years since this study’s publication stand as proof that high
quality group experimental investigations of autism-specific interventions are both possible and
necessary in order to to unquestionably establish the effectiveness of interventions that are so
routinely recommended. In the meantime, clinicians are encouraged to expand their knowledge
and skills to include naturalistic approaches that center the principles of early childhood
development. States with insurance mandates that explicitly cover traditional behavioral
interventions should furthermore revise their policies to also include NDBI and developmental
approaches, given that these approaches have now accrued substantial evidence for effects in
young children on the autism spectrum from recently-published RCTs.
Intervention Types with Little Evidence of Effectiveness
Sensory. Several previous systematic reviews have concluded that sensory-based
interventions have amassed little evidence supporting their effectiveness to date (e.g., Barton,
Reichow, Schnitz, Smith, & Sherlock, 2015; Case-Smith, Weaver, & Fristad, 2015). Our results
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 44
are consistent with these conclusions. Relatively few group design studies of sensory-based
interventions specifically focused on young children with ASD (i.e., with a mean age < 8 years)
were located. Furthermore, there were not a sufficient number of studies measuring and reporting
sensory outcomes in a manner that permitted extraction of effect size information and estimation
of the summary effect of this intervention approach on what would presumably be the most
proximal outcome (i.e., improvements in sensory function). This is particularly concerning in
light of the fact that sensory differences are highly prevalent in this population (e.g., Ausderau,
Sideris, Furlong, Little, Bulluck, & Baranek, 2014; Ben-Sasson, Hen, Fluss, Cermak,
Engel-Yeger, & Gal, 2009; Leekam, Nieto, Libby, Wing, & Gould, 2007) and have been found
to be associated with some aspects of child stress (Corbett, Schupp, Levine, & Mendoza, 2009).
Unfortunately, across all included studies, we found no evidence that any
intervention type had
the potential to influence sensory outcomes in children with ASD. When we were able to
estimate summary effects of sensory-based interventions, as was the case for language outcomes,
the relative paucity of studies limited the precision of our estimates. Though the summary effect
estimate for sensory-based interventions on language outcomes is similar in magnitude to those
of behavioral and NDBI approaches, this estimate is surrounded by a much wider confidence
band, which overlaps with zero (i.e., the effect is not significant).
It should be noted that our category of sensory-based interventions was broad and
included intervention approaches as distinct as Sensory Integration Therapy, Tomatis Sound
Therapy™, and music therapy. The heterogeneity of these intervention approaches may limit the
conclusions that can be drawn from this summary effect size estimate, as the theoretical
underpinnings and clinical procedures do vary across approaches. It may be useful to consider
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 45
the evidence for each of these intervention approaches separately, though the limited number of
studies for each prevented us from computing subgroup effect sizes here. However, we did not
come across any noteworthy high quality studies that suggested that any of the aforementioned
intervention approaches had markedly positive effects on outcomes (though see Schaaf et al.,
2014 which unfortunately did not report outcome data in a manner that would permit derivation
of effect size information for synthesis). We did locate two exceptionally high quality studies
demonstrating null effects of two sensory-based interventions, music therapy (Bieleninik et al.,
2017) and auditory stimulation (Corbett et al., 2008). Therefore, our conclusion that there is
limited high quality evidence to date to support sensory-based interventions for young children
with ASD is based on our quantitative findings as well as our more fine-grained qualitative
observations about this set of literature. Given that sensory features are now a core diagnostic
criteria of ASD (APA, 2013), and given the already widespread implementation of sensory-based
interventions for this population (e.g., Goin-Kochel, Mackintosh, & Myers, 2009; Schaaf &
Case-Smith, 2014), we suggest that more rigorous research of these interventions be conducted
to precisely determine their effects for children with ASD.
TEACCH. Though TEACCH was among the first interventions designed specifically for
individuals with ASD, it also remains relatively under-studied compared to several other
intervention approaches geared towards this population. Few eligible studies of TEACCH were
located, and most were quasi-experimental. This may be because TEACCH is often
conceptualized as a classroom wide intervention, necessitating large, cluster-randomized trials
that are substantially more expensive to implement than clinically-based RCTs. The summary
effect estimated across these studies suggests that there is limited evidence to support the
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 46
effectiveness of TEACCH for improvement of social communication skills, and almost no
evidence to support the effectiveness of TEACCH for the improvement of other core and related
symptoms of ASD.
Technology-based interventions. Although assistive technology is an important support
that must be accessible to autistic individuals, early interventions mediated entirely
through
technology have little evidence to support their effectiveness for improving social
communication or social emotional outcomes in children with ASD. Both of the summary effect
sizes for these outcome types had confidence intervals which included zero. The majority of
technology-based interventions represented in this meta-analysis were DVDs or video games that
targeted social emotional learning and social communication skills. The limited effectiveness of
these interventions may be attributable to the near or total absence of a human interaction partner
in these intervention contexts. Though technological supports have characteristics that might
make them particularly useful to autistic people (e.g., predictable formats of information
delivery, self-paced usage, highly motivating), these supports likely need to be integrated into
interpersonal interactions, which could include computer-mediated interpersonal interactions,
rather than replacing interaction partners entirely in learning situations. This may be particularly
true when the targeted developmental achievements are social in nature. In fact, the integration
of technological supports into other interaction-based interventions is an approach that is
supported by high-quality studies. For example, Kasari and colleagues (2014) integrated speech
generating devices (SGD) into their JASP-EMT early intervention approach, and found gains on
a variety of communication outcomes for preschoolers who were initially minimally verbal,
compared to those that received the same intervention without use of the SGD. In this study,
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 47
technology was integrated into an already well-developed intervention, that had amassed some
degree of empirical support.
This may be a sensible path forward for conceptualizing the utility of new technologies
for early intervention. That is, technology may be most useful when it is integrated into
previously developed and validated approaches as a means to expand the populations of children
with ASD for whom the intervention is accessible, rather than as an intervention in its own right.
In this regard, it is important to consider that the ultimate use of technology is usually separable
from the means by which children are taught to use it. So even the most intuitively designed
technologies will still need to be paired with a validated teaching approach to ensure that
children are able to learn to use the technology in a meaningful way.
Animal-assisted interventions. Although we did locate studies of animal-assisted
interventions, there were too few to permit estimation of summary effect sizes for any outcomes.
The two interventions represented in these studies were EAAT and canine assistance. Several of
these studies relied on caregiver report to index change, and two were flagged for possible
unreported conflicts of interest, as the authors currently provide the interventions in question for
profit (Bass, Duchowny, & Llabre, 2009; Page, 2012). Therefore, there is little quality evidence
to support the effectiveness of animal-assisted interventions for any outcomes for children with
ASD at this time.
Issues Related to Quality Indicators
The results of this study indicate that study quality remains an issue plaguing intervention
research in young children with ASD. Three issues appear especially important to point out,
including the preponderance of quasi-experimental group designs, reliance on caregiver/teacher
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 48
report, and correlated measurement error due to interaction partners or assessors who
participated in the intervention.
Although it is well established that randomized controlled trials offer the best protection
against alternative explanations for intervention effects, quasi-experimental studies continue to
be relied upon in autism intervention research. There are some circumstances wherein
quasi-experimental methods may be appropriate, such as studies aiming to move established
interventions into community settings where groups are already intact and randomizing
participants would be prohibitively costly (e.g., Vivanti et al., 2014). However, our results
suggest that we do not yet have intervention types that can be considered ‘established’ to an
extent that would warrant this strategy. Since there were too few studies to permit the estimation
of summary effects once study design and performance bias were taken into account, we suggest
that researcher and funding resources should continue to focus on establishing study efficacy
using the highest quality designs.
Another area of particular concern is continued reliance on parent/teacher report. These
measures are nearly impossible to administer in such a way that the respondent is unaware of the
child’s participation in an intervention. Indeed, research has shown that when caregivers
complete such measures, an intervention ‘effect’ will be demonstrated if they believe their child
is receiving an intervention even when no intervention has actually occurred (Jones, Carberry,
Hamo, & Lord, 2017). We therefore suggest that early intervention researchers should not rely
on such measures, and instead seek alternative measurement systems that can be administered
and scored by assessors who are unaware of group assignment.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 49
Finally, correlated measurement error that occurs when parents or teachers are trained in
an intervention and also participate as assessors is a common threat to validity that has received
little attention from the field. Continued use of observational measures taken from interactions
with trained caregivers may be fruitful for mediation analyses, in order to verify that
post-treatment group differences in developmentally distal and generalized outcomes are
explained at least in part by changes in reciprocal interactions with caregivers within the context
of intervention. However, researchers should recognize that these measures are biased in favor of
the intervention group, and should therefore not rely on them as a primary index of intervention
effects. Researchers should also employ valid, standardized, independently-administered
assessments as primary outcomes whenever possible. While changes in interactions between a
trained caregiver and child may be important to measure if those interactions are expected to be
the ‘mechanism’ through which the child achieves later developmental milestones, these
interactions may not themselves index improvements in the child’s interactional repertoire. If
researchers consider interactions with a familiar person as the most valid context for outcome
assessment, they can avoid this threat to validity by relying on observational measures taken
from interactions with familiar but untrained interaction partners (e.g., untrained teachers,
untrained parents, untrained siblings, or untrained peers). Use of untrained interaction partners
that are also naive to group assignment will further help researchers address the added threat of
detection bias.
Understanding Intervention Outcomes - Boundedness and Proximity
Replicating previous research syntheses (Yoder et al., 2013; Fuller & Kaiser, 2019) and
confirming our hypotheses, effect sizes were larger for indices of context-bound behaviors as
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 50
compared to generalized child characteristics. This finding confirms that interventions (broadly
considered) produce larger effects on behaviors that are potentially bound to the treatment
context, which are likely easier to change, than on more highly generalized characteristics of
young children with ASD. In certain circumstances, context-bound behavior change may be
considered important. For example, if a study aims to improve children’s classroom engagement,
many would consider it acceptable if these effects did not generalize beyond the classroom, as
the effects are likely only relevant in classroom contexts.
However, many stakeholders may expect interventions aiming to improve child
characteristics associated with longer-term development (e.g., social communication) to produce
gains that generalize to contexts beyond intervention settings. If developmentally important
effects cannot be demonstrated outside intervention settings, it is unlikely that they will continue
to be a part of the child’s behavioral repertoire, in any context, once the intervention has stopped.
Unfortunately, researchers do not always indicate whether their measurement system was
restricted to detecting context-bound behaviors, or if it was able to detect gains in generalized
child characteristics. We encourage researchers to make this distinction clear when presenting
their study design, and when describing potential limitations in the case of studies that
exclusively examine context-bound behavior change.
Our hypothesis was also confirmed in regards to proximity; effect sizes for proximal
outcomes were larger than effect sizes for distal outcomes. Parallel to our findings on
boundedness, this indicates that interventions are more effective at achieving gains on outcomes
that reflect what was directly addressed in the intervention than gains on outcomes that are
broader or beyond what was directly taught. Evidence of distal effects provide some evidence
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 51
that the intervention is tapping into a developmental pathway, which can give researchers
confidence that the intervention will continue to influence children’s development after the
intervention period is over.
There are some caveats to our approach in categorizing outcome proximity. One is that
this concept is likely more accurately described as continual rather than binary. There are
degrees of proximity and distality that we were not able to capture by restricting our coding to
only two categories. A second caveat is that we were limited to the information about the
intervention provided by study authors, which was often quite sparse. When delineating the
focus of the intervention, authors did not always clarify if they were describing the immediate
targets of the intervention, or a developmentally downstream target. Similarly, many studies did
not offer a detailed description of the intervention, which hampered our ability to determine
which outcomes were directly addressed by intervention procedures. Finally, proximity and
distality are conflated with type of measurement system. Norm-referenced, standardized
measures generally assess broad contexts which by definition cannot be directly targeted by
intervention procedures and are therefore categorized as distal. On the other hand, observational
measures of particular behaviors are often designed by researchers specifically to detect the most
immediate effects of intervention (e.g., observational measures of joint engagement for
interventions that seek to increase the amount of time children spend jointly engaged), which
would be categorized as proximal. Thus, proximal measures may be more sensitive to change
than distal measures, while distal measures are likely more construct valid than
researcher-created proximal measures.
Interpreting Findings in Light of the Exclusion of Evidence from SSDs
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 52
It should be reiterated that we exclusively synthesized findings from randomized and
nonrandomized group design studies of interventions for children with ASD. By excluding
studies with single group pretest-posttest designs and SSDs, we have omitted a substantial body
of research that has been used to draw conclusions about evidence-based practice, particularly in
regards to the effectiveness of behavior analytic approaches. In fact, as of 2015, the majority of
the available studies of intervention techniques for children with autism employed SSD (Wong et
al., 2015), though our review and other reviews published since attest to the recent precipitous
increase in group design literature published in this field (French & Kennedy, 2017).
Our decision to exclude SSDs from this meta-analysis was rooted primarily in the lack of
adequate and agreed upon effect size metrics for synthesizing effects (Kratochwill et al., 2013).
However, we believe there are additional insights to be gained from limiting our conclusions
specifically to evidence offered by group design studies. Though SSDs are well-equipped to
identify effective techniques for teaching specific targeted skills, group design studies are
particularly useful for determining whether interventions can facilitate gains in generalized
development. The repeated measurement that is a hallmark of SSDs may allow investigators to
understand variability in specific behaviors associated with careful and controlled changes in the
independent variable, but it limits reliance on validated standardized assessments as outcome
measures. Such assessments, though often time consuming to administer, are likely better
equipped to tap improvements in generalized development than researcher-created
operationalizations of specific behaviors. Thus, if we wish to evaluate whether intervention
facilitates developmental progress in young children with autism on average, an evaluation of
group design studies may, arguably, be more methodologically suited for this purpose. However,
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 53
even though group design studies may be preferable in this regard, ours and other recent work
has shown that a substantial portion of the outcome measures used in clinical trials were overly
specific to the intervention context and targets (Provenzani et al., 2019). Thus, fragmented
measurement approaches continue to limit the conclusions that can be drawn regarding the
effectiveness of autism interventions, both in SSDs and group design studies. This remains a
limitation, both for the body of evidence as a whole, and our conclusions here.
Recommendations for Primary Intervention Research
Given the results of this series of meta-analyses, we propose several recommendations.
While our confidence in summary effect estimates for any
intervention type is hindered by a lack
of high quality studies, we do have single examples of studies that meet the majority of quality
indicators (e.g., Green et al., 2010). This suggests that designing a high-quality study is not an
unreachable challenge for early intervention researchers. It would perhaps incentivize future
high-quality research if funding agencies held investigators to a higher standard and required
basic quality features such as randomized trials and measurement systems that can be
administered in such a way that assessors remain naive to treatment status. At the very least,
caregiver and teacher reports should likely be discarded altogether, as it is already clear that they
introduce bias and render findings largely uninterpretable (Jones et al., 2017). For some domains,
this may mean that new measures will need to be developed and validated that are low-cost to
administer and adequately sensitive to change.
A second recommendation, also related to measurement systems, is that researchers
should provide detailed descriptions of each measure (especially if they are researcher-created),
and the assessment process in which each measure is used. This will allow for an adequate
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 54
assessment of the kinds of bias introduced or avoided by particular approaches to measurement,
and will allow for a determination of whether measures are capturing context-bound behavior
change or generalized characteristics. To make this latter determination, aspects such as the
measurement context, who administered the measure, and the materials and activities used
during measurement should be made clear.
Third, we were quite struck by how little information many studies contained in regards
to the intervention that was tested. Though it is not necessary that every study on a given
intervention provide minute detail of the procedures, it would be helpful if there were at least one
manualized protocol available for each intervention that describes the full set of strategies and
activities involved in implementing the intervention. This would encourage independent
replication of intervention studies, and would allow for a determination of whether the outcomes
measured were proximal or distal to the intervention procedures. To make this distinction,
researchers need to go beyond describing the aims of the intervention- they need to specifically
describe the protocol in such a way that the immediate outcomes of implementing the
intervention are readily discernible.
Fourth, fifty studies were excluded because relevant effect size information was not
published or extractable. In many cases, this was due to exclusive reporting of change scores or
post-intervention means adjusted for various baseline covariates, which should not be
meta-analyzed alongside standardized mean differences extracted from unadjusted means
(Deeks, Higgins, & Altman, 2008). Though we contacted authors in every case wherein studies
were less than 10 years old, many failed to respond. Therefore, we recommend that authors
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 55
reporting results that control for covariates include unadjusted means and SDs in supplementary
materials, to facilitate future attempts at meta-analysis.
Finally, we suggest that an “optimal” intervention design would include paired proximal
and distal measures (or perhaps even include a third, far distal measure) that are expected to be
developmentally connected and malleable to change. The proximal measure should be selected
to capture the immediate effects of the intervention, while the distal measure should be selected
to measure effects hypothesized to be developmentally downstream from proximal effects.
Mediation analyses, in which the proximal measure is the mediating variable and the distal
measure is the outcome variable, could then confirm whether the proposed developmental
pathway between proximal and distal effects was activated by participation in the intervention.
This would allow for a better understanding of the mechanisms or ‘active ingredients’ through
which interventions achieve cascading developmental gains.
Limitations and Future Meta-analytic Research
There are at least three limitations to consider when interpreting the results of this study.
First, despite our best efforts, we were unable to collect any unpublished effect sizes or datasets
apart from dissertations and theses. This could mean that the effect size estimates presented here
are larger than the ‘true’ effects (an interpretation supported by inspection of funnel plots). Our
attempts to gather unpublished effect sizes included searching NIH, NDAR, and IES databases,
and requesting data directly from investigators who were reported to have received funding for
group design intervention research in children with ASD. However, we did not receive any
unpublished data from any researchers, suggesting there may be reticence among researchers to
share their unpublished data. This is unfortunate, as access to unpublished data is critical for
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 56
accurately estimating effect sizes, and accurately assessing the ‘state of the science’. Further,
data sharing practices are critical to ensure replicability of findings (Nuijten, 2018).
A second limitation to consider is that there were too few studies to adequately synthesize
effect sizes for all outcomes and intervention types. This was especially true when quality
indicators were taken into consideration. Researchers will need to commit to conducting
high-quality intervention research in order for future syntheses to accurately draw conclusions
about intervention effectiveness for outcomes of interest in children with autism.
Finally, the heterogeneity of variables within each “outcome type” and treatments
represented within each “intervention type” may limit the interpretability of our summary effect
estimates. While we note that variables and intervention approaches were similar enough to be
categorized with high reliability – kappa coefficients for outcome type and intervention type
coding were 0.862 and 0.907, respectively – categorization of items that differ on a continuum
will always result in the loss of information, and this information may be important for
understanding key components that drive intervention effects. For example, the same
intervention provided with different intensities (i.e., number of hours per week) may yield
different effects. Similarly, intervention effects may differ for variables that share a domain but
are distinct (e.g., social communication variables such as responding to joint attention and
initiating joint attention). More fine-grained analyses within each outcome type could allow us to
answer questions about putative moderators, as well as to calculate subgroup effect sizes for
identical outcome types across studies (e.g., Vineland scores), or identical interventions (e.g.,
PECS) as the literature base on treatment effects in children with ASD continues to grow.
Conclusions
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 57
The current study differs from existing reviews on intervention in children with ASD in
two important ways. First, this study is one of few attempts to consider all intervention types and
intervention outcomes as broadly as possible. This allows us to report the state of the science in
regards to which interventions have accrued the most convincing evidence of effectiveness for
young children with ASD, and to report on the full range of outcomes that these interventions are
able to influence. Second, this study accounts for rigorous quality criteria that are common
considerations in other areas of psychology, but that are applied less often to evaluations of
autism research (e.g., Reichow, Volkmar, & Cicchetti, 2008). Finally, several syntheses that are
similar to ours in scope consider some of the design factors of included studies in order to
classify intervention types according to levels of evidence (e.g., Wong et al., 2015). However,
these syntheses have not provided an examination of intervention effects according to
characteristics of the outcome variable, which prevents researchers from drawing conclusions in
regards to whether interventions are able to influence generalized characteristics that extend
beyond the skills directly targeted by the interventions. Our findings echo recent sentiments from
intervention researchers who are heartened by the relative increase in RCTs over the past 15
years, but also raise concerns in regards to the availability of high quality study designs that
reliably and consistently link established interventions with meaningful child outcomes
(Charman, 2019).
Even given these concerns, the evidence base regarding intervention for children with
ASD has been rapidly transforming. The last decade has seen the publication of over 100 group
design studies of intervention, including at least 50 RCTs. These studies attest to the fact that
access to intervention in early childhood can yield a range of positive outcomes for the children
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 58
receiving it. NDBIs have emerged as a new intervention category with significant summary
effects even when several quality indicators are taken into account. High quality studies also
suggest that developmental intervention can improve some core challenges associated with ASD,
particularly difficulties in social communication. Traditional behavioral intervention approaches
show some evidence of effectiveness, but methodological rigor remains a pressing concern for
this body of research. There is little evidence to date, however, to support the effectiveness of
several other interventions that are geared towards young children with autism, including
TEACCH, sensory-based interventions, animal-assisted interventions, and interventions
mediated solely through technology (though approaches that integrate technology, such as
high-tech augmentative and alternative communication devices, into more established
interventions appear promising). More high quality randomized-controlled trials that feature
independently-administered assessments are needed to unquestionably establish the efficacy of
any intervention type. Finally, researchers should consider the characteristics (i.e., distality and
boundedness) of outcomes being tracked in intervention studies and interpret findings
accordingly to permit a more ready assessment of the extent to which any particular treatment
approach is likely to yield desired effects on developmental trajectories of young children
affected by autism.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 59
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 60
References
*Adkins, K. W., Molloy, C., Weiss, S. K., Reynolds, A., Goldman, S. E., Burnette, C., ... &
Malow, B. A. (2012). Effects of a standardized pamphlet on insomnia in children with
autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics
, 130
, S139-S144.
*Aldred, C., Green, J., & Adams, C. (2004). A new social communication intervention for
children with autism: Pilot randomised controlled treatment study suggesting
effectiveness. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
, 45
, 1420-1430.
American Psychological Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders
(5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author.
Autism and insurance coverage; state laws. (2018). Retrieved from
http://www.ncsl.org/research/health/autism-and-insurance-coverage-state-laws.aspx
Ayres, A. J. (1979). Sensory integration and the child
. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological
Services.
Ayres, A. J. (2005). Sensory integration and the child, 25th anniversary edition
. Los Angeles:
Western Psychological Services.
*Azarbehi, A. C. (2009). The effectiveness of early intervention programs for children with
autism: A one-year follow-up study of intense behavioural intervention versus preschool
integration
(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Academic Unit of Psychology of The
University of New Brunswick, Fredericton, New Brunswick.
Bahrick, L. E., & Todd, J. T. (2012). Multisensory processing in autism spectrum disorders:
Intersensory processing disturbance as a basis for atypical development. In B. E. Stein
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 61
and M. T. Wallace (Eds), The new handbook of multisensory processes
(pp. 1453–1508).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
*Bailey, B., Arciuli, J., & Stancliffe, R. J. (2017). Effects of ABRACADABRA literacy
instruction on children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Educational
Psychology
, 109
, 257-268.
Baio J, Wiggins L, Christensen DL, et al. (2018). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among
children aged 8 years — autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11
sites, United States, 2014. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report Surveillance
Summaries, 67
(No. SS-6):1–23.
Barton, E. E., Reichow, B., Schnitz, A., Smith, I. C., & Sherlock, D. (2015). A systematic review
of sensory-based treatments for children with disabilities. Research in Developmental
Disabilities, 37
, 64-80.
Baron-Cohen, S., Golan, O., & Ashwin, E. (2012). Educational cognitive neuroscience:
Designing autism-friendly methods to teach emotion recognition. Neuroscience in
Education: The Good, the Bad and the Ugly, Oxford University Press, Oxford
, 299-311.
*Bass, M. M., Duchowny, C. A., & Llabre, M. M. (2009). The effect of therapeutic horseback
riding on social functioning in children with autism. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 39
, 1261-1267.
*Bieleninik, Ł., Geretsegger, M., Mössler, K., Assmus, J., Thompson, G., Gattino, G., ... &
Suvini, F. (2017). Effects of improvisational music therapy vs enhanced standard care on
symptom severity among children with autism spectrum disorder: The TIME-A
randomized clinical trial. Jama
, 318
, 525-535.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 62
*Boyd, B. A., Hume, K., McBee, M. T., Alessandri, M., Gutierrez, A., Johnson, L., ... & Odom,
S. L. (2014). Comparative efficacy of LEAP, TEACCH and non-model-specific special
education programs for preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders
, 44
, 366-380.
Boyd, B. A., Odom, S. L., Humphreys, B. P., & Sam, A. M. (2010). Infants and toddlers with
autism spectrum disorder: Early identification and early intervention. Journal of Early
Intervention, 32,
75-98.
*Boyd, B. A., Watson, L. R., Reszka, S. S., Sideris, J., Alessandri, M., Baranek, G. T., ... &
Belardi, K. (2018). Efficacy of the ASAP intervention for preschoolers with ASD: A
cluster randomized controlled trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48
,
3144-3162.
*Bremer, E., Balogh, R., & Lloyd, M. (2015). Effectiveness of a fundamental motor skill
intervention for 4-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder: A pilot study. Autism
,
19
, 980-991.
*Brian, J. A., Smith, I. M., Zwaigenbaum, L., & Bryson, S. E. (2017). Cross-site randomized
control trial of the Social ABCs caregiver-mediated intervention for toddlers with autism
spectrum disorder. Autism Research
, 10
, 1700-1711.
Bruner, J. (1982). The organization of action and the nature of the adult-infant transaction. In E.
Tronick (Ed.), Social interchange in infancy: Affect, cognition, and communication.
Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 63
* Carter, A. S., Messinger, D. S., Stone, W. L., Celimli, S., Nahmias, A. S., & Yoder, P. (2011).
A randomized controlled trial of Hanen’s ‘More Than Words’ in toddlers with early
autism
symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
, 52
, 741-752.
Cascio, C. J., Woynaroski, T., Baranek, G. T., & Wallace, M. T. (2016). Toward an
interdisciplinary approach to understanding sensory function in autism spectrum disorder.
Autism Research
, 9
, 920-925.
Case-Smith, J., Weaver, L. L., & Fristad, M. A. (2015). A systematic review of sensory
processing interventions for children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism
, 19
,
133-148.
* Casenhiser, D. M., Binns, A., McGill, F., Morderer, O., & Shanker, S. G. (2015). Measuring
and supporting language function for children with autism: Evidence from a randomized
control trial of a social-interaction-based therapy. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders
, 45
, 846-857.
* Casenhiser, D. M., Shanker, S. G., & Stieben, J. (2013). Learning through interaction in
children with autism: Preliminary data from a social-communication-based intervention.
Autism
, 17
, 220-241.
* Chang, Y. C., Shire, S. Y., Shih, W., Gelfand, C., & Kasari, C. (2016). Preschool deployment
of evidence-based social communication intervention: JASPER in the classroom. Journal
of Autism and Developmental Disorders
, 46
, 2211-2223.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 64
Charman, T. (2019). Editorial: Trials and tribulations in early autism intervention research.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
. Advance online
publication.
* Cheldavi, H., Shakerian, S., Boshehri, S. N. S., & Zarghami, M. (2014). The effects of balance
training intervention on postural control of children with autism spectrum disorder: Role
of sensory information. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
, 8
, 8-14.
*Chiang, C. H., Chu, C. L., & Lee, T. C. (2016). Efficacy of caregiver-mediated joint
engagement intervention for young children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism
, 20
,
172-182.
*Clionsky, L. N. (2012). Child directed interaction training for young children with autism
spectrum disorders: The impact on child language, social skills, adaptive skills, and
problematic behaviors
(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Florida,
Gainesville, FL.
Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and psychological
measurement
, 20
, 37-46.
*Cohen, H., Amerine-Dickens, M., & Smith, T. (2006). Early intensive behavioral treatment:
Replication of the UCLA model in a community setting. Journal of Developmental &
Behavioral Pediatrics, 27
, S145-S155.
Corbett, B. A., Schupp, C. W., Levine, S., & Mendoza, S. (2009). Comparing cortisol, stress,
and sensory sensitivity in children with autism. Autism Research, 2
(1), 39-49.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 65
*Corbett, B. A., Shickman, K., & Ferrer, E. (2008). Brief report: The effects of Tomatis sound
therapy on language in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 38
, 562-566.
*D’Elia, L., Valeri, G., Sonnino, F., Fontana, I., Mammone, A., & Vicari, S. (2014). A
longitudinal study of the TEACCH program in different settings: The potential benefits
of low intensity intervention in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44
, 615-626.
*Dawson, G., Jones, E. J., Merkle, K., Venema, K., Lowy, R., Faja, S., ... & Webb, S. J. (2012).
Early behavioral intervention is associated with normalized brain activity in young
children with autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 51
, 1150-1159.
*Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, J., Greenson, J., ... & Varley, J. (2010).
Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: The Early Start
Denver Model. Pediatrics
, 125
, e17-e23.
Dawson, G., & Zanolli, K. (2003). Early intervention and brain plasticity in autism. In M. Rutter
(Ed.), Autism: Neural bases and treatment possibilities
(pp. 266-280). London: Novartis.
Deeks, J. J., Higgins, J. P., & Altman, D. G. (2008). Analysing data and undertaking
meta-analyses. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions: Cochrane
book series
, 243-296.
*Drew, A., Baird, G., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Slonims, V., Wheelwright, S., ... & Charman,
T. (2002). A pilot randomised control trial of a parent training intervention for pre-school
children with autism. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 11
, 266-272.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 66
*Duifhuis, E. A., Den Boer, J. C., Doornbos, A., Buitelaar, J. K., Oosterling, I. J., & Klip, H.
(2017). The effect of pivotal response treatment in children with autism spectrum
disorders: A non-randomized study with a blinded outcome measure. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 47
, 231-242.
*Durand, V. M., Hieneman, M., Clarke, S., Wang, M., & Rinaldi, M. L. (2013). Positive family
intervention for severe challenging behavior I: A multisite randomized clinical trial.
Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 15
, 133-143.
*Eikeseth, S., Smith, T., Jahr, E., & Eldevik, S. (2007). Outcome for children with autism who
began intensive behavioral treatment between ages 4 and 7: A comparison controlled
study. Behavior Modification, 31
, 264-278.
*Eikeseth, S., Klintwall, L., Jahr, E., & Karlsson, P. (2012). Outcome for children with autism
receiving early and intensive behavioral intervention in mainstream preschool and
kindergarten settings. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
, 6
, 829-835.
* Eikeseth, S., Smith, T., Jahr, E., & Eldevik, S. (2002). Intensive behavioral treatment at school
for 4-to 7-year-old children with autism: A 1-year comparison controlled study. Behavior
Modification
, 26
, 49-68.
*Elder, L. M. (2011). The relationship between parent training and parent-child interaction in
autism
(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Washington, Seattle, WA.
* Eldevik, S., Hastings, R. P., Jahr, E., & Hughes, J. C. (2012). Outcomes of behavioral
intervention for children with autism in mainstream pre-school settings. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42
, 210-220.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 67
*ElGarhy, S., & Liu, T. (2016). Effects of psychomotor intervention program on students with
autism spectrum disorder. School Psychology Quarterly
, 31
, 491-506.
*Espanola, E. (2015). A randomized comparison of two instructional sequences for imitation
intervention for children with autism spectrum disorders
(Unpublished doctoral
dissertation). Florida International University, Miami, FL.
*Estes, A., Munson, J., Rogers, S. J., Greenson, J., Winter, J., & Dawson, G. (2015). Long-term
outcomes of early intervention in 6-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder.
Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54
, 580-587.
*Fava, L., Strauss, K., Valeri, G., D’Elia, L., Arima, S., & Vicari, S. (2011). The effectiveness of
a cross-setting complementary staff-and parent-mediated early intensive behavioral
intervention for young children with ASD. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
, 5
,
1479-1492.
*Fernell, E., Hedvall, Å., Westerlund, J., Carlsson, L. H., Eriksson, M., Olsson, M. B., ... &
Gillberg, C. (2011). Early intervention in 208 Swedish preschoolers with autism spectrum
disorder. A prospective naturalistic study. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32
,
2092-2101.
Ferster, C. B., & DeMeyer, M. K. (1962). A method for the experimental analysis of the
behavior of autistic children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 32
, 89–98.
Fisher, Z., Tipton, E., & Zhipeng, H. (2017). robumeta: Robust variance meta-regression
(Version 2.0). Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=robumeta
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 68
*Flanagan, H. E., Perry, A., & Freeman, N. L. (2012). Effectiveness of large-scale
community-based Intensive Behavioral Intervention: A waitlist comparison study
exploring outcomes and predictors. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6
, 673-682.
Fletcher-Watson, S. (2018, June). Is early intervention compatible with neurodiversity? [Blog
post]. Retrieved from https://dart.ed.ac.uk/intervention-neurodiversity/
*Fletcher-Watson, S., Petrou, A., Scott-Barrett, J., Dicks, P., Graham, C., O’Hare, A., ... &
McConachie, H. (2016). A trial of an iPad™ intervention targeting social communication
skills in children with autism. Autism, 20
, 771-782.
*Fridenson-Hayo, S., Berggren, S., Lassalle, A., Tal, S., Pigat, D., Meir-Goren, N., ... & Golan,
O. (2017). ‘Emotiplay’: A serious game for learning about emotions in children with
autism: Results of a cross-cultural evaluation. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
26
, 979-992.
French, L., & Kennedy, E. M. M. (2017). Annual research review: Early intervention for infants
and young children with, or at-risk of, autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review.
Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines, 59,
444-456.
Fuller, E. A., & Kaiser, A. P. (2019). The effects of early intervention on social communication
outcomes for children with autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders
. Advance Online Publication.
Gabriels, R. L., Agnew, J. A., Holt, K. D., Shoffner, A., Zhaoxing, P., Ruzzano, S., ... &
Mesibov, G. (2012). Pilot study measuring the effects of therapeutic horseback riding on
school-age children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism
Spectrum Disorders, 6
, 578-588.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 69
Gamer, M., Lemon, J., Fellows, I., & Singh, P. (2012). irr: Various coefficients of interrater
reliability and agreement. R package v. 0.84.
Gernsbacher, M. A. (2017). Editorial perspective: The use of person-first language in scholarly
writing may accentuate stigma. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
, 58
,
859-861.
*Golan, O., Ashwin, E., Granader, Y., McClintock, S., Day, K., Leggett, V., & Baron-Cohen, S.
(2010). Enhancing emotion recognition in children with autism spectrum conditions: An
intervention using animated vehicles with real emotional faces. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 40
, 269-279.
*Grahame, V., Brett, D., Dixon, L., McConachie, H., Lowry, J., Rodgers, J., ... & Le Couteur, A.
(2015). Managing repetitive behaviours in young children with Autism Spectrum
Disorder (ASD): Pilot randomised controlled trial of a new parent group intervention.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45
, 3168-3182.
*Green, J., Charman, T., McConachie, H., Aldred, C., Slonims, V., Howlin, P., ... & Barrett, B.
(2010). Parent-mediated communication-focused treatment in children with autism
(PACT): A randomised controlled trial. The Lancet, 375
, 2152-2160.
Green, V. A., Pituch, K. A., Itchon, J., Choi, A., O’Reilly, M., & Sigafoos, J. (2006). Internet
survey of treatments used by parents of children with autism. Research in Developmental
Disabilities,
27
, 70–84.
Greenspan, S. I. & Wieder, S. (2007). The developmental individual-difference,
relationship-based (DIR/Floortime) model approach to autism spectrum disorders. In E.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 70
Hollander & E. Anagnostou (Eds.) Clinical manual for the treatment of autism
(pp.
179-209). Arlington, VA, US: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.
Grelotti, D. J., & Kaptchuk, T. J. (2011). Placebo by proxy. BMJ (Clinical research ed.), 343,
d4345.
Grynszpan, O., Weiss, P. L., Perez-Diaz, F., & Gal, E. (2014). Innovative technology-based
interventions for autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis. Autism
, 18
, 346-361.
*Gulsrud, A. C., Kasari, C., Freeman, S., & Paparella, T. (2007). Children with autism's response
to novel stimuli while participating in interventions targeting joint attention or symbolic
play skills. Autism, 11
, 535-546.
*Gulsrud, A. C., Hellemann, G. S., Freeman, S. F., & Kasari, C. (2014). Two to ten years:
Developmental trajectories of joint attention in children with ASD who received targeted
social communication interventions. Autism Research, 7
, 207-215.
*Hardan, A. Y., Gengoux, G. W., Berquist, K. L., Libove, R. A., Ardel, C. M., Phillips, J., ... &
Minjarez, M. B. (2015). A randomized controlled trial of Pivotal Response Treatment
Group for parents of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
56
(8), 884-892.
Harrell, F. E. Hmisc: Harrell Miscellaneous (Version 4.1-1). Retrieved from
https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=Hmisc
Hedges, L. V., Tipton, E., & Johnson, M. C. (2010). Robust variance estimation in
meta-regression with dependent effect size estimates. Research Synthesis Methods, 1
,
39-65.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 71
*Hemdi, A., & Daley, D. (2017). The Effectiveness of a Psychoeducation Intervention delivered
via WhatsApp for mothers of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) in the
Kingdom of Saudi Arabia: A randomized controlled trial. Child: Care, Health and
Development, 43
, 933-941.
Higgins, J. P., Altman, D. G., Gøtzsche, P. C., Jüni, P., Moher, D., Oxman, A. D., ... & Sterne, J.
A. (2011). The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised
trials. British Medical Journal
, 343
, d5928.
Houting, J. den. (2019). Neurodiversity: An insider’s perspective. Autism, 23
(2), 271–273.
*Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (1989). Mothers' speech to autistic children: A preliminary causal
analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30
, 819-843.
Howling, P., & Magiati, I. (2017). Autism spectrum disorder: Outcomes in adulthood. Current
Opinion in Psychiatry, 30(2)
, 69-76.
Hróbjartsson, A., Thomsen, A. S. S., Emanuelsson, F., Tendal, B., Hilden, J., Boutron, I., ... &
Brorson, S. (2012). Observer bias in randomised clinical trials with binary outcomes:
Systematic review of trials with both blinded and non-blinded outcome assessors. BMJ,
344
, e1119.
*Hudson, R. F., Sanders, E. A., Greenway, R., Xie, S., Smith, M., Gasamis, C., ... & Hackett, J.
(2017). Effects of emergent literacy interventions for preschoolers with autism spectrum
disorder. Exceptional Children, 84
, 55-75.
*Iadarola, S., Shih, W., Dean, M., Blanch, E., Harwood, R., Hetherington, S., ... & Smith, T.
(2018). Implementing a manualized, classroom transition intervention for students with
ASD in underresourced schools. Behavior Modification, 42
, 126-147.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 72
*Ichikawa, K., Takahashi, Y., Ando, M., Anme, T., Ishizaki, T., Yamaguchi, H., & Nakayama,
T. (2013). TEACCH-based group social skills training for children with high-functioning
autism: A pilot randomized controlled trial. BioPsychoSocial Medicine, 7
, 14.
*Ingersoll, B. (2010). Brief report: Pilot randomized controlled trial of reciprocal imitation
training for teaching elicited and spontaneous imitation to children with autism. Journal
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40
, 1154-1160.
*Ingersoll, B. (2012). Brief report: Effect of a focused imitation intervention on social
functioning in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42
,
1768-1773.
*Ingersoll, B., Wainer, A. L., Berger, N. I., Pickard, K. E., & Bonter, N. (2016). Comparison of a
self-directed and therapist-assisted telehealth parent-mediated intervention for children
with ASD: A pilot RCT. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46
,
2275-2284.
*Jalili, M., Jahangiri, N., Yazdi, A. A., & Ashrafzadeh, F. (2014). The effects of imitative vs.
cognitive methods on the speech development of children with autism. Iranian Journal of
Child Neurology, 8
, 37-46.
*Jocelyn, L. J., Casiro, O. G., Beattie, D., Bow, J., & Kneisz, J. (1998). Treatment of children
with autism: A randomized controlled trial to evaluate a caregiver-based intervention
program in community day-care centers. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral
Pediatrics
, 19
, 326-334.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 73
*Johnson, C. R., Turner, K. S., Foldes, E., Brooks, M. M., Kronk, R., & Wiggs, L. (2013).
Behavioral parent training to address sleep disturbances in young children with autism
spectrum disorder: A pilot trial. Sleep Medicine, 14
, 995-1004.
Jones, R. M., Carberry, C., Hamo, A., & Lord, C. (2017). Placebo-like response in absence of
treatment in children with Autism. Autism Research, 10
, 1567-1572.
*Jouen, A. L., Narzisi, A., Xavier, J., Tilmont, E., Bodeau, N., Bono, V., ... & Muratori, F.
(2017). GOLIAH (Gaming Open Library for Intervention in Autism at Home): A
6-month single blind matched controlled exploratory study. Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry and Mental Health, 11
, 17.
*Kaale, A., Smith, L., & Sponheim, E. (2012). A randomized controlled trial of preschool-based
joint attention intervention for children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 53
, 97-105.
*Kaale, A., Fagerland, M. W., Martinsen, E. W., & Smith, L. (2014). Preschool-based social
communication treatment for children with autism: 12-month follow-up of a randomized
trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53
, 188-198.
Kaiser, A. P. (1993). Parent-implemented language intervention: An environmental system
perspective. In A. P. Kaiser & D. B. Gray (Eds.), Enhancing children’s communication:
Research foundations for intervention
(Vol. 2, pp. 63-84). Baltimore: Brookes.
Kaiser, A. P., & Roberts, M. Y. (2012). Parent-implemented enhanced milieu teaching with
preschool children who have intellectual disabilities. Journal of speech, language, and
hearing research : JSLHR
, 56
, 295–309.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 74
*Kalyva, E., & Avramidis, E. (2005). Improving communication between children with autism
and their peers through the ‘Circle of Friends’: A small-scale intervention study. Journal
of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 18
, 253-261.
*Kamps, D., Heitzman-Powell, L., Rosenberg, N., Mason, R., Schwartz, I., & Romine, R. S.
(2016). Effects of Reading Mastery as a small group intervention for young children with
ASD. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 28
, 703-722.
Kasari, C. (2002). Assessing change in early intervention programs for children with autism.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
, 32
, 447-461.
*Kasari, C., Freeman, S., & Paparella, T. (2006). Joint attention and symbolic play in young
children with autism: A randomized controlled intervention study. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 47
, 611-620.
*Kasari, C., Gulsrud, A. C., Wong, C., Kwon, S., & Locke, J. (2010). Randomized controlled
caregiver mediated joint engagement intervention for toddlers with autism. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40
, 1045-1056.
*Kasari, C., Gulsrud, A., Paparella, T., Hellemann, G., & Berry, K. (2015). Randomized
comparative efficacy study of parent-mediated interventions for toddlers with autism.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 83
, 554-563.
Kasari, C., Kaiser, A., Goods, K., Nietfeld, J., Mathy, P., Landa, R., ... & Almirall, D. (2014).
Communication interventions for minimally verbal children with autism: A sequential
multiple assignment randomized trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Adolescent Psychiatry
, 53
, 635-646.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 75
*Kasari, C., Paparella, T., Freeman, S., & Jahromi, L. B. (2008). Language outcome in autism:
Randomized comparison of joint attention and play interventions. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology
, 76
, 125-137.
Kasari, C., & Smith, T. (2013). Interventions in schools for children with autism spectrum
disorder: Methods and recommendations. Autism, 17
, 254-267.
*Keen, D., Couzens, D., Muspratt, S., & Rodger, S. (2010). The effects of a parent-focused
intervention for children with a recent diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder on parenting
stress and competence. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4
, 229-241.
Kenny, L., Hattersley, C., Molins, B., Buckley, C., Povey, C., & Pellicano, E. (2016). Which
terms should be used to describe autism? Perspectives from the UK autism community.
Autism
, 20
, 442-462.
Knight, V., McKissick, B. R., & Saunders, A. (2013). A review of technology-based
interventions to teach academic skills to students with autism spectrum disorder. Journal
of autism and developmental disorders
, 43
, 2628-2648.
Koegel, R. L., Koegel, L. K., & Carter, C. M. (1999). Pivotal teaching interactions for children
with autism. School Psychology Review
, 28, 576–594.
Kolb B, & Gibb R. (2011). Brain plasticity and behaviour in the developing brain. Journal of the
Canadian Academy of Child & Adolescent
, 20, 265–276.
*Kovshoff, H., Hastings, R. P., & Remington, B. (2011). Two-year outcomes for children with
autism after the cessation of early intensive behavioral intervention. Behavior
Modification, 35
, 427-450.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 76
Kratochwill, T. R., Hitchcock, J. H., Horner, R. H., Levin, J. R., Odom, S. L., Rindskopf, D. M.,
& Shadish, W. R. (2013). Single-case intervention research design standards. Remedial
and Special Education
, 34
(1), 26-38.
*LaGasse, A. B. (2014). Effects of a music therapy group intervention on enhancing social skills
in children with autism. Journal of Music Therapy, 51
, 250-275.
*Landa, R. J., Holman, K. C., O’Neill, A. H., & Stuart, E. A. (2011). Intervention targeting
development of socially synchronous engagement in toddlers with autism spectrum
disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52
,
13-21.
*Lawton, K., & Kasari, C. (2012). Teacher-implemented joint attention intervention: Pilot
randomized controlled study for preschoolers with autism. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 80
, 687-693.
*Lawton, K., & Kasari, C. (2012). Brief report: Longitudinal improvements in the quality of
joint attention in preschool children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 42
, 307-312.
*Leaf, J. B., Leaf, J. A., Milne, C., Taubman, M., Oppenheim-Leaf, M., Torres, N., ... & Yoder,
P. (2017). An evaluation of a behaviorally based social skills group for individuals
diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 47
, 243-259.
*Lerna, A., Esposito, D., Conson, M., Russo, L., & Massagli, A. (2012). Social–communicative
effects of the Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS) in autism spectrum
disorders. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 47
, 609-617.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 77
*Lim, H. A. (2010). The effect of “Developmental speech and language training through music”
on speech production in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Music
Therapy, 47
, 2-26.
Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Applied social research methods series; Vol. 49.
Practical meta-analysis.
Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Sage Publications, Inc.
Lord, C., Bristol-Power M., Cafiero, J. M., Filipek, P. A., Gallagher, J. J., Harris, S. L., et al.
(2001). Educating Children with autism. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. L. (2012). Autism
Diagnostic Observation Schedule, second edition (ADOS-2) manual (Part I): Modules 1-
4.
Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning in
young autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55
, 3-9.
*Magiati, I., Charman, T., & Howlin, P. (2007). A two-year prospective follow-up study of
community-based early intensive behavioural intervention and specialist nursery
provision for children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 48
, 803-812.
*Mahoney, G., & Solomon, R. (2016). Mechanism of developmental change in the PLAY
project home consultation program: Evidence from a randomized control trial. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46
, 1860-1871.
*Malow, B. A., Adkins, K. W., Reynolds, A., Weiss, S. K., Loh, A., Fawkes, D., ... & Clemons,
T. (2014). Parent-based sleep education for children with autism spectrum disorders.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44
, 216-228.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 78
*Mandell, D. S., Stahmer, A. C., Shin, S., Xie, M., Reisinger, E., & Marcus, S. C. (2013). The
role of treatment fidelity on outcomes during a randomized field trial of an autism
intervention. Autism, 17
, 281-295.
*McCalla, M. K. (2014). Enhancing the social interactions of preschool children with autism:
The effectiveness of a peer-mediated
(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). The University
of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL.
*McEachin, J. J., Smith, T., & Ivar Lovaas, O. (1993). Long-term outcome for children with
autism who received early intensive behavioral treatment. American Journal of Mental
Retardation, 97
, 359-372.
Mesibov, G. B., Shea, V., & Schopler, E. (2004). The TEACCH approach to autism spectrum
disorders.
New York, NY: Springer Science & Business Media.
Murad, M. H., Asi, N., Alsawas, M., & Alahdab, F. (2016). New evidence pyramid. BMJ
Evidence-Based Medicine, 21,
125-127.
*Murdock, L. C., Dantzler, J. A., Walker, A. N., & Wood, L. B. (2014). The effect of a platform
swing on the independent work behaviors of children with autism spectrum disorders.
Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities
, 29
, 50-61.
National Autism Center (2015). Findings and conclusions: National standards project, phase 2.
Randolph, MA.
*Nefdt, N., Koegel, R., Singer, G., & Gerber, M. (2010). The use of a self-directed learning
program to provide introductory training in pivotal response treatment to parents of
children with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 12
, 23-32.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 79
Nuijten, M. B. (2018). Practical tools and strategies for researchers to increase replicability.
Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology.
Advance online publication.
Odom, S.L., Boyd, B.A., Hall, L.J., Hume K. (2010). Evaluation of comprehensive treatment
models for individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord.
, 40, 437.
Oliver, P.C., Piachaud, J., Done, J., Regan, A., Cooray, S., Tyrer, P. (2002). Difficulties in
conducting a randomized controlled trial of health service interventions in intellectual
disability: Implications for evidence-based practice. Journal of Intellectual Disability
Research, 46,
340-345.
*Ozonoff, S., & Cathcart, K. (1998). Effectiveness of a home program intervention for young
children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28
, 25-32.
*Page, C. E. (2012). The social and emotional benefits of therapeutic riding on children with
autism spectrum disorder
(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Institute of Transpersonal
Psychology, Palo Alto, CA.
*Pajareya, K., & Nopmaneejumruslers, K. (2011). A pilot randomized controlled trial of
DIR/Floortime™ parent training intervention for pre-school children with autistic
spectrum disorders. Autism, 15
, 563-577.
*Pan, C. Y. (2010). Effects of water exercise swimming program on aquatic skills and social
behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 14
, 9-28.
*Paul, R., Campbell, D., Gilbert, K., & Tsiouri, I. (2013). Comparing spoken language
treatments for minimally verbal preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43
, 418-431.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 80
*Paynter, J., & Peterson, C. C. (2013). Further evidence of benefits of thought-bubble training
for theory of mind development in children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7
, 344-348.
*Peters-Scheffer, N., Didden, R., Mulders, M., & Korzilius, H. (2013). Effectiveness of low
intensity behavioral treatment for children with autism spectrum disorder and intellectual
disability. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7
, 1012-1025.
*Pickles, A., Le Couteur, A., Leadbitter, K., Salomone, E., Cole-Fletcher, R., Tobin, H., ... &
Green, J. (2016). Parent-mediated social communication therapy for young children with
autism (PACT): Long-term follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. The Lancet, 388
,
2501-2509.
*Piravej, K., Tangtrongchitr, P., Chandarasiri, P., Paothong, L., & Sukprasong, S. (2009). Effects
of Thai traditional massage on autistic children's behavior. The Journal of Alternative and
Complementary Medicine, 15
, 1355-1361.
Provenzani, U., Fusar-Poli, L., Brondino, N., Damiani, S., Vercesi, M., Meyer, N., Rochetti, M.,
& Politi, P. (2019). What are we targeting when we treat autism spectrum disorder? A
systematic review of 406 clinical trials. Autism,
1362361319854641.
*Poslawsky, I. E., Naber, F. B., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van Daalen, E., van Engeland,
H., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2015). Video-feedback Intervention to promote Positive
Parenting adapted to autism (VIPP-AUTI): A randomized controlled trial. Autism
, 19
,
588-603.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 81
*Powell, G., Wass, S. V., Erichsen, J. T., & Leekam, S. R. (2016). First evidence of the
feasibility of gaze-contingent attention training for school children with autism. Autism
,
20
, 927-937.
R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing (Version 3.4.1).
R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from
https://www.R-project.org/
Reichow, B., Volkmar, F. R., & Cicchetti, D. V. (2008). Development of the evaluative method
for evaluating and determining evidence-based practices in autism. Journal of autism and
developmental disorders, 38(7), 1311-1319.
*Reitzel, J., Summers, J., Lorv, B., Szatmari, P., Zwaigenbaum, L., Georgiades, S., & Duku, E.
(2013). Pilot randomized controlled trial of a Functional Behavior Skills Training
program for young children with autism spectrum disorder who have significant early
learning skill impairments and their families. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7
,
1418-1432.
*Remington, B., Hastings, R. P., Kovshoff, H., Degli Espinosa, F., Jahr, E., Brown, T., ... &
Ward, N. (2007). Early intensive behavioral intervention: Outcomes for children with
autism and their parents after two years. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 112
,
418-438.
Renty, J. O., & Roeyers, H. (2006). Quality of life in high-functioning adults with autism
spectrum disorder: The predictive value of disability and support characteristics. Autism,
10
(5), 511-524.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 82
*Rentz, E. A. (2015). Autism spectrum disorders: Developmental, individual-difference,
relationship-based (DIR)/Floortime outcomes
(Unpublished doctoral dissertation).
Fordham University, Bronx, NY.
*Rice, L. M., Wall, C. A., Fogel, A., & Shic, F. (2015). Computer-assisted face processing
instruction improves emotion recognition, mentalizing, and social skills in students with
ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45
, 2176-2186.
*Rickards, A. L., Walstab, J. E., Wright-Rossi, R. A., Simpson, J., & Reddihough, D. S. (2007).
A randomized, controlled trial of a home-based intervention program for children with
autism and developmental delay. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 28
,
308-316.
*Roberts, J., Williams, K., Carter, M., Evans, D., Parmenter, T., Silove, N., ... & Warren, A.
(2011). A randomised controlled trial of two early intervention programs for young
children with autism: Centre-based with parent program and home-based. Research in
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5
, 1553-1566.
Robison, J. E. (2019). Talking about autism—Thoughts for researchers. Autism Research
, 12
,
1004–1006.
Rogers, S. J., & Dawson, G. (2010). Early Start Denver Model for young children with autism:
Promoting language, learning, and engagement
. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
*Rogers, S. J., Hayden, D., Hepburn, S., Charlifue-Smith, R., Hall, T., & Hayes, A. (2006).
Teaching young nonverbal children with autism useful speech: A pilot study of the
Denver Model and PROMPT interventions. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 36
, 1007-1024.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 83
*Rogers, S. J., Estes, A., Lord, C., Vismara, L., Winter, J., Fitzpatrick, A., ... & Dawson, G.
(2012). Effects of a brief Early Start Denver Model (ESDM)–based parent intervention
on toddlers at risk for autism spectrum disorders: A randomized controlled trial. Journal
of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51
, 1052-1065.
*Ruble, L. A., Dalrymple, N. J., & McGrew, J. H. (2010). The effects of consultation on
individualized education program outcomes for young children with autism: The
collaborative model for promoting competence and success. Journal of Early
Intervention, 32
, 286-301.
Rutter, M., LeCouteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised
. Los
Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
*Sainato, D. M., Morrison, R. S., Jung, S., Axe, J., & Nixon, P. A. (2015). A comprehensive
inclusion program for kindergarten children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of
Early Intervention, 37
, 208-225.
*Sallows, G. O., & Graupner, T. D. (2005). Intensive behavioral treatment for children with
autism: Four-year outcome and predictors. American Journal on Mental Retardation,
110
, 417-438.
*Salt, J., Shemilt, J., Sellars, V., Boyd, S., Coulson, T., & McCool, S. (2002). The Scottish
Centre for Autism preschool treatment programme. II: The results of a controlled
treatment outcome study. Autism, 6
, 33-46.
*Sampanthavivat, M., Singkhwa, W., Chaiyakul, T., Karoonyawanich, S., & Ajpru, H. (2012).
Hyperbaric oxygen in the treatment of childhood autism: A randomised controlled trial.
Diving and Hyperbaric Medicine, 42
, 128-33.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 84
*Scarpa, A., & Reyes, N. M. (2011). Improving emotion regulation with CBT in young children
with high functioning autism spectrum disorders: A pilot study. Behavioural and
Cognitive Psychotherapy, 39
, 495-500.
Schaaf, R. C., Benevides, T., Mailloux, Z., Faller, P., Hunt, J., Van Hooydonk, E., ... & Kelly, D.
(2014). An intervention for sensory difficulties in children with autism: A randomized
trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
, 44
(7), 1493-1506.
*Schertz, H. H., Odom, S. L., Baggett, K. M., & Sideris, J. H. (2013). Effects of joint attention
mediated learning for toddlers with autism spectrum disorders: An initial randomized
controlled study. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28
, 249-258.
*Schertz, H. H., Odom, S. L., Baggett, K. M., & Sideris, J. H. (2018). Mediating parent learning
to promote social communication for toddlers with autism: Effects from a randomized
controlled trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48
, 853-867.
Schopler E., Reichler R., Rochen Renner B. The childhood autism rating scale. Western
Psychological Services; 1988.
*Schreibman, L., & Stahmer, A. C. (2014). A randomized trial comparison of the effects of
verbal and pictorial naturalistic communication strategies on spoken language for young
children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44
, 1244-1251.
Schreibman, L., Dawson, G., Stahmer, A. C., Landa, R., Rogers, S. J., McGee, G. G., …
Halladay, A. (2015). Naturalistic Developmental Behavioral Interventions: Empirically
Validated Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders
, 45
, 2411–2428.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 85
*Sheinkopf, S. J., & Siegel, B. (1998). Home-based behavioral treatment of young children with
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28
, 15-23.
*Shire, S. Y., Gulsrud, A., & Kasari, C. (2016). Increasing responsive parent–child interactions
and joint engagement: Comparing the influence of parent-mediated intervention and
parent psychoeducation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46
,
1737-1747.
*Shire, S. Y., Chang, Y. C., Shih, W., Bracaglia, S., Kodjoe, M., & Kasari, C. (2017). Hybrid
implementation model of community-partnered early intervention for toddlers with
autism: A randomized trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58
, 612-622.
Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability.
Psychological bulletin
, 86
, 420.
*Silva, L., & Schalock, M. (2013). Treatment of tactile impairment in young children with
autism: Results with qigong massage. International Journal of Therapeutic Massage &
Bodywork, 6
, 12-20.
*Silva, L. M., Schalock, M., Gabrielsen, K. R., Budden, S. S., Buenrostro, M., & Horton, G.
(2015). Early intervention with a parent-delivered massage protocol directed at tactile
abnormalities decreases severity of autism and improves child-to-parent interactions: A
replication study. Autism Research and Treatment, 2015
.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 86
*Simpson, K., Keen, D., & Lamb, J. (2015). Teaching receptive labelling to children with autism
spectrum disorder: A comparative study using infant-directed song and infant-directed
speech. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 40
, 126-136.
*Simpson, K., Keen, D., & Lamb, J. (2013). The use of music to engage children with autism in
a receptive labelling task. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7
, 1489-1496.
* Smith, T., Eikeseth, S., Klevstrand, M., & Lovaas, O. I. (1997). Intensive behavioral treatment
for preschoolers with severe mental retardation and pervasive developmental disorder.
American Journal on Mental Retardation, 102
, 238-249.
*Smith, T., Groen, A. D., & Wynn, J. W. (2000). Randomized trial of intensive early
intervention for children with pervasive developmental disorder. American Journal on
Mental Retardation, 105
, 269-285.
*Sokmun, S., Singh, S. J., & Vandort, S. (2018). The impact of Hanen More Than Words
programme on parents of children with ASD in Malaysia. Malaysian Journal of Health
Sciences, 15
, 43-51.
*Solomon, R., Van Egeren, L. A., Mahoney, G., Huber, M. S. Q., & Zimmerman, P. (2014).
PLAY Project Home Consultation intervention program for young children with autism
spectrum disorders: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Developmental and
Behavioral Pediatrics, 35
, 475-485.
*Spjut Jansson, B., Miniscalco, C., Westerlund, J., Kantzer, A. K., Fernell, E., & Gillberg, C.
(2016). Children who screen positive for autism at 2.5 years and receive early
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 87
intervention: A prospective naturalistic 2-year outcome study. Neuropsychiatric Disease
and Treatment, 12
, 2255-2263.
*Srinivasan, S. M., Eigsti, I. M., Gifford, T., & Bhat, A. N. (2016). The effects of embodied
rhythm and robotic interventions on the spontaneous and responsive verbal
communication skills of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD): A further
outcome of a pilot randomized controlled trial. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,
27
, 73-87.
*Srinivasan, S. M., Park, I. K., Neelly, L. B., & Bhat, A. N. (2015). A comparison of the effects
of rhythm and robotic interventions on repetitive behaviors and affective states of
children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Research in Autism Spectrum
Disorders, 18
, 51-63.
*Srinivasan, S. M., Kaur, M., Park, I. K., Gifford, T. D., Marsh, K. L., & Bhat, A. N. (2015). The
effects of rhythm and robotic interventions on the imitation/praxis, interpersonal
synchrony, and motor performance of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD): A
pilot randomized controlled trial. Autism Research and Treatment, 2015
.
*Stadnick, N. A., Stahmer, A., & Brookman-Frazee, L. (2015). Preliminary effectiveness of
Project ImPACT: A parent-mediated intervention for children with autism spectrum
disorder delivered in a community program. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 45
, 2092-2104.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 88
Stahmer, A. C., Collings, N. M., & Palinkas, L. A. (2005). Early intervention practices for
children with autism: Descriptions from community providers. Focus on Autism and
Other Developmental Disabilities,
20
, 66–79.
*Strain, P. S., & Bovey, E. H. (2011). Randomized, controlled trial of the LEAP model of early
intervention for young children with autism spectrum disorders. Topics in Early
Childhood Special Education, 31
, 133-154.
*Strauss, K., Vicari, S., Valeri, G., D’Elia, L., Arima, S., & Fava, L. (2012). Parent inclusion in
early intensive behavioral intervention: The influence of parental stress, parent treatment
fidelity and parent-mediated generalization of behavior targets on child outcomes.
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33
, 688-703.
*Sullivan, K. A. (2013). The Early Start Denver Model: Outcomes and moderators of an
intervention for toddlers with Autism
(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of
Washington, Seattle, WA.
*Suzuki, M., Yamada, A., Watanabe, N., Akechi, T., Katsuki, F., Nishiyama, T., ... & Ota, A.
(2014). A failure to confirm the effectiveness of a brief group psychoeducational program
for mothers of children with high-functioning pervasive developmental disorders: A
randomized controlled pilot trial. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 10
,
1141-1153.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 89
Tanner-Smith, E. E., Tipton, E., Polanin, J. R. (2016). Handling complex meta-analytic data
structures using robust variance estimates: A tutorial in R. Journal of Developmental and
Life-Course Criminology 2,
85-112.
*Tellegen, C. L., & Sanders, M. R. (2014). A randomized controlled trial evaluating a brief
parenting program with children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 82
, 1193-1200.
*Thompson, G. A., McFerran, K. S., & Gold, C. (2014). Family-centred music therapy to
promote social engagement in young children with severe autism spectrum disorder: A
randomized controlled study. Child: Care, Health and Development, 40
, 840-852.
*Tonge, B., Brereton, A., Kiomall, M., Mackinnon, A., & Rinehart, N. J. (2014). A randomised
group comparison controlled trial of ‘preschoolers with autism’: A parent education and
skills training intervention for young children with autistic disorder. Autism, 18
, 166-177.
*Tsang, S. K., Shek, D. T., Lam, L. L., Tang, F. L., & Cheung, P. M. (2007). Brief report:
Application of the TEACCH Program on Chinese pre-school children with autism––Does
culture make a difference?. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37
,
390-396.
*Venker, C. E., McDuffie, A., Ellis Weismer, S., & Abbeduto, L. (2012). Increasing verbal
responsiveness in parents of children with autism: A pilot study. Autism, 16
, 568-585.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 90
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of
Statistical Software, 36
, 1-48. doi:10.18637/jss.v036.i03
*Vivanti, G., Paynter, J., Duncan, E., Fothergill, H., Dissanayake, C., Rogers, S. J., & Victorian
ASELCC Team. (2014). Effectiveness and feasibility of the Early Start Denver Model
implemented in a group-based community childcare setting. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 44
, 3140-3153.
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wallace, B., Small, K., Brodley, C.E., Lau, J., & Trikalinos, T.A. (2012). Deploying an
interactive machine learning system in an evidence-based practice center: abstrackr. In
Proceedings of the 2nd ACM SIGHIT International Health Informatics Symposium
,
819-824.
Warren, Z., McPheeters, M. L., Sathe, N., Foss-Feig, J. H., Glasser, A., &
Veenstra-Vanderweele, J. (2011). A systematic review of early intensive intervention for
autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 127
, e1303-1311.
*Weiner, R. H., & Greene, R. L. (2014). Intention-based therapy for autism spectrum disorder:
Promising results of a wait-list control study in children. EXPLORE
: The Journal of
Science and Healing, 10
, 13-23.
*Welsh, K. (2009). The use of dogs to impact joint attention in children with Autism Spectrum
Disorders
(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Walden University, Minneapolis, MN.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 91
*Welterlin, A., Turner-Brown, L. M., Harris, S., Mesibov, G., & Delmolino, L. (2012). The
home TEACCHing program for toddlers with autism. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 42
, 1827-1835.
*Wetherby, A. M., & Woods, J. J. (2006). Early social interaction project for children with
autism spectrum disorders beginning in the second year of life: A preliminary study.
Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 26
, 67-82.
*Whitehouse, A. J., Granich, J., Alvares, G., Busacca, M., Cooper, M. N., Dass, A., ... &
Anderson, A. (2017). A randomised controlled trial of an iPad-based application to
complement early behavioural intervention in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 58
, 1042-1052.
*Whittingham, K., Sofronoff, K., Sheffield, J., & Sanders, M. R. (2009). Stepping Stones Triple
P: An RCT of a parenting program with parents of a child diagnosed with an autism
spectrum disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37
, 469.
*Wild, D. L. (2012). The impact of canine assistance for children with autism and the family unit
(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Walden University, Minneapolis, MN.
*Williams, B. T., Gray, K. M., & Tonge, B. J. (2012). Teaching emotion recognition skills to
young children with autism: A randomised controlled trial of an emotion training
programme. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53
, 1268-1276.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 92
Williams, K., Wray, J. A., & Wheeler, D. M. (2012). Intravenous secretin for autism spectrum
disorders (ASD). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews,
4.
Wolery, M., Busick, M., Reichow, B., & Barton, E. E. (2010). Comparison of overlap methods
for quantitatively synthesizing single-subject data. The Journal of Special Education, 44,
18-28.
*Wong, V. C., & Kwan, Q. K. (2010). Randomized controlled trial for early intervention for
autism: A pilot study of the Autism 1-2-3 Project. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 40
, 677-688.
Wong, C., Odom, S. L., Hume, K. A., Cox, A. W., Fettig, A., Kucharczyk, S., ... & Schultz, T. R.
(2015). Evidence-based practices for children, youth, and young adults with autism
spectrum disorder: A comprehensive review. Journal of autism and developmental
disorders
, 45, 1951-1966.
*Woo, C. C., Donnelly, J. H., Steinberg-Epstein, R., & Leon, M. (2015). Environmental
enrichment as a therapy for autism: A clinical trial replication and extension. Behavioral
Neuroscience, 129
, 412-422.
*Woo, C. C., & Leon, M. (2013). Environmental enrichment as an effective treatment for
autism: A randomized controlled trial. Behavioral Neuroscience, 127
, 487-497.
Yoder, P. J., Bottema-Beutel, K., Woynaroski, T., Chandrasekhar, R., & Sandbank, M. (2013).
Social communication intervention effects vary by dependent variable type in
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 93
preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. Evidence-based communication assessment
and intervention
, 7
, 150-174.
*Young, R. L., & Posselt, M. (2012). Using The Transporters DVD as a learning tool for
children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 42
, 984-991.
*Yun, S. S., Choi, J., Park, S. K., Bong, G. Y., & Yoo, H. (2017). Social skills training for
children with autism spectrum disorder using a robotic behavioral intervention system.
Autism Research, 10
, 1306-1323.
*Zachor, D. A., Vardi, S., Baron-Eitan, S., Brodai-Meir, I., Ginossar, N., & Ben-Itzchak, E.
(2017). The effectiveness of an outdoor adventure programme for young children with
autism spectrum disorder: A controlled study. Developmental Medicine & Child
Neurology, 59
, 550-556.
Zimmerman, K. N., Pustejovsky, J. E., Ledford, J. R., Barton, E. E., Severini, K. E., & Lloyd, B.
P. (2018). Single-case synthesis tools II: Comparing quantitative outcomes measures.
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 79,
65-76.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 94
Figure 1.
PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) flow
diagram.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 95
Figure 2.
Decision tree used to code whether a study outcome was proximal or distal to
treatment targets adapted from “Social communication intervention effects vary by dependent
variable type in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders,” by P. Yoder, K. Bottema-Beutel,
T. Woynaroski, R. Chandrasekhar, and M. Sandbank, 2013, Evidence-based Communication
Assessment and Intervention, 170. Copyright 2013 by Taylor and Francis.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 96
Figure 3.
Decision tree used to code whether a study outcome measured a potentially
context-bound or more highly generalized characteristic adapted from “Social communication
intervention effects vary by dependent variable type in preschoolers with autism spectrum
disorders,” by P. Yoder, K. Bottema-Beutel, T. Woynaroski, R. Chandrasekhar, and M.
Sandbank, 2013, Evidence-based Communication Assessment and Intervention, 171. Copyright
2013 by Taylor and Francis.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 97
Figure 4.
Summary of quality indicator ratings for studies of behavioral, developmental, and
naturalistic developmental behavioral intervention (NDBI) types.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 98
Figure 5.
Summary of quality indicator ratings for sensory-based, Treatment and Education of
Autistic and related Communication-handicapped Children (TEACCH), and technology-based
intervention types.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 99
Figure 6.
Forest plot of robust variance estimation (RVE) summary estimates with small sample
bias correction for each outcome by intervention type, when all outcomes from
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 100
quasi-experimental and RCT group design studies are included. * denotes summary effect size
estimates with confidence intervals that do not overlap with zero.
Figure 7.
Forest plot of robust variance estimation (RVE) summary estimates with small sample
bias correction for each outcome by intervention type, when all outcomes from RCTs are
included. * denotes summary effect size estimates with confidence intervals that do not overlap
with zero.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 101
Figure 8.
Forest plot of robust variance estimation (RVE) summary estimates with small sample
bias correction for each outcome by intervention type, when only non-caregiver report outcomes
from RCTs are included. * denotes summary effect size estimates with confidence intervals that
do not overlap with zero.
AUTISM INTERVENTION META-ANALYSIS 102
Figure 9.
Forest plot of robust variance estimation (RVE) summary estimates with small sample
bias correction for each outcome by intervention type, when only outcomes from RCTs that are
not threatened by detection bias are included. denotes summary effect size estimates that have
p-values < 0.10.
... Despite caregiver concerns arising at the typical age behavioral signs of autism fist emerge, diagnoses tended to be delayed until 24 months later throughout Latin America and the Caribbean. Early identification is essential to implement early, specialized interventions as they represent the best opportunity to maximize developmental outcomes (Fuller & Kaiser, 2020;Sandbank et al., 2020). Many factors, however, have been associated with delays in autism identification, including symptom severity, socioeconomic status, ethnic minority status, low caregiver awareness of the early signs of autism, and visiting greater numbers of health providers before diagnosis (Mandell et al., 2005). ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Speech, language, and hearing professionals are mandated to provide culturally responsive services for individuals who hold multiple identities and may face unique challenges resulting from multiple discrimination and cumulative disadvantage. This study presents a thematic analysis of ethnographic interviews with an Indigenous Mayan family with a minimally speaking autistic child. This article aims to improve the ability of speech, language, and hearing professionals to be informed of unique challenges at the intersection of autism and ethnicity. Method This study adopted an exploratory approach to the analysis of semistructured ethnographic interviews with three members of a Mayan family regarding their experiences having a minimally speaking autistic family member. A thematic analysis was carried out in collaboration with the second author, a member of the community and an ethnographic researcher. Results Six themes were identified in the thematic analysis: (1) autistic from birth; (2) communication challenges; (3) lack of resources; (4) autism awareness and inclusion are not widespread; (5) intervention is important, but access is limited; and (6) discrimination can sometimes come from within one's own community. Conclusions This study outlined several themes in the experiences of an Indigenous Mayan family with a minimally speaking autistic child. Speech, language, and hearing professionals should be aware of the lack of resources and the exclusion from educational opportunities that Indigenous autistic individuals may face. Specific examples of implications for clinical practice are discussed.
... In particular, many early behavioral interventions for autism focus on improving social approach motivation (Dawson et al. 2010;Hardan et al. 2015;Uljarević et al. 2022), yet substantial variability exists in treatment responses (Sandbank et al. 2020;Trembath et al. 2023), and the factors influencing intervention outcomes are not fully understood. Our findings suggest that when focusing on promoting social engagement, it is essential to tailor interventions to the specific motivational profile underlying each individual's form of social withdrawal. ...
Article
Full-text available
Considerable variability in social engagement among individuals with autism is well documented. Since multiple processes may contribute to this heterogeneity, validating tools to assess these differences is crucial. Originally developed in the general population, the Child Social Preference Scale (CSPS‐3) aims to assess distinct forms of social disengagement arising from different combinations of approach and avoidance motivations and holds promise for delineating variability in social behaviors within autism spectrum disorder (ASD). This study investigated the factor structure and psychometric properties of the CSPS‐3 in a sample of 689 children diagnosed with autism (Mage = 11.23, SD = 3.56; 76% male). The results suggest that a bifactor model, consisting of a general factor and three subscales (shyness, unsociability, and social avoidance), provided the best fit to the data, with the general factor accounting for most of the variance. While the subscales demonstrated adequate internal consistency, their construct reliability and stability varied, with much of the reliable variance attributed to the general factor. The structure was consistent across age and sex subgroups, and the subscales showed distinct patterns of associations with key clinical correlates. These findings support the CSPS‐3's utility in assessing diverse forms of social disengagement in the autism population, while indicating that the subscales could be refined to better capture their unique aspects.
... Autism Center 2015), and, more recently, comprehensive metaanalyses (Sandbank et al. 2020(Sandbank et al. , 2023. ...
Article
Full-text available
The evidence base on autism diagnosis and intervention has grown exponentially in the past two decades, but there continue to be gaps in the path connecting research, policy, and practice. For example, although standardized autism screening tools have been shown to be helpful for identifying early signs of autism and facilitating early diagnosis, many pediatricians in the United States do not use them as recommended. Similarly, despite the sound evidence supporting Naturalistic Developmental Behavioral Interventions, they are seldom used in early intervention practice. This commentary examines the nature of these gaps using the Exploration, Preparation, Implementation, Sustainment (EPIS) framework, with a focus on the role of “big P” policies, which include legislation and agency regulations, and “little p” policies, which include guidelines set by professional organizations. Efforts to bridge the gap between research and practice through policy offer the potential for improving the lives of those on the autism spectrum through early detection and intervention programs and beyond.
... 25 Developmental interventions are a form of early support, designed to enhance children's learning and development through their interactions with other people 19 and within the context of developmentally appropriate, naturalistic settings and daily activities. 16 Evidence shows that developmental interventions, including those that are caregiver-mediated, can have significant positive effects on young autistic children's social communication and engagement [26][27][28][29][30] and caregiver communication, responsiveness and interaction. 19 26 However, in most intervention studies, caregiver coaching has been provided using a one-to-one model of direct support 24 31-33 ; a resourceintensive model for community providers. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Vast empirical evidence highlights the importance of early identification, diagnosis and support for autistic children. Caregivers of autistic children often experience high levels of psychological distress; hence there is a need for parallel child and caregiver support. Autism New Zealand’s Let’s Play programme is a caregiver-mediated, community-based programme based on the principles of developmental and relational interventions (henceforth, developmental). Developmental interventions are evidence-based supports designed to enhance children’s learning within the context of developmentally appropriate, naturalistic settings (eg, everyday routines, play). We aim to evaluate the effects of the Let’s Play programme on autistic children’s engagement and caregiver stress. Methods and analysis This study will be a single-blind (rater) randomised controlled trial with two parallel arms: immediate programme access (intervention) versus a waitlist control. Participants will be 64 caregivers of children aged 0–5 years with diagnosed or suspected autism, allowing for 20% attrition, based on power calculations. The Let’s Play programme will be delivered over 9 weeks using a combination of small group workshops and in-home coaching. Primary outcome variables include child engagement and caregiver stress. Caregivers will complete measures at three time points (baseline, immediately post-programme and at the 6-month follow-up), and effectiveness will be analysed using generalised estimating equation models and intention-to-treat and per protocol analyses. Ethics and dissemination This trial was approved by Aotearoa New Zealand Ministry of Health’s Health and Disability Ethics Committee (2022 FULL 13041). Findings will be communicated nationally and internationally via conferences, journal publications and stakeholder groups (eg, service providers for autistic children). Results will be shared regardless of magnitude or direction of effect. Trial registration number ACTRN12622001139763.
... First, this study strictly adhered to the PICO principle, defining "Intervention (I)" as PMIs in the inclusion criteria, which led to a different scope and an increased number of included studies. Second, we conducted this meta-analysis by referencing and following the methods of Sandbank et al. (2020). These differences may account for the inconsistencies in the research findings. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to assess the effectiveness of PMIs for ASD under 3 years, and explore potential moderators influencing the effectiveness through moderator analyses. The study searched five English databases for randomized controlled trials (RCTs). The meta-analysis was conducted using a random-effects model to calculate Hedges’s g. Subgroup analyses and meta regression assessed the effects of potential moderators on PMIs effectiveness, with evidence quality evaluated using GRADE. A total of 31 RCTs were included in the systematic review, with 26 included in the meta-analysis. The results showed a small overall beneficial effect of PMIs on ASD under 3 years (g = 0.20). Small to trivial positive effects were found in several subdomains, including adaptive skills (g = 0.29), parent responsiveness (g = 0.23), parent–child interaction (g = 0.35), social communication (g = 0.18), and symptoms (g = − 0.22). However, PMIs did not show statistically significant effects on children’s cognitive competence, language, or motor skills domains. Subgroup analysis and meta-regression explored potential moderators, but none significantly influenced the effectiveness of PMIs. The GRADE assessment showed that the certainty of the evidence ranged from moderate to low. This study confirmed that PMIs demonstrate positive effects on children under 3 years old with ASD, and showed beneficial outcomes in most subdomains. However, the evidence was of moderate to low certainty, so these findings should be interpreted with caution. In the future, broader databases and more large-scale, multicenter, high-quality clinical studies are needed to confirm these effects.
Article
Restricted and repetitive behaviours vary greatly between autistic people. Some are a source of pleasure or create opportunities for learning; others may be detrimental in day-to-day life or cause harm. We have developed, in close collaboration with parents/carers, the Understanding Repetitive Behaviours programme, designed for families of young autistic children, to help them recognise, understand and respond sensitively to their child’s impactful restricted and repetitive behaviours. This study is a clinical and cost-effectiveness, multi-site randomised controlled trial of the Understanding Repetitive Behaviours parent programme versus a psychoeducation programme (equivalent to current best practice), learning about autism. Participants were parents/carers, with an autistic child aged between 3–9 years and 11 months. The study was delivered across three sites in England and Scotland. Analyses were completed using intention-to-treat principles. Two hundred and twenty seven families were randomised (113 in LAA; 114 in Understanding Repetitive Behaviours arm). No differences were found between the arms on the primary outcome measure (The Clinical Global Impression – Improvement scale). Analysis of secondary outcomes indicated that children in the Understanding Repetitive Behaviours arm were more likely to be rated as responders in target impactful restricted and repetitive behaviours at 24 weeks but that this effect was not maintained at 52 weeks. Improvements in parent and family functioning were apparent, with no evidence of differences between the arms. The study reconfirms that it is important that clinicians consider both restricted and repetitive behaviours and social communication needs of autistic children with parents when planning appropriate support. Lay abstract Autistic children, frequently repeat the same behaviours over and over, have specific interests or like things to stay the same. These behaviours and interests are often fun and helpful. However, sometimes they can impact negatively on day-to-day life or put the child at risk of harm. Working closely with parents of autistic children, we developed an 8-week programme (Understanding Repetitive Behaviours) to help them recognise and understand these behaviours. This study aimed to find out whether the understanding repetitive behaviour programme was helpful and good value for money. Two hundred and twenty seven families were allocated by chance to receive either Understanding Repetitive Behaviours or a learning about autism programme. When experts made judgements about whether children showed positive changes across various measures, and these were analysed, there were no differences between the programmes. However, parents who attended the Understanding Repetitive Behaviours programme reported improvement in one of their child’s specific repetitive behaviour (selected to be the main focus of the Understanding Repetitive Behaviours programme) at 24 weeks after the end of the programme. Parents who attended either programme reported more confidence, greater wellbeing and less stress up to 1 year after the end of the study.
Article
Full-text available
Autism has historically been defined by the presence of differences in social communication and restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, or activities (RRBs). Since 2013 when the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders was published, sensory features were added as one of the polythetic restricted and repetitive behavior diagnostic criteria of autism, though it has remained understudied. Here, we summarize theory and research to provide support for the perspective that early sensory functions and experiences play a primary role in autism and have downstream effects on social communication and repetitive behavioral features of autism. The goals of this article are to provide an understanding of the current sensory research landscape over the early developmental period; to contextualize our knowledge autism within a developmental framework; to delineate a cascading developmental model that provides testable hypotheses; and to identify current gaps in research that would allow us to further our understanding of the role, and primacy of sensory differences in the development of the autistic phenotype. We close by offering a set of recommendations for the field.
Article
Full-text available
This meta-analysis examined the effectiveness of Applied Behavior Analysis-based (ABA) interventions, including Naturalistic Developmental Behavioral Interventions (NDBI), in improving communication, adaptive, and cognitive skills as well as severity of challenging characteristics in children identified as being on the autism spectrum. Furthermore, our investigation extended to the evaluation of potential moderator variables. Twenty-five studies meeting the inclusion criteria were identified. Pre- to post-intervention effects were compared between experimental and control groups. Compared to control-groups ABA-based interventions revealed a large effect size for receptive language skills and moderate effect sizes for adaptive, and cognitive skills. Meta-regression revealed that ABA-based interventions with greater treatment dose and duration yielded greater improvements in adaptive behaviors. Implications for future research and practice are being discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Autism spectrum disorder encompasses diverse patterns of social communication and repetitive, restricted behaviours. Various interventions have been developed to reduce the negative consequences of this disorder and improve levels of functioning, and recently interest in parent-mediated interventions has increased. Previous reviews and meta-analyses have investigated the effects of the parent-mediated interventions, however; a systematic review with meta-analysis of high quality has not been performed since 2013. This protocol for a systematic review with meta-analysis aims to describe the methods and purpose of synthesising current evidence regarding the effects (both positive and adverse) of parent-mediated interventions in both children with autism and their parents. Methods Electronic searches will be conducted in Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL), Medical Literature Analysis and Retrieval System Online (MEDLINE), Excerpta Medica database (EMBASE), Latin American and Caribbean Health Sciences Literature (LILACS), American Psychological Association PsycInfo (PsycInfo) and Science Citation Index Expanded (SCI-EXPANDED). Randomised clinical trials of parent-mediated interventions for children with autism and control groups of usual care, waiting list or no treatment will be included. Two reviewers will independently screen, select and collect data. Methodological quality of included studies will be evaluated using Cochrane methodology. The primary outcome will be autism symptom severity as measured by the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Secondary outcomes will be adaptive functioning, adverse effects, child language, child´s quality of life, parental quality of life and parental stress. Meta-analyses and Trial Sequential Analysis will be performed. Discussion This is the study protocol for a systematic review and meta-analysis of parent-mediated interventions versus usual care for children with ASD. Results of the review will inform clinicians and parents about the current evidence of the effects, both positive and negative, of parent-mediated interventions on younger children with autism and their parents, through improved methodology and inclusion of new studies. PROSPERO registration number: 385188
Article
Full-text available
Background Restricted and repetitive behaviours vary greatly across the autism spectrum, and although not all are problematic some can cause distress and interfere with learning and social opportunities. We have, alongside parents, developed a parent group based intervention for families of young children with autism, which aims to offer support to parents and carers; helping them to recognise, understand and learn how to respond to their child’s challenging restricted repetitive behaviours. Methods The study is a clinical and cost-effectiveness, multi-site randomised controlled trial of the Managing Repetitive Behaviours (MRB) parent group intervention versus a psychoeducation parent group Learning About Autism (LAA) ( n = 250; 125 intervention/125 psychoeducation; ~ 83/site) for parents of young children aged 3–9 years 11 months with a diagnosis of autism. All analyses will be done under intention-to-treat principle. The primary outcome at 24 weeks will use generalised estimating equation (GEE) to compare proportion of children with improved RRB between the MRB group and the LAA group. The GEE model will account for the clustering of children by parent groups using exchangeable working correlation. All secondary outcomes will be analysed in a similar way using appropriate distribution and link function. The economic evaluation will be conducted from the perspective of both NHS costs and family access to local community services. A ‘within trial’ cost-effectiveness analysis with results reported as the incremental cost per additional child achieving at least the target improvement in CGI-I scale at 24 weeks. Discussion This is an efficacy trial to investigate the clinical and cost-effectiveness of a parent group based intervention designed to help parents understand and manage their child’s challenging RRB. If found to be effective, this intervention has the potential to improve the well-being of children and their families, reduce parental stress, greatly enhance community participation and potential for learning, and improve longer-term outcomes. Trial registration Trial ID: ISRCTN15550611 Date registered: 07/08/2018. Sponsor and Monitor: Cumbria, Northumberland, Tyne and Wear NHS Foundation Trust R&D Manager Lyndsey Dixon, Address: St Nicholas Hospital, Jubliee Road, Gosforth, Newcastle upon Tyne NE3 3XT, lyndsey.dixon@cntw.nhs.uk, Tel: 0191 246 7222
Article
Full-text available
The number of trials aimed at evaluating treatments for autism spectrum disorder has been increasing progressively. However, it is not clear which outcome measures should be used to assess their efficacy, especially for treatments which target core symptoms. The present review aimed to provide a comprehensive overview regarding the outcome measures used in clinical trials for people with autism spectrum disorder. We systematically searched the Web of KnowledgeSM database between 1980 and 2016 to identify published controlled trials investigating the efficacy of interventions in autism spectrum disorder. We included 406 trials in the final database, from which a total of 327 outcome measures were identified. Only seven scales were used in more than 5% of the studies, among which only three measured core symptoms (Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Childhood Autism Rating Scale, and Social Responsiveness Scale). Of note, 69% of the tools were used in the literature only once. Our systematic review has shown that the evaluation of efficacy in intervention trials for autism spectrum disorder relies on heterogeneous and often non-specific tools for this condition. The fragmentation of tools may significantly hamper the comparisons between studies and thus the discovery of effective treatments for autism spectrum disorder. Greater consensus regarding the choice of these measures should be reached.
Article
Full-text available
At the 2019 strategic planning meeting the International Society for Autism Research (INSAR) board discussed the question of appropriate language to be used when speaking or writing about autism or affected individuals. Board members articulated a wide range of views on this subject, making clear that there is no single simple answer. This commentary was inspired by that discussion. It is by John Elder Robison who is both an INSAR board member and an individual diagnosed with autism. Autism Res 2019, 12: 1004–1006. © 2019 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Lay Summary How should researchers talk about autism? Personal reflections on writing and speaking about autism, with particular regard for affected individuals, be they autistic people, people with autism, or family members. This commentary is authored by John Elder Robison who is both an INSAR board member and an individual diagnosed with autism.
Article
Full-text available
This meta-analysis examined the effects of early interventions on social communication outcomes for young children with autism spectrum disorder. A systematic review of the literature included 1442 children (mean age 3.55 years) across 29 studies. The overall effect size of intervention on social communication outcomes was significant (g = 0.36). The age of the participants was related to the treatment effect size on social communication outcomes, with maximum benefits occurring at age 3.81 years. Results did not differ significantly depending on the person implementing the intervention. However, significantly larger effect sizes were observed in studies with context-bound outcome measures. The findings of this meta-analysis highlight the need for further research examining specific components of interventions associated with greater and more generalized gains.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: The multifactorial nature of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is the reason why complementary and alternative methods of treatment are sought in order to support the classic approach. Objectives: The aim of the study was to assess the effectiveness of Equine-Assisted Activities and Therapies (EAAT) in ASD patients based on a review of the literature. Methods: A review of the literature and a meta-analysis were conducted in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines. PUBMED, Cochrane Library, Web of Science, ClinicalTrials.gov and PEDro databases were searched until July 20, 2017. Only articles published in English, in a journal with a review process, after 1999, with a control group or presentation of comparative pre-/post-therapy results in ASD patients, and clear inclusion/exclusion criteria were considered. The methodological quality of the included studies was assessed using the Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies (QATQS).The meta-analysis of three studies was conducted. Results: A total of 15 studies with 390 participants (aged: 3–16 years) were included. The interaction between psychosocial functioning and EAAT was investigated in most studies. Improvement was reported in the following domains: socialization, engagement, maladaptive behaviors, and shorter reaction time in problem-solving situations after EAAT. The meta-analysis revealed no statistically significant differences for the investigated effects. Conclusions: Despite the need for further, more standardized research, the results of the studies included in this review allow us to conclude that EAAT may be a useful form of therapy in children with ASD.
Article
Full-text available
A growing body of evidence indicates that the effects reported in many scientific fields may be overestimated or even false. This problem has gained a lot of attention in the field of psychology, where researchers have even started to speak of a ‘replication crisis’. Fortunately, a number of measures to rectify this problem have already been proposed and implemented, some inspired by practices in other scientific fields. In this review, I briefly examine this issue in the field of psychology and suggest some practical tools and strategies that researchers can implement to increase replicability and the overall quality of their scientific research. What this paper adds Researchers can implement many practical tools and strategies to improve replicability of their findings. Strategies include improving statistical inference, pre‐registration, multisite collaboration, and sharing data. Different scientific fields could benefit from looking at each other's best practices.
Chapter
This chapter describes the principles and methods used to carry out a meta-analysis for a comparison of two interventions for the main types of data encountered. A very common and simple version of the meta-analysis procedure is commonly referred to as the inverse-variance method. This approach is implemented in its most basic form in RevMan, and is used behind the scenes in many meta-analyses of both dichotomous and continuous data. Results may be expressed as count data when each participant may experience an event, and may experience it more than once. Count data may be analysed using methods for dichotomous data if the counts are dichotomized for each individual, continuous data and time-to-event data, as well as being analysed as rate data. Prediction intervals from random-effects meta-analyses are a useful device for presenting the extent of between-study variation. Sensitivity analyses should be used to examine whether overall findings are robust to potentially influential decisions.
Article
Despite a slow start,¹ the past 15 years has seen an unprecedented increase in the number and quality of randomized controlled trials (RCTs) being conducted in the early autism field. This is welcome, because many young children with autism struggle to communicate and interact with others, restricting their opportunities to learn and develop, and impacting on their parents who can find their child's behavior perplexing and challenging to manage. However, as with so many areas of clinical science, with progress come challenges. Rogers et al.² report on a multi-site RCT of the Early Start Denver Model (ESDM), an intensive, naturalistic developmental−behavioural intervention program. The original small-scale (n = 48) ESDM study³ found improvements in IQ and adaptive behavior (on both measures largely in the language/communication domains) and a marginal improvement in diagnostic classification but no differences on continuous measures of autism severity. The study is highly cited and has been influential as a key part of the evidence for proponents arguing for the effectiveness of comprehensive early intervention programs.