ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Serpent‐god worship is an ancient tradition still practiced in many sacred groves across the Western Ghats of India. Although sacred groves there hold ecological conservation value, few studies have focused on arguably the most iconic taxon in the region, snakes. We thus investigated the impact of sacred groves and snake deity worshipping on attitudes towards snakes by conducting surveys with people who had entered sacred groves in the past. We found that very few participants who had encountered snakes inside sacred groves in the past harmed them during these encounters. However, nearly a quarter of all participants do harm snakes if encountered outside sacred groves. We also found that a larger proportion of participants who do not harm snakes outside sacred groves worship snake deities, relative to those that do harm them. Our work thus highlights the influence of sacred groves and snake deity worshipping on pacifistic human–snake relations in Southwestern India. A free Plain Language Summary can be found within the Supporting Information of this article.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
People and Nature. 2020;2:111–122. 
|
 111
wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/pan3
1 | INTRODUCTION
Overshadowed by growing concerns over contemporary human‐in
duced threats to ecosystems associated with global urbanization,
numerous cultures throughout the world have upheld the preserva
tion of natur al sites for generations by valuing their sanctit y (Dudley,
Higgins‐Zogib, & Mansourian, 2009; Verschuuren, Wild, McNeely, &
Oviedo, 2010). By preventing the development or extensive deg
radation of sacred natural sites, such beliefs and traditions have
helped maintain local biodiversity despite ongoing nearby urban
growth and land‐use changes (Verschuuren et al., 2010). There has
consequently been a sharpening focus on their value in current
conservation biology (Mcleod & Palmer, 2015; Waylen, Fischer,
Mcgowan, Thirgood, & Milner‐Gulland, 2010), perhaps as an ideal
for the prosperity of both nature and society (Pardo‐de‐Santayana
& Macía, 2015). Although found throughout the world (Verschuuren
et al., 2010), the potential role for sacred natural sites in modern
conservation practices has garnered significant attention in places
such as Ethiopia (Aerts et al., 2016; Teketay et al., 2010), Ghana
(Decher, 1997; Sarfo‐Mensah, Oduro, Antoh Fredua, & Amisah,
2010), Tanzania (Kideghesho, 20 08; Mgumia & Oba, 2003), and
Southwestern China (Salick et al., 2007; Shen, Lu, Li, & Chen, 2012),
where studies have exemplified their efficacy in safeguarding native
flora and fauna.
Received:22July2019 
|
Accepted:6October2019
DOI: 10.100 2/pan3.10 059
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Sacred groves and serpentgods moderate humansnake
relations
Félix Landry Yuan1| U. Prashanth Ballullaya2| Ramesh Roshnath2|
Timothy C. Bonebrake1| Palatty Allesh Sinu2
This is an op en access article under t he terms of the Creat ive Commons Attributio n License, which permits use, dist ribution and reproduc tion in any medium,
provide d the orig inal work is proper ly cited .
© 2019 The Auth ors. People and Nature published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of British Ecolo gical Society
1School of Biological Scie nces, The
University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR ,
China
2Depar tment of A nimal Science, School of
Biologi cal Science, Central Uni versit y of
Kerala, Kasaragod, India
Correspondence
Félix La ndry Yuan
Email: flyuan@connect.hku.hk
Funding information
Science Engineering Rese arch Board,
New Delhi, Grant/Award Number: SB/FT/
LS‐325/2012; U niversity of Hong Kong;
Kerala St ate Council for Science, Technology
& Environment; Malabar Awareness and
Rescue Center for Wildlife
Handling Editor: Arjen Buijs
Abstract
1. Serpent‐god worship is an ancient tradition still practiced in many sacred groves
across the Western Ghats of India. Although sacred groves there hold ecological
conservation value, few studies have focused on arguably the most iconic taxon in
the region, snakes.
2. We thus investigated the impact of sacred groves and snake deity worshipping
on attitudes towards snakes by conducting surveys with people who had entered
sacred groves in the past.
3. We found that very few participants who had encountered snakes inside sacred
groves in the past harmed them during these encounters. However, nearly a quar‐
ter of all participants do harm snakes if encountered outside sacred groves.
4. We also found that a larger proportion of participants who do not harm snakes
outside sacred groves worship snake deities, relative to those that do harm them.
5. Our work thus highlights the influence of sacred groves and snake deity worship‐
ping on pacifistic human–snake relations in Southwestern India.
KEY WORDS
biocultural landscape, environmental perception, human–snake relation, religion, sacred
grove, snakebite, The Western Ghats
112 
|
    
People and Nature
LAND RY YUAN et AL .
But perhaps the most prominent case of environmental protec‐
tion resulting as a by‐product of traditional beliefs is to be found
in India, where relatively undisturbed patches of forest, or “sacred
groves”, have been sites of worship towards gods, deities and an‐
cestral spirits (Freeman, 1999; Gadgil & Vartak, 1976). As a conse‐
quence of the values placed on these forests by local communities,
many have been preser ved for extensive periods of time, with some
having persisted for as long as 40 0 years (Bhagwat, Nogué, & Willis,
2014). Sacred groves are especially common in the Western Ghats
(Bhagwat, Kushalappa, Williams, & Brown, 2005), the mount ain
range straddling the South‐Western edge of the Indian subconti‐
nent widely known as one of the world's top “hotspots” for biodi‐
versity (Myers, Mittermeier, Mittermeier, Fonseca, & Kent, 2000).
Located in an ecologically distinct region, the sacred groves of the
Western Ghats have for tuitously served as sanctuaries for local flora
and small to medium‐sized fauna (Bhagwat et al., 2005; Bhagwat
& Rutte, 20 06; Ray, Chandran, & Ramachandra, 2014). They have
also been shown to host a high number of endemic plant species
(Chandrashekara & Sankar, 1998), and to maintain this richness rel‐
ative to nearby areas, including disturbed forest s and, occasionally,
government‐managed protected areas (Dudley et al., 2009; Ormsby
& Bhagwat, 2010).
Although the extent of the Western Ghats spans six states, a
significant region of interest for such forests is Southwestern India,
where Kerala is the Indian state with the most sacred groves (Khan,
Ashalat a, & Tripathi, 20 08; Rajendraprasad, 1995) and borders
Southern Karnataka state, itself displaying the densest distribu
tion of sacred groves (Khan et al., 20 08; Kushalappa, Bhagwat, &
Kushalappa, 2001). Known as kavu in Malayalam and devarakadu in
Kannada, these forests are usually visited by local inhabitants for
the worship of one or many gods in particular (Ballullaya et al., 2019).
However, what is especially unique for the sacred groves of this re‐
gion is the frequent presence of idols, shrines or temples devoted
to serpent‐gods, often known as Nāga in Sanskrit, or by a variety of
other local names. While the origins of this tradition in sacred groves
in particular are obscure, serpent depictions in art pre‐date the pres
ence of the Indo‐Aryan culture in South Asia, where the “earliest
evidence s of the topic of snakes are to b e found in the pictor ial repre‐
sentations from Harappa, Mohenjo‐Daro and Lothal, that is from the
time of about 2000 BC” (Härtel, 1976:664). Härtel further reports
that “the oldest images of Nāgas after the Harappa times appear
as late as the end of the second centur y BC” (1976:667). Referring
to early disseminations on the antiquity of serpent worship in the
region by architectural historian James Fergusson, Wake writes “he
supposes it not to have been adopted by any nation belonging to
the Semitic or Aryan stock; the serpent worship of India and Greece
originat ing, as he believes, w ith older peopl es” (1873:373). Such ideas
from the 19th century Western thinkers still hold some relevance
today, as the worship of snake deities in sacred groves is thought to
potentially be the resulting blend of deeply rooted indigenous folk
traditions with later rituals and beliefs of devotion towards Nāgas
(Murugan, Ramachandran, Swarupanandan, & Remesh, 2008), in the
same way that representations and traditions surrounding local folk
deities in sacred groves have been assimilated with other pan‐Indian
gods over time (Ormsby, 2011; Tomalin, 200 4).
Inside sacred groves in which serpent‐gods are among the deities
revered, or sarpa kavu in Malayalam (roughly translating to “snake
garden”), locals perform rituals of worship out of devotion for the
Nāgas. These serpent‐gods in question are not necessarily synony‐
mous with actual living snakes, but are divine beings or deities which
are depicted as displaying the same physical features as snakes,
with a specific allusion to cobras. According to early 20 th century
art historian Jean Philippe Vogel, “the Nāga of Indian mythology
and folklore is not really the snake in general, but the cobra raised
to the rank of a divine being” and it is thus “evident that the Nāga
in his animal form is conceived as the hooded snake” (1995[1926]:
27). Therefore, any following mention of “snake deit y” or “serpent‐
god” in this study will be in reference to these divine beings, while
the term “snake” alone will be in reference to actual living snakes.
Nonetheless, the two are inexorably connected in the sense that any
affliction posed towards snakes, whether intentional or accidental,
is believed to bring forth the wrath of the Nāgas in various forms; as
Allocco writes, referring to contemporary South India, “A number of
authors note that killing a snake is regarded as a sin and detail the
lengths that individuals may go to in order to avoid injuring a snake,
as nāgas are believed to deliver formidable curses with far‐reaching
implications” (2013:231).
But beyond their relationship with actual living snakes, Nāgas
also symbolize human fertility and childbirth, as a reflection of the
forest's produc tivity (Das & Balasubramanian, 2017). The perception
of these life‐giving powers is perhaps rooted in the association of
Nāgas with water, one of the limiting factors of agricultural produc‐
tion. As Vogel suggested, “though easily moved to anger, [the Nāgas]
are worthy of being propitiated, as their activity is, on the whole,
beneficial to the welfare of man, especially in connexion with their
power over the element of water.” (1995[1926]: 3). With both fear
and admiration at play, snake deity worshipping is therefore polar
ized in its motivations, and is not restricted to sarpa kavu, with such
beliefs stretching across the Western Ghats as well as many other
parts of India (Nair, 2017).
Yet, despite the prevailing devotion towards snake deities in the
sacred groves of Southwestern India in particular, the vast majority
of diversit y assessments in such forests have focused on flora, with
few studies concerning reptiles (Ray et al., 2014). With an estimated
45,900 human deaths annually in India, the country has one of the
highest rates of deadly venomous snake bites globally (Mohapatra
et al., 2011; Warrell, 2010). Thus, recognized as life‐threatening ani
mals, this awareness permeates into the daily lives of people, whom
simultaneously respect and revere snake deities (Allocco, 2013). At
the crossroads of danger and devotion, how do such beliefs and per‐
ceptions translate into the way locals co‐exist with mortal, physical
snakes (Narayanan & Bindumadhav, 2018)?
According ly, in this case stud y, our aim was to e xplore the rel ation‐
ship between visitors of sacred groves and snakes in Southwestern
India within the context of a prevalent snake deity worshipping
tradition, and it s potential implications for current conservation
    
|
 113
People and Nature
LAND RY YUAN et AL .
strategies. For this inquiry, we visited sacred groves which did or did
not host snake deities and conduc ted surveys using questionnaires
(Moon, Brewer, Januchowski‐Hartley, Adams, & Blackman, 2016).
Our main objectives were (a) to determine if attitudes and reactions
towards snakes depended on the presence of a snake deity in the
sacred grove, and (b) whether snake deity worshipping was asso‐
ciated with more pacifistic reactions to snake encounters inside or
outside sacred groves. We also aimed (c) to investigate if reactions
to snakes were more pacifistic inside sacred groves than outside
of them, regardless of the presence of a snake deity. As one of the
oldest examples of nature worship still in existence today, the be‐
liefs surrounding sarpa kavu have long been understood to preser ve
its native species (Murugan et al., 2008), yet this study is the first
to quantif y just how this reflects the views of visitors towards the
snakes inhabiting them.
2 | MATERIALS AND METHODS
2.1 | Study area
The focal area for our work comprised the Southwestern portion
of the Western Ghats of India. Located in the coastal Malabar re‐
gion of the state of Kerala, the districts of Kasaragod and Kannur
are predominantly inhabited by various castes belonging to Thiyya
communities, which themselves have unique customs and rituals as‐
sociated with its sacred groves (Chandrashekara, Joseph, & Sreejith,
2002). Although the floral communities within these sacred groves
can be quite diverse, they are usually surrounded by urbanized
areas or agriculture such as rubber plantations and rice paddies
(Chandrashekara & Sankar, 1998).On the other hand, in the south
of Karnataka state, the predominant cultural group found through‐
out the district of Kodagu is that of the indigenous Kodava people.
Found at slightly higher elevations, the landscapes there are more
sparsely urbanized and dominated by sacred groves alongside cof‐
fee plantations (Bhagwat et al., 2005; Kushalappa et al., 2001). We
thus identified sites in these regions based on our prior knowledge
of their existence as a result of past research, or through information
obtained from local inhabitants.
In October 2018, we visited 10 sacred groves in each of the
three district s varying in how urban or rural the sites generally
were: Kannur for the most urban sacred groves, Kodagu for the
most rural ones and Kasaragod for the more intermediary sites
(Figure 1). While varying in their struc ture, these sacred groves
always contained a central worshipping compound for the perfor
mance of rituals by designated priests. To accommodate more vis
itors, the more urban sacred groves tended to contain larger areas
covered by man‐made infrastructure such as cement, cobblestone,
halls and temples. On the other hand, the more rural sacred groves
were more comparable to relatively undisturbed forest, with the
only built structures being smaller worshipping compounds and
the occasional shrine or temple. Accordingly, the average tree
cover percentage within 1 km from the centroid for our sites vis
ited were 25.96 ± 4.90%, 30.72 ± 6.15% and 41.42 ± 5.35% for
Kannur, Kasaragod and Kodagu, respectively (based on an analysis
using the forest cover dataset described in Hansen et al., 2013).
We also aimed to cover a wide range of sacred grove sizes, with
FIGURE 1 Map of the por tion
of Southwestern India, including
Northwestern Kerala and Southern
Karnataka states, where we conducted
our surveys. The 30 sacred grove sites we
have visited are indicated by the points,
and coded according to the districts of
Kannur (yellow diamonds), Kasaragod (red
squares) and Kodagu (blue circles). The 18
sacred groves devoted to snake deities
are demarcated by a black circle at the
centre of each point. Panels (a), (b) and
(c) each depic t examples of sites visited
which were, respectively, rural, urban, and
intermediary
(c)
(a)
(b)
114 
|
    
People and Nature
LAND RY YUAN et AL .
sites ranging from 0.17 to 104 acres, and the overall mean size of
all 30 sacred groves visited being 11.01 acres.
Serpent‐gods were among the deities worshipped in 18 of the 30
sites (Figure 1). To be certain of whether snake deities were among
the acknowledged deities in each sacred grove or not, we always
consulted associated elders or priests. This is because by visiting
alone, the representation of a snake deity inside a sacred grove is not
always evident. They can sometimes be depicted by statues showing
typical snake‐like features one could expect of serpent‐gods, while
they can also be symbolized by more abstract idols, such as rounded
stones, with little indication of their significance.
2.2 | Questionnaires for participants
For each sacred grove visited, we sur veyed 10 people opportunisti‐
cally, between the day time hours of 9:00 and 17:00, when encoun‐
tered either inside the forest s or within 1 km, and conducted these
in either Malayalam, Kannada or Tulu, in addition to one in Hindi. To
be considered as a participant, each individual must have had visited
the sacred grove in question in the past. Therefore, both devotees
of the sacred grove's deit y and non‐devotees could be considered.
Although the vast majority of the 300 participants identified as dev‐
otees (96%, n = 289), we also surveyed some non‐devotees identify‐
ing themselves as visitors (3%, n = 8), management members (1%,
n = 2), as well as one local resident (<1%, n = 1). Notably, as well
as being devotees, 5% of total participants were also priests or as‐
sistant priests (n = 15), while 26% were either the owner's family,
management, committee or former committee members (n = 77).
Additionally, the participants we surveyed varied in other demo‐
graphic characteristics, such as age, which ranged from 11 to 87, and
gender, where 34% were female (n = 101) and 66% male (n = 199).
Regarding religious or caste association, teasing apart the t wo terms
is not a simple task, since some castes carry their own unique re‐
ligious beliefs. Accordingly, 18% of participants identified only as
Hindu (n = 53), 17% as Hindu Thiyya (n = 52), 6% as Hindu Kodava
(n = 18) and 15% as Hindu in addition to another caste or religious
identity (n = 45). Without mention of being Hindu, 14% of partici‐
pants identified only as Kodava (n = 41), 10% as Thiyya (n = 3 0) and
16% as other less common groups (n = 47). Of the total, 5% chose not
to comment on their religious or caste identity (n = 14).
These surveys consisted of a questionnaire made up of a com‐
bination of closed and open‐ended questions (Appendix S1; Moon
et al., 2016), and lasted 10–25 min. For the purpose of the analyses
presented in this study, we focused on the answers given for the
questions outlined in Table 1.
The purpose of questions (a) and (b) was to deduce the attitudes
of participants towards snakes, while questions (c) and (d) were to
gain a sense of how participants co‐exist with snakes. For question
(e), we asked whether, in general, participants worshipped snake de‐
ities, without contextualizing the question as cultural, religious or
spiritual, thus leaving it open to interpretation by the participant.
Although we surveyed a total of 300 par ticipants, que stion f) was
only posed to 268 (Table 1). This question is distinc t from question
(e), which was to determine whether the participant worships any
snake deity. Rather, question (f) was meant to determine in parallel
whether participants considered any actual living snake species to
be sacred. For question (f) (ii) (Table 1), participants first responded
by describing or giving the local names of snakes they believed to
be sacred. We then showed them photos of 15 commonly found
snake species in the region (Palot, 2015; Sathish, 2008) for them to
visually identify these sacred snakes. All sur veys were conducted
by the same researchers for consistency (UPB), and informed verbal
consent was received before each survey from participants to be
included in this research. Surveys were conducted with approval by
The University of Hong Kong's Human Research Ethics Committee
(EA1806029),
2.3 | Data analysis
We applied qualitative content analysis to quantify answers and col‐
late them according to connecting themes (Hsieh & Shannon, 2005).
We categorized answers based on a code designed according to the
nature of the statements with regard to attitudes towards snakes
(Erlingsson & Brysiewicz, 2017). For answers given according to
questions (a) and (b) (Table 1), we collated any extra clarifications
according to the nature of the attitude implied towards snakes as
either “positive”, “negative”, “neutral”, or as a “warning sign”, with
the latter implying a conditional statement, that the response could
change depending on the circumstance, or that there was the possi‐
bility for human‐snake conflict (Appendix S2; McMillan, Wong, Hau,
& Bonebrake, 2019). We similarly collated answers to question (e)
(ii) according to their interpreted attitude towards snakes (Appendix
S2). We also collated answers to each questions (c) and (d), based on
whether reactions to snake encounters implied harm to snakes. For
both questions, these consisted of three categories; “Yes”, “No” or
“Maybe” (Appendix S3).
In order to test for differences in answer proportions, we ap
plied either the parametric chi‐square test or non‐parametric Z‐test
TABLE 1 Sample of closed and open‐ended questions asked
during the surveys and for which answers were included within the
analyses of this study
Questions for which answers were ana lyzed in this study
a) Do you like snakes?
□Yes□No□Donotlikeordislike
b) In the future, would you like to see:
□Moresnakesinthesacredgrove□Fewersnakesinthesacredgrove
□Nochangeinthenumberofsnakesint hesacredgrove
c) How do you react upon encountering a snake inside the s acred
grove?
d) How do you reach when encountering a snake outside the sacred
grove?
e) i) Do you ever pray/show worship towards a snake deity? ii) Why?
iii) If yes to ei): Could you describe any rituals or prayers you typi‐
cally perform towards this/these snake deity/deities?
f) i) Do you consider snakes as sacred? ii) Which ones?
    
|
 115
People and Nature
LAND RY YUAN et AL .
depending on the nature of the comparison. To examine whether
there were social determinants of participants harming snakes
outside sacred groves, we built a generalized linear model with
type of visitor (devotee and non‐devotee), Hindu class (upper and
lower class), as well as gender (male and female) as the predictors,
and snake deity presence, sacred grove size, district, and number of
years of association with the sacred grove added as covariates. We
used a binomial error distribution as the response variable consisted
of a binary outcome (“ Yes” or “No”).
We conducted all statistical analyses in R version 3.4.1 (R Core
Tea m 20 17 ).
3 | RESULTS
3.1 | Attitudes towards snakes
We found the general sentiment of par ticipants towards snakes
to be of tolerance and, to a lesser degree, endearment rather than
antipathy. Most said they liked actual living snakes (51%, p < .01,
𝜒2
3
= 133.95, n = 154), a trend which did not differ between sacred
groves with or without snake deities present ( p = .46, Z = 0.09),
while a quarter said they did not like snakes (25%, n = 75) and 18%
did not like or dislike snakes (n = 54, Figure 2a). The remaining 6%
specified the kinds of snakes they did or did not like (n = 17), with
most of these explicitly expressing a dislike for venomous snakes,
only liking non‐venomous snakes, or both statements (65%, n = 11).
Although the vast majority of those that said they liked snakes
did not add any clarifications regarding their attitudes towards
snakes (85%, n = 131, Appendix S2), we found that a moderate pro‐
portion of participants still expressed a fear of snakes (9%, n = 14).
Intuitively, fear was slightly more prevalent for those that did not
like snakes (17%, n = 13, Appendix S2). In both cases, added expres‐
sions of fear made up most of the “warning sign” clarifications for
both “yes” or “no” responses. The one exception for a “warning sign”
not explicitly mentioning fear was a participant who said, in a tone
evoking a newly found vigilance, that although she liked snakes, her
son was bitten by one in the past.
On the other hand, some participants were particularly enthusi
astic about how much they liked snakes (5%, n = 7), adding positive
clarifications explaining that “they are innocent”, or that they “will
not harm them (snakes)”. In a similar sense, within the minority of
participants that did not like snakes, three added to their responses
to the question that despite this, they “will not harm them”.
Moreover, there were a few cases of responses avoiding a po‐
larized “yes” or “no” answer, for which the perceived sanctity of liv‐
ing snakes was the apparent cause. Among clarifications which we
classified as “positive”, one participant said that they did not like or
dislike snakes but had "devotion towards them”, while another sim‐
ply said “I do not dislike them because I worship them”. There were
also two other response clarifications which evoked a clear distinc‐
tion between actual living snakes that are sacred, and others that
are not, such as “I like sacred snakes but dislike venomous snakes”,
which we classified as a “warning sign” given there was a specific
dislike for one type of snake, or “I only like sacred snakes”, which we
classified as “neutral” since it did not explicitly express a dislike for
specific snakes.
This sense of a tolerant attitude towards snakes was further rep
resented by a larger number of participant s wanting more snakes
in sacred groves than those who wanted there to be fewer (p < .01,
𝜒2
1
= 6.72, n = 47 and n = 25 respectively, Figure 2b), implying a sense
of will to co‐exist with them. This trend also stood regardless of the
presence of snake deities within sacred groves, as we found no dif‐
ference in the proportion of those wanting to see more snakes inside
the sacred groves in the future across those with or without snake
deities (p = .09, Z=−1.36).Despitethis,westillfoundthemajor
ity of par ticipants to not want to see any changes in the number of
snakes in the respective sacred groves in the future (51%, p < .01,
𝜒2
3
= 124.91, n = 153).
In some instances, these responses were motivated by the aware
ness of a certain ecological importance of snakes. Among those that
FIGURE 2 Responses of participants
on whether they liked snakes (a) and
what change in the number of snakes
inside sacred groves they would want to
happen (b), with bars representing the
numbers of individuals per response type.
Where extra clarifications were added
to responses, these were categorized
according to the nature of the statement
regarding attitudes towards snakes
0
50
100
150
Ye sNo Does not
like or dislike
Other
Do you like snakes?
Number of individuals
Nature of statement
Negative
Neutral
None
Positive
Warning
(a)
More FewerNo change Other
Wanted change in number of snakes
inside sacred grove
(b)
116 
|
    
People and Nature
LAND RY YUAN et AL .
wanted more snakes in the sacred groves, clarifications to such re
sponses expressed a suppor t for the livelihood of snakes, which we
thus classified as “positive” (n = 6). Examples include one participant
who explicitly stated that snakes “play an important ecological role”,
while others vouched for their protection and the prevention of their
extinction. There were also “neutral” clarifications to responses which
hinted towards an ecological knowledge of snakes, such as one partic
ipant saying that, despite wanting more snakes in sacred groves, the
“space and conditions are not good for snakes to thrive” there. Other
“neutral” clarifications amongst both those that wanted to see no
change in sacred grove snake numbers and those that did not specifi
cally answer the question spoke about the natural fluctuation in snake
numbers; a recognition that these populations are variable over time
regardless of human intervention.
In parallel to this, however, there were clear concerns for the
safety of people within the hypothetical case of larger snake numbers
in sacred groves. For example, while wanting there to be more snakes
in sacred groves, 9% of these participant s clarified that this was under
the condition that they “should not harm people” or “should not come
out of the sacred grove”, which we interpreted as a “warning sign”
(n = 4, Appendix S2). Such caution in light of the potential dangers
of higher snake numbers was also represented among the remaining
quarter of participants that either gave specific answers to the ques
tion, or no comment whatsoever. Notably, 3% of total par ticipants
specifically said that they either wanted there to be fewer venomous
snakes (n = 3), more non‐venomous snakes (n = 1), or both more non‐
venomous and fewer venomous snakes (n = 6).
3.2 | Co‐existence with snakes inside and outside
sacred groves
Moreover, concerns about venomous snakes were especially fea‐
tured in re sponses to ques tion (d) (Table 1), which dea lt with reactio ns
to snake encounters out side of sacred groves. While we found the
proportion of participants implying harm to snakes outside sacred
groves to be 23% (n = 68), nearly half of these responses consisted of
killing them under the condition that the snake was venomous (43%,
n = 29). Similarly, amongst reactions maybe implying harm to snakes
outside sacred groves (18% of total participants, n = 53), 17% (n = 9)
of these involved a conditional statement pertaining to whether the
snake was venomous, with a common response explaining that they
would “inform others if venomous”. However, we also found that
a minorit y of responses implying harm to snakes did not bother to
make such discrimination, and outright killed them unconditionally
during encounters, regardless of whether they were or venomous
or not (13%, n = 9).
In stark contrast, out of the participants that had encountered
snakes inside sacred groves at least once in the past (46%, n = 139),
the distinction between venomous and non‐venomous snakes was
nearly absent in their responses to question (c) (Table 1). Only one
of the two participants that did imply harm to snakes upon encoun‐
tering them inside sacred groves, a propor tion significantly lower
than for reactions outside sacred groves implying harm (1%, p < .05,
Z = −5.63, n = 2, Figure 3), said they would “kill it if it is venom‐
ous”. There was no such condition for those maybe implying harm
to snakes inside sacred groves (2%, n = 3), which was also in a pro‐
portion significantly lower than for those maybe implying harm to
snakes when encountering them outside of sacred groves (p < .05,
Z=−4.51,Figu re3).Infact,mostresp onsestoqu es tio n(c)consisted
of responses which were pacifistic towards snakes and did not imply
harm to them (96%, n = 133), in a proportion significantly higher than
for participants with the same type of reaction to snake encounters
outside sacred groves (60%, p < .05, Z = −7.88, n = 179, Figure 3).
This was apparently true regardless of the type of deity or god wor‐
shipped in the sacred grove, as we found no difference between the
propor tions of participants which did not imply harm to snakes in‐
side sacred groves with (95%, n = 82) or without (98%, n = 51) a snake
deity present (p = .20, Z=−0.83).
3.3 | Snake deity worshipping, the perceived
sanctity of actual living snakes, and their influence
on non‐harmful human–snake relations
In addition to its prevalence inside versus outside sacred groves, we
also found pacifism towards snakes to be associated with devotional
beliefs. While the vast majority of par ticipants said they worshipped
snake deiti es (84%, p < .01,
𝜒2
1
= 138.72, n = 252), a signif icantly larger
propor tion of those which did not imply harm to snakes outside sa‐
cred groves said they worshipped snake deities (87%, n = 155) than
those that did imply harm (75%, p = .01, Z=−2.19,n = 51, Figure 4a).
In parallel to this, while most participants said they considered ac tual
living snakes to be sacred (69%, p < .01,
𝜒2
2
= 89.24, n = 186), this
belief was shared in larger proportion by those that did not imply
harm to snakes outside sacred groves (73%, n = 124) than by those
that did (64%, n = 38), although this difference was not significant
statistically (p = .09, Z=−1.37,Figure4b).
We should note, however, that we are not necessarily outlining a
direct cause and effect relationship here, as there are multiple layers
FIGURE 3 Proportion of participants which would either harm,
not harm, or maybe harm snakes when encountering them either
inside (orange with solid lines; n = 139) or out side (blue with dotted
lines; n = 30 0) sacred groves
0
25
50
75
100
Maybe No Ye s
Implied harm to snakes
Pe
rcent of individuals (%
)
Inside sacred grove
Outside sacred grove
    
|
 117
People and Nature
LAND RY YUAN et AL .
of social strata at play, especially for reactions to snake encounters
outside sacred groves. For instance, in this case, gender had a sig‐
nificant effect on whether participants implied harm to snakes out‐
side of sacred groves (p < .01, Table 2). More specifically, we found
harmful human–snake interactions to be mostly driven by the male
demographic, where there was a much higher tendency for males to
imply harm to snakes out side of sacred groves (31%, n = 61, p < .05,
Z=−4.64)thanfemales(7%,n = 7). This was despite there being no
difference in the proportion of females (75%, n = 65) versus males
(6 7.9 7 % , n = 121) considering actual living snakes as sacred (p = .26,
Z = 1.13), and only a slightly higher propor tion of females worship‐
ping snake deities (90%, n = 91) than males (81%, n = 161).
Although the worship of snake deities and the perception of
the sanc tity of actual living snakes may be easy to confuse from an
outsider's point of view, these were self‐evidently understood to be
distinc t by the par ticipants. Given, they are not entirely mutually
exclusive, as both were relatively more common in sacred groves
containing snake deities, where the proportion of snake deity wor‐
shipping participants was slightly larger for sacred groves with snake
deities present (88%, n = 159) than absent (78%, p < .01, Z=−2.51,
n = 93). The proportion of participants that considered snakes to
be sacred was also significantly larger for sacred groves with snake
deities (75%, n = 121 of 162) than for those without (61%, p = .01, Z
=−2.32,n = 65 of 106). However, in this case, the worshipped snake
deities are assumed to be ethereal beings transcending the physical
world, with rituals performed at temples, shrines or idols devoted to
them. Frequently mentioned snake deities said to be worshipped in‐
cluded nāgakanni and nāgakandan for the participants in the Kannur
district, nāgakanyaka for the Kasaragod district, the serpent‐gods as‐
sociated with subramanya for the Kodagu district, as well as nāga and
nāgaraja throughout all districts. Based on responses to question (e)
(iii) (Table 1), most participants explained how they would visit sites
of supplication to offer items such as milk, hen eggs, tender coco‐
nuts, eggs and snake statues made of silver or gold, as well as money.
Many participant s mentioned the rituals of nāga puja, a ceremony of
worship towards a serpent‐god, as well as abhishekam, the cleansing
of the deity at the beginning of a puja by pouring a mixture of liquids
on the idol (Malley & Barrett, 2003), and sarpa bali, a ritual involving
chants and mantras performed by priests in order to ward of f ser‐
pent‐god curses (Das & Balasubramanian, 2017).
Contrastingly, the snakes that are believed to be sacred are cor‐
poreal and thus visually tangible while inhabiting the same world as
humans. Within this context, when asked to specify which snakes
were sacred, 33% of those that perceived snakes as sacred implied
this to mean all living snakes (n = 61), whereas 14% said only snakes
FIGURE 4 Responses of participants
on whether they implied harm to snakes
when encountering them outside sacred
groves where bars represent numbers of
individuals per response type. Categories
within response types represent the
propor tion of individuals that worship
snake deities (a, where orange and blue
indicate no and yes, respectively; n = 300)
or believe actual living snakes to be sacred
(b where yellow, green and dark blue
represent no, yes, and other responses,
respectively; n = 268)
0
50
100
150
200
No Yes
Number of individuals
Worships
snake deities
No
Ye s
Maybe NoMaybe Yes
Snakes
are sacred
No
Ye s
Other
Implied harm to snakes outside sacred grove
(a) (b)
TABLE 2 Results of generalized linear model analyzing the
potential determinants of par ticipants implying harm to snakes
outside of sacred groves, which was compared against the
reference level of each factor, where a negative "Slope" indicates
the level has a lower estimate (regardless of their significance)
relative to their reference
Reference level Slope p value
Intercept n.a. −3. 59 <.0 01
Non‐devotee
visitors
Devotees 0.58 .591
Upper class Hindu Lower class Hindu −0.57 .195
Male Female 1.46 .001
Snake deit y
present
Snake deit y
absent
0.42 .284
Size of sacred
grove
n.a. 0.01 .572
Kasaragod district Kannur district 1.68 <.00 1
Kodagu district Kannur district 0.94 .059
Number of years
of association
with sacred
grove
n.a. 0.01 .145
Note: p values below the threshold of .05 are bolded to indic ate statisti‐
cal significance. "n.a." under the “Reference Level” column indicates the
variable is continuous rather than nominal.
118 
|
    
People and Nature
LAND RY YUAN et AL .
in or around sacred groves, temples, or other places of worship were
sacred (n = 26). 28% and 14% made mention of the Indian cobra, Naja
naja (n = 57), and king cobra, Ophiophagus Hannah (n = 26), respec
tively, as sacred, via identification from either the photos shown or
by local names. 17% included in their answers a type of sacred snake,
often named as sarpa, which could not be identified from photos,
but could be described as small, shining, golden or yellow coloured,
and hooded (n = 32), with occasional similarities explicitly drawn to
the Indian cobra but with the insistence that it was not the same.
Therefore, pooled together, we found nearly half (42%, n = 79) of
the participant s that believed snakes to be sacred to have described
what could be interpreted as cobras, arguably the most recognizably
venomous snakes in South Asia. In spite of the descriptive similar‐
ities though, a few of the participants gave us the impression that
sacred snakes are not dangerous to people. In effect, one par ticipant
emphasized that these “will not harm us”, whereas t wo others speci‐
fied that the sacred snake “does not bite people”. On the other hand,
two participants alluded to these snakes, despite their sanctity, not
being wanted in the vicinity, as these “listen” and “go away when
told to”.
Accordingly, herein lies another connection between actual liv
ing snakes and the worshipping of snake deities, to the effect that
the latter belief was often said to be motivated by the hopeful aver‐
sion of snake encounters. In this respect, a combined 22% of partici‐
pants gave reasons for worshipping snake deities which we classified
either as a “warning sign” (n = 43) or “negative” (n = 12) regarding
attitudes towards snakes (n = 55). All answers classified as “negative”
explicitly mentioned worshipping for the sake of gaining “protec
tion from snakes”, whereas “warning sign” reasons mostly involved
the wish to “avoid snakes”, or the prevention of serpent‐god curses
known as nāga dosha (Appendix S2). Yet the significant majorit y of
reasons given for the worship of snake deities in general, were “neu‐
tral” with respect to attitudes towards snakes, with simple answers
including “belief”, “tradition”, “for prosperity”, “for blessings” and
“out of devotion” (74%, p < .01,
𝜒2
1
= 56.29, n = 183, Appendix S2).
On the other hand, a minority of participants said they worshipped
snake deities for reasons suggesting “positive” attitudes towards
snakes (2%, n = 5), with the occasional reference towards the sanc‐
tity of actual living snakes, such as “snakes are sacred” or “snakes
are gods”. In one response exuding profoundness, one participant
said they worshipped snake deities to attain the “mindfulness not
to harm snakes”; a reflection of the role this belief is playing on the
pacifism towards snakes overall.
4 | DISCUSSION
As important predators of early anthropoids, snakes have influenced
contemporary human psychology across evolutionary time (Isbell,
2006). Although it is not certain to what ex tent stigmas and fears
about snakes are innate, learned, or a combinatory result of both,
we know that humans are predisposed to the recognition of snakes
relative to other stimuli (LoBue & DeLoache, 2008; Rakison, 2018;
Thrasher & LoBue, 2016). In South India, natural and societal envi‐
ronmental factors have been conducive to an exceptionally high rate
of human mor tality by snake envenomation (Mohapatra et al., 2011;
Warrell, 2010), consequently inspiring the reverence of serpent‐
gods in ancient folk beliefs still in existence today (Allocco, 2013).
Nearly one century ago, Vogel supposed that “Indian ophiolatry had
its first cause in the dread inspired by the poisonous reptiles”, and
that for those communities, serpent‐gods “possessed no doubt as
much reality, as the creeping things of the ear th which constantly
endangered their lives” (1995[1926]: 7). This suggestion may not be
entirely outdated, since the fear of encountering or being harmed
by actual living snakes as the principal motive behind the worship‐
ping of snake deities is something we have found to be prevalent
amongst a portion of the par ticipants. Yet there is of course a rich
mix of motives for this devotion beyond fear or dread. For example,
and for the most part, participants tended not to include allusions to
snakes in their reasons for worshipping snake deities. Rather, their
primar y motives where a combination of the continuation of an age‐
old custom which they have been raised with, and a genuine sense
of reverence for serpent‐gods, which agrees with Fergusson's opti
mistic view on the matter of snake deity worship not only in India,
but across the ancient world, as far as he perceived it in the mid‐19th
century. He thought that “if fear were the…principal characteristic
of Serpent Worship, it might be sufficient, in order to account for
its prevalence, to say, that like causes produce like effects all the
world over” and that rather, “love and admiration, more than fear
or dread, seem to be the main features of this faith” (200 4[1847]:
3). Indeed, the tolerance and fondness expressed towards actual
living snakes by the sur veyed par ticipants reflect this idea. But of
course, in this case, the relationship between humans and snakes is
a complicated one. For example, venomous snakes featured heavily
in responses to our sur veys despite there being no questions making
such a distinction. While there inevit ably are day‐to‐day concerns
about the dangers of snakebite, there is still an eagerness to co‐exist
with snakes. A relevant example of this is the specific reverence for
cobras which frequently came up in our sur veys. Although they are
usually recognized as being dangerous, the beliefs about cobras are
most likely inspired by the common depictions of serpent‐gods in the
forms of hooded snakes in Hindu ar t, such as the snake coiled around
Lord Shiva's neck, or Ananta, a multiple‐headed serpent serving as
the bed of Vishnu. Yet, these representations typically include ele‐
ments unique to deities, such as a multiple yet uneven number of
heads (Vogel, 1995[1926]). Whether in the forms of statues, carv‐
ings or paintings, portrayals of snake deities were ubiquitous in most
temples, villages and even cities of the regions we visited. Even in
Bangalore, known as the “Silicon Valley of India” for its emergence
in software technologies on the global front (Parthasarathy, 2004),
it is easy to notice the presence of idols dedicated to snake deities
throughout the cit y despite its ongoing “modernization”.
While there is evidence for the existence of serpent worship
throughout the ancient world ( Wake, 1873), very few instances of
it lasting through to contemporary society remain as it has in the
Western Ghats region, where this belief translates into the pacifist
    
|
 119
People and Nature
LAND RY YUAN et AL .
views towards snakes por trayed in this study. Yet we should note
that societal roles are at play as well, especially that of gender, where
males were more predisposed to harm snakes than females. This
corroborates Allocco's obser vations in the state of Tamil Nadu that
“men are less capable of recognizing a snake as the goddess and thus
more likely to harm or kill it” (2013:245). Nonetheless, the tendency
to allow snakes to live is seemingly the underlying factor conducive
to a society in which humans and snakes can co‐exist with minimal
detriment to the latter.
This overall societal taboo against the harming o r killing of snakes
of course benefit s conservation ef forts on loc al snake species (Ba ker,
Tanimola, & Olubode, 2018; Saraswat, Sinha, & Radhakrishna, 2015).
Yet the importance of sacred groves in promoting pacifistic interac‐
tions with snakes is integral to this concept, since our results show
that harm to snakes by people is exceptionally rare in these forests.
While the idea of sacred groves as havens for the conservation of na‐
tive species is often noted (Chandrashekara & Sankar, 1998; Ormsby
& Bhagwat, 2010; Verschuuren et al., 2010), some would say that the
traditions surrounding them are completely misaligned in respects
to their natural preservation which would have occurred as an unin‐
tentional side effect. Freeman argues that “there is little correlation
between the concerns and depictions of the modern environmental
ist's models, and the actual local reasons for instituting and maintain‐
ing sacred groves” (1999:262). Tomalin further adds that the “belief
that religious prac tices have preserved sacred groves intact” blinds
people from realising “the ex tent of their decline” (2004:279). With
modern conservation strategies hailing from the Western dichoto‐
mous view pitting pristine wilderness versus urban society, careful
attention should be paid as to not instrumentalize indigenous taboos
in a near colonial, top‐down manner.
Sacred groves in Southwestern India are not always pristine pri‐
mary forest and many are rapidly degrading and shrinking, often to
the point where they are reduced to just the central sacred com‐
pound cont aining the deities (Murugan, 2008). To this point, in a
conversation we had with a Brahmin priest not as part of the sur‐
vey, he explained how the occasional land owner would hire tantric
priest s to conduct a ceremony to lift up and displace the local snake
deity of their sacred grove to another location so as to justify the
implementation of plantations or other development in the area.
Despite this, the influence of the reverence shown towards snake
deities permeates across all sacred groves, since we found the par‐
ticipant s’ general attitudes and pacifistic behavior towards snakes
to be largely independent of the presence of a snake deity inside a
given sacred grove. Perhaps more importantly is that although local
beliefs may not preserve nature under the modern conservation
model, sacred groves in the Western Ghats permit the persistence
of the snakes inhabiting them.
While the detrimental exploitation of indigenous values for the
advancement of conservation should be avoided, so should the
overly cynical generalizations about the ecological awareness of
these societies. Although there is a rich histor y of natural preser‐
vation with religion and spirituality globally (Lowman & Sinu, 2018),
wildlife awareness by local inhabitant s is often underestimated by
researchers (Ulicsni et al., 2018). Their perceptions on conservation
as it pertains to their daily lives, and especially their relationships
with local fauna, form a key component for the improvement of im‐
plemented strategies (Bennett, 2016). In our surveys, although few
in number, some participants did mention the ecological importance
of snakes for sacred groves. We also encountered a case of societal
ecological awarness at one of the sacred groves we visited in the
Kasaragod district, where the inhabitants of the areas surrounding
it had convinced the local council to construct a fence demarcat‐
ing the forest to raise it up a certain height from the ground so as
to allow the passage of animals. We should thus reconcile the goal
of preser ving biodiversit y together with the respect towards and
welfare of local communities. This is especially relevant during the
laying out of urban infrastruc ture, where the continued, bottom‐up
valuation of a co‐existence between humans and other non‐human
animals relies on a shif t in the mentality of authorities (Narayanan &
Bindumadhav, 2018).
Given the nature of the information we collected with the sur‐
veys, we should note that there are limitations to our interpretations
regarding attitudes towards snakes. Chiefly is the assumption that
participants which did not imply harm to snakes when encountering
them actually respond in this way in reality. Together with the per‐
ceptions we measured, we recognize that these do not necessarily
equate to actual attitudes in a 1:1 manner. But all in all, it is appar‐
ent that there is a social taboo against harming snakes inside sacred
groves and that this could be pushed forward as leverage for the in‐
tegrative conservation of both snakes and sacred groves in the face
of encroachment by development and plantations (Bhagwat & Rutte,
2006; Khan et al., 2008; Ormsby & Bhag wat, 2010). Documented
cases where traditional taboos in sacred sites have successfully led
to the protection of species otherwise considered as dangerous or
pests include the Sclater's monkey (Cercopithecus sclateri) in Nigeria
(Baker et al., 2018), rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) in India
(Saraswat et al., 2015), as well as the snow leopard (Panthera uncia) in
China (Li et al., 2014). This is especially important for snake species
which are currently at risk either in the Western Ghats or in other
parts of their distribution due to the direct harvest of individuals for
international trade. Notably, the King cobra is listed as vulnerable
with a decreasing population trend by the IUCN Red list (Stuart et al.,
2012). It is also included in the CITES Appendix II list together with
the Indian cobra, Indian rat snake (Ptyas mucosus), Russell's viper
(Daboia russelii) and the checkered keelback (Xenochrophis piscator),
while the Indian rock python (Python molurus) is listed under CITES
Appendix I.
However, despite generating potential conservation benefits for
native snake species, their co‐existence with people cannot be thor‐
oughly discussed without also prioritizing the protection of human
lives. As in many other tropical regions of the world, the danger of
venomous snakes in Southwestern India is a preeminent day‐to‐day
concern over other threats to safety. Yet, despite this and the fact
that deaths by snake envenomation make up a significant propor‐
tion of all injury deaths across India (Mohapatra et al., 2011), snake‐
bite remains a globally neglected tropical disease (Warrell, 2010).
120 
|
    
People and Nature
LAND RY YUAN et AL .
Therefore, moving forward, we wish to highlight the role of sacred
groves in promoting non‐harmful human–snake relations for the
conservation of both snakes and sacred groves, all while stressing
the importance of the beliefs, perspectives and safety of local com‐
munities in their co‐existence with snakes.
5 | CONCLUSIONS
The co‐existence of humans and snakes in the Western Ghats in
this case appears to be largely supported by the devotional beliefs
of local communities in regards to snake deities as well as sacred
groves. This has proved to be mostly independent of the presence
or absence of snake deities in the specific sacred groves we visited,
and is thus not restricted as a localized perspective. Rather, by tran‐
scending the scale of individual sacred groves, the tolerance and
pacifistic behaviour towards snakes across our sites demonstrate
how deeply traditions related to serpent‐gods permeate across com
munities in Southwestern India.
Although this taboo against harming snakes has been discussed in
the past literature (Allocco, 2013; Narayanan & Bindumadhav, 2018),
this is the first study to exhibit the intensification of it within the con
fines of sacred groves. Accordingly, this taboo against harming snakes
inside sacred groves would be a key component to be integrated as
a biocultural strategy (Gavin et al., 2015) for the future preser vation
of not only snakes which could be at risk locally or in other par ts of
their range, but also of other faunal and floral species relying on the
persistence of sacred groves (Bhagwat & Rut te, 2006; Chandrashekara
& Sankar, 1998; Ray et al., 2014). Importantly, this approach also in
cludes the human dimension, where the cultures and traditions from
which these beliefs originate are simultaneously protected (Pardo‐de‐
Santayana & Macía, 2015). While the risks to safety imposed by snakes
to people are real and should be critically dealt with, the acknowledge
ment of the valuation of snakes as their own social entity by local com
munities during future development could prove to be instrumental in
bolstering the advocacy of biodiverse environments in Southwestern
India.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Surveys were conducted with approval by The University of Hong
Kong's Human Research Ethics Committee (E A1806029). Funding
was provided by the Faith and Science Collaborative Research
Forum at The Universit y of Hong Kong and the Kerala State Council
for Science, Technology & Environment (KSCSTE). The study
was also supported by the Young Scientist grant awarded to PAS
by the Science Engineering Research Board, New Delhi (SB/FT/
LS‐325/2012), and the Malabar Awareness and Rescue Center for
Wildlife, Kannur.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
AUTHORS' CONTRIBUTIONS
We wish to thank the reviewers and editors for their helpful com‐
ments during the revising of this manuscript. All authors contributed
in conceiving the ideas and designing the methodology; F.L.Y., U.P.B.
and R.R. collected the data; F.L.Y. and P.A. S. analysed the data; F.L.Y.
led the writing of the manuscript. All authors contributed critically to
the drafts and gave final approval for publication.
DATA AVAIL ABI LIT Y S TATEM ENT
The survey data we have collected for this study are available from
the Dryad Digital Repository: https ://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.1rn8p
k0pd (L andry Yuan et al., 2019). The distribution of these data is in
line with the participants and protects their privacy.
ORCID
Félix Landry Yuan https://orcid.org/0000‐0001‐5310‐9032
REFERENCES
Aerts, R., Van Overtveld, K., November, E., Wassie, A., Abiyu, A.,
Demissew, S., … Teketay, D. (2016). Conser vation of the Ethiopian
church forests: Threats, opportunities and implications for their
management. Science of the Total Environment, 5 51, 4 04–414 .
Allocco, A. L. (2013). Fear, reverence and ambivalence: Divine snakes in
contemporary south India. Religions of South Asia, 7, 230 –248 .
Baker, L. R., Tanimola, A. A., & Olubode, O. S. (2018). Complexities of local
cultural protection in conservation: The case of an Endangered African
primate and forest groves protected by social taboos. Oryx, 52, 262–
270. https ://doi.org/10.1017/S0030 60531 7001223
Ballullaya, U. P., Reshmi, K. S., Rajesha, T. P., Manoj, K., Lowman, M., &
Sinu, P. A. (2019). Stakeholder motivation for the conservation of sa‐
cred groves in south India: An analysis of environmental perceptions
of rural and urban neighbourhood communities. Land Use Policy, 89.
https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.landu sepol.2019.104213
Bennet t, N. J. (2016). Using perceptions as evidence to improve conser‐
vation and environmental management. Conservation Biology, 30,
582–592. https ://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.12681
Bhagwat, S. A ., Kushalappa, C. G., Williams, P. H., & Brown , N. D. (2005).
A landscape approach to biodiversity conservation of sacred groves
in the Western Ghats of India. Conser vation Biology, 19, 1853–1862.
https ://doi.org/10.1111/j.1523‐1739.2005.00248.x
Bhagwat, S. A ., Nogué, S., & Willis, K. J. (2014). Cultural drivers of refor‐
estat ion in tropical f orest groves of th e Western Ghats of I ndia. Forest
Ecology and Management, 329, 393–400. https ://doi.org /10.1016/j.
foreco.2013.11.017
Bhagwat, S. A ., & Rutte, C. (2006). Sacre d groves: Potential for biodi‐
versity management. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment, 4,
519–524. https ://doi.org/10.1890/1540‐9295(2006)4[519:SGPFB
M]2.0.CO;2
Chandrashekara, U. M., Joseph, S. P., & Sreejith, K . A. (20 02). Ecological
and socio‐cultural dimensions of sacred groves of northern Kerala.
Man in India, 82, 323–340.
Chandrashekara, U. M., & Sankar, S. (1998). Ecolog y and management of
sacred groves in Kerala, India. Forest Ecology and Management, 112,
165–177. https ://doi.org/10.1016/S0378‐1127(98)00326‐0
Das, D., & Balasubramanian, A . (2017). The practice of traditional ritu‐
als in Naga Aradhana (Snake worship): A case study on Aadimoolam
    
|
 121
People and Nature
LAND RY YUAN et AL .
Vetticode Sree Nagarajaswami Temple in Kerala, India. In SHS web of
conferences (Vo l. 33). EDP Sciences.
Decher, J. (1997). Conservation, small mammals, and the future of sacred
groves in West Africa. Biodiversity & Conservation, 6, 1 0 07.
Dudley, N., Higgins‐Zogib, L ., & Mansourian, S. (2009). The link s between
protected areas, faiths, and sacred natural sites. Conser vation Biology,
23, 568–577.
Erlingsson, C., & Brysiewicz, P. (2017). A hands‐on guid e to doing content
analysis. African Journal of Emergency Medicine, 7, 93–99. https ://doi.
org/10.1016/j.afjem.2017.08.001
Fergusson, J. (20 04[1847]). Tree and serpent worship: or, Illustrations of
mythology an d art in India in the first and four th centuries after Christ.
Delhi, India: Asian Educational Services.
Freeman, J. R. (1999). Gods, groves and the culture of nature in Ker ala.
Modern A sian Studies, 33, 2 57–3 02.
Gadgil, M., & Vartak, V. D. (1976). The sacred groves of Western Ghats in
India. Econ omic Botany, 30, 152–160. ht tps ://doi.org/10.1007/BF028
62961
Gavin, M. C., McC arter, J., Mead, A., Berkes, F., Stepp, J. R., Peterson , D.,
& Tang, R. (2015). Defining biocultural approaches to conser vation.
Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 30, 140–145. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.
tree.2014.12.005
Hansen, M. C., Pot apov, P. V., Moore, R., Hancher, M., Turubanova, S. A.,
Tyukavina, A ., … Townshend, J. R. G. (2013). High‐resolution global
maps of 21st‐century forest cover change. Science, 342, 850–853.
https ://doi.org/10.1126/scien ce.1244693. Retrieved from http://
earth engin epart ners.appsp ot.com/scien ce‐2013‐global‐forest
Härtel,H.(1976).Aspects ofearlynāgacult inIndia.Journal of the Royal
Society of Arts, 1 24, 663–683.
Hsieh, H. F., & Shannon, S. E. (2005). Three approaches to qualitative
content analysis. Qualitative Health Research, 15, 1277–1288. https ://
doi .or g/10 .1177/10497 323 05 276687
Isbell, L. A. (2006). Snakes as agent s of evolutionary change in pri‐
mate brains. Journ al of Human Evolution, 51, 1–35. https ://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.12.012
Khan, M. L., Ashalata, D. K., & Tripathi, R. S. (2008). The sacred groves and
their significance in conserving biodiversity: An overview. International
Journal of Ecology and Environmental Sciences, 34, 277–291.
Kideghesho, J. R. (2008). Co‐existence between the traditional societies
and wildlife in western Serengeti, Tanzania: Its relevancy in contem‐
porary wildlife conservation efforts. Biodiversity and Conservation, 17,
1861–1881.
Kushalappa, C. G ., Bhag wat, S. A ., & Kushalappa, K. A. (2001).
Conser vation and management of sacred groves of Kodagu,
Karnat aka, South India, a unique approach. In K. N. Ganeshaiah,
R. U. Shaanker, & K. S. Bawa (Eds.), Tropical Ecosystems: Structure,
Diversity and Human Welfare (pp. 565–569). New Delhi: Oxford and
IBH Publishing.
Landr y Yuan, F. L., Ballullaya, U. P., Roshnath, R., Bonebr ake, T. C., &
Sinu, P. A. (2019). Data from: Sacred groves and serpent‐gods mod‐
erate human–snake relations. Dryad Digital Repository, h t t p s ://d o i.
org/10.5061/dryad.1rn8p k0pd
Li, J., Wang, D., Yin, H., Zhaxi, D., Jiagong, Z., Schaller, G. B., … Lu, Z.
(2014). Role of Tibetan Buddhist monasteries in snow leopard con‐
servation. Conser vation Biology, 28 , 87–94. h tt ps ://doi. org/10.1111/
cobi .12135
LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Detecting the snake in the grass: Attention
to fear‐relevant stimuli by adults and young children. Psychological Science,
19, 284–289. https ://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‐9280.2008.02081.x
Lowman, M . L., & Sinu, P. A. (2018). Can the spiritual values of forests
inspire effective conservation? BioScience, 68, 688–690. https ://doi.
org/10.1093/biosc i/bix057
Malley, B., & Barrett , J. (2003). Can ritual form be predic ted from reli‐
gious belief? A tes t of the Lawson‐McCauley hypotheses. Journal of
Ritual Studies, 17, 1–14 .
Mcleod, E., & Palmer, M. (2015). Why conservation needs religion.
Coastal Management, 43, 238–252. https ://doi.org/10.1080/08920
753. 2015 .1030 297
McMillan, S. E., Wong , T. C., Hau, B. C. H., & Bonebrake, T. C. (2019). Fish
farmers highlight opportunities and warnings for urban carnivore
conservation. Conservation Science and Practice, 1, e79. https ://doi.
org /10.1111/csp2.7 9
Mgumia, F. H., & Oba, G. (20 03). Potential role of sacred groves in bio‐
diversity conservation in Tanzania. Environmental Conservation, 30,
259–265. https ://doi.org/10.1017/S0376 89290 3000250
Mohapatra, B., Warrell, D. A ., Suraweera, W., Bhatia, P., Dhingra, N.,
Jotkar, R. M., … Jha, P.; and Million Death Study Collaborators.
(2011). Snakebite mortality in India: A nationally representative mor‐
tality survey. PLoS Neglected Tropical Diseases, 5, e1018. http s ://doi.
org/10.1371/journ al.pntd.0001018
Moon, K ., Brewer, T. D., Januchowski‐Hartley, S. R ., Adams, V. M., &
Blackman, D. A. (2016). A guideline to improve qualitative social sci‐
ence publishing in ecology and conser vation journals. Ecology and
Society, 21(3), 17. https ://doi.org/10.5751/ES‐08663‐210317
Murugan, K., Ramachandran, V. S., Swarupanandan, K ., & Remesh, M.
(2008). Socio‐cultural perspective s to the sacred groves and serpen‐
tine worship in Palakkad district, Kerala. Indian Journal of Traditional
Knowledge, 7, 455–462.
Myers, N., Mittermeier, R. A ., Mittermeier, C. G., Da Fonseca, G . A., &
Kent, J. (20 00). Biodiver sity hotspots for conservation priorities.
Nature, 403, 853. https ://doi.org/10.1038/35002501
Nair, G. R. (2017). Snake worship in India. India: Publications Division
Ministry of Information & Broadcasting.
Narayanan, Y., & Bindumadhav, S. (2018). ‘Posthuman cosmopolitan
ism’ for the Anthropocene in India: Urbanism and human‐snake re
lations in the Kali Yuga . Geoforum, https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.geofo
rum.2018.04.020
Ormsby, A. A. (2011). The impact s of global and national policy on the
management and conservation of sacred groves of India. Human
Ecology, 39, 783–793. htt ps ://doi.org /10.1007/s10745‐011‐9441‐8
Ormsby, A. A., & Bhagwat, S . A. (2010). Sacred forests of India: A
strong tradition of community‐based natural resource management.
Environmental Conservation, 37, 320–326. https ://doi.org /10.1017/
S0376 89291 0000561
Palot, M . J. (2015). A checklist of reptiles of Kerala, India. Jou rnal of
Threatened Taxa, 13, 8010–8022.
Pardo‐de‐Santayana, M., & Macía, M. J. (2015). Biodiversit y: The
benefits of traditional knowledge. Nature, 518, 487. https ://doi.
org /10.103 8/518487a
Parthasarathy, B. (2004). India's Silicon Valley or Silicon Valley's India?
Socially embedding the computer sof tware industry in Bangalore.
International Journal of Urban and Regional Research, 28, 664–685.
https ://doi.org/10.1111/j.0309‐1317.2004.00542.x
Rajendraprasad, M. (1995). The floristic, structural and functional analy-
sis of sacred groves of Kerala. Ph.D. Thesis. T hiruvanthapuram, India:
University of Kerala.
Rakison, D. H. (2018). Do 5‐month‐old infants possess an evolve d detec‐
tion mechanism for snakes, sharks, and rats? Journal of Cognition and
Development, 19, 456 –476 .
Ray, R., Chan dran, M . D. S., & Ramachandr a, T. V. (2014). Biodiversit y
and ecological assessments of Indian sacred groves. Journal of
Forestry Research, 25, 21–28. htt ps ://doi.org/10.10 07/s11676‐014‐
04 29‐2
R Core Team. (2017). R : A language and env iron ment for stat istical comput-
ing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ht tps.
www.R‐proje ct.org.
Salick, J., Amend, A., Anderson, D., Hoffmeister, K., Gunn, B., &
Zhendong, F. (2007). Tibetan s acred sites conser ve old growth tre es
and cover in the eastern Himalayas. Biodiversity and Conservation, 16,
693–706. https ://doi.org/10 .1007/s10531‐005 ‐4381‐5
122 
|
    
People and Nature
LAND RY YUAN et AL .
Saraswat, R., Sinha, A ., & Radhakrishna, S. (2015). A god becomes a pest?
Human‐rhesus macaque interactions in Himachal Pradesh, northern
India. European Journal of Wildlife Research, 61, 435–443.
Sarfo‐Mensah, P., Oduro, W., Antoh Fredua, E., & A misah, S. (2010).
Traditional representations of the natural environment and biodi
versity conser vation: Sacred groves in Ghana. Fondazione Eni Enrico
Mattei, Noto di Lavoro 8 7.
Sathish, S. (2008). Snakes of Coorg: A field guide to the snakes of Kodag u.
Madikeri: Coorg Wildlife Society.
Shen, X ., Lu, Z., Li, S., & Chen, N. (2012). Tibetan sacred sites:
Understanding the traditional management system and its role in
modern conservation. Ecology and Society, 17(2), 13. https ://doi.
org /10. 5751/E S‐ 04785‐170213
Stuart,B.,Wogan,G.,Grismer,L.,Auliya,M.,Inger,R.F.,Lilley,R.,…Jelić,D.
(2012). Ophiophagus hannah. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species
2012: e.T177540A1491874. 2019, https ://doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.
UK.2012‐1.RLTS.T1775 40A14 91874.en. Downloaded on 24 June.
Teketay, D., Lemenih, M., Bekele, T., Yemshaw, Y., Feleke, S., Tadesse, W.,
… Nigussie, D. (2010). Fores t resources and challenges of sustainable
forest management and conservation in Ethiopia. In F. Bongers & T.
Tennigkeit (Eds.), Degraded forest s in Eastern Africa: Management and
restoration (pp. 19–63). London, UK: Earthscan.
Thrasher, C., & LoBue, V. (2016). Do infants find snakes aver‐
sive? Infant s’ physiological responses to “fear‐relevant” stimuli.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 14, 382–390. https ://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.013
Tomalin, E. (20 04). Bio‐divinity and biodiversity: Perspec tives on religion
and environmental conservation in India. Numen, 51, 265–295. https
://doi.org /10.116 3/156 85 27041 945 481
Ulicsni,V.,Babai,D.,Vadász,C.,Vadász‐Besnyői,V.,Báldi,A.,&Molnár,
Z. (2018). Bridging conservation science and traditional knowledge
of wild animals: The need for expert guidance and inclusion of
local knowledge holders. Ambio, 1–10. https ://doi.org/10 .1007/
s13280‐018‐1106‐z
Verschuuren, B., Wild, R., McNeely, J., & Oviedo, G . (2010). Sacred natu-
ral sites: Conserving nature and culture. London, UK: Earthscan.
Vogel, J. P. (1995[1926]). Indian serpent‐lore: or, The Nāgas in Hindu legend
and art. New Delhi: Asian Educational Services.
Wake, C. S. (1873). The origin of serpent‐worship. The Journal of the
A nt hr o p ol o gi ca l I n st i tu te o f G r e a t B r i ta in a nd I r e l a n d, 2, 373–390. ht tps ://
doi.org/10.2307/2841458
Warrell, D. A . (2010). Snake bite. The Lancet, 375, 77–88. https ://doi.
org /10.1016/S 0140 ‐6736(0 9)61754‐2
Waylen, K. A., Fischer, A., Mcgowan, P. J., Thirgood, S. J., & Milner‐
Gulland, E. J. (2010). Ef fect of local cultural context on the success of
community‐based conservation interventions. Conservation Biology,
24, 1119–1129. https ://doi.org/10.1111/j.1523‐1739.2010.0144 6. x
SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Additional suppor ting information may be found online in the
Supporting Information section.
How to cite this article: Landr y Yuan F, Ballullaya UP,
Roshnath R , Bonebrake TC, Sinu PA. Sacred groves and
serpent‐gods moderate human–snake relations. People Nat.
2020;2:111–122. https ://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.10059
... However, snakes also play an important role in their ecosystem as links in the food chain, bio-monitors in the control of rodents, and excellent ecological indicators due to their high degree of sensitivity to equalize a slight change in the environment (Yadav et al., 2014). Snakes are used by local communities for cultural, spiritual, mythical, food, and economic purposes (Landry Yuan et al., 2020;Urban, 2018;van Vliet et al., 2015;Toudonou, 2003) and they include nonvenomous species like the Ball python (Python regius). P. regius is worshipped (Toudonou, 2007) and popular exotic pet Bush et al., 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
The ball python or royal python (Python regius) is a widespread snake in western Africa and the most exported live CITES-listed species from Africa (Benin, Ghana, and Togo) through ranching and informal networks. The research aimed to map the current distribution and future distribution of the ball python (Python regius) in Benin and to assess threats to the ball python in Benin. The spatial distribution of the ball python was modeled considering 20 environmental variables in MaxEnt (Maximum Entropy) software 3.3.3. Occurrence data from surveys combined with bioclimatic data derived from the Africlim database were used. 80 % over 97 total quadrats were surveyed to get the perception of ball python (P. regius) threats of 393 hunters randomly selected. Under current conditions, southern Benin is very favourable to the species. By 2050, almost all Benin would be favourable to the species under both 4.5 and 8.5 scenarios. The effects of the different threats differ from one region to another (North, Centre, and South). We suggest two research avenues: a) study the formal and the informal commercialization networks of Python regius in Benin and its impacts in the ball python conservation, and b) Modelling the future distribution of P. regius using four other models.
... of adaptive management, and has been transferred and put into practice across generations. An example of its manifestation is the creation and management of sacred groves by communities across the world (Govigli et al., 2015;Sheridan, 2009;Yuan et al., 2020). In the field of natural resource management there is evidence to show that local knowledge and skills can be very efficient and cost effective (Gadgil et al., 1993;Hartwig et al., 2009;Niamir-Fuller et al., 2012). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Impact evaluations assess the causal link between an action (e.g. erecting a fence) and the outcomes (e.g. a change in the rate of crop raiding by elephants). This goes beyond understanding whether a project has been implemented (e.g. whether activities were completed) to understanding what changes happened due to the actions taken and why they happened as they did. Impact evaluation is thus defined as the systematic process of assessing the effects of an intervention (e.g. project or policy) by comparing what actually happened with what would have happened without it (i.e. the counterfactual)
... of adaptive management, and has been transferred and put into practice across generations. An example of its manifestation is the creation and management of sacred groves by communities across the world (Govigli et al., 2015;Sheridan, 2009;Yuan et al., 2020). In the field of natural resource management there is evidence to show that local knowledge and skills can be very efficient and cost effective (Gadgil et al., 1993;Hartwig et al., 2009;Niamir-Fuller et al., 2012). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Historically, conservationists have focused on financial and technical solutions to human-wildlife conflicts (Redpath et al., 2013). It has become clear that although these are important to generate a context where change is possible, more attention to human behaviour is needed to achieve longer-term human-wildlife coexistence (Veríssimo & Campbell, 2015). Interventions targeting human behaviour have been largely focused on measures such as regulation and education. Regulation in this context refers to the system of rules made by a government or other authority, usually backed by penalties and enforcement mechanisms, which describes the way people should behave, while education is concerned with the provision of information about a topic. However, the degree of influence of these interventions depends on the priority audience being motivated (i.e. the individual believes change is in their best interest) and/or able to change (i.e. overcome social pressure, inertia and social norms) (Figure 21) (Smith et al., 2020b).
... of adaptive management, and has been transferred and put into practice across generations. An example of its manifestation is the creation and management of sacred groves by communities across the world (Govigli et al., 2015;Sheridan, 2009;Yuan et al., 2020). In the field of natural resource management there is evidence to show that local knowledge and skills can be very efficient and cost effective (Gadgil et al., 1993;Hartwig et al., 2009;Niamir-Fuller et al., 2012). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
The human dimension aspects of conflicts over wildlife are largely determined by the thoughts, feelings and, ultimately, behaviours of people. Because all human-wildlife conflicts involve people, approaches that provide a better understanding of human behaviour – and facilitate behaviour change – are crucially important for helping manage such conflicts. Efforts to mitigate human-wildlife conflict commonly include actions to try to influence or change the attitudes or behaviours of the people involved. Another extremely common approach for reducing human-wildlife conflict is to conduct education and awareness campaigns. These activities are well intentioned in attempting to change the human dimension of the human-wildlife conflict, but unfortunately are often ineffective for one very common reason – they are based on incorrect assumptions about cause-and-effect relationships of concepts within social psychology.
... of adaptive management, and has been transferred and put into practice across generations. An example of its manifestation is the creation and management of sacred groves by communities across the world (Govigli et al., 2015;Sheridan, 2009;Yuan et al., 2020). In the field of natural resource management there is evidence to show that local knowledge and skills can be very efficient and cost effective (Gadgil et al., 1993;Hartwig et al., 2009;Niamir-Fuller et al., 2012). ...
... of adaptive management, and has been transferred and put into practice across generations. An example of its manifestation is the creation and management of sacred groves by communities across the world (Govigli et al., 2015;Sheridan, 2009;Yuan et al., 2020). In the field of natural resource management there is evidence to show that local knowledge and skills can be very efficient and cost effective (Gadgil et al., 1993;Hartwig et al., 2009;Niamir-Fuller et al., 2012). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
The potential success of wildlife damage prevention measures can be significantly increased by taking the natural behaviour of animals into account, identifying ways in which some species have already adapted to the presence of humans and applying this knowledge elsewhere. It is also important to understand how individual differences in behaviour (animal and human personality) can vary the perception, presence and intensity of conflict from one landscape or conflict location to the next. The chapter includes sections on: Animal decision making - negative impacts on human-dominated landscapes and ‘problem’ animals; key behavioural considerations; HWC scenarios linked to animal behaviour; and concludes with a step-by-step guide to considering animal behaviour in human-wildlife conflict mitigation strategy development.
... of adaptive management, and has been transferred and put into practice across generations. An example of its manifestation is the creation and management of sacred groves by communities across the world (Govigli et al., 2015;Sheridan, 2009;Yuan et al., 2020). In the field of natural resource management there is evidence to show that local knowledge and skills can be very efficient and cost effective (Gadgil et al., 1993;Hartwig et al., 2009;Niamir-Fuller et al., 2012). ...
Chapter
An overview of the IUCN SSC guidelines on human-wildlife conflict and coexistence (First Ed.), covering the global scale of the challenge, thoughts on defining HWC and Coexistence, and some essential considerations for management.
... of adaptive management, and has been transferred and put into practice across generations. An example of its manifestation is the creation and management of sacred groves by communities across the world (Govigli et al., 2015;Sheridan, 2009;Yuan et al., 2020). In the field of natural resource management there is evidence to show that local knowledge and skills can be very efficient and cost effective (Gadgil et al., 1993;Hartwig et al., 2009;Niamir-Fuller et al., 2012). ...
Chapter
Engaging with the social, psychological, economic and political dimensions of wildlife management and conservation is essential for robust and effective actions and policies regarding human-wildlife conflicts. Specifically, in the context of human-wildlife conflicts, understanding different interest groups’ perspectives and their different value systems, beliefs, priorities and agendas is necessary to find out how to address challenges for improved actions for people and wildlife. The chapter focuses on the basics of social science and desigining social science research, with a section on ethics, and two case studies.
... of adaptive management, and has been transferred and put into practice across generations. An example of its manifestation is the creation and management of sacred groves by communities across the world (Govigli et al., 2015;Sheridan, 2009;Yuan et al., 2020). In the field of natural resource management there is evidence to show that local knowledge and skills can be very efficient and cost effective (Gadgil et al., 1993;Hartwig et al., 2009;Niamir-Fuller et al., 2012). ...
Chapter
Culture influences how people respond to or interact with wildlife, and how they respond to and manage conflicts. Culture is a set of principles, habits and symbols that are learnt and shared; it unites groups of people and influences their worldview and behaviour. Culture is also symbolic, whereby people have a shared understanding of symbolic meaning within their group or society. Culture may differ markedly within nations, regions and even local communities and can change over time. As outlined in Chapter 10 (How histories shape interactions), local cultures and environmental relationships are not static and do not exist in isolation; they are influenced by local and global developments, past and present, and this needs to be taken into consideration when examining or working with human-wildlife conflict.
Article
Full-text available
Squamates include more than 11,000 extant species of lizards, snakes, and amphisbaenians, and display a dazzling diversity of phenotypes across their over 200-million-year evolutionary history on Earth. Here, we introduce and define squamates (Order Squamata) and review the history and promise of genomic investigations into the patterns and processes governing squamate evolution, given recent technological advances in DNA sequencing, genome assembly, and evolutionary analysis. We survey the most recently available whole genome assemblies for squamates, including the taxonomic distribution of available squamate genomes, and assess their quality metrics and usefulness for research. We then focus on disagreements in squamate phylogenetic inference, how methods of high-throughput phylogenomics affect these inferences, and demonstrate the promise of whole genomes to settle or sustain persistent phylogenetic arguments for squamates. We review the role transposable elements play in vertebrate evolution, methods of transposable element annotation and analysis, and further demonstrate that through the understanding of the diversity, abundance, and activity of transposable elements in squamate genomes, squamates can be an ideal model for the evolution of genome size and structure in vertebrates. We discuss how squamate genomes can contribute to other areas of biological research such as venom systems, studies of phenotypic evolution, and sex determination. Because they represent more than 30% of the living species of amniote, squamates deserve a genome consortium on par with recent efforts for other amniotes (i.e., mammals and birds) that aim to sequence most of the extant families in a clade.
Article
Full-text available
As urbanization globally drives mammals and carnivores into compact spaces, they will increasingly come into conflict with development and natural resource extraction pressures. The management of these populations is further complicated by difficulties in monitoring what are often rare and elusive species. We used local ecological knowledge (LEK) to collect data on the historical and current status of Eurasian otter (Lutra lutra) in Hong Kong as well as determine how local fish farmers and residents perceived management and conservation issues surrounding the species. We found evidence for small population size and decline in numbers and distribution over recent decades for L. lutra. Fish farmers had extensive and familiar experience with otters and expressed largely negative opinions about otter impacts on fish stocks but positive attitudes towards their conservation. However, if otters were to have real or perceived effects on livelihood, then opinions about their conservation were mixed and cautious. In the context of the Pearl River Delta megacity, biodiversity is under high threat from development and urbanization. We here show the value of LEK and human dimensions of conservation in balancing the complex challenges of managing land for both local livelihoods and environmental stewardship.
Article
Full-text available
Many people call for strengthening knowledge co-production between academic science and indigenous and local knowledge systems. A major barrier to cooperation seems to be a lack of experience regarding where and how traditional knowledge can be found and obtained. Our key question was whether the expert judgment of academic zoologists or a feature-based linear model is better at predicting the observed level of local familiarity with wild animal species. Neither the zoologists nor the model proved sufficiently accurate (70 and 60%, respectively), with the inaccuracy probably resulting from inadequate knowledge of the local ecological and cultural specificities of the species. This indicates that more knowledge is likely to come from local knowledge than zoologists would expect. Accuracy of targeting the relevant species for knowledge co-production could be improved through specific understanding of the local culture, provided by experts who study traditional zoological knowledge and by local knowledge holders themselves.
Article
Full-text available
There is a growing recognition for the important role played by qualitative research and its usefulness in many fields, including the emergency care context in Africa. Novice qualitative researchers are often daunted by the prospect of qualitative data analysis and thus may experience much difficulty in the data analysis process. Our objective with this manuscript is to provide a practical hands-on example of qualitative content analysis to aid novice qualitative researchers in their task.
Article
The four experiments reported here used the preferential looking and habituation paradigms to examine whether 5-month-olds possess a perceptual template for snakes, sharks, and rodents. It was predicted that if infants possess such a template then they would attend preferentially to schematic images of these non-human animal stimuli relative to scrambled versions of the same stimuli. The results reveal that infants look longer at a schematic snake than at two scrambled versions of that image and generalize from real snakes to the schematic image. The experiments also demonstrate that 5-month-olds show no preferential looking for schematic sharks or schematic rodents relative to scrambled versions of those images. These data add to the growing support for the view that humans, like many non-human animals, possess an evolved fear mechanism for detecting threats that were recurrent across evolutionary time.
Article
India's rapid urbanisation and biodiversity decline together have critical global implications in the Anthropocene. However, the complex socio-religious dimensions of urban biodiversity are overlooked in current planning. This paper casts animals as vital components of urban societies in India to argue for species-inclusive zoöpolises as viable cities of the future. It proposes ‘posthuman cosmopolitanism’ as a planning ethic that extends pluralism to multispecies in the Anthropocene, cognisant of the socio-cultural and religious frames in which animals are enmeshed in India. These narratives have significant implications in the Kali Yuga or the apocalyptic cosmological epoch, which Hindus believe is currently underway. Akin to the Anthropocene, human action bears an exceptional significance in the events of the Kali Yuga, which is believed to be a precursor to human, ecological, and even planetary annihilation. The paper examines human-snake conflict, one of the most widespread human-animal encounters in Indian cities. Snakes play vital roles in urban ecologies and religio-cultural narratives in India. Simultaneously, religious and social perceptions of serpents contribute to a fear of snakes. Fundamental to snake preservation in the Indian urban Anthropocene is an expansion of diversity to ‘multinatural diversity’ and a reconfiguration of human-snake relations in socio-cultural frames.
Article
Globally, some species and habitats receive protection through local belief systems (e.g. indigenous religions) and informal institutions (e.g. social norms and taboos). Where such systems represent the only form of protection for threatened species or environments, they may be critical to the survival of those taxa and sites. We evaluated the effectiveness of long-standing social taboos protecting an endangered primate, Sclater’s monkey (Cercopithecus sclateri), and forest groves in a community complex in Nigeria. Across its range (southern Nigeria), Sclater’s monkey is effectively protected only through informal institutions. In our study site, we conducted a census of the monkey population; measured the area of sacred groves; and compared these findings with estimates from 2010 and 2005, respectively. We observed a 36% increase in the monkey population (from 249 to 339 individuals) in a core survey area. No groves that we assessed in 2005 had been fully cleared. Although we observed a decline in tree cover of several sacred forests, most groves regularly used by monkeys had changed little. While the social taboos related to monkeys and sacred groves remain largely intact, other factors threaten the monkey population and remaining forests in this community complex, including the removal of tree patches to accommodate the construction of large residential buildings and the demand for cropland, as well as increased dumping of waste in forested sites. This study highlights the conservation importance and limitations of local cultural protection, as well as the challenges presented when such protection conflicts with community-perceived development needs.