ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

In most studies on awe, the stimuli used to elicit the emotion involves nature, music, space, or grand theories – but awe elicited by the actions of other people has generally not been studied in depth. In the few cases in which the social component of awe has been acknowledged, the focus has been on charismatic leaders. The current study explores this social aspect of awe by asking whether awe can be elicited in close interpersonal relationships and how this experience may be distinct from awe elicited by other stimuli. Using both quantitative and qualitative approaches in a mixed-methods study (N = 636), we found empirical support for the notion that awe is elicited in the context of close relationships. Awe was elicited by close others compared to a neutral control, although the interpersonal form of awe was less intense than awe caused by nature. Qualitative analyses revealed that awe triggered by nature was defined by themes of beauty, while interpersonal awe was defined by themes of virtue or excellence of character.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rpos20
The Journal of Positive Psychology
Dedicated to furthering research and promoting good practice
ISSN: 1743-9760 (Print) 1743-9779 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rpos20
Interpersonal awe: Exploring the social domain of
awe elicitors
Marianna Graziosi & David Yaden
To cite this article: Marianna Graziosi & David Yaden (2019): Interpersonal awe:
Exploring the social domain of awe elicitors, The Journal of Positive Psychology, DOI:
10.1080/17439760.2019.1689422
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2019.1689422
Published online: 14 Nov 2019.
Submit your article to this journal
View related articles
View Crossmark data
Interpersonal awe: Exploring the social domain of awe elicitors
Marianna Graziosi and David Yaden
University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
ABSTRACT
In most studies on awe, the stimuli used to elicit the emotion involves nature, music, space, or
grand theories but awe elicited by the actions of other people has generally not been studied in
depth. In the few cases in which the social component of awe has been acknowledged, the focus
has been on charismatic leaders. The current study explores this social aspect of awe by asking
whether awe can be elicited in close interpersonal relationships and how this experience may be
distinct from awe elicited by other stimuli. Using both quantitative and qualitative approaches in
a mixed-methods study (N = 636), we found empirical support for the notion that awe is elicited in
the context of close relationships. Awe was elicited by close others compared to a neutral control,
although the interpersonal form of awe was less intense than awe caused by nature. Qualitative
analyses revealed that awe triggered by nature was dened by themes of beauty, while inter-
personal awe was dened by themes of virtue or excellence of character.
ARTICLE HISTORY
Received 7 September 2019
Accepted 8 October 2019
KEYWORDS
Awe; awe experience;
positive emotion;
relationships; well-being
Awe is the feeling that overtakes astronauts as they gaze
down on the earth from space, grips listeners as they
hear a beautiful symphony, or overwhelms one while
contemplating a truly brilliant idea. At least, these are
the kinds of triggers that we usually associate with awe.
But what about awe about and between people? This
study explores social, as opposed to solitary, triggers
of awe.
Keltner and Haidt (2003) describe awe as an emotion
that must involve an encounter with a perceptually vast
stimulus and that elicits a need for accommodation.
Subsequent work on awe since Keltner and Haidt
(2003) article has further unpacked the rst necessary
condition of awe, that of vastness. Research has shown
that awe can be elicited by literally vast objects, such as
nature or space (Joye & Bolderdijk, 2015; Rudd, Vohs, &
Aaker, 2012; Silvia, Fayn, Nusbaum, & Beaty, 2015), as
well as guratively vast objects, such as spiritual experi-
ences, and grand theories (Preston & Shin, 2016; Yaden
et al., 2016). The second necessary condition of awe (i.e.
accommodation) is that it compels one to alter some
aspect of their mental representation of the world.
Colloquially, it is said that awe is mind-blowingand
this an apt phrase to capture the cognitive component
that is purported to accompany an experience of awe.
There are other components of awe beyond the two
denitional features (i.e. vastness and accommodation).
Researchers have isolated some of these other qualities
of awe by creating a scale that measures the various
domains of awe experience, without using the word
awein any of its questions (Awe Experience Scale;
Yaden, Haidt, Hood, Vago, & Newberg, 2018). That awe
induction leads to a small sense of self (i.e. self-
diminishment) has been empirically demonstrated in
a wide variety of studies (Bai et al., 2017;Pi, Dietze,
Feinberg, Stancato, & Keltner, 2015; Preston & Shin,
2016; Stellar et al., 2018). More importantly, this small
sense of self was found to be signicantly higher for awe
when compared to the other positive emotions of amu-
sement, contentment, gratitude, interest, joy, love and
pride (Campos, Shiota, Keltner, Gonzaga, & Goetz, 2013).
Awe has also been found to alter perceptions in various
other ways. For instance, awe has been related to an
altered perception of time, where participants feel time
as taking on an expansiveness (Rudd et al., 2012)or
slowing down (Joye & Dewitte, 2016).
Awe has been found to lead to improvements in well-
being and prosocial behavior. One study found that
awe-induction increased perceived time-availability,
reduced impatience and led to increases in perceived
life-satisfaction (Rudd et al., 2012). In recent work by
Anderson, Monroy, and Keltner (2018), the awe experi-
enced by military veterans and underserved youth pre-
dicted these participantsincreased well-being scores
and decreased stress responses at a one-week follow-
up. Other work has found that awe-induction led to
positive eects on mood (Joye & Bolderdijk, 2015)as
well as serving as a buer against negative mood (Koh,
CONTACT Marianna Graziosi marigraz@sas.upenn.edu
THE JOURNAL OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2019.1689422
© 2019 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
Tong, & Yuen, 2017). Awe induction has also been found
to relate to both prosocial values and behaviors (Joye &
Bolderdijk, 2015;Piet al., 2015; Prade & Saroglou, 2016;
Rudd et al., 2012).
In their foundational work on awe, Keltner and Haidt
(2003) discuss the potential for social elicitors of awe.
They suggest that this social domain of awe is one where
awe is a response to charismatic leaders, which can be
either positive or negative. Keltner and Haidt (2003) also
suggest, however, that the sort of awe experienced
interpersonally can be involve more than just social
dominance. For instance, their avorsof awe that
include beauty, virtue and ability seem to allude to an
interpersonal element (see Appendix A for the original
framework proposed by Keltner and Haidt in 2003).
While some beauty, virtue, and ability might be said to
be objectively vast, there are others experiences that
might be said to be relatively vast. Take the example of
watching ones child struggle for months to stand, they
nally take their rst steps. While this sight might seem
dull to a passer-by, this small step is an accomplishment
of enormous proportions for the proud parent.
There seem to be qualities of the experience of the
emotion elevation, that accompany (or overlap) with this
category of social awe elicitors. Elevation refers to our
emotional response to witnessing the virtuous acts of
others (Haidt, 2000). Elevation has been found to lead to
feelings of inspiration and uplift (Aquino, McFerran, &
Laven, 2011) Schall, Roper, and Fessler (2010); Silvers &
Haidt, 2008) and to have physiological qualities such as
tearing up and experiencing warm feelings in the chest
(Haidt, 2000). Furthermore, elevation (like awe) has been
found to elicit chills or goosebumps (Silvers & Haidt, 2008).
Additionally, like awe, elevation has been linked with pro-
social tendencies (Aquino et al., 2011; Schall et al., 2010;
Silvers & Haidt, 2008).
The current study seeks to determine whether awe
can be elicited by those closest to us in order to add to
the social domain of possible awe elicitors. This study rst
uses a mixed-method approach to determine if there are
dierences between recollections of awe elicited in nat-
ure versus awe elicited by other people. Second, this
study seeks to make use of qualitative data analysis in
order to better appreciate the thematic dierences
between these two categories of awe elicitors.
Study 1a quantitative analyses
Method
Participants
A total of 893 participants were recruited and provided
consent using the online survey platform Amazon
Mechanical Turk. The Institutional Review Board at the
rst authors University approved this study.
Participant characteristics. Online research methods
are prone to a great deal of subject loss for this reason
the authors emphasized collecting a large sample.
A total of 893 participants originally responded to the
survey and began taking it. Of those, 235 participants
started the survey, but did not complete it and were
removed from analyses. Additionally, 22 participants n-
ished the survey, but did not respond to the recollection
task with a story instead they copied and pasted con-
tent from the internet, wrote a random string of letters
and numbers, or copied and pasted the question instruc-
tions from the prompt into the response box. Despite
losing these participants, a reasonably large sample size
(N= 636) remained, which is more than twice the sample
size of most past studies on awe research.
The mean age of the sample (N= 636) was 35.72
(SD = 10.76), 51.1% identied as male and 48.9% identi-
ed as female. 73% identied as Non-Hispanic white,
8.5% identied as Black or African American, 9.9% iden-
tied as Asian or Asian American, 0.2% identied as
American Indian or Alaska Native, 1.7% identied as
LatinX and 5.3% identied as Mixed Race or Biracial.
Materials
Recollection task prompts. The three emotion condi-
tions that represented the dierent awe elicitors targeted
by this study included: 1) interpersonal awe (awe elicited
by a close loved one) 2) awe in nature, and 3) general
positivity (targeting positive emotions of contentment or
joy). Emotional states were induced by asking participants
to write about a time when they had experienced awe in
either a natural or interpersonal setting or a positive emo-
tion in general. The recollection task prompts were
adapted from previous research by Rudd et al. (2012). The
writing prompt used for the interpersonal awe condition
was:
Awe is our emotional response to things perceived to be
vast and astonishing that alters the way we understand
the world. Please describe a recent experience of awe
where one of your loved ones caused you to feel this
way. Try to tell the story exactly as you experienced, from
your point of view, in as much detail as possible. Feel free
to write as much as youd like.
The prompt used for the awe in nature condition was:
Awe is our emotional response to things perceived to be vast
and astonishing that alters the way we understand the
world. Please describe a recent experience of awe where
nature caused you to feel this way. Try to tell the story exactly
as you experienced, from your point of view, in as much
detail as possible. Feel free to write as much as youdlike.
2M. GRAZIOSI AND D. YADEN
The prompt used for the general positivity condi-
tion was:
Please describe a recent experience where you were happy
and remember feeling contentment or joy. Try to tell the
story exactly as you experienced, from your point of view,
in as much detail as possible. Feel free to write as much as
youd like.
Awe. The Awe Experience Scale (AWE-S) developed by
Yaden and colleagues (2018) was given in its entirety to
participants. This scale contains six subscales that target
the various qualities associated with awe experience,
they are: vastness, accommodation, self-diminishment,
connectedness
1
, time alteration, and physiological
responses (Yaden et al., 2018). It is important to note
that this scale does not use the word awein the body of
any of its questions. Up until now, researchers have
relied on single-item self-report measures of awe nested
within a set of items drawn from the PANAS.
Participants were presented with statements and then
asked to evaluate the degree to which they agreed with the
given statement on a 7-point scale ranging from 1
(Strongly Disagree)to7(Strongly Agree). The subscales
were found to be reliable: vastness (α= .87), accommoda-
tion (α= .76), self-diminishment (α= .90), connectedness
(α= .88), time alteration (α= .89), physiological reactions
(α= .81). Additionally, the subscales of the AWE-S can be
summed and all 30 prompts can be scored together (30
items; α= .93).
Procedure
Participants were randomly assigned to one of the three
recollection writing task conditions: the interpersonal
awe task, the awe in nature task, or the general positivity
task. Participants then completed the AWE-S. For the
quantitative component of the study, contrasts between
the conditions were planned to test for dierences
between conditions in AWE-S scores for the total scale,
as well as each of the six subscales.
Quantitative results
Awe
A three-way analysis of variance revealed signicant dif-
ferences in overall awe (the total of the AWE-S) between
all three conditions. Additionally, results from Levenes
test for homogeneity of variance were used to determine
whether to report interger or non-interger df when
reporting results. Participants reported feeling signi-
cantly more awe in the nature condition (M= 4.90,
SD = 0.90) than the interpersonal condition (M=4.40,
SD = 1.00) or general positivity condition (M=4.06,
SD =1.22),F(1, 633) = 34.398, p=.000,n
2
=.10.The
dierences in mean score for the overall AWE-S scale, as
well subscales, are illustrated in Figure 1. T-tests revealed
that participants reported feeling signicantly more awe
in the awe in nature condition (M= 4.90, SD = .90) than
the general positivity condition (M=4.06,SD = 1.22), t
(399.896) = 8.096, p= .002. Participants reported signi-
cantly more awe in the interpersonal awe condition
(M=4.40,SD = 1.00) than the general positivity condition
(M=4.06,SD = 1.22), t(412.834) = 3.166, p=.002.
Participants reported signicantly more awe in the awe
in nature condition (M= 4.90, SD = 0.90) than the inter-
personal awe condition (M=4.40,SD =1.00),t
(407.018) = 5.277, p= .000.
Planned contrasts revealed signicant dierences in all
of the other AWE-S sub-scales were reported between all
three conditions (Figure 1). The only exceptions were the
connectedness subscale where a three-way analysis of
variance revealed marginally signicant dierences
between all three conditions, F(1, 633) = 2.990, p=.051,
n
2
= .00. Furthermore, for the connectedness subscale,
dierences were not found between the interpersonal
awe and general positivity conditions, t(419.709) = .284,
p= .776. Lastly, for the time subscale, dierences between
the interpersonal awe (M= 4.38, SD = 1.41) and general
positivity (M=4.29,SD = 1.55) conditions were not found
to be signicant, t(633) = .694, p= .488. For details on
these planned contrasts, supplemental materials have
been hosted via Open Science Framework (OSF) at the
following link: https://osf.io/wcvd9/?view_only=
9c487feba5a84794a15f06e0069b2d1c).
Discussion of quantitative results
The results of quantitative analysis revealed that the
quality of awe experience elicited by close others
(referred to as interpersonal awe) is distinct from
the quality of awe elicited by nature or general posi-
tivity in terms of overall awe (totaled AWE-S score). It
should be noted that interpersonal awe tends to t
between general positivity and awe from nature. This
suggests either that interpersonal awe is distinct form
of awe triggered by nature, or that awe is
a continuum and dierent triggers result in dierent
average intensities and qualities of awe (which seems
to align with the original framework proposed by
Keltner & Haidt, 2003).
The results of the planned contrasts help to make
sense of this dierence, suggesting rst that the awe
elicited by close others does, in fact, include both of the
necessary conditions of awe: vastness and accommoda-
tion. This indicates that those participants asked to write
THE JOURNAL OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 3
about awe elicited by a close loved one, were indeed
feeling awe. Furthermore, the interpersonal type of awe
also elicits a greater degree of self-diminishment, and
increased likelihood of physiological eects (both of
which are other aspects of awe experience) than general
positivity. However, the interpersonal awe elicitor did
not seem to aect ones perception of time or sense of
connectedness (in the broad sense; see footnote 1) any
dierently than general positivity, whereas, awe elicited
by nature does have eects on these domains.
Study 1b qualitative analyses
In order to further explore the dierences between the
quality of awe elicited by nature and the quality of awe by
close interpersonal relationships, it was important to
explore the thematic aspects of these experiences. To
do this, a qualitative analysis was conducted which
looked further into the dierent themes between awe in
nature and interpersonal awe, making use of the content
of the excerpts written by participants. To the authors
knowledge, this is the rst study to attempt to empirically
evaluate the avors of awe outlined by Keltner and Haidt
(2003) in their landmark work, which serve as the founda-
tion for the qualitative coding frame used in this study.
Method
Participants
The participants (N= 636) in the quantitative study
described above were asked to write about their awe
experiences as a way to prompt recollection of the target
emotional experience. This written data was then ana-
lyzed using qualitative methods.
Materials
Qualitative coding frame
Three independent coders used a coding frame devel-
oped for the purposes of this study to aid in qualitative
data analysis of the recollection task excerpts. This cod-
ing frame was based on the conceptual analysis of awe
conducted by Keltner and Haidt (2003) (see Appendix
Afor a visual representation of this conceptual analysis).
The coding frame developed for this study sought to
focus on the avors of awe proposed by Keltner and
Haidt (see Appendix B for the coding frame used in this
study). The descriptors in this coding framework aided
three independent members of the coding team (which
did not include the authors) in their coding of the qua-
litative excerpts.
Figure 1. Dierences in AWE-S overall scale scores and subscale scores for each of the three emotion induction tasks. Dotted lines
indicated ANOVA results, solid lines indicate results of planned contrast t-tests. A single asterisk denotes dierences found to be
statistically signicant at the .05 level, a double asterisk at the .01 level and a triple asterisk at the .001 level.
4M. GRAZIOSI AND D. YADEN
Procedure
Independent coders were supplied with the written
excerpts in a randomized order and then asked to eval-
uate the degree to which these excerpts were indicative
of a given avor of awe (according to a coding frame
which can be seen in Appendix B). The coders received
only the excerpts and the coding frame, and were other-
wise kept blind to the details of the study. The excerpts
written by participants were manually coded by the
three independent coders. In order to evaluate how
the avors(or themes) of awe mapped onto each con-
dition (Interpersonal Awe, Awe in Nature, or General
Positivity), the coders evaluated each excerpt and either
assigned to it, one of the ve avors (ability, beauty,
threat, virtue/strength of character, or supernatural caus-
ality) or left the excerpt without a code if none of the
avors were captured by a given excerpt. Coders were
kept blind to the condition and prompt that the partici-
pantsexcerpts were responses too. Coders were also
kept blind to each othersresponses. After their codes
were applied, Interrater Reliability (IRR) was obtained by
calculating the percent agreement among multiple
coders. This was achieved by dividing all of the instances
where two or more coders agreed (i.e. coded the same
response) over the total number of possible instances.
The coders evaluations of the excerpts were found to be
highly reliable with percent agreement of 90.72%.
Results
Awe in nature
Of the 213 excerpts in this condition, 61.2% were coded
as Beauty, followed by 8.5% coded as Threat, 3.3%
coded as Ability, 2.8% coded as Supernatural
Causality, and 0.5% coded as Virtue or Excellence of
Character. In this condition, 10.3% of excerpts were
left blank (i.e. coded as no avor), and for 12.7% of
excerpts coders could not agree. This distribution is
illustrated in Figure 2.
Qualitative analysis revealed that the code for the
Beauty avor of awe was used most in the Awe in
Nature Condition (132 excerpts), which corroborates
the predominance of words that suggest aesthetic plea-
sure in the Awe in Nature condition, which did not
appear in the Interpersonal Awe or General Positivity
conditions. For example:
I saw the super moon at the end of January. I never
noticed one before so seeing how big and close it
looked was astonishing. (116)
This quality is described again here, even from an indir-
ect experience with nature:
I saw pictures from the hubble telescope and was
amazed. I had no idea so many galaxies and space
could be so vast and beautiful. I was inspired. (341)
Figure 2. Number of excerpts in each condition coded for the avors of awe.
THE JOURNAL OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 5
Interpersonal awe
Of the 204 excerpts in this condition, 35.8% were coded
as Virtue or Excellence of Character, followed by 11.8%
coded as Ability, 10.8% coded as Beauty, 2.9% coded as
Threat, and 1.5% coded as Supernatural Causality. In this
condition, 26.0% of excerpts were left blank (i.e. coded
as no avor), and for 11.3% of excerpts coders could not
agree (revisit Figure 2).
Qualitative analysis revealed that code for the Virtue
or Excellence of Character avor of awe was used most in
the Interpersonal Awe (73 excerpts). In the interpersonal
awe condition, the excerpts involved a sense of inspira-
tion, amazement or astonishment toward loved ones.
For instance:
I felt a sense of awe, among other emotions, when I saw
and heard my wife react to the diagnosis and prognosis
she was given recently by the oncologist at the hospital.
She was basically told, in so many words, that she was
going to die, how long it might take, and what they
could do to help limit her suering. Her comment was,
It is what it is. Thank you for all your help.She reacted
stronger than I did. (32)
Most unique to this condition and avor combination of
awe experience was that participantsviews of their
loved ones were changed by the experience. One parti-
cipant recalls:
A few weeks ago my mother came to visit us and we
were out to dinner. we were discussing our past relation-
ships and my mother went into detail about her past
and what she had gone through. Thing that i frankly
couldnt have imagined. She told it with such grace and
humility that it honestly surprised me and i respect her
for doing it. It felt like a woman I didnt know at all, but
felt good in that it was a stronger version rather than
weaker. (617)
General positivity
Of the 219 excerpts in this condition, 11.0% were coded
as Ability, followed by 10.5% coded as Beauty, 5.5%
coded as Virtue or Excellence of Character, 0.5% coded
as Threat, and 0.5% coded as Supernatural Causality. In
this condition 68.0% of excerpts were left blank (i.e.
coded as no avor), and for 4.1% of excerpts coders
could not agree (revisit Figure 2). Besides the excerpts
from this condition that were coded as No Flavor, the
remaining responses were likely to be coded as Ability
(24 excerpts) or beauty (23 excerpts).
Excerpts that were coded as expressing the Ability
Flavor in the General Positivity Condition were more
likely to be directed at a participantsown behavior.
There was a strong connection between participants
behavior and other positive emotions, such as accom-
plishment, pride, and happiness. For instance:
I won a trophy for being the best in my class. I was so
happy because I felt like my hard work paid o. (406)
Even in excerpts that involved the ability of other people,
there was a sense of accomplishment and pride. More
importantly, the excerpts from the General Positivity
condition regarding the ability of others lacked the qual-
ity of astonishment and amazement that was present in
the Interpersonal awe condition.
Discussion of qualitative study
The qualitative ndings were able to identify which
avorof awe participants were more likely to feel
when writing about their experiences. The excerpts
from the Interpersonal Awe condition (where awe was
elicited by writing about a close other) were coded most
for the Virtue or Excellence of Character Flavor of Awe
(i.e. an awe experience tinged with warm and pleasant
feelings of elevation; see Figure 2). The excerpts demon-
strate that interactions in close relationships can take on
a sense of gurative vastness in the face of magnani-
mous virtue or imposing excellence that rise to an awe
experience.
The qualitative results further elucidated the nuances
of the interpersonal type of awe elicitor. The excerpts
that were coded most for the Ability avor of awe (i.e.
awe experience that includes admiration at witnessing
great talent or skill) came from the Interpersonal Awe
condition (see Figure 2). When it came to Awe in Nature,
these ndings showed that excerpts were coded most
often as exhibiting the Beauty avor of Awe, which refers
to awe experiences that involve aesthetically pleasing
feelings (Keltner & Haidt, 2003) (see Appendix B).
Overall discussion
Across these two studies, ndings suggest that the
quality of awe elicited in the Interpersonal Awe condi-
tion was distinct from both general positivity and the
quality of awe elicited by nature. Overall, these ndings
suggest that there is a phenomenological experience
of awe, elicited by close loved ones, that meet the
widely accepted preconditions of awe experience,
namely vastness and a need for accommodation. The
vastness is relative and gurative, that is, it is a vastness
experienced by the subject in terms of their relation-
ship and the accommodation that occurs likely entails
their schemas of a close loved one. Additionally, this
experience of awe elicited by close others is similar to
awe elicited by nature in that it involves self-
diminishment and physiological eects, though with
a lesser degree of intensity. Finally, interpersonal awe
6M. GRAZIOSI AND D. YADEN
experiences tend not to alter perceptions of time or
increase feelings of connectedness any more than gen-
eral positivity.
The results of this study corroborate much of the past
research on awe, which has found that nature is
a powerful elicitor of awe (Chirico et al., 2017; Joye &
Bolderdijk, 2015; Rudd et al., 2012; Silvia et al., 2015;
Valdesolo & Graham, 2014; Van Cappellen & Saroglou,
2012). Participants scores were highest for overall awe,
as well as every single subscale on the AWE-S when
responding to the awe in nature task (see Figure 1). In
fact, given that scores were found to be signicantly
higher on each of these dimensions for the awe in nature
condition, it is likely that stimuli involving nature have
the ability to elicit awe to a greater degree of intensity
than social elicitors.
Limitations and directions for future research
Data were collected online using Amazon Mechanical
Turk (M-Turk). This method of data collection is not
without limitations and participants have been
known to rush through tasks or participants might
take surveys multiple times with dierent user names
(Johnson & Borden, 2012). M-Turk tries to minimize
these infractions through user limitations, and, in this
case, the survey contained attention checks.
Nevertheless, in this study, 257 participants still had
to be removed for various reasons, such as failing the
attention checks and for failing to submit a complete
survey.
One of the biggest limitations of the current study
is that it only asked participants to complete the
AWE-S and did not take any outcome measures.
Therefore, while interpersonal awe has been estab-
lished as distinct experience from that of Awe elicited
by nature or general positivity, the degree to which
this experience is related to other outcomes typical of
awe such as well-being (Rudd et al., 2015) and pro-
social behavior (Joye & Bolderdijk, 2015;Piet al.,
2015; Rudd et al., 2015) are viable directions for
future research.
Finally, recall that the experience of interpersonal
awe seems to be driven by a virtue or moral compo-
nent, as evidenced by the qualitative results (revisit
Figure 2). These data suggest that the emotion eleva-
tion may be closely related to (or even synonymous
with) interpersonal awe due to the overwhelming
endorsement of the moral component. Future research
might dig deeper into the comparisons between the
social domain of awe and its relationship with the
emotion of elevation.
Conclusion
This study explored the social domain of awe elicitors
by looking into aspects of the quality of awe experi-
ence elicited by close others (or interpersonal awe).
A quantitative study demonstrated that interpersonal
awe is a unique quality of awe experience that is less
intense than awe elicited by nature but distinct from
general positivity. Interpersonal awe shares the quali-
ties of Vastness, Accommodation, Self-Diminishment
and Physiological Eects with awe elicited by Nature,
but lacks the qualities of connectedness (i.e. a general
sense of feeling connected with nature or the uni-
verse) and altered time perception often attributed
to awe experience elicited by Nature. This was the
rst quantitative study to distinguish the quality of
awe experience elicited by a loved one or close
other (i.e. interpersonal awe). A qualitative study
helped to further distinguish interpersonal awe from
awe elicited by nature or general positivity. Awe that
was elicited by nature was dominated by the avors
of beauty and threat, while the interpersonal type of
awe was dominated by virtue/excellence of character,
which again indicates that it may be related to experi-
ence of elevation. These ndings illuminate the
degree to which awe can be elicited socially, even in
contexts close to home by those closest to us. This is
a crucial step in beginning to explore how to leverage
ordinary conditions as gateways into awe experience.
Perhaps awe, while an ordinary response to the extra-
ordinary, is also an extraordinary response to the
ordinary.
Note
1. It is important to note that in terms of the AWE-S (Yaden
et al., 2018), the connectedness subscale is measuring
abroad sense of connectedness. In the context of the
paper, it may be confusing and one may get the sense
that connectedness refers to an intimate sense of con-
nectedness in close interpersonal relationships this is
not the case. Connectedness is taken to mean a sense of
relationship to the universe as a whole, often thought of
as oneness.
Disclosure statement
No potential conict of interest was reported by the authors.
ORCID
Marianna Graziosi http://orcid.org/0000-0002-4177-3304
David Yaden http://orcid.org/0000-0002-9604-6227
THE JOURNAL OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 7
References
Anderson, C. L., Monroy, M., & Keltner, D. (2018). Awe in nature
heals: Evidence from military veterans, at-risk youth, and
college students. Emotion,18(8), 11951202. doi:10.1037/
emo0000442
Angyal, A. (1941). Disgust and related aversions. The Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology,36, 393412. doi: 10.1037/
h0058254
Aquino, K., McFerran, B., & Laven, M. (2011). Moral identity and
the experience of moral elevation in response to acts of
uncommon goodness. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,100(4), 703718.
Bai, Y., Maruskin, L. A., Chen, S., Gordon, A. M., Stellar, J. E.,
McNeil, G. D., . . . Keltner, D. (2017). Awe, the diminished self,
and collective engagement: Universals and cultural varia-
tions in the small self. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,113(2), 185209.
Buss, D. (1994). The evolution of desire. New York, NY: Basic
Books.
Campos, B., Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., Gonzaga, G. C., &
Goetz, J. L. (2013). What is shared, what is dierent? Core
relational themes and expressive displays of eight positive
emotions. Cognition and Emotion,27(1), 3752.
Chirico, A., Cipresso, P., Yaden, D. B., Biassoni, F., Riva, G., &
Gaggioli, A. (2017). Eectiveness of immersive videos in
inducing awe: An experimental study. Scientic Reports,7
(1), 1218.
Haidt, J. (2000). The positive emotion of elevation. Prevention
and Treatment,3(3), 15.
Johnson, D. R., & Borden, L. A. (2012). Participants at your
ngertips: Using Amazons Mechanical Turk to increase stu-
dent-faculty collaborative research. Society for the Teaching
of Psychology,39(4), 245251.
Joye, Y., & Bolderdijk, J. W. (2015). An exploratory study into the
eects of extraordinary nature on emotions, mood, and
prosociality. Frontiers in Psychology,5, 1557.
Joye, Y., & Dewitte, S. (2016). Up speeds you down.
Awe-evoking monumental buildings trigger behavioral
and perceived freezing. Journal of Environmental
Psychology,47, 112125.
Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (2003). Approaching awe, a moral, spiri-
tual, and aesthetic emotion. Cognition and Emotion,17(2),
297314.
Koh, A. H. Q., Tong, E. M. W., & Yuen, A. Y. L. (2017). The
buering eect of awe on negative aect towards lost
possessions. The Journal of Positive Psychology,14(2), 156
165. doi:10.1080/17439760.2017.1388431
Pi, P. K., Dietze, P., Feinberg, M., Stancato, D. M., & Keltner, D.
(2015). Awe, the small self, and prosocial behavior. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,108(6), 883899.
Prade, C., & Saroglou, V. (2016). Aweseects on generosity and
helping. The Journal of Positive Psychology,11(5), 522530.
Preston, J. L., & Shin, F. (2016). Spiritual experiences evoke awe
through the small self in both religious and non-religious
individuals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,70,
212221.
Rudd, M., Vohs, K. D., & Aaker, J. (2012). Awe expands peoples
perception of time, alters decision making, & enhances well-
being. Psychological Science,23(10), 11301136.
Schall, S., Roper, J., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2010). Elevation leads to
altruistic behavior. Psychological Science,21(3), 315320.
Silvers, J. A., & Haidt, J. (2008). Moral elevation can induce
nursing. Emotion,8(2), 291295.
Silvia, P., Fayn, K., Nusbaum, E. C., & Beaty, R. E. (2015).
Openness to experience and awe in response to nature
and music: Personality and profound aesthetic
experiences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the
Arts,9(4), 376384.
Stellar, J. E., Gordon, A., Anderson, C. L., Pi, P. K., McNeil, G. D.,
& Keltner, D. (2018). Awe and humility. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology,114(2), 258269.
Valdesolo, P., & Graham, J. (2014). Awe, uncertainty, and
agency detection. Psychological Science,25(1), 170178.
Van Cappellen, P., & Saroglou, V. (2012). Awe activates religious
and spiritual feelings and behavioral intentions. Psychology
of Religion and Spirituality,4(3), 223236.
Wilson, E. O. (1984). Biophilia: The human bond with other
species. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Yaden, D., Kaufman, S. B., Hyde, E., Chirico, A., Gaggioli, A.,
Zhang, J., & Keltner, D. (2018). The development of the
awe experience scale (awe-s): A multifactorial for a complex
emotion. The Journal of Positive Psychology,14(4), 474488.
doi:10.1080/17439760.2018.1484940
Yaden, D. B., Haidt, J., Hood, R. W., Jr, Vago, D. R., &
Newberg, A. B. (2018). The varieties of self-transcendent
experience. Review of General Psychology,21(2), 143.
Yaden, D. B., Iwry, J., Slack, K. J., Eichstaedt, J. C., Zhao, Y.,
Vaillant, G. E., & Newberg, A. B. (2016). The overview eect:
Awe and self-transcendent experience in space ight.
Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice,
3(1), 111.
8M. GRAZIOSI AND D. YADEN
Appendix A.
Re-creation of Keltner and Haidt (2003) prototype approach: Social elicitors*
Appendix B.
Qualitative coding frame
Flavorsof the Awe Experience
Threat
This avor of awe describes experiences that include feelings of fear in response to threat or danger.
–‘As discussed by Burke, threat and danger cause an experience of awe to be avored by feelings of fear. Variation in whether an
entity is threatening or not might account for how charismatic leaders (e.g. Hitler vs. Gandhi) or natural scenes (e.g. an electrical
storm vs. a sunset) evoke awe- related experiences of dramatically dierent valence.(Keltner & Haidt, 2003, p. 304).
Beauty
Aesthetically pleasing feelings that accompany an experience of awe in response to beautiful people, nature, art, etc.
–‘Beautiful people and scenes can produce awe-related experiences that are avored with aesthetic pleasure. We cannot give
here an account of what makes something beautiful, although we think it likely that there will be at least two sources of
intuitions about beauty, one stemming from ``biophilia’’ [love of nature] (Wilson, 1984), and the other from the evolution of
sexual desire and attraction (Buss, 1994).(Keltner & Haidt, 2003, p. 304)
Virtue/Strength of Character
Feelings of elevation that accompany an awe experience in response to witnessing great acts of virtue or strength of character in
other people.
–‘People who display virtues or strength of character often trigger in other people a state that has been called ``elevation’’ (Haidt,
2000). Elevation is an emotional response to ``moral beauty’’ or human goodness; it usually includes a warm and pleasant
feeling in the chest and a desire to become a better person, or to lead a better life. Elevation appears to be a member of the
family of awe-related states, but because experiences of elevation do not usually involve perceived vastness or power, they
should be labelled as ``elevation’’, not awe(Keltner & Haidt, 2003, p. 305).
Ability
Feelings of admiration that accompany an awe experience in response to witnessing exceptional ability, talent or skill.
–‘Perceptions of exceptional ability, talent, and skill will avor an experience with admiration in which the perceiver feels respect
for the other person that is not based on dominance and submission within a hierarchy. Exceptional ability may often trigger
a need for accommodation, but if there is no perception of vastness, then the experience should simply be labelled ``admira-
tion’’, not awe(Keltner & Haidt, 2003, p. 305).
Supernatural Causality
An awe experience accompanied the uncanny feeling that God or a supernatural entity is manifesting itself.
–‘The perception that God or some other supernatural entity is manifesting itself (e.g. seeing an angel or a ghost, or seeing an
object levitate) will avor an experience with an element of the uncanny. The uncanny is usually terrifying (Angyal, 1941), but it
can be glorious if the entity is perceived as benevolent(Keltner & Haidt, 2003, p. 306).
Eliciting Situations Vastness Accommodation Threat Beauty Ability Virtue Supernatural Causality
Prototype: Powerful Leader XX?
Encounter with God X?? XX
Great Skill (Admiration) XX X
Great Virtue (Elevation) XX
XDenotes the appraisal usually made in response to the elicitor
? Denotes the response to the elicitor that might be made, which would add the given avorto the awe experience
*Visit Keltner and Haidt (2003) landmark article for a full framework, which also includes physical and cognitive elicitors.
THE JOURNAL OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 9
... In a recent study examining the triggers of awe worldwide, Keltner (2024: 11) found that experiencing or observing "other people's courage, kindness, strength, or overcoming" is the most common awe trigger. Social triggers motivate a desire to transform oneself by imitating, replicating, and/or bringing oneself into greater alignment with the behaviors of an admired social actor (Graziosi & Yaden, 2021;Prade & Saroglou, 2023); they motivate individuals to experience the "we" in place of the "self" and generate a feeling of belonging to a broader collective (Collins, 2004). ...
... Social exemplars or role models frequently trigger awe in and around organizations (Graziosi & Yaden, 2021). After seeing what others have achieved, individuals are often inspired to alter their perspectives to be future-oriented and to focus on what is possible for themselves (Algoe & Haidt, 2009;Prade & Saroglou, 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent psychology research has uncovered the transformative power of awe in spurring change. Awe emotional experiences induce change through two functions: the feeling of being in the presence of something vast that challenges existing worldviews and a sense of needing to adjust one's mental framework to accommodate the vastness. This paper introduces a theoretical framework exploring how awe impacts processes of pursuing transformational opportunities-namely, opportunities that are radical, fulfill dual goals (social and economic), or have a broad scope for scaling. This paper theorizes that entrepreneurial venturing offers a structured way for individuals to process stimuli from an awe episode to identify and validate a transformational opportunity. Our theorizing builds on psychology theories of self-transcendent emotions (generally) and awe (specifically) and extends those theories to mainstream entrepreneurship and management research. We contribute to research on entrepreneurship and management by demonstrating how awe episodes can impact individuals and other stakeholders in pursuing transformational opportunities.
... In the definition outlined by Keltner and Haidt [1] one necessary condition of awe is that of vastness. Research has shown that awe can be elicited by literally vast objects, such as nature [6,7] or space [8,9] as well as figuratively vast objects, such as spiritual [10,11] or psychedelic [12,13] experience or grand theories [2], and finally relatively vast objects such as interpersonally significant experiences with loved ones (e.g., seeing your child walk for the first time [12]. ...
... In the definition outlined by Keltner and Haidt [1] one necessary condition of awe is that of vastness. Research has shown that awe can be elicited by literally vast objects, such as nature [6,7] or space [8,9] as well as figuratively vast objects, such as spiritual [10,11] or psychedelic [12,13] experience or grand theories [2], and finally relatively vast objects such as interpersonally significant experiences with loved ones (e.g., seeing your child walk for the first time [12]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to develop and validate a short-form version of the AWE-S (AWE-SF) within psychedelic samples, to reduce participant burden while maintaining psychometric integrity. Across five studies, we first replicated the original six-factor structure of the AWE-S through exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses (Study 1), leading to the creation of the 12-item AWE-SF (Study 2–3). We then established the AWE-SF’s initial and predictive validity by correlating it with relevant emotional, psychedelic, and well-being outcomes (Study 4–6). The AWE-SF demonstrated strong positive associations with positive emotions and was also linked to openness to experience. Moreover, the AWE-SF effectively predicted both mystical-type and challenging psychedelic experiences, as well as long-term well-being outcomes such as life satisfaction and psychological richness. In particular, the facets of connection and vastness were associated with positive emotional states and mystical-type experience, while accommodation and self-loss were associated with negative emotional states and challenging psychedelic experience. These findings suggest that the AWE-SF is a robust, reliable, and accessible tool for measuring awe experience.
... In merito all'udito, sono ancora pochi gli studi che si sono occupati della profonda meraviglia suscitata dalla musica ma un dato è emerso chiaramente: la profonda meraviglia si innesta su gusti, preferenze musicali pregresse da cui non possiamo prescindere quando progettiamo situazioni, scenari tesi a indurla. Inoltre, nonostante esistano alcuni stimoli collettivi in grado di suscitare la profonda meraviglia (Graziosi, Yaden 2019), è sempre la dimensione intima e privata che favorisce l'emergere di tale emozione. Probabilmente, non è un caso che tale emozione sia così intrinsecamente collegata con momenti di profonda trasformazione personale (Chirico, Pizzolante, Kitson, et al. 2022, Pearsall 2007, dove la persona costruisce un nuovo sé, nuove strutture cognitive e, in definitiva, un nuovo sguardo sul mondo. ...
Article
In this work, I explored the complex relationship between psychological awe and the philosophical concept of the sublime. While psychology has often considered awe as a positive emotion distinct from the negative variant of the sublime, philosophical discourse has suggested an inherent historical and aesthetic connection between the two phenomena. I aimed to provide a preliminary contribution towards solving this puzzle, by relying on empirical findings from psychology and drawing from philosophical theories of Longinus, Kant, Schopenhauer and Hegel. I suggested a conceptualization of awe as a potential cultural evolution of the sublime, thus highlighting an overlap of key dimensions, such as vastness, self-transcendence, and alteration of sense of time. Moreover, I outlined practical insights and guidelines for eliciting awe in everyday life. Finally, the transformative nature of awe was explored by integrating psychological and philosophical perspectives, thus enabling a broader understanding of complex emotions and how they can influence human experience and wellbeing.
... As an emotion encourages individuals to contribute more towards environmental protection and assisting others, awe can prompt a greater incidence of social and charitable behaviors. Graziosi and Yaden [38] discovered that awe elicited by natural sources is defined through beauty, whereas social awe is characterized by virtue or exceptional ability, indicating that the two types of awe differ, with social awe generally being less intense than nature awe. There is a lot of research that shows that emotions have an effect on behavior [39][40][41]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The development of the construction industry has generated a large amount of construction waste, and resource utilization of construction waste is an effective means of recycling. However, such recycled construction waste products still lack market competitiveness and recognition. Consumers’ psychological activities are often influenced by emotions, and the sense of awe plays an important role in green consumption. This study aims to investigate how the sense of awe affects consumers’ willingness to pay for construction waste recycled products. The study used functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) with a willingness-to-pay task paradigm for experiments, which aims to reveal how different types of awe affect willingness to pay for construction waste recycled products. The behavioral results showed that two conditions effectively induced awe and enhanced consumers’ willingness to pay, but the difference between nature awe and social awe was not significant. The neural activation results showed significant activation in the inferior prefrontal gyrus and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. In particular, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity was significantly enhanced in the social awe condition. The functional connectivity results showed that, compared to the control condition experiment, the awe condition experiment triggered stronger functional connectivity. Therefore, exploring the effect of awe on the willingness to pay for construction waste recycled products can provide a basis reference for companies to develop marketing strategies and corporate pricing and promote the promotion and application of construction waste recycled products in the market.
... By contrast, AR relationships appear to have evolved from dominance hierarchies, with some humans taking precedence over access to resources, and extending hierarchy to responsibilities for leading, guiding, and standing up for subordinates (Fiske, 2002;Keltner & Haidt, 2003). In social situations, awe arises from the sudden appraisal of AR relationships between the person who experiences awe and the higher-status individual who evokes it, whereby the former is inclined to evaluate the rank difference as legitimate and to show loyalty or respect to the latter (Graziosi & Yaden, 2021). According to Keltner and Haidt (2003), AR relationships would prototypically emerge in response to non-social nature stimuli that are appraised as superior to the self in size, magnitude, vastness, or profundity of meaning, and which are linked to elicitation of awe. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Across cultures, the relationship between humans and nature affects both the well-being of humans and the natural environment. While the concept of nature connectedness is recognized as an important topic in this regard, little is known about the psychological processes that establish and foster it. Positioned at the intersection of environmental psychology and outdoor studies, this article-based thesis adopts a critical realism perspective to explore how social relational emotions, such as kama muta (≈ being moved) and awe, are specifically significant to the process of connecting in and to nature.
... Existing evidence has suggested that awe could be induced by various stimuli (e.g. natural scenes, nonnatural sceneries, music, groundbreaking theory concept, and close relationships; Graziosi & Yaden, 2021;Krogh-Jespersen et al., 2020;Piff et al., 2015;Villar et al., 2022) and among different populations (e.g. teenagers, elders, employees, and patients;Anderson et al., 2020;Büssing et al., 2014;Hu & Meng, 2022;T. ...
... Awe is more generally defined in terms of two appraisal dimensions: the perception of vastness (both literal and figurative) and the need for cognitive accommodation (incorporating unexpected experiences into one's mental schemata) 128,129 . Awe has been investigated across several triggers, such as natural scenery, virtual reality, videos (for example, nature documentaries) and witnessing other people engaging in acts of excellence and virtue 75,[130][131][132][133] , and has been found to promote learning 134,135 and decrease dependence on internal knowledge (what an individual already knows) 136 . In psychedelic research contexts, awe has been proposed as a major therapeutic mechanism of psychedelics 125,126 . ...
Article
Full-text available
The administration of classic psychedelics has been associated with well-being and mental health benefits as well as risks and adverse events. The acute subjective effects of psychedelics might have a causal role in these risks and therapeutic benefits, but inconsistencies and limitations in the conceptualization and measurement of these acute subjective effects hinder research and clinical advances. In this Review, we outline current characterizations and psychometric examinations of the acute subjective effects of psychedelics, evaluate the construct validity of commonly used measures and describe findings showing that specific acute subjective effects predict certain outcomes. We discuss how to balance the limitations of existing measures with methodological advances in practice and elaborate on well-known methods and other psychological processes that can help inform the creation of new measures. We suggest actionable recommendations for how the field can transcend current conceptualizations and provide guidance on best practices until the next generation of measures is validated. Sections
... Awe was confirmed by both the questionnaire, and pictorial method. The AWE-S score (Mdn = 4.73) was similar to aweexperiences in other studies, and higher than neutral conditions (Graziosi and Yaden 2019;. Drawing gains were calculated from the pre-drawing (Mdn = 5.50) to the post-drawing (Mdn = 5.00). ...
Article
Full-text available
The Overview Effect is a complex experience reported by astronauts after viewing Earth from space. Numerous accounts suggest that it leads to increased interconnectedness to other human beings and environmental awareness, comparable to self-transcendence. It can cause fundamental changes in mental models of the world, improved well-being, and stronger appreciation of, and responsibility for Earth. From a cognitive perspective, it is closely linked to the emotion of awe, possibly triggered by the overwhelming perceived vastness of the universe. Given that most research in the domain focuses on self-reports, little is known about potential neurophysiological markers of the Overview Effect. In the experiment reported here, participants viewed an immersive Virtual Reality simulation of a space journey while their brain activity was recorded using electroencephalography (EEG). Post-experimental self-reports confirmed they were able to experience the Overview Effect in the simulated environment. EEG recordings revealed lower spectral power in beta and gamma frequency bands during the defining moments of the Overview Effect. The decrease in spectral power can be associated with reduced mental processing, and a disruption of known mental structures in this context, thereby providing more evidence for the cognitive effects of the experience.
Article
Full-text available
Aesthetic experiences influence the way we perceive the world, often at a subliminal level. But relatively little is known about how these aesthetic experiences affect us cognitively and how they shape our identity and improve our well-being. The purpose of this paper is thus to: (1) examine and review the current literature in the emerging field of neuroaesthetics—how the brain responds to beauty and sublime experiences; (2) extrapolate from current literature the properties of beauty and what it means when people perceive something to be beautiful; (3) synthesize the current literature and my own research on the topic to bring more awareness on the health benefits and transcendent power of experiencing awe or sublimity; (4) use the PERMA (positive emotion, engagement, relationship, meaning, achievement) theory of well-being proposed by positive psychologist Martin Seligman to present an action framework I established, acronymed NUMA (nature walks, unconditional love, meditation, affirmative goals). Within this paper, I argue that these perceptually aesthetic experiences (which can happen in a variety of forms such as in nature, artworks, music, or poetry) elicit in us a sense of awe and wonder, which performs a “scale effect” or “overview effect” on us and if experienced regularly could guide us toward living a more meaningful life.
Article
Full-text available
Awe is a complex emotion composed of an appraisal of vastness and a need for accommodation. The purpose of this study was to develop a robust state measure of awe, the Awe Experience Scale (AWE-S), based on the extant experimental literature. In study 1, participants (N = 501) wrote about an intense moment of awe that they had experienced and then completed a survey about their experience. Exploratory factor analysis revealed a 6-factor structure, including: altered time perception (F1); self-diminishment (F2); connectedness (F3); perceived vastness (F4); physical sensations (F5); need for accommodation (F6). Internal consistency was strong for each factor (α ≥ .80). Study 2 confirmed the 6-factor structure (N = 636) using fit indices (CFI = .905; RMSEA = .054). Each factor of the AWES is significantly correlated with the awe items of the modified Differential Emotions Scale (mDES) and Dispositional Positive Emotion Scale (D-PES). Triggers, valence, and themes associated with awe experiences are reported.
Article
Full-text available
Humility is a foundational virtue that counters selfish inclinations such as entitlement, arrogance, and narcissism (Tangney, 2000). We hypothesize that experiences of awe promote greater humility. Guided by an appraisal-tendency framework of emotion, we propose that when individuals encounter an entity that is vast and challenges their worldview, they feel awe, which leads to self-diminishment and subsequently humility. In support of these claims, awe-prone individuals were rated as more humble by friends (Study 1) and reported greater humility across a 2-week period (Study 2), controlling for other positive emotions. Inducing awe led participants to present a more balanced view of their strengths and weaknesses to others (Study 3) and acknowledge, to a greater degree, the contribution of outside forces in their own personal accomplishments (Study 4), compared with neutral and positive control conditions. Finally, an awe-inducing expansive view elicited greater reported humility than a neutral view (Study 5). We also elucidated the process by which awe leads to humility. Feelings of awe mediated the relationship between appraisals (perceptions of vastness and a challenge to one's world view) and humility (Study 4), and self-diminishment mediated the relationship between awe and humility (Study 5). Taken together, these results reveal that awe offers one path to greater humility. (PsycINFO Database Record
Article
Full-text available
Awe has been theorized as a collective emotion, one that enables individuals to integrate into social collectives. In keeping with this theorizing, we propose that awe diminishes the sense of self and shifts attention away from individual interests and concerns. In testing this hypothesis across 6 studies (N = 2137), we first validate pictorial and verbal measures of the small self; we then document that daily, in vivo, and lab experiences of awe, but not other positive emotions, diminish the sense of the self. These findings were observed across collectivist and individualistic cultures, but also varied across cultures in magnitude and content. Evidence from the last 2 studies showed that the influence of awe upon the small self accounted for increases in collective engagement, fitting with claims that awe promotes integration into social groups. Discussion focused on how the small self might mediate the effects of awe on collective cognition and behavior, the need to study more negatively valenced varieties of awe, and other potential cultural variations of the small self.
Article
Full-text available
Various forms of self-loss have been described as aspects of mental illness (e.g., depersonalization disorder), but might self-loss also be related to mental health? In this integrative review and proposed organizational framework, we focus on self-transcendent experiences (STEs)—transient mental states marked by decreased self-salience and increased feelings of connectedness. We first identify common psychological constructs that contain a self-transcendent aspect, including mindfulness, flow, peak experiences, mystical-type experiences, and certain positive emotions (e.g., love, awe). We then propose psychological and neurobiological mechanisms that may mediate the effects of STEs based on a review of the extant literature from social psychology, clinical psychology, and affective neuroscience. We conclude with future directions for further empirical research on these experiences.
Article
Full-text available
Awe, a complex emotion composed by the appraisal components of vastness and need for accommodation, is a profound and often meaningful experience. Despite its importance, psychologists have only recently begun empirical study of awe. At the experimental level, a main issue concerns how to elicit high intensity awe experiences in the lab. To address this issue, Virtual Reality (VR) has been proposed as a potential solution. Here, we considered the highest realistic form of VR: immersive videos. 42 participants watched at immersive and normal 2D videos displaying an awe or a neutral content. After the experience, they rated their level of awe and sense of presence. Participants’ psychophysiological responses (BVP, SC, sEMG) were recorded during the whole video exposure. We hypothesized that the immersive video condition would increase the intensity of awe experienced compared to 2D screen videos. Results indicated that immersive videos significantly enhanced the self-reported intensity of awe as well as the sense of presence. Immersive videos displaying an awe content also led to higher parasympathetic activation. These findings indicate the advantages of using VR in the experimental study of awe, with methodological implications for the study of other emotions.
Article
The power of nature to both heal and inspire awe has been noted by many great thinkers. However, no study has examined how the impact of nature on well-being and stress-related symptoms is explained by experiences of awe. In the present investigation, we examine this process in studies of extraordinary and everyday nature experiences. In Study 1, awe experienced by military veterans and youth from underserved communities while whitewater rafting, above and beyond all the other positive emotions measured, predicted changes in well-being and stress-related symptoms one week later. In Study 2, the nature experiences that undergraduate students had during their everyday lives led to more awe, which mediated the effect of nature experience on improvements in well-being. We discuss how accounting for people’s emotional experiences during outdoors activities can increase our understanding of how nature impacts people’s well-being.
Article
Although the positive emotion of awe is of growing interest, past research has not directly examined its buffering effect in negative circumstances. As awe has been theoretically linked to experiences of vastness and spirituality, the present study proposes that awe helps individuals alleviate their negative affect, in the context of possession loss. Study 1 manipulated awe and examined participants’ responses in an imagined situation in which they lost a cherished possession. Study 2 manipulated awe and happiness and compared their effects on participants’ response to an actual loss in the form of points obtained and deducted during a laboratory task. In Study 3, daily experiences of awe, other positive emotions, and affect in response to actual loss, were measured using event sampling. In all studies, awe predicted lower negative affect towards loss of possessions. Implications of the function of awe in coping with loss and other future directions are discussed.
Article
Posted 3/2000. The previously unstudied emotion of elevation is described. Elevation appears to be the opposite of social disgust. It is triggered by witnessing acts of human moral beauty or virtue. Elevation involves a warm or glowing feeling in the chest, and it makes people want to become morally better themselves. Because elevation increases one's desire to affiliate with and help others, it provides a clear illustration of B. L. Fredrickson's (see record 2000-03082-001) broaden-and-build model of the positive emotions.
Article
Spiritual experiences are profound moments of personal transcendence, connection, and wonder. Five studies (total N = 1064) investigate how spiritual experiences induce feelings of awe, in both religious and non-religious people, through a sense of small self. Recalling spiritual experiences increased feelings of Awe (Studies 1–5), Small Self (Studies 2, 4, & 5), and Spiritual Humility (Studies 3 & 4), but did not impact Intellectual Humility (Studies 3 & 4). We thus note a paradox – spirituality promotes humility toward the divine, but not humility about one's beliefs. Moreover, the effect of spiritual experiences on Awe was mediated by feelings of Small Self (Studies 2, 4 & 5) and Spirituality Humility (Studies 3 & 4). The effects of spiritual experiences on Awe and Small Self were found in both religious and non-religious individuals, but religious people recalled more explicit religious events and life and death events as sources of spirituality, whereas non-religious people were more likely to report experiences in nature, peak experiences, science, and yoga/ meditation as spiritual experiences. Though religious and non-religious people may generate different types of spiritual experiences, we conclude that spirituality induces awe through the feelings of small self that are shared by religious and non-religious individuals, and this may help us to understand the meaning of spirituality without religion.