Content uploaded by Christina Cross
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Christina Cross on Nov 18, 2019
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Racial/Ethnic Differences in the Association Between Family Structure and Children’s
Education
Christina J. Cross
Department of Sociology
Center for Population and Development Studies
Harvard University
33 Kirkland Street
Cambridge, MA 02138
crossc@fas.harvard.edu
ABSTRACT
While an extensive literature has shown that children raised by both biological parents fare better
academically than children raised in any other family structure, there has been little research to
explain an important finding: living apart from a biological parent is less negatively
consequential for racial/ethnic minority children than white children. To address this gap, I test
two explanations that have been posited to account for racial/ethnic differences in the association
between family structure and children’s educational attainment: socioeconomic stress and
extended family embeddedness. I assess whether racial/ethnic variation in these two mechanisms
explain group differences in the association between family structure and on-time high school
completion and college enrollment for white, black, and Hispanic children. Results indicate that
both socioeconomic stress and extended family embeddedness attenuate the effect of family
structure on these two measures of educational attainment, though the former to a much greater
extent. Differences in socioeconomic resources accounted for up to nearly 50% of the gap in
these outcomes, and extended family embeddedness explained roughly 15-20%. These findings
lend support for the socioeconomic stress hypothesis, which posits that the negative effect of
familial disruption may be less independently impactful for racial/ethnic groups facing many
socioeconomic disadvantages to begin with. Results are less consistent with the hypothesis that
racial/ethnic minority children’s deeper embeddedness in their extended family network protects
against the negative effects of familial disruption.
Keywords: Family Structure, Race/Ethnicity, Extended Family, Educational Attainment, Family
Diversity, Child Wellbeing
2
Introduction
Decades of research have shown that children fare better academically when they
are raised in a two-biological-parent family (i.e., a household with a child and his or her
birth mother and biological father; McLanahan & Percheski, 2008). Children who live
apart from a parent during childhood are less likely to finish high school, attend college,
or earn a bachelor’s degree (Amato & Keith, 1991; Hetherington, Camara, &
Featherman, 1983; Manning & Lamb, 2003; McLanahan & Sandefur, 1994). A growing
body of research has demonstrated, however, that the effects of family structure are not
equal across all groups. Studies have found that this negative association between
familial disruption and children’s educational attainment is significantly weaker for
minority children than white children (e.g., Amato, 2000; Shaw, Winslow, & Flanagan,
1999; Sun & Li, 2007). While they have yet to be formally tested, two main mechanisms
have been posited to explain racial/ethnic differences in the effect of family structure on
children’s academic outcomes: socioeconomic stress and extended family embeddedness
(McLoyd, Cauce, Takeuchi, & Wilson, 2000).
Using nationally representative, longitudinal data this study investigates the extent
to which racial/ethnic variation in these two mechanisms mediates the association
between family structure and two important measures of educational attainment: on-time
high school completion and college enrollment. This paper builds on previous work in
several key ways. First, unlike prior research that typically uses cross-sectional or short-
term measures of family structure, I construct children’s full histories of family structure
(from birth to age 17), including both nuclear and extended family household
configurations. My analyses also include lifetime measures of a wide range of family
3
factors including parents’ employment and health statuses and family wealth. This allows
me to better articulate how cumulative childhood family experiences influence long-term
outcomes for children. Second, this study moves beyond black-white comparisons that
have traditionally characterized research in this area to incorporate Hispanics, who now
represent the largest minority group in the U.S. (Passel & Cohn, 2008 [inconsistencies in
racial categorizations over time in the dataset and small cell sizes do not permit inclusion
of other racial/ethnic groups]). Given group differences in levels of exposure to
socioeconomic stress, participation in extended family networks, and rates of two-parent
households, including Hispanic families provides a unique opportunity to compare
families of color to one another and to non-Hispanic whites and to assess whether
patterns of association for Hispanic children are similar to those of black or white
children, or altogether distinctive. Third, as it relates to measures of extended family
embeddedness, instead of using coresidence with extended relatives as the sole proxy for
extended family support, I also include indicators of the levels of practical and emotional
support that parents receive from family members. Including additional measures of
support allows me to better approximate the total amount of extended family support that
is available to parents, both within and across households.
Understanding whether racial/ethnic differences in socioeconomic and extended
family resources help explain the differential association between family structure and
children’s educational attainment has implications for research and policy. To begin, a
focus on group differences in the effects of family structure advances a more diverse
portrait of American families, one that more adequately reflects the longstanding and
growing racial and ethnic diversity of the United States, which has been lacking in extant
4
family research (Gerstel, 2011; McLoyd et al., 2000). It also provides an important
corrective to work that assumes the primacy of the white middle class two-biological-
parent family model (Coontz, 2004; Roschelle, 1999). If minority children are less
vulnerable to the adverse effects of parental separation than white children, then
valorizing the two-biological-parent family over other family forms is unwarranted and
may prove to be unfruitful in reducing racial/ethnic disparities in child outcomes. Further,
contemporary policy discussions about family structure center on questions concerning
the role of the two-parent family in promoting child wellbeing. (U.S. House of
Representatives, 1996). If racial/ethnic differences in socioeconomic resources mediate
the relationship between family structure and educational attainment, then what deserves
policy attention is not minority families’ deviation from the two-biological-parent family
model, but rather the social structures that produce and maintain racialized
socioeconomic inequities. Moreover, many welfare reform policies assume that extended
families will act as a social safety net to their families during hard times (Cherlin &
Seltzer, 2014). However, recent studies have called into question the viability of the
extended family safety net (Garrett-Peters & Burton, 2016; McDonald & Armstrong,
2001). Examining the extent to which embeddedness in one’s extended family network
serves as a buffer against the negative effects of familial disruption can better inform
policies that encourage reliance on extended family networks to enhance child wellbeing.
Background and Theoretical Perspectives
Family Structure and Children’s Educational Attainment
Children raised in two-biological-parent families tend to fare better academically
than children raised in any other family form (Brown, 2010). In their landmark study on
5
single-parent families and child wellbeing, McLanahan and Sandefur (1994) found that
youth living with both biological parents were less likely to drop out of high school and
more likely to enroll in college, compared to youth living with single or divorced parents
(though the negative effect of family structure was somewhat smaller for college
enrollment than high school graduation). Subsequent studies have produced similar
findings related to these outcomes (e.g., Deleire & Kalil, 2002; Ginther & Pollak, 2004;
Hill, Yeung, & Duncan, 2001).
Theoretical explanations that account for why children growing up outside of a
two-biological-parent family are at an elevated risk of experiencing academic problems
can be classified into three categories: (1) economic resources (2) parental socialization,
and (3) stress and family instability (Amato, 2005; Brown, 2010; Fomby & Cherlin,
2007; Wu, 1996). Economic explanations maintain that some family structures,
particularly single-parent families, tend to be economically disadvantaged, having fewer
economic resources such as income and wealth available to facilitate effective family
functioning. Economic deprivation undermines parents’ ability to provide the material
goods and services needed to cultivate children’s academic growth (Amato, 2005).
Parental socialization explanations focus on child-rearing, foremost parenting practices.
The argument here is that there is typically a weaker parental authority structure in
single-, step-, and cohabiting families than in two-biological-parent families, and there is
less monitoring and supervision, which in turn reduces the quality of parenting that
children receive (Amato 2005, Brown 2010; Sandberg and Hofferth, 2001). Family
instability explanations assert that the number of family transitions a child experiences
during childhood, independent of family type, has negative consequences for children’s
6
academic performance. The idea behind this is that children fare better when family
structure is stable; transitions lead to stress due to people moving in and out of the
household, potentially disrupting family routines and leading to new role configurations.
This familial disruption may also be accompanied by residential and school changes, that
potentially further undermine children’s educational outcomes (Fomby and Cherlin,
2007; Teachman, 2008; Wu 1996).
In addition to these explanations, researchers have noted that selection may also
account for the association between family structure and children’s academic outcomes.
This perspective suggests that it is not family structure or instability per se that influence
child wellbeing. Rather, children perform better in two-biological-parent families because
adults who form and maintain such households may be more well-adjusted and
socioeconomically advantaged in the first place (Brown, 2010; Hofferth, 2006).
Racial/Ethnic Disparities in the Effects of Family Structure on Educational Outcomes
While numerous studies have examined the relationship between family structure
and children’s educational attainment, only a few have investigated whether the effect of
family structure varies across racial/ethnic groups. Among those focused on racial/ethnic
differences, scholars generally find that the negative effects of living outside of a two-
biological-parent family are weaker for minority children than white children. For
example, in a rigorous meta-analysis of research on the long-term effects of divorce,
Amato & Keith (1991) found that the magnitude of the association between parental
divorce and educational attainment was nearly twice that for white children than for black
children. Amato (2001) found similar racial differences in his follow-up meta-analysis a
decade later. In another study focused on high school academic achievement, Smith
7
(1997) observed a stronger effect of parental separation on children’s grades for white
youth, relative to their black peers. Further, McLanahan and Sandefur (1994) found that
the proportionate increase in risk of dropping out of high school due to familial disruption
was significantly greater for whites than for blacks or Hispanics. However, for high
school idleness, they found that the effect of familial disruption was stronger for blacks
than whites. This latter finding suggests that the differential association between family
structure and educational outcomes may depend on the outcome of interest under
investigation. Two major mechanisms have been hypothesized to explain why minority
children may be less vulnerable to the negative consequences of living outside of a two-
biological-parent family than white children: socioeconomic stress and extended family
embeddedness.
Socioeconomic stress hypothesis. In the U.S., race and ethnicity are fundamental
axes of inequality along which socioeconomic resources are distributed (Bobo, Kluegel,
& Smith, 1997; Bonilla-Silva, 2006; Conley, 2009). Due to historic and contemporary
structural racism, minority families on average have less access to socioeconomic
resources such as income, wealth, and quality neighborhoods than white families
(Charles, 2006; Krysan, Couper, Farley, & Foreman, 2009; Mazumder, 2008; Proctor,
Semega & Kollar, 2016; Wilson, 1987). Because of this, minority children are more
likely to be exposed to socioeconomically stressful environments, and some scholars
have suggested that their exposure to sustained socioeconomic deprivation means that the
additional stress incurred by living apart from a parent is only marginally impactful,
above and beyond existing social disadvantages (McLoyd et al., 2000; Smith, 1997). This
is referred to as the socioeconomic stress hypothesis.
8
This hypothesis is derived from family stress theory, which focuses on a family’s
ability to cope with stressful situations and crises. It maintains that a families’ ability to
respond and adapt to stressors depends on the nature and number of stressful events, the
family resources available to facilitate coping, and family members’ perception of the
event (McCubbin, 1993; McCubbin & McCubbin, 1989; Friedman 1998; Fomby et al.
2010). Given the cumulative number of socioeconomic stressors that minority children
face, and the extent to which the pervasiveness of these stressors is more normative, one
might expect that the independent effect of an event like parental divorce to be smaller
for this group. On the other hand, for children of a racial/ethnic group who on average
benefit from greater access to socioeconomic resources (i.e., white children), one might
expect that the effect of familial disruption may be more pronounced.
Extended family embeddedness hypothesis. Several studies suggest that minority
children are more deeply embedded in extended family networks compared to white
children (Burton, 1992; Hunter, 1997; Margolis, Fosco, & Stormshak, 2014; Sarkisian &
Gerstel, 2012; Stack 1974; Taylor, 1986). Indeed, research on the extended family
networks of Black Americans finds that this group is engaged in ongoing and reciprocal
exchanges of practical, emotional, and financial support, and black families are more
involved in practical support (e.g., help with household chores, transportation, and child
care) than white families (Jayakody, Chatters, & Taylor, 1993; Sarkisian & Gerstel, 2004;
Stack & Burton, 1993; Taylor, Chae, Lincoln, & Chatters, 2015). They also tend to live
in closer proximity to extended relatives than whites (Farber, Miller-Cribbs, & Reitmeier,
2005). Although limited, extant research on Hispanic extended family networks suggest
that they are more integrated and may offer higher levels of support than white extended
9
family networks (Baca Zinn & Wells, 2000; Kamo, 2000). Greater involvement in these
extended family networks may reduce of some the negative psychosocial effects
associated with parental conflict or separation (Lamborn & Nguyen, 2004; Pittman, 2007;
McLoyd et al., 2000; Smith, 1997). This is known as the extended family embeddedness
hypothesis.
This hypothesis is grounded in social capital theory, which asserts that social
capital within the family, that is supportive relations between parents and children, is
crucial for children’s intellectual growth (Coleman, 1988). Social capital within the
family gives children access to other forms of parental capital that help facilitate
children’s educational performance (i.e., economic, cultural, and human capital)
(Coleman, 1988). The absence of a parent from the household may diminish the
availability of these resources to children, and thus undermine their academic success.
Extended family networks, however, may increase the number of nurturant and
supportive adults available in the child’s life to provide such resources (McLoyd, et al.
2000). Thus, to the extent that extended family networks contribute additional resources
such as monitoring and supervision and familial involvement in education, one would
expect that embeddedness in these networks may weaken or even countervail the
negative effects of parental absence. However, if demands for support exchanges increase
stress and diminish household resources, one would anticipate that greater embeddedness
in extended family networks would adversely affect children’s educational attainment
(Pilkauskas, Campbell, & Wimer, 2017; Dominguez & Watkins, 2003).
The Current Study
10
While plausible, the relative importance of these mechanisms in explaining racial
differences in the effects of family structure has yet to be examined (but see Fomby et al.,
2010 for an investigation of racial/ethnic differences in the effects of family instability on
children’s risk behaviors and see Sun & Li, 2007 for a test of the socioeconomic stress
hypothesis on parental divorce/separation during late adolescence). This study seeks to
overcome this limitation by using nationally representative longitudinal data to assess
whether racial/ethnic differences in socioeconomic stress and extended family
embeddedness account for group differences in the association between family structure
and children’s educational attainment. I begin by investigating whether children’s
lifetime experience of family structure influences their on-time high school completion
and college enrollment. I then determine if any observed associations between family
structure and these two measures of educational attainment differ by race/ethnicity.
Finally, I evaluate the extent to which factors associated with socioeconomic stress and
extended family embeddedness explain any group differences in associations by family
structure.
Method
Data
This study draws on data from the Panel Study of Income Dynamics (1985-2015)
and its two youth-centered supplements, the Child Development Supplement (CDS)
(1997-2007) and the Transition into Adulthood Supplement (TAS) (2005-2015). The
PSID began in 1968 as a nationally-representative sample of nearly 5,000 U.S.
households. Original sample members and their descendants were followed annually until
1997 and have been followed biennially since then. To maintain population
11
representativeness, in 1997, a sample refresher added approximately 500 households
headed by immigrants who had entered the United States since 1968. At each wave, the
household head or the spouse or cohabiting partner of the head reports on household
composition, and household members’ employment, income, educational attainment, and
health status. In 2015, the study collected information on nearly 25,000 individuals in
approximately 9,000 households.
While the PSID has always collected some information about children, in 1997,
the PSID supplemented its main data collection with the CDS, which provides additional
information on up to two children aged 0 to 12 years per household. Information is
obtained through interviews with primary and secondary caregivers and with older
children, and through assessments and interviewer observations. Children and their
caregivers were re-interviewed in 2002 and 2007, or until children reached age 18. In
2005, the Transition to Adulthood Supplement (TAS) was developed to continue
following the original CDS cohort when they aged out of CDS or left high school. TAS
has continued to follow respondents biennially as they complete their educations and
enter the labor force. In 2015, TAS respondents were between 18 and 28 years old. My
analytic sample includes children who were present in at least 50% of the waves in which
they could have been observed in the PSID core data and lived with mothers with a
known union history, who responded to at least one wave of the Transition to Adulthood
Supplement between 2005 and 2015, and who provided information on their educational
status and attainment (N=2,589). This includes 86% of the age-eligible original CDS
sample. Young adults who were not included in this analytic sample were typically
younger, Hispanic, and from families with lower household incomes and wealth at birth.
12
Weighted statistics are representative of young adults born between 1985 and 1997 who
were residing in the United States in 1997.
Measures
Dependent variables. Two dependent variables are used as indicators of
children’s educational attainment: on-time high school completion and college-
enrollment. On-time high school completion is a binary variable based on whether the
child (1) completed high school, and (2) graduated without repeating a grade in school.
This information is gathered from CDS and TAS. In CDS, individuals are asked whether
they ever repeated a grade in school, and in TAS, respondents are asked whether they
completed high school, and in which month and year they did so. If participants
responded affirmatively to having repeated a grade, they are assigned a value of 0,
regardless of completion status. If participants did not repeat a grade, but also did not
finish high school, they are also assigned a value of 0. Only respondents who did not
repeat a grade and finished high school are assigned a value of 1. I have chosen this
measure over any-time high school completion because high school graduation rates are
universally high in the U.S. (Chapman, Laird, Ifill, KewalRamani, 2011), but on-time
completion rates are significantly lower, and youth who do not graduate high school on-
time are more likely to never graduate, not pursue higher education, and be unemployed
(National Center for Education Statistics, 2014). College enrollment is also a binary
variable. Respondents are coded as 1 if they reported attending a two-year or four-year
college in at least one wave of the TAS in which they were observed. They are coded as 0
if they did not finish high school and/or if they did not report attending college in any of
the TAS waves in which they were observed. About one-quarter of college enrollees did
13
not report an initial enrollment date, so the analysis considers ever having been enrolled,
rather than enrollment immediately after high school.
Independent variable. Children’s lifetime experience of family structure (from
birth to age 17) is based on mother’s union status: (1) the proportion of childhood spent
living with two biological parents, (2) the proportion of childhood spent living with a
mother and a non-biological parent, and (3) the proportion of childhood spent with a
single mother. Following the example of Carlson & Corcoran (2001), this categorization
of single-mother families includes never married, divorced, widowed, and separated
mothers. Although these categories of single-parenthood are not identical, I combined
them due to sample-size limitations and to be consistent with prior research. Additionally,
a child’s non-biological parent may include either the married or cohabiting partner of the
child’s mother.
I used the PSID’s marriage and birth history files to construct the family structure
variable. Based on the child’s reported birth month and year and his or her mother’s
reported start and end date for each of her unions (also reported by month and year), I
determined the union status of each child’s mother when he or she was born, and in each
subsequent year of his or her life until age 17. This approach is an improvement upon
measurements that rely solely on PSID relationship status variables and/or household
roster information, as mother’s union status information is not available in non-interview
years after 1997 or in years in which she did not participate in the survey. If a child’s
mother was unobserved in one year, but she reports information on her union status in a
subsequent year of the marriage history file, I used the reported union status start and end
dates to recover information about her union status for missing years, retrospectively. To
14
obtain the proportion of childhood spent in each family structure type, I totaled the
number of waves a child is observed living in a given family structure and divided this
value by the total number of waves a child is observed before age 18.
Socioeconomic stress. To test the socioeconomic stress hypothesis, I used several
measures that have been previously identified as indicators of children’s socioeconomic
circumstances: (Conley, 2009; Fomby et al. 2010): parents’ education, self-reported
measures of neighborhood quality, safety, and homeownership, mother’s age at child’s
birth, and lifetime measures of family wealth and parents’ employment and health status.
Mothers’ education is specified as the highest level of education completed by the child’s
mother: less than high school, high school, some college, and bachelor’s degree or higher
(reference). Education was used instead of family income in this analysis due to high
levels of collinearity between the two variables and because income is likely to be
endogenous (affected by mother’s union status). For neighborhood quality, I used the
child’s primary caregiver’s (PCG) self- reported rating of their neighborhood as a place
to raise children (1=excellent/very good, 2=good, 3=fair/poor). Neighborhood safety was
measured based on the PCG’s report of how safe they perceived their neighborhood to be
to walk around alone after dark (0=neighborhood is somewhat or extremely dangerous,
1=neighborhood is completely or fairly safe). Homeownership is a binary variable that is
based on the PCG’s report of the percentage of their neighbors who owned their home
(0=less than 50%, 1=more than 50%). Mother’s age at birth is a categorical variable: 19
and under (reference), 20-29, and 30+. Average family wealth and parents’ employment
and health statuses are all lifetime measures that I created by summing the values for
each of these variables across all the waves that a child was observed and dividing them
15
by the total number of waves that a child was observed. For family wealth, I included the
inverse hyperbolic sine of wealth excluding home equity to account for the highly
skewed distribution of wealth in the sample. Mothers’ employment and health status are
both dichotomous variables indicating whether the child’s mother is employed and in
good health (0=no, 1=yes).
Extended family embeddedness. I included five indicators to test the extended
family embeddedness hypothesis: (1) coresidence with extended relatives, (2) geographic
proximity to extended relatives, (3) frequency of contact with extended relatives, and (4)
the PCG’s reported satisfaction with the level of practical support and (5) emotional
support he or she received from extended family members. Coresidence with extended
relatives was determined based on information from the household roster and from the
Family Identification Mapping System (FIMS). A child was identified as living with an
extended relative if he or she resided with an adult relative (by birth, adoption, or
marriage) who is not his or her parent or sibling. This includes grandparents, aunts,
uncles, and adult cousins; it does not include nonrelatives such as friends or boarders or
underage extended family members. Following Fomby et al. (2010), I distinguish
between short-term and long-term extended family coresidence with a three-category
variable: 1=never lived with an extended relative (reference), 2=lived with an extended
relative for less than a quarter of childhood, and 3=lived with an extended relative for at
least a quarter of childhood. Geographic proximity to extended relatives is measured
based on the PCG’s report of the number of family members (besides the child’s other
parent if they are not present in the household) who live in the same neighborhood. Due
to the highly positively skewed distribution of responses, I constructed a categorical
16
variable that collapsed responses into quartiles. Frequency of contact with extended
relatives is based on how often the PCG reports that the nuclear family “gets together
with relatives or friends”: less than once a month (reference), one to three times a month,
or once a week or more. Measures of practical and emotional support from extended
family is based on the PCG’s report of how satisfied he or she is with the level of support
that he or she is receiving from family members besides the child’s other parent.
Responses are based on a 7-point Likert scale with 1 indicating complete dissatisfaction
and 7 indicating complete satisfaction. Higher values on these scales signify higher
levels of satisfaction with practical and emotional support received by the primary
caregiver.
Moderator. Race/ethnicity is coded into four categories based on the PCG’s report
of the child’s race: (1) non-Hispanic white (reference); (2) non-Hispanic black; (3)
Hispanic; and (4) other race.
Control variables. In addition to these variables, I also control for several
demographic characteristics of the child: gender, region where the child grew up, and the
average number of children living in the child’s household. Gender is measured as male
(reference) or female. Region (South vs non-South) measures the proportion of all
observed waves that a child lived in a state in the US South. Average number of children
in the household was constructed in a similar fashion. At each wave that a child was
observed, he or she was assigned a value indicating the total number of children living in
his or her household. These values were then totaled and averaged across the total
number of waves that a child was observed.
17
All covariates except child’s race, gender, age, region, family income, and
number of children present in the household included missing data on at least some
observations. To restore missing cases and improve the generalizability of my findings, I
used multiple imputation with chained equations in Stata 15. The imputation model
included all dependent, independent, and control variables with missing values, as well as
child’s race, gender, and age, a family-level identifier shared by all related individuals in
the sample, and stratum and clustering indicators. My multivariate regressions used
multiply imputed datasets where covariates include imputed values but dependent
variables are not imputed (von Hippel 2007).
Analytic Strategy
I use logistic regression analysis to examine the relationship between children’s
lifetime experience of family structure and their educational attainment. I begin with
baseline models that estimate the strength of the association between the main effects of
family structure and on-time high school completion and college enrollment, taking into
account the aforementioned control variables (Model 1), before including interaction
terms for white, black, and Hispanic children (Model 2). I then separately add in
indicators of the socioeconomic stress (Model 3) and extended family embeddedness
(Model 4) hypotheses, before jointly including all covariates (Model 5) to assess the
extent to which these mechanisms help explain group differences in the association of
family structure between family structure and educational attainment. Next, I conduct
Wald tests to determine whether the family structure-race/ethnicity interactions are
jointly significant. Then, I calculate predicted probabilities to show how, if at all, the
effects of family structure on high school completion and college enrollment vary by
18
race/ethnicity. To appropriately compare changes in coefficients across logistic
regression models and to quantify the amount of variance in the two dependent variables
that is explained by socioeconomic stress and extended family embeddedness, I calculate
the average partial effects of these two sets of factors using the Karlson, Hohm and
Breen (KHB) method for decomposing total effects (Kohler, Karlson and Holm 2011).
Results
Descriptive Results
Table 1 displays the distribution of the dependent variables and sample characteristics by
race/ethnicity. Statistically significant group differences (black vs. Hispanic, black vs.
white, and Hispanic vs. white) are indicated with footnotes. Starting with outcome
variables, I found that there were significant differences by race/ethnicity in educational
attainment. White youths had the highest rates of on-time high school completion (85%)
and college enrollment (81%) among the three groups. Hispanic youths followed with
74% for both outcomes, and black youths with 61% and 70%, respectively.
Consistent with findings from cross-sectional research (U.S. Department of
Health and Human Services, Health Resources and Services Administration, 2014), the
longitudinal estimates of exposure to various family structures indicate that black
children less of their childhood living in a two-biological-parent and more time living
with a single mother, compared to white and Hispanic children. Black children also spent
more time living with their mothers and her partner who is not their biological parent
(both married and cohabiting), relative to their white peers, whereas Hispanic youths and
white youths spent similar amounts of time with their mother’s non-biological partner.
19
Turning to socioeconomic stress, black and Hispanic children had significantly
less access to socioeconomic resources during childhood than their white peers. For
instance, the average family wealth for white children was roughly 1.5 to two times
higher than that of Hispanic and black children, respectively. Similarly, white parents
were approximately 1.5 times more likely to rate their neighborhood as “excellent” or
“very good”, relative to black and Hispanic parents. Black and Hispanic children were,
however, more likely to live with or in close proximity to extended relatives, and their
parents expressed higher levels of satisfaction with the amount of practical support
received from extended family members. Eighty percent of white children never lived in
an extended family, compared to approximately 40% of black children and 60% of
Hispanic children. Further, white parents reported significantly lower levels of
satisfaction with the amount of practical support received from extended family members
than black and Hispanic parents.
In sum, descriptive results generally support findings from prior research
indicating racial/ethnic differences in levels of educational attainment and family
structure. They also show that on average, black and Hispanic children are confronted
with more socioeconomic hardship than their white peers. Moreover, they lend some
evidence to support the assertion that minority children are more deeply embedded in
their extended family networks than white children. The multivariate analyses in the
following section assess the extent to which racial/ethnic differences in these family
resources account for group differences in the effects of family structure.
Multivariate Results
20
On-time high school completion. In the baseline model (Model 1) for Table 2, the
variables indicating the main effects of family structure show that more time spent living
with a single-mother or with a mother and a non-biological parent are associated with a
lower likelihood of finishing high-school on-time, compared with more time spent with
two biological parents. The main effects of race/ethnicity are statistically significant,
indicating that black and Hispanic youth are less likely to complete high school on-time,
relative to white youth. The interaction term between single-mother family and
race/ethnicity (Model 2) is highly significant and positive for black youth (OR 3.049),
signifying a weaker negative association between family structure and on-time high
school completion for black youth, compared to white youth. The association between
family structure and this outcome was not different between white and Hispanic youth.
Model 3 accounts for indicators of socioeconomic stress. Children who spent
more time not living in a two-biological-parent family remained less likely to graduate
high school on-time (though the coefficient for time spent living with mother and a non-
biological parent is only marginally significant). The interaction term for single-mother
family and black children is no longer statistically significant, signifying that
socioeconomic stressors help explain the differential association between family structure
and on-time high-school completion between blacks and whites. Indicators of
socioeconomic stress, namely parents’ education, age, and health status are positively
related to high school completion and appear to be explaining much of the variation in
this outcome. Children whose parents earned a bachelor’s degree or higher and those
whose mother was older at childbirth and whose mother spent more time in good health
during their childhood were more likely to graduate on-time.
21
Model 4 includes indicators of extended family embeddedness. Here, we see no
change in the significance of the association between family structure and on-time high
completion or the family structure-race/ethnicity interaction term for black youth and
high school completion. This suggests that indicators of extended family embeddedness
explained little of the main interaction effects of family structure on high school
graduation between blacks and whites. When both sets of factors are taken into account
in the full model (model 5), the main effects for family structure are no longer
statistically significant, and neither is the interaction term for blacks. Thus, combined,
indicators of socioeconomic stress and extended family embeddedness help explain the
relationship between family structure and high school completion, as well as the
differential effect of family structure for black youth. Wald tests indicate that the family
structure-race/ethnicity interactions are statistically significant in models 2 through 5
(Table 2), providing evidence that the association between family structure and on-time
high school completion differs for at least one group (in this case, black children).
To better understand the interaction terms for race/ethnicity and family structure, I
calculated and graphed the predicted probabilities for each family structure-race/ethnicity
combination from Table 2 model 2 that had statistically significant differences by
race/ethnicity, setting all other values to their mean or modal values. Figure 1 displays
these values for on-time high school completion. This graph shows that as black children
spend longer periods of time in a single-mother family, the change in slope for the effect
of living in a single-mother family declines less sharply than the change in slope for
whites. This pattern indicates that more time spent in this family structure is less
negatively consequential for black youth. This pattern is consistent for Hispanic children,
22
though as demonstrated in Table 2, this differential effect is not statistically significant at
p<.05. Summarizing Table 2 and Figure 1, the results show that living in a single-mother
family for the majority of childhood is less detrimental for black children’s on-time high
school completion, compared to white children.
College enrollment. In the baseline model for college enrollment (Table 3), the
main effect of family structure indicates that children who spent some time in a single-
mother family or with a mother and a non-biological parent are less likely to enroll in
college, relative to those who spent a greater proportion of childhood living in a two-
biological-parent family. The main family structure-race/ethnicity interaction terms for
blacks and Hispanics, however, are not significant (Model 2). This suggests that the
effect of family structure on college enrollment does not differ by race/ethnicity.
Model 3 adds in factors related to socioeconomic stress. Unlike the baseline
model, the main effect of family structure is no longer statistically significant, indicating
that socioeconomic factors help explain the association between the main effect of family
structure and college enrollment. In particular, parents’ education, family wealth, and
mother’s age at childbirth are positively related to college enrollment. On the contrary,
when indicators of extended family embeddedness are taken into account (Model 4), the
main effect of family structure remains similar to that in the baseline model. This
suggests that the extended family embeddedness hypothesis does little to explain the
association between family structure and college enrollment. Extended family
embeddedness does appear to be independently associated with college enrollment,
however. Specifically, children who spent some time living with an extended relative had
23
a roughly 40% lower odds of attending college than those who never lived with an
extended relative during childhood.
In the full model, when I account for factors related to both socioeconomic stress
and extended family embeddedness, the main effects of family structure are
nonsignificant. Similar to Model 3, the coefficients for parents’ education, family wealth,
and mother’s age are highly and positively related to college enrollment. Coresidence
with extended family members is no longer related to college-enrollment. This finding
suggests that socioeconomic factors are more predictive of college enrollment than
family structure itself or extended family embeddedness. Wald tests show that the family
structure-race/ethnicity interactions are not statistically significant in each model (see
Table 3), providing further evidence that the association between family structure and
college enrollment does not differ by race/ethnicity.
Table 4 displays the average partial effect of family structure in the baseline and
full models estimated using the Karlson, Hohm and Breen (KHB) method for
decomposing total effects (Kohler, Karlson and Holm 2011). The average partial effect
indicates the difference in the predicted probability that a child experienced an outcome
of interest (in this case, on-time high school completion and college enrollment), given
the time he or she spent in a single-mother family or with a mother and a non-biological
parent, relative to time spent in a two-biological-parent family. The percentage change in
the average partial effect between the baseline and full models demonstrates the
difference in how much of the variance is explained by family structure for each outcome
before and after taking into account indicators of socioeconomic stress and extended
family embeddedness and controlling for sociodemographic characteristics.
24
Average partial effects from the KHB decomposition method reveal that
indicators of both socioeconomic stress and extended family embeddedness significantly
attenuated the relationship between family structure and children’s educational
attainment, though the latter did so to a much lesser degree. The reduced model estimated
that on average, the probability of on-time high school completion for each additional
year a child spent living with his or her mother and a non-biological parent was 17.6
percentage points lower than for each additional year spent in a two-biological-parent
family. In the full model, once indicators of socioeconomic stress are taken into account,
the average partial effect decreased to 10.2 percentage points. Thus, the difference in the
size of average partial effects in the reduced and full models indicates that 42% of the gap
in on-time high school completion between children who spend an additional year living
with a mother and a non-biological parent compared to with two biological parents is
explained by socioeconomic factors included in the full model. Extended family
embeddedness, on the other hand, explained 15% of the gap. When we compare
differences between the amount of time spent living with a single mother compared to
two biological parents, we see that socioeconomic factors explained 22% and extended
family embeddedness explained 13% of the gap in on-time high school completion. The
results for college enrollment echo those of on-time high school completion. For each
additional year a child spent with his or her mother and a non-biological parent compared
to with both biological parents, socioeconomic factors accounted for 48% of the variance
in college enrollment, whereas extended family embeddedness accounted for 19% of the
variance in this outcome. Socioeconomic factors explained 36% and extended family
embeddedness explained 13% of the gap in college enrollment between children who
25
spent more time living with a single mother, compared to those who spent more time
living with both parents.
Sensitivity Analysis
I assess the robustness of my findings in several ways. First, research suggests
that children who experience multiple transitions in family structure face worse
educational outcomes than children raised in stable families, independent of the type of
stable family structure (Fomby & Cherlin, 2007). In a supplemental analysis, I controlled
for whether a child experienced one or more changes in family structure during
childhood. Children who spent more time outside of a two-biological-parent family were
still less likely to complete high school on-time and enroll in college, and black children
in single-mother families remained marginally more likely to finish high school on-time,
relative to white children who spent similar periods of time in this family structure.
Second, I considered an alternative approach to measuring family structure that
captures both mother’s relationship type and transitions (but not biological parental
status): (1) stable two-parent family, (2) stable one-parent family, (3) two-parent to one-
parent family (4), one-parent to two-parent family, and (5) two or more family
transitions. Consistent with my main findings, children who grew up in a stable two-
parent family were more likely to graduate high school on-time, compared to children
raised in any other family type. Black children raised in a stable one-parent family were
more likely to complete high school on-time than white children raised in a stable one-
parent family.
Third, my analytic sample includes children who were present in at least half of
the waves in which they could have been observed in the PSID. I also ran my analysis on
26
children who were present in all waves of the study. Results from this more restricted
sample are representative of the results that I obtained when I included individuals who
were missing in up to 50% of waves. Further, my analytic sample also contains 326
sibling pairs. Given that observations between siblings are non-independent, I ran all
models clustering on family unit identifiers rather than on the indicators of the PSID’s
multistage sampling design and observed similar results.
Finally, research has suggested that there is negative selection into extended
families; that is, children who live with extended relatives may do so because their
parents lack the resources to live independently (Chase-Lansdale, Brooks-Gunn, &
Zamsky, 1994; Pilkauskas & Dunifon, 2016). If this is the case, then the observed
negative association between extended family coresidence and on-time high school
completion and college enrollment may be attributable, in part to, to the fact that children
raised in these households are less well-off to begin with.
To account for factors related to the selection into an extended family, I used
propensity score matching (PSM) methods to estimate the average treatment effect of
extended family coresidence. This was accomplished by comparing the on-time high
school completion and college enrollment of children who spent time in an extended
family during childhood with children who never spent time in an extended family but
are otherwise similar in terms of family background characteristics. Measures in the
matching model included the extended family coresidence status (treatment), child’s
gender, race, sex, and region, number of children in the household at birth, mother’s
education, and mother’s age, union, and health status at birth. In a sample balanced on
these characteristics, children who lived in an extended family for less than or a quarter
27
or more of their childhood were not significantly less likely to finish high school on-time
or enroll in college, compared to those who never lived in an extended family. These
findings suggest that the negative association between extended family coresidence and
children’s educational attainment is largely explained by the negative selection into this
living arrangement. Findings from PSM methods may differ from those of the logistic
regression analyses, which regress toward the mean, if the treatment group (i.e., those
likely to experience extended family coresidence) and the control group do not have
substantial overlap on characteristics that distinguish the two groups. All in all, however,
the results of the sensitivity checks considered here lend confidence to the robustness of
my main findings.
Discussion
This study used nationally representative, longitudinal data to examine the
association between children’s lifetime experience of family structure and their
educational attainment, the extent to which this association varied by race/ethnicity, and
the relative importance of two hypothesized mechanisms in explaining any observed
group differences. More specifically, I assessed the relationship between childhood
family structure and on-time high school completion and college enrollment and tested
whether racial/ethnic differences in socioeconomic stress and extended family
embeddedness mediate these relationships for white, black, and Hispanic children.
Results from this study confirm findings from prior research demonstrating that children
raised outside of a two-biological-parent family are at an elevated risk of experiencing
lower educational attainment than those raised in this family type. They are also
consistent with previous studies showing that the consequences of living in a single-
28
mother family are less negatively impactful for black children, relative to white children
who spend similar amounts of time in this family structure (e.g., McLanahan & Sandefur,
1994). This suggests that the experience of single-parenthood for white children has
limited generalizability to black children, as the two groups may adjust differently to life
in a single-parent family.
I did not, however, observe racial differences in the link between family
structure and college enrollment. This indicates that the differential association between
family structure and educational attainment, or child wellbeing more generally, may
depend on the outcome of interest. Indeed, prior research has identified stronger family
structure effects for more proximate outcomes such as behavioral and psychological
wellbeing, than more distal ones such as educational attainment (Amato, 2005), and only
in some instances have researchers observed racial/differences in these outcomes
(Dunifon & Kowaleski-Jones, 2002; Manning & Brown, 2006; McLanahan & Sandefur,
1994). Overall, findings on racial differences suggest that our examination of the impact
of family structure on children’s educational attainment, and wellbeing broadly speaking,
is best approached from a pluralistic perspective, one that does not privilege one family
type over another, since family structure does not always have the same social
consequences for all groups.
There were no observed differences between whites and Hispanics in the effects
of family structure on either measure of educational attainment. While this finding is
consistent with Fomby et al. (2010)’s work on racial differences in the effect of family
instability on adolescent’s risk behavior, it does not preclude the possibility of group
differences for other dimensions of child wellbeing. Given the limited number of studies
29
that focus on Hispanic children, more work is needed to investigate potential differential
effects of family structure on child wellbeing for this group.
What accounts for racial/ethnic variation in the association between family
structure and educational attainment? Findings from this study suggest that both
socioeconomic stress and extended family embeddedness attenuate the effect of family
structure on on-time high school completion and college enrollment. Socioeconomic
stress, however, appears to have greater relative influence. Differences in socioeconomic
resources accounted for 22-48% of the gap in on-time high school completion and
college enrollment between amount of time spent in a two-biological-parent family and
those living with a mother and nonbiological parent or single mother. Extended family
embeddedness factors helped explain roughly 15-20% of the variance in these outcomes.
These findings lend support for the socioeconomic stress hypothesis, which posits that
the negative effect of familial disruption may be less independently impactful for children
of racial/ethnic groups facing a large number of socioeconomic disadvantages to begin
with. In contrast, they provide little evidence to support the hypothesis that racial/ethnic
minority children’s deeper embeddedness in their extended family network protects
against the negative effects of familial disruption.
In interpreting these findings, it is important to acknowledge that exposure to
socioeconomic hardship is harmful to children, irrespective of race/ethnicity and that
black children are not uniquely impervious to the negative consequences of familial
disruption. Rather, they may have developed adaptive strategies derived from their
structural position in society that aids them in coping with this stressor. Indeed, research
suggests that while exposure to racially and economically problematic life circumstances
30
is detrimental to child wellbeing, black children may develop competencies that help
them respond to major life events such as parental separation (Smith and Carlson, 1997).
Future work should explore racial/ethnic variation in children’s responses to familial
disruption and its link to child outcomes.
Given the key role that socioeconomic factors play in explaining the relationship
between family structure and children’s educational attainment, these results call into
question whether welfare funds should be diverted to encourage the formation and
maintenance of two-parent nuclear families and discourage nonmarital childbearing (U.S.
House of Representatives, 1996), rather than being spent directly on alleviating financial
hardships for disadvantaged groups. They also suggest that expectations for the extended
family to compensate for family hardship may be misplaced. Extended family
embeddedness does not appear to fully protect against the negative impact of living
outside of a two-biological-parent family. Therefore, efforts aimed at improving child
wellbeing for disadvantaged groups may be more effective by focusing on reducing
socioeconomic hardships for low-income families and adopting reasonable expectations
for the extended family to meet the needs of its disadvantaged members.
Results should be interpreted within the context of the study’s limitations. First,
while I included a robust set of indicators of socioeconomic stress and extended family
embeddedness, the factors used in this study are not exhaustive. I would have liked to
include direct measures of the amount and frequency of practical and emotional support
that mothers received from extended family members, as well as measures of relationship
quality among extended family members, but was unable to do so due to limitations of
the data. This would allow me to more accurately assess the levels of extended family
31
support available to mothers in various family types, as well the amount of extended
family cohesion they experience. Second, the relatively small sample size for Hispanic
children diminishes my statistical power, making it difficult to observe statistically
significant differences and to generalize to the broader U.S. population for this group.
Third, although my use of the birth and marriage history files allows me to
retrospectively identify mothers’ union status between waves and during skipped waves, I
miss children whose mothers attrited. As noted earlier, individuals who attrited were
more likely to be economically disadvantaged. To the extent that children of
disadvantaged mothers are more likely to have lower levels of educational attainment,
attrition is likely to have underestimated the influence of socioeconomic stress on
children’s on-time completion and college enrollment. Lastly, while the two hypotheses
that I tested help partially account for racial/ethnic differences in the association between
family structure and children’s educational attainment, neither of them fully explain this
phenomenon. Future research should investigate how other factors related to children’s
familial and community context may account for these differences.
Despite its limitations, this study takes important steps towards understanding
why black children are less affected by family structure with regard to educational
attainment than their white peers. Using nationally representative longitudinal data, this
research shows that group differences in access to socioeconomic resources are an
important mechanism for explaining this gap. Combined, study findings highlight the
importance of examining racial/ethnic differences in the consequences of family structure
for children’s educational attainment and their wellbeing more generally.
32
References
Amato, P. R. (2000). The consequences of divorce for adults and children. Journal of
Marriage and Family, 62, 1269-1287.
Amato, P. R. (2001). Children of divorce in the 1990s: An update of the Amato and Keith
(1991) meta-analysis. Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 355-370.
Amato, P. R. (2005). The impact of family formation change on the cognitive, social, and
emotional well-being of the next generation. The Future of Children, 15, 75-96.
Amato, P. R., & Keith, B. (1991). Parental divorce and adult well-being: A meta-
analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 43-58.
Baca Zinn, M. & Wells, B. (2000). Diversity within Latino families: New lessons for
family social science. In D. H. Demo, K. R. Allen, & M. A. Fine (Eds.).
Handbook of Family Diversity (262–73). New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
Brown, S. L. (2010). Marriage and child well-being: Research and policy perspectives.
Journal of Marriage and Family, 72, 1059-1077.
Bobo, L., Kluegel, J. R., & Smith, R. A. (1997). Laissez faire racism: The crystallization
of a ‘kinder, gentler’ anti-black ideology. In S.A. Tuch & J. Martin (Eds.). Racial
attitudes in the 1990s: continuity and change (15-44). Greenwood, CT: Praeger.
Bonilla-Silva, E. (2006). Racism without racists: Color-blind racism and the persistence
of racial inequality in America. Lanham, Maryland: Rowman and Littlefield
Publishers.
Bourdieu, P. & Passerson, J. (1977). Reproduction in education, society and culture.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Burton, L.M. (1992). Black grandparents rearing children of drug-addicted parents:
Stressors, outcomes, and social service needs. Gerontologist, 32, 744-751.
Bradley, R. H., Corwyn, R. F., Burchinal, M., McAdoo, H. P., & Coll, C. G. (2001a).
The home environments of children in the United States Part I: Relations with
behavioral development through age thirteen. Child Development 72, 6, 1868-
1886.
Bradley, R. H., Corwyn, R. F., McAdoo, H. P., & Coll, C. G. (2001b.) The home
environments of children in the United States Part I: Variations by age, ethnicity,
and poverty status. Child Development 72, 6, 1844-67.
Carlson, M. J. & Corcoran, M. E. (2001). Family structure and children's behavioral and
cognitive outcomes. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 779-792.
Chapman, C., Laird, J., Ifill, N., & KewalRamani, A. (2011). Trends in high school
dropout and completion rates in the United States: 1972-2009. National Center
for Education Statistics, Compendium Report, 1-53.
Charles, C. Z. (2006) Won’t you be my neighbor? Race, class and residence in Los
Angeles. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.
Chase-Lansdale, P. L., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Zamsky, E. S. (1994). Young African-
American multigenerational families in poverty: Quality of mothering and grand-
mothering. Child Development, 65, 373-393.
Cherlin, A. J., & Seltzer, J. A. (2014). Family complexity, the family safety net, and
public policy. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social
Science, 654, 231-239.
33
Coleman, J. (1988). Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Journal of
Sociology, 94, S95-S120.
Conley, D. (2009). Being black, living in the red (2nd ed.). Berkeley, CA: University of
California Press.
Deleire, T & Kalil, A. (2002). Good things come in threes: Single-parent
multigenerational family structure and adolescent adjustment. Demography, 39,
393-413.
Dominguez, S. & Watkins, C. (2003). Creating networks for survival and mobility:
Social capital among African-American and Latin-American low-income
mothers. Social Problems, 50, 111-135.
Dunifon, R., & Kowaleski-Jones, L. (2002). Who’s in the house? Race differences in
cohabitation, single-parenthood, and child development. Child Development, 73,
1249 – 1264.
Dunifon, R., Ziol-Guest, K., & Kopko, K. (2014). Grandparent coresidence and family
well-being implications for research and policy. The ANNALS of the American
Academy of Political and Social Science, 654, 110-126.
Farber, N., Miller-Cribbs, J., & Reitmeier, M. Kin networks in the South: A comparison
of low-income, rural African-American and white women. Rural Social Work and
Community Practice, 10, 52–63.
Fomby, P. & Cherlin, A. J. (2007). Family instability and child wellbeing. American
Sociological Review, 72, 181-204.
Fomby, P., Mollborn, S., & Sennott, C. (2010). Race/ethnic differences in effects of
family instability on adolescents’ risk behavior. Journal of Marriage and Family,
72, 234-253.
Friedman, M. M. (1998). Family nursing (4th Ed.). Stamford, CO: Appleton & Lange.
Garrett-Peters, R. & Burton, L.M. (2016). Tenuous ties: The nature and costs of kin
support among low-income rural black mothers. Women, Gender, and Families of
Color, 4, 4-35.
Gerstel, N. (2011). Rethinking families and community: The color, class, and centrality
of extended kin ties. Sociological Forum, 26, 1–20.
Ginther, D. K. & Pollak, R. A. (2004) Family structure and children's educational
outcomes: Blended families, stylized facts, and descriptive regressions.
Demography, 41, 671-696.
Hill, M. S., Yeung, W. J., & Duncan, G. J. (2001). Childhood family structure and young
adult behaviors. Journal of Population Economics, 14, 271-299.
Hofferth, S. L. (2006). Residential father family type and child well-being: Investment
versus selection. Demography, 43, 53–77.
Hetherington, E., Camara, K., & Featherman, D. (1983). Achievement and intellectual
functioning of children in one-parent households. In J. Spence (Ed.). Achievement
and achievement motives: Psychological and sociological approaches (pp. 205-
284). San Francisco: W. H. Freeman and Company.
Hunter, A. G. (1997). Counting on grandmothers: Black mothers’ and fathers’ reliance on
grandmothers for parenting support. Journal of Family Issues, 18, 251-269.
Jayakody, R., Chatters, L.M., & Taylor, R.J. (1993). Family support to single and married
African American mothers: The provision of financial, emotional, and child care
assistance. Journal of Marriage and the Family 55, 261–276.
34
Karlson, K. B., & Holm, A. (2011). Decomposing primary and secondary effects: a new
decomposition method. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility, 29, 221-
237.
Kohler, U., Karlson, K. B., & Holm, A. (2011). Comparing coefficients of nested
nonlinear probability models. The Stata Journal, 11, 420-438.
Krysan, Maria, Mick P. Couper, Reynolds Farley, and Tyrone A. Forman. (2009). Does
race matter in neighborhood preferences? Results from a video experiment.
American Journal of Sociology, 115, 527-559.
Lamborn, S. G., & Nguyen, D. T. (2004). African American adolescents' perceptions of
family interactions: Kinship support, parent-child Relationships, and teen
adjustment. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 33, 547-558.
Lareau, Annette. 2000. Home Advantage: Social Class and Parental Intervention in
Elementary Education. Lanham, MD: Rowman and Littlefield.
Lareau, Annette. 2011. Unequal Childhoods Class, Race, and Family Life (2nd Edition).
Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Panel Study of Income Dynamics. 2010. Child development supplement user's guide
for CDS-II. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research.
Pilkauskas, N.V., Campbell, C., & Wimer, C. (2017). Giving unto others: Private
financial transfers and hardship among families with children. Journal of
Marriage and Family, 79, 709-722.
Pilkauskas, N. V., & Dunifon, R. E. (2016). Understanding grandfamilies: Characteristics
of grandparents, nonresident parents, and children. Journal of Marriage and
Family, 78, 623-633.
Pittman, L. D. (2007). Grandmothers' involvement among young adolescents growing up
in poverty. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 17, 89-115.
Proctor, B. D., Semega, J.L, & Kollar, M.A. (2016). Income and poverty in the United
States: 2015. Washington, DC: United States Census Bureau.
Manning, W. D., & Brown, S. L. (2006). Children’s economic well-being in marriage
and cohabiting parent families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 68, 345 – 362.
Manning, W. D., & Lamb, K. A. (2003). Adolescent well-being in cohabitating, married,
and single-parent families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 65, 876-893.
Margolis, K. L., Fosco, G. M., & Stormshak, E. A. (2014). Circle of care: Extending
beyond primary caregivers to examine collaborative caretaking in adolescent
development. Journal of Family Issues, 37, 1179-1202.
Mazumder, B. (2008). Upward intergenerational economic mobility in the United States.
Economic Mobility Project. Washington, DC: Pew Charitable Trusts.
Mc Cubbin, M.A. (1993). Family stress theory and the development of nursing
knowledge about family adaptation. In S.L. Feetham, S.B. Meister, J.M.Bell, &
C.L. Gillis (Eds.). The Nursing Family (46-58). New Bury Park, CA: Sage
Publications.
McCubbin, M. A., & McCubbin, H. I. (1989). Families coping with illness: The
resiliency model of family stress, adjustment and adaptation. In, C.B. Danielson,
B. Hamel-Bissel, & P.Winstead-Fry (Eds.). Families, health & illness:
Perspectives on coping and intervention (21-64). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
35
McDonald, K. B., & Armstrong, E. M. (2001). De-romanticizing black intergenerational
support: The questionable expectations of welfare reform. Journal of Marriage
and Family, 63, 213-223.
McLanahan, S. & Percheski, C. (2008). Family structure and the reproduction of
inequalities. Annual Review of Sociology, 34, 257-276.
McLanahan, S. & Sandefur, G. (1994). Growing up with a single-parent: What hurts,
what helps. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
McLoyd V. C., Cauce A. M., Takeuchi, D., & Wilson, L. (2000). Marital processes and
parental socialization in families of color: A decade review of research. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 62, 1070–1093.
National Center for Education Statistics. (2014). On-time high school graduation: The
opportunity picture. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved
from: http://opportunityindex.org/wp-content/uploads/2014/12/HS-Graduation-
2014.pdf.
Passel, J. S., & Cohn, D. (2008). U.S. population projections: 2005 – 2050. Washington,
DC: Pew Research Center.
Roschelle, A. R. 1997. No more kin: Exploring race, class, and gender in family
networks. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Sandberg, J. F., & Hoffereth, S. F. (2001). Changes in children's time with parents:
United States, 1981-1997. Demography, 38, 423-436.
Sarkisian, N. & Gerstel, N. (2004). Kin support among blacks and whites: Race and
family organization. American Sociological Review, 69, 812-837.
Sarkisian, N. & Gerstel, N. (2012). Nuclear family values, extended family lives: The
power of race, class, and gender. New York, NY: Routledge.
Shaw, D. S., Winslow, E. B., & Flanagan, C. (1999). A prospective study in the effects of
marital status and family relations on young children’s adjustment among African
American and European American families. Child Development, 70, 742–755.
Smith, T. E. (1997). Differences between black and white students in the effect of
parental separation on school grades. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 27, 25-
42.
Smith, C. & Carlson B.E. (1997). Stress, coping, and cesilience in youth. Social Service
Review, 71, 231-256.
Stack, C. B. (1974). All our kin: strategies for survival in a black community.
New York, NY: Harper & Row.
Stack, C. B. & Burton, L. M. (1993). Kinscripts. Journal of Comparative Family Studies,
24, 157-170.
Sun, Y. & Li, Y. (2007). Racial and ethnic differences in experiencing parents' marital
disruption during late adolescence. Journal of Marriage and Family, 69, 742-762.
Taylor, R. J. (1986). Receipt of support from family among black Americans:
Demographic and familial differences. Journal of Marriage and Family, 48, 67-
77.
Taylor, R. J., Chae, D.H., Lincoln, K.D., & Chatters, L.M. (2015). Extended family and
friendship support networks are both protective and risk factors for major
depressive disorder and depressive symptoms among African-Americans and
Black Caribbeans. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 203, 132-140.
36
Teachman, J. D. (2008). The living arrangements of children and their educational well-
being. Journal of Family Issues, 29, 734-761.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Health Resources and Services
Administration, Maternal and Child Health Bureau. (2014). Child health USA
2014. Rockville, Maryland: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
U.S. House of Representatives, Committee on Ways and Means. 1996. Personal
Responsibility and Work Opportunity Reconciliation Act of 1996: Conference
Report to Accompany H.R. 3734.Section 401, Purpose (p.9), July 30.
Von Hippel, P. T. (2007). Regression with missing Ys: An improved strategy for
analyzing multiply imputed data. Sociological Methodology, 37, 83-117.
Winship, C. & Mare, R. D. (1984). Regression with ordinal variables. American
Sociological Review, 49, 512-525.
Wilson, W. J. (1987). The truly disadvantaged: The inner city, the underclass, and public
policy. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.
Wu, L. (1996). Effects of family instability, income and income instability on the risk of
a premarital birth. American Journal of Sociology, 68, 386-406.
37
Table 1. Sample characteristics by race/ethnicity, Panel Study of Income Dynamics, Child Development
Supplement, Transition into Adulthood Study, (1985-2015)
Variables
Full
Sample
Black
White
Hispanic
Other
Proportion of childhood with two biological parentsabc
0.68
0.33
0.78
0.73
0.62
Proportion of childhood with mother and non-biological partnerab
0.09
0.12
0.09
0.08
0.10
Proportion of childhood with single motherabc
0.22
0.55
0.13
0.19
0.28
On-time high school completionabc
0.79
0.63
0.85
0.74
0.83
College enrollmentab
0.77
0.61
0.81
0.74
0.76
Femalea
0.48
0.42
0.49
0.48
0.53
Child's ageab
24.6
25.1
24.6
24.0
24.0
(SD)
0.09
0.16
0.12
0.31
0.34
Proportion of childhood spent living in the Southabc
0.33
0.59
0.30
0.22
0.24
No. of childrenabc
2.33
2.52
2.18
2.80
2.39
(SD)
0.02
0.06
0.02
0.09
0.13
Parents' education
<HSabc
0.13
0.15
0.05
0.47
0.17
HS
0.34
0.38
0.32
0.25
0.27
Some collegebc
0.29
0.33
0.27
0.15
0.25
BA or higherabc
0.25
0.14
0.37
0.13
0.31
Family wealth without home equityabc
7.24
4.54
8.33
5.97
6.30
(SD)
0.14
0.23
0.17
0.41
0.71
Proportion of childhood mom is employedabc
0.65
0.60
0.69
0.53
0.57
Neighborhood quality
Excellent/very goodac
0.56
0.44
0.71
0.43
0.49
Goodab
0.26
0.30
0.21
0.32
0.25
Fair/poorac
0.18
0.26
0.08
0.25
0.26
>50% of neighbors own their homeabc
0.68
0.43
0.80
0.59
0.45
Neighorhood is completely or fairly safeac
0.88
0.74
0.94
0.74
0.90
Mother's age at child's birth
19 and belowa
0.12
0.17
0.07
0.13
0.11
20-29c
0.55
0.54
0.54
0.56
0.51
30 and aboveac
0.33
0.27
0.39
0.29
0.36
Proportion of childhood mom is in good healthac
0.90
0.84
0.93
0.85
0.82
Extended family coresidence
Never lived with an extended relativeac
0.60
0.39
0.80
0.62
0.61
Lived with an extended relative for <1/4 of childhoodab
0.18
0.24
0.13
0.14
0.21
Lived with an extended relative for >=1/4 of childhoodac
0.22
0.37
0.07
0.24
0.18
No. of extended relatives living in R's neighborhood by quartile
Bottom 25th percentileac
0.45
0.35
0.53
0.45
0.53
25th-50th percentile
0.07
0.07
0.07
0.04
0.08
50th-75th percentileac
0.23
0.27
0.19
0.27
0.16
75th-99th percentileac
0.25
0.31
0.20
0.25
0.23
Frequency of contact with extended relatives and friends
Less than once a month
0.24
0.28
0.19
0.30
0.25
One to three times a montha
0.16
0.13
0.18
0.15
0.19
Greater than or equal to once a weekbc
0.60
0.59
0.62
0.55
0.55
38
PCG satisfaction with the level of practical support received from
extended family membersabc
5.13
5.32
4.89
5.83
5.53
(SD)
0.04
0.10
0.05
0.12
0.17
PCG satisfaction with the level of emotional support received from
extended family membersbc
5.47
5.28
5.41
5.76
5.86
(SD)
0.04
0.10
0.05
0.14
0.15
No. of Observations
2,589
1,105
1,163
183
137
Notes: Values are percentages unless otherwise noted. Values are weighted to account for the complex multistage
clustered design of the PSID. Total for the time-varying characteristics is the child-year average. aBlack-White difference
significant at p<0.05. bBlack-Hispanic difference significant at p<0.05. cHispanic-White difference significant at p<.05.
No.=Number. SD=Standard deviation. PCG=Primary caregiver.
39
Table 2: Odds ratios from logistic regression models predicting on-time high school completion, Panel Study of Income Dynamics,
Child Development Supplement (CDS), Transition into Adulthood Supplement (TAS), 1985-2015
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Model 4
Model 5
OR
OR
OR
OR
OR
Proportion of childhood with single mother
0.466***
0.231***
0.428**
0.294***
0.462*
(-3.141)
(-3.858)
(-2.055)
(-3.220)
(-1.837)
Single mother X Black
3.049**
2.254
2.896**
2.239
(2.219)
(1.557)
(2.119)
(1.547)
Single mother X Hispanic
1.750
1.473
1.673
1.388
(0.667)
(0.449)
(0.619)
(0.376)
Proportion of childhood with mother and non-biological partner
0.233***
0.271***
0.446*
0.312***
0.459*
(-4.309)
(-3.118)
(-1.752)
(-2.795)
(-1.695)
Mother and non-biological partner X Black
3.822*
2.889
5.264**
3.596
(1.723)
(1.325)
(2.157)
(1.621)
Mother and non-biological partner X Hispanic
0.726
0.374
0.698
0.362
(-0.267)
(-0.763)
(-0.289)
(-0.795)
Race/ethnicity (vs. White)
Black
0.473***
0.280***
0.412***
0.310***
0.424***
(-3.701)
(-4.614)
(-3.114)
(-4.319)
(-3.074)
Hispanic
0.600**
0.578*
0.927
0.688
1.016
(-2.179)
(-1.797)
(-0.209)
(-1.174)
-0.044
Female
1.676***
1.652***
1.728***
1.675***
1.738***
(3.774)
(3.686)
(3.856)
(3.687)
(3.836)
Child's age
0.938***
0.943***
0.935***
0.946***
0.937***
(-3.163)
(-2.924)
(-3.055)
(-2.687)
(-2.927)
Proportion of childhood spent living in the South
0.790
0.807
0.839
0.831
0.855
(-1.564)
(-1.414)
(-1.142)
(-1.236)
(-1.027)
No. of children
0.798***
0.783***
0.854**
0.792***
0.856**
(-3.034)
(-3.329)
(-2.018)
(-3.131)
(-1.996)
Parents' education (vs. BA or higher)
Less than high school
0.495**
0.509**
(-2.522)
(-2.416)
High school
0.366***
0.384***
(-4.737)
(-4.488)
Some college
0.522***
0.520***
(-2.958)
(-2.955)
Family wealth without home equity
1.001
1.000
(0.095)
(-0.022)
Proportion of childhood mom is employed
1.099
1.060
(0.393)
(0.245)
Neighborhood quality (vs. excellent/very good)
Good
0.796
0.784
(-1.243)
(-1.320)
Poor
0.781
0.784
(-1.146)
(-1.121)
>50% of neighbors own their homeabc
1.153
1.189
(0.713)
(0.872)
Neighorhood is completely or fairly safe
1.357
1.295
40
(1.456)
(1.244)
Mother's age at child's birth
20-29
2.201***
2.109***
(3.598)
(3.325)
30 and above
2.108***
2.000***
(3.008)
(2.737)
Proportion of childhood mom is in good health
3.526***
3.501***
(3.691)
(3.707)
Extended family coresidence (vs. never lived with an extended relative)
Lived with an extended relative for <1/4 of childhood
0.705*
0.839
(-1.822)
(-0.911)
Lived with an extended relative for >=1/4 of childhood
0.613**
0.820
(-2.456)
(-0.972)
No. of extended relatives living in R's neighborhood by quartile (vs. below
25th percentile)
25th-50th percentile
1.025
1.172
(0.074)
(0.456)
50th-75th percentile
0.689*
0.715
(-1.848)
(-1.614)
75th-99th percentile
1.071
1.067
(0.329)
(0.300)
Frequency of contact with extended relatives (vs. less than once a month)
One to three times a month
1.458*
1.425
(1.721)
(1.590)
Greater than or equal to once a week
1.387**
1.280
(2.006)
(1.503)
PCG satisfaction with practical support received from extended family
members
0.954
0.970
(-0.707)
(-0.444)
PCG satisfaction with practical support received from extended family
members
1.088
1.048
(1.286)
(0.680)
Wald Test p-value
0.003***
0.002***
0.002***
0.001***
Constant
50.663***
51.588***
8.333***
31.266***
6.847**
(7.009)
(7.187)
(2.886)
(5.140)
(2.380)
Observations
2,589
2,589
2,589
2,589
2,589
t-statistics in parentheses
*** p<0.01, ** p<0.05, * p<0.1
Notes: "Other" race category omitted. Analyses use sample weights to account for the complex multistage clustered design of the PSID.
The Wald test provides evidence for whether the family structure-race/ethnicity interactions are jointly significant. No.=Number.
OR=Odds ratios.
41
Table 3: Odds ratios from logistic regression models predicting college enrollment, Panel Study of Income Dynamics, Child
Development Supplement (CDS), and Transition into Adulthood Supplement (TAS), 1985-2015
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Model 4
Model 5
OR
OR
OR
OR
OR
Proportion of childhood with single mother
0.429***
0.348***
0.915
0.450**
1.007
(-3.302)
(-2.853)
(-0.213)
(-2.048)
(0.016)
Single mother X Black
1.663
0.928
1.631
0.906
(0.974)
(-0.131)
(0.921)
(-0.173)
Single mother X Hispanic
0.510
0.264
0.470
0.239
(-0.803)
(-1.416)
(-0.845)
(-1.498)
Proportion of childhood with mother and non-biological partner
0.228***
0.208***
0.435*
0.241***
0.445*
(-4.497)
(-3.845)
(-1.762)
(-3.492)
(-1.727)
Mother and non-biological partner X Black
1.811
1.044
2.489
1.215
(0.680)
(0.043)
(1.061)
(0.198)
Mother and non-biological partner X Hispanic
6.623*
3.404
7.594*
3.812
(1.800)
(1.028)
(1.806)
(1.084)
Race/ethnicity (vs. White)
Black
0.740
0.552*
0.975
0.638
1.052
(-1.363)
(-1.841)
(-0.073)
(-1.389)
(0.146)
Hispanic
1.009
0.991
1.966*
1.180
2.160*
(0.035)
(-0.028)
(1.739)
(0.487)
(1.924)
Female
1.737***
1.739***
1.841***
1.803***
1.867***
(3.884)
(3.878)
(4.077)
(3.985)
(4.056)
Child's age
1.020
1.023
1.022
1.025
1.019
(0.989)
(1.127)
(0.941)
(1.224)
(0.844)
Proportion of childhood spent living in the South
0.834
0.862
0.902
0.861
0.889
(-1.090)
(-0.893)
(-0.597)
(-0.903)
(-0.683)
No. of children
0.782***
0.777***
0.888
0.778***
0.882
(-3.207)
(-3.331)
(-1.363)
(-3.292)
(-1.447)
Proportion of childhood mom is in good health
1.410
1.407
(0.898)
(0.897)
Parents' education (vs. BA or higher)
Less than high school
0.270***
0.280***
(-3.903)
(-3.823)
High school
0.214***
0.223***
(-6.413)
(-6.221)
Some college
0.373***
0.374***
(-3.948)
(-3.952)
Family wealth without home equity
1.046***
1.047***
(2.965)
(3.056)
Proportion of childhood mom is employed
1.087
1.050
(0.303)
(0.176)
Neighborhood quality (vs. excellent/very good)
Good
0.890
0.888
(-0.605)
(-0.613)
Poor
0.837
0.826
(-0.731)
(-0.781)
>50% of neighbors own their homeabc
1.323
1.364
(1.322)
(1.456)
Neighorhood is completely or fairly safe
1.381
1.405
(1.411)
(1.477)
Mother's age at child's birth
20-29
1.820**
1.780**
(2.463)
(2.325)
30 and above
1.870**
1.756**
42
(2.230)
(1.988)
Extended family coresidence (vs. never lived with an extended relative)
Lived with an extended relative for <1/4 of childhood
0.572***
0.738
(-2.891)
(-1.545)
Lived with an extended relative for >=1/4 of childhood
0.581**
0.834
(-2.550)
(-0.798)
No. of extended relatives living in R's neighborhood by quartile (vs. below 25th
percentile)
25th-50th percentile
0.963
1.254
(-0.102)
(0.570)
50th-75th percentile
0.767
0.846
(-1.140)
(-0.705)
75th-99th percentile
1.155
1.253
(0.690)
(1.056)
Frequency of contact with extended relatives (vs. less than once a month)
One to three times a month
1.227
1.135
(0.851)
(0.513)
Greater than or equal to once a week
1.134
1.015
(0.736)
(0.083)
PCG satisfaction with practical support received from extended family members
0.928
0.940
(-1.108)
(-0.844)
PCG satisfaction with practical support received from extended family members
1.085
1.026
(1.211)
(0.344)
Wald Test p-value
0.209
0.501
0.191
0.451
Constant
5.891***
5.820***
1.540
4.951**
1.921
(3.226)
(3.287)
(0.546)
(2.473)
(0.747)
Observations
2,436
2,436
2,436
2,436
2,436
t-statistics in parentheses
*** p<0.01, ** p<0.05, * p<0.1
Notes: "Other" race category omitted. Analyses use sample weights to account for the complex multistage clustered design of the PSID. The
Wald test provides evidence for whether the family structure-race/ethnicity interactions are jointly significant. No.=Number. OR=Odds
ratios.
43
Table 4. Average partial effects of socioeconomic stress and extended family embeddedness on children's
educational attainment, Panel Study of Income Dynamics, Child Development Supplement (CDS), and Transition
into Adulthood Supplement (TAS), 1985-2015
Socioeconomic stress
Extended family embeddedness
Family structure
Mother and non-
biological partner
Single
mother
Mother and non-
biological partner
Single
mother
On-time high school completion
Reduced model
-0.176***
-0.221***
-0.177***
-0.220***
(0.024)
(0.039)
(0.025)
(0.039)
Full model
-0.102***
-0.173***
-0.151***
-0.192***
(0.027)
(0.040)
(0.026)
(0.041)
Percentage change
42.0%
21.7%
14.7%
12.7%
Observations
2,589
2,589
2,589
2,589
Socioeconomic stress
Extended family embeddedness
Family structure
Mother and non-
biological partner
Single
mother
Mother and non-
biological partner
Single
mother
College enrollment
Reduced model
-0.155***
-0.169***
-0.157***
-0.170***
(0.025)
(0.039)
(0.026)
(0.040)
Full model
-0.082***
-0.109***
-0.128***
-0.148***
(0.027)
(0.041)
(0.027)
(0.040)
Percentage change
48.4%
35.5%
18.5%
12.9%
Observations
2,436
2,436
2,436
2,436
Standard errors in parentheses
*** p<0.01, ** p<0.05, * p<0.1
Notes: Table 4 describes the average partial effect of family structure on on-time high school completion and college
enrollment in the reduced (baseline) and full models, estimated using the Karlson, Hohm and Breen (KHB) method for
decomposing total effects (Kohler, Karlson and Holm 2011).