ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Work interruptions are ubiquitous in today’s workplaces due to the proliferation of technology, and a growing emphasis on collaboration and open workspaces. Although a large body of research on interruptions has accumulated over the last two decades, this research is scattered across disciplines, with little integration. While this fragmentation indicates the complex nature of interruptions, it has also led to inconsistencies in how interruptions are defined and studied. Such differences reduce generalizability of results, lead to conflicting findings, and hinder knowledge development. We present here an integrative review of prior research on work interruptions, based on an analysis of 247 publications. As part of the review, we examine prior definitions of interruption and advance a new integrative definition that can anchor a range of future research. We also discuss and summarize the assumptions and implications of the different investigative approaches used to study interruptions. An awareness of these approaches can help scholars better align their theory and investigative approach to adequately capture constructs/ relationships of interest. We then synthesize theory and research, across disciplines, to present a process-based model that comprehensively captures our current understanding of how, when, and why work interruptions affect employees in different ways. Lastly, we highlight several avenues in need of more research attention and provide recommendations on how to advance the work interruption literature ahead meaningfully. Our review can act as an important reference for scholars new to interruption research, as well as for established interruption researchers looking to move their research in new directions.
Content may be subject to copyright.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Pardon the interruption: An integrative review and future research agenda for research on
work interruptions
Harshad Puranik
University of Illinois at Chicago
Joel Koopman
Texas A&M University
Heather C. Vough
George Mason University
University of Cincinnati
Acknowledgements: We gratefully thank Action Editor Christopher Rosen and four anonymous
reviewers for their helpful guidance. We also want to thank Suzanne Masterson, Jaime Windeler,
Bina Ajay, and Shivangi Kane-Puranik for their helpful comments on previous versions of this
manuscript. The first author would like to acknowledge the receipt of the University of Cincinnati
Graduate School Dean's Fellowship (2018-19).
Corresponding author: Harshad Puranik, University of Illinois at Chicago
Email: hpuranik@uic.edu
This article has been accepted for publication and undergone full peer review but has not been
through the copyediting, typesetting, pagination and proofreading process, which may lead to
differences between this version and the Version of Record. Please cite this article as doi:
10.1177/0149206319887428
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 1
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
ABSTRACT
Work interruptions are ubiquitous in today’s workplaces due to the proliferation of technology,
and a growing emphasis on collaboration and open workspaces. Although a large body of
research on interruptions has accumulated over the last two decades, this research is scattered
across disciplines, with little integration. While this fragmentation indicates the complex nature
of interruptions, it has also led to inconsistencies in how interruptions are defined and studied.
Such differences reduce generalizability of results, lead to conflicting findings, and hinder
knowledge development. We present here an integrative review of prior research on work
interruptions, based on an analysis of 247 publications. As part of the review, we examine prior
definitions of interruption and advance a new integrative definition that can anchor a range of
future research. We also discuss and summarize the assumptions and implications of the different
investigative approaches used to study interruptions. An awareness of these approaches can help
scholars better align their theory and investigative approach to adequately capture constructs/
relationships of interest. We then synthesize theory and research, across disciplines, to present a
process-based model that comprehensively captures our current understanding of how, when,
and why work interruptions affect employees in different ways. Lastly, we highlight several
avenues in need of more research attention and provide recommendations on how to advance the
work interruption literature ahead meaningfully. Our review can act as an important reference for
scholars new to interruption research, as well as for established interruption researchers looking
to move their research in new directions.
Keywords: work interruption; task interruption; distraction; disruption; intrusion
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 2
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
PARDON THE INTERRUPTION: AN INTEGRATIVE REVIEW AND FUTURE
RESEARCH AGENDA FOR RESEARCH ON WORK INTERRUPTIONS
Work interruptions abound in today’s workplaces. Coworkers drop by unexpectedly to
seek help, provide updates, or socialize, and supervisors check in on work or assign new tasks
(Mark, Gonzalez & Harris, 2005; Perlow, 1999). Moreover, the volume and the unpredictability
of emails, texts, phone calls, and instant messages can get overwhelming (e.g., González &
Mark, 2004; Rosen, Simon, Gajendran, Johnson, Lee & Lin, 2019; Sonnentag, Reinecke, Mata &
Vorderer, 2018). If this sounds like your typical workday, you are not alone. Leroy and Glomb
(2018: 380) note that “completing tasks without interruptions has become a luxury” for today’s
employees. Wajcman and Rose (2011) recently documented an average of 86 daily workplace
interruptions, and Spira and Feintuch (2005) noted that the average 2.1 hours of daily work
interruptions faced by knowledge workers cost the U.S. economy $588 billion annually. Beyond
financial losses, interruptions also affect employee performance, attitudes, emotions, and well-
being (e.g., Baethge & Rigotti, 2013; Hunter, Clark & Carlson, 2019; Jett & George, 2003).
Despite vital implications for organizations, the previous review of interruptions in the
field of management was 16 years ago (Jett & George, 2003). The workplace has changed
significantly since then; indeed, this review came four years before the first iPhone. Beyond the
spread of technology in workplaces, an increased emphasis on collaboration and open offices has
made interruptions highly pervasive today (Mark, 2015). It is no wonder then that a large body of
research on interruptions has emerged in the past two decades. But this research is spread across
disciplines with little integration, as scholars focus on theories and outcomes aligned most with
their disciplines. While this proliferation of research reveals the complexity of the interruption
construct, it has also led to inconsistencies in how interruptions are defined and studied. This has
resulted in independent and scattered pools of knowledge, each giving a unique, but incomplete,
view of interruptions. Ultimately, the fragmented state of the extant literature prevents a holistic
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 3
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
understanding of interruptions and hinders future research. Thus, the interruption literature will
benefit from an integrative review that synthesizes prior findings and channels future research by
acting as a comprehensive guide of what has been done and what needs to be done in this area.
We present here a review that accomplishes this goal and makes several contributions to
the literature. First, based on an analysis of prior work interruption definitions across disciplines,
we advance a definition that not only integrates these disparate streams of research, but also
draws precise boundaries around the construct. Thus, we lay a common foundation to anchor
future interruption research. Second, to help interruption scholars better align their theory with
investigative approach, we review prior approaches to studying work interruptions. For each
approach, we summarize the underlying assumptions and their implications for the study of
interruption-related theoretical topics and study designs. Third, we synthesize findings related to
the sources, mediators, outcomes, and moderators of work interruptions, across theoretical and
disciplinary boundaries, to present an integrative process-based model that explains when, how,
and why interruptions affect employees in different ways. Fourth, we highlight several avenues
in need of additional research, with recommendations on how to advance the literature further.
By synthesizing research across multiple disciplines, our review is broader in scope and
more comprehensive than prior reviews that are either domain specific (e.g., healthcare
interruptions: Biron, Loiselle & Lavoie‐Tremblay, 2009b; Grundgeiger & Sanderson, 2009;
Hopkinson & Jennings, 2013; Raban & Westbrook, 2014; Rivera-Rodriguez & Karsh, 2010; IT
interruptions: McFarlene & Latorella, 2002; Rissler, Nadj, Adam & Maedche, 2017; Trafton &
Monk, 2007), or focused mainly on defining and classifying work interruptions (e.g., Brixey,
Robinson, Johnson, Johnson, Turley & Zhang, 2007; Jett & George, 2003; Sasangohar, Donmez,
Trbovich & Easty, 2012). Overall, we intend for our review to serve as a comprehensive
reference for scholars new to research on work interruptions, as well as for established work
interruption scholars looking to move their research in new directions.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 4
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
For the purpose of our review, we obtained research publications (journal/conference
papers, books, and book chapters) by searching research databases (e.g., Google Scholar and
Web of Science) using relevant terms (‘interruption,’ ‘intrusion,’ ‘distraction,’ ‘disruption’),
coupled with the descriptors ‘work,’ ‘task,’ and ‘workplace.’ We identified two foundational
articles in the field of management (Jett & George, 2003; Perlow, 1999) and several more from
other disciplines (e.g., Adamczyk & Bailey, 2004; Altmann & Trafton, 2002; Chisholm,
Collison, Nelson & Cordell, 2000; Czerwinski, Horvitz & Wilhite, 2004; Gillie & Broadbent,
1989; González & Mark, 2004; Mark et al., 2005; McFarlane & Latorella, 2002; Speier,
Valacich & Vessey, 1999; Westbrook, Woods, Rob, Dunsmuir & Day, 2010). We then searched
for research publications focused on work/task interruption that cited the above articles, and
further checked the reference lists from these publications as well. This resulted in a total of 247
publications that were then coded for theory, definition, antecedents, mediators, outcomes,
moderators, and methodology. This helped us identify themes to organize the below review.
DEFINITIONS AND APPROACHES TO STUDYING WORK INTERRUPTIONS
The proliferation of differing definitions and approaches to studying work interruptions is
a major reason the interruption literature is not well integrated. We address these issues by first
presenting an analysis of prior definitions of work interruption. Our review shows that scholars
have focused on five attributes (see Table 1). We assess each attribute with two questions: Is it
critical, or ancillary, to the core experience of a work interruption? And, does it help delineate
work interruption from other constructs? Following this, we advance a new definition based on
our analysis. We then classify the different approaches to studying interruptions as varying along
two dimensions, and discuss the implications of each dimension for the study of interruptions.
Defining Work Interruption
Suspension of an Ongoing Work Task’s Execution. This is a core element of prior
interruption definitions and is said to occur in two wayssuspension of behavioral performance
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 5
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
of a task (e.g., Biron, Lavoie‐Tremblay & Loiselle, 2009a; Van Den Berg, Roe, Zijlstra &
Krediet, 1996) or suspension of attentional focus from the task (e.g., Myers, McCarthy,
Whitlatch & Parikh, 2016; Sanderson & Grundgeiger, 2015). We posit that both constitute an
interruption, and this helps us address an ongoing debate about interruptions versus distractions.
The crux of this debate is that while there is wide agreement that suspending behavioral
performance of a task interrupts it (e.g., Mitchell, Harman, Lee & Lee, 2008), there is a group of
scholars who consider the suspension of only attentional focus from an ongoing task, defined as
distraction, to be different from an interruption (e.g., Flynn, Barker, Gibson, Pearson, Berger &
Smith, 1999; Hickam et al., 2003). For example, consider an employee who is unable to focus on
a task because he/she is distracted by loud coworker conversations. In this case, even if the
employee’s attentional resources are diverted away from the task, at a behavioral level, he/she
can still continue working on the task. Since the behavioral execution of the task can continue in
this case, this group of scholars would not consider this case to be a work interruption (Speier,
Vessey & Valacich, 2003). As per them, suspending a task’s behavioral performance is a
necessary condition for an interruption to occur (Relihan, O'brien, O'hara & Silke, 2010).
Another group of scholars, however, view distractions and work interruptions as
overlapping constructs. They consider the suspension of the behavioral performance of a task to
be a sufficient, but not necessary, condition for an interruption to occur; instead, distractions or
lapses of attentional focus from ongoing work constitute an interruption as well (Fisher, 1998;
Jett & George, 2003). The reason is that attentional resources diverted by distracting stimuli
cannot be applied to the ongoing task, thus interfering with its execution (Beal, Weiss, Barros &
MacDermid, 2005). So, even if the behavioral execution of the ongoing task can ostensibly
continue during a distraction, lapses of attentional focus from the task interrupt its overall
performance (Fletcher, Potter & Telford, 2018; Jett & George, 2003; Wilkes, Barber & Rogers,
2018). We concur with this view, and position distraction as a specific type of work interruption
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 6
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
involving the suspension of only attentional focus from an ongoing task.
Taken together, we submit that suspension of the behavioral performance of a task, or
one’s attentional focus from it, both interrupt the task. Also, since these two processes co-occur
and are closely intertwined during most interruptions (e.g., Li, Magrabi & Coiera, 2012), a
definition that accounts for both these occurrences better captures the real-world experience of
being interrupted. Hence, we posit that work interruptions involve the suspension of the
behavioral performance of a task, or suspension of attentional focus from the task, or both.
Unexpectedness. While some scholars see work interruptions as necessarily unexpected
occurrences (e.g., Brixey et al., 2007; Speier et al., 1999), others do not (e.g., Andreasson,
Lindblom & Thorvald, 2017; Chisholm et al., 2000). Jett and George (2003) included both
unexpected and expected events as interruptions in their typology (see also: Couffe & Michael,
2017; Fletcher et al., 2018; Wilkes et al., 2018). In contrast, Brixey et al. (2007) argued that
unexpectedness is integral to the interruption experience, and others have also similarly stressed
the unplanned nature of work interruptions (e.g., O'Conaill & Frohlich, 1995; Rivera, 2014).
We see interruptions as necessarily unexpectedan attribute that we posit differentiates
them from other constructs. We argue that considering both expected and unexpected events as
interruptions conflates interruptions with constructs such as task-switching and multi-tasking,
and allows any discrete action to be considered an interruption, which is untenable. For instance,
consider task-switching. While interruptions can involve switching of tasks, Baethge, Rigotti and
Roe (2015: 309) have differentiated interruptions from pre-planned task-switches that occur at
meaningful breakpoints within a task “after (sub)actions are closed and mental resources have
been released.” For example, if you plan a coffee break after reading this section and execute this
plan, then this preplanned switching of tasks will not amount to an interruption of your work
(Kim, Park & Headrick, 2018). Another instance where task-switching will not be a work
interruption is when a task is switched after its completion (Dabbish, Mark & González, 2011).
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 7
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
With regard to multi-taskingsimultaneous execution of two or more tasks (González &
Mark, 2004)research does show that work interruptions can at times involve multi-tasking
(Kirchberg, Roe & Van Eerde, 2015). However, similar to the above argument, Szumowska and
Kossowska (2017) did not consider multi-tasking of two tasks that were pre-defined and hence
expected by their study participants to be interruptions. It was only when these participants were
unexpectedly asked to work on a third task that these authors considered an interruption to have
occurred. Thus, while work interruptions can at times involve task-switching or multi-tasking,
not all instances of task-switching or multi-tasking are interruptions. Instead, we suggest that
unexpectedness is the core attribute that determines whether an event is an interruption or not.
Interrupting Task. Some definitions stress the presence of an interrupting task as a
necessary condition for a work interruption to occur, implying that work interruptions involve
both the suspension of an ongoing task and switching to another task (e.g., Brixey et al., 2007;
Couffe & Michael, 2017; Li et al., 2012). We think this condition is unnecessary because, while
other tasks can interrupt work, this may not always be the case (Fisher, 1998). For example, one
of the authors sat through a mandatory but unexpected computer restart following an IT update
while writing this section, but did not start work on another task (e.g., impediments and waiting
time: Weigl, Müller, Vincent, Angerer & Sevdalis, 2012). Thus, it is possible that employees are
interrupted without the need to perform a second task during the work interruption.
Further, the necessity of an interrupting task for the occurrence of a work interruption is
incompatible with interruptions that involve the suspension of only attentional focus from an
ongoing task. For instance, loud coworker conversations may interrupt work (Jett & George,
2003; Wilkes et al., 2018), but employees do not always initiate another task in such cases. Thus,
while interruptions can involve an interrupting task, we do not see this as a defining attribute.
Intention to Resume the Interrupted Task. Some scholars view the intention to return
to the interrupted task after the interruption as a defining feature of a work interruption (e.g.,
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 8
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Baethge et al., 2015; Boehm-Davis & Remington, 2009; Couffe & Michael, 2017). Indeed,
people often form such intentions when interrupted (Trafton, Altmann, Brock & Mintz, 2003).
But this may not always be the casesome interruption sources (e.g., unexpected face-to-face
interactions) provide little opportunity and time to reflect on post-interruption activities (Nees &
Fortna, 2015). Further, an interruption’s unexpectedness may make it difficult for interrupted
individuals to predict how the interruption will unfold, and when, and how, they can resume the
interrupted task (Perlow, 1999). Hence, we suggest that while an intention to resume the
interrupted task may occur when interrupted, it need not be a defining feature of an interruption.
Internal-External Interruptions. Scholars also often include in their definitions whether
the source of a work interruption is external or internal to the interrupted person. While some
stress that work interruptions are caused by only external stimuli such as emails, phone calls etc.
(e.g., O'Conaill & Frohlich, 1995; Speier et al., 1999), others suggest that internal factors such as
one’s own thoughts, pain, fatigue etc. can also lead to work interruptions (e.g., Fisher, 1998;
Gatzounis, Schrooten, Crombez & Vlaeyen, 2014). We agree with the latter view, and omit the
descriptor of the origin of a work interruption’s source from our definition to decouple the work
interruption from its antecedents. Our position is that the core experience of a work interruption
involves the unexpected suspension of an ongoing task’s execution, irrespective of whether this
is caused by sources internal or external to the interrupted person (e.g., Grundgeiger &
Sanderson, 2009). Thus, the ‘internal’ and ‘external’ descriptors of an interruption’s causes are
antecedents to the core experience of a work interruption and unnecessary for defining it.
Summary. By analyzing the pros and cons of the five attributes commonly found in
definitions of work interruption, we have made explicit some disagreements in the literature. We
submit that two attributes—suspension of an ongoing task’s execution and the unexpectedness of
its occurrenceare necessary to defining an interruption and delineating it from other constructs.
In contrast, the other three attributespresence of an interrupting task, an intention to resume the
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 9
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
interrupted task, and whether the interruption source is internal or externaladd unnecessary
conditions to the definition. Hence, we advance the following definition of a work interruption:
A work interruption is an unexpected suspension of the behavioral performance of,
and/or attentional focus from, an ongoing work task
Approaches to Studying Work Interruptions
Scholars also vary widely in their approaches to studying interruptions. An awareness of
these approaches is crucial, as they have implications for the scope of theoretical questions
studied, as well as for effective study design. Recently, Grundgeiger and colleagues (2016) noted
that a common issue in the research on interruptions and their outcomes is the poor fit between
theoretical questions and investigative approaches. For example, these authors noted that when
studies on the forgetting of interrupted tasks rely only on self-reports of whether participants
forgot to resume the interrupted task, they capture only those interruptions that the participants
subsequently remembered when providing the self-report. Instead, an observation method, where
external observers note whether the interrupted tasks were resumed or not, might yield greater
domain coverage. That being said, there are instances in which self-reports may be more
effective than observations. For instance, Fletcher et al. (2018) noted that studies focused on
stress caused by work interruptions that rely only on observations do not capture interrupted
persons’ appraisal of the interruption, thus leaving unmeasured a key theoretical driver of stress.
In sum, poor fit between theoretical questions and investigative approaches can lead to
incomplete construct coverage, and possibly incorrect or inadequate conclusions. To help
scholars avoid this situation, we review extant approaches to studying work interruptions, and
explain their underlying assumptions and implications for interruption-related theoretical topics
and study designs. Our review shows that approaches to studying interruptions can be described
using two dimensions: (a) objectivesubjective and (b) episodicfrequency, as explained below.
ObjectiveSubjective. This dimension reflects whether participants’ assessments of an
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 10
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
interruption are captured or not. An objective approach assumes an event that halts ongoing work
to be interruptive by its very occurrence, and does not incorporate participant assessments (e.g.,
Foroughi, Malihi & Boehm-Davis, 2016a; Monk & Kidd, 2008). Put differently, the interruptive
nature of an event is decided by the researcher and not the participant, and so, it is necessary that
researchers are able to clearly observe the work interruptions. Hence, this approach is seen in
experiments where work interruptions are externally induced by confederates or online tasks
interrupting participants in the middle of a given task (e.g., Mansi & Levy, 2013; Monk, Trafton
& Boehm-Davis, 2008). It is also seen in field studies where work interruptions are noted by
observing participants either directly or indirectly via video recordings (e.g., Flynn et al., 1999;
Weigl, Müller, Zupanc, Glaser & Angerer, 2011).
Since this approach does not capture participants’ subjective experience, it is suitable for
studying those work interruptions that are externally observable by the researcher (e.g., Brumby,
Cox, Back & Gould, 2013; Monk & Kidd, 2008), but not those where only attentional focus on a
task is suspended without an observable behavioral change. Further, this approach is also less
suitable for the study of emotional and stress reactions to work interruptions as it does not
capture interrupted persons’ appraisal of the interruption, which is theoretically a more proximal
and meaningful predictor of these reactions than just the occurrence of the work interruption
(Fletcher et al., 2018; Lazarus, 1991). Generally, the objective approach is useful to study how
observable interruptions of a sequence of work activities affect objectively measurable outcomes
such as task resumption and completion times, errors, and task performance (e.g., Altmann &
Trafton, 2007; Altmann, Trafton & Hambrick, 2014; Monk, 2004).
A subjective approach, in contrast, captures interrupted persons’ subjective assessment of
the interruption (Fletcher et al., 2018). Thus, the research participant, and not the researcher,
decides the interruptive nature of an event. Researchers use surveys, diaries, logs, and interviews
to capture participants’ assessment of work interruptions (e.g., Perlow, 1999; Rosen et al., 2019;
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 11
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Sonnentag et al., 2018). This diversity of method helps the study of not only work interruptions
that are observable to the researcher, but also those where only attentional focus on a task is
suspended, without any observable behavioral change. Also, the focus on participants’ appraisal
of work interruptions makes this approach well-suited to study attitudinal, emotional, and stress
reactions to interruptions (e.g., Fletcher et al., 2018; Hunter et al., 2019; Lin, Kain & Fritz,
2013). A drawback, however, is that the act of capturing the subjective experience of participants
can itself become an interruption. Adjustments in study design (e.g., administering surveys in
non-work hours) can offset this issue, but may come at other costs (e.g., retrospective bias).
EpisodicFrequency. This dimension reflects whether scholars focus on the effects of
individual interruption episodes, or on the aggregate effect of multiple interruptions together.
Studies with an episodic approach presume that the content of each interruption may differ (e.g.,
Butts, Becker & Boswell, 2015). This assumption leads to study designs where scholars study
individual interruption episodes in experimental setups (e.g., Cades, Werner, Boehm-Davis,
Trafton & Monk, 2008; Monk et al., 2008), or in field studies by asking participants to recall a
specific interruption episode (e.g., Leroy & Glomb, 2018; Russell, Woods & Banks, 2017). An
episodic approach helps scholars zoom in on the content of a specific work interruption and
unpack how the attributes of the tasks, the people, and the context of that interruption episode
affect its outcomes (e.g., Butts et al., 2015; Hodgetts & Jones, 2006; Pankok Jr et al., 2017).
However, this event-level focus makes the episodic approach less suitable to study the
cumulative effect of multiple interruptions, commonly seen in today’s workplaces.
A frequency approach instead focuses on the aggregate effect of multiple interruptions,
rather than on the experience of any one interruption. The assumption is that interruptions are
similar in content and that their impact is the result of their combined effect (e.g., Sonnentag et
al., 2018). This assumption results in study designs such as experimental studies with multiple
interruptions (e.g., Mansi & Levy, 2013), observational studies where frequency of interruptions
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 12
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
is noted by observing participants (e.g., Westbrook et al., 2010), or field studies that use
interruption scales with frequency anchors (e.g., 1=Never to 7=Most of the time: Parke,
Weinhardt, Brodsky, Tangirala & DeVoe, 2018). Regarding research topics, given the large
number of daily workplaces interruptions (Wajcman & Rose, 2011), this approach is useful to
study the aggregate effect of multiple interruptions occurring over a day/week/month (e.g.,
Hunter et al., 2019; Lin et al., 2013; Rosen et al., 2019). It can also be used to note the trend and
distribution of workplace interruptions (e.g., Weigl et al., 2012; Westbrook, Raban, Walter &
Douglas, 2018), which can serve as an input for improving work-flows to manage interruptions.
The focus on aggregate effects, however, makes it difficult to isolate and study the attributes of
the interrupted/interrupting tasks or the interrupter involved in any one work interruption.
Summary. Knowing the assumptions and implications of the above dimensions will help
scholars combine them to design studies that adequately capture the construct/relationship of
interest. For instance, returning to our earlier example, scholars interested in the number of
interrupted tasks forgotten by participants may benefit by combining the objective and frequency
approaches. As another example, an episodic approach may help in the study of the attributes of
the interrupter or the interrupting task by isolating these attributes. This can be merged with an
objective approach to study observable attributes (e.g., interrupting task duration) or a subjective
approach to study attributes based on participants’ assessments (e.g., interpersonal liking of
interrupter). Alternately, scholars interested in the study of how multiple interruptions occurring
over a period of time affect employee stress may benefit by combining the subjective and
frequency approaches. It is also possible for scholars to combine the episodic and frequency
approaches to focus on the content of multiple episodes of specific types of interruptions and
evaluate the relative frequency and impact of one type versus another. Overall, our exposition of
the above dimensions will act as a template that guides interruption scholars in selecting an
approach that maximizes the alignment between theory and investigative approach.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 13
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
INTEGRATIVE PROCESS-BASED MODEL OF WORK INTERRUPTIONS
We present our review and synthesis of the prior research on the sources, mediators,
outcomes, and moderators of work interruptions as a process-based model in Figure 1. This
model incorporates scholarship from multiple disciplines to provide an integrative accounting of
work interruption research. In so doing, we link the approaches described above to the theoretical
lenses applied in extant scholarship. The result is a comprehensive depiction of the state of the
literature that advances understanding of how, why, and when interruptions affect employees.
Sources of Work Interruptions
Sources of interruption are the stimuli or causes that initiate an interruption (Rivera-
Rodriguez & Karsh, 2010). There are broad (e.g., Jett & George, 2003; Jin & Dabbish, 2009), as
well as domain-specific (e.g., manufacturing: Andreasson et al., 2017; healthcare: Chisholm,
Dornfeld, Nelson & Cordell, 2001; pharmacy: Flynn et al., 1999; IT: Rissler et al., 2017)
typologies of interruption sources. However, the most common classification is based on whether
a source lies outside (external) or inside (internal) the interrupted person (e.g., Fisher, 1998;
González & Mark, 2004; Mark et al., 2005; Miyata & Norman, 1986). While scholars have noted
the presence of both external and internal sources of interruptions, our review shows that most
prior research has focused exclusively on the external sources of interruptions. As a result, the
nature, experience, and outcomes of internal sources of interruptions are poorly understood.
Hence, we discuss internal sources as a future research topic in a later section. Reflecting the
current state of the literature, our review here is limited to external sources of interruption.
Scholars have documented various external sources of interruptions such as face-to-face
interactions (e.g., Nees & Fortna, 2015), emails (e.g., Jackson, Dawson & Wilson, 2003), phone
calls/texts (e.g., Grandhi & Jones, 2015), instant messenger chats (e.g., Gupta, Li & Sharda,
2013), equipment failures/work delays (e.g., Cai, Gong, Lu & Zhong, 2017), and distracting
workplace stimuli (e.g., Oldham, Kulik & Stepina, 1991). The distribution and use of these
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 14
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
sources varies across workplacesemails and instant messages are common for knowledge
workers and telecommuters (e.g., Fonner & Roloff, 2012; Sykes, 2011), equipment failures are
seen in manufacturing setups (e.g., Cai et al., 2017), and face-to-face meetings, phone calls, and
pagers are common for healthcare workers (e.g., Biron et al., 2009a; Weigl et al., 2011).
These sources of interruption also vary in attributes such as their channel of interaction
(i.e. direct or via technology) (McFarlane & Latorella, 2002), the sensory cues associated with
them (Lu, Wickens, Prinet, Hutchins, Sarter & Sebok, 2013), their information richness (Chong &
Siino, 2006), and the flexibility they provide in deciding when to engage with the interruption
(Nees & Fortna, 2015). Wajcman and Rose (2011) noted that the attributes of different sources of
interruptions combined with organizational norms to shape how employees prioritized these
sources. For instance, based on their organizational norms, employees in their study prioritized
cell phone calls over landline calls, and both were viewed to be more important than emails.
Although this hierarchy may vary by organizations, it can shape the occurrence of interruptions.
Overall, scholars have noted a variety of stimuli that can interrupt work. Our review,
however, indicates that these sources are usually studied separately (e.g., email interruptions), or
the focus is on their overall frequency. Very few studies have compared the effects of different
interruption sources (e.g., face-to-face vs. virtual interruptions: Nees & Fortna, 2015; instant
message vs. email interruptions: Tan & Richardson, 2011). As a result, the complex interplay
among these sources in today’s dynamic workplaces is poorly understooda point we return to
in the future research section. Next, we focus on what happens after the above sources interrupt
an ongoing work activity, in terms of the mediators and outcomes of a work interruption.
Mediating Mechanisms and Outcomes of Work Interruptions
Scholars have explored different mediators that transmit the effect of an interruption onto
various outcomes (see Figure 1). Our review shows that the outcomes of work interruptions,
studied in prior research, can be grouped into two broad categoriesperformance and well-
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 15
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
being. The former category is about an interruption’s effect on the performance of the interrupted
task and includes indicators of this effect such as errors, task resumption time, quality of work
etc. The latter category is about the interruption’s effect on the interrupted person and includes
indicators of this effect such as satisfaction, stress, emotional exhaustion, physical distress etc.
Regarding mediators, our review shows that research has used three broad theoretical
frameworkscognitive, self-regulatory, and affectiveto explain the mediating mechanisms of
an interruption. We review and integrate the theory and research related to mediators under each
framework and make explicit how different mediators are linked to the two types of outcomes
stated above. Thus, we describe the mechanisms that link interruptions to relevant outcomes.
Cognitive Pathway. Cognitive mediators are the most widely studied in prior research.
This stream of research adopts an ability-based focus to explore how the cognitive implications of
an interruption affect the performance of the interrupted task. Usually, since externally observable
and objectively measurable variables such as errors or task resumption/completion time are used
as indicators of the impact of an interruption’s cognitive demands, most studies adopt an objective
approach within experimental setups to unpack these effects. Theoretically, the most widely used
theory is the memory for goals theory that focuses on the memory-based suspension (activation)
of the goals of the interrupted (interrupting) task during an interruption (Altmann & Trafton,
2002; Trafton et al., 2003). Multiple studies have shown that the suspended interrupted task goals
decay from memory during a work interruption, leading to longer time for the resumption and
completion of the interrupted task, more errors, and lower performance (e.g., Altmann & Trafton,
2007; Altmann et al., 2014; Cades et al., 2008; Hodgetts & Jones, 2006; Monk et al., 2008).
A second group of studies focus on interruptions’ impact on attentional resources of the
interrupted person. Scholars have used the distraction-conflict theory (e.g., Baron, 1986) to argue
that the division of attentional resources between the interrupted and the interrupting task can
cause attentional conflict and high mental workload that negatively affect the quality of task
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 16
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
performance, especially on complex tasks (Gupta et al., 2013; Speier et al., 1999, 2003; Tan &
Richardson, 2011). The theory of attention residue also focuses on attentional division during an
interruption and theorizes that people have a tendency to cognitively linger on the interrupted
task, even after stopping it and starting a new task (Leroy, 2009). Such attention residue on the
interrupted task reduces the amount of attentional resources available to perform the interrupting
task, negatively affecting its performance (Leroy & Glomb, 2018; Leroy & Schmidt, 2016).
Lastly, scholars have focused on why people forget/remember to resume the interrupted
task (Grundgeiger et al., 2013). The prospective memory theory (Einstein & McDaniel, 1990,
1996) suggests that on being interrupted, people often form an intention of resuming the
interrupted task after the interruption (Dodhia & Dismukes, 2009). They then devote attentional
resources to monitoring these prospective intentions (Smith, 2003), and can cognitively retrieve
and execute these intentions on encountering cues related to these intentions (Cook, Meeks,
Clark‐Foos, Merritt & Marsh, 2014). The cognitive load of an interruption, however, can cause a
failure in these processes, leading to the forgetting of the interrupted task (McDaniel, Einstein,
Graham & Rall, 2004; Rummel, Wesslein & Meiser, 2017). In sum, studies on cognitive
mediators have adopted an ability-based focus to show that the cognitive toll of interruptions
usually impairs interrupted persons ability to perform the interrupted task efficiently.
Self-regulatory Pathway. Instead of an ability-based focus like above, self-regulatory
researchers focus on the motivational and energetic effects of work interruptions on both
performance and well-being outcomes. These studies can be grouped into two groups. The first
group focuses on work interruptions’ impact on the self-regulatory process in terms of the effect
on the goal progress of the interrupted task. Scholars use the control theory of self-regulation
(Carver & Scheier, 1990) and action regulation theory (Hacker, 2003), which posit a monitoring
system that compares one’s current state with a desired state. Negative discrepancies between
states imply low goal progress and have further motivational implications. For instance, Beck,
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 17
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Scholer and Hughes (2017) recently showed that interruptions impeded the rate of goal progress
on the interrupted task and were associated with frustration and low enthusiasm that led to
lowered effort, goal commitment, and performance. Relatedly, Rosen et al. (2019) showed that
leaders’ rating of daily email interruptions was linked to low goal progress, which led them to
cut back on daily leadership behaviors. Given the importance of people’s assessment of their
own goal progress and how it is affected by work interruptions, studies under this framework
usually adopt a subjective approach, often with a frequency focus. For example, employees in
Parke et al.’s (2018) study provided a subjective rating of the frequency of daily work
interruptions they encountered, which tended to weaken the positive relationship between daily
planning and job engagement. Over time, the hampered goal progress due to multiple work
interruptions can lead to high time pressure and subsequent irritation (Baethge & Rigotti, 2013;
Perlow, 1999). Overall, work interruptions were found to impede goal progress on the
interrupted task and thus impair self-regulation toward one’s work goals (Mitchell et al., 2008).
The second group of studies focus on work interruptions’ energetic implications and draw
on conservation of resources theory (Hobfoll, 1989) and ego-depletion theory (Baumeister &
Vohs, 2007). Interruptions are positioned as events that can deplete the limited store of self-
regulatory resources due to the need to unexpectedly halt work, understand and address the
interruption, and re-engage with the interrupted activity (Freeman & Muraven, 2010). Further,
self-regulatory resource depletion due to interruptions has the potential to cause anxiety,
emotional exhaustion, and physical distress (Lin et al., 2013). Scholars have also used the
compensatory cost model (Hockey, 1997) to posit that the extra effort needed to regulate around
interruptions to continue striving toward work goals can lead to unwanted compensatory costs
such as emotional exhaustion, low job satisfaction, and low situational well-being (Baethge &
Rigotti, 2015; Pachler, Kuonath, Specht, Kennecke, Agthe & Frey, 2018; Russell et al., 2017).
Thus, while interruptions unexpectedness adversely affects goal progress on the interrupted
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 18
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
task, the extra effort needed in regulating around interruptions can take a toll on well-being.
Affective Pathway. Since interruptions affect work goals, they have the potential to
trigger affective reactions (Frijda, 1986). While the above two approaches discuss affect briefly,
the focus is only on negative emotion as an outcome (e.g., Bailey & Konstan, 2006). But because
affect and emotions have ‘control precedence’ and divert resources away from an ongoing task
onto the affective/emotional state, these factors represent alternate mediators that can transmit
the effect of interruptions onto employee attitudes and behaviors (Frijda, 1986). Thus, to gain a
complete understanding of the interruption experience, a focus on affective mediators is crucial.
Mandler (1964) laid the foundation of this stream of research via interruption theory that
posits anxiety as a response to interruptions. Since people’s assessment of an event and their
internal affective states play a key role in affect-based theories, studies on affective reactions to
interruptions usually adopt a subjective approach with either a frequency focus (when studying
broad affect) or an episodic focus (when studying discrete emotions). More recently, scholars
have used affective events theory (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996), which posits that goal-relevant
work events can trigger affective reactions, as an overarching framework (e.g., Sonnentag et al.,
2018). As one example, Hunter et al. (2019) showed that daily family interruptions that hamper
work elicited negative affect, which was positively related to work-family conflict. However, the
same interruptions also triggered positive affect to the extent that they helped family goals.
Scholars have also used appraisal theory of emotions (Lazarus, 1991), which posits that
people’s appraisals of an event shape their affective reactions. Zohar and colleagues (2003) used
this theory to show that goal hindering (enhancing) events cause negative (positive) affect, and
Butts et al. (2015) used this theory to study the emotions triggered by email interruptions in non-
work hours. Using an episodic approach, they found that email interruptions could lead to anger
or happiness, depending on their length and tone, and anger was further related to work-family
conflict. Overall, while affective reactions are crucial, more research on discrete emotions
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 19
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
elicited by interruptions will add precision to our predictions and can help explain why the same
interruption leads to different outcomes, based on the emotion elicited (Frijda, 1986).
Summary. A look across the above three frameworks leads to several high-level
conclusions. First, work interruptions are complex events with cognitive, self-regulatory, and
affective implications for interrupted employees. Past research suggests that work interruptions
tend to burden one’s limited memory and attentional resources, impede goal progress on the
interrupted task, and trigger affective reactions. Typically, the cognitive mechanism is linked to
performance outcomes, the affective mechanism to well-being outcomes, and the self-regulatory
mechanism to both. However, while these relationships have been generally emphasized, it is
possible that cognitive mechanisms affect wellbeing outcomes and affective mechanisms affect
performance outcomes. As one example, Tams, Thatcher, Grover and Pak (2015) linked mental
workload (a cognitive mediator) arising from interruptions to stress. Further exploration of these
possible connections is thus a fruitful future research avenue that can expand our current
understanding of the relationships between mediators and outcomes shown in Figure 1. Our
review also showed that despite some studies indicating positive outcomes of interruptions in the
form of positive affect, overall, past research has generally stressed that work interruptions tend
to harm interrupted individuals performance and well-being.
Second, our review uncovered several imbalances in the extant interruption research. Not
only have studies focused heavily on external interruption sources at the expense of internal
sources, but the focus has also mainly been on the interrupted task and person. That is, although
the interrupting task and person are key constituents of an interruption, they are usually relegated
to being design elements in lab studies or boundary conditions. As we note in the future research
section, studies on the interrupter (e.g., Rivera, 2014) and the interrupting task (e.g., Leroy &
Schmidt, 2016) can broaden our understanding of interruptions. In terms of outcomes, how
interruptions affect team- and organization- level outcomes has been largely ignored in favor of
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 20
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
studying individual level outcomes. Taken together, we feel this lopsided focus of research on
work interruptions is a key reason for the heavy skew of past findings toward negative outcomes.
Third, despite the possible involvement of other people, the quality of social interaction
and the relational context of an interruption have been mostly ignored in prior work. Thus, the
scope of outcomes has been limited to performance/well-being outcomes, with interpersonal/
relational consequences remaining unexplored. In sum, our review presents a complex and
multifaceted view of interruptions, and suggests that this view may still be incomplete due to the
imbalance in prior interruption researcha point we return to in the future research section.
Moderators of the Occurrence and Impact of Work Interruptions
Many of the above relationships are subject to moderators that explain when, and for
whom, these effects are more or less intense. Moderators thus add nuance to our understanding
of work interruptions and their outcomes. As seen in Figure 1, our review reveals that these
moderators can be organized based on whether they reflect the attributes of the context, the tasks,
or the people involved in an interruption. Below, we elaborate more on the research on how these
attributes moderate the cognitive, self-regulatory, and affective pathways described above.
Contextual Attributes. Various contextual attributes have been studied to understand
when the sources of interruptions lead to a work interruption. For instance, workplace layouts
with open offices and collocation of coworkers typically lead to more inopportune interactions
and hence more work interruptions (Chong & Siino, 2006; Mark, 2015; Mark et al., 2005). The
work processes of an organization also matterPerlow (1999) found that high interdependence,
coupled with a competitive reward system, prompted employees to interrupt each other more
frequently. Relatedly, Perlow and Weeks (2002) showed that employees in an individualistic
culture tended to view help requests as work interruptions. Another factor is organizational
polychronicitythe extent to which employees feel their organization values multi-tasking
(Mattarelli, Bertolotti & Incerti, 2015). Employees in highly polychronic organizations may
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 21
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
engage in more simultaneous work activities, increasing the possibility of interrupting each
other. Contextual attributes also tend to influence the relationship between cognitive mediators
and subsequent outcomes. For instance, placing cues such as markings or signals that indicate the
progress made on the task before it was interrupted can help the resumption of that task after the
interruption by reminding people where they left the task when they were interrupted
(Grundgeiger et al., 2013; Trafton, Altmann & Brock, 2005).
Interrupted Task Attributes. The attributes of the interrupted task make up another set
of moderators, studied mainly in relation to interruptions’ impact on mediators. With regard to
cognitive processes, complexity of the interrupted task is a key moderator, since interruption of
more complex tasks leads to greater information processing burden and high mental workload
(e.g., Gupta et al., 2013; Tan & Richardson, 2011). The timing of when a task is interrupted is
also crucial, because the mental workload during an activity made of several subtasks fluctuates
constantly. It is lowest between two subtasks rather than in the middle of a subtask, making the
interruptions near the former point cognitively less taxing (e.g., Bailey & Iqbal, 2008; Bailey &
Konstan, 2006). Further, the regulatory framing of the interrupted task affects the attention
residue or the extent to which thoughts about the interrupted task persist even after it has been
stopped. Leroy and Schmidt (2016) found that participants found it easier to switch attention on
being interrupted, leading to low attention residue, when the interrupted task was promotion-
focused, and its completion was framed as helping them achieve their goals successfully.
While the above moderators affect cognitive mediators, stage of completion of the task
impacts self-regulatory mechanisms. Since motivation increases as one nears the completion of a
task, suspending the task on being interrupted near its completion requires more self-control and
causes more self-regulatory depletion than if interrupted earlier (Freeman & Muraven, 2010).
Interrupting Task Attributes. Although not always the case, people can be interrupted
by another task that needs to be performed. Studies have mainly emphasized the impact of the
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 22
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
attributes of interrupting tasks for the cognitive mediators. Like above, the complexity of the
interrupting task increases mental workload during a work interruption (Hodgetts & Jones, 2006;
Monk et al., 2008), and its regulatory framing affects attention residue. Here, however,
interrupting tasks that are prevention-focused and whose non-completion is framed as leading to
failure, make it easier to switch attention, leading to lower attention residue (Leroy & Schmidt,
2016). The duration of an interruption is also crucial as longer duration leads to greater decay of
the goals related to the interrupted task, increasing the time needed to resume and complete that
task (Foroughi, Werner, McKendrick, Cades & Boehm-Davis, 2016b; Monk & Kidd, 2008).
The effects of other interrupting task attributes in relation to cognitive mediators are
equivocal. Some scholars linked the similarity of the interrupting and interrupted tasks to fewer
errors (Eatchel, Kramer & Drews, 2012), while others did not find this effect (Pankok Jr et al.,
2017; Speier et al., 1999), and still others found the opposite effect (Edwards & Gronlund, 1998).
Mark, Gudith and Klocke (2008) suggested that variations in how similarity was operationalized
could have led to mixed results, but there is scope to probe these effects further. Findings about
interrupting task frequency are also mixed. While some scholars found that the frequency of the
interrupting task negatively affects performance on the interrupted task (Gupta et al., 2013; Parke
et al., 2018), others reported that it caused people to perform the interrupted task faster, but at a
cost to their well-being (Monk, 2004; Zijlstra, Roe, Leonora & Krediet, 1999). To reconcile
these findings, Baethge et al. (2015) advanced an inverted-U relationship, arguing that while the
psychological activation due to work interruptions may initially benefit performance, an increase
in interruption frequency may take a toll on the interrupted person and hinder performance.
For the self-regulatory pathway, Rosen et al. (2019) showed that the centrality of the
interrupting task to the core work of the interrupted person is an important moderator. In their
study, the negative impact of email interruptions on the goal progress of interrupted leaders was
less severe when emails were a central rather than a peripheral aspect of the leader’s core work.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 23
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
While this is a crucial finding, there is scope to further explore how interrupting task attributes
can moderate not only the self-regulatory but also the affective pathway as this can help predict
when these pathways are likely to be more or less severely experienced. As one example, while
simplicity-complexity of the interrupting task was shown to moderate the cognitive pathway, it
can also have implications for the other two pathways. Being interrupted by simple tasks that the
employee can complete quickly and easily may lead to a perception of goal progress and related
positive affect, as compared to when one is interrupted by complex, time-consuming tasks.
Interrupted Person Attributes. Apart from the attributes of an interruption’s tasks, the
attributes of the person who is interrupted also matter for all stages of our model. Starting with
the link between the sources and work interruption, researchers have shown that certain attributes
can help employees prevent workplace stimuli from interrupting them. For example, people high
in stimulus-screening ability and need for closure can weed out unnecessary workplace stimuli
and prevent them from becoming interruptions (Oldham et al., 1991; Szumowska & Kossowska,
2017). Similarly, Russell et al. (2017) showed that conscientiousness helped employees resist
checking emails in the middle of a task, thus preventing the emails from interrupting them.
Scholars have also studied how these attributes shape the relationship of an interruption
with its mediators. Starting with cognitive mediators, research indicates that high working
memory capacity can buffer the decay of interrupted task goals during interruptions, leading to
quicker task resumption and less post-interruption errors (e.g., Foroughi et al., 2016b; Westbrook
et al., 2018). Related to affective mediators, Zohar et al. (2003) found that employees with higher
workload experienced more negative affect following goal hindering events.
Finally, interrupted person attributes also affect the link between mediators and outcomes
of work interruptions. For the cognitive pathway, Tams et al. (2015) showed that interruption
self-efficacy—belief about one’s ability to be successful, despite interruptionsbuffered the
negative impact of mental workload due to interruptions on stress. Mark et al. (2008) found that
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 24
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
task completion time was shorter for people high in openness to experience and the need for
personal structure. For the self-regulatory pathway, people vary in their temperament and
resilience to the negative task-based effects of work interruptions (Van Den Berg et al., 1996;
Zide, Mills, Shahani-Denning & Sweetapple, 2017). In a study on leaders, Rosen et al. (2019)
showed that high self-control attenuated the negative effect of low goal progress due to email
interruptions on leader behavior. Pachler et al. (2018) showed that trait polychronicitya
preference for doing multiple tasks togetherreduced the negative impact of interruptions on
daily job satisfaction. Lastly, in a study on nurses, Baethge and Rigotti (2015) found a three-way
interaction effect of job tenure, alertness, and interruptions on well-being, such that the negative
effect of interruptions on well-being was strongest for nurses with short tenure and low alertness.
Interrupter Attributes. The attributes of the person doing the interrupting have not been
widely studied as moderators (Rivera-Rodriguez & Karsh, 2010). The few studies that have been
done indicate that people respond differently to interruptions from supervisors than to those by
peers. In a study on cognitive processes, Gupta et al. (2013) found that instant messenger
interruptions from supervisors, as opposed to peers, were given more importance, took up more
time, and led to lower task quality. Relatedly, theory posits that employees may feel less able to
delay interruptions from supervisors than from others (Rennecker & Godwin, 2005). With regard
to affective mediators, Butts et al. (2015) showed that emotional reaction to email interruptions in
non-work hours was shaped by whether the sender was a supervisor, and whether the supervisor
was abusive such that people experienced more anger when interrupted by an abusive supervisor.
Summary. Research on moderators of work interruptions adds nuance to our
understanding of how interruptions occur, and how they translate to final outcomes. Several
trends are apparent here. First, contextual attributes play a key role in shaping the occurrence of
interruptions, indicating that targeting an organization’s context can help manage the prevalence
of interruptions. However, research is still needed to explore how contextual attributes shape the
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 25
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
mediators and outcomes of interruptions. For example, since interruptions by coworkers are
situated in the broader context of team interactions or organizational communication norms, it
would be useful to examine how these aspects shape the experience of interruptions. Further,
since leaders tend to influence employee attitudes, researchers can also examine how leader
preference for time management or multitasking shapes employee response to interruptions.
Second, our review shows that attributes of the interrupted person are crucial moderators
of all the stages of our model, with implications for all the three mediating pathways. Overall,
past research has shown that employees differ in their sensitivity to both the occurrence and
impact of work interruptions. This suggests that employees may potentially respond differently
to various interruption management interventions, depending on the mechanism(s) targeted.
Third, research has exposed the complexity of interruptions by explaining how attributes
of the interrupted and interrupting tasks shape the cognitive impact of interruptions. Yet, this
research is limited mainly to the cognitive pathway. Scholars can expand the focus onto studying
how these attributes shape the motivational and affective impact of interruptions as well, since it
is possible that different attributes of these task (e.g., complexity, duration etc.) can shape the
impact of an interruption on employee goal progress or affect in important ways.
Fourth, while some studies have examined the moderating effect of interrupters’
attributes, there is scope for more research in this area. Moreover, the impact of the interrupter’s
relationship with the interrupted person also needs to be unpacked further. Since interruptions
tend to be embedded within this relationship, it may play a crucial role in shaping the responses
to interruptions. Finally, beyond studying different moderators in isolation, the literature will
also benefit from considering the interplay among the different moderators revealed in our
review. Overall, Figure 1 integrates prior findings across theoretical and disciplinary boundaries
to provide a comprehensive view of the causes, experiences, and outcomes of work interruptions.
Next, we turn to a discussion of how to meaningfully advance the interruption literature ahead.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 26
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
FUTURE RESEARCH RECOMMENDATIONS
Building further on the trends evident in our review, we have identified several broad
focus areas for future research. Table 2 provides a summary of the critical observations and also
lists future research recommendations related to each focus area. As noted previously, there are
several ways in which research on interruptions is lopsided, giving precedence to some aspects
of interruptions over other parallel aspects. Hence, we call for a more balanced approach to
studying interruptions that focuses on (a) internal sources of interruptions in addition to external
sources, (b) the interrupting task in addition to the interrupted task, (c) the interrupting person in
addition to the interrupted person and (d) the positive, in addition to the negative, outcomes of
interruptions. We also call for an enhanced focus on the social/relational aspects of interruptions,
the higher level effects of interruptions, and the complexity of workplace interruptions.
Internal Sources of Interruptions
Scholars have indicated that people at times interrupt themselves due to causes that are
internal to them such as mindwandering, rumination, pain, exhaustion, or prioritizing of other
tasks/activities (e.g., Adler & Benbunan-Fich, 2013; Fisher, 1998; Gatzounis et al., 2014; Jin &
Dabbish, 2009). But research on these sources remains sparse due to the difficulty of isolating
and measuring them. Scholars usually equate self-initiated task switches to internally caused
interruptions (e.g., Dabbish et al., 2011). Yet, as we have argued, not all task switches are work
interruptions. Also, while some internal sources from past research such as mindwandering or
pain originate within the interrupted person (e.g., Fisher, 1998; Gatzounis et al., 2014), other
internally caused interruptions may have more ambiguous origin. For example, Jin and Dabbish
(2009) viewed the suspension of a task on recalling something in response to an external cue or
stimulus to be an internally caused interruption, and Fletcher et al. (2018) viewed work
interruptions caused by discrepancies in one’s expectations and environmental conditions to be
internally caused. Are such interruptions that are self-initiated, but in response to an external cue
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 27
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
or condition, internally or externally caused? In this regard, Iqbal and Horvitz (2007) suggested
that stopping a task within 15 seconds of receiving or being exposed to an external cue can be
considered to be an externally caused interruption, while stopping the task after 15 seconds will
be an internally caused interruption. However, we lack theoretical basis for this cut-off, and as
such, there is less clarity, and hence scope for future research, on how interruptions caused by
internal sources differ from those caused by external sources in their experience and outcomes.
Scholars have already begun unpacking this complexity, and initial studies have yielded
interesting findings. For instance, internally caused interruptions, as compared to externally
caused ones, were associated with quicker resumption of the interrupted task (Cades, Werner,
Boehm-Davis & Arshad, 2010), but were also linked to longer overall time of completion for the
interrupted task (Katidioti, Borst, van Vugt & Taatgen, 2016). There is substantial scope to build
further on these studies and clarify how internally and externally caused interruptions differ or
overlap. For instance, scholars can study whether the attributes of the internal/external sources
moderate the experience of being interrupted. As one example, scholars can explore whether
internal sources provide more autonomy as they originate inside the interrupted employees, and
how this autonomy affects the experience of the interruption.
An additional point is that at times internal causes of interruption (e.g., mind-wandering
or rumination) may emerge from the outcomes (e.g., depletion) of prior interruptions. This points
to the possibility of ‘interruption cycles,’ where prior interruptions lead to subsequent internally
triggered interruptions (Dabbish et al., 2011). Future scholars can examine when (e.g., high daily
workload) and for whom (e.g., employees with low self-control) such work interruption cycles
are more or less likely to occur and how they shape the interruption experience. A focus on such
cycles can also aid the agenda of understanding the relationship between external sources of
initial interruptions (e.g., receiving email in the morning), and internal sources of subsequent
interruptions (e.g., rumination about the email in the afternoon). Overall, we call for more
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 28
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
research on what, why, and when internal causes interrupt work and how this affects employees.
Interrupting Task
Interruptions are often triggered by interrupting tasks that can be more urgent than the
ongoing task (Perlow, 1999). Thus, the outcome of the interrupting task will also have crucial
implications for employees, but these tasks have been largely overlooked in prior research. We
suggest two areas for research in this regard: (a) exploring factors that influence performance on
the interrupting task (e.g., Leroy & Schmidt, 2016), and (b) studying the combined effect of the
interrupting and interrupted tasks on employee outcomes. Scholars can adopt a multi-goal
perspective to model the simultaneous impact of the interrupting and the interrupted tasks on
employee outcomes (e.g., Hunter et al., 2019). As one example, Sonnentag et al. (2018) showed
that while online messages interrupted ongoing work and elicited negative affect, responding
quickly to these online messages led to a feeling of responsiveness and positive affect. More
such studies taking a broader perspective on work interruptions are needed.
Interrupter
Interruptions by other people usually occur at the convenience of the interrupter, but not
necessarily the interrupted person. The interrupter can thus benefit by gaining help or resources
(Rivera, 2014). But apart from work on when interrupters are considerate in interrupting others
(e.g., Romero, Szóstek, Kaptein & Markopoulos, 2007), questions about the interrupter remain
mostly unanswered. For example, when do interruptions harm or benefit the interrupter? Are
people aware that they are interrupting others? What attributes make people more or less likely
to interrupt others? Research on help/information seeking could provide guidance here as it
shows that employees seek help from, and possibly interrupt, coworkers to boost their own
performance, creativity, and satisfaction (Geller & Bamberger, 2012; Mueller & Kamdar, 2011).
Scholars can explicitly focus on instances where help-seekers interrupt others and examine how
such episodes unfold and affect the interrupter. They can also examine traits like extraversion,
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 29
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
self-monitoring, and agreeableness that could shape one’s tendency of interrupting others.
Bright Side of Interruptions
Despite the heavy emphasis on the negative outcomes of work interruptions in prior
research, scholars have also hinted at possible positive outcomes. For instance, interruptions can
at times provide key information that boosts interrupted employees’ goal progress, thus
positively affecting the self-regulatory pathway (e.g., Jett & George, 2003). Future scholars can
unpack when this is likely to happen. Perhaps the goal-boosting value of interruptions is high
early in the life of a work project when things are more uncertain and tentative. Apart from the
interrupted person, interruptions can also bring self-regulatory benefits for the interrupter by
providing access to crucial information that boosts their goal progress (Rivera, 2014). The
cognitive mediators of work interruptions can also at times lead to positive outcomes. Speier et
al. (1999) showed that the attentional narrowing due to interruptions helped decision making on
simple tasks. It is also possible that the attentional diversion caused by work interruptions
provides variety and intellectual stimulation to employees engaged in monotonous jobs, thus
serving as a source of job enrichment (Baethge et al., 2015). Interruptions can also satisfy
different high-level needs of interrupted employees, depending on their content. For example,
being interrupted for help/feedback may satisfy the need for competence, and having an
enjoyable chat with the interrupter may satisfy the relatedness need, leading to positive affect
and increased wellbeing (Gagné & Deci, 2005). An episodic approach will help scholars unpack
the impact of the specific content of work interruption episodes on interrupted employees’ needs.
Thus, interruptions can have positive effects in a variety of ways. Scholars must explore
how the valence, general affective nature, or goal pursuit implications of interruptions positively
affect the cognitive, self-regulatory, and affective pathways from our model. Overall, a focus on
the bright side will add much needed nuance to our understanding of interruptions’ outcomes.
Social and Relational Dynamics and Interpersonal Outcomes of Work Interruptions
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 30
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Building on the previous point, while interruptions often involve social interaction and
are embedded in the relational dynamics of the workplace, these factors are rarely examined. Our
review points to several reasons. First, lab studies cannot recreate these complex dynamics, nor
do these studies vary the social interaction quality between participants and the confederate (e.g.,
Conard & Marsh, 2016; Zijlstra et al., 1999). Second, in field studies, the social interaction
during an interruption is often ignored, or is impossible to capture due to a frequency approach
that focuses on the frequency rather than content of interruptions (e.g., Baethge & Rigotti, 2013;
Wilkes et al., 2018). Third, most interruption research focuses on performance and well-being
outcomes, while ignoring interpersonal outcomes (e.g., Lin et al., 2013; Parke et al., 2018).
Although initial evidence shows that relational factors such as interdependence and the
interrupter’s hierarchical level can shape the experience of an interruption (Gupta et al., 2013;
Perlow, 1999), there is room for much more insight in this space. First, scholars need to expand
their focus onto the quality of interaction during an interruption. Work on interpersonal justice
could be helpful here (e.g., Colquitt, 2001). For instance, do the parties to an interruption adhere
to, or violate, interpersonal justice rules such as respect or propriety, and how does this affect an
interruption’s outcomes? Second, scholars can explore the effects of interpersonal closeness or
exchange relationship with an interrupter with questions such as do interruptions from family
members differ from those by coworkers? In this case again, an episodic approach will help
scholars isolate these social/relational dynamics. Third, given leaders’ influence on employees
(Avolio, Walumbwa & Weber, 2009), does a leader’s leadership style or time management
preference shape employee attitudes toward interruptions? Lastly, scholars also need to expand
their focus onto interpersonal responses such as citizenship, incivility, and emotion regulation as
possible outcomes of interruptions. For instance, given that interruptions can trigger negative
emotions, does the interrupted person engage in surface acting to avoid showing these emotions?
Moving Beyond the Individual Level
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 31
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Although interruptions are individual level events, their effects can manifest at higher
levels of analysis (Addas & Pinsonneault, 2013). Research indicates that work interruptions can
lead to team level information sharing and benefit team flexibility, but can also increase
coordination burden (Okhuysen & Eisenhardt, 2002; Tremblay, Vachon, Lafond & Kramer,
2012; Zellmer-Bruhn, 2003). More work is needed, however, to completely understand the team
level implications of work interruptions. For instance, how are the communication patterns
within and between teams affected by work interruptions? Are virtual teams affected differently
than non-virtual teams? How are interruptions shaped by team cohesion, conflict, and politics?
It could also be that while work interruptions harm the goal progress of an individual, they
may benefit the team via knowledge sharing, or by providing information to an interrupting team
member at a critical time. It is also possible that norms around when and how interruptions occur,
and who is interrupted emerge in teams. For instance, Oldroyd and Morris (2012) noted that star
employees face more interruptions for help due to their elevated status, leading to information
overload and decreased engagement. It is crucial to examine norms around work interruptions and
the effect of a fit/misfit between these norms and individual interruption handling preferences. In
sum, there is substantial scope for future research on higher level effects of work interruptions.
Complexity of Workplace Interruptions
Over time, research has moved from studying static, linear tasks in the lab to studying
workplace interruptions (e.g., Baethge et al., 2015). Yet, interruptions from different sources are
either studied separately (e.g., email interruptions) or are clubbed together to focus on their
frequency. This hides the interplay of multiple, nested, and simultaneous interruptions common
in today’s dynamic workplaces, due to which the complexity of workplace interruptions is not
well understood (Werner & Holden, 2015). One way of addressing this issue is by using
methodologies that capture this complexity. Scholars have recently used microworld simulations
(e.g., Tremblay et al., 2012), the dual perspectives method where the interrupter and interrupted
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 32
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
person are observed together (e.g., McCurdie, Sanderson & Aitken, 2017), social network
analysis (e.g., McCurdie, Sanderson & Aitken, 2018), and socio-technical system approaches
(e.g., Rivera, 2014; Werner & Holden, 2015) for this purpose. We call for more studies using
these approaches to unpack the dynamic interplay among different sources of work interruptions.
Another largely unexplored aspect of work interruptions’ complexity is the impact of
their distribution. While the effects of work interruptions’ mean level have been widely studied,
the impact of their distribution is less explored. Some work interruptions occur more regularly
(e.g., unexpected emails) than others (e.g., video conferences). Mean-based research might
suggest that the former are more impactful than the latter. But it is possible that people develop
standardized responses to frequently occurring work interruptions, as opposed to less routine
work interruptions. The repeated execution of these responses can help people adapt to chronic
work interruptions, in turn, making these interruptions less disruptive. In that case, infrequent
interruptions would have more impact on employee outcomes. Apart from variation in sources,
the distribution of interruptions may also vary based on the time of the day/week or stage of a
project. Thus, scholars can study how this variance in interruptions affects different outcomes.
Another avenue for future research is the agency of the interrupted employee. Past
research largely considers people to be passive recipients of interruptions, with little recourse but
to react to them. Lab participants have little choice but to attend to interruptions imposed on
them (e.g., Zijlstra et al., 1999), and field studies focus on reactions after interruption occurrence
(e.g., Parke et al., 2018). However, employees can seek to actively manage work interruptions
(Wajcman & Rose, 2011). Assuming employee agency in this experience expands the outcomes
to proactive behaviors and job-crafting. Given the increasing autonomy about the time and place
of work, employees can proactively craft their jobs to manage how, when, and by whom they are
interrupted. For example, they may use artifacts such as closed doors, do-not-disturb signs, post-
it notes, headphones or ear buds, and even flow lights to manage the experience and impact of
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 33
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
interruptions (e.g., González & Mark, 2004; Sykes, 2011; Züger et al., 2017). They may even
time their arrival/departure from the workplace, choose specific work shifts or work locations,
and manage coworker interactions and relationships to regulate exposure to interruptions (Chong
& Siino, 2006). Overall, we call for more research on the agency of interrupted employees.
PRACTICAL IMPLICATIONS
Given their impact on daily work life, research on interruptions has practical relevance.
Our review uncovered two somewhat competing types of practical advice in the interruption
literature. The first is the elimination, or at least minimization, of work interruptions, based on
studies that view work interruptions as stressors (e.g., Baethge & Rigotti, 2013; Lin et al., 2013;
Perlow, 1999). The second is the acceptance and active management of work interruptions to
reduce their disruptive effects while preserving their benefits (Jett & George, 2003). We feel the
latter is more realistic, as removal of all work interruptions is not feasible in today’s dynamic
workplaces (Leroy & Glomb, 2018). Further, as we have noted, work interruptions can at times
have beneficial effects that organizations may lose out on if they prioritize only elimination of
work interruptions. Thus, appropriate management of workplace interruptions is warranted.
For employees, this could mean anticipating interruptions and adding back-up plans that
ensure goal progress, despite interruptions (Parke et al., 2018). They can also use practices such
as marking the interrupted task’s progress or making ready-to resume plans before engaging with
the interruption to ensure quick resumption of work post-interruption (e.g., Leroy & Glomb,
2018; Trafton et al., 2005). There is also continued refinement of different features of
applications and software to reduce their disruptive effects and increase flexibility of handling
interruptions (e.g., Dabbish & Kraut, 2008; Hodgetts, Tremblay, Vallières & Vachon, 2015;
McFarlane, 2002). For example, email platforms now allow one to organize, batch, and respond
to emails, and also snooze alerts (e.g., Addas & Pinsonneault, 2018; Mark, Iqbal, Czerwinski,
Johns, Sano & Lutchyn, 2016). Also relevant in today’s workplaces is the management of social
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 34
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
media activity to reduce its disruptive impact. For example, there is software that blocks social
media websites during work hours (e.g., Chang, 2017; Mark, Czerwinski & Iqbal, 2018).
Managers can play a key role in the management of work interruptions in several ways.
First, they can adopt ready-to-use solutions or redesign workspaces to control interruptions’
disruptive effects, while still ensuring collaboration (e.g., Sykes, 2011; Wiberg & Whittaker,
2005; Züger et al., 2017). Second, managers can shape employee attitudes toward interruptions
in a beneficial way (Donmez, Matson, Savan, Farahani, Photiadis & Dafoe, 2014). For example,
they can frame help requests as learning opportunities rather than interruptions (Perlow &
Weeks, 2002). Further, training employees on work interruptions’ disruptive effects, and
promoting team identification can make them considerate when interrupting each other (Cades,
Boehm-Davis, Trafton & Monk, 2011; Dabbish & Kraut, 2004). Third, it is crucial for managers
to empower employees to manage work interruptions on their own terms because people differ in
their resilience to work interruptions (Mark et al., 2016; Zide et al., 2017). Moreover, fostering a
multi-goal outlook can highlight a work interruption’s contribution to the interrupting task and
thus alleviate the negative reactions due to hindered goal progress on the interrupted task (Hunter
et al., 2019). In sum, we recommend that instead of a ‘reductionist’ approach, organizations must
focus on better management of workplace interruptions (Relihan et al., 2010).
CONCLUSION
In today’s dynamic workplaces, work interruptions are widespread and inevitable. In our
review, we have advanced an integrative definition of work interruption, reviewed the different
approaches to studying this construct, organized findings from prior research, and presented
avenues for future research. While there has been substantial research on interruptions that has
yielded a great deal of knowledge about interruptions’ experience and outcomes, there is also a
great deal we still do not know. Based on our review, we call for a balanced approach to studying
work interruptions that unpacks the complexity of this widely occurring workplace phenomenon.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 35
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
REFERENCES
Adamczyk, P. D., & Bailey, B. P. 2004. If not now, when?: The effects of interruption at
different moments within task execution. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on
Human Factors in Computing Systems: 271-278.
Addas, S., & Pinsonneault, A. 2013. IT interruptions and coordination effectiveness in software
development groups: A conceptual, multilevel model. Proceedings of the SIGCHI
Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems: 1-5.
Addas, S., & Pinsonneault, A. 2018. E-Mail interruptions and individual performance: Is there a
silver lining? Management Information Systems Quarterly, 42: 381-405.
Adler, R. F., & Benbunan-Fich, R. 2013. Self-interruptions in discretionary multitasking.
Computers in Human Behavior, 29: 1441-1449.
Altmann, E. M., & Trafton, J. G. 2002. Memory for goals: An activation-based model. Cognitive
Science, 26: 39-83.
Altmann, E. M., & Trafton, J. G. 2007. Timecourse of recovery from task interruption: Data and
a model. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14: 1079-1084.
Altmann, E. M., Trafton, J. G., & Hambrick, D. Z. 2014. Momentary interruptions can derail the
train of thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143: 215-226.
Andreasson, R., Lindblom, J., & Thorvald, P. 2017. Interruptions in the wild: Portraying the
handling of interruptions in manufacturing from a distributed cognition lens. Cognition,
Technology & Work, 19: 85-108.
Avolio, B. J., Walumbwa, F. O., & Weber, T. J. 2009. Leadership: Current theories, research,
and future directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 60: 421-449.
Baethge, A., & Rigotti, T. 2013. Interruptions to workflow: Their relationship with irritation and
satisfaction with performance, and the mediating roles of time pressure and mental
demands. Work and Stress, 27: 43-63.
Baethge, A., & Rigotti, T. 2015. Three-way interactions among interruptions/multitasking demands,
occupational age, and alertness: A diary study. Work, Aging and Retirement, 1: 393-410.
Baethge, A., Rigotti, T., & Roe, R. A. 2015. Just more of the same, or different? An integrative
theoretical framework for the study of cumulative interruptions at work. European
Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 24: 308-323.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 36
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Bailey, B. P., & Iqbal, S. T. 2008. Understanding changes in mental workload during execution
of goal-directed tasks and its application for interruption management. Transactions on
Computer-Human Interaction (TOCHI), 14: 21-28.
Bailey, B. P., & Konstan, J. A. 2006. On the need for attention-aware systems: Measuring effects
of interruption on task performance, error rate, and affective state. Computers in Human
Behavior, 22: 685-708.
Baron, R. S. 1986. Distraction-conflict theory: Progress and problems. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology: 1-40. New York: Academic Press.
Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. 2007. Self‐Regulation, ego depletion, and motivation. Social
and Personality Psychology Compass, 1: 115-128.
Beal, D. J., Weiss, H. M., Barros, E., & MacDermid, S. M. 2005. An episodic process model of
affective influences on performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90: 1054-1068.
Beck, J. W., Scholer, A. A., & Hughes, J. 2017. Divergent effects of distance versus velocity
disturbances on emotional experiences during goal pursuit. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 102: 1109-1123.
Bellandi, T., Cerri, A., Carreras, G., Walter, S., Mengozzi, C., Albolino, S., Mastrominico, E.,
Renzetti, F., Tartaglia, R., & Westbrook, J. 2018. Interruptions and multitasking in
surgery: A multicentre observational study of the daily work patterns of doctors and
nurses. Ergonomics, 61: 40-47.
Biron, A. D., Lavoie‐Tremblay, M., & Loiselle, C. G. 2009a. Characteristics of work interruptions
during medication administration. Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 41: 330-336.
Biron, A. D., Loiselle, C. G., & Lavoie‐Tremblay, M. 2009b. Work interruptions and their
contribution to medication administration errors: An evidence review. Worldviews on
Evidence‐Based Nursing, 6: 70-86.
Boehm-Davis, D. A., & Remington, R. 2009. Reducing the disruptive effects of interruption: A
cognitive framework for analysing the costs and benefits of intervention strategies.
Accident Analysis & Prevention, 41: 1124-1129.
Brixey, J. J., Robinson, D. J., Johnson, C. W., Johnson, T. R., Turley, J. P., & Zhang, J. 2007. A
concept analysis of the phenomenon interruption. Advances in Nursing Science, 30: E26-E42.
Brumby, D. P., Cox, A. L., Back, J., & Gould, S. J. J. 2013. Recovering from an interruption:
Investigating speed− accuracy trade-offs in task resumption behavior. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Applied, 19: 95-107.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 37
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Butts, M. M., Becker, W. J., & Boswell, W. R. 2015. Hot buttons and time sinks: The effects of
electronic communication during nonwork time on emotions and work-nonwork conflict.
Academy of Management Journal, 58: 763-788.
Cades, D. M., Boehm-Davis, D. A., Trafton, J. G., & Monk, C. A. 2011. Mitigating disruptive
effects of interruptions through training: What needs to be practiced? Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17: 97-109.
Cades, D. M., Werner, N., Boehm-Davis, D. A., Trafton, J. G., & Monk, C. A. 2008. Dealing
with interruptions can be complex, but does interruption complexity matter: a mental
resources approach to quantifying disruptions. Proceedings of the Human Factors and
Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting: 398-402.
Cades, D. M., Werner, N. E., Boehm-Davis, D. A., & Arshad, Z. 2010. What makes real-world
interruptions disruptive? Evidence from an office setting. Proceedings of the Human
Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting: 448-452.
Cai, X., Gong, J., Lu, Y., & Zhong, S. 2017. Recover overnight? Work interruption and worker
productivity. Management Science, 1: 28-57.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. 1990. Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A
control-process view. Psychological Review, 97: 19-35.
Chang, H. J. 2017. Facebook interruptions in the workplace from a media uses perspective: A
longitudinal analysis. In M. Friedrichsen and Y. Kamalipour (Eds.), Digital
transformation in journalism and news media: 319-337. Cham, Switzerland: Springer.
Chisholm, C. D., Collison, E. K., Nelson, D. R., & Cordell, W. H. 2000. Emergency department
workplace interruptions: Are emergency physicians “interrupt‐driven” and
“multitasking”? Academic Emergency Medicine, 7: 1239-1243.
Chisholm, C. D., Dornfeld, A. M., Nelson, D. R., & Cordell, W. H. 2001. Work interrupted: a
comparison of workplace interruptions in emergency departments and primary care
offices. Annals of Emergency Medicine, 38: 146-151.
Chong, J., & Siino, R. 2006. Interruptions on software teams: A comparison of paired and solo
programmers. Proceedings of the 20th Anniversary Conference on Computer Supported
Cooperative Work: 29-38.
Colquitt, J. A. 2001. On the dimensionality of organizational justice: A construct validation of a
measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86: 386-400.
Conard, M. A., & Marsh, R. F. 2016. Self-efficacy matters more than interruptions in a
sequential multitasking experiment. Psicológica, 37: 15-34.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 38
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Cook, G. I., Meeks, J. T., Clark‐Foos, A., Merritt, P. S., & Marsh, R. L. 2014. The role of
interruptions and contextual associations in delayed‐execute prospective memory.
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28: 91-103.
Couffe, C., & Michael, G. A. 2017. Failures due to interruptions or distractions: A review and a
new framework. American Journal of Psychology, 130: 163-181.
Czerwinski, M., Horvitz, E., & Wilhite, S. 2004. A diary study of task switching and
interruptions. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing
Systems: 175-182.
Dabbish, L., & Kraut, R. 2008. Research noteawareness displays and social motivation for
coordinating communication. Information Systems Research, 19: 221-238.
Dabbish, L., & Kraut, R. E. 2004. Controlling interruptions: Awareness displays and social
motivation for coordination. Proceedings of the 2004 ACM Conference on Computer
Supported Cooperative Work: 182-191.
Dabbish, L., Mark, G., & González, V. M. 2011. Why do I keep interrupting myself?:
Environment, habit and self-interruption. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on
Human Factors in Computing Systems: 3127-3130.
Dodhia, R. M., & Dismukes, R. K. 2009. Interruptions create prospective memory tasks. Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 23: 73-89.
Donmez, B., Matson, Z., Savan, B., Farahani, E., Photiadis, D., & Dafoe, J. 2014. Interruption
management and office norms: Technology adoption lessons from a product
commercialization study. International Journal of Information Management, 34: 741-750.
Eatchel, K. A., Kramer, H., & Drews, F. 2012. The effects of interruption context on task
performance. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual
Meeting: 2118-2122.
Edwards, M. B., & Gronlund, S. D. 1998. Task interruption and its effects on memory. Memory,
6: 665-687.
Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. 1990. Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16: 717-726.
Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. 1996. Retrieval processes in prospective memory:
Theoretical approaches and some new empirical findings. In M. Brandimonte, G. O.
Einstein and M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications: 115-
141. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 39
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Eyrolle, H., & Cellier, J. M. 2000. The effects of interruptions in work activity: Field and
laboratory results. Applied Ergonomics, 31: 537-543
Fisher, C. D. 1998. Effects of external and internal interruptions on boredom at work: Two
studies. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19: 503-522.
Fletcher, K. A., Potter, S. M., & Telford, B. N. 2018. Stress outcomes of four types of perceived
interruptions. Human Factors, 60: 222-235.
Flynn, E. A., Barker, K. N., Gibson, J. T., Pearson, R. E., Berger, B. A., & Smith, L. A. 1999.
Impact of interruptions and distractions on dispensing errors in an ambulatory care
pharmacy. American Journal of Health-System Pharmacy, 56: 1319-1325.
Fonner, K. L., & Roloff, M. E. 2012. Testing the connectivity paradox: Linking teleworkers'
communication media use to social presence, stress from interruptions, and
organizational identification. Communication Monographs, 79: 205-231.
Foroughi, C. K., Malihi, P., & Boehm-Davis, D. A. 2016a. Working memory capacity and errors
following interruptions. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5: 410-414.
Foroughi, C. K., Werner, N. E., McKendrick, R., Cades, D. M., & Boehm-Davis, D. A. 2016b.
Individual differences in working-memory capacity and task resumption following
interruptions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
42: 1480-1488.
Freeman, N., & Muraven, M. 2010. Don’t interrupt me! Task interruption depletes the self’s
limited resources. Motivation and Emotion, 34: 230-241.
Frijda, N. H. 1986. The emotions: Studies in emotion and social interaction. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Gagné, M., & Deci, E. L. 2005. Self‐determination theory and work motivation. Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 26: 331-362.
Gatzounis, R., Schrooten, M. G. S., Crombez, G., & Vlaeyen, J. W. S. 2014. Interrupted by pain:
An anatomy of pain-contingent activity interruptions. Pain, 155: 1192-1195.
Geller, D., & Bamberger, P. A. 2012. The impact of help seeking on individual task
performance: The moderating effect of help seekers' logics of action. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 97: 487-497.
Gillie, T., & Broadbent, D. 1989. What makes interruptions disruptive? A study of length,
similarity, and complexity. Psychological Research, 50: 243-250.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 40
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
González, V. M., & Mark, G. 2004. Constant, constant, multi-tasking craziness: Managing
multiple working spheres. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in
Computing Systems: 113-120.
Grandhi, S. A., & Jones, Q. 2015. Knock, knock! who׳ s there? Putting the user in control of
managing interruptions. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 79: 35-50.
Grundgeiger, T., Dekker, S., Sanderson, P., Brecknell, B., Liu, D., & Aitken, L. M. 2016.
Obstacles to research on the effects of interruptions in healthcare. BMJ Quality and
Safety, 25: 392-395.
Grundgeiger, T., & Sanderson, P. 2009. Interruptions in healthcare: Theoretical views.
International Journal of Medical Informatics, 78: 293-307.
Grundgeiger, T., Sanderson, P. M., Beltran Orihuela, C., Thompson, A., MacDougall, H. G.,
Nunnink, L., & Venkatesh, B. 2013. Prospective memory in the ICU: The effect of visual
cues on task execution in a representative simulation. Ergonomics, 56: 579-589.
Gupta, A., Li, H., & Sharda, R. 2013. Should I send this message? Understanding the impact of
interruptions, social hierarchy and perceived task complexity on user performance and
perceived workload. Decision Support Systems, 55: 135-145.
Hacker, W. 2003. Action regulation theory: A practical tool for the design of modern work
processes? European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 12: 105-130.
Healey, A. N., Primus, C. P., & Koutantji, M. 2007. Quantifying distraction and interruption in
urological surgery. BMJ Quality and Safety, 16: 135-139.
Hickam, D. H., Severance, S., Feldstein, A., Ray, L., Gorman, P., Schuldheis, S., Hersh, W. R.,
Krages, K. P., & Helfand, M. 2003. The effect of health care working conditions on
patient safety. Evidence Report/Technology Assessment Number 74: 1-206. Rockville,
MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality.
Hobfoll, S. E. 1989. Conservation of resources: A new attempt at conceptualizing stress.
American Psychologist, 44: 513-524.
Hockey, G. R. J. 1997. Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress
and high workload: A cognitive-energetical framework. Biological Psychology, 45: 73-93.
Hodgetts, H. M., & Jones, D. M. 2006. Interruption of the Tower of London Task: Support for a
goal-activation approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135: 103-115.
Hodgetts, H. M., Tremblay, S., Vallières, B. R., & Vachon, F. 2015. Decision support and
vulnerability to interruption in a dynamic multitasking environment. International
Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 79: 106-117.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 41
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Hopkinson, S. G., & Jennings, B. M. 2013. Interruptions during nurses' work: A state‐of‐the‐
science review. Research in Nursing & Health, 36: 38-53.
Hunter, E. M., Clark, M. A., & Carlson, D. S. 2019. Violating work-family boundaries:
Reactions to interruptions at work and home. Journal of Management, 45: 1284-1308.
Iqbal, S. T., & Horvitz, E. 2007. Disruption and recovery of computing tasks: field study, analysis,
and directions. SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems: 677-686.
Jackson, T., Dawson, R., & Wilson, D. 2003. Reducing the effect of email interruptions on
employees. International Journal of Information Management, 23: 55-65.
Jett, Q. R., & George, J. M. 2003. Work interrupted: A closer look at the role of interruptions in
organizational life. Academy of Management Review, 28: 494-507.
Jin, J., & Dabbish, L. A. 2009. Self-interruption on the computer: A typology of discretionary
task interleaving. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in
Computing Systems: 1799-1808.
Katidioti, I., Borst, J. P., van Vugt, M. K., & Taatgen, N. A. 2016. Interrupt me: External
interruptions are less disruptive than self-interruptions. Computers in Human Behavior,
63: 906-915.
Kim, S., Park, Y., & Headrick, L. 2018. Daily micro-breaks and job performance: General work
engagement as a cross-level moderator. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103: 772-786.
Kirchberg, D. M., Roe, R. A., & Van Eerde, W. 2015. Polychronicity and multitasking: A diary
study at work. Human Performance, 28: 112-136.
Lazarus, R. S. 1991. Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.
Leroy, S. 2009. Why is it so hard to do my work? The challenge of attention residue when switching
between work tasks. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109: 168-181.
Leroy, S., & Glomb, T. M. 2018. Tasks interrupted: How anticipating time pressure on
resumption of an interrupted task causes attention residue and low performance on
interrupting tasks and how a “ready-to-resume” plan mitigates the effects. Organization
Science, 29: 380-397.
Leroy, S., & Schmidt, A. M. 2016. The effect of regulatory focus on attention residue and
performance during interruptions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 137: 218-235.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 42
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Li, S. Y. W., Magrabi, F., & Coiera, E. 2012. A systematic review of the psychological literature
on interruption and its patient safety implications. Journal of the American Medical
Informatics Association, 19: 6-12.
Lin, B. C., Kain, J. M., & Fritz, C. 2013. Don’t interrupt me! An examination of the relationship
between intrusions at work and employee strain. International Journal of Stress
Management, 20: 77-94.
Lu, S. A., Wickens, C. D., Prinet, J. C., Hutchins, S. D., Sarter, N., & Sebok, A. 2013.
Supporting interruption management and multimodal interface design: three meta-
analyses of task performance as a function of interrupting task modality. Human Factors,
55: 697-724.
Mandler, G. 1964. The interruption of behavior. Nebraska symposium on motivation, 12: 163-219.
Mansi, G., & Levy, Y. 2013. Do instant messaging interruptions help or hinder knowledge workers’
task performance? International Journal of Information Management, 33: 591-596.
Mark, G. 2015. Multitasking in the digital age. San Rafael, CA: Morgan & Claypool.
Mark, G., Czerwinski, M., & Iqbal, S. T. 2018. Effects of individual differences in blocking
workplace distractions. Proceedings of the CHI Conference on Human Factors in
Computing Systems: 1-12.
Mark, G., Gonzalez, V. M., & Harris, J. 2005. No task left behind?: Examining the nature of
fragmented work. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in
Computing Systems: 321-330.
Mark, G., Gudith, D., & Klocke, U. 2008. The cost of interrupted work: More speed and stress.
Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems: 107-110.
Mark, G., Iqbal, S. T., Czerwinski, M., Johns, P., Sano, A., & Lutchyn, Y. 2016. Email duration,
batching and self-interruption: Patterns of email use on productivity and stress.
Proceedings of the CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems: 1717-1728.
Mattarelli, E., Bertolotti, F., & Incerti, V. 2015. The interplay between organizational
polychronicity, multitasking behaviors and organizational identification: A mixed-
methods study in knowledge intensive organizations. International Journal of Human-
Computer Studies, 79: 6-19.
McCurdie, T., Sanderson, P., & Aitken, L. M. 2017. Traditions of research into interruptions in
healthcare: A conceptual review. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 66: 23-36.
McCurdie, T., Sanderson, P., & Aitken, L. M. 2018. Applying social network analysis to the
examination of interruptions in healthcare. Applied Ergonomics, 67: 50-60.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 43
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
McCurdie, T., Sanderson, P., Aitken, L. M., & Liu, D. 2017. Two sides to every story: The Dual
Perspectives Method for examining interruptions in healthcare. Applied Ergonomics, 58:
102-109.
McDaniel, M. A., Einstein, G. O., Graham, T., & Rall, E. 2004. Delaying execution of intentions:
Overcoming the costs of interruptions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18: 533-547.
McFarlane, D. C. 1997. Interruption of people in human-computer interaction: A general
unifying definition of human interruption and taxonomy. Technical Report - NRL Formal
Report NRL/ FR/551097-9870. Washington, DC: US Naval Research Lab.
McFarlane, D. C. 2002. Comparison of four primary methods for coordinating the interruption of
people in human-computer interaction. Human-Computer Interaction, 17: 63-139.
McFarlane, D. C., & Latorella, K. A. 2002. The scope and importance of human interruption in
human-computer interaction design. Human-Computer Interaction, 17: 1-61.
Mitchell, T. R., Harman, W. S., Lee, T. W., & Lee, D. Y. 2008. Self-regulation and multiple
deadline goals. In R. Kanfer, G. Chen and R. D. Pritchard (Eds.), Work motivation: Past,
present, and future:197-231. New York: Routledge.
Miyata, Y., & Norman, D. A. 1986. Psychological issues in support of multiple activities. In D.
A. Norman and S. W. Draper (Eds.), User centered system design: New perspectives on
human-computer interaction: 265-284. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Monk, C. A. 2004. The effect of frequent versus infrequent interruptions on primary task resumption.
Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting: 295-299.
Monk, C. A., & Kidd, D. G. 2008. The effects of brief interruptions on task resumption.
Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting: 403-407.
Monk, C. A., Trafton, J. G., & Boehm-Davis, D. A. 2008. The effect of interruption duration and
demand on resuming suspended goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14:
299-313.
Mueller, J. S., & Kamdar, D. 2011. Why seeking help from teammates is a blessing and a curse:
A theory of help seeking and individual creativity in team contexts. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 96: 263-276.
Myers, R. A., McCarthy, M. C., Whitlatch, A., & Parikh, P. J. 2016. Differentiating between
detrimental and beneficial interruptions: A mixed-methods study. BMJ Quality and
Safety, 25: 881-888.
Nees, M. A., & Fortna, A. 2015. A comparison of human versus virtual interruptions.
Ergonomics, 58: 852-856.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 44
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
O'Conaill, B., & Frohlich, D. 1995. Timespace in the workplace: Dealing with interruptions.
Conference Companion on Human Factors in Computing Systems: 262-263.
Okhuysen, G. A., & Eisenhardt, K. M. 2002. Integrating knowledge in groups: How formal
interventions enable flexibility. Organization Science, 13: 370-386.
Oldham, G. R., Kulik, C. T., & Stepina, L. P. 1991. Physical environments and employee
reactions: Effects of stimulus-screening skills and job complexity. Academy of
Management Journal, 34: 929-938.
Oldroyd, J. B., & Morris, S. S. 2012. Catching falling stars: A human resource response to social
capital's detrimental effect of information overload on star employees. Academy of
Management Review, 37: 396-418.
Pachler, D., Kuonath, A., Specht, J., Kennecke, S., Agthe, M., & Frey, D. 2018. Workflow
interruptions and employee work outcomes: The moderating role of polychronicity.
Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 23: 417-427.
Pankok Jr, C., Zahabi, M., Zhang, W., Choi, I., Liao, Y. F., Nam, C. S., & Kaber, D. 2017. The
effects of interruption similarity and complexity on performance in a simulated visual-
manual assembly operation. Applied Ergonomics, 59: 94-103.
Parke, M. R., Weinhardt, J. M., Brodsky, A., Tangirala, S., & DeVoe, S. E. 2018. When daily
planning improves employee performance: The importance of planning type,
engagement, and interruptions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103: 300312.
Perlow, L., & Weeks, J. 2002. Who's helping whom? Layers of culture and workplace behavior.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 23: 345-361.
Perlow, L. A. 1999. The time famine: Toward a sociology of work time. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 44: 57-81.
Raban, M. Z., & Westbrook, J. I. 2014. Are interventions to reduce interruptions and errors
during medication administration effective?: A systematic review. BMJ Quality and
Safety, 23: 414-421.
Relihan, E., O'brien, V., O'hara, S., & Silke, B. 2010. The impact of a set of interventions to
reduce interruptions and distractions to nurses during medication administration. Quality
& Safety in Health Care, 19 (5): 1-6.
Rennecker, J., & Godwin, L. 2005. Delays and interruptions: A self-perpetuating paradox of
communication technology use. Information and Organization, 15: 247-266.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 45
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Rissler, R., Nadj, M., Adam, M., & Maedche, A. 2017. Towards an integrative theoretical
framework of IT-mediated interruptions. 25th European Conference on Information
Systems (ECIS): 1950-1967.
Rivera-Rodriguez, A. J., & Karsh, B. T. 2010. Interruptions and distractions in healthcare:
Review and reappraisal. BMJ Quality & Safety, 19: 304-312.
Rivera, A. J. 2014. A socio-technical systems approach to studying interruptions: Understanding
the interrupter's perspective. Applied Ergonomics, 45: 747-756.
Romero, N., Szóstek, A. M., Kaptein, M., & Markopoulos, P. 2007. Behaviours and preferences
when coordinating mediated interruptions: Social and system influence. In Bannon L.J.,
Wagner I., Gutwin C., Harper R.H.R. and S. K. (Eds.), ECSCW 2007: 351-370. London:
Springer.
Rosen, C. C., Simon, L. S., Gajendran, R. S., Johnson, R. E., Lee, H. W., & Lin, S.-H. J. 2019.
Boxed in by your inbox: Implications of daily e-mail demands for managers’ leadership
behaviors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 104: 19-33.
Rummel, J., Wesslein, A. K., & Meiser, T. 2017. The role of action coordination for prospective
memory: Task-interruption demands affect intention realization. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43: 717-735.
Russell, E., Woods, S. A., & Banks, A. P. 2017. Examining conscientiousness as a key resource
in resisting email interruptions: Implications for volatile resources and goal achievement.
Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 90: 407-435.
Sanderson, P. M., & Grundgeiger, T. 2015. How do interruptions affect clinician performance in
healthcare? Negotiating fidelity, control, and potential generalizability in the search for
answers. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 79: 85-96.
Sasangohar, F., Donmez, B., Trbovich, P., & Easty, A. C. 2012. Not all interruptions are created
equal: Positive interruptions in healthcare. Proceedings of the Human Factors and
Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting: 824-828.
Smith, R. E. 2003. The cost of remembering to remember in event-based prospective memory:
Investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29: 347-361.
Sonnentag, S., Reinecke, L., Mata, J., & Vorderer, P. 2018. Feeling interruptedBeing
responsive: How online messages relate to affect at work. Journal of Organizational
Behavior, 39: 369-383.
Speier, C., Valacich, J. S., & Vessey, I. 1999. The influence of task interruption on individual
decision making: An information overload perspective. Decision Sciences, 30: 337-360.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 46
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Speier, C., Vessey, I., & Valacich, J. S. 2003. The effects of interruptions, task complexity, and
information presentation on computer‐supported decision‐making performance. Decision
Sciences, 34: 771-797.
Spira, J. B., & Feintuch, J. B. 2005. The cost of not paying attention: How interruptions impact
knowledge worker productivity. New York: Basex.
Sykes, E. R. 2011. Interruptions in the workplace: A case study to reduce their effects.
International Journal of Information Management, 31: 385-394.
Szumowska, E., & Kossowska, M. 2017. Motivational rigidity enhances multitasking performance:
The role of handling interruptions. Personality and Individual Differences, 106: 81-89.
Tams, S., Thatcher, J., Grover, V., & Pak, R. 2015. Selective attention as a protagonist in
contemporary workplace stress: Implications for the interruption age. Anxiety, Stress, &
Coping, 28: 663-686.
Tan, M. K. S., & Richardson, A. 2011. Please do not disturb: Managing interruptions and task
complexity. Proceedings of the Pacific Asia Conference on Information Systems: 1-15.
Trafton, J. G., Altmann, E. M., & Brock, D. P. 2005. Huh, what was I doing? How people use
environmental cues after an interruption. Proceedings of the Human Factors and
Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting: 468-472.
Trafton, J. G., Altmann, E. M., Brock, D. P., & Mintz, F. E. 2003. Preparing to resume an
interrupted task: Effects of prospective goal encoding and retrospective rehearsal.
International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 58: 583-603.
Trafton, J. G., & Monk, C. A. 2007. Task interruptions. In D. A. Boehm-Davis (Ed.), Reviews of
human factors and ergonomics (vol. 3): 111-126. Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and
Ergonomics Society.
Tremblay, S., Vachon, F., Lafond, D., & Kramer, C. 2012. Dealing with task interruptions in
complex dynamic environments: Are two heads better than one? Human Factors, 54: 70-83.
Van Den Berg, P. T., Roe, R. A., Zijlstra, F. R. H., & Krediet, I. 1996. Temperamental factors in
the execution of interrupted editing tasks. European Journal of Personality, 10: 233-248.
Wajcman, J., & Rose, E. 2011. Constant connectivity: Rethinking interruptions at work.
Organization Studies, 32: 941-961.
Weigl, M., Müller, A., Vincent, C., Angerer, P., & Sevdalis, N. 2012. The association of
workflow interruptions and hospital doctors' workload: A prospective observational
study. BMJ Quality and Safety, 21: 399-407.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 47
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Weigl, M., Müller, A., Zupanc, A., Glaser, J., & Angerer, P. 2011. Hospital doctors' workflow
interruptions and activities: An observation study. BMJ Quality & Safety, 20: 491-497.
Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. 1996. Affective events theory: A theoretical discussion of the
structure, causes and consequences of affective experiences at work. Research in
Organizational Behavior, 18: 1-74.
Werner, N. E., & Holden, R. J. 2015. Interruptions in the wild: Development of a sociotechnical
systems model of interruptions in the emergency department through a systematic review.
Applied Ergonomics, 51: 244-254.
Westbrook, J. I., Raban, M. Z., Walter, S. R., & Douglas, H. 2018. Task errors by emergency
physicians are associated with interruptions, multitasking, fatigue and working memory
capacity: a prospective, direct observation study. BMJ Quality and Safety, 27: 655-663.
Westbrook, J. I., Woods, A., Rob, M. I., Dunsmuir, W. T. M., & Day, R. O. 2010. Association of
interruptions with an increased risk and severity of medication administration errors.
Archives of Internal Medicine, 170: 683-690.
Wiberg, M., & Whittaker, S. 2005. Managing availability: Supporting lightweight negotiations to
handle interruptions. Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction (TOCHI), 12: 356-387.
Wilkes, S. M., Barber, L. K., & Rogers, A. P. 2018. Development and validation of the
Workplace Interruptions Measure. Stress and Health, 34: 102114.
Zellmer-Bruhn, M. E. 2003. Interruptive events and team knowledge acquisition. Management
Science, 49: 514-528.
Zide, J. S., Mills, M. J., Shahani-Denning, C., & Sweetapple, C. 2017. Work interruptions
resiliency: Toward an improved understanding of employee efficiency. Journal of
Organizational Effectiveness: People and Performance, 4: 39-58.
Zijlstra, F. R. H., Roe, R. A., Leonora, A. B., & Krediet, I. 1999. Temporal factors in mental
work: Effects of interrupted activities. Journal of Occupational and Organizational
Psychology, 72: 163-185.
Zohar, D., Tzischinski, O., & Epstein, R. 2003. Effects of energy availability on immediate and
delayed emotional reactions to work events. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88: 1082-1093.
Züger, M., Corley, C., Meyer, A. N., Li, B., Fritz, T., Shepherd, D., Augustine, V., Francis, P.,
Kraft, N., & Snipes, W. 2017. Reducing interruptions at work: A large-scale field study
of flowlight. Proceedings of the CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing
Systems: 61-72.
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 48
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Table 1
Review of Prior Definitions of Work Interruptions
Source
Definition/description of a work interruption
Attributes referred to as defining features of a work interruption
Suspension of an
ongoing work task’s
execution
Interrupting
task
Intention to
resume
interrupted
task
External/
internal
cause of
interruption
Behavioral
Attentional
Andreasson et
al. (2017: 87)
Any event that causes the current activity, i.e. the primary task, to stop temporarily and
requests or forces the person’s attention towards a new task, i.e. the interruption task.
Not
mentioned
Baethge et al.
(2015: 309)
An interruption is a temporary suspension of a person’s goal-directed action (Brixey et
al., 2007, p. E30). Being interrupted means that the person stops the execution of an
action before a chosen goal or sub-goal (task goals are typically broken down into sub-
goals that can be executed successively or at separate time intervals) is reached, with the
intention of resuming the action at a later point in time (cf. Altmann & Trafton, 2002).
Not
mentioned
Bellandi et al.
(2018: 41)
Any temporary diversion from the current task performed within in individual’s main
activity, due to an external intervention that moves the professional’s attention to a
secondary task until the main task is resumed or forsaken.
External
Biron et al.
(2009a: 331)
A break in task activity, evidenced by cessation of a task.
Not
mentioned
Boehm-Davis
& Remington
(2009: 1125)
Suspension of one stream of work prior to completion, with the intent of returning to
and completing the original stream of work.
Not
mentioned
Brixey et al.
(2007: E38)
A break in the performance of a human activity initiated by a source internal or external
to the recipient, with occurrence situated within the context of a setting or a location.
This break results in the suspension of the initial task by initiating the performance of an
unplanned task with the assumption that the initial task will be resumed.
Either
Chisholm et al.
(2000: 1240)
Any event that briefly required the attention of the subject but did not result in switching
to a new task.
Not
mentioned
Chong & Siino
(2006: 30)
Any instance in which workers turn attention away from their primary work task, either
on their own or in response to others’ actions.
Either
Couffe &
Michael
(2017: 165)
A primary task is suspended temporarily; there is the intention to return to and complete
it; the new task (i.e., interruption task) is introduced by an event, unanticipated or not;
and the event can be either external or internal to the person.
Either
Dabbish et al.
(2011: 3127)
Self-interruptions involve abandoning an ongoing task prior to completion, and
changing focus to a different task without prompting by an external event or entity.
Internal
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 49
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Source
Definition/description of a work interruption
Attributes referred to as defining features of work interruptions
Suspension of an
ongoing work task’s
execution
Interrupting
task
Intention to
resume
interrupted
task
External/
internal
cause of
interruption
Behavioral
Attentional
Fletcher et al.
(2018: 224)
Events or stimuli with either an internal or an external impetus that may result in
concurrent or sequential multitasking with a primary task.
Either
González &
Mark
(2004: 118)
An internal interruption refers to self-initiated switching among working spheres. An
external interruption is a condition in the environment that motivates switching.
Either
Healey et al.
(2007: 136)
We defined distraction as observed behaviour such as orienting away from a primary task
or verbally responding to a secondary task. We defined interruption as a distraction
resulting in a break in primary task activity.
Not
mentioned
Jett & George
(2003: 495)
Intrusion: An unexpected encounter initiated by another person that interrupts the flow
and continuity of an individual's work and brings that work to a temporary halt.
External
Jett & George
(2003: 497-498)
Breaks: Planned or spontaneous recesses from work on a task that interrupt the task’s
flow and continuity.
Either
Jett & George
(2003: 500)
Distractions: Psychological reactions triggered by external stimuli or secondary
activities that interrupt focused concentration on a primary task.
External
Jett & George
(2003: 502)
Discrepancies: Perceived inconsistencies between one’s knowledge and expectations
and one’s immediate observations that are perceived to be relevant to both the task at
hand and personal well-being.
Not
mentioned
Li et al.
(2012: 6)
A secondary activity that requires one’s attention and stops interaction with the primary
task.
Not
mentioned
Lin et al.
(2013: 78)
Temporary halts in task-related behavior due to the onset of a demand or secondary task
(Eyrolle & Cellier, 2000).
Not
mentioned
Mark et al.
(2005: 322)
External interruptions are those that stem from events in the environment… Internal
interruptions are those in which one stops a task of their own volition.
Either
McCurdie et al.
(2017: 104-105)
A diversion of attention from the current task due to an attention request from an
external source (person, phone, device), briefly or for an extended amount of time,
which may or may not result in switching to a new task.
External
McFarlane
(1997: 67)
The process of coordinating abrupt change in people’s activities.
Not
mentioned
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 50
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Source
Definition/description of a work interruption
Attributes referred to as defining features of work interruptions
Suspension of an
ongoing work task’s
execution
Interrupting
task
Intention to
resume
interrupted
task
External/
internal
cause of
interruption
Behavioral
Attentional
Miyata &
Norman
(1986: 268)
External interruptions result from events in the environment. Internal interruptions come
from our own thought processes-new ideas that draw attention from the current activity.
Interruptions introduce new tasks on top of the ongoing activity, often unexpectedly.
Either
Myers et al.
(2016: 883)
Anything that takes your attention away from a task or communication activity that you
were already engaged in as part of your job
Not
mentioned
O'Conaill &
Frohlich
(1995: 262)
A synchronous interaction which was not initiated by the subject, was unscheduled and
resulted in the recipient discontinuing their current activity.
External
Relihan et al.
(2010: 2)
Interruption: An external factor causing the cessation of productive activity before a
current task is complete. Distraction: A stimulus from an external source that results in
an observable response, but not the cessation of activity
External
Rivera
(2014: 748)
Unplanned break in workflow caused by an external source (i.e. the interrupter)
External
Sanderson &
Grundgeiger
(2015: 86)
An interruption occurs when an event leads a person to remove their attention fully but
temporarily from a primary, or current, task to another task, and then move their
attention back to the primary task
Not
mentioned
Sasangohar et
al. (2012: 826)
Externally or internally generated, usually unexpected events that may cause a break in
the primary task (if they don’t, they are merely distractions), diverting attention to a
related or unrelated secondary task, which can have both negative and positive effects on
the interrupter’s or the interrupted person’s main task.
Either
Speier et al.
(1999: 339)
"Externally generated, randomly occurring, discrete event that breaks continuity of
cognitive focus on a primary task" (Corragio, 1990, p. 19)
External
Van Den Berg
et al.
(1996: 236)
Externally generated, temporary cessation in the current flow of behavior, typically meant
for the subject to execute activities that belong to a secondary set of actions. The activities
of their primary task are resumed after a certain lapse of time.
External
Weigl et al.
(2011: 492)
An intrusion of an unplanned and unscheduled task, causing a discontinuation
of tasks, a noticeable break or task switch behaviour
Not
mentioned
Westbrook et
al. (2018: 656)
An observable external stimulus resulting in a change in a physician’s task
External
Zijlstra et al.
(1999: 169)
Events which result in the cessation and postponement of an ongoing activity. Typical for
interruptions is that the main activity is resumed after a certain lapse of time.
Not
mentioned
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 51
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Table 2
Work Interruptions: Critical Assessment and Future Research Recommendations
Focus area
Critical observations about prior research related to the focus area
Key questions/recommendations for future research on the focus area
Internal
sources of
interruption
Despite noting the presence of internal sources of interruptions,
theoretical and empirical research on the nature, experience, and
outcomes of internal sources of interruption remains sparse
Build theory around the nature and characteristics of ‘internal’ sources of interruptions
Explore similarities/differences between internally and externally triggered interruptions
Examine the possibility of ‘interruption cycles
Interrupting
task
Included as design elements in lab studies or as boundary conditions
The effect of interruption on interrupting tasks is mostly unexplored
How are interrupting tasks affected by interruptions? What factors influence this effect?
How do interrupted and interrupting tasks jointly affect employee outcomes?
Interrupter
Despite sporadic studies, systematic investigation of who
interrupts, and how they are affected by the interruption is lacking
When are interruptions harmful or beneficial for the interrupter? Why?
What influences the decision to interrupt others? Do interrupters realize they are
interrupting others? How does this affect their behavior and experience of interruption?
Bright side
of work
interruptions
While theory indicates positive interruption outcomes, most studies
focus only on negative outcomesleading to interruptions being
viewed as undesirable occurrences that need to be eliminated
When, why, and how do interruptions benefit the interrupted and interrupting tasks?
When, why, and how do interruptions benefit the interrupted and interrupting persons?
Do interruptions lead to positive non-task outcomes (e.g., cohesion)? When and why?
Social and
relational
dynamics
and
interpersonal
outcomes
Lack of focus on the social/relational dynamics and interpersonal
outcomes related to a work interruption
Social interaction/relationship between the interrupter and the
interrupted ignored in experimental and observational studies
Most field studies use frequency-based items that club interruptions
from different people and prevent the study of relational dynamics
(e.g., I am frequently interrupted by others:’ Lin et al., 2013)
How does an interruption’s quality of social interaction affect interruption outcomes?
How do relationships affect the experience of interrupting and being interrupted? How
do leaders shape employee attitudes toward interruptions?
Do work interruptions have interpersonal consequences? When and why do work
interruptions lead to positive versus negative interpersonal responses?
An episodic approach will help isolate and study the relational context of an interruption
Moving
beyond the
individual
level
Almost all prior interruption research resides at the individual level
Incomplete understanding of interruptions’ impact at the team and
organization level
How do interruptions influence intra and inter team communication patterns and affect
collaboration, team performance, team cohesion, team identification etc.?
How do shared team norms about interruptions develop? What impact does a fit/misfit
between individual interruption handling preference and team interruption norms have?
Complexity
of modern
workplaces
Lack of focus on the complex interplay of multiple, nested, and
simultaneous interruptions in today’s dynamic workplaces
Lack of focus on how interruption dispersion affects outcomes
Employees as passive recipients of interruptions who respond
after interruptions occur. Employee agency not examined.
Use methodologies that can capture the complexity/interplay of various interruptions (e.g.,
microworld simulations, Dual Perspectives Method, socio-technical system approach etc.)
How does the variance in work interruptions impact employees? How do employees
manage chronic versus less regular interruptions?
How do employees agentically shape the occurrence and experience of interruptions?
RUNNING HEAD: Work Interruptions Review 52
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Figure 1
Integrative Process-based Model of Sources, Mediators, Outcomes, and Moderators of Work Interruptions
Note: a. For the second and third stage moderators, the letter(s) in the parenthesis indicate the pathway that these variables moderate. C = Cognitive pathway, SR = Self-
regulatory pathway, and A = Affective pathway.
b. While work can also be interrupted by factors internal to the employees (e.g., mindwandering) (e.g., Fisher, 1998), due to the sparse research on internal sources,
the nature, experience, and outcomes of interruptions caused by internal sources are poorly understood. Hence, we discuss internal sources as a future research topic in
the text. Thus, the mediators, outcomes, and moderators shown above are related to external sources of interruptions, reflecting the current state of the literature.
... While prior research acknowledges that smartphone use affects the boundaries between work and nonwork domains (Derks et al., 2021;Morandin et al., 2018), creating either conflict or balance (Cousins and Robey, 2015;Yun et al., 2012), which in turn influences performance at work (Puranik et al., 2020) and at home, an integrative model that identifies the underlying psychological processes explaining these relationships is lacking. In response to calls for research to clarify the impact of constant connectivity on blurred work-nonwork boundaries (e.g., Chen and Karahanna, 2018) and firm performance (Hanelt et al., 2021), we aim to establish such an integrative model by adopting an interdisciplinary approach. ...
... For example, using smartphones for work at night can engender "morning depletion," reducing work engagement the next day (Lanaj et al., 2014). Constant connectivity through smartphones also might distract employees (Derks et al., 2021) even if they do not check them ("brain drain" hypothesis; Ward et al., 2017), impede goal progress and productivity through task interruptions (Chen and Karahanna, 2018), and diminish performance (Puranik et al., 2020). Such implications extend to adaptive performance, in that the ubiquity of mobile technologies and increased connectivity can undermine the continuity of ongoing task efforts and concentration (Orhan et al., 2021), causing diminished time management (Puranik et al., 2020) and flexibility (Wang et al., 2020). ...
... Constant connectivity through smartphones also might distract employees (Derks et al., 2021) even if they do not check them ("brain drain" hypothesis; Ward et al., 2017), impede goal progress and productivity through task interruptions (Chen and Karahanna, 2018), and diminish performance (Puranik et al., 2020). Such implications extend to adaptive performance, in that the ubiquity of mobile technologies and increased connectivity can undermine the continuity of ongoing task efforts and concentration (Orhan et al., 2021), causing diminished time management (Puranik et al., 2020) and flexibility (Wang et al., 2020). Smartphones tend to create expectations of constant connectivity; because they can always be available, employees might feel compelled to remain so to respond to work and personal requests, such that they become imprisoned in a digital cage (Wang et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Constant connectivity through smartphone use represents a major societal challenge, particularly in relation to work performance and work–nonwork boundaries. This research leverages an interdisciplinary theoretical framework, crisis settings, and a cross‐cultural approach (France and the United Kingdom) to specify the impacts of smartphone use on work performance and work–life interfaces, before and during the COVID‐19 crisis, among large samples of employees (NPilot = 229; N1 = 938; N2 = 1042). An empirical test of this integrative model shows that smartphone use influences work performance directly, work–life balance negatively mediates this relationship, and work–family conflict moderates it. It advances knowledge by considering both dimensions of work performance, generic task performance and adaptive performance, which is crucial for adapting to technologies and crises. This research also emphasizes the need to consider the context in which smartphone use affects work performance, notably in situations marked by high pressure, such as crises that become chronic. By introducing the marketing concept of perceived value as a mediator, this research shows that utilitarian value improves work performance while hedonic value decreases it, thus advancing the scholarly conversation and helping to manage constant connectivity.
... Indeed, completing work tasks without experiencing intrusions has become a luxury not afforded to most. Importantly, in addition to harming task progression, intrusion may also impair employee wellbeing (Baethge et al., 2015;Baethge & Rigotti, 2013;Lin et al., 2013;Puranik et al., 2020). Hence, research has started to devote attention to resources that may buffer the negative impact of various work interruptions, including supervisor appreciation (Stocker et al., 2019) and time-management skills (Ma et al., 2020). ...
... First, we contribute to research on intrusions by examining attention residue as a mechanism linking fluctuations in intrusion frequency and duration to exhaustion. While work interruptions are well-known to deplete resources and cause strain (Chen & Karahanna, 2018;Lin et al., 2013;Puranik et al., 2020Puranik et al., , 2021, to date, no research has explored how attention residue explains the connection between work intrusions and exhaustion. Prior studies on attention residue from unanticipated task switching have primarily focused on performance outcomes (Leroy, 2009;Leroy & Schmidt, 2016). ...
... Second, in today's dynamic and technology-imbued hybrid workplaces, work intrusions and interruptions are widespread and inevitable (Puranik et al., 2020). While some may argue that efforts should focus on eradicating intrusions to reduce their negative effects in the workplace, the complexity of modern work environments, characterized by high levels of technological connectivity and information flow, often makes this impractical. ...
Article
Full-text available
The proliferation of technology-mediated work practices and the growing emphasis on open and multilocational work have highlighted the importance of navigating and managing work interruptions. Work interruptions are omnipresent in contemporary work environments and have accumulated abundant attention from scholars and managers during the past decades. Despite this attention, research only scarcely explored strategies employees could utilize to manage the growing number of work interruptions. Building on self-regulation theory, we propose that intrusions relate to exhaustion through attention residue but that an employee-centered planning intervention (i.e., writing ready-to-resume plans) mitigates this process. We tested our predictions in an experimental field study utilizing a daily diary methodology (N = 50 × 4 = 200 occasions). The findings demonstrate that employees experience greater attention residue and higher levels of exhaustion on days when intrusions are more prevalent. We argue that the findings imply that the intervention facilitates self-regulation processes. The intervention reduces attention residue and attenuates the daily association between attention residue and exhaustion. The findings advance the literature on work interruptions and attention residue by demonstrating that writing about task progress reduces attention residue and limits the depleting effects of persisting thoughts regarding the interrupted task.
... Our study makes several contributions. First, we shift the primary focus of TMI literature from the impact on those being interrupted to the impact on those who interrupt, particularly the supervisors themselves, responding to the call for research from the interrupters' perspective (Puranik et al., 2020;Toebben et al., 2025). Specifically, based on the COR theory, we challenge the view that interrupters benefit from interruption behavior. ...
... First, this study contributes to the after-hours TMI literature by shifting the focus from the interruptees to the interrupters, addressing the call for research from the interrupters' perspective (Puranik et al., 2020;Toebben et al., 2025). While existing studies have consistently shown that afterhours TMI can cause the interrupted party to be fatigued and negatively impact their work performance, there remains a significant knowledge gap regarding the other party-the interrupter. ...
Article
Full-text available
Existing research on after-hours technology-mediated interruptions (TMI) has primarily focused on the interruptees. This study shifts the focus from the interruptees to the interrupters (i.e., supervisors) and explores how supervisors’ after-hours TMI affects their psychological and behavioral outcomes. Integrating the conservation of resources (COR) theory and work-life interface literature, the study tests the mediating role of information overload and the moderating effect of IT experience. Analyzing experience sampling data from 113 participants over 10 working days, the results reveal that supervisors’ after-hours TMI is positively associated with information overload, which in turn increases both work and nonwork exhaustion and reduces performance in both areas. Moreover, the relationship between after-hours TMI and information overload is moderated by IT experience, i.e., when supervisor have higher levels of IT experience, the relationship is lower (vs. higher). In conclusion, this study reveals how and when supervisors’ after-hours TMI negatively impacts their own psychological and behavioral outcomes.
... Some experiments show that unsatisfied expectations at work cause negative emotions in individuals, such as depression or anxiety (Puranik et al. 2019). In the medical field, as the use of digital technology in the clinical environment becomes more intelligent, synchronous, universal and complex, new problems and uncertainties brought by this digital technology also arise. ...
Article
Full-text available
Aims This study investigates how unmet expectations in the digital clinical practice environment for elderly care impact the negative affect and turnover intentions of students majoring in elderly care and examines the moderating role of proactive personality traits. Methods A cross‐sectional study was conducted between December 2022 and March 2023. Participants included 609 nursing students from 16 schools in Anhui Province, China, who met inclusion criteria and consented to participate. They completed the Digital Technology Unmet Expectations Scale, Negative Affect Scale, Proactive Personality Scale and Turnover Intention Scale. Data analysis was performed using SPSS and SmartPLS. Results Unmet expectations regarding digital technology were identified as significant barriers to the successful transition of elderly care students into the workforce. However, no moderating effects of proactive personality were found on the relationships between unmet expectations concerning system quality and negative affect, unmet expectations regarding information quality and negative affect or unmet expectations regarding system quality and turnover intention. Conclusion Findings suggest that unmet expectations related to digital technology in the clinical training environment hinder the transition of elderly care students to the workforce. It is essential to implement measures to facilitate a smoother transition for students in this field. Summary What Is Already Known About This Topic? The high rate of nursing student turnover is one of the major reasons for the shortage of elderly care nurses. The turnover intention of nursing students is influenced by many factors. Few studies have examined the mechanism of unmet expectations in the digital clinical practice environment of elderly care on the turnover intentions of students. What This Paper Adds? Extends the application of the Stimulus‐Organism‐Response (S‐O‐R) model to explore the relationship between digital technology use and turnover intentions in the nursing field. This study shows that unmet expectations regarding digital technology in the clinical practice environment affect the successful transition of students to the workforce. Expands understanding of the application of proactive personality traits in nursing, exploring their moderating role in digital technology and turnover intentions. The Implications of This Paper: This study highlighted the importance of the digital clinical practice environment for nursing students in elderly care.
... The use and role of new digital technologies and rapid pace of technological change, however, brings increasing complexity and makes academic research necessary in many ways to ensure beneficial interaction between humans and technology. Constant interruptions during task performance (Chen and Karahanna 2018;Puranik et al. 2020;Stangl andRiedl 2023b, 2023c), continuous electronic performance monitoring (Smith et al. 1992;Aiello and Kolb 1995;Riedl 2020, 2021), unstable systems and long response times (Emurian 1991(Emurian , 1993Riedl et al. 2012Anderson et al. 2016;Jenkins et al. 2016;Riedl and Fischer 2018;) are just some of the triggers for worry, anxiety, and stress that individuals face in an increasingly digital world, which may referred to as the "dark side" of digital technologies (Tarafdar et al. 2013). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
The development and increasing use of technology worldwide can lead to potential negative consequences for individuals’ well-being and productivity. To counteract negative consequences, both scientific research and practice have shown increasing interest in digital detox research, a rising phenomenon of abstinence and temporary or complete disengagement from digital technologies. To lay a foundation for future research, we conducted a systemic literature review with a focus on the methodological aspects of the existing empirical digital detox studies. Our literature search process revealed a total of 65 studies. Our analyses of this literature basis revealed five different research fields (communication, education, tourism, well-being and health, work environment), and we analyzed the empirical studies in these fields regarding applied research approach, research method, and sample size. This review provides methodological insights to advance the scientific inquiry on digital detox research, a relatively nascent, yet increasingly relevant research topic.
... While such interactions may strengthen social connections, they can also negatively affect workflow and hinder task progress [77]. Over time, repeated disruptions impose increasing time pressures on individuals who are frequently approached for advice, leading to frustration, burnout, and a decline in organizational efficiency [78][79][80]. Notably, individuals with high Machiavellian tendencies are more likely to leverage organizational networks to acquire information, using it as a tool to enhance their personal power and influence. While this strategy may be advantageous in certain contexts, excessively self-centered approaches can undermine collaboration within the organization. ...
Article
Full-text available
Modern organizations operate not only through formal structures but also through informal networks, which play a critical role in fostering a resilient organization. This study focused on informal advice networks within organizations as a key mechanism for strengthening contextual resilience, one of the core components of organizational resilience. By analyzing the activation of informal advice networks, this study conceptualized advice-seeking networks as a critical informal system that enhances contextual resilience and examined the individual, structural, and attitudinal factors influencing their formation. Specifically, we hypothesized that employees with higher levels of Machiavellianism are more likely to engage in advice-seeking behaviors, whereas the relationship between Machiavellianism and advice-seeking behaviors is moderated by betweenness centrality and organizational commitment, such that the positive effect of Machiavellianism on advice-seeking is weaker when betweenness centrality or organizational commitment is high. To empirically test these hypotheses, we conducted a network survey of employees at the headquarters of a life insurance company in Seoul, South Korea, and analyzed the data using an Exponential Random Graph Model (ERGM). The findings provide empirical support for all hypotheses. Based on these results, we discussed the theoretical contributions and practical implications of the study, along with its limitations.
Article
Drawing on boundary theory, this study examined how boundary control influences satisfaction with work-life balance through interruptions among involuntary remote workers during the COVID-19 pandemic. Specifically, we investigated whether family intrusions and nonwork task distractions mediate the boundary control–work-life balance relationship and whether having a home office moderates these effects. In Study 1 (N = 130), we tested the mediating role of nonwork task distractions among behavior analysts across two time points. Study 2 ( N = 278), using a Mechanical Turk sample across three time points, extended these findings by examining both family intrusions and the moderating role of home office access. Results from both studies showed that nonwork task distractions mediated the relationship between boundary control and satisfaction with work-life balance. Study 2 further revealed that family intrusions mediated this relationship and that access to a home office conditionally moderated the indirect effects of boundary control via both mediators. These findings highlight the complex role of interruptions in remote work settings and suggest that access to a dedicated office space can mitigate interruptions' negative impacts. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed to inform decisions around the adoption or discontinuation of remote work practices.
Article
Full-text available
We develop a laboratory paradigm for studying prospective memory and examine whether or not this type of memory is especially difficult for the elderly. In two experiments, young and old subjects were given a prospective memory test (they were asked to perform an action when a target event occurred) and three tests of retrospective memory (short-term memory, free recall, and recognition). From the perspective that aging disrupts mainly self-initiated retrieval processes, large age-related decrements in prospective memory were anticipated. However, despite showing reliable age differences on retrospective memory tests, both experiments showed no age deficits in prospective memory. Moreover, regression analyses produced no reliable relation between the prospective and retrospective memory tasks. Also, the experiments showed that external aids and unfamiliar target events benefit prospective memory performance. These results suggest some basic differences between prospective and retrospective memory.
Article
Full-text available
Over the past 30 years, the nature of communication at work has changed. Leaders in particular rely increasingly on email to communicate with their superiors and subordinates. However, researchers and practitioners alike suggest that people frequently report feeling overloaded by the email demands they experience at work. In the current study, we develop a self-regulatory framework that articulates how leaders’ day-to-day email demands relate to a perceived lack of goal progress, which has a negative impact on their subsequent enactment of routine (i.e., initiating structure) and exemplary (i.e., transformational) leadership behaviors. We further theorize how two cross-level moderators — centrality of email to one’s job and trait self-control — impact these relations. In an experience sampling study of 48 managers across 10 consecutive workdays, our results illustrate that email demands are associated with a lack of perceived goal progress, to which leaders respond by reducing their initiating structure and transformational behaviors. The relation of email demands with leader goal progress was strongest when email was perceived as less central to performing one’s job, and the relations of low goal progress with leadership behaviors were strongest for leaders low in trait self-control.
Article
Full-text available
This study explores the dimensionality of organizational justice and provides evidence of construct validityfor a new justice measure. Items for this measure were generated by strictly following the seminal works in the justice literature. The measure was then validated in 2 separate studies. Study 1 occurred in a university setting, and Study 2 occurred in a field setting using employees in an automobile parts manufacturing company. Confirmatory factor analyses supported a 4-factor structure to the measure, with distributive, procedural, interpersonal, and informational justice as distinct dimensions. This solution fit the data significantly better than a 2- or 3-factor solution using larger interactional or procedural dimensions. Structural equation modeling also demonstrated predictive validity for the justice dimensions on important outcomes, including leader evaluation, rule compliance, commitment, and helping behavior.
Article
Full-text available
Despite the growing research on work recovery and its well-being outcomes, surprisingly little attention has been paid to at-work recovery and its job performance outcomes. The current study extends the work recovery literature by examining day-level relationships between prototypical microbreaks and job performance as mediated by state positive affect. Furthermore, general work engagement is tested as a cross-level moderator weakening the indirect effects of microbreaks on job performance via positive affect. Using multisource experience sampling method, the authors collected two daily surveys from 71 call center employees and obtained objective records of daily sales performance for two consecutive weeks (n = 632). Multilevel path analysis results showed that relaxation, socialization, and cognitive microbreaks were related to increased positive affect at work which, in turn, predicted greater sales performance. However, breaks for nutrition-intake (having snacks and drinks) did not show significant effects. Importantly, microbreaks had significant indirect effects on job performance via positive affect only for workers who had lower general work engagement, whereas the indirect effects did not exist for workers who had higher general work engagement. Furthermore, Bayesian multilevel analyses confirmed the results. Theoretical and practical implications, limitations, and future research directions are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Background Interruptions and multitasking have been demonstrated in experimental studies to reduce individuals’ task performance. These behaviours are frequently used by clinicians in high-workload, dynamic clinical environments, yet their effects have rarely been studied. Objective To assess the relative contributions of interruptions and multitasking by emergency physicians to prescribing errors. Methods 36 emergency physicians were shadowed over 120 hours. All tasks, interruptions and instances of multitasking were recorded. Physicians’ working memory capacity (WMC) and preference for multitasking were assessed using the Operation Span Tas (OSPAN) and Inventory of Polychronic Values. Following observation, physicians were asked about their sleep in the previous 24 hours. Prescribing errors were used as a measure of task performance. We performed multivariate analysis of prescribing error rates to determine associations with interruptions and multitasking, also considering physician seniority, age, psychometric measures, workload and sleep. Results Physicians experienced 7.9 interruptions/hour. 28 clinicians were observed prescribing 239 medication orders which contained 208 prescribing errors. While prescribing, clinicians were interrupted 9.4/hour. Error rates increased significantly if physicians were interrupted (rate ratio (RR) 2.82; 95% CI 1.23 to 6.49) or multitasked (RR 1.86; 95% CI 1.35 to 2.56) while prescribing. Having below-average sleep showed a >15-fold increase in clinical error rate (RR 16.44; 95% CI 4.84 to 55.81). WMC was protective against errors; for every 10-point increase on the 75-point OSPAN, a 19% decrease in prescribing errors was observed. There was no effect of polychronicity, workload, physician gender or above-average sleep on error rates. Conclusion Interruptions, multitasking and poor sleep were associated with significantly increased rates of prescribing errors among emergency physicians. WMC mitigated the negative influence of these factors to an extent. These results confirm experimental findings in other fields and raise questions about the acceptability of the high rates of multitasking and interruption in clinical environments.
Article
Interruption of work by e-mail and other communication technologies has become widespread and ubiquitous. However, our understanding of how such interruptions influence individual performance is limited. This paper distinguishes between two types of e-mail interruptions (incongruent and congruent) and draws upon action regulation theory and the computer-mediated communication literature to examine their direct and indirect effects on individual performance. Two empirical studies of sales professionals were conducted spanning different time frames: a survey study with 365 respondents and a diary study with 212 respondents. The results were consistent across the two studies, showing a negative indirect effect of exposure to incongruent interruptions (interruptions containing information that is not relevant to primary activities) through subjective workload, and a positive indirect effect of exposure to congruent interruptions (interruptions containing information that is relevant to primary activities) through mindfulness. The results differed across the two studies in terms of whether the effects were fully or partially mediated, and we discuss these differences using meta-inferences. Technology capabilities used during interruption episodes also had significant effects: rehearsing (fine-tuning responses to incoming messages) and reprocessing (reexamining received messages) were positively related to mindfulness, parallel communication (engaging in multiple e-mail conversations simultaneously) and leaving messages in the inbox were positively related to subjective workload, and deleting messages was negatively related to subjective workload. This study contributes to research by providing insights on the different paths that link e-mail interruptions to individual performance and by examining the effects of using capabilities of the interrupting technology (IT artifact) during interruption episodes. It also complements the experimental tradition that focuses on isolated interruptions. By shifting the level of analysis from specific interruption events to overall exposure to interruptions over time and from the laboratory to the workplace, our study provides realism and ecological validity.
Article
This paper explores the attention regulation challenges brought by interruptions. In contrast to much of the research on interruptions that looks at the effects on the interrupted task, this paper examines the difficulty of focusing attention and performing well on interrupting tasks. Integrating research on attention residue, time pressure, and implementation intention, we predict that when people anticipate resuming their interrupted work under time pressure, they will find it difficult to switch their attention to the interrupting task, leading to attention residue and low performance. A ready-to-resume intervention, in which one briefly reflects on and plans one’s return to the interrupted task, mitigates this effect such that attention residue is reduced and performance on the interrupting task does not suffer. Data collected across four studies support these hypotheses. The e-companion is available at https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.2017.1184 .
Conference Paper
Information workers are experiencing ever-increasing online distractions in the workplace, and software to block distractions is becoming more popular. We conducted an exploratory field study with 32 information workers in their workplace using software to block online distractions for one week. We discovered that with online distractions blocked, participants assessed their focus and productivity to be significantly higher. Those who benefited most were those who reported being less in control of their work, associated with personality traits of lower Conscientiousness and Lack of Perseverence. Unexpectedly, those reporting higher control of work experienced a cost of higher workload with online distractions blocked. Those who reported the greatest increase in focus with distractions blocked were those who were more susceptible to social media distractions. Without distractions, people with higher control of work worked longer stretches without physical breaks, with consequently higher stress. We present design recommendations to promote focus for our observed coping behaviors.
Article
Examinations of interruptions in healthcare often focus on a single clinical discipline, and solutions are targeted accordingly. This approach does not take into account the inter-disciplinary dependencies and other sociotechnical aspects that make up the healthcare work system, and suggested solutions may not meet the needs of all stakeholders. In this article a sociotechnical systems perspective is used to uncover the interdependencies between 16 unique work roles that result in interruptions in an intensive care unit (ICU). By applying social network analysis techniques to data collected using the Dual Perspectives Method, we identified targeted systems-based interventions that may reduce unnecessary interruptions while avoiding unintended consequences that impose additional burden on ICU staff. The rich insights gained into the interruptive communication patterns in the ICU work system stand in contrast to findings that would have otherwise been obtained by focusing only on a single clinical discipline or a single perspective.