ChapterPDF Available

Personality and Friendships

Authors:

Abstract

Synonyms: Personality and interpersonal closeness; Personality traits and interpersonal attraction; Personality traits and relationships with friends. Definition: Described as one of basic human needs, friendship is one of the most important relations in people’s lives and a common personal experience (Baumeister and Leary 1995). Personality plays a significant role in initiation, formation, quality, and maintenance, as well as dissolution of relationships between friends. It can also be deeply affected and possibly transformed by these relationships. In this entry, we will present the most fundamental findings about the links between personality and friendship, focusing on the impact exerted by personality (Big Five and Dark Triad traits models) on relationships with friends.
P
Personality and Friendships
Marta Doroszuk
1
, Marta Kupis
2
and
Anna Z. Czarna
3
1
Faculty of Philosophy, Institute of Psychology,
Jagiellonian University, Krakow, Poland
2
Faculty of International and Political Studies,
Institute of Intercultural Studies, Jagiellonian
University, Krakow, Poland
3
Faculty of Management and Social
Communication, Institute of Applied Psychology,
Jagiellonian University, Krakow, Poland
Synonyms
Personality and interpersonal closeness;Personal-
ity traits and interpersonal attraction;Personality
traits and relationships with friends
Definition
Described as one of basic human needs, friend-
ship is one of the most important relations in
peoples lives and a common personal experience
(Baumeister and Leary 1995). Personality plays a
signicant role in initiation, formation, quality,
and maintenance, as well as dissolution of rela-
tionships between friends. It can also be deeply
affected and possibly transformed by these rela-
tionships. In this entry, we will present the most
fundamental ndings about the links between
personality and friendship, focusing on the impact
exerted by personality (Big Five and Dark Triad
traits models) on relationships with friends.
Introduction
At every stage of life, people try to build relation-
ships with others. Social connections are related to
support that people receive and give to each other.
They have positive inuence on ones health,
mood, and sense of belonging. Provided and
received support makes relationships stronger
and last longer. Denitions of friendship distin-
guish it from other relations, as it is voluntary,
informal, pleasant, and based on reciprocity. As
such, this kind of relationship is quite different
from ofcial contacts at work or bonds with fam-
ily members, where there is no freedom of choice.
Friendship can be described from various per-
spectives, one of which emphasizes the role of
positive affect in a state of being friends.
A friend is someone a person chooses to play a
signicant role in her or his life, to share time
with, and to engage in various activities together.
How do people select their friends and which
aspects play a signicant role in the feeling of
comfort and pleasure experienced in another per-
sons company? One of the most important factors
is personalities of the people searching for this
type of connection (Harris and Vazire 2016). In
this entry we present several traits of personality
which play a vital role in choosing friends and
© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019
V. Zeigler-Hill, T. K. Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_712-1
being chosen to be ones by others. We briey
report their role in initiation, maintenance, and
dissolution of friendships, where evidence is
available. We do not explicitly differentiate
between actor, partner, and dyadic effects (for a
systematic overview of those differences, see Har-
ris and Vazire 2016). While many personality
conceptualizations could be considered, we
focus on the Big Five (Costa and McCrae 1988)
and Dark Triad traits (Paulhus and Williams
2002). We chose the former as the one stemming
from the most basic and established model of
personality and the latter due to their pronounced
negative impact on relationships.
Sufciently stable and long-term relationships
can also exert inuences on personality traits
(Asendorpf and Wilpers 1998; Mund et al.
2018). Emotional closeness and frequency of con-
icts impact several personality traits, such as
neuroticism, extraversion, agreeableness, and
conscientiousness, making the relationship between
personality and friendship reciprocal, dynamic, and
complex. Close friendships provide distinctive
opportunities for reinforcing dispositional tenden-
cies and for fostering personality (or at least
behavioral) accommodation or change. Prelimi-
nary evidence suggests that matched friends
(e.g., two high extraverts) mutually reinforce
each others similar dispositional tendencies,
while friends with contrasting personalities (e.g.,
an introvert and an extravert) show patterns of
personality accommodation (e.g., extraverts meet-
ing introverts close to home in line with intro-
vertspreferences) as well as complementary
reinforcement (e.g., extraverts talking more and
introverts listening more when together; Nelson
et al. 2011). These complex processes warrant
further investigation. In this entry, however, we
will focus on the inuence that personalities have
on friendships rather than on the one that friend-
ships have on personalities.
The Big Five Personality Traits
Extraversion
Extraversion is related to having a high number of
friends (Harris and Vazire 2016). This might stem
from the most prominent difference between
extraverts and introverts, namely, extraverts
stronger afliative tendencies expressed in their
desire for company in pleasant and enjoyable
situations and in the overall desire to associate
with strangers. Extraverted individuals make
new friends faster in novel settings, often in
untypical ways (Anderson et al. 2001). They
attract other people with their style, condent
behaviors, and friendly expressions, such as
smile signifying acceptance. They initiate interac-
tions easily, look at their interaction partners
more, are less self-conscious, and make impres-
sions of more talkative and relaxed individuals
(Back et al. 2011; Cuperman and Ickes 2009).
These behavioral manifestations of positive atti-
tudes lead to more positive perceptions of them at
early stages of acquaintance and result in a higher
number of people who would like to spend time
with them (Back et al. 2011; Cemalcilar et al.
2018), as opposed to introverts with a pessimistic
approach.
However, highly extraverted people do not
select their friends haphazardly: there is no asso-
ciation between extraversion and the number of
friends selected by individuals (Back et al. 2011).
Similarity in levels of extraversion facilitates sat-
isfaction from interactions, even in cases of two
introverts (Cuperman and Ickes 2009). High
extraversion is connected with reciprocal support
(Asendorpf and Wilpers 1998). As the only one of
the Big Five traits, it is associated with a higher
frequency of meetings with friends and spending
more time interacting with them (Wilson et al.
2015). Thus, friendships of extraverted people
seem to be of high quality, and they involve
less insecurity, more emotional closeness, more
self-disclosures, deeper conversations (Wilson
et al. 2015), and better conict management.
More nonkin relations are found among highly
extraverted people (Wagner et al. 2014), as gen-
erally their contacts with others are more positive
and their friend networks are wider.
Better interpersonal skills of extraverts might
help them create more enjoyable experiences and
relationships that are more satisfying to themselves
(Wilson et al. 2015). Interestingly, the higher qual-
ity of their friendships is only self-reported and
2 Personality and Friendships
not conrmed by their friends, which suggests
that some other, less adaptive conduct character-
istic of extraverts might cancel out the benets of
their positive behaviors towards friends (Harris
and Vazire 2016). In particular, the less
communal that is, dominant and assertive
actions might be the ones driving their friends
away (Harris and Vazire 2016; Wortman and
Wood 2011).
Agreeableness
Agreeableness has been associated with an over-
whelmingly positive impact on interpersonal rela-
tionships via friendliness, warmth, and sociability
it entails (Harris and Vazire 2016; Wortman and
Wood 2011). Agreeable people are focused on
others and liked more (Selfhout et al. 2010),
though not all studies conrm this association
(e.g., Back et al. 2011). Though agreeableness
does not predict initiating more friendships
(Harris and Vazire 2016), prosocial and altruistic
behaviors, high empathy, and focus on coopera-
tion, observed among agreeable people, exert pos-
itive inuence on selecting them as friends
(Selfhout et al. 2010). However, where the
encounters are too brief (such as at zero acquain-
tance), supercial or indirect for the advantages of
agreeableness to emerge, the association is not
detectable. Agreeableness turns out to be a boon
for being liked in face-to-face interactions but not
necessarily in social media contexts (Cemalcilar
et al. 2018). Where direct interactions are not
required, agreeableness appears less conducive
to interpersonal attraction: online proles of
agreeable people may not provide sufcient enter-
tainment for passive viewers. The real value of
agreeableness emerges in direct contact, where
some interpersonal accommodation is necessary.
Agreeable people look at their interaction part-
ners, smile, laugh, and nod more during their
interactions, providing more active acknowledg-
ment to them (Cuperman and Ickes 2009). They
are more communal: kind, polite, humble, and
grateful, as well as less irritable, short-tempered,
offensive, devious, suspicious, manipulative, and
conceited and thus more likeable (Wortman and
Wood 2011). Similarity in high levels of agree-
ableness in both partners may facilitate
commencement of a friendship (Selfhout et al.
2010). However, two highly disagreeable persons
are particularly unlikely to form a friendly bond.
In this case similarity is a disadvantage, perhaps
due to the fact that neither partner would or could
compensate for the other ones disagreeableness
(Cuperman and Ickes 2009). Friendships with
highly agreeable people are of high quality, with
deeper conversations and more disclosures, as
well as more satisfying (Wilson et al. 2015).
Since agreeable people are eager to forgive and
use effective problem-solving strategies, conicts
do not endanger their friendshipscontinuation.
Altogether, agreeableness is related to better rela-
tionship maintenance behaviors and higher relation-
ship satisfaction and stability. Low agreeableness is
probably an important factor in friendship ending
(Harris and Vazire 2016).
Asendorpf and Wilpers (1998) described kin
attractionamong people with high agreeable-
ness, as they tend to stay in constant contact with
their relatives. Friendships formed by them are
more traditional, with high levels of stability and
geographical proximity between the partners.
Openness to Experience
In spite of the fact that openness has not been
identied as a substantial predictor of relationship
initiation in existing literature, there are some
contexts that appear to facilitate the emergence
of such an association. In particular, higher open-
ness is associated with higher interpersonal attrac-
tion at zero acquaintance in new environments
(such as among incoming freshmen in the rst
semester at the university) or in social media
(Cemalcilar et al. 2018). In the latter context
cues related to openness may be more discernable
than in face-to-face interactions, as potential
friends can have access to more self-expressive
statements in zero-acquaintance situations (Back
et al. 2011; Cemalcilar et al. 2018).
Constant contact is not necessary to maintain
friendships of individuals open to experience, and
even short periods of time are sufcient to build a
relationship. This is why their friendships are
described as less typical and more varying than
usual, as they are eager to become friends with
people who live far away or are known to them
Personality and Friendships 3
only via Internet. This is likely due to frequent
migrations of highly open individuals. Openness
may be associated with more liberal attitudes and
lead to higher diversity among people chosen to
be ones friends (e.g., from different ethnicities).
Homophily in terms of gender or cultural back-
ground is not vital in their relationships, only
similarity of age may play a role here (Laakasuo
et al. 2017). Openness is associated with having a
larger network of friends, but not with ones level
of closeness with network members. Also, high
openness to experience is unrelated to friendship
satisfaction (Wilson et al. 2015) and may lead to
low stability of relationships (Selfhout et al. 2010).
Conscientiousness
Conscientiousness is one of the traits exerting
small but positive impact on friendship formation
and satisfaction (e.g., Selfhout et al. 2010). It
might be more important for romantic relationship
satisfaction than for friendships, as the processes
(cognitive, affective, and behavioral) that lead to
satisfying friendships are likely overlapping but
distinct from the processes that result in romantic
relationship satisfaction (Wilson et al. 2015).
Conscientiousness can, however, inuence main-
tenance of a relationship (Selfhout et al. 2010), as
the emotional support and productive conict
management techniques, characteristic of highly
conscientious individuals, have a positive impact
on continuation of a relation. Their high self-
control results in appropriate social behaviors,
which lead to better relationships. Similarly, hon-
esty, social responsibility, and rule orientation,
associated with high conscientiousness, may pos-
itively inuence the number of a persons friends,
as well as quality and stability of their relation-
ships (Jensen-Campbell and Malcolm 2007).
High conscientiousness is related to less insecu-
rity, higher social competence, and more positive
social behaviors (Jensen-Campbell et al. 2007).
Perceived similarity at higher (but not lower)
levels of conscientiousness is conducive to inter-
personal attraction (Cemalcilar et al. 2018). More-
over, high conscientiousness is associated with
willingness to spend time with relatives, who
may be chosen for close friends (Asendorpf and
Wilpers 1998).
Neuroticism
Neurotic people do not initiate a lot of interac-
tions, which might be due to their belief that they
are not liked by others (Back et al. 2011). They
struggle both with starting and maintaining rela-
tionships, because of their low self-esteem and
negative affect. Possibly due to their high self-
absorption and apparently low communion, neu-
rotic people are perceived as less likeable
(Wortman and Wood 2011). Even in the context
of social media, higher neuroticism translates into
lower interpersonal attraction among prole
viewers (Cemalcilar et al. 2018). Neurotic indi-
viduals are also likely to engage in unstable and
unsatisfying relationships. This effect might be
exacerbated by the fact that in face-to-face inter-
actions, highly neurotic individuals are attracted
to each other (Cemalcilar et al. 2018). Highly
neurotic persons experience less emotional close-
ness and higher insecurity, as well as excessive
reassurance-seeking than emotionally stable peo-
ple, which may lead to lower relationship quality
and friendship satisfaction, as well as a higher
number of conicts (e.g., Wagner et al. 2014;
Wilson et al. 2015). Neurotic people are impul-
sive, get upset easily, have poor conict manage-
ment skills, hold grudges, and often fail to provide
their friends with sufcient emotional support
(Mund et al. 2018). These features may easily
lead to friendship dissolution, as low investment
in a relationship is connected with its low quality.
As a result, high neuroticism is associated with
having a smaller friendship network (Harris and
Vazire 2016).
Dark Triad
The Dark Triad,i.e., Machiavellianism, narcis-
sism, and psychopathy, also shows links with
friendship formation and maintenance (e.g.,
Jonason and Schmitt 2012). Relatively little
research has been done in this area, and the liter-
ature on this topic is far from exhausting.
Machiavellianism
Those with high levels of Machiavellianism
attach little value to establishing close, intimate
4 Personality and Friendships
friendships (Lyons and Aitken 2010), and their
supercial friend selection criteria reect this
low importance: Machiavellians (especially
Machiavellian women) seek physically attractive
friends (Jonason and Schmitt 2012). Such friends
might be used instrumentally to increase their
chances of meeting attractive men through
advertisementafforded by their physical attrac-
tiveness. Machiavellians also seek out friends
among kind persons, likely because they are
easy to manipulate and exploit. Machiavellianism
has high negative impact on relationships, as it
involves marked tendencies to distrust and manip-
ulate others, and may lead to exploitation of
another person for ones selsh goals. It is related
to low empathy, cynicism, and self-serving social
motivations (e.g., nancial success) which
increase competitiveness, as well as decrease
cooperativeness and reciprocity, tendencies that
are undesirable among friends (Abell et al.
2016). The actions of Machiavellians are rarely
connected to sincere interest in the other person,
their self-disclosures are less honest and accurate,
and, thus, their friendships suffer from low affec-
tive quality (Brewer et al. 2014). Machiavellian
women rarely show emotional support to their
friends; frequently report their friendships to be
lower in companionship, help, intimacy, and
emotional security; and often employ subtle
manipulative tactics, such as making them feel
shame or guilt. However, since Machiavellianism
is associated with hostile views, but not hostile
actions, they try to conform to friendship norms
and avoid detection of distrust and manipulations
from same-sex friends, so as to ensure their sup-
port. They may, through strategically seeking
closeness in others coupled with a lack of overt
hostile behavior, nd acceptance among their
peers. Ironically, they believe that it is actually
their friends who are manipulating them, which
further lowers trust and intimacy in their relation-
ships. Machiavellians are unwilling to commit
emotionally to other people, which prevents for-
mation of strong bonds with them.
Narcissism
Highly narcissistic individuals make good rst
impressions (Back et al. 2011). Those people are
charming, have fashionable appearances, display
condent behaviors, and have many stories to tell.
Their dominant-expressive behaviors, social
boldness, and being perceived as assertive help
them make friends easily (Leckelt et al. 2015);
however over time narcissists become less popu-
lar or are even actively disliked in their social
circles (Czarna et al. 2014; Czarna et al. 2016).
Even though they might use strategies to keep a
friendship going, provide some support to their
friends, spend time with them in exciting ways,
and even self-disclose (to self-promote), their
antagonistic behaviors are likely to lead the
friendship to end. Bragging, arrogance and
aggression, lack of empathy, hostility, blaming
others for ones own failures, and perceived
untrustworthiness are difcult to accept by friends
(possibly unless one is narcissistic themselves,
Hart and Adams 2014; Maaß et al. 2016). These
characteristics have an increasingly negative
effect on narcissistspopularity over time, leading
to friendsdisappointment and relationship disso-
lution (Czarna et al. 2016; Leckelt et al. 2015).
Nonetheless, narcissists are equally likely to leave
their friends as they often report experiencing
negative interactions, perceive more transgres-
sions, and tend to distance themselves from friends
who outperform them (Nicholls and Stukas 2011).
They are likely to quickly enter new short-lasting
friendships. Thus, narcissism is linked to short-
term relations and difculties in prolonging
friendships.
Psychopathy
Little research has been devoted to friendships of
psychopathic individuals. Common sense would
suggest that individuals who are manipulative,
dishonest, and callously insensitive to others
would not be perceived as high-quality friends.
Highly psychopathic persons tend to see them-
selves as faultless. They perceive any problems
in their relationship as the responsibility of their
partner, which may lead to the perception of poor
relationship quality. Indeed, high impulsivity, lack
of remorse for their misdeeds, and low empathy
towards other people produce complications in
relationships they form. Adult psychopaths view
relationships as unimportant and have mostly
Personality and Friendships 5
short-lived relationships in which they essentially
use people for their own purposes. There is some
evidence supporting homophily in friendships
among psychopathic individuals: they tend to
choose volatile others,that is, people who are
similarly unkind or untrustworthy and those with
similar values, presumably so that they can serve
as wingmen,making the negative consequences
of their fast life strategies less problematic
(Jonason and Schmitt 2012; Muñoz et al. 2008).
The priority in commencing such friendships
appears to be facilitation of their lifestyles and
excitement they nd in the company of less agree-
able people. Research shows that among young
people, peers selected youths who were high on
psychopathic traits to be their friends nearly as
often as youths who were low on these traits,
albeit this occurred mostly for boys. Psychopathic
girls were less popular among peers and experi-
enced less reciprocation in their choices (Muñoz
et al. 2008). However, the moderate popularity of
psychopaths among those low on psychopathy
might be true exclusively among teenagers,
because adolescence is a developmental stage at
which experimentation with antisociality and
delinquent acts is more normative than at other
stages, and thus highly psychopathic peers can t
in their social environment more easily (Muñoz
et al. 2008).
Some research suggests that aggressiveness
and rule breaking observed among young people
with disruptive behavior disorders do not exert
negative inuence on quality of friendship.
Thus, those youths are still capable of building
and maintaining intimate relationships. Their
friendships are fairly stable and perceived as par-
ticularly conicted only by the psychopathic indi-
viduals and not by their friends. The latter seem to
be willing to endure a higher level of conict than
others would before reporting it as problematic
(Muñoz et al. 2008). Youths who show high levels
of instrumental aggression form friendships that
they report as being satisfactory, at least in the
early stages of its formation (Poulin and Boivin
2000). In contrast, those who show a more reac-
tive type of aggression, which involves impulsive
responses to perceived provocations or threats,
report low satisfaction and more conict in their
friendships than instrumentally aggressive youths
(Poulin and Boivin 2000). The detrimental inu-
ence that high levels of psychopathy have on
friendships seems to result from lack of empathy,
rather than potentially dangerous behaviors. Indi-
viduals with psychopathic traits, lacking in empa-
thy, are likely to fail to consider the wishes of
others in their actions. They may believe that
friends are there only to please them and may
perceive others as being unsupportive when they
do not submit to their wishes.
Conclusion
Personality is an important factor that has an
impact on building friendships and is related to
differences in friendship characteristics (Laakasuo
et al. 2017). Even though it presumably explains
only a relatively small fraction of the total vari-
ance in liking ratings and has a modest predictive
power when it comes to friendship formation and
maintenance, this impact is nontrivial (Wortman
and Wood 2011). The importance of friendsper-
sonalities might in fact seem obvious to most
people. However, not all personality traits have a
substantial inuence on the nature of those rela-
tionships, and a given feature may have different
impact on separate parts of friendshipsdynamic.
Extraversion or agreeableness, as opposed to, e.g.,
conscientiousness, emerges as more vital in
friendship formation. Emotional stability may be
seen as an important factor helpful to maintain a
relationship. While the Big Five traitsrelation-
ship to this particular social bond is relatively well
researched, more study appears to be required in
regard to the Dark Triad traits.
Are There General Rules to the Selection of
Friends?
Research consistently nds that similarity in per-
sonality is a strong predictor of friendship forma-
tion (Selfhout et al. 2010). Since personalities
have an impact on peoples everyday decisions,
the more similar two people are, the higher the
chance that they choose similar leisure activities
or work tasks. Actual location, work, or type of
education inuence who people are likely to meet
6 Personality and Friendships
and with whom they can commence a friendship.
Similar day schedule and choice of leisure and
work activities lead to opportunities for meeting
people who are similar in some ways. Moreover,
perceived similarity of personalities seems to be at
least equally (if not more) important for interper-
sonal attraction as actual similarity (Cemalcilar
et al. 2018). Though similarities in more ne-
grained and specic dimensions, such as attitudes,
preferences, economic statuses, and political ori-
entation, seem to play an even more proximate
role in determining whether initial liking trans-
forms into enduring friendship or not, it can be
argued that many of those specic dimensions are
manifestations of ones personality (Mund et al.
2018). Interestingly, similarity in the levels of
personality traits seems to play a more important
role in interpersonal attraction in face-to-face and
other interactions where reciprocity (of communi-
cation and exchange) is expected (Cemalcilar
et al. 2018). It is not nearly as important in
computer-mediated communications, including
social media and networks (such as Facebook,
Twitter, or Instagram), where users are more in
control of whether or not to further their interac-
tion or continue being an observer only.
While similarity remains one of the most com-
mon principles of friend selection, it does not by
any means explain all variance in this domain, and
there is certainly more complexity to how we
choose friends. Recently, a new exciting line of
research, one requiring rened study designs and
advanced statistical tools, started to examine how
certain combinations of personality traits of each
member of a dyad are important and how they
interactively contribute to person perception and
interpersonal attraction (Cemalcilar et al. 2018).
Are FriendsPersonalities Related to the
Functions of Friendship?
Dissimilarity, when modest, does not necessitate
dislike. In the realm of interpersonal attraction, it
has been found that perceivers seek those who are
slightly but not too much better versionsof
themselves (Cemalcilar et al. 2018). Afliating
with such individuals who complement them-
selves but not threaten their self-evaluation pro-
vides a good balance in their strivings towards two
potentially conicting goals (i.e., self-afrmation
and self-enhancement). While individuals gener-
ally seek to enhance their self-esteem by engaging
in upward comparison with people who are simi-
lar to themselves, there is a comfort zone for
associating with people who we perceive as better
than ourselves: perceiving others as being too
superior may be ego-deating.
Finding a nonthreatening but advantageous
object of social comparison might be an important
but not the only or main objective of initiating
friendships. Having friends provides a multitude
of benets, mostly via prolonged social accep-
tance, as well as emotional and physical support.
In fact, the quality of social relationships has been
found to have a tremendous impact on mental and
physical health and mortality (Wortman and
Wood 2011). Those multiple benets of having
friends point to the functional or adaptive expla-
nations of the origins of interpersonal liking
(Wojciszke et al. 2009). Research ndings linking
personality traits with liking and other aspects of
friendships are consistent with these explanations:
personality traits that are more likely to aid or
facilitate the interests of others are more strongly
associated with being liked by others and per-
ceived as attractive to potential friends. The rela-
tionships between traits and liking can therefore
be largely explained by communion (Wortman
and Wood 2011). How much a trait is liked by
others could consistently be explained by how
other-oriented that trait item is, as suggested by
Wojciszke et al. (2009).
Do Personalities of Our Friends Influence Our
Personalities?
Friendships can naturally have both positive and
negative consequences, and those frequently stem
from the personalities of people with whom one
interacts often and closely. As mentioned in the
introduction, personality traits continue to change
in the adulthood, and those changes may have
important inuence on health and mortality. Expe-
riences gained in relationships and social roles can
have an enduring impact on trait development.
Personalities of our friends inuence our own
personalities and behaviors. Depending on how
similar our personalities are to those of our
Personality and Friendships 7
friends, our traits can be reinforced or their
expression suppressed (Nelson et al. 2011). Such
inuence can be particularly alarming in case of
friendships between individuals with less desir-
able traits, such as psychopaths. Preliminary evi-
dence suggests that, indeed, young people with
psychopathic traits might engage in antisocial acts
together, reinforcing their delinquency. On the
positive side, the same can be true to more socially
valuable traits, such as agreeableness.
How Do Personalities Influence Friendships?
Simply documenting that links exist between per-
sonality traits and experience of friendship does
not clarify the mechanisms through which person-
ality exerts its effects. More research is needed to
uncover processes through which personality
traits shape relationships with friends. The way
in which personality inuences friendship forma-
tion, maintenance, and satisfaction is likely
mostly indirect (or synergistic) via perceptions
that drive motivations, social actions, behaviors
and choices, the way people treat others, and
through activities which give them opportunities
to meet individuals interested in similar things.
More research is required to clarify these mecha-
nisms. For instance, quantity of time spent with
friends and quality of friend interactions (depth of
conversation, self-disclosure, and lack of emotion
suppression), although associated with friendship
satisfaction, failed to explain the associations
between personality traits and friendship satisfac-
tion (Wilson et al. 2015). Future research should
examine other potential interpersonal processes
that explain how personalities impact each stage
and aspect of relationships with friends, including
why some people are more satised with their
friendships than others.
Can We Help People Form Better Friendships?
By studying the mechanisms and processes that
account for individual differences in friendship,
we can improve our ability to help people under-
stand what may promote or impede the develop-
ment of quality friendships (Wilson et al. 2015).
As we gain better understanding of how person-
ality exerts its effects on friendship formation,
maintenance, and dissolution, we can achieve
new insights into the most relevant processes
that need to be targeted in prevention of negative
interpersonal phenomena and (therapeutic) inter-
vention to change ones own maladaptive behav-
iors. Importantly, it may be possible to improve
individualslives by targeting those processes
without directly changing the personality traits
driving those processes (Wilson et al. 2015).
Cross-References
Big Five Model
Interpersonal Self
Loneliness
Manipulativeness
Machiavellianism
Narcissism
Need to Belong
Psychopathy
Social Connection Seeking
Shyness
Social Interaction
Acknowledgments The present work was supported by
grant no. 2016/23/G/HS6/01397 from the National Science
Center, Poland, awarded to the last author.
References
Abell, L., Brewer, G., Qualter, P., & Austin, E. (2016).
Machiavellianism, emotional manipulation, and friend-
ship functions in womens friendships. Personality and
Individual Differences, 88, 108113.
Anderson, C., John, O. P., Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M.
(2001). Who attains social status? Effects of personality
and physical attractiveness in social groups. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 81,116132.
https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.81.1.116.
Asendorpf, J. B., & Wilpers, S. (1998). Personality effects
on social relationships. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 74, 15311544.
Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2011).
A closer look at rst sight: Social relations lens model
analysis of personality and interpersonal attraction at
zero acquaintance. European Journal of Personality,
25, 225238. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.790.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to
belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fun-
damental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin,
117 ,497529. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781351153683.
8 Personality and Friendships
Brewer, G., Abell, L., & Lyons, M. (2014). Machiavellian-
ism, competition and self-disclosure in friendship. Indi-
vidual Differences Research, 12(1), 17.
Cemalcilar, Z., Baruh, L., Kezer, M., Kamiloglu, R. G., &
Nigdeli, B. (2018). Role of personality traits in rst
impressions: An investigation of actual and perceived
personality similarity effects on interpersonal attraction
across communication modalities. Journal of Research
in Personality, 76, 139149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jrp.2018.07.009.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). Personality in adult-
hood: A six year longitudinal study of self-reports and
spouse ratings on the NEO personality inventory. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 853863.
Cuperman, R., & Ickes, W. (2009). Big ve predictors of
behavior and perceptions in initial dyadic interactions:
Personality similarity helps extraverts and introverts,
but hurts disagreeables. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 97(4), 667684.
Czarna, A. Z., Dufner, M., & Clifton, A. D. (2014). The
effects of vulnerable and grandiose narcissism on
liking-based and disliking-based centrality in social
networks. Journal of Research in Personality, 50,
4245.
Czarna, A. Z., Leifeld, P., Śmieja, M., Dufner, M., &
Salovey, P. (2016). Do narcissism and emotional intel-
ligence win us friends? Modeling dynamics of peer
popularity using inferential network analysis. Person-
ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 15881599.
Harris, K., & Vazire, S. (2016). On friendship development
and the big ve personality traits. Social and Person-
ality Psychology Compass, 10, 647667. https://doi.
org/10.1111/spc3.12287.
Hart, W., & Adams, J. M. (2014). Are narcissists more
accepting of othersnarcissistic traits? Personality and
Individual Differences, 64, 163167.
Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Malcolm, K. T. (2007). The
importance of conscientiousness in adolescent interper-
sonal relationships. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 33(3), 368383.
Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Knack, J. M., Waldrip, A. M., &
Campbell, S. D. (2007). Do Big Five personality traits
associated with self-control inuence the regulation of
anger and aggression?. Journal of Research in Person-
ality, 41, 403424.
Jonason, P. K., & Schmitt, D. P. (2012). What have you
done for me lately? Friendship-selection in the shadow
of the dark triad traits. Evolutionary Psychology, 10.
https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491201000303.
Laakasuo, M., Rotkirch, A., Berg, V., & Jokela, M. (2017).
The company you keep: Personality and friendship
characteristics. Social Psychological and Personality
Science, 8(1), 6673.
Leckelt, M., Küfner, A. C., Nestler, S., & Back, M. D.
(2015). Behavioral processes underlying the decline of
narcissistspopularity over time. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 109(5), 856871.
Lyons, M., & Aitken, S. (2010). Machiavellian friends?
The role of Machiavellianism in friendship formation
and maintenance. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and
Cultural Psychology, 4(3), 194202.
Maaß, U., Lämmle, L., Bensch, D., & Ziegler, M. (2016).
Narcissists of a feather ock together: Narcissism and
the similarity of friends. Personality and Social Psy-
chology Bulletin, 42(3), 366384.
Mund, M., Jeronimus, B. F., & Neyer, F. J. (2018). Person-
ality and social relationships: As thick as thieves. In
C. Johansen (Ed.), Personality and disease
(pp. 153183). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.
Muñoz, L. C., Kerr, M., & Besic, N. (2008). The peer
relationships of youths with psychopathic personality
traits: A matter of perspective. Criminal Justice and
Behavior, 35(2), 212227.
Nelson, P. A., Thorne, A., & Shapiro, L. A. (2011). Im
outgoing and shes reserved: The reciprocal dynamics
of personality in close friendships in young adulthood.
Journal of Personality, 79(5), 11131148.
Nicholls, E., & Stukas, A. A. (2011). Narcissism and the
self-evaluation maintenance model: Effects of social
comparison threats on relationship closeness. The Jour-
nal of Social, 151(2), 201212.
Paulhus,D.L.,&Williams,K.M.(2002).Thedarktriadof
personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychop-
athy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36,556563.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6.
Poulin, F., & Boivin, M. (2000). The role of proactive and
reactive aggression in the formation and development
of boysfriendships. Developmental Psychology,
36(2), 233240.
Selfhout, M., Burk, W., Branje, S., Denissen, J., Van Aken,
M., & Meeus, W. (2010). Emerging late adolescent
friendship networks and big ve personality traits:
A social network approach. Journal of Personality,
78(2), 509538.
Wagner, J., Lüdtke, O., Roberts, B. W., & Trautwein,
U. (2014). Who belongs to me? Social relationship
and personality characteristics in the transition to
young adulthood. European Journal of Personality,
28(6), 586603.
Wilson, R. E., Harris, K., & Vazire, S. (2015). Personality
and friendship satisfaction in daily life: Do everyday
social interactions account for individual differences in
friendship satisfaction? European Journal of Personal-
ity, 29(2), 173186.
Wojciszke, B., Abele, A. E., & Baryla, W. (2009). Two
dimensions of interpersonal attitudes: Liking depends
on communion, respect depends on agency. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 973990.
Wortman, J., & Wood, D. (2011). The personality traits of
liked people. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(6),
519528.
Personality and Friendships 9
... Over the life course, people build connections with others (Doroszuk et al., 2019). Social relationships are closely related to the support they give to and receive from each other, which are closely related to belongings, health, and mood. ...
... Social relationships are closely related to the support they give to and receive from each other, which are closely related to belongings, health, and mood. A close friendship differs from other kinds of relations, which is voluntary, informal, and pleasant (Doroszuk et al., 2019). It is unlike the kind of relationship built with family members or the bonds built at work, where people cannot choose whom to build connections with (Doroszuk et al., 2019). ...
... A close friendship differs from other kinds of relations, which is voluntary, informal, and pleasant (Doroszuk et al., 2019). It is unlike the kind of relationship built with family members or the bonds built at work, where people cannot choose whom to build connections with (Doroszuk et al., 2019). A close friend is regarded as someone who plays a significant role in their life and shares time together. ...
Article
Full-text available
Close friends are important across the life span, who spend time together, provide support, and share happiness. But what determines the number of close friends one would have? One of the most important factors would be personality traits, which capture the most basic differences among individuals in terms of how they feel, think, and behave. This report aimed to establish the associations between the Big Five personality traits and the number of close friends cross-sectionally and longitudinally. By analyzing a cross-sectional (N = 32, 990) and longitudinal dataset (N = 22, 383) from Understanding Society: the UK Household Longitudinal Study (UKHLS), the current report found that Neuroticism has a negative connection with the number of close friends, whereas Agreeableness, Openness, and Extraversion were positively connected to the number of close friends in the cross-sectional study. In the longitudinal study, Openness was positively associated with changes in the number of close friends.
... Rather, there would be variation in the strategies employed due to individual traits such as personality. Personality is a primary predictor of human behavior, including friendship (Doroszuk et al., 2019). Consequently, we would anticipate that specific personality traits would predict the inclination to utilize particular strategies. ...
... We also found that individuals low in extraversion were more willing to employ the "give gifts" strategy compared to high scorers, with a moderate to large effect. Research indicates that extraverts tend to have warm, caring, and generally low-conflict friendships (Doroszuk et al., 2019). Thus, it is surprising that this personality dimension did not predict additional strategies for strengthening friendships. ...
Article
Full-text available
Friendship constitutes an important aspect of human interaction. However, not all friendships are equally desirable, and the current study aimed to identify the strategies that people use to strengthen desirable friendships. More specifically, Study 1 employed open-ended questionnaires on a sample of 218 Greek-speaking participants and identified 54 acts that people use for this purpose. Study 2 utilized a close-ended questionnaire on a sample of 434 Greek-speaking participants and classified these acts into seven broader strategies that people employ to strengthen desirable friendships. The most commonly used strategy was “provide support,” followed by “more frequent interaction,” and “show trust.” Additionally, women indicated a higher willingness to use these strategies than men, while age effects were observed for several strategies. Furthermore, individuals who scored higher in agreeableness and openness indicated a greater willingness to use most of the identified strategies.
... Additionally, throughout this study, it is found that when dealing with stress, the majority of research participants seek help from their social network or their close ones. This shows that social connection is associated with the support that people give and receive from one another, which also has a positive impact on one's health, mood, and sense of belonging (Doroszuk et al., 2019). Ali has also further enlightened the researchers about how his friendship arose with another good friend: Close friends continue to be important companions and sources of support for older adults, despite the fact that friendships vary in terms of quality and stability. ...
... (Carstensen, 2006). This was demonstrated by Ali's friendship with his close ones, on which they could rely and provide instrumental as well as emotional support for one another, as providing and receiving support strengthens and extends relationships (Doroszuk et al., 2019). ...
... Previous research has shown that individuals scoring high on narcissism tend to tolerate their own traits in others (e.g., Hart & Adams, 2014;Kay, 2021). This tolerance may help individuals scoring high on narcissism to maintain relationships with others scoring similarly high on narcissism (Doroszuk et al., 2019;Maaß et al., 2018). In line with this, we hypothesized that the tolerance of narcissistic traits would help two individuals with high narcissism scores to maintain their friendship, but at the expense of low friendship quality. ...
... Despite these issues, many individuals scoring high on narcissism are clearly capable of maintaining long-lasting relationships (e.g., see Gore & Widiger, 2016). One possible explanation for why an individual may accept the difficulties that accompany having a relationship with a narcissistic person is to be at least somewhat narcissistic oneself (Doroszuk et al., 2019;Grosz et al., 2015;Hart & Adams, 2014;Maaß et al., 2018). Thus, it is possible that scoring similarly high on narcissism may foster the maintenance of a relationship. ...
Thesis
Who is willing to be in a close relationship to an individual with high narcissism, and how do individuals with high narcissism perceive their friendships? Three aspects of narcissism were distinguished (agentic, antagonistic, neurotic) to determine their association with four aspects of friendship quality (appreciation, intimacy, conflict, dominance). In the first study, a dyadic perspective was taken to observe whether friendship quality differs depending on the dyadic narcissism level of friends. As hypothesized, individuals in dyads with higher narcissism perceived their friendship quality as lower, compared to individuals in dyads with lower narcissism. More conflicts were perceived across narcissism aspects. Dyads with high antagonistic narcissism also perceived lower appreciation and intimacy. Results were interpreted in favor of the assumption that maladaptive traits are tolerated by those who possess these traits themselves. In the second study, a longitudinal perspective was taken to examine interactional effects of narcissism and friendship quality across 4 measurement occasions. On a within-person level, individuals scoring lower than usual on narcissism were found to subsequently perceive higher appreciation, and those perceiving lower appreciation than usual subsequently increased in antagonistic narcissism. Results suggested that the effects found in relationship formation tend to generalize to relationship maintenance. Overall, this work expanded previous research on narcissism and social relationships by observing relationship quality in long-term friendships including a dyadic as well as a longitudinal perspective. To answer the question of who is willing to be friends with someone high in narcissism, results suggest that it would be individuals who also score high on narcissism. In regard to the question of how individuals with high narcissism perceive their friendships it was found that they tend to be willing to accept lower friendship quality.
... Idealnya, panti asuhan dalam mengembangkan konsep diri seorang remaja memberikan sentuhan memfasilitasi remaja untuk mulai menyongsong diri secara mandiri dibuktikan dengan mulai membuka diskusi untuk menentukan pilihan dari mereka sendiri selepas pendidikan menengah atas mereka selesai (Nwaneri & Sadoh, 2016). Namun dalam menentukan dan mengenali potensi diri, terkadang setiap individu masih dipengaruhi oleh seberapa kondusif lingkungan sekitarnya, termasuk teman sebaya yang juga memiliki pengaruh terhadap kepribadian seseorang (Doroszuk et al., 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
Merujuk data Kementerian Sosial RI, tercatat sekitar 30 ribu anak menjadi yatim piatu akibat COVID-19. Sebagian dari mereka akhirnya ditempatkan di panti asuhan. Namun, dalam memenuhi keperluan penghuni, panti menghadapi banyak kendala, salah satunya berkaitan dengan anggaran yang terbatas dan penghuni panti yang masih kesulitan dalam mengembangkan potensi diri karena tidak maksimalnya pembelajaran daring saat pandemi. Sehingga, yayasan perlu memutar otak untuk mengakomodasi kebutuhan finansial sekaligus kesehatan mental, termasuk pemenuhan akademik bagi para penghuninya. Melihat permasalahan yang ada, penulis kemudian mencoba membantu Panti Asuhan bertahan dengan menyelenggarakan pelatihan dan kursus intensif bagi anak-anak di sana dan membantu pemberdayaan unit usaha yang dikelola oleh panti. Metode pelaksanaan kegiatan yang dilaksanakan dibagi menjadi 3 tahapan yakni: tahap koordinasi, tahap persiapan, dan tahap pelaksanaan. Tahap pelaksanaan difokuskan pada penggalian potensi anak dan pendampingan psikologis. Sedangkan untuk optimalisasi unit usaha, diberikan pemahaman mengenai pemaksimalan promosi dan penjualan. Artikel ini menggambarkan bagaimana penulis mendampingi panti asuhan untuk bertahan saat pandemi dengan jangka waktu dari minggu terakhir Bulan Februari serta minggu pertama dan kedua di Bulan Maret 2021 dengan pelaksanaan bertempat di Panti Asuhan Sentosa Kota Banjarmasin. Dari pendampingan yatim piatu yang telah dilakukan, anak-anak menjadi lebih stabil emosionalnya, dan mampu mengenali potensi diri lebih baik. Sedangkan unit usaha panti asuhan dapat memaksimalkan promosi dan tingkat penjualan mereka.
Article
Full-text available
International students are susceptible to threats to their well-being, as their immigrant status in host societies exposes them to unique challenges such as language difficulties, cultural differences, social isolation, and discrimination (to list a few). Relationships are an important component of individuals’ well-being that can mitigate these challenges. Hence, this study aimed to explore the experiences of relational well-being of African international students at a South African university. The study adopted a qualitative design wherein semi-structured interviews were employed to collect data from 16 participants (mean age = 24; females = 5; males = 11). Using thematic analysis, we found that African international students have reciprocal positive relationships with local and other immigrant students, and these contribute to well-being outcomes in the social, psychological, and educational domains of life. In addition, the prevailing institutional culture and the availability of spaces that provide opportunities for interaction are important determinants of relational experiences in the university campus space. It is recommended that student affairs departments create spaces in which African international students could further engage and interact with a diverse group of students to enhance their relational well-being.
Chapter
Cultural, political, and legal changes in the 21st century have changed the landscapes in which our close interpersonal relationships take place. Many choose to remain single or get married later. Online dating and cohabitation are more readily accepted and common. Our friendships, especially on social media, have gained importance. Issues surrounding gender identity, equity, and sexual orientation also loom large. With the help of technology, more couples, including same-sex couples, are now able to become parents. From same-sex to open and polyamorous marriages, how we define and perceive some of our most important close relationships have changed. The dissolution and reformation of partnerships and families have gained new importance as nontraditional families have become more common. The formation of families through adoption raises questions of identity and successful parenting. Relationships across cultural, racial, religious, and national lines are also more relevant in today’s pluralistic societies. Finally, what types of relationships are considered worthy of scholarly and scientific attention, and the lenses with which to study them, have also evolved. This volume compiles the latest research and theory on close relationships of the 21st century from multidisciplinary and international perspectives with the intent of taking stock of this shifting terrain.
Thesis
Freundschaftsfähigkeiten, die die Anzahl an Freundschaften, Homophilität des Freundeskreises und das Erleben von Freundschaften beeinflussen werden im Rahmen eines systematischen Literaturreviews identifiziert. Dabei werden die Persönlichkeitsmerkmale der Big Five: Offenheit für Erfahrung, Gewissenhaftigkeit, Extraversion, Verträglichkeit und Neurotizismus, sowie die der dunklen Triade: Narzissmus, Machiavellismus und Psychopathie im Zusammenhang mit Freundschaft analysiert. Insgesamt wurden 112 Studien gesichtet und davon 21 in das Review einbezogen. Es lässt sich deutlich mehr Literatur für die Big Five als für die dunkle Triade und Freundschaft finden. Die Ergebnisse zeigen, dass Extraversion für ein mehr an Freundschaften (0.30 < d < 2.20), Offenheit für Erfahrung für weniger homophile Freundeskreise (d = 0.12) und die dunkle Triade für ein negativeres Erleben von Freundschaften stehen (d = -0.30). Zusammenfassend lässt sich sagen, dass ein Zusammenhang von Freundschaft und Persönlichkeit besteht. Allerdings bedarf es der Aufklärung der Ursachen-Wirkungsbeziehung und weitere Forschung in Hinblick auf die dunkle Triade und Freundschaft. Schlüsselwörter: Freundschaftsfähigkeit, Freundschaft, Persönlichkeit, Review und Erwachsene
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter shows that personality traits and social relationships are deeply entwined in a bidirectional way: Individuals select relationships partly based on their personality traits but at the same time develop across the lifespan partly in response to changes in their social environment. Life transitions are an important catalyst of changes in personality-relationship transactions. We argue that personality traits and social relationships are so closely tied that, in our view, the link between personality and health can only be understood against the backdrop of individuals’ relationships.
Article
Full-text available
Studies on personality and friendship have focused on similarities between friends, while differences in friendship patterns have received less attention. We used data from the British Household Panel Survey data (N = 12,098) to investigate how people’s personalities are related to various characteristics of their three closest friends. All personality traits of the five-factor model were associated with several friendship characteristics with effect sizes corresponding to correlations between −.06 and .09. Openness was especially prominent and idiosyncratic; individuals with high (vs. low) openness were about 3% more likely to have friends who live further away, are of the opposite sex and another ethnicity, and whom they meet less often. Agreeableness and extroversion were related to more traditional friendship ties. Individuals with high agreeableness had known their friends for a longer time, lived close to them, and had more “stay-at-homes” among their friends.
Article
Full-text available
This research investigated effects of narcissism and emotional intelligence (EI) on popularity in social networks. In a longitudinal field study we examined the dynamics of popularity in 15 peer groups in two waves (N=273). We measured narcissism, ability EI, explicit and implicit self-esteem. In addition, we measured popularity at zero acquaintance and three months later. We analyzed the data using inferential network analysis (temporal exponential random graph modeling, TERGM) accounting for self-organizing network forces. People high in narcissism were popular, but increased less in popularity over time than people lower in narcissism. In contrast, emotionally intelligent people increased more in popularity over time than less emotionally intelligent people. The effects held when we controlled for explicit and implicit self-esteem. These results suggest that narcissism is rather disadvantageous and that EI is rather advantageous for long-term popularity.
Article
Full-text available
Following a dual-pathway approach to the social consequences of grandiose narcissism, we investigated the behavioral processes underlying (a) the decline of narcissists' popularity in social groups over time and (b) how this is differentially influenced by the 2 narcissism facets admiration and rivalry. In a longitudinal laboratory study, participants (N = 311) first provided narcissism self-reports using the Narcissistic Personality Inventory and the Narcissistic Admiration and Rivalry Questionnaire, and subsequently interacted with each other in small groups in weekly sessions over the course of 3 weeks. All sessions were videotaped and trained raters coded participants' behavior during the interactions. Within the sessions participants provided mutual ratings on assertiveness, untrustworthiness, and likability. Results showed that (a) over time narcissists become less popular and (b) this is reflected in an initially positive but decreasing effect of narcissistic admiration as well as an increasing negative effect of narcissistic rivalry. As hypothesized, these patterns of results could be explained by means of 2 diverging behavioral pathways: The negative narcissistic pathway (i.e., arrogant-aggressive behavior and being seen as untrustworthy) plays an increasing role and is triggered by narcissistic rivalry, whereas the relevance of the positive narcissistic pathway (i.e., dominant-expressive behavior and being seen as assertive) triggered by narcissistic admiration decreases over time. These findings underline the utility of a behavioral pathway approach for disentangling the complex effects of personality on social outcomes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2015 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
We investigate the effects of perceivers’ impressions of others’ personality traits on their social attraction, after a brief encounter face-to-face or via Facebook. We first examine the main effects of perceived personality traits. Next, we compare and contrast the effects of perceived- and actual- personality similarity through polynomial regressions with response surface analysis (RSA) accounting for dyads’ respective levels of personality traits. Results suggest that RSA yield estronger effects of perceived similarity (vs. actual) on attraction. Perceivers are more attracted to targets who are “better versions” of themselves, yet too much discrepancy impede attraction, more so when face-to-face.
Article
The current study focuses on the emergence of friendship networks among just-acquainted individuals, investigating the effects of Big Five personality traits on friendship selection processes. Sociometric nominations and self-ratings on personality traits were gathered from 205 late adolescents (mean age=19 years) at 5 time points during the first year of university. SIENA, a novel multilevel statistical procedure for social network analysis, was used to examine effects of Big Five traits on friendship selection. Results indicated that friendship networks between just-acquainted individuals became increasingly more cohesive within the first 3 months and then stabilized. Whereas individuals high on Extraversion tended to select more friends than those low on this trait, individuals high on Agreeableness tended to be selected more as friends. In addition, individuals tended to select friends with similar levels of Agreeableness, Extraversion, and Openness.
Article
In this paper, we argue that friendship should receive more attention in social/personality research. Here, we focus on our area of expertise, personality traits, and review the literature on how personality traits influence friendship formation, maintenance, and dissolution. Specifically, we examine how personality traits of the actor and partner influence friendship development (i.e., who is more likely to initiate and end friendships, and who is more likely to be the target of friendship initiation and termination). We also discuss dyadic effects, such as personality similarity, on friendship development. Throughout this review, we draw on the literature on personality and romantic relationships to identify the most important gaps in the literature on personality and friendships. Our review suggests that agreeableness has the most consistent effects on both romantic relationships and friendships, followed by neuroticism. Extraversion, conscientiousness, and openness to experience have all been shown to influence relationship development, but their effects are inconsistent. We end with a call for more research on friendship beyond the role of personality traits and suggestions for specific future directions.
Article
Who is willing to expose himself or herself to narcissists on a long-term basis? Studies that address the interactions of narcissists focus mainly on their interactions with strangers. Hence, the aim of the present study was to investigate the extent to which two best friends’ similarity in narcissism would influence their similarities in other personality profiles. A total of 290 best friends’ dyads filled out measurements of the whole Dark Triad as well as the Big Five. For each personality domain, profile similarity and its dependence on the similarity in the Dark Triad were determined. Results showed that the friends’ similarity in narcissism significantly predicted similarity in all Big Five domains. For the general Big Five similarity as well as extraversion, the effect of narcissism similarity was stronger for male than female or mixed friends. Similarity in psychopathy and Machiavellianism significantly predicted all domains except for openness and extraversion, respectively.
Article
Who are the people who maintain satisfying friendships? And, what are the behaviours that might explain why those people achieve high friendship satisfaction? We examined the associations between personality (self-reports and peer-reports) and friendship satisfaction (self-reports) among 434 students. We also examined whether role personality (how people act with their friends) and quantity and quality of social interactions using ecological momentary assessment mediate the associations between personality and friendship satisfaction. Extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness and (low) neuroticism were associated with higher levels of friendship satisfaction. These associations could not be accounted for by individual differences in role personality. In addition, our results suggest that quantity of time spent with friends and quality of friend interactions (depth of conversation, self-disclosure and lack of emotion suppression), although associated with friendship satisfaction, do not account for the associations between trait personality and friendship satisfaction. Future research should examine other potential interpersonal processes that explain why some people are more satisfied with their friendships than others and the consequences of friendship satisfaction (e.g. for well-being). Copyright © 2015 European Association of Personality Psychology
Article
Not much is known about how social network characteristics change in the transition out of school and what role Big Five personality plays in this context. The aim of this paper was twofold. First, we explored changes in social network and relationship characteristics across the transition out of secondary school. Second, we examined within-person and between-person effects of personality on these social network changes. Results based on a series of multilevel models to a longitudinal sample of 2287 young adults revealed four main findings. First, social networks increased in size, and this increase was mainly due to a larger number of nonkin. Stable social networks during the transition consisted mainly of family ties but were generally characterized by high closeness. Second, extraversion and openness consistently predicted network size, whereas agreeableness predicted network overlap. Third, increases in emotional closeness were found only for kin; closeness was generally lower for unstable relationships. Fourth, changes in emotional closeness were related to personality, particularly neuroticism, agreeableness, and conscientiousness for stable relationships; for unstable relationships, however, closeness was related to extraversion and openness. The article concludes by discussing the role of personality for social relationship development and the active moulding of social networks in young adulthood. Copyright © 2014 European Association of Personality Psychology