ArticlePDF Available

A Consumer Neuroscience Study of Conscious and Subconscious Destination Preference

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

In studying consumer behaviors, the inclusion of neuroscience tools and methods is improving our understanding of preference formation and choice. But such responses are mostly related to the consumption of goods and services that meet an immediate need. Tourism represents a consumer behavior that is related to a more complex decision-making process, involving a stronger relationship with a future self, and choices typically being of a higher level of involvement and of a transformational type. The aim of this study was to test whether direct emotional and cognitive responses to travel destination would be indicative of subsequent stated destination preference. Participants were shown images and videos from multiple travel destinations while being monitored using eye-tracking and electroencephalography (EEG) brain monitoring. The EEG responses to each image and video were further calculated into neurometric scores of emotional (frontal asymmetry and arousal) and cognitive load metrics. Our results show that arousal and cognitive load were significantly related to subsequent stated travel preferences, accounting for about 20% of the variation in preference. Still, results also suggested that subconscious emotional and cognitive responses are not identical to subjective travel preference, suggesting that other mechanisms may be at play in forming conscious, stated preference. This study both supports the idea that destination preferences can be studied using consumer neuroscience and brings further insights into the mechanisms at stake during such choices.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
1
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:15102 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-51567-1
www.nature.com/scientificreports
A Consumer Neuroscience Study
of Conscious and Subconscious
Destination Preference
Thomas Zoëga Ramsøy1,2*, Noela Michael
3 & Ian Michael4
In studying consumer behaviors, the inclusion of neuroscience tools and methods is improving our
understanding of preference formation and choice. But such responses are mostly related to the
consumption of goods and services that meet an immediate need. Tourism represents a consumer
behavior that is related to a more complex decision-making process, involving a stronger relationship
with a future self, and choices typically being of a higher level of involvement and of a transformational
type. The aim of this study was to test whether direct emotional and cognitive responses to travel
destination would be indicative of subsequent stated destination preference. Participants were shown
images and videos from multiple travel destinations while being monitored using eye-tracking and
electroencephalography (EEG) brain monitoring. The EEG responses to each image and video were
further calculated into neurometric scores of emotional (frontal asymmetry and arousal) and cognitive
load metrics. Our results show that arousal and cognitive load were signicantly related to subsequent
stated travel preferences, accounting for about 20% of the variation in preference. Still, results also
suggested that subconscious emotional and cognitive responses are not identical to subjective travel
preference, suggesting that other mechanisms may be at play in forming conscious, stated preference.
This study both supports the idea that destination preferences can be studied using consumer
neuroscience and brings further insights into the mechanisms at stake during such choices.
In understanding human preference formation and decision-making, one recent successful approach has been to
combine a neuroscientic approach with the study of real-life choices such as consumer behaviors. is approach
has demonstrated the brain mechanisms underlying attentional, emotional and cognitive responses that drive
consumer choices, going under headings such as “consumer neuroscience” and “neuromarketing”16.
Previous studies in consumer neuroscience have primarily focused on consumption behaviors that are related
to more immediate rewards such as food choices, product purchase, and luxury goods. In doing so, these studies
have been successful in providing insights into the mechanisms of these types of consumer behaviors, and even
be able to predict such choices up to several seconds before they occur or are consciously felt79. Conversely, fewer
studies have looked at choices that are more future-oriented, such as which career path to take or where to travel
for holidays.
e purpose of this study is to employ the same approach as previously done in consumer neuroscience stud-
ies to these types of behaviors, to better understand whether immediate emotional and cognitive responses to
future choice options are related to subsequent choices. Here, we focus on travel destination preference as a model
to understand this type of non-direct consumer preference formation and choice. is area falls in a broader area
of destination marketing, which recently has seen the rst steps of including neuroscience tools and insights10,11.
To better situate the current study, we have provided a Supplementary Section that goes through the background
of destination marketing and how the study of emotional and cognitive responses have been conceptualized
and studied, ranging from qualitative research methods to the recent inclusion of neuroscience methods (see
Supplementary Materials).
At the core of prior research on destination preference formation lies both theoretical and empirical research
suggesting that destination preference both has conscious and subconscious components, but that our under-
standing of the role of the subconscious is woefully lacking. Hence, the current study aims to capture the sub-
conscious emotional responses to destination marketing stimuli through images and videos, to test whether such
1Neurons Inc, Taastrup, Denmark. 2Integrative Center for Applied Neuroscience, Copenhagen, Denmark. 3College
of Communication and Media Science, Zayed University, Dubai, United Arab Emirates. 4College of Business, Zayed
University, Dubai, United Arab Emirates. *email: thomas@neuronsinc.com
OPEN
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
2
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:15102 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-51567-1
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
measures predict subsequent self-reported destination preference. In this study, our basic assumption was that
variations in SDP would also manifest as rapid emotional responses to visual representations of destinations.
Methodology
is study involves a multi-modal approach including self-reported destination preference, eye-tracking meas-
ures, and neuroimaging measures of emotional and cognitive responses. In the following we present the partici-
pant selection, choice of stimuli, measures, and analytical approaches.
Institutional approval for this study was obtained from the Zayed University (ZU14_086a_F). All participants
lled out an informed consent form, and all recorded data were anonymized as part of the data acquisition. All
experimental procedures were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.
Participants. To test the conscious and subconscious emotional and cognitive destination responses we
recruited participants from a local convenience sample of participants who were possible candidates for travel
due to vacation, studies, and/or work (N = 32, 15 women, age mean ± std = 20.3 ± 1.9) in the larger Copenhagen
Region, Denmark. All participants provided informed consent following the declaration of Helsinki prior to
enrolling in the study.
Stimuli selection. e destination marketing stimuli used were images, names, and promotion videos from
travel destinations. ese destinations were Abu Dhabi, Dubai, Hong Kong, London, Madrid, New York, Paris,
San Francisco, Singapore, and Sydney. We used three independent raters to identify images according to whether
they were representative and creatively similar. e images and videos used were selected using the following
criteria:
• e creative image and video should be representative of the destination based on the elements in the image
(e.g., symbols, ags, status/icons etc.).
• If possible, the creative image should be representative of materials provided by each representative destina-
tion (e.g. their travel agency or other tourism entity).
• e creative images were compared on visual aspects such as color composition and visual complexity, using
the NeuroVision tool (https://www.neuronsinc.com/neurovision-app).
Apparatus and procedure. After signing an informed consent sheet, participants were fitted with
eye-tracking glasses and a mobile brain monitor. ey then underwent eye-tracking and neuroimaging calibra-
tion procedures. We used Tobii Glasses Pro 2 eye-tracking system and an ABM X-10 electroencephalography
(EEG) brain monitor. e eye-tracking was recorded using the Tobii Glasses Controller soware (www.tobii.
com) and the EEG signals were recorded using the B-Alert Lab soware (www.advancedbrainmonitoring.com)
running in a Windows 10 environment (www.Microso.com). e following specications apply for the EEG
recordings: Nine sensor sites were used following the 10–20 system, including Fz, F3, F4, Cz, C3, C4, POz, P3, P4,
xed gain referenced to linked mastoids.
Eye-tracking calibration was done with the 1-point xation proprietary Tobii solution. Eye-tracking data were
used to ensure that participants were indeed paying attention to the images and videos presented on the screen,
but not analyzed specically for this project.
For the EEG recording, linked reference electrodes were located behind each ear on the mastoid bone.
Impedances were ensured to be below 40 k for all sites before recording commenced, following the recom-
mended levels through the ABM system (http://tinyurl.com/y2s9uplz). e EEG data acquisition was sampled at
256 Hz with a high pass lter at 0.1 Hz and a h order, low pass lter at 100 Hz. e EEG data were transmitted
wirelessly via Bluetooth to a nearby laptop computer which stored the psychophysiological data. We then used
ABM’s proprietary acquisition soware for artifact decontamination algorithms for eye blink, muscle movement,
and environmental/electrical interference such as spikes and saturations.
EEG calibration was done using functional localizer tests, based on the ABM B-ALERT calibration pro-
cess. e acquisition of benchmark data was used to create individualized EEG proles required for calcu-
lating emotional arousal and cognitive load scores. e benchmarking session included three separate tasks:
The Three-Choice Vigilance Task (3CVT), the Verbal Psycho-Vigilance Task (VPVT), and the Auditory
Psycho-Vigilance Task (APVT). Data recorded from these tasks were then used to individualize the algorithms
by adjusting the centroids and through this produce the metric scores of arousal and working memory load,
as described in a previously published protocol12. is algorithm was saved as an individualized denition le,
which was used as a regressor when calculating and normalizing metrics.
EEG data were calculated into selected dierent “neurometric” scores, including frontal asymmetry, emo-
tional arousal, and working memory load, as described in more detail below. Here, each participant’s benchmark
was used as a calibration le upon which EEG data were normalized to scores ranging from 0 (minimum) to 1
(maximum). Each emotional and cognitive scores were calculated with a 1-second temporal resolution. is pro-
cedure allowed us to reliably track emotional and cognitive responses over time. Additional scores for distraction
and drowsiness were calculated but not included in the analyses.
Each participant was then presented with several images, names and promotion videos from travel destina-
tions. Images and destination names were presented for 8 seconds and videos for the duration of the video, sepa-
rated by a 2 second inter-stimulus interval, while promotional videos were played in their full length (see Fig.1).
Aer the test, all participants underwent a surprise survey, which assessed their memory for destinations shown,
conscious preference for traveling to the destination (“travel preference”) and destination associations. For the
present study, responses to destination names are not included in the analyses.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
3
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:15102 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-51567-1
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
All data were integrated, synchronized, and analyzed at the 1st level using R v3.2.1 (www.R-Project.org) and a
2nd level (group level) analysis was run in JMP v14.1 (www.jmp.com) running on a Windows 10 computer (www.
Microso.com).
Emotional responses were calculated as frontal asymmetry and arousal scores based on previously published
studies. Here, emotional valence and motivational direction was calculated based on the asymmetric engagement
of the frontal part of the brain, as demonstrated by previous research9,1318. e calculation used was based on
prior studies using the gamma frequency band8, where the ratio between the mean power in the gamma band of
frontal le electrodes (F3 and C3) relative to the mean of the right electrodes (F4 and C4), divided by the sum of
both hemisphere pairs, and then normalizing the scores to a 0–1 range. On the normalized 0–1 range of scores,
scores higher than 0.5 indicate increasingly positive scores and “approach motivation.” Conversely, scores lower
than 0.5 denote increasingly negative emotional responses and “avoidance motivation.
e second type of emotional response is referred to as emotional engagement or arousal, and reects a
bi-valent score that shows peak values for highly positive and highly negative events, and low scores for neu-
tral emotions. e score was calculated as the posterior probability of arousal based on a neural network based
model12 Arousal denotes emotional intensity but does not contain information about the actual direction of the
emotional response1922. Together, the arousal and frontal asymmetry scores provide a two-dimensional score for
emotional responses. ese two dimensions reect neuroscience work showing that emotional responses can be
evaluated on two dimensions: one dimension signifying the intensity of the emotion (here: “arousal”), and one
denoting the positive-negative valence or direction (here: “frontal asymmetry”) of emotional responses.
e working memory load metric is a measure of mental processing load, i.e. the demand put on working
memory, and increases when the amount of information being processed or kept active in memory is increased.
e metric was calculated as the posterior probability of a given brain state to be in high workload, and thereby
provide a continuous measure of working memory load12.
Finally, travel preferences were assessed through self-reported scores on willingness to travel to destinations,
for vacation, studies, or work. Further analyses into the correlation between each of these scores were performed
to assess whether they were highly correlated and would constitute a single type of destination preference, using
both correlation analyses and Cronbach’s alpha.
Results
Self-reported preferences showed a signicant dierence between destinations in terms of participants’ willing-
ness to consider the destination for a vacation (F = 66.82, p < 0.0001), study abroad (F = 56.36, p < 0.0001), work-
ing abroad (F = 50.21, p < 0.0001) and recommending to others (F = 59.64, p < 0.0001). e responses to each
destination were also highly correlated (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.85) suggesting that an aggregate score would be
sucient to capture self-reported measures of destination preference. To do this, we created an aggregate score
of the four sub-scores (vacation, study, work, recommend) and named this the Travel Motivation Score (TMS).
e TMS score was used throughout the rest of the study as a stated preference, to which we relate emotional and
cognitive subconscious responses.
Figure 1. e study design, where images and names were presented for 8 seconds, and videos for the entirety
of their duration (not shown). All stimuli were interspersed by an inter-stimulus interval of 2 seconds where a
xation cross was shown. Images in the photo are examples taken by Edward He and ZQ Lee on unsplash.com.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
4
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:15102 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-51567-1
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
When looking at the emotional and cognitive responses we found a signicant dierence between the places
on how they score, including frontal asymmetry (R2 = 0.029, F = 6.38, p < 0.0001), arousal (R2 = 0.009, F = 2.04,
p = 0.0321), but not for cognitive load (R2 = 0.003, F = 0.80, p = 0.6142). Figure2 shows the distribution of emo-
tional responses to destinations.
We then tested whether emotional and cognitive responses when watching tourism images and videos
were related to subsequent TMS scores. By running a random eects regression model we found that arousal
(β = 1.858, F = 15.38, p < 0.0001) and cognitive load (β = 3.619, F = 21.06, p < 0.0001), but not frontal asym-
metry (β = 0.136, F = 0.06, p = 0.8018), was related to subsequent TMS scores, and explaining almost 20% of the
variation in TMS (model R2 = 0.193, RMSE = 0.46). Notably, arousal was negatively related to TMS and cognitive
load was positively related to TMS. Figure3 displays these eects along with the relative distribution of arousal
and cognitive load scores for each destination.
A post-hoc exploratory analysis was then run to test for additional interaction eects. Here, we included fron-
tal asymmetry, arousal, cognitive load and their interaction eects, and correcting for multiple comparisons using
False Discovery Rate (FDR) correction. In doing so, arousal and cognitive load were still signicant. In addition,
a three-way interaction between frontal asymmetry, arousal and cognitive load (see Table1). An exploration of
the results showed a complex relationship between frontal asymmetry, arousal and cognitive load on predicting
subsequent TMS. Motivation showed a positive relationship with TMS when arousal was low and cognitive load
was high, and when arousal was high and cognitive load was low. Conversely, motivation showed a negative rela-
tionship with TMS when arousal and cognitive load were both either high or low.
Exploring the data further, we ran analyzes separately on images and videos. Here, we found that the emo-
tional eect is only signicant for videos (R2 = 0.139, F = 6.81, p = 0.0095) but not images (R2 = 0.173, F = 1.41,
p = 0.236). ese results indicate that dierences in emotional responses to destinations are driven only by watch-
ing videos, suggesting that videos are more emotionally engaging than single images. ere may be a number of
ways to explain these dierences: rst, a single video collectively contains quantitatively more visual materials
than single images do. Second, videos contain moving images which may be more visually engaging to look at.
ird, videos include auditory elements such as voices, sounds and other elements that can produce and increase
emotional responses.
Conclusion
is paper contributes to the scientic literature in at least two ways. In one line of conclusions, it provides among
the rst insights into the basic mechanisms of the subconscious processes that underlie destination preference
formation, and the distinction between subconscious and conscious processes. is paper suggests that there is
a distinction between subconscious emotional responses and overt destination preference. Indeed, in the study
of consumer psychology in conjunction with neuroscience, also known as consumer neuroscience, studies have
repeatedly demonstrated a distinction between a subconscious “wanting” system and a conscious “liking” system,
and that these systems contribute dierently to consumer behavior and choice. e present study ndings suggest
that there may be dierent mechanisms at stake in driving emotional responses and overt preference ratings. As
Figure 2. Distribution of emotional responses to travel destinations. e plot displays average scores for frontal
asymmetry (x-axis) and arousal (y-axis) for each travel destination. Dotted lines are indicative of shis between
negative and positive emotions (x-axis) and low vs high arousal (y-axis). Destinations that score high on frontal
asymmetry and arousal scores (e.g., Dubai) represent highly positive responses.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
5
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:15102 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-51567-1
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
this study did not include any overt choice, an obvious next step in research is to conduct studies that include an
element of choice, in which participants make actual overt destination choices. Here, based on both our results,
and prior literature, we could contend that emotional responses during video/image perception may be signi-
cantly related to actuated choice, and that a conjunction between subconscious and conscious scores may be more
predictive of actual choice than any scores individually. is is in line with prior consumer neuroscience studies
on choice studies on choice7,2372.
Another line of implications of this research is how it can influence the study of consumers’ minds.
Understanding consumption behavior, from tangible choices of food to more intangible and future goods such
Figure 3. Distribution of emotional and preference scores between dierent destinations. (A) Distribution
of average self reported travel preferences (TMS) for dierent destinations, showing that New York ranked
highest and Abu Dhabi lowest on group averaged TMS. (B) Regression analysis results from the relationship
between TMS and frontal asymmetry, arousal and cognitive load. Here, the black line represents the linear
regression, gray area denotes the 95% condence interval. (C) Contour plot shows the distribution of arousal
(x-axis) and cognitive load (y-axis) scores for each of the travel destinations, using a Gaussian blur function and
with intensity values going from low (light colors) to high (full colors), with further subdivision into responses
for images (green) and videos (red). As this plot shows, image responses tend to be more variable than video
responses.
Ter m Estimate Std Error DFDen t Rati o Prob > |t|
Intercept 3.651 0.65 505.0 5.59 <0.0001
Frontal asymmetry 1.003 0.58 1900.0 1.73 0.1357
Arousal 1.813 0.48 881.3 3.76 0.0004
Frontal asymmetry * Arousal 6.794 4.09 1902.3 1.66 0.1357
Cognitive Load 3.351 0.80 316.5 4.16 0.0003
Frontal asymmetry * Cognitive load 1.312 4.83 1905.5 0.27 0.7861
Arousal * Cognitive load 4.502 3.66 1588.2 1.23 0.2552
Frontal asymmetry * Arousal * Cognitive load 113.770 29.84 1897.6 3.81 0.0004
Table 1. Results from the exploratory regression analysis, showing that besides the main eects of arousal and
cognitive load, there is a signicant three-way interaction between frontal asymmetry, arousal and cognitive
load. All p-values are reported aer FDR correction for multiple comparisons.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
6
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:15102 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-51567-1
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
as travel and insurance, requires testing of such choice. Here, our study contributes to the understanding of more
abstract and future-oriented choice through the study of destination preference formation. While our study was
not designed to include a nal choice, the results are highly relevant to our understanding of preference formation
in these conditions. e nding that customers display subconscious emotional responses that are not related to
conscious destination preference conrms prior ndings and ideas about a dual-system for decision-making.
While the present study demonstrates the feasibility of using neuroscience to inform destination preferences,
a few limitations should be noted. First, this study only focused on general measures of emotional and cognitive
responses, and did not include any level of spatial reconstruction of where in the brain the given activity was
found. Subsequent studies should consider using neuroimaging methods that allow a higher spatial resolution
and reconstruction, such as functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI), high-resolution EEG (e.g., allowing
for LORETA or other reconstruction methods), and magnetoencephalography (MEG). Such studies are expected
to provide a better understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying destination preferences, and to what
extent they overlap with other comparable consumer-related choices.
Another notable issue in the present study is that the stimulus materials diverged on the type and number of
senses that were aected. Pictures are perceived visually, while videos contained music and narration in addition
to the visual materials. While the present study was not aimed at testing for the eects of additional sensory
information on emotional and cognitive responses and destination preference formation, future studies should
seek to better understand how multimodal vs unimodal perception can aect destination preference and choice.
Finally, in the present study, we did not test for the eects of attention on destination preference. Since all
stimuli were presented on-screen during a highly controlled setting, we would expect little variance in on-screen
activity that was related to such preference. Also, for the present study, we did not have any prior hypotheses
related to attention to certain elements. Should such hypotheses be suggested (e.g., that attention to faces is posi-
tively related to destination preference) such answers would be possible to targeted, even with the present data set.
Taken together, our ndings are in line with the literature and now extend such ndings to more complex
decision-making. Future studies should seek to also include destination choices that vary in the temporal dimen-
sion (e.g., comparing choices of planned travel in a year vs those that are spontaneous and instant) to better
understand how subconscious and conscious processes contribute to actual destination choices.
Received: 18 June 2019; Accepted: 3 October 2019;
Published: xx xx xxxx
References
1. Bell, L. et al. Beyond self-report: A review of physiological and neuroscientic methods to investigate consumer behavior. Front.
Psychol. 9, 1655 (2018).
2. Hsu, M. & Yoon, C. e neuroscience of consumer choice. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 5, 116–121 (2015).
3. armarar, U. . & Yoon, C. Consumer neuroscience: Advances in understanding consumer psychology. Current Opinion in
Psychology 10, 160–165 (2016).
4. Plassmann, H., amsøy, T. Z. & Milosavljevic, M. Branding the brain: A critical review and outloo. J. Consum. Psychol. 22, 18–36
(2012).
5. Shaw, S. D. & Bagozzi, . P. e neuropsychology of consumer behavior and mareting. Consum. Psychol. Rev. 1, 22–40 (2018).
6. Solnais, C., Andreu-Perez, J., Sánchez-Fernández, J. & Andréu-Abela, J. e contribution of neuroscience to consumer research: A
conceptual framewor and empirical review. Journal of Economic Psychology 36, 68–81 (2013).
7. nutson, B., ic, S., Wimmer, G. E., Prelec, D. & Loewenstein, G. Neural Predictors of Purchases. Neuron 53, 147–56 (2007).
8. amsøy, T. Z., Sov, M., Christensen, M. . & Stahlhut, C. Frontal brain asymmetry and willingness to pay. Front. Neurosci. 12
(2018).
9. avaja, N., Somervuori, O. & Salminen, M. Predicting purchase decision: e role of hemispheric asymmetry over the frontal
cortex. J. Neurosci. Psychol. Econ. 6, 1–13 (2013).
10. Bastiaansen, M. et al. My destination in your brain: A novel neuromareting approach for evaluating the eectiveness of destination
mareting. J. Destin. Mark. Manag. 7, 76–88 (2018).
11. otsi, F., Balarishnan, M. S., Michael, I. & amsøy, T. Z. Place branding: Aligning multiple staeholder perception of visual and
auditory communication elements. J. Destin. Mark. Manag. 7, 112–130 (2018).
12. Stiic, M. et al. Modeling temporal sequences of cognitive state changes based on a combination of EEG-engagement, EEG-
worload, and heart rate metrics. Front. Neurosci. 8 (2014).
13. Ohme, ., eyowsa, D., Wiener, D. & Choromansa, A. Analysis of Neurophysiological eactions to Advertising Stimuli by
Means of EEG and Galvanic Sin esponse Measures. J. Neurosci. Psychol. Econ. 2, 21–31 (2009).
14. Berman, E. T. & Lieberman, M. D. Approaching the bad and avoiding the good: lateral prefrontal cortical asymmetry distinguishes
between action and valence. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1970–1979 (2010).
15. Coan, J. A. & Allen, J. J. B. Frontal EEG asymmetry and the behavioral activation and inhibition systems. Psychophysiology 40,
106–14 (2003).
16. Davidson, . J. What does the prefrontal cortex ‘do’ in aect: Perspectives on frontal EEG asymmetry research. Biol. Psychol. 67,
219–233 (2004).
17. Plassmann, H., Venat raman, V., Huettel, S. & Yoon, C. Consumer Neuroscience: Applications, Challenges, and Possible Solutions.
J. Mark. Res. 52, 427–435 (2015).
18. Winler, I. et al. Frontal EEG asymmetry based classication of emotional valence using common spatial patterns. Worls Acad. Sci.
Eng. Technol. 45, 373–378 (2010).
19. Vecchiato, G. et al. Changes in brain activity during the observation of TV commercials by using EEG, GS and H measurements.
Brain Topogr. 23, 165–179 (2010).
20. Carver, C. S. Approach, Avoidance, and the Self-egulation of Aect and Action. Motiv. Emot. 30, 105–110 (2006).
21. Deitz, G. D. & Coleman, J. T. EEG-Based Measures versus Panel atings Predicting Social Media-Based Behavioral esponse to
Super Bowl Ads. Source J. Advert. Res. 56 (2016).
22. Johnson, . . et al. Drowsiness/alertness algorithm development and validation using synchronized EEG and cognitive
performance to individualize a generalized model. Biological Psychology 87 (2011).
23. dos Santos, . O. J. & de Oliveira, J. H. C. Eye Tracing in Neuromareting: A esearch Agenda for Mareting Studies. Int. J. Psychol.
Stud. 7, 32–42 (2015).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
7
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:15102 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-51567-1
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
24. Michael, N., James, . & Michael, I. Australia’s cognitive, aective and conative destination image: an Emirati tourist perspective. J.
Islam. Mark. 9, 36–59 (2018).
25. amissoon, H. & Uysal, M. S. e eects of perceived authenticity, information search behaviour, motivation and destination
imagery on cultural behavioural intentions of tourists. Curr. Issues Tour. 14, 537–562 (2011).
26. Gartner, W. C. Image Formation Process. J. Travel Tour. Mark. 2, 191–216 (1994).
27. oc, F., Josiassen, A. & Assaf, A. G. Advancing destination image: e destination content mo del. Ann. Tour. Res. 61, 28–44 (2016).
28. Li, S., Walters, G., Pacer, J. & Scott, N. Using sin conductance and facial electromyography to measure emotional responses to
tourism advertising. Curr. Issues Tour. 21, 1761–1783 (2018).
29. Li, Q., Huang, Z. (Joy) & Christianson, . Visual attention toward tourism photographs with text: An eye-tracing study. To ur.
Manag. 54, 243–258 (2016).
30. Wang, Y. & Spars, B. A. An Eye-Tracing Study of Tourism Photo Stimuli. J. Travel Res. 55, 588–602 (2016).
31. oelstra, S. et al. DEAP: A Database for Emotion Analysis;Using Physiological Signals. IEEE Trans. Aect. Comput. 3, 18–31 (2012).
32. Hubert, M. & enning, P. A current overview of consumer neuroscience. J. Consum. Behav. 7, 272–292 (2008).
33. armarar, U. . & Plassmann, H. Consumer Neuroscience: Past, Present, and Future. Organ. Res. Methods 22, 174–195 (2019).
34. ustichini, A. Neuroeconomics: Present and future. Games Econ. Behav. 52, 201–212 (2005).
35. Li, S., Scott, N. & Walters, G. Current and potential methods for measuring emotion in tourism experiences: a review. Curr. Issues
Tou r. 18, 805–827 (2015).
36. Matuin, M., Ohme, . & Bosho, C. Toward a Better Understanding Of Advertising Stimuli Pro cessing. J. Advert. Res. 56, 205–216
(2016).
37. Hulme, O. J., Sov, M., Chadwic, M. J., Siebner, H. . & amsøy, T. Z. Sparse encoding of automatic visual association in
hippocampal networs. Neuroimage 102, 458–464 (2014).
38. Berridge, . C., obinson, T. E. & Aldridge, J. W. Dissecting components of reward: ‘liing’, ‘wanting’, and learning. Curr Opin
Pharmacol 9, 65–73 (2010).
39. Haber, S. N. & nutson, B. e reward circuit: Lining primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 4–26
(2010).
40. Chua, H. F., Gonzalez, ., Taylor, S. F., Welsh, . C. & Liberzon, I. Decision-related loss: egret and disappointment. Neuroimage 47,
2031–2040 (2009).
41. Che-Ha, N., Nguyen, B., Yahya, W. ., Melewar, T. & Chen, Y. P. Country branding emerging from citizens’ emotions and the
perceptions of competitive advantage. J. Vacat. Mark. 22, 13–28 (2016).
42. Gunn, C. A. Vacationscape: designing tourist regions. (Van Nostrand einhold, 1988).
43. Mao, I. Y. & Zhang, H. Q. Structural elationships among Destination Preference, Satisfaction and Loyalty in Chinese Tourists to
Australia. Int. J. Tour. Res. 16, 201–208 (2014).
44. Echtner, C. M. & itchie, J. . B. e Measurement of Destination Image: An Empirical. Assessment. J. Travel Res. 31, 3–13 (1993).
45. Pezena, I. & Buchta, C. Measuring the resemblance between pictorial and verbal city image spaces. Int. J. Cult. Tour. Hosp. Res. 6,
326–339 (2012).
46. Manhas, P. S., Manrai, L. A. & Manrai, A. . ole of tourist destination development in building its brand image: A conceptual
model. J. Econ. Financ. Adm. Sci. 21, 25–29 (2016).
47. Lee, J. (Jiyeon). Visitors’ Emotional esponses to the Festival. Environment. J. Travel Tour. Mark. 31, 114–131 (2014).
48. Yüsel, A. & Yüsel, F. Shopping ris perceptions: Eects on tourists’ emotions, satisfaction and expressed loyalty intentions. Tou r.
Manag. 28, 703–713 (2007).
49. Bigné, J. E., Andreu, L. & Gnoth, J. e theme par experience: An analysis of pleasure, arousal and satisfaction. Tour. Manag. 26,
833–844 (2005).
50. Hosany, S. & Gilber t, D. Dimensions of Tourists’ Emotional Experiences Towards Hedonic Holiday Destinations. SSRN Electron. J.,
https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1871768 (2009).
51. Han, H. & Jeong, C. Multi-dimensions of patrons’ emotional experiences in upscale restaurants and their role in loyalty formation:
Emotion scale improvement. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 32, 59–70 (2013).
52. Lin, I. Y. & Mattila, A. S. estaurant Servicescape, Service Encounter, and Perceived Congruency on Customers’ Emotions and
Satisfaction. J. Hosp. Mark. Manag. 19, 819–841 (2010).
53. Prayag, G., Hosany, S. & Odeh, . The role of tourists’ emotional experiences and satisfaction in understanding behavioral
intentions. J. Destin. Mark. Manag. 2, 118–127 (2013).
54. Faullant, ., Matzler, . & Mooradian, T. A. Personality, basic emotions, and satisfaction: Primary emotions in the mountaineering
experience. Tour. Manag. 32, 1423–1430 (2011).
55. Sheen, M., emp, S. & ubin, D. Twins dispute memory ownership: A new false memory phenomenon. Mem. Cognit. 29, 779–788
(2001).
56. Dijstra, W., Smit, J. H. & Comijs, H. C. Using Social Desirability Scales in esearch among the Elderly. Qual. Quant. 35, 107–115
(2001).
57. Nederhof, A. J. Methods of coping with social desirability bias: A review. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 15, 263–280 (1985).
58. Ambler, T., Ioannides, A. & ose, S. Brands on the brain: Neuro-images of advertising. Bus. Strateg. Rev. 11, 17–30 (2000).
59. Goodall, B. How tourists choose their holidays: An analytical framewor. In Marketing in the tourism industry (eds Goodall, B. &
Ashworth, G.) 1–17 (outledge, 1990).
60. Decrop, A. Personal Aspects of Vacationers’ Decision Maing Processes: An Interpretivist Approach. J. Travel Tour. Mark. 8, 59–68
(2000).
61. Lin, C.-H., Morais, D. B., erstetter, D. L. & Hou, J.-S. Examining the ole of Cognitive and Aective Image in Predicting Choice
Across Natural, Developed, and eme-Par Destinations. J. Travel Res. 46, 183–194 (2007).
62. Chon, .-S. Self-image/destination image congruity. Ann. Tour. Res. 19, 360–363 (1992).
63. Sirgy, M. J. & Su, C. Destination Image, Self-Congruity, and Travel Behavior: Toward an Integrative Model. J. Travel Res. 38, 340–352
(2000).
64. Tapachai, N. & Warysza, . An Examination of the ole of Benecial Image in Tourist Destination Selection. J. Travel Res. 39,
37–44 (2000).
65. Woodside, A. G. & Lysonsi, S. A General Model Of Traveler Destination Choice. J. Travel Res. 27, 8–14 (1989).
66. Eringa, . & Zhou, S. A visual analysis of a cultural tourism destination. Res. Hosp. Manag. 5, 85–92 (2015).
67. Yüsel, A. & Agül, O. Tourism management. Tour. Manag. 28, 714–725 (2007).
68. Pieters, . & Wedel, M. Attention Capture and Transfer in Advertising: Brand, Pictorial, and Text-Size Eects. J. Mark. 68, 36–50
(2004).
69. Pan, B., Zhang, L. & Law, . e Complex Matter of Online Hotel Choice. Cornell Hosp. Q. 54, 74–83 (2013).
70. Connell, J. Toddlers, tourism and Tobermory: Destination mareting issues and television-induced tourism. Tour. Manag. 26,
763–776 (2005).
71. Hanefors, M. & Mossberg, L. TV travel shows — A pre-taste of the destination. J. Vacat. Mark. 8, 235–246 (2002).
72. Goossens, C. Tourism information and pleasure motivation. Ann. Tour. Res. 27, 301–321 (2000).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
8
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:15102 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-51567-1
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Acknowledgements
is research was funded by a Research Incentive Fund (RIF) by Zayed University, United Arab Emirates.
Author contributions
All authors were involved in the study design, interpretation and writing the manuscript. N.M. and I.M. were
instrumental in providing the theoretical background for tourism research, while T.Z.R. provided the review
of prior consumer neuroscience work. T.Z.R. was responsible for data collection, data handling and statistical
analyses.
Competing interests
Dr. N.M. and Dr. I.M. declare no competing interests. Dr. T.Z.R. is the founder and owner of Neurons Inc,
which is a consumer neuroscience company. is research was funded by the United Arab Emirates Ministry of
Travel and Tourism. Authors’ employment and remuneration do not depend on the outcomes or publication of
this study. e authors declare no other nancial or otherwise competing conicts of interest.
Additional information
Supplementary information is available for this paper at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-51567-1.
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to T.Z.R.
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Cre-
ative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. e images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not per-
mitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the
copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
© e Author(s) 2019
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
... This research was able to demonstrate that the present context has pushed companies to increasingly integrate a consumer's perceptions about the quality of a product and, subsequently, a consumer's overall assessment, into their approach [1,[14][15][16][17][18]. Indeed, companies have been forced to use constant innovation in order to better meet the needs of their consumers [16]. ...
... The user's sensation during this interaction with equipment can influence their purchasing behavior, their rate of use, etc. [1,3,14,15]. For this reason, it appeared relevant to assess a user's sensations when touching a product in the shape of a computer mouse. ...
... In order to define a unique sensory vocabulary for a given population, a psychophysical approach was used. Several psychological studies have attempted to determine what descriptive words (descriptors) could be used for the sense of touch and, especially, for the sensations related to textures [12,14,[17][18][19]. A descriptor is a term that is defined as precisely as possible to describe a sensation, and its role is to be understood in a homogeneous way by all subjects. ...
Article
Full-text available
Based on a comparative method, this paper discusses a sustainable perspective for the use of a certain bio-based material instead of synthetic materials, using human beings, with their sensory perceptions, as the central measurement tools. The innovative eco-design approaches are aimed at radical environmental improvements by focusing on the services provided to consumers. In improving the quality of a product, equity and environmental harmony have become issues and constant challenges in companies’ quests for modernization. In order to achieve this goal, one of the solutions taken into account by companies in order to be increasingly competitive is to replace, sometimes partially and other times totally, synthetic materials with new non-food, bio-based materials in the manufacturing process. The approach in this paper is aimed at better integrating tactile characteristics in designing green products. The detailed review of the literature shows that a consumer’s subjective perception is of paramount importance in their decision to accept a new product. Focusing on the sensory characteristics of materials with bio-based and synthetic origins, this paper draws conclusions about their resemblances and differences. The various subjective sensations when touching the two types of material are compared in order to obtain results that can protect the environment in the future.
... To distinguish between different destination choices, Zoëga Ramsøy et al. [23] explored whether the direct emotional responses of visitors and the cognitive load toward various places may predict their stated preferences after monitoring them watching images and videos related to destinations using ET and EEG. They enlisted 32 individuals and discovered that combining subconscious measurements and conscious scores would improve forecasts of their actual decisions. ...
... As an illustration, Michael et al. [22] and Zoëga Ramsøy et al. [23] applied both modalities to 30 and 32 participants, respectively. Both research groups sought to comprehend the immediate, underlying emotional and cognitive reactions that are involved in the preferences for vacation destinations. ...
... They employed images with text indicating various risk levels from well-known Chinese travel websites for an ET experiment. In addition to using images with text, some experiments merged graphics with videos [22,23,31]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Neuro-tourism is the application of neuroscience in tourism to improve marketing methods of the tourism industry by analyzing the brain activities of tourists. Neuro-tourism provides accurate real-time data on tourists’ conscious and unconscious emotions. Neuro-tourism uses the methods of neuromarketing such as brain–computer interface (BCI), eye-tracking, galvanic skin response, etc., to create tourism goods and services to improve tourist experience and satisfaction. Due to the novelty of neuro-tourism and the dearth of studies on this subject, this study offered a comprehensive analysis of the peer-reviewed journal publications in neuro-tourism research for the previous 12 years to detect trends in this field and provide insights for academics. We reviewed 52 articles indexed in the Web of Science (WoS) core collection database and examined them using our suggested classification schema. The results reveal a large growth in the number of published articles on neuro-tourism, demonstrating a rise in the relevance of this field. Additionally, the findings indicated a lack of integrating artificial intelligence techniques in neuro-tourism studies. We believe that the advancements in technology and research collaboration will facilitate exponential growth in this field.
... an example of the combination of EEG and GSR,Bastiaansen et al. (2019) employ it to assess the effectiveness of tourist destination marketing content in films. As an example of combining EEG and eye tracking,Ramsøy et al. (2019) employed these two neuroscience methods to test whether direct emotional and cognitive responses to travel destination (through the viewing of images and videos) is indicative of subsequent stated destination preference. Eye tracking on its own is also employed.Muñoz-Leiva et al. (2019), for instance, crossed it with self-report data to analyse advertising effectiveness in social media in terms of customers' visual attention and memory recall. ...
... BaraybarFernández et al. (2023) combined EEG with heart rate and electrodermal response to study the effects of emotional stimuli in advertising.Finally, a trend in the application of neuroscience techniques in recent years is its triangulation with self-report data. The mentioned works ofRamsøy et al. (2019),Muñoz-Leiva et al. (2019), andHsu and 15 ...
Article
Full-text available
The academic discourse surrounding tourism's interdisciplinary approach has long piqued the interest of the scholarly community. More recently, attention has pivoted towards the intersection of neuroscience and neuromarketing within the realm of tourism, giving rise to the concept of "neurotourism". This emergence requires a comprehensive elucidation of the relevance of neuromarketing and neuroscience to the field of tourism, thereby addressing the imperative to bridge existing knowledge gaps. In light of this, the present paper endeavours to fulfil this objective by synthesizing global research in this domain. The study adopts an interdisciplinary approach, employing a hybrid systematic review methodology that encompasses a literature review encompassing nine key indicators and a bibliometric analysis through co-word analysis of author keywords. In total, our investigation unearthed 45 papers from Scopus, each exploring the application of neuroscience and neuromarketing theories and methods within the context of tourism. Among these, three papers delve into the concept of neurotourism. In this paper, we underscore the deep connection between neuroscience, neuroscientific methodologies, and neuromarketing within the realm of tourism research. The outcomes of this research significantly enhance our comprehension of the current state of neurotourism research, revealing both existing voids and emerging areas of interest. Furthermore, this study introduces a pioneering methodological approach, fusing Scival topic prominence and hybrid systematic review techniques into bibliometric analysis. Ultimately, our findings illuminate a notable research lacuna, presenting a fertile terrain for prospective investigations. Additionally, we deliberate on current trends and propose directions for future research within the neurotourism landscape.
... The choice index measures the alteration of the beta and gamma frequency bands in both frontal hemispheres, with a higher value representing a greater possibility of making a choice [33]. ...
... The choice index calculates the inconstancy of the beta and gamma frequency bands in both frontal hemispheres where a higher value represents more likelihood for making a decision [33]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Individual choices and preferences are important factors that impact decision making. Artificial intelligence can predict decisions by objectively detecting individual choices and preferences using natural language processing, computer vision, and machine learning. Brain–computer interfaces can measure emotional reactions and identify brain activity changes linked to positive or negative emotions, enabling more accurate prediction models. This research aims to build an individual choice prediction system using electroencephalography (EEG) signals from the Shanghai Jiao Tong University emotion and EEG dataset (SEED). Using EEG, we built different deep learning models, such as a convolutional neural network, long short-term memory (LSTM), and a hybrid model to predict choices driven by emotional stimuli. We also compared their performance with different classical classifiers, such as k-nearest neighbors, support vector machines, and logistic regression. We also utilized ensemble classifiers such as random forest, adaptive boosting, and extreme gradient boosting. We evaluated our proposed models and compared them with previous studies on SEED. Our proposed LSTM model achieved good results, with an accuracy of 96%.
... These studies explore various aspects of tourism, such as destination advertisements, accommodation, experiences, and pricing, using techniques such as electrodermal activity (EDA), electroencephalogram, eye tracking (ET), and facial electromyography (fEMG), which, in many cases, demonstrate better performances than traditional self-report measures (Bastiaansen et al., 2018(Bastiaansen et al., , 2022González-Rodríguez et al., 2020;S. Li et al., 2018a;Michael et al., 2019;Zoëga Ramsøy et al., 2019). The combination of neuro-techniques and traditional surveys has been highlighted as beneficial for understanding emotional experiences in tourism (Hadinejad et al., 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
As tourism research has paid limited attention to children, this study investigates children's reactions to tourism development, focusing on their unique viewpoints on the World Heritage Site of Dubrovnik, Croatia. It employed cognitive neuroscience methods with 397 participants, revealing that, despite their preference for sustainable tourism scenarios, children exhibit a notable fixation on images emblematic of overtourism and associated challenges, particularly overcrowding. When exposed to sustainable tourism photographs, there was an observable increase in physiological arousal, albeit not as pronounced as when confronted with an overtourism scenario. Intriguingly, regardless of the scenario, children predominantly expressed neutral emotions. Within the sustainable tourism context, gender differences manifest as girls exhibiting lower levels of place attachment. Furthermore, inner-city residents exhibit diminished levels of nature connectedness, and emotions are indirectly linked to nature connectedness, place attachment, or pro-environmental behaviour. Conversely, in the unsustainable scenario, older children and inner-city residents exhibited a heightened sense of neutrality towards overtourism-related concerns, whereas those outside the inner city displayed a stronger affinity for nature connectedness. Positive emotions were negatively associated with nature connectedness and pro-environmental behaviour but positively associated with place attachment. Accordingly, this study advocates a more inclusive and sustainable future through children's empowerment in tourism development.
... Нейрофизиологические исследования и изыскания в поведенческой экономике показали, что зачастую принятие решений происходит неосознанно, вопреки объективной выгоде [30]. Если говорить о «быстрых» решениях, которые не связаны с длительным целенаправленным обдумыванием какой-либо проблемы, то мозг принимает решение в течение нескольких миллисекунд (500-1000 мс), за 30 миллисекунд до того, как оно осознается [15]. ...
Article
Full-text available
p style="text-align: justify;">The article is devoted to a review of modern research on the neuronal foundations of moral decision-making. Psychological approaches to the study of the problem of decision-making and moral choice are analyzed. The data of empirical studies of their temperamental and characterological correlates are presented. The results of neurobiological and neurophysiological studies of worldview and moral assessments, taking into account age and cross-cultural factors, are discussed. Empirical studies of the neurophysiological foundations of decision-making in persons with mental disorders are highlighted. It is concluded that different moral tasks can involve different neural mechanisms. The significance of the results of the detected activity of the brain departments for understanding the neurophysiological and psychophysiological correlates of moral decision-making, and allowing the transition to understanding higher, conscious regulators of behavior, is substantiated. The prospects of comparing the dynamics of the activity of brain structures with the personal profile and the level of subjective stress of a person for the development of prognostic and diagnostic methods for assessing behavior in life-threatening situations are indicated.</p
... However, as implicit perceptions of a brand play a fundamental role (Walla et al., 2017), classic self-reports as well as data mining could fall short. Moreover, while big data is capable of collecting a large amount of data on consumers or market trends, and subsequently building the base for developing marketing actions (Seung-Pyo et al., 2018), consumer neuroscience can be a helpful tool when emotions or subconscious phenomena play a fundamental role (Ramsoy et al., 2019). ...
... In addition to eye tracking, EEG technology has also been applied to landscape evaluation and preference. The development of neuroscience over the last century has greatly enriched people's understanding of the bioelectrical signals emitted by brain neurons [50]. EEG is a physiological signal that uses physiological methods to measure nerve signals on the surface of the scalp and record them. ...
Article
Full-text available
Plants play a very important role in landscape construction. In order to explore whether different living environment will affect people's preference for the structural features of plant organs, this study examined 26 villagers and 33 college students as the participants, and pictures of leaves, flowers and fruits of plants as the stimulus to conduct eye-tracking and EEG detection experiments. We found that eye movement indicators can explain people's visual preferences, but they are unable to find differences in preferences between groups. EEG indicators can make up for this deficiency, which further reveals the difference in psychological and physiological responses between the two groups when viewing stimuli. The final results show that the villagers and the students liked leaves best, preferring aciculiform and leathery leaves; solitary, purple and capitulum flowers; and medium-sized, spathulate, black and pear fruits. In addition, it was found that the overall attention of the villagers when watching stimuli was far lower than that of the students, but the degree of meditation was higher. With regard to eye movement and EEG, the total duration of fixations is highly positively correlated with the number of fixations, and the average pupil size has a weak negative correlation with attention. On the contrary, the average duration of fixations has a weak positive correlation with meditation. Generally speaking, we believe that Photinia×fraseri, Metasequoia glyptostroboides, Photinia serratifolia, Koelreuteria bipinnata and Cunninghamia lanceolata are superior landscape building plants in rural areas and on campuses; Pinus thunbergii, Myrica rubra, Camellia japonica and other plants with obvious features and bright colours are also the first choice in rural landscapes; and Yulania biondii, Cercis chinensis, Hibiscus mutabilis and other plants with simple structures are the first choice in campus landscapes. This study is of great significance for selecting plants for landscape construction and management according to different environments and local conditions.
... Novel techniques such as the use of biometric measurements (including facial expressions, heart rate, skin conductance, body temperature, and eye-tracking), virtual environments (virtual and augmented reality), and artificial senses (e-nose and e-tongue) are being explored as tools to understand the complex nature of human responses in sensory tests. Human reactions can be roughly divided into self-reported (conscious) and subconscious responses [20]. Traditional sensory techniques are based on self-reported responses, which are the results of the cognitive processing after perception [2]. ...
Article
Sensory evaluation incorporates methodologies from different scientific disciplines. Studying humans’ reactions to different stimuli, such as foods or beverages, is complex as multiple dimensions are involved in sensory perception. Currently, traditional sensory protocols (discrimination, descriptive, and affective) are heavily employed in the industry and/or academia for testing a wide variety of stimuli. Nonetheless, these methodologies have drawbacks such as physiological and psychological biases when evaluating participants’ behaviors and choices. Novel methods have been developed to capture more holistic responses from sensory panels. This brief review article provides a snapshot of these current novel methods. New techniques such as the use of biometrics (facial expressions, heart rate, skin conductance, body temperature, and eye-tracking), virtual environments (virtual and/or augmented reality), and artificial smart senses are being explored as tools to understand the complex nature of human perception to foods and beverages.
Article
Full-text available
Since its inception, the field of consumer neuroscience and neuromarketing has undergone significant development. The principal objective of this work is to identify current research and to define emerging topics in both consumer neuroscience and neuromarketing using electroencephalography (EEG) since no studies have thus far examined this issue. To this end, a bibliometric analysis was conducted with the Science Mapping Software tool SciMAT. In total, 497 articles published between 2002 and 2022 were examined. The analysis encompassed all research from brain regions, technologies, and marketing which can be applied for a better understanding of consumer behaviour. The main contribution of this work is the comprehensive and objective review of the topic, which highlights the potential interest in applying EEG to emerging technologies (e.g., augmented reality, mixed reality, or virtual reality), tourism marketing communications, healthy food products, consumer willingness-to-pay, service marketing, dynamic stimuli, and consumers’ emotions.
Article
Full-text available
The current paper investigates the value and application of a range of physiological and neuroscientific techniques in applied marketing research and consumer science, highlighting new insights from research in social psychology and neuroscience. We review measures of sweat secretion, heart rate, facial muscle activity, eye movements, and electrical brain activity, using techniques including skin conductance, pupillometry, eyetracking, and magnetic brain imaging. For each measure, after a brief explanation of the underlying technique, we illustrate concepts and mechanisms that the measure allows researchers in marketing and consumer science to investigate, with a focus on consumer attitudes and behavior. By providing reviews on recent research that applied these methods in consumer science and relevant related fields, we also highlight methodological and theoretical strengths and limitations, with an emphasis on ecological validity. We argue that the inclusion of physiological and neuroscientific techniques can advance consumer research by providing insights into the often unconscious mechanisms underlying consumer behavior. Therefore, such technologies can help researchers and marketing practitioners understand the mechanisms of consumer behavior and improve predictions of consumer behavior.
Article
Full-text available
Consumers frequently make decisions about how much they are willing to pay (WTP) for specific products and services, but little is known about the neural mechanisms underlying such calculations. In this study, we were interested in testing whether specific brain activation—the asymmetry in engagement of the prefrontal cortex—would be related to consumer choice. Subjects saw products and subsequently decided how much they were willing to pay for each product, while undergoing neuroimaging using electroencephalography. Our results demonstrate that prefrontal asymmetry in the gamma frequency band, and a trend in the beta frequency band that was recorded during product viewing was significantly related to subsequent WTP responses. Frontal asymmetry in the alpha band was not related to WTP decisions. Besides suggesting separate neuropsychological mechanisms of consumer choice, we find that one specific measure—the prefrontal gamma asymmetry—was most strongly related to WTP responses, and was most coupled to the actual decision phase. These findings are discussed in light of the psychology of WTP calculations, and in relation to the recent emergence of consumer neuroscience and neuromarketing.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose The purpose of this study is to understand the destination image perceptions about Australia – a Western culture country – as held by the rapidly increasing, high spending, culturally dissimilar new segment of travellers, the Emiratis[1] from the United Arab Emirates (UAE). Design/methodology/approach A qualitative methodology was used to understand the cognitive, affective and conative images of Australia. A structured categorisation matrix was used to analyse the data so that only aspects fitting the matrix were selected. Findings Within the cognitive variable, Australia was found to be pleasant, family oriented, a fun place, laid back and the local people friendly. From an affective factor perspective, Australia was seen as being exciting, because of the variety of activities available for these tourists. Exciting was expressed by words like fantastic, amazing and extreme experience. In terms of the conative variable most of the Emirati tourists expressed strong feelings to go back to Australia and to even re-visit with friends. They also mentioned that they would recommend Australia to family and friends. Research limitations/implications A limitation of this study was that our sample comprised informants mainly from the Emirates of Abu Dhabi and Dubai, the two largest Emirates of the nation. The study offers destination marketing organisations’ (DMOs) insights into Emirati travellers’ perceptions about Australia, which would benefit destination marketing. Originality/value This study examines the under researched area of how Australia – with its liberal Western culture – could be better marketed to the growing numbers of culturally conservative, high spending Emirati outbound tourists from the officially Islamic UAE, and also more generally to the socio-culturally homogeneous Gulf Cooperation Council region that the UAE is part of. Whilst destination image is an intensively analysed topic within the realm of tourism research, and reportedly a powerful influence on destination choice, the extant literature on how Australia is perceived as a travel destination by Emiratis is scant. For DMO’s attempting to attract wealthy Emirati tourists into Australia, this research is valuable and timely, as several Emiratis are seeking newer travel destinations away from the Western hemisphere, where the general anti-Arab/Islamic sentiments are currently quite strong.
Article
Full-text available
This research investigates how a relatively unknown town that is elected as cultural capital of Europe can use visual materials to attract visitors from totally different areas in the world, particularly China. The study uses visual cues for two purposes: first, to evaluate the motivating factors that lead travellers with different cultural background to select their travel destination, and second, to explore the influence of visual communication in the promotion of cultural tourism. The research studied the impact of visual information during several stages in the visit: before the visit in the decision-making process; during the visit in the experience stage; and after the visit in the post-experience evaluation. It was found that visual material can help to frame the experience in all three stages. For that reason it is advisable for destinations to employ some kind of visual identity system management to package the city image into a clear brand.
Article
Full-text available
Although an objective and increasingly common technique in marketing, media and psychology, psycho-physiological measures are rarely used in tourism research to detect tourism consumers’ spontaneous emotional responses. This study examines the use of psycho-physiological measures in tourism and in particular explores the usefulness of skin conductance and facial electromyography methods in tracking emotional responses to destination advertisements. Thirty-three participants were exposed to three destination advertisements while their self-report ratings, real-time skin conductance and facial electromyography data as well as post hoc interview data were obtained. The results demonstrate that, compared with self-report measures, psychophysiological measures are able to better distinguish between different destination advertisements, and between different dimensions of emotion. Participants’ affective experience reported in post hoc interviews was found to be consistent with emotional peaks identified from continuous facial electromyography and skin conductance monitoring. These results validate the ability of psychophysiological techniques to capture moment-to-moment emotional responses and it is concluded that psycho-physiological methods are useful in measuring emotional responses to tourism advertising. Methodological insights regarding the constraints associated with the use and application of psychophysiological methods are discussed.
Article
Insights and tools from neuroscience are of great value to marketers. Neuroscientific techniques allow consumer researchers to understand the fundamental neural underpinnings of psychological processes that drive consumer behavior, and elucidate the “black box” that is the consumer's mind. In the following review, we provide an overview of the fundamental tenets of consumer neuroscience, selectively outline key areas of marketing that consumer neuroscience has contributed to, compare and contrast neuroscientific tools and methods, and discuss future directions for neurophysiological work in marketing. In doing so, we illustrate the broad substantive landscape that neuroscience can add value to within marketing.
Article
In this article, we give an overview of the growing field of consumer neuroscience and discuss when and how it is useful to integrate neurophysiological data into research conducted in business fields. We first discuss the foundational elements of consumer neuroscience and showcase a range of studies that highlight the ways that neuroscientific research and theory can add to existing lines of research in marketing. Next, we discuss the new domains and questions that brain data allow us to address, such as an emerging ability to predict market-level behavior in a range of decision types. We conclude by providing insights about the emerging frontiers in the field that we think will have an important impact on our understanding of marketing behavior, as well as organizational behavior.
Article
Place branding is complex due to its interdisciplinary nature, the highly competitive market, diverse stakeholder needs, and its ability to influence national priorities. This paper looks at the role of visual and auditory branding signatures in forming place attachment within three groups of stakeholders (nationals, expatriates, tourists). While research has focused on place consumers from either tourism or government perspectives, there is a need to take an interdisciplinary lens to look at new methodologies to see how place brands can manage multiple stakeholders. This paper presents new methodology for place brand studies called causal layered analysis. From a scholarly point of view, the paper presents a unique methodology in destination branding studies that aligns multiple stakeholder views yet still roots visual and auditory signatures of stakeholder perception of the nation's brand through its heritage. The paper justifies the importance of story-telling and collecting multiple brand interpretations to create place attachment. The findings highlight the importance of resolving multiple stakeholder perspectives and the importance of the stories that can link various narratives that are important for nation branding and building, since common visuals have layered interpretations. While this study is qualitative in nature, the findings show that there is a need for more theory building in this field. From a practitioner's point of view, organizations can use the methodology for perception mapping to create a distinct place communication platform. This, in turn, can reinforce a place's identity based on both heritage and modernity.
Article
Emotional reactions to marketing stimuli are essential to tourist destination marketing, yet difficult to validly measure. A neuromarketing experiment was peformed to establish whether brain event-related potentials (ERPs), elicited by destination photos, can be used to evaluate the effectiveness of tourist destination marketing content in movies. Two groups of participants viewed pictures from the cities of Bruges and Kyoto. Prior to viewing the pictures, one group saw an excerpt from the movie In Bruges, which positively depicts Bruges' main tourist attractions. The other group saw a movie excerpt that did not feature Bruges (the Rum Diary). An early emotional response was osberved to the subsequently presented Bruges pictures for the In Bruges group only; no reliable between-group differences were found in ERPs to pictures from Kyoto. In conclusion, EEG-based neuromarketing is a valuable tool for evaluating the effectiveness of destination marketing, and popular movies can positively influence affective destination image.